1
Mark Timmer and Nellie Verhoef
Increasing insightful thinking in analytic geometry
Mark Timmer
Nellie Verhoef
Formal Methods & Tools
University of Twente
P.O. Box 217
7500 AE Enschede
timmer@cs.utwente.nl
ELAN
University of Twente
P.O. Box 217
7500 AE Enschede
N.C.Verhoef@utwente.nl
217
NAW 5/13 nr. 3 september 2012
Increasing insightful thinking in
analytic geometry
Elsewhere in this issue Ferdinand Verhulst described the discussion of the interaction of analysis and geometry in the 19th century. In modern times such discussions come up again and
again. As of 2014, synthetic geometry will not be part of the Dutch ‘vwo – mathematics B’
programme any more. Instead, the focus will be more on analytic geometry. Mark Timmer and
Nellie Verhoef explored possibilities to connect the two disciplines in order to have students
look at analytical exercises from a more synthetic point of view.
Although analytic geometry is a wonderful
technique to prove a variety of theorems in
Euclidean geometry in a convincing and easy
manner, it rarely provides many insights. Secondary school students often apply it without
any consideration of what they are actually
doing. We conjecture that this leads to fragmented understanding. Rather than developing an overall picture of the geometric concepts the students are working with, the analytic and synthetic geometry remain isolated
domains. This results in limited understanding of the mathematical structures at hand,
and a limited set of techniques and strategies
for solving exercises from these different domains. Analytic geometry becomes an end in
itself; students manipulate formulas without
any feeling for the underlying concepts.
Additionally, an analytical approach might
sometimes even be much more cumbersome
than a synthetic argument. By using analytical techniques for dealing with geometric figures, students sometimes forget about
the properties of these objects, resulting in
lengthy, unnecessary calculations.
In the context of the first author’s Master’s
thesis for his mathematics teaching degree
at the University of Twente, we tried to emphasise the underlying concepts of synthetic
geometry when covering a chapter on analytic geometry. This was often accompanied by
visualisations using the GeoGebra computer
programme. The overall goal was to provide
students a richer understanding of geometry [12]. More specifically, we were hoping
for them to develop richer cognitive units [2].
That way, students understand better how different representations of geometric concepts
such as ellipses relate, and are able to quickly switch between them. Hence, they might
work more efficiently when solving exercises
for which a purely analytical approach is unnecessarily difficult.
We already extensively discussed the lesson series and research project that resulted
from the ideas above in a previous article [13].
Here, we elaborate more on the theoretical
background regarding cognitive units and visualisation of geometric objects. Moreover,
we discuss the way in which the results of
this research project were put into practice as
a workshop during the National Mathematics
Days (NWD).
Underlying school mathematics
Our research primarily focused on the ellipse.
This mathematical object can be defined as
follows.
be r . In Figure 2, MPi + Pi F equals the radius
of the circle, for both point P1 and P2 , and all
other points on the ellipse. The ellipse consisting of all points that are equidistant from
a point F and a circle with centre M and radius r , therefore coincides with the ellipse
consisting of all points with cumulative distance r to M and F . Stated differently, M
and F are the focus points of the ellipse in
Figure 2.
Placing an ellipse in a Cartesian coordinate
system with the focus points on the horizontal
axis (see Figure 3), we can show that it coincides with the set of points (x, y) such that
y2
x2
+ b2 = 1. Here, a is half of the length of
a2
the horizontal axis, and b half of the length
of the vertical axis. In Figure 3 this yields
y2
x2
25 + 9 = 1. Interestingly, such an analytical
representation relates in several ways to the
P1
F1
F2
P2
Figure 1 Equal cumulative distance to two focus points
Definition 1. An ellipse is a set of points that
all have the same sum of distances to two
given focus points.
Definition 2. An ellipse is a set of points that
are equidistant from a circle (the directrix circle) and a point within that circle.
P1
∗
∗
M
F
◦
P2
The first definition is illustrated in Figure 1,
the second one in Figure 2.
It is not hard to see that these two definitions coincide. In Figure 1, by definition
F1 P1 + P1 F2 = F1 P2 + P2 F2 ; let this constant
◦
Figure 2 Equal distance to circle and point
1
2
218
NAW 5/13 nr. 3 september 2012
Increasing insightful thinking in analytic geometry
3
e
2
1
F1
−6
−5
−4
F2
−3
−2
−1
−1
0
1
2
3
4
5
−2
−3
considered. Initially, such characterisations
will probably not be strongly connected in the
students’ brains. Later on, rich cognitive units
might develop, allowing the students to perceive the characterisations as different representations of the same object. This is expected to yield more efficiency and understanding.
Figure 3 An ellipse in a coordinate system
synthetic definitions discussed above. For instance, 2a corresponds to the radius of the di√
rectrix circle, and 2 a2 − b2 is the distance
between the focus points.
We expected proficiency in such conversions between the analytical and the synthetic domain to increase understanding and insight, helping students solve exercises more
effectively and efficiently.
Theoretical framework
In this study we investigated students’ cognitive items with respect to geometric objects.
In particular, we assessed the effects of a
teaching method based on visualisation and
synthetic geometry on these units. Hence,
this section provides an overview of the theory regarding cognitive units and visualisation.
Cognitive units
The human brain is not capable of thinking
about many things at once. Complicated activities such as mathematical thinking therefore have to be made manageable by abstracting away unnecessary details and focusing on
the most important aspects [2]. The term cognitive unit originated from this idea:
“A cognitive unit consists of a cognitive
item that can be held in the focus of attention of an individual at one time, together with
other ideas that can be immediately linked to
it.” [10]
The ‘cognitive item’ mentioned here could
be a formula such as a2 + b2 = c 2 , a fact such
as 10 + 3 = 13 or a mental image of an ellipse.
The connectivity between cognitive items and
related ideas depends on the degree of understanding. For instance, most people would
probably immediately relate 3 + 4, 4 + 3 and 7,
and hence have strong connections between
these cognitive items. They can then be considered as a single cognitive structure: a cognitive unit.
Barnard and Tall emphasise the importance of rich cognitive units, having strong internal connections between different objects
or representations of objects, and leading to
powerful ways of thinking. In our case, several different characterisations of the ellipse are
Compression to rich cognitive units. Rich
cognitive units do not develop out of thin air.
At first, a student will have a fragmented understanding of a new concept. Then, several
different approaches might be needed to obtain a full understanding. However, once a
concept has been fully understood, a significant mental compression can often be observed. Thurston explains how this results in
a complete mental perspective — although at
first obtained by a long process — to be easily
used as part of a new mental process [11].
The notion of compression is applied on
the one hand for the compression of knowledge into small cognitive items [3], and on
the other hand for the way in which different cognitive items are coupled into stronglyconnected cognitive units [10]. Since both
processes yield richer cognitive units, we do
not distinguish between these two meanings.
Causing compression. In order to induce
compression, brain sections have to be connected to such an extent that addressing one
of them also activates the others. After all,
this makes the combined knowledge and understanding of these sections function together as a single cognitive structure [10].
More specifically, compression can be
brought about in several different ways [9]. A
student could categorise concepts or perform
thought experiments, leading to connections
between properties of those concepts. Repeatedly practising certain procedures until
they are automated may also yield rich cognitive units. Finally, compression can be induced by abstraction: introducing symbols or
names. Gray and Tall indeed indicate that
we can only effectively talk about phenomena once they have been given a name [3].
As this compresses them to a cognitive unit,
it enables us to think about them in a more
sophisticated manner.
Visualisation
In this study, the underlying concepts from
synthetic geometry were often visualised using GeoGebra, a computer programme for dynamic geometry [4]. The geometric objects
under consideration indeed perfectly fit dy-
Mark Timmer and Nellie Verhoef
namic visualisation. For instance, we can easily use an equation for an ellipse and a slider determining its parameter a, to teach students this parameter’s effect on the ellipse.
Scientific literature indicates that visualisation may improve mathematical understanding, although this does not necessary
has to happen. Stols explains how the use of
IT — more specifically, GeoGebra and Cabri 3D
— only positively affects geometric insights of
students that did not have much understanding yet, and even then only marginally [8]. He
recommends to deploy applications such as
GeoGebra to improve visualisation skills and
conceptual understanding, and enable students to discover important relations. However, these programmes should not be expected
to improve reasoning skills. We indeed only
used GeoGebra for visualisation and to observe connections between concepts.
Langill also describes that software like
GeoGebra should mainly be used as a supplement to non-technological sources, such
as books [6]. She noticed that distance measuring and point dragging are among the
most powerful applications of dynamic geometry. Therefore, we indeed combined visualisations with additional exercises, and extensively applied dragging and measurements to
illustrate geometric properties.
Other researchers confirmed that technology can help students discover connections between different representations of the
same concept, but also noticed that it should
not be deployed too early [1]. They found
that visualisations should be linked directly to
knowledge that the students already possess,
to avoid frustration and misconceptions. We
therefore only used GeoGebra to clarify concepts the students were already familiar with,
avoiding this pitfall.
Despite the potential merits of dynamic geometry software, it is still not used very often.
Stols and Kriek report that a negative attitude
towards the added value of such software,
as well as a lack of confidence in their own
technical skills, prohibit teachers from using
applications like GeoGebra [14]. Zhao, Pugh,
Sheldon and Byers also reached this conclusion, and observed that teachers have to take
small evolutionary steps when introducing ICT
in the classroom; a revolutionary approach
would only lead to failure and frustration [15].
In this study, GeoGebra was only used by
the teacher. Obviously, it is also possible to
have the students play with the application.
Although this is indeed expected to help students discover geometric theorems [7] or understand geometric transformations [5], we
2
3
Mark Timmer and Nellie Verhoef
only applied GeoGebra for demonstrations.
After all, we did not focus on developing new
geometric skills, but more on the application
of available geometric knowledge in the context of analytic geometry.
This study
We performed our study in a vwo 5 mathematics D class at the Stedelijk Lyceum Kottenpark in Enschede. Since this class consisted of only four students (for privacy reasons all addressed by ‘he’ in this article), we
were able to observe the students in much
detail and question them individually. The
researcher taught Chapter 14 of the Getal &
Ruimte vwo D4 method. This chapter covers
symmetry, parametric equations and difference quotients, based on parabolas, ellipses
and hyperbolas.
We tried to encourage the students to focus on connections between synthetic and
analytic geometry in three different ways:
(1) by giving additional explanations — often
accompanied by GeoGebra visualisations —
to make students aware of what they are doing, (2) by discussing how several analytical
exercises from the book can be solved more
easily using geometric reasoning, and (3) by
introducing a number of new exercises for the
students to practice these skills on. We refer
to [12–13] for an extensive description of the
lesson series.
Semi-structured interviews before and after the lesson series have shown quite a different effect on each of the four students. For
one of them, the focus on synthetic geometry seemed to work out poorly. This student
Increasing insightful thinking in analytic geometry
NAW 5/13 nr. 3 september 2012
219
showed only limited knowledge and insight,
both before and after the lesson series. He
preferred to rely on an analytical approach,
and already declared upfront to rather just calculate than think of a smarter way to solve an
exercise. Additionally, he often indicated to
not have much confidence in his own mathematical understanding, explaining his preference for structured rules and procedures.
The other three students were much more
enthusiastic, and showed a positive attitude
towards the new way of approaching analytic geometry. They most liked the feeling of
deeper understanding, as well as the simplicity to achieve results. One student indeed
showed considerably more insight during the
post-test. He switched rapidly between different representations of the same concept,
for instance by using symmetry for an analytical exercise and by combining both definitions of the ellipse in a smart manner.
Additionally, he often first took a moment
to think before relying on calculations, and
showed growth in his associations with geometric concepts.
The other two students showed slightly less progress, but still improved visibly.
They were able to identify more representations and more often applied geometric concepts such as symmetry. Interestingly, it appeared that some insights were present, but
only surfaced after considerable encouragement. This indicates that certain connections
between cognitive items have been made,
but also that more practice is needed to enable fast switching between the accumulated
knowledge from different domains.
National Mathematics Days
To share our findings with a larger group of
teachers, we conducted a workshop during
the most recent National Mathematics Days
(www.fisme.science.uu.nl/nwd). There appeared to be quite some interest in our topic; teachers were happy to discuss a more
insightful manner of working with analytic
geometry.
After a short introduction of the subject,
the teachers were asked to work on some of
the exercises the students also tried to solve
during their post-test. They intensely calculated and discussed, and appeared to pursue
many different approaches. We found that
they did not always fully use all available data and possible connections to other representations.
The determination to solve
the difficult exercises, however, was inspiring. Such an attitude would benefit every
student!
The teachers asked many questions about
the translation from our ideas to the classroom: how can we make students follow
our approach, combining different representations and thinking before computing? As we
mentioned before, frequent practice seems
to be key. The workshop participants were
pleased to hear and experience a creative way
of addressing synthetic geometry in the current mathematics curriculum.
More details on the lessons and exercises can be found in [13]. For an extensive
description of the research project, we refer
to [12]. Both articles, as well as all material
used at the NWD, can be found at http://fmt.
cs.utwente.nl/˜timmer/research.php.
k
ics classroom: a meta-analysis, 2009 (unpublished).
12 M. Timmer, Rijkere cognitieve eenheden door
het benadrukken van synthetische meetkunde tijdens de behandeling van analytische meetkunde, Master’s thesis, Universiteit
Twente, 2011.
References
1
2
M. Alagic, Technology in the mathematics classroom: Conceptual orientation, Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching
22(4), 2003, pp. 381–399.
T. Barnard and D.O. Tall, Cognitive Units, Connections and Mathematical Proof, Proceedings
of the 21st Conference of the International Group
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
1997, pp. 41–48.
3
E. Gray and D.O. Tall, Abstraction as a natural
process of mental compression, Mathematics
Education Research Journal 19(2), 2007, pp. 23–
40.
4
M. Hohenwarter and J. Preiner, Dynamic Mathematics with GeoGebra, Journal of Online Mathematics and its Applications 7, 2007.
5
K.F. Hollebrands, High school students’ understandings of geometric transformations in the
context of a technological environment, Journal
of Mathematical Behavior 22(1), 2003, pp. 55–
72.
6
J. Langill, Requirements to make effective use of
dynamic geometry software in the mathemat-
7
8
9
R.A. Saha, A.F.M. Ayub and R.A. Tarmizi, The
Effects of GeoGebra on Mathematics Achievement: enlightening Coordinate Geometry Learning, Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 8,
2010, pp. 686–693.
G.H. Stols, Influence of the use of technology
on students’ geometric development in terms
of the Van Hiele levels, Proceedings of the 18th
Annual Conference of the Southern African Association for Research in Mathematics, 2010,
pp. 149–155.
D.O. Tall, A theory of mathematical growth
through embodiment, symbolism and proof,
Annales de Didactique et de Sciences Cognitives 11, 2006, pp. 195–215.
13
M. Timmer and N.C. Verhoef, Analytische
meetkunde door een synthetische bril, Nieuwe
Wiskrant 31(4), 2012, pp. 13–18.
14 G.H. Stols and J. Kriek, Why don’t all maths
teachers use dynamic geometry software in
their classrooms?, Australasian Journal of Educational Technology 27(1), 2011, pp. 137–151.
15
Y. Zhao, K. Pugh, S. Sheldon and J.L. Byers, Conditions for Classroom Technology Innovations,
Teachers College Record 104(3), 2002, pp. 482–
515.
10 D.O. Tall and T. Barnard, Cognitive Units, Connections and Compression in Mathematical
Thinking, 2002 (unpublished).
11
W.P. Thurston, Mathematical education, Notices
of the American Mathematical Society 37(7),
1990, pp. 844–850.
3