The Developmental Transition to Secondary
Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year:
A Microgenetic Case Study
Alan Fogel
Department of Psychology
University of Utah
alan.fogel@psych.utah.edu.
Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros
Department of Psychology
University of Utah
Abstract: To explore the relational-historical processes by which infants may develop an
intersubjective sense of self in a relational context, one mother-infant dyad was observed
weekly across the 9-month developmental transition, using a microgenetic research design. Qualitative research methods were used to study the developmental changes in the
dynamic system of the mother-infant relationship. We discovered four developmental periods that marked qualitative differences in the patterns of communication between mother
and infant and the infant’s emerging self-awareness. In the first developmental period, the
mother’s attunements to the infant temporarily stabilize the infant’s attention to her own
action. The ability to self-attune, crucial to emerging self and other-awareness, becomes a
dynamically stable “attractor” by the second period, in which the infant attends consistently to her own and her mother’s behavior. This leads to the emergence of a stable communication frame for pounding in the third period, involving increasing temporal contiguity of self and other actions, brief glimpses of secondary intersubjectivity during the
pounding frame, and changes in the mother’s behavior (such as emphasizing the word
“you”). These changes lead to the consolidation and spread of secondary intersubjectivity to other frames in the mother-infant relationships (as reported by the mother’s diary)
in the fourth developmental period.
Introduction
Some theories of the development of self-awareness in childhood have emphasized representational, reflective types of self-knowledge (Harter, 1999; Pipp, 1993).
Most developmental psychologists agree that toddlers around 18 months of age show
63
64
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
evidence of reflective self-awareness when they begin to recognize themselves in a
mirror and separate the “I-self” from the “me-self” (Harter, 1999; Lewis, 1995). Research on infants prior to mirror self-recognition, however, have revealed that prerepresentational forms of self-awareness can be found in the familiar sensations and
movements of the body, the experience of emotions, and the awareness of others’ emotions and intentions (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998; Gibson, 1993; Neisser,
1993; Rochat, 1995; Stern, 1985; Tomasello, 1993; Trevarthen, 1993).
In this paper, we are interested in examining the developmental transition in self- and
other-awareness that occurs around the age of 9 months. This developmental transition
has been described in many different ways in the infancy literature. Some focus on infants’ developing skills to coordinate joint attention, the onset of deictic gestures such as
pointing, and the awareness that the other person has the intention to look at or refer to
the same object to which their own attention is directed (Carpenter et al. 1998; Gustafson,
Green, & West, 1979; Tomasello, 1993). Others focus on the change in the infant’s
awareness of the feelings and intentions that are shared in the relationship between the
parent and infant. Trevarthen calls this new developmental level “secondary intersubjectivity” (Trevarthen, 1993; 1998; Trevarthen & Hubley, 1978), defined as coordinating
and sharing with another person one’s attention, feelings and intentions toward a third
pole of an object, event, or action. Secondary intersubjectivity implies that the infant is
aware of co-affectivity and co-agency with another person in relation to something else.
Similarly, Stern (1985) proposes the emergence of the “intersubjective self”
around 9 months of age in which the infant is thought to notice that others are affected
by her agency just as she is affected by the agency of others. Secondary intersubjectivity develops after “primary intersubjectivity” (2–9 months), in which the infant is
aware of moving and feeling in relation to another person, a “resonance” that is felt
during interaction and that feels different to the infant than when alone (Trevarthen,
1993). A similar perspective is taken by Stern (1993) who describes the “core self”
(2–9 months) in terms of emotions such that it is “not only the feeling experienced but
also the experience of interpersonal evocation or regulation or sharing” (p. 205).
Qualitative clinical case study observations of parent-child interactions suggest
that infants in the first half year whose parents mirror the their emotions and attune to
them are more likely by the end of their first year to show an awareness of and emotional responsiveness to others, and to have a secure attachment relationship with their
parents (Beebe & Lachmann, 2002; Fogel, 2001; Stern, 1985; Winnicott, 1971, 1990).
Quantitative research on larger samples has shown that parental responsiveness,
warmth, and emotional openness are linked to more adaptive child functioning, including secure attachment, social competence, and self-esteem (Ainsworth, Behar,
Waters, & Wall, 1978; Baumrind, 1989; Black & Logan, 1995; Coopersmith, 1967; De
Wolff & Van IJzendoorn, 1997; Feshbach, 1987; Koren-Karie, Oppenheim, Dolev,
Sher, & Etzion-Carasso, 2002; Maccoby, 1992; Oppenheim & Koren-Karie, 2002;
Oppenheim, Koren-Karie, & Sagi, 2001). In addition, normally developing mothers
and infants both change their behavior in relation to each other in coordinated ways
over time, suggesting that the dyad develops as a shared affective and communicative
system (Beebe & Lachmann, 2002; Hsu & Fogel, 2001; Lavelli & Fogel, 2005).
From the late fetal period, infants are aware of at least some of their bodily states,
movements, and senses (Damasio, 1999; Fifer, Monk, & Grose-Fifer, 2001). Multiple,
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
65
repeated experiences of social interaction form experience-dependent neuromotor
pathways of how to do things with intimate others, called “implicit relational knowing” (Beebe, 1998; Lyons-Ruth, 1998; Panksepp, 2001; Schore, 2001; Stern, 1998).
The right limbic system and pre-frontal cortex appear to be especially attuned to implicit relational knowing which is manifested in continuing, largely automatic, patterns of doing things with others (Shonkoff, & Phillips, 2000; Schore, 2001; Siegel,
2001). The neurological traces of these repeated interpersonal and emotional routines
may be the foundation of “internal working models” of attachment (Bowlby, 1973;
Bretherton, 1990, 1991, 1993).
Intersubjectivity can also be understood from a dynamic systems perspective because the awareness of the self goes beyond the skin boundaries of the individual to
include the “self-regulating other” (e.g., Beebe & Lachman, 2002; Stern, 1985). Pointing, for example, implies the awareness of another person who shares the same focus
of attention. From the infant’s perspective at the end of the first year, the personal intention toward an object, the gesture (pointing), the referent (an object), and the other
person appear to be part of a single system. Social referencing, to take another example of an infant’s awareness at the end of the first year, implies a personal intention to
regulate emotion that is organized together with directing attention to another’s emotional expression, apparently suggesting that the infant’s emotion regulation is organized into a dyadic system.
There are many experimental studies showing a developmental transition at 9
months in self-awareness, emotional communication, attachment behavior, and cognition (Behne, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2005; Camaioni, Perucchini, Bellagamba, &
Colonessi, 2004; Legerstee, 2005; Phillips, Wellman, & Spelke, 2002; Sommerville
& Woodward, 2005; Striano & Rochat, 1999; Tomasello, 1993). These studies are
fundamental in establishing, via experimental manipulation, that particular forms of
infant behavior indeed reveal developmental changes in self- and other-awareness.
On the other hand, however, they do not reveal anything about the process of change
by which new levels of self-awareness emerge. Theoretical explanations refer to
shifts in cognitive understanding of self and others or to neurological changes or to
changes in social communication, but little is known about how these developments
occur in individual children in the context of their relationships with parents.
In order to study the change process from a dynamic systems perspective, two
strategies are required. First, one must examine multiple elements of the system as
they change together over time. As Thelen’s work on motor development from a dynamic systems perspective reveals, recurring linkages form across neurological, sensory, motor, and contextual domains. These so-called “attractors”—such as kicking,
crawling, and walking—are revealed to observers as recurring sequences and cooccurrences of coordinated action in relation to particular contexts (Thelen, Fisher, &
Ridley-Johnson, 1984; Thelen & Ulrich, 1991). Along the same lines, the infant’s
awareness of self and others can be viewed as a dynamic psychosocial system that partakes of the relationship between the infant and significant others (Fogel, 1993, 1995,
2001; Fogel & Thelen, 1987; Garvey & Fogel, in press; Granic, 2000; Shanker &
King, 2002). These attractors are called “frames” (cf. “formats,” Bruner, 1983): regularly recurring patterns of communication that involve coordinated action and shared
cognition and emotion (Fogel, 1993; Fogel, Garvey, Hsu, & West-Stroming, 2006).
66
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
Particular parent-infant games, like peek-a-boo or tickle, are frames in the relationship
system, as are book reading, mealtimes, bedtime rituals, and the like.
The second dynamic systems research strategy is to observe change as it is occurring, not simply before and after the change. One of the central methodological principles of dynamic systems as applied to the study of developmental change is the microgenetic research design (Flynn & Siegler, 2007; Fogel, 1990; Lavelli, Pantoja, Hsu,
Messinger, & Fogel, 2005; Siegler & Crowley, 1991; Thelen, 1990). Individual cases
are observed frequently across a key period of developmental change. This gives a
picture of the change process by observing before, during, and after with a sufficient
density of observations to reveal how the different components and emergent attractors of the dynamic system become organized and re-organized as a function of time
(Fogel et al., 2006). This is a very different approach to developmental research compared to cross-sectional experimental designs or to large-N longitudinal designs in
which presumed factors contributing to change are assessed via patterns of shared
variance across subjects. The latter uses statistical relationships between variables,
while dynamic systems research focuses on real time sequential and co-occurring relationships between observed actions and on historical changes over developmental
time within the same case.
Microgenetic designs have been mostly applied to the study of cognitive development (Piaget, 1952; Siegler & Crowley, 1991). There are, by contrast, only a few published microgenetic studies on the development of self-awareness. Trevarthen’s work,
cited above, is one example. Another study examines the developmental transition that
occurs after 18 months using a discourse analysis of monthly videotapes of mealtimes
with one child and her family (Forrester, 2001). Findings of this study show how the
parent-child discourse shifts in a way that leads to a growing awareness of the child’s
self-agency and dialogical positioning vis-à-vis the parents. It also shows clearly how
the self develops in relational frames. A similar approach is taken by Garvey and Fogel
(in press) on emotional development between 2 and 6 months.
The purpose of the present paper is to describe the developmental changes in the
communication system of one mother-infant dyad across the 9 month developmental
transition, i.e., the transition between primary and secondary intersubjectivity. Our
focus is on the process of change in secondary intersubjectivity, a topic which has not
been previously investigated. We selected a single representative frame, a spontaneous
game of pounding the table of a high chair in which the infant is sitting, and describe
the ways in which actions and emotions bear on the infant’s development of secondary intersubjectivity with respect to her mother.
Method
Participants
The dyad we selected for this case study was part of a group of 13 mother-infant
dyads that were videotaped weekly over the infants’ first year of life and bi-weekly in
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
67
their second year. Between 6 and 9 months, the infants were seated in a high chair with
their mothers opposite them. “Susan” is the youngest of three daughters in a Caucasian, middle-class family from a Midwestern U.S. city. So far as we could tell from
our videos, Susan was developing normally. Although the transition to secondary intersubjectivity occurred in all of our dyads, we chose Susan and her mother for this investigation primarily because communication patterns specific to intersubjectivity occurred frequently on their videos, and more often than in the other dyads. In the kinds
of free-play observations recorded here, there is a balance between letting each dyad
develop in its own way and providing sufficient control to capture phenomena of interest. Because this one dyad’s data had a primacy of content related to the subject of
this study, we cannot be certain that the change process in intersubjectivity was similar for the other dyads during times when our cameras were not on them.
Procedures
Video recordings. At the dyad’s first visit to the laboratory, Susan was 4 weeks
old. At this time, the mother “Sheryl” was asked to play with her infant in any way she
wanted. In subsequent sessions, no further instructions were given in order to leave the
dyad free rein to create their own relationship in the context of the laboratory setting.
At 27 weeks of age, Susan was moved into a high chair and at 42 weeks, the mother
and infant started playing at a low table. With Susan in the high chair, the dyad played
face-to-face without toys, although two toys were available (which could be used, for
instance, if the infant became fussy). For this study, we used only the high chair sessions between 27 and 40 weeks of age. The carpeted laboratory playroom (3.81 m ×
3.81 m) was equipped with two cameras, operated from the room next door. Videotapes utilized in the present study show split-screen recordings of the two cameras operated from the observation room. Mother’s and infant’s faces and upper bodies were
visible as much as possible. A time code giving elapsed minutes and seconds was superimposed on the screen. Susan and Sheryl made 11 visits to the laboratory with
Susan between 27 and 40 weeks of age and their interactions were video recorded for
1 hour and 55 minutes total (mean duration = 10 minutes, 30 seconds range 9:03–
11:17). Visits at 31, 38, and 39 weeks are missing because the dyad could not attend
and the one at 41 weeks is missing because of technical difficulties.
Journal. In addition to being videotaped every week, Sheryl was asked to make
daily or weekly journal entries, noting any changes in the infant’s social, emotional,
and physical development. The journal instructions did not mention intersubjectivity
as a focus, again, in order to allow the mother to use her own constructions of what
she considered important. For this study, entries were used that started at 26 weeks and
ended at 40 weeks. Sheryl made 38 near-daily entries between 26 and 34 weeks. Starting at 34 weeks and ending at 40 weeks, she made seven weekly entries. The second
author read and reread these 45 journal entries, focusing on any entries that were
deemed to be observations about the infant’s emerging self-awareness and pounding
behavior. All of these entries are given verbatim in this paper.
68
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
Qualitative Analyses and Data Selection
Qualitative research is particularly well-suited to the study of relationship change
because communication systems are regulated by the way people make meaning of
each other’s actions rather than by the physical parameters of those actions (such as
velocity and force). The intersubjective meaning of communicative actions can be
thought of as co-regulated within the system of communication in the same way as
any other pattern formation process studied from a dynamic systems perspective.
Qualitative research differs from traditional quantitative approaches by taking the observer’s perspective explicitly into account (Patton, 1990). This fits well with the dynamic systems perspective if we generalize the system under investigation to include
subject, environment, and observer (Fogel, 2006).
Many dynamic systems have been modeled as iterative processes in which innovation and variability transform one dynamically stable set of attractors into another
(van Geert, 1994; 1998). Multiple iterative passes through videotapes and narrative
transcriptions are at the heart of qualitative data analysis and interpretation. When one
studies developmental change qualitatively, therefore, the iterative process of communication sessions recorded over time becomes embedded within the iterative process of data analysis and interpretation (see Figure 1). This ethnographic approach becomes scientific to the extent that we make explicit the process of change in the
observer-observed system, and also provide sufficient detail from the transcriptions
for the reader to follow the discourse. For that reason, we provide narrative and photographic evidence in our results. More details about qualitative dynamic systems research can be found elsewhere (Fogel, 2006; Fogel et al., 2006). The complete narrative descriptions of this study are available by contacting the first author.
First Phase. Because the focus of this study was awareness of self and others related to intersubjectivity, we decided to focus our observations on moments in which
the infant paid visual, auditory, or tactile attention to parts of her own body, and/or to
her own actions or to her mother’s actions. Initial observations of the videos of the selected sessions in this dyad suggested that such moments most often occurred in the
context of a frame that eventually developed between the mother and the infant, in
which one partner would hit the tray of the high chair and the other would respond
(“the pounding game”). In this phase, we as observers are letting the form of the existing data guide the selection of segments to be observed. Because of the timeintensive commitment required by qualitative analysis, observers need to be highly selective in order to focus their efforts on those segments of data most relevant to the
research questions, in our case, the process of change in intersubjectivity.
Second Phase. All sessions were viewed to identify (by time code) all events in
which the infant hit the tray of the high chair, however slightly, or one of the toys. All
instances in which the mother hit or tapped the table with her hands or fingertips were
also selected because such maternal behaviors were often part of dyadic pounding
events (with the mother tapping and the infant pounding). These events were then captured in digital format utilizing video-editing software (Adobe Premiere 5.1; see Secrist, de Koeyer, Bell, & Fogel, 2002). In order to capture the context of the events,
video clips were captured starting 3 seconds before the onset of the pounding-related
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
69
Developmental time for participants
Real time sessions for participants
Initial
impression
Emergent
Interpretive
frame
Developmental time for research process
Real time observation and interpretation sessions
FIGURE 1. The Dynamics of the Observer-World System in Qualitative Research on
Developmental Change. The prospectively recorded material (narratives, videos) consists of repeated real-time observation sessions across a developmental transition (the
participant’s developmental change). The timeline of this material is nested within the
timeline of the research process in which the recorded material is repeatedly observed
and interpreted, which leads to a developmental change in the interpretation of the observations (a developmental change in the conceptualization of the researchers about
the recorded material). The results of the study are emergent from this dynamic iterative process of engagement and re-engagement with the recorded material.
behavior and ending 3 seconds after its offset. When the infant pounded repeatedly,
with only seconds between the poundings, this was captured as a single episode. We
included behavior indicating self-awareness related to the pounding in the captured
events, such as when the infant pounded and then looked intently at her hands, lying
still on the table. Also, we considered it part of the pounding event if Sheryl’s tapping
evolved into a tickling game. Such events were captured until the mother stopped tickling. The captured events containing pounding or tapping and related behaviors occurred for 41% of the total elapsed time in the selected high chair sessions (total time
44 minutes, 31 seconds). The mean duration per week of the captured events was 4:03
minutes (range 1:33 minutes, occurring during week 32, to 5:54 minutes, occurring
during week 34). Infant pounding events accounted for 67% of the total captured event
duration (29 minutes, 54 seconds), and maternal tapping and tickling games accounted
for the remaining 33% (14 minutes, 37 seconds).
After all the events were captured, they were compiled and edited in sequential order to form a “developmental movie” of pounding between 27 to 40 weeks. To
70
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
ensure that the movie contained all behaviors relevant to pounding, they were repeatedly checked against the original videotapes in an iterative process. If necessary, the
movie was edited by adding or deleting captured video material. The developmental
movie is a crucial step in this type of change process research. First of all, it collects
everything of interest in one relatively short documentary film. Second, it makes it extremely easy to view and review the movie in an iterative way in order to assess what
changes and what remains the same from one session to the next.
Third Phase. After this, the developmental movie was viewed repeatedly, both
within segments and for the entire movie. For a more in-depth view of what occurred,
detailed descriptive narratives were also written, in an iterative process called the constant comparative method in which narratives are re-written until nothing new can be
added (Patton, 1990; Strauss & Corbin, 1990). All socio-emotional events surrounding the pounding activities were described (i.e., gaze direction, emotional expressions,
vocalizations, intensity, and rhythm patterns). The developmental movie and narratives were then reviewed in an iterative process to compare the weekly sessions with
regard to the infant’s attention to the pounding, her emotional expressions, and dyadic
interaction patterns around the pounding behaviors.
Fourth Phase. After reviewing all infant pounding and maternal tapping or tickling events, a second developmental movie was created, including only those moments in which the infant focused on the pounding (i.e., by glancing or gazing at her
pounding hands, repeating the pounding actions, by looking at mother after mother
imitated her pounding actions, etc.). This was done because we concluded from our
repeated observations that these were the most relevant to intersubjectivity. We also
found that pounding events in which the infant was not focused on the pounding, or
in which the infant was pounding toward a toy, did not clearly contribute to the emergence of intersubjectivity (from our observations in the earlier phases).
Fifth Phase. Finally, a third developmental movie was created that contained only
those events in which mother and infant paid attention to the infant’s pounding
actions, attention being the key behavioral component of self-awareness. This final
movie was 15 minutes and 56 seconds in length. It was reviewed repeatedly in an iterative process to clarify the processes by which intersubjectivity within this dyadic
pounding frame eventually developed.
Each of these research phases required months of work and the final project lasted
several years. In the end, the results of this work emerged from this data analysis
process (Figure 1) using the constant comparative method. Results, therefore, are not
final or deterministic, but rather reflect the point at which no new interpretations
emerged from our personal engagement with these data.
Reliability and Validity
In qualitative research, the traditional forms of reliability and validity are not useful indices of the quality of the research. Qualitative research assumes that any description of reality is colored by one’s perspective—in our case, a long-term engagement with the data—and that observers cannot be independent because of the research
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
71
process that requires personal and long-term engagement with the data. From this perspective, the results are importantly and necessarily emergent from the observer-world
system. Training an independent observer is typically not desirable because of the
need for long-term engagement with the data, a research process that is similar to anthropological field work.
Qualitative research is assessed according to the credibility of the observers. Credibility has three criteria: prolonged engagement of the researcher with the data, the observers’ cumulative experience doing similar investigations, and making the data
available for readers to inspect (Denzin & Lincoln, 1994; Rogoff, Mistry, Gioncu, &
Mosier, 1993; Savage-Rumbaugh & Fields, 2000). Credibility is typically the way in
which people assess the trustworthiness of professionals such as physicians, attorneys,
or psychologists. It is also used, at least implicitly, in other case-based approaches,
such as in the evaluation of clinical case reports, anthropological field work, and judgments in legal cases. Our research methods, described above, testify to our prolonged
engagement with these data. The authors of this paper worked as a peer research team
with the second author as the primary observer. Between the two authors, we have
over 40 years of experience observing parent-infant communication and its development. Finally, although we cannot share the original videos because of confidentiality
concerns, we do share most of our narratives and many still photographs in the results.
Even researchers with fewer years of experience can be considered credible assuming
that they meet the other two criteria listed here.
Validity in population-based research depends in part on the sampling procedure,
the size of the sample, and the confidence with which a measure is thought to reflect
stable latent characteristics. Our final developmental movie, from a single dyad, is
only 15 minutes and 56 seconds in duration. While this may seem scant information
on which to base scientific conclusions, this needs to be placed in the context of the
qualitative and microgenetic methods used here. First of all, these data are developmental: not a single observation of 15 minutes but rather a string of observations that
capture a change process. Second, our lengthy sampling procedure testifies to that fact
that this movie was culled from an ongoing process of communication in this dyad.
The dyad, in other words, was not merely observed for a few minutes each week. The
larger communicative context is essential for the emergence of the events captured in
the movie and for their spontaneity.
In the end, however, the final developmental movie can be considered a type of
salient anecdote because of its brevity and because it is from a single dyad. How is
such data credible scientifically? One must consider the problem of how an investigator might go about capturing the key events in any developmental change process. Ideally, one would live in a household and become a participant observer. Even with this
amount of observation, it would still be up to the trained observer to identify key observations that illustrate most clearly a salient event or series of events. In our case,
we felt fortunate that we had indeed captured a sufficient number of key events to
piece together a reliable portrait of intersubjective change in at least this one dyad
from our larger sample.
Piaget is known to have said that “an acute observation is worth a thousand statistics” (quoted by Lock, 1992, p. 500). According to Lock (1992),
72
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
. . . what counts as an anecdote is not merely observed, nor stumbled upon, but
actively selected by an observer, filtered out of on-going reality as a result of
perception intuitively pre-informed, in a way we have yet to fully elucidate. . .”
(p. 501)
In this paper, we have endeavored to elucidate this process, and to provide an explicit account of its potential importance to scientific discovery in the context of doing
qualitative, microgenetic research. Like any research, it awaits replication and confirmation with larger samples.
Results
Based on the qualitative research process described above, we were able to distinguish four qualitatively different periods in the development of the infant’s attention
to her pounding actions. In the first period, from 26 to 32 weeks, the pounding game
was just emergent. Susan did not seem aware that she was the agent of the pounding
but she would look up immediately when the mother mirrored or attuned to her actions. In the second period, 33 and 34 weeks, Susan began to attend to her own pounding and her mother again attuned to the pounding movements. During the third period,
between 35 and 37 weeks, the dyad established a mutually understood game of pounding, a stable communicative frame, which Sheryl called “the slap game.” During the
final period, between 38 and 40 weeks, the infant appeared to become aware of her
agency in this game, and her abilities to initiate, accept, and refuse to play the game,
all signs of an emergent secondary intersubjectivity.
We observed action as it spontaneously unfolded over time. We rely on the support
of existing experimental studies to interpret particular forms of infant behavior as indicative of self- and intersubjective-awareness. In the following descriptions, we use boldface to indicate the times when Susan appears to show intersubjective self-awareness by
co-regulating her behavior with respect to her mother, as when Susan smiles and looks
at her mother following her own or her mother’s pounding. Italics are used to indicate
when Susan shows awareness of her own actions, typically by using some type of crossmodal self-reference, as when looking at her own hands while pounding.
The First Developmental Period,
The Emergence of the Pounding Behavior: 26 to 32 Weeks
When Susan was 26 weeks old, Sheryl wrote in her journal about Susan’s behavior at home, “As I was holding my hand out in front of Susan, palm up, she brought
her hand down on top of mine – she ‘slapped five.’ Actually, she is pretty ‘slappy’ –
any hard surface in front of her feels the wrath of her hands.” Several instances of such
pounding behaviors were observed in the first two videotaped high chair sessions. For
example, during the first minute of the first high chair observation at 27 weeks,
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
73
Susan starts to pound energetically with her left hand, gazing to her right. She
is not particularly attentive to the pounding until Sheryl starts to vocalize in the
same rhythm, “Oom! Boom, boom!” Immediately, Susan looks into Sheryl’s
eyes and pauses, after which Sheryl repeats “Oom!” Mother and infant
smile and Susan’s jaw slightly drops.
In this example, the first part of which is shown in Figure 2, Susan’s pattern of attention suggests that she does not seem aware of her abilities to intentionally initiate
or modify the pounding action, or of its effects on Sheryl. Based on Susan’s gazing at
her mother in response to Sheryl’s attunements (primary intersubjectivity), however,
it appears as though the mother’s cross-modal attunement (vocalizing in the same
rhythm and intensity as the infant’s pounding; cf. Stern, 1985) amplifies the infant’s
attention to the pounding actions in line with Stern’s ideas about the way in which infants’ sense of their own actions become regulated by their mothers.
FIGURE 2. The first developmental period: The emergence of pounding behavior (26
to 32 weeks). In this sequence taken from the videos, Susan looks away to her right
while pounding the table of the high chair. (Note: the on-screen timer was not working for this segment; view pictures in the sequence upper left, upper right, lower left,
and lower right).
74
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
When Sheryl vocalizes along with her own tapping motions, the infant shows a
different response. At 28 weeks, we observed a sequence in which both Susan’s and
Sheryl’s hands are involved in pounding or tapping, while mother vocalized in tune
with her own actions.
Susan pounds a few times, looking at her hand. She forcefully pounds once
while gazing away, then pounds again, gazing at her hand as it lands. Sheryl
exclaims, “Boom!” Susan looks at her mother, drops her jaw, and pounds
again. This time, as Susan’s hands hit the table tray from the top, Sheryl’s hit
it from the bottom! Sheryl repeats this tap from the bottom, accompanied by
“Boom!” Now, Susan’s hand wavers in the air for an instant and Sheryl repeats
again. For 2 seconds, Susan stares at her mother with an open mouth, and
then tries to look under the tray. Sheryl hits faster, saying “Boom! Boom!”
and Susan pounds twice, as if in response to her mother’s vocalizations. Sheryl
repeats Susan’s rhythm by hitting under the table twice. Susan looks up at her.
They smile. Susan pounds twice, gazing at her hand, while Sheryl taps
“Boom–Boom” under the table. The infant drops her jaw.
A possible interpretation, following Stern (1985), is that the infant notices the auditory similarity between her own and her mother’s pounding but only feels proprioceptive sensations with her own actions (and not with her mother’s). Sheryl’s tapping,
which has a similar rhythm and intensity as Susan’s pounding, is not authored by the
infant and Susan seems to be curious about the ownership of these actions (pausing,
experimenting with making sounds herself, and listening to her mother’s).
Between 29 and 32 weeks, Susan starts to shift her gaze between Sheryl’s face, her
own pounding hands, and back. At 32 weeks, for example, she watches her pounding
hands with great attention before she shifts her gaze between her own hands and her
mother’s face. Attention to her pounding seems related to Susan’s physiological state.
Throughout all observations, Susan rarely pounds during moments of fussiness. At 30
weeks, she is particularly fussy and she rarely pounds or looks at her hands, although
she is still attending to mother’s tapping hands.
Susan’s attention to her pounding movements also seems to be related to her
mother’s attention to the pounding. From 29 to 32 weeks, Sheryl is mainly a commentator, talking about other things and not commenting on the pounding. She does not explicitly focus on the pounding and rarely co-orients her movements or vocalizations
with the infant’s pounding movements. Without Sheryl’s amplifications, Susan seems
less likely to pay attention to her own pounding actions, again consistent with Stern’s
hypothesis. At the beginning of the session at 29 weeks, for example, Susan pounds for
a long time, occasionally glancing at her hands but mostly gazing away. When Sheryl
does participate, it is more likely that Susan pays attention to her pounding actions. At
29 weeks, Sheryl vocalizes in the rhythm of the infant’s pounding movement only once,
and Susan immediately looks up at her mother. At 32 weeks, Sheryl mimics the pounding movements with her voice and again Susan immediately looks up at her. When
Sheryl repeats this, they smile. Similar to the earlier observations, Sheryl appears to
provide a mirror for Susan’s actions, reflecting the contours of the pounding actions
(up–down, up–down). One day after this observation, Sheryl writes in her journal,
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
75
While on mommy’s bed, Susan recognized her shadow on the wall. She seemed
to catch something was “moving” on the wall and looked intently, probably
trying to figure out what (or who) it was. She appeared to realize that the object moved with her.
In summary, for this first developmental period, Susan’s ability to track her own actions is rudimentary. She seems to require her mother’s close attunements in order to
begin to sense her own activity and its effects, as if the mother’s attunements amplify
the feedback into her own sense of self. The example of the shadow suggests that
Susan’s awareness of her own agency is becoming more stable by the end of this period.
The Second Developmental Period, Attentive Pounding: 33 to 34 Weeks
At 33 and 34 weeks, the various components of Susan’s pounding—mainly her
hand and eye movements—appear to become more strongly coordinated as a stable
system of cross-modal self-awareness. She shows more visual fascination for her
pounding hands than during any of the earlier sessions. She clearly stretches out her
arms and uses flat hands as she pounds, and she looks at her own hands at length and
intently (see Figure 3). She also vocalizes during pounding, a possible self-attunement
that may amplify her attention to the action. Finally, she varies the force and height of
the movements while looking at her hands and she alternates pounding with her left and
right hand. The following observation is from the outset of the session at 33 weeks:
Susan starts to pound with her right hand, gazing at it attentively. Sheryl
watches her, smiling and talking about other things. Susan rests her right hand
upon her left and continues to look at her hands. She looks up at Sheryl and vocalizes, “Hm.” With a smile, Sheryl says, “Hi,” warmly acknowledging her
daughter. They share a back-and-forth of smiles and vocalizations, while Susan
continues to rest her hands on the table with her fingers touching and exploring each other (for a total of 21 seconds). Then, she watches them intently for
5 seconds, as if entranced with them.
Another observation during this session was the alternation of markedly different
feeling contours (Stern, 1985, 1993). Quiet moments suddenly transition into episodes
of fast and intense pounding, rising with force, then suddenly dropping, and rising
again. These different emotional moments are connected by a quality of visual concentration. It is likely that there is a growing awareness of self-affectivity, a possible
linking for Susan of herself as the agent of these different feeling states.
Although she appears to be self-focused during her moments of stillness (gazing
at her own hands), Susan gazes intently at Sheryl during pounding moments. From her
side of the dyad, Sheryl attunes to her daughter’s pounding as soon as she becomes
aware of the intensity of the infant’s self-focus. For example, after the quiet period described above,
Susan started to pound so energetically that Sheryl immediately interrupted
her own talking and said, “Oooo, what a lot of noise!” Again, Susan pounded
76
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
FIGURE 3. The second developmental period: Attentive pounding (33 to 34 weeks).
In this sequence, Susan is pounding her own stationary hand while watching the stationary hand. This the first evidence of coordination between gazing, pounding, and
other parts of her own body.
vigorously, looking Sheryl straight into the eyes, her whole body engaged
in the action. As Sheryl said, “Oooo. Bang! Bang! Bang!” moving her head
up and down in the same rhythm, Susan’s jaw dropped and she faintly smiled.
After this segment, the feeling contour again changed dramatically when Susan
stopped pounding and watched her hands for 12 seconds with a quality of intense concentration while Sheryl was quietly observing. After this serene break, the feeling contour changed yet again when Susan suddenly started to pound vigorously, alternating
left and right arms, watching her hands. Next, she started to gaze at Sheryl, who then
began to vocalize, matching the rhythm and intensity of the infant’s pounding. Following this, Susan forcefully threw her body back in the chair, smiling. Seeing this, Sheryl
laughed and said, in a similar outburst of energy, “Wow! You make a lot of noise.”
In summary, for the second developmental period, our impression is that Susan
seems to be beginning to sense her own agency, suggested by her increased attention
to her own actions and her noticing (via gaze and smiling) the attunement between her
pounding actions and her mother’s pounding and vocalizing. One can see the outlines
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
77
of an emerging dyadic pattern in which the possibility exists for Susan to eventually
link an awareness of her own intentions with an awareness of her mother’s intentions.
This is reminiscent of Bruner’s (1983) idea of communication formats as “place holders” for future developmental additions and also to the dynamic systems ideas of multiple components developing at different rates as they self-organize into an attractor.
Susan’s attention to both herself and to her mother is clearly a change from the first
developmental period in which she primarily attuned with her mother and seemed relatively unaware of her own pounding. The mother’s attunement to Susan, however,
continues to scaffold Susan’s attention. In the second developmental period, we can
see how Susan’s own contrasting emotions seem to create pathways in her awareness
that link different activities together.
The Third Developmental Period,
Emergence of a Stable Pounding Frame: 35 to 37 Weeks
At 35 weeks, Sheryl indicates in her journal that the pounding (or “slapping”) has
now become a game between them. She writes, “Mommy stumbled onto the ‘slap
game’ with Susan and she plays with such fervor. I’ll slap the table, or my knee, with
my hand. And she’ll slap the same with her hands. This will go on for 5–7 rounds,
where Susan will put her whole body into the slap, staring directly at mommy the
whole time.” Thus, the pounding actions seem to have evolved into a dyadically
shared game or “frame” (Fogel, 1993), with each partner expecting certain reactions
from the other partner and each partner implicitly aware of the other’s intentions.
From our videos at 35 weeks, we can see the outlines of this frame beginning to form.
Susan pounds twice, looking at her hand. Sheryl says, “Boo Boo” in the same
rhythm. Susan looks at her mother immediately, with her jaw dropped, and
pounds once. Sheryl taps twice, as Susan did before. Glancing at her own
hand, Susan lightly pounds once, and immediately shifts her gaze to Sheryl’s
hand. Sheryl taps once (as if in response), slides her hand to Susan’s hand and
taps it. Susan’s gaze follows Sheryl’s hand as it covers Susan’s hand. Susan
stares at the hands for 3 seconds until the toy moves and attracts her attention.
The immediacy of the infant’s gaze shift and the duration of her gaze at mother’s
hand after mother responded by tapping suggested to us that Susan implicitly expected
an answer from her mother to her pounding movement. This expectation seems to reflect a growing awareness of her mother’s intentionality. Her pounding and Sheryl’s
tapping actions follow each other sequentially in time, linked by their rhythm (note the
contiguity of bold and italic fonts). It appears that this is the beginning of a sense of
secondary intersubjectivity via coordinated joint attention, a shared sense of coagency, and the infant’s awareness of the link between her own and her mother’s actions (see Figure 4).
At 36 weeks, it is clear that the game does not always unfold as expected. This observation not only suggests the emergence of a shared frame, but also that the infant
begins to experiment with being an agent, able to refuse or to accept participation.
78
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
FIGURE 4. Third developmental period: The emergence of a stable pounding frame
(35–37 weeks). There is more coordination between self and other, as Susan watches
her own pounding hand, then looks at her mother’s tapping while she continues to
pound, and then looks back at her own pounding hand.
Sheryl asks, “Wanna play a little slap game?” Susan looks at Sheryl’s tapping
hand and then at her face. Sheryl repeats her request in various, creative ways.
However, no matter how playful Sheryl is in her attempts to engage the infant
in play, Susan just watches. Eventually, she pounds a few times and then smiles
at her smiling mother. Shortly after, she stops and turns away.
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
79
But at 37 weeks, Susan does participate.
Sheryl asks, “Show me how you pound,” tapping in a fast, staccato rhythm and
gazing at her daughter. Susan looks into Sheryl’s eyes, while Sheryl repeats the
request—moving her head and hand up and down in the same rhythm as her
vocalization of the spoken sentence—and starts to smile. It is as if Sheryl is
pounding with her head, hand, and speech. Susan now starts to pound with
her right hand. She smiles—tilting her head and raising her eyebrows—
and gazes intently into mother’s eyes. Sheryl exclaims, “Yeah! That’s a good
girl!” Susan turns to look at her own hand as Sheryl taps twice. She alternates
her gaze between Sheryl’s tapping hand and her own pounding hand, until
she turns to the side, staring in the distance, forcefully pounds with both hands,
looks at them, and shouts “Ah!” After this, she rests her head on her arm.
For the first time, Susan alternates her gaze between her own and her mother’s hands,
while continuing to pound, which we indicate with bold and italic together. This
may enable Susan to notice the difference between her own and Sheryl’s intentional
movements.
Between 35 and 37 weeks, the mother’s behaviors also changed. First, her attunements to Susan’s actions became more accentuated. One example of this can be found
in the description above. Another one was also observed at 37 weeks.
Susan is pounding, while Sheryl vocalizes, “Bang! Bang!” moving her head,
hand, and intonation in the same up-and-down contour. They smile at each
other. Then, Susan pounds more forcefully, alternating her left and right hand.
Sheryl now moves her head from left to right, continuing to vocalize rhythmically. As soon as she notices Sheryl’s left-to-right movements, Susan stops
pounding and gazes into Sheryl’s eyes. They look at each other and smile,
and Sheryl wrinkles her nose.
Second, Sheryl also seems to accentuate the use of the word “you” more when addressing her infant. From the beginning of our observations, at 27 weeks, she often
commented on Susan’s gaze direction, actions, likes and dislikes, and past and future
events involving Susan, placing no special emphasis on the word “you.” For example,
at 33 weeks, she says “You like to do that in the bathtub, don’t you? You like to really
splash.” At 35 weeks, however, Sheryl begins to emphasize “you” to amplify the infant’s current actions. For instance, the infant makes a toy beep and mother comments
enthusiastically, “Yeah! You did it! You’re making that noise now.” At 36 weeks, the
same happens and mother says “You did that” and “You made it go beep.” At 37 weeks,
when the infant makes the toy beep, mother repeats several times, “You made that
noise!” emphasizing “you” each time. One interpretation of these changes in the
mother’s behaviors toward Susan is that Sheryl is noticing Susan’s emerging awareness of being an active agent of her own pounding actions, and amplifying it with her
actions and language. Another interpretation is that mother expects more awareness of
self-agency to emerge at this age, and that she is providing a scaffold within which the
infant can develop this awareness.
80
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
In summary, for the third developmental period in comparison with the second, it
is clear that both mother and infant change in their behavior in relation to each other
and that they achieve novel types of coordinated co-actions. Of particular interest with
respect to our focus on the development of intersubjectivity is the infant’s growing
ability to alternate and coordinate self and other-awareness, along with the mother’s
novel use of the word “you.” There is no way to tell for certain from our data whether
the mother intends to highlight the infant’s growing sense of agency and intersubjectivity or whether it is an implicit action that in some way reflects the mother’s sense
of her infant’s developmental changes. Nevertheless, the infant seems to have a few
glimpses into a sense of secondary intersubjectivity, scaffolded by the now stable
pounding frame.
The Fourth Developmental Period,
Emergence of Secondary Intersubjectivity: 38 to 40 Weeks
Unfortunately, observations at 38 and 39 weeks are missing, because mother and
infant were visiting Susan’s grandmother. Sheryl notes in her journal, “This has been
a good visit for Grandma, she got a lot of firsts.” Susan gives her first kiss, claps her
hands, and learns how to shake her head “no.” These are all signs of a growing referential communication, that is, communication about some shared focus of attention
(Camaioni et al., 2004). At 39 weeks, Sheryl writes, “Grandma marveled at how much
[Susan] had grown, changed (physically and emotionally) since we had been home [at
Grandma’s] the two weeks.” At 40 weeks, mother describes a new game, “Susan likes
to play a game shaking her head ‘no’ when Mommy says ‘yes.’ She responds with a
big smile and starts the game all over again. The beginnings of defiance? It’s cute anyway.” This description indicates that Susan’s sense of secondary intersubjectivity has
spread from a few instances within the pounding game to become a stable and more
pervasive feature of different types of frames with mother and with other people.
Susan seems to understand that her mother has a different intention (yes) from her own
(no) and creates a teasing game around that (cf. Reddy, 2001). We observed an instance of this new game at 40 weeks, following an interaction involving pounding.
Susan glances at the table and then looks at Sheryl’s face. Raising her eyebrows, she starts to pound. Pounding the table top with flat hands, she follows Sheryl’s movements with her gaze. It appears as though she wants to
initiate the pounding game. But Sheryl says, “I wanna show off and show how
you can do pat-a-cake.” Susan stops and reaches for Sheryl’s face. Sheryl starts
to sing and clap “pat-a-cake.” Continuing to stare intently at her singing and
clapping mother, Susan starts to pound the table again. Continuing to pound,
she shifts her gaze to her hands and then back to Sheryl, who says, “No,
show them how you clap, okay?” She takes hold of Susan’s hands and claps
them until they finish the song, both smiling. Sheryl starts again, clapping her
hands demonstratively in front of Susan. This time, Susan kicks the rhythm
against the bottom of the high chair. Sheryl laughs, “Are you doing it with
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
81
your feet?” Susan looks at Sheryl with her jaw dropped and continues to kick.
As they end the song, Susan softly laughs. Sheryl mimics this and asks,
“You’re not going to do it, are ya?” while slightly moving her head side to side
and clapping her hands. Susan looks at Sheryl, tilts her head, and raises her
eyebrows. She smiles and then shakes her head “no.” Sheryl smiles broadly
and they start a game in which Sheryl playfully repeats the words “yes” and
“no,” nodding and shaking her head. Susan shakes her head a few more
times. They smile and laugh together.
In this observation, the latter part of which is shown in Figure 5, the dyad reveals
a different pattern within the pounding frame, with new roles for mother and infant
and an apparently new awareness of the infant’s unique role and ability to choose, initiate, and refuse. Consistent with experimental studies on the development of infants’
awareness of others’ intentions at this age (e.g., Behne et al., 2005), Susan seems to
guess Sheryl’s intention, but chooses to act differently. Susan now initiates her own
unique expression, maintaining connection to Sheryl by pounding in the same rhythm.
Sheryl appears to notice the cross-modal similarity between her daughter’s pounding
and kicking movements and her own clapping, suggested by her laughter and her asking, “Are you doing it with your feet?”
In summary, at 40 weeks, mother and infant have a different “feel” to us as observers (cf. Stern, 1985), indicative of a developmental transition to secondary intersubjectivity. This developmental change is also marked by observable shifts in the entire system of communication and self-awareness, both for mother and infant. The
infant now seems to realize that she can intentionally act differently than Sheryl wants
her to: she shakes her head “no,” laughs, and her vocalizations sound more word-like
(e.g., she now clearly articulates “dadada”). She is able to smoothly coordinate her
gaze between Sheryl and her pounding hands. Sheryl acts differently, too. She uses her
voice more matter-of-factly, speaks faster and moves more with her entire body, in
comparison to all previous sessions. Such changes in maternal communication patterns have been related to infants’ ability to engage in gestural and verbal “acts of
meaning” (Bruner, 1983; Stern, 1985; Trevarthen, 1998). Sheryl writes, “She likes to
get my attention by wrinkling her nose into a funny face. It’s her personality starting
to blossom. She really is a happy baby.”
Discussion
Summary of Findings
The story of Susan and Sheryl provides one possible developmental pathway of
the process of change in secondary intersubjectivity within the mother-infant relationship around the 9-month developmental transition. Our goal, taking a dynamic systems perspective, was to examine the historical unfolding of the change process within
the dyad in order to better understand how a re-organization of relationship components may come about to create an emergent psychological milestone.
82
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
FIGURE 5. Fourth developmental period: The emergence of secondary intersubjectivity (38–40 weeks). Susan looks at Sheryl (who wants Susan to play pat-a-cake), tilts
her head, and raises her eyebrows. She smiles and then shakes her head “no.” They
smile and laugh together.
During the first developmental period, 26–32 weeks, the mother’s co-orientations
with Susan’s pounding established an innovation of helping to focus the infant’s
attention to her own movements, at least occasionally (Stern, 1985, 1993). Sheryl’s
spontaneous attunements—mainly mimicking the infant’s feeling contours with her
voice—served to amplify the infant’s movements and to temporarily bring them into
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
83
the infant’s awareness. They seemed to enhance the infant’s incipient awareness of
herself as the agent of the pounding actions, suggested by her gazing at mother with
her jaw dropped after Sheryl vocalized in tune with her pounding (see Figure 6 [a]).
Stern (1985, 1993) has suggested that cross-modal attunements are particularly
important to the infant’s developing sense of intersubjectivity. Attunement behaviors
“. . . recast the event and shift the focus of attention to [. . .] the quality of feeling that
is being shared” (Stern, 1985, p. 142). According to the intersensory redundancy
hypothesis (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000), cross-modal sensory coordination enhances
attention to those sensations in both pre- and post-natal development. In our observations, cross-modal co-orientations were scaffolded by the mother and served to stabilize the infant’s hand and eye movements into a pattern of focused pounding. It is this
growing stability that is the signature of an attractor or frame beginning to form in the
communication system.
This coordinative system was not completely stable in the first developmental period. When Sheryl co-oriented less with the pounding, between 29 and 32 weeks,
Susan focused less on her pounding movements, which strengthens our impression
that the mother’s actions serve to stabilize and regulate the infant’s innovative selfawareness of her own actions. Nevertheless, the infant increasingly glanced at her
pounding hands and at Sheryl’s face before or after pounding, suggesting an emerging
attractor for more self-sustained attention to her pounding actions and of the infant’s
emerging initiative in engaging the mother in her pounding activities.
A second developmental period, at 33 and 34 weeks, was marked by Susan’s increased stability of attention to her pounding and by more variations in emotional intensity than before. Susan alternated strikingly focused and deliberate pounding with
prolonged pauses during which she was not pounding but quietly focused on her hands.
During vigorous pounding, Susan intently gazed at Sheryl, who again attuned to the
pounding vocally and through body movements. It is likely that these dyadic emotional
shifts played a role in the felt experience of being with the self-regulating other, over
and above the sense of motor and perceptual coordination between attention to self and
to mother (see Figure 6 [b]). These types of shared dyadic emotional periods have been
described as “moments of meeting” (Stern, 1998) or dyadic states of consciousness
(Tronick, 1998) and are hypothesized to serve the developmental function of creating
shared psychological states upon which future forms of coordination depend. These
moments of meeting were the innovations that emerged in the second period.
This hypothesis makes sense because what follows, between 35 and 37 weeks during the third developmental period, is the emergence of a stable pounding frame involving mutual expectations and coordinated co-actions. Susan seems to realize,
“Whenever I pound, Mother will tap.” Her gaze at mother’s hands after she herself
starts to pound implies an expectation of mother replying to her movements in the
same rhythm. The rhythm of the pounding hands may have helped this infant and
mother to further develop and stabilize shared meanings of how their mutual pounding frame unfolds, affording the infant with a sense of agency to alter the flow of
events in the pounding frame. From the other side of the dyad, the mother began
to call the pounding a “slap game,” and emphasized the word “you,” suggesting an
innovative mutual expectation of their response to each other (see Figure 6 [c]). This
84
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
Mother attunes to infant
Gaze
Mother attunes to pounding
Gaze
Gaze
Own hand Mother
(a)
Co-attunement, shared emotions
Own hand Mother
Infant attunes
Infant attunes
vigorously
quietly
(b)
Developmental Change to
Emergent Secondary Intersubjectivity
Gaze
Gaze
Temporal contiguity
between self and
mother reference
Own hand
Mother
Infant attunes
quietly
Gaze
Teasing
Refusing
Own hand
Mother
New postures, tones of voice
Infant attunes
vigorously
(c)
(d)
FIGURE 6. (a) The first developmental phase (26–32 weeks). The mother’s attunement
creates an emotional “container” that helps the infant self-regulate by attending to her
own pounding actions and also to her mother’s face and pounding actions. When there is
no maternal attunement, the infants does not attend to mother, nor to her own pounding
hands. (b) The second developmental phase (33–34 weeks). Infant begins to co-attune
with mother, creating two different types of contours: one more quiet and the other more
vigorous and more likely to be shared with mother. (c) Third developmental phase (35–37
weeks). There is a closer contiguity between infant gazing at self and at mother, the emergence of a stable pounding frame that contains the actions and emotions, and increasingly
shared intentions. The first instance of secondary intersubjectivity occurs at the end of this
period. (d) Fourth developmental period (38–40 weeks). This marks the emergence of a
new frame for secondary intersubjectivity in which both infant’s and mother’s behaviors
have changed. New patterns emerge such as teasing, refusing, changed use of the pronoun
“you” by the mother, and shifts of expression and tone of voice.
illustrates a novel reorganization of many components of the pounding and the dyadic
nature of the infant’s growing self-awareness. Research in music perception and
mother-infant interaction have shown exquisite sensitivity to rhythm patterns as a way
to establish and maintain coordinated co-action and shared emotion (Jaffe, Beebe,
Feldstein, Crown, & Jasnow, 2001; Stern, 2001; Trevarthen, 1998).
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
85
The developmental transition to widespread and stable sense of secondary intersubjectivity appears to occur in the fourth developmental period, between 38 and
40 weeks. During this period, a qualitatively different communication pattern emerged
and stabilized in the dyad: both mother and infant began to behave differently than
before—in posture, tone of voice, and communicative expressions—suggesting true
developmental change. By participating in pat-a-cake by pounding or kicking while
refusing to clap, the infant shows that she understands the mother’s intention but
chooses to engage in the game on her own terms. The rhythm of the game is similar
between the members of the dyad but the exact form of its enactment is different, to
mother’s and infant’s great enjoyment. Both appear to have developed a new understanding of themselves and how they relate to each other (see Figure 6 [d]).
These observations concur with those of Trevarthen and Hubley (1978) who observed a similar novel sense of confidence and understanding of intention in the infant
Tracey around 9 months of age. They are also reminiscent of Reddy’s (2001; Reddy,
Hay, Murray, & Trevarthen, 1993) observations of teasing games, during which infants deliberately provoke their partners to mischievously disrupt the others’ ongoing
actions, not comply with a previously accepted prohibition, or disrupt normally accepted gestures and routines. Teasing presumes secondary intersubjectivity. During
such provocations, infants have also been observed to immensely enjoy their partners’
exaggerated protests, as during Susan’s “shaking the head ‘no’” game.
Inferences about the Process of Developmental Change
From these observations, we can infer systematic regularities in the process of
change. In each developmental period, there appears to have been some kind of dyadic
innovation that emerged via the coordinated and mutually amplifying action of the
mother and infant. In the first developmental period, for example, Sheryl’s attunements seem to temporarily stabilize Susan’s attention to her own action. The ability to
self-attune, crucial to emerging self and other-awareness, becomes a dynamically stable attractor by the second period, in which Susan attends consistently to her own and
her mother’s behavior. This leads, in the second developmental period, to an emerging attractor of coordinated action and attention across multiple modalities of emotion,
action, and gazing, an innovation that becomes the stable pounding frame in the third
period. Innovation in the third period involved the increasing contiguity of self and
other actions, brief glimpses of secondary intersubjectivity during the pounding frame,
and changes in the mother’s behavior (such as emphasizing the word “you”). These
innovations seem to lead to the consolidation and spread of secondary intersubjectivity to other frames in the mother-infant relationships (and in other family relationships) in the fourth period.
These observations fit well with our previous research on the developmental
change process. In a larger sample (n = 13) of change in infants between 2 and 6
months (Fogel et al., 2006), we found that innovations in one developmental period
always preceded and foreshadowed the emergence of new frames in the next period.
This seems to be the chief mechanism by which the dyad conserves its own history
86
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
while at the same time gradually transforming its relationship. Second, we found that
major developmental change, the fourth developmental period in this study, always
had the characteristic of “permeability.” This means, as we observed with the infant
Susan as well, that multiple frames in the relationship system began to blend as the
history of innovations spreads across the entire system. We found, for all of our 13
dyads, that permeability, and hence developmental change and the emergence of new
frames, occurred relatively rapidly, within one or two sessions.
This rapid reorganization of the entire system of frames, the hallmark of a developmental change, is not, however, discontinuous or non-linear. One can observe the
threads of change occurring systematically during each developmental period, with
new attractors that are built from historically familiar and readily accessible components. It is as if the dyad can change quickly when it is ready, but readiness implies
carrying the familiar existing patterns of co-action with them (in such a way as to
avoid catastrophic non-linear, unprecedented) transition. Research on clinical cases
and on developmental psychopathology suggests that non-linear change in human development is most often experienced by participants as traumatic with deleterious psychological consequences. Put another way, normally developing social relationships
create bridges between the past and future that buffer participants from precipitous
changes (cf. Fogel et al., 2006).
Conclusions
One advantage of our qualitative microgenetic research approach is the opportunity to trace developmental change. Within a single dyad, we can see how new forms
of dyadic communication emerge in each different developmental period. More crucially, however, we can see how each new developmental period depends upon the
specifics of what occurred earlier. This relational-historical perspective is missing in
much of developmental science but it is central to understanding the process of how
development unfolds over time. Siegler’s microgenetic studies (cited earlier), to take
one example, have contributed much to the understanding of how children learn novel
cognitive strategies (e.g., math).
We cannot generalize these findings to larger populations. Focusing on a small
number of observations from a single dyad from a dynamic systems perspective, however, is not a limitation but rather a strength because it allows for an in-depth examination of the process of change in the whole system of communication. The ability to
observe a system before, during, and after a change using a microgenetic design provides insights about change that are generative if not generalizable. They are generative in the sense that our findings of particular processes of mutual attunement—
rhythm and affect matching—as amplifiers for infant attention, the emergence of
stable attractors for shared awareness as a foundation for later forms of coordination,
and our particular description of the four developmental periods could be applied to
study other dyads across the same transition. According to dynamic systems principles, generalities about the population must emerge by finding out what is common
across cases, and not by statistical averaging and group-based correlational models
(Bergman, Cairns, & Nilsson, 2000; Fogel, 1990; Thelen, 1990). Future work using
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
87
this approach can also facilitate understanding of the specific historical processes in
cases where development is derailed or delayed.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by grants to Alan Fogel by the National Institutes of
Health (R01 HD21036) and the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH48680 and
MH 57669). We thank Susan and her mother for their participation in this study. The authors thank Manuela Lavelli for her helpful comments on this paper. Correspondence
concerning this article should be addressed to Alan Fogel, Department of Psychology,
University of Utah, 380 South 1530 East, Salt Lake City, UT, 84112-0251, USA.
References
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (2000). Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and
perceptual learning in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 36, 190–201.
Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and
tomorrow (pp. 349–378). New York: Jossey Bass.
Beebe, B. (1998). A procedural theory of therapeutic action: Commentary of the symposium, “Interventions that effect change in psychotherapy.” Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, (3),
333–340.
Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F. M. (2002). Infant research and adult treatment: Co-constructing interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative
intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8(6), 492–499.
Bergman, L. R., Cairns, R. B., & Nilsson, L. (2000). Developmental science and the holistic approach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Black, B., & Logan, A. (1995). Links between communication patterns in mother-child, father-child,
and child-peer interactions and children’s social status. Child Development, 66, 255–271.
Bowlby, J. (1973/1991). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation, anxiety and anger. London: Penguin Books.
Bretherton, I. (1990). Open communication and internal working models: Their role in the development of attachment relationships. In R. Dienstbier & R. A. Thompson (Eds.), Nebraska
symposium on motivation 1988: Socioemotional development (pp. 57–113). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Bretherton, I. (1991). Pouring new wine into old bottles: The social self as internal working model.
In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Self processes and development (pp. 1–41), Vol. 23.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Bretherton, I. (1993). From dialogue to internal working models: The co-construction of self in
relationships. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Memory and affect in development: The Minnesota
symposia on child psychology, 26, 237–263. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Publishers.
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., & Colonnesi, C. (2004). The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy, 5(3), 291–308.
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998), Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 63, 4, no. 176.
88
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Friedman and
Company.
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness.
San Diego, CA: Harcourt, Inc.
De Wolff, M. S., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on
parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.
Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Handbook of qualitative research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications, Inc.
Feshbach, N. D. (1987). Parental empathy and child adjustment/maladjustment. In N. Eisenberg &
J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 271–291). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Fifer, W., Monk, C., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2001). Prenatal development and risk. In G. Bremmer and A.
Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 505–542). United Kingdom:
Blackwell Publishers.
Flynn, E. & Siegler, R. (2007). Measuring change: Current trends and future directions in microgenetic research. Infant and Child Development, 16, 135–149.
Fogel, A. (1990). The process of developmental change in infant communicative action: Using dynamic systems theory to study individual ontogenies. In J. Colombo & J. Fagen (Eds.), Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability and prediction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Fogel, A. (1995). Relational narratives of the prelinguistic self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Fogel, A. (2001). A relational perspective on the self and emotions. In H. A. Bosma & E. S. Kunnen
(Eds.), Identity and emotion: Development through self-organization (pp. 93–114). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fogel, A. (2006). Dynamic systems research on interindividual communication: The transformation
of meaning making. Journal of Developmental Processes, 1, 7–30.
Fogel, A., Garvey, A., Hsu, H., & West-Stroming, D. (2006). Change processes in relationships:
Relational–historical research on a dynamic system of communication. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Forrester, M. A. (2001). The embedding of the self in early interaction. Infant and Child Development, 10, 189–202.
Garvey, A. & Fogel, A. (in press). Dialogical change processes, emotions, and the early emergence
of self, International Journal of Dialogical Sciences.
Gibson, E. J. (1993). Ontogenesis of the perceived self. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self
(pp. 25–42). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Granic, I. (2000). The self-organization of parent-child relations: Beyond bidirectional models. In
M. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, Development and Self-Organization: Dynamic Systems
Approaches to Emotional Development (pp. 267–348). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Gustafson, G. E., Green, J. A., West, M. J. (1979). The infant’s changing role in mother-infant
games: The growth of social skills. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 301–308.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford
Press.
Hsu, H. C., & Fogel, A. (2001). Infant vocal development in a dynamic mother-infant communication system. Infancy, 2, 87–109.
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 66.
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., Dolev, S., Sher, E., & Etzion-Carasso, A. (2002). Mothers’ insightfulness regarding their infants’ internal experience: Relations with maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. Developmental Psychology, 38, 534–542.
The Developmental Transition to Secondary Intersubjectivity in the Second Half Year
89
Lavelli, M. & Fogel, A. (2005). Developmental changes in the relationship between infant’s attention and emotion during early face-to-face communication: The 2-month transition. Developmental Psychology, 41, 265–280.
Lavelli, M., Pantoja, A. P. F., Hsu, H., Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2005) Using microgenetic designs to study change processes. In D. G. Teti (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in developmental psychology. (pp. 1–50) Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers.
Legerstee, M. (2005). Infants’ sense of people: Precursors to a theory of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lewis, M. (1995). Embarrassment: The emotion of exposure and evaluation. In J. P. Tangney &
K. W. Fisher (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 198–218). New York: The Guildford Press.
Lock, A. (1992). Anecdotal events may be really important events. Human Movement Science, 11,
497–504.
Lyons-Ruth, K. (1998). Implicit relational knowing: Its role in development and psychoanalytic
treatment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 282–289.
Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview.
Developmental Psychology, 28, 1006–1017.
Neisser, U. (1993). The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge.
Emory symposia in cognition, 5. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2002). Mothers’ insightfulness regarding their children’s internal worlds: The capacity underlying secure child-mother relationships. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 23, 593–605.
Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N, & Sagi, A. (2001). Mothers’ empathic understanding of their
preschoolers’ internal experience: Relations with early attachment. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 25, 16–26.
Panksepp, J. (2001). The long-term psychobiological consequences of infant emotions: Prescriptions
for the twenty-first century. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, (1–2), 132–173.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.
Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
Phillips, A., Wellman, H., & Spelke, E. (2002). Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85(1), 53–78.
Pipp, S. (1993). Infants’ knowledge of self, other, and relationship. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 185–204). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Reddy, V. (2001). Mind knowledge in the first year: Understanding attention and intention. In G.
Bremner & A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 241–264). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
Reddy, V., Hay, D., Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1993). Communication in infancy: Mutual regulation of affect and attention. In G. Bremner, A. Slater, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Infant development: Recent Advances, pp. 247 – 274. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Rochat, P. (1995). Early objectification of the self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory
and research (pp. 53–71). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by
toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 236,
58(8).
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., & Fields, W. M. (2000). Linguistic, cultural and cognitive capacities of
bonobos. Culture & Psychology, 6, 131–153.
Schore, A. (2001). Effects of a secure attachment on right brain development, affect regulation, and
infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, (1–2), 7–66.
Secrist, C., de Koeyer, I., Bell, H., & Fogel, A. (2002). Combining digital video technology and
narrative methods for understanding infant development. Forum for Qualitative Sociology Research, 3, (2). Retrieved from http://www.qualitative-research.net/fqs-texte/2-02/2-02
secristetal-e.htm.
90
ALAN FOGEL AND ILSE DEKOEYER-LAROS
Shanker, S. & King, B. (2002). The emergence of a new paradigm in ape language research, Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 25, 605–626.
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC, US: National Academy Press.
Siegel, D. (2001). Toward an interpersonal neurobiology of the developing mind: Attachment relationship, “mindsight,” and neural integration. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, (1–2), 67–94.
Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46(6), 606–620.
Sommerville, J., & Woodward, A. (2005). Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation
between action processing and action production in infancy. Cognition, 95(1), 1–30.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Stern, D. N. (1993). The role of feelings for an interpersonal self. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived
self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 205–215). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Stern, D. N. (1998). The process of therapeutic change involving implicit knowledge: Some implications of developmental observations for adult psychotherapy. Infant Mental Health Journal,
19, 300–309.
Stern, D. N. (2001). Putting time back into our considerations of infant experience: A michrodiachronic view. Infant Mental Health Journal, 21, 21–28.
Strauss, A. & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and
techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Striano, T. & Rochat, P. (1999). Developmental links between dyadic and triadic social competence
in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 551–562.
Thelen, E. (1990). Dynamical systems and the generation of individual differences. In J. Colombo
& J. Fagen (Eds.), Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability, prediction. (pp. 19–
43). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Thelen, E., Fisher, D. M., & Ridley-Johnson, R. (1984). The relationship between physical growth
and a newborn reflex. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 479–493.
Thelen, E., & Fogel, A. (1989). Toward an action-based theory of infant development. In J. Lockman & N. Hazen (Eds.), Action in social context: Perspectives on early development (pp. 23–
64). New York: Plenum Press.
Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills. Monographs of the Society for Research, Child
Development, 56, 6–97.
Tomasello, M. (1993). On the interpersonal origins of self-concept. In U. Neisser (Ed), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Emory symposia in cognition, 5 (pp. 174–184). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1993). The self born in intersubjectivity: An infant communicating. In
U. Neisser (Ed.), The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge,
(pp. 121–173). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In S. Bråten (Ed.),
Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, (pp. 15–46). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of
meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of
language (pp. 183–227). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tronick, E. Z. (1998). Dyadically expanded states of consciousness and the process of therapeutic
change. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 290–299.
van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity and chaos.
Hertfordshire, UK: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
van Geert, P. (1998). We almost had a great future behind us: The contribution of non-linear dynamics to developmental-science-in-the-making. Developmental Science, 1(1), 143–159.
Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.
Winnicott, D. W. (1990). Playing and reality. London/New York: Routledge. (Originally published
in 1971).