Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
…
2 pages
1 file
This paper reviews the work of Sneve et al. (2015) on the neural processes involved in creating lasting memories, particularly examining the brain activity patterns that predict memory retention over short (1.5 hours) and long (6 weeks) delays. It highlights the role of hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity in encoding long-lasting memories and discusses the interplay between encoding and post-encoding brain activity. The review suggests further examination of memory processes across different stages to build a comprehensive understanding of how memories are formed and maintained over time.
NeuroImage, 2014
The ability to form durable memory is critical for human survival and development, but its underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms have not been well understood. In particular, existing studies have not clearly dissociated the neural processes supporting short-and long-duration memories. The present study addressed this issue with functional MRI and a modified subsequent memory paradigm. Participants were asked to make semantic judgment on a list of 320 words in the scanner. Half of the words were tested after a short delay (i.e., 1 day, T1) and again after a long delay (i.e., 1 week, T12), whereas the other half were tested only once after the long delay (T2). Materials forgotten during T1 were categorized as forgotten trials, and those remembered during T2 were categorized as long-duration trials. In contrast, trials remembered during T1 but not during T12 were categorized as short-duration trials. We found that compared to forgotten trials, short-duration trials showed decreased activation in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus, which is consistent with many previous observations. Importantly, long-duration trials showed stronger activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) but less deactivation in the PCC relative to short-duration trials. Psychophysiological interactions (PPI) analysis revealed stronger functional connectivity between LIFG and PCC for long-duration trials than for forgotten trials. Our results suggest that strong PCC activity, in combination with strong LIFG activity, supports long-lasting memory.
Science, 1998
A fundamental question about human memory is why some experiences are remembered whereas others are forgotten. Brain activation during word encoding was measured using blocked and event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine how neural activation ...
Neuropsychologia, 2017
Several prominent theories posit that information about recent experiences lingers in the brain and organizes memories for current experiences, by forming a temporal context that is linked to those memories at encoding. According to these theories, if the thoughts preceding an experience X resemble the thoughts preceding an experience Y, then X and Y should show an elevated probability of being recalled together. We tested this prediction by using multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) of fMRI data to measure neural evidence for lingering processing of preceding stimuli. As predicted, memories encoded with similar lingering thoughts about the category of preceding stimuli were more likely to be recalled together. Our results demonstrate that the "fading embers" of previous stimuli help to organize recall, confirming a key prediction of computational models of episodic memory.
Journal of Neuroscience, 2005
Memories vary in their durability even when encoding conditions apparently remain constant. We investigated whether, under these circumstances, memory durability is nonetheless associated with variation in the neural activity elicited during encoding. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired while volunteers semantically classified visually presented words. Using the "remember/know" procedure, memory for one-half of the words was tested after 30 min and for the remaining half after 48 h. In several regions, including left hippocampus and left dorsal inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), activity at encoding differed depending on whether items were later recollected regardless of study-test delay. Delay-selective effects were also evident, however. Recollection after 48 h was associated with enhanced activity in bilateral ventral IFG, whereas recollection after 30 min was associated with greater fusiform activity. Thus, there is a relationship between the neural activity elicited by an event as it is encoded and the durability of the resulting memory representation.
bioRxiv (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory), 2022
Systems consolidation of new experiences into lasting episodic memories involves interactions between hippocampus and the neocortex. Evidence of this process is seen already during early awake post-encoding rest periods. Functional MRI (fMRI) studies have demonstrated increased hippocampal coupling with task-relevant perceptual regions and reactivation of stimulus-specific encoding patterns following intensive encoding tasks. Here we investigate the spatial and temporal characteristics of these hippocampally anchored post-encoding neocortical modulations. Eighty-nine adults participated in an experiment consisting of interleaved memory task-and resting-state periods. As expected, we observed increased post-encoding functional connectivity between hippocampus and individually localized neocortical regions responsive to stimulus categories encountered during memory encoding. Post-encoding modulations were however not restricted to stimulus-selective cortex, but manifested as a nearly system-wide upregulation in hippocampal coupling with all major functional networks. The spatial configuration of these extensive modulations resembled hippocampal-neocortical interaction patterns estimated from active encoding operations, suggesting hippocampal post-encoding involvement by far exceeds reactivation of perceptual aspects. This reinstatement of encoding patterns during immediate post-encoding rest was not observed in resting-state scans collected 12 hours later, nor in control analyses estimating post-encoding neocortical modulations in functional connectivity using other candidate seed regions. The broad similarity in hippocampal functional coupling between online memory encoding and offline post-encoding rest suggests reactivation in humans may involve a spectrum of cognitive processes engaged during experience of an event. .
The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 2014
Making sense of previous experience requires remembering the order in which events unfolded in time. Prior work has implicated the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe cortex in memory for temporal information associated with individual episodes. However, the processes involved in encoding and retrieving temporal information across extended sequences is relatively poorly understood. Here we used fMRI during the encoding and retrieval of extended sequences to test specific predictions about the type of information used to resolve temporal order and the role of the hippocampus in this process. Participants studied sequences of images of celebrity faces and common objects followed by a recency discrimination test. The main conditions of interest were pairs of items that had been presented with three intervening items, half of which included an intervening category shift. During encoding, hippocampal pattern similarity across intervening items was associated with subsequent successful o...
PLoS ONE, 2011
Electrophysiological studies in animals have shown coordinated reactivation of neuronal ensembles during a restricted time period of behavioral inactivity that immediately followed active encoding. In the present study we directly investigated offline processing of associative memory formation in the human brain. Subjects' regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) as a surrogate marker of neural activity during rest was measured by MR-based perfusion imaging in a sample of 14 healthy male subjects prior to (Pre2) and after (Post) extensive learning of 24 face-name associations within a selective reminding task (SR). Results demonstrated significant Post-Pre2 rCBF increases in hippocampal and temporal lobe regions, while in a control comparison of two perfusion scans with no learning task in-between (Pre2-Pre1) no differences in rCBF emerged. Post perfusion scanning was followed by a surprise cued associative recall task from which two types of correctly retrieved names were obtained: older names already correctly retrieved at least once during one of the SR blocks, and recent names acquired during the last SR block immediately prior to the Post scan. In the anterior hippocampus individual perfusion increases were correlated with both correct retrievals of older and recent names. By contrast, older but not recently learned names showed a significant correlation with perfusion increases in the left lateral temporal cortex known to be associated with long-term memory. Recent, but not older names were correlated with dopaminergic midbrain structures reported to contribute to the persistence of memory traces for novel information. Although the direct investigation of off-line memory processing did not permit concomitant experimental control, neither intentional rehearsal, nor substantial variations in subjects' states of alertness appear to contribute to present results. We suggest that the observed rCBF increases might reflect processes that possibly contribute to the long-term persistence of memory traces.
2000
Neuronal models predict that retrieval of specific event information reactivates brain regions that were active during encoding of this information. Consistent with this prediction, this positron-emission tomography study showed that remembering that visual words had been paired with sounds at encoding activated some of the auditory brain regions that were engaged during encoding. After word-sound encoding, activation of auditory brain regions was also observed during visual word recognition when there was no demand to retrieve auditory information. Collectively, these observations suggest that information about the auditory components of multisensory event information is stored in auditory responsive cortex and reactivated at retrieval, in keeping with classical ideas about ''redintegration,'' that is, the power of part of an encoded stimulus complex to evoke the whole experience.
Human Brain Mapping, 2013
New episodic memory traces represent a record of the ongoing neocortical processing engaged during memory formation (encoding). Thus, during encoding, deep (semantic) processing typically establishes more distinctive and retrievable memory traces than does shallow (perceptual) processing, as assessed by later episodic memory tests. By contrast, the hippocampus appears to play a processing-independent role in encoding, because hippocampal lesions impair encoding regardless of level of processing. Here, we clarified the neural relationship between processing and encoding by examining hippocampal-cortical connectivity during deep and shallow encoding. Participants studied words during functional magnetic resonance imaging and freely recalled these words after distraction. Deep study processing led to better recall than shallow study processing. For both levels of processing, successful encoding elicited activations of bilateral hippocampus and left prefrontal cortex, and increased functional connectivity between left hippocampus and bilateral medial prefrontal, cingulate and extrastriate cortices. Successful encoding during deep processing was additionally associated with increased functional connectivity between left hippocampus and bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and right temporoparietal junction. In the shallow encoding condition, on the other hand, Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article. Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary. com).
Progress in brain research, 2008
The principle of transfer-appropriate processing and the cortical reinstatement hypothesis are two influential theoretical frameworks, articulated at the psychological and neurobiological levels of explanation, respectively, that each propose that the processes supporting the encoding and retrieval of episodic information are strongly interdependent. Here, we integrate these two frameworks into a single model that generates predictions that can be tested using functional neuroimaging methods in healthy humans, and then go on to describe findings that are in accord with these predictions. Consistent with the transfer-appropriate processing and cortical reinstatement frameworks, the neural correlates of successful encoding vary according to how retrieval is cued, and the neural correlates of retrieval are modulated by how items are encoded. Thus, encoding and retrieval should not be viewed as separate stages of memory that can be investigated in isolation from one another.
Review of Sneve et al.
Although we have countless experiences daily, relatively few are retained in memory. Great efforts have been devoted to understanding the neural processes and mechanisms that contribute to making memories that last. Much of this research has focused on delineating encoding-related patterns of neural responses that predict subsequent memory. Yet, even though humans can remember experiences after substantially long delays, most studies measure memory after relatively short retention intervals (within minutes or hours of encoding).
Pushing the envelope on this topic, Sneve and colleagues (2015) used fMRI to identify encoding-related brain activity patterns that predicted what information would be remembered after 1.5 h (short delay) and after 6 weeks (long delay). Not surprisingly, subsequently remembered items were those that were associated with increased brain activation during encoding. This pattern was observed regardless of the retention interval examined. However, memories that were retained over the long delay were uniquely associated with stronger encoding-related coupling between the hippocampus and cortical regions, suggesting that distinct encoding processes lead to more durable memories. The authors propose that this observed pattern of hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity may reflect a tagging mechanism that potentiates subsequent consolidation of certain experiences into enduring memories. In other words, as we encode some types of information, we are already preparing it for the long haul.
An emerging literature indeed suggests that memory-related neural processes also extend beyond the formation of the initial memory trace. For example, recent work has shown that encoding-related brain activity "spills over" into post-encoding resting states, and the magnitude of this persistent brain activation is related to subsequent memory performance across individuals (Tambini and Davachi, 2013), in accordance with the notion that the replay of encoded experiences during rest (or sleep) facilitates memory consolidation (see also van Kesteren et al., 2010). Intriguingly, Tompary and colleagues (2015) recently demonstrated that patterns of post-encoding functional connectivity between the hippocampus and the ventral tegmental area predicts episodic memory performance after a delay (24 h), but is unrelated to immediate memory performance. Moreover, the authors showed that although both encoding and post-encoding connectivity patterns predicted subsequent delayed memory independently, these patterns of connectivity were not related to each other. Together, these findings suggest that both common and distinct encoding and consolidation mechanisms predict the formation of long-lasting memories. Furthermore, the notion of systems consolidation, i.e., that some memories are reorganized as information moves from the hippocampus to the cortex (Dudai, 2004), suggests that differences in brain activity for memories retained over long intervals, relative to shorter intervals, may also manifest when memories are being retrieved (Takashima et al., 2009). Hence, it may be fruitful to examine encoding-, consolidation-, and retrievalrelated brain activation patterns within the same study to determine not only how the patterns observed at each memory stage relate to long-lasting memories, but also how these patterns specifically evolve across these stages.
More broadly, if the authors' hypothesis is correct, i.e., that increased functional connectivity between hippocampal and cortical regions for long-lasting memories reflects a tagging mechanism, it remains important to determine the precise neural locus of this tagging effect. As connectivity analyses do not specify the direction (causality) of information flow, this approach cannot be used to determine whether the tagging signal originates in the hippocampus or elsewhere in the brain. Moreover, it is not yet known what types of memories undergo this kind of tagging process (i.e., are episodic and nonepisodic memories associated with common or distinct tagging mechanisms?). Such inquiries can shed important light on the nature of systems consolidation, which is poorly understood and greatly debated in humans.
Apart from pinpointing the neural mechanisms that support the formation of more durable memories, it is also important to understand the cognitive factors that may account for this enhanced durability. In addressing this, one would need to determine whether there were specific types of items that participants were more likely to retain over the long delay in Sneve et al.'s (2015) study. Although the authors used stimuli that were comprised of everyday objects, it is possible that some items were more likely to elicit cognitive processes that are known to enhance memory. To illustrate, a to-be-encoded item such as "cake" is more likely to elicit positive emotional valence and appetitive reward value relative to an item such as "hammer." To the extent that these cognitive processes can affect how items are encoded (for review, see Madan, 2013), they may account for differences in patterns of learning and associated functional connectivity (Ritchey et al., 2008) and predict the stability of memory traces over a long delay. Indeed, many studies have found that emotional memories are retained over longer delays relative to neutral memories, whereby emotional information is forgotten more slowly (for in-depth discussions, see Talmi, 2013;Yonelinas and Ritchey, 2015). A similar case could be made for reward-related processes; for example, Wittmann et al. (2005) did not observe a reward-related memory effect in an immediate memory test, but found significant differences after a 3 week delay. Thus, it is possible that item-specific properties may have played an important role in the formation of long-lasting memories and associated brain activity in Sneve et al. (2015). Still, which items were remembered may have also been specific to an individual, as noted by the authors, suggesting that interindividual differences in how people processed the stimuli (e.g., prior experiences or transient mind wandering during the experimental session) are likely also be critical in the formation of long-lasting memories. An item analysis could provide insight into the cognitive mechanisms that underlie more durable memories.
In addition to item-specific features influencing memory, procedural details of Sneve et al.'s (2015) study may have played a role in the greater hippocampal-cortical connectivity found to be involved in the formation of long-lasting memories. Specifically, during encoding, the presented items were accompanied by one of two questions: "Can you eat it?" or "Can you lift it?" The authors speculate that the involvement of right hippocampal connectivity in longlasting memory retention may be related to constructive simulations, i.e., the imagination of the items in relation to the encoding instruction (e.g., eating a hammer). We additionally suggest that these simulations likely involved automatic motor simulations of body-object interactions (Witt et al., 2010; for an in-depth discussion, see Madan and Singhal, 2012) and thus may have elicited greater connectivity with cortical regions in the ventral pathway (Sneve et al., 2015, their Fig. 5) related to automatic processing of body-object interactions (Kellenbach et al., 2003). Importantly, such body-object simulations may have varied across stimuli. For example, given the nature of their stimuli, there may have been a compatibility effect between a given object and its associated encoding instruction; food objects, such as apples, can readily be thought of as objects that may be eaten, while such is not true of nonfood objects such as a hammer. Here we would predict that objects which were more compatible with the provided instruction (e.g., eating an apple) would elicit a more shallow levelof-processing relative to judgments that were more difficult or unnatural, which would have instead resulted in more deliberate and effortful (deeper) processing. Critically, these differences in levels of processing may have influenced the longevity of a given memory, as this is a well known predictor of subsequent memory performance (Craik and Lockhart, 1972). This type of motor-simulation processing could be avoided, for instance, if the stimuli were abstract nouns (e.g., "virtue," "ratio") and the encoding judgments were more perceptual, e.g., word length (odd or even number of letters), capitalization (all uppercase or lowercase letters), or font color. Thus, item-and instruction-related effects likely play a critical role in the formation of memories that last. This topic would be a fruitful avenue for future research.
Despite outstanding questions regarding the precise neural and cognitive mechanisms that can account for the findings of Sneve et al. (2015), the approach used by the authors represents an important step toward bridging the gap between two dominant approaches used in human memory research: one that examines episodic memory in the context of laboratory stimuli at shorter intervals (often minutes to hours), and another that examines real-life (autobiographical) memories after longer intervals (often months to years). While studies of the latter have greatly shed light on the neural and cognitive mechanisms underlying memory for distant events, they inherently lack the ability to examine the encoding of such experiences. Sneve et al.'s (2015) approach, particularly if applied to even longer retention intervals, offers a promising window of opportunity for determining not only the factors influencing the fidelity of long-lasting memories but also the manner by which their neural instantiation changes over time.
Optics Express, 2004
ISU AKTUALISASI, 2019
Precision Agriculture, 2010
Modern Physics Letters A, 1994
Journal of Radioanalytical and Nuclear Chemistry, 1995
Scientific Reports, 2021
International Business Research …
Laser Physics Review, 2011
Cancer Research, 2012
Scientific Reports, 2017
Connotations: A Journal for Critical Debate, 2009
Pacific Journalism Review : Te Koakoa