Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Violence in sleep

2010, Brain

Therei sn oa ctual or potential conflict of interest in relation to this article.

Review article Violence in sleep Francesca Siclaria, Ramin Khatamia, c, Frank Urbaniokd, Claudio L. Bassettia, b Depar tment of Neurology, University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland Neurocenter of Southern Switzerland c Barmelweid Sleep Clinic, Switzerland d Psychiatric-Psychological Ser vice, Zürich Justice Depar tment, Switzerland a b There is no actual or potential conflict of interest in relation to this ar ticle. Summary Sleep-related violence occurs in 2% of the adult population and can result from various conditions, including parasomnias (such as arousal disorders and rapid eye movement behavior disorder), epilepsy (in particular nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy) and psychiatric diseases (including delirium, dissociative states and factitious disorders). Its occurrence is probably favored by hypoactivity or dysfunction of the prefrontal cerebral areas, and selective activation of pathways involved in complex emotional and motor behavior. The present review outlines the different sleep disorders associated with violence, and aims to provide information on diagnosis, therapy and forensic issues. It also discusses current physiopathological models, establishing a link between sleep-related violence and violence observed in other settings. Key words: sleep; parasomnias; violence; epilepsy Introduction Although generally considered as mutually exclusive, sleep and violence can coexist. What appears to be one of the first of such reports dates back to medieval times and relates to a Silesian woodcutter, who after a few hours of sleep woke up abruptly, aimed his axe at an imaginary intruder and killed his wife instead (cited by Gastaut and Broughton [1]). Another early case was reported by Yellowless in 1878 [2] and describes a young man with a history of sleep terrors who killed his 18-month old son by smashing him against the wall during the night, taking him for a wild beast that was about to attack his family. Nowadays, dramatic reports of somnambulistic homicide still gain considerable attention in the media [3, 4]. The most widely known condition in this respect is probably sleepwalking, but numerous other disorders with a potential for sleep-related violence exist, including other parasomnias, epilepsy and psychiatric diseases. Violence in the course of these disorders occurs when boundaries defining wakefulness and sleep disrupt, resulting in a dissociation between “mind sleep” and “body sleep” [5]. Neurologists and sleep specialists should be familiar with these conditions, not least because sleep-related violence is more frequent than generally assumed, occurring in up to 2% of the adult population [6]. Important advances in the fields of genetics, neuroimaging and behavioral neurology have expanded the understanding of the mechanisms underlying violence and its particular relation to sleep. Along with this increasing knowledge, sleep specialists assume a growing role in legal issues related to violent acts committed during sleep. Lastly, most sleep disorders associated with violence are treatable; making the correct diagnosis thus constitutes the first step in the prevention of further violence. The present review outlines the different sleep disorders associated with violence, and aims at providing information on diagnosis, therapy and forensic issues. It also discusses current physiopathological models, establishing a link between sleep-related violence and violence observed in other settings. Definitions For the purpose of this review, violence is defined as an aggressive act that inflicts unwarranted physical harm on others [7]. It is a subset of aggression, a broader term encompassing both mental and physical damage. Unlike violence, aggression may be considered legitimate in certain circumstances. In the light of current physiopathological models, two types of aggression can be distinguished [8–10]. Premeditated aggression (also referred to as instrumental, predatory or proactive aggression) is purposeful and goal-directed. It commonly occurs in psychopaths and is thought to result from of a failure of moral socialization, involving the mechanisms of aversive conditioning and instrumental learning. Premeditated aggression may develop as a lack of formative learning experience, or because the underlying neuro-cognitive architecture is dysfunctional [8]. Impulsive aggression (also termed affective, reactive or hostile aggression) constitutes a response to a frustrating or threatening event that induces anger, and occurs without regard for any potential goal. Impulsive aggression has a particular relevance with regard to sleep-related violence. Disorders underlying sleep-related violence Correspondence: Prof. Dr. med. Claudio L. Bassetti Neurocenter of Southern Switzerland Neurology Department, University of Zürich Via Tesserete 46 CH-6903 Lugano claudio.bassetti©eoc.ch Parasomnias On the basis of electrophysiological variables (eye movements, muscle tone, EEG activity), one can distinguish three different states of vigilance, namely wakefulness, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and Non REM (NREM) sleep. In S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 322 Review article some circumstances however, boundaries between these states disrupt, resulting in conditions that share features of more than one state. Examples of such a state dissociation include, amongst others, disorders of arousal (dissociation between NREM sleep and wakefulness), rapideye-movement behavior disorder, lucid dreaming, peduncular hallucinosis, cataplexy, sleep-related hallucinations and sleep paralysis (dissociation between REM sleep and wakefulness). An extreme form of dissociation, referred to as “status dissociatus”, with the simultaneous presence of features of each one of the three states, has also been reported [11]. It is in the context of state dissociation, when motor activity becomes dissociated from wakefulness, that sleep-related violence occurs in the course of parasomnias. Disorders of arousal Disorders of arousal encompass sleepwalking, confusional arousals and sleep terrors. They consist of an incomplete awakening from NREM sleep characterized by reduced vigilance, impaired cognition, retrograde amnesia for the event and variable motor activity, ranging from repetitive and purposeless movements to more complex behaviors such as eating, drinking, driving, sexual intercourse and aggression [12]. Disorders of arousal are common during childhood (with a prevalence of 10% for sleepwalking, 17% for confusional arousals and 1–6.5% for sleep terrors), but may persist or arise de novo during adulthood in 2–4% of cases [12]. It is generally assumed that they result from the interplay between predisposing, priming and precipitating factors [13]. Predisposition is based on genetic susceptibility, which in the case of sleepwalking, is suggested by the 10-fold increased prevalence among first degree relatives of sleepwalkers [14]. The genes that confer the risk of sleepwalking remain essentially unknown. The only established marker is the presence of the HLA DQ1B allele found in 35% of sleepwalkers, compared to only 13% of normal subjects [15]. Priming factors act by either increasing slow wave sleep or by heightening the threshold for arousal during NREM sleep and include the acute effect of alcohol, fever, stress, a large variety of psychotropic medications, reviewed elsewhere [13], and probably sleep deprivation, although studies yield conflicting results in this respect [16–18]. Among precipitating factors, sleep disordered breathing, periodic leg movements as well as noise and touch have been identified [13]. Confusional arousals consist of mental confusion or confusional behavior upon awaking from sleep, most often from slow wave sleep in the first part of the night. If an individual leaves the bed and starts walking in the course of a confusional arousal, the disorder is referred to as sleepwalking. In sleep terrors, the arousal is characterized by intense autonomic activation and typical behavioral features such as sitting up in bed and screaming. Different arousal disorders may coexist in the same individual, and, not infrequently, an episode may start as one arousal disorder and evolve into another (i.e., sleep terror evolving into sleepwalking). They normally occur in the first half of the night and tend not to recur during the same night. Disorders of arousal have a potential for sleep related violence. Homicide, attempted homicide, filicide, suicide and inappropriate sexual behaviors have been reported in this setting [19]. Pressman [20] reviewed 32 legal and medical case reports of violence associated with disorders of arousal, and found that aggressive behavior occurred in different ways in confusional arousals, sleepwalking and sleep terrors. In confusional arousals, violence was usually elicited when individuals were awakened from sleep by someone else. Bonkalo [21], for instance, reported the case of a night shift supervisor who fell asleep in the office, was awakened by an employee and pulled the gun in confusion, killing the employee. Violent behavior during sleepwalking, in contrast, tended to occur when the sleepwalking episode was already underway, and the individual was approached by another person or incidentally encountered someone else. As an example, Broughton [22] examined the case of a patient who during a presumed episode of somnambulism, completed a 15–20 minute drive to his parents in-law’s house, beat his father-in-law unconscious and killed his mother-in-law with a knife from her own kitchen. Violence related to sleep terrors appears to be a reaction to a concrete, frightening image that the individual can subsequently describe. In this context, Howard and D’Orban [23] reported the case of a man who fell asleep next to his friend, and upon awakening, found this friend severely injured. He described a frightening image of three figures trying to attack him, and recalled hitting and punching them. In the same series of cases, Pressman examined the role of physical contact and proximity as triggering factors of sleep-related violence, and found that 100% of confusional arousals, 81% of sleep terrors and 40–90% of sleepwalking cases were associated with provocations including noise, touch and/or close proximity. Two studies tried to further identify risk factors for the occurrence of violence in disorders of arousal. Moldofsky and colleagues [24] who recruited 64 consecutive patients with sleepwalking behaviour or sleep terrors, found that serious violent behavior directed towards other people occurred more frequently in men, and was significantly associated with more stressors, excessive caffeinated beverages, drug abuse and less stage four sleep. Guilleminault and coworkers [25] retrospectively reviewed a series of 41 adult individuals with nocturnal wandering of different etiologies. Compared to the non-violent individuals, the 29 violent patients were predominantly male (65% versus 42%), and comprised of the only two subjects with temporal lobe epilepsy. Among the complex behaviors that can be observed during disorders of arousal, sleep-related sexual activity is frequently associated with violence. Sexual behaviors during sleep (also termed sleepsex or sexsomnia) encompass fondling another person, sexual intercourse, masturbation, and sexual vocalizations. In over 90% of cases, sleepsex occurs during confusional arousals; the remainder of cases are related to sleepwalking, REM behavior disorder, seizures, Kleine-Levin Syndrome, severe chronic insomnia, restless leg syndrome, narcolepsy and psychiatric diseases [26]. Among the 31 patients with parasomnia-related sleepsex reviewed by Schenck and coworkers [26], 45% displayed assaultive behavior, 29% had sex with minors, and 36% S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 323 Review article Figure 1 82-year-old man with a 2-year history of symmetrical extrapyramidal symptoms and cognitive dysfunction, attributed to vascular leucencephalopathy. According to his wife he displayed frequent vocalizations and violent behavior during sleep. Polysomnographic recordings show loss of normal muscle atonia during REM-sleep and increased phasic muscle activity as well as acting out of dreams with kicking (Fig. a), hitting (Fig. b) and menacing gestures. Based on these features the diagnosis of rapid eye movement disorder was established. sustained legal consequences from their sexual sleep-related behavior. More than half of the patients induced physical harm to others, and 6% to themselves. Men were clearly overrepresented (80%) and were the only ones to initiate sexual intercourse, whereas women preferentially engaged in masturbation or displayed sexual vocalizations. None of the patients recalled their sexual behavior the next morning. Of the seven patients with seizure-related sleepsex, only one patient displayed assaultive behavior, and two patients injured themselves, while none had sex with minors, injured others or sustained legal consequences. Males and females were equally distributed in this group, and only one third were amnestic for sleepsex. REM sleep Behavior disorder REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD) is characterized by a loss of normal muscle atonia and an increase of phasic muscle activity during REM sleep, and is associated with altered dream content and acting out of dreams. It is disruptive and causes injury to the patient himself or the bed partner. RBD is more prevalent in men and after the age of 50. In 45% of cases, it represents the first manifestation of a neurodegenerative disorder including Parkinson’s disease, multisystem atrophy and dementia with Lewy bodies [27]. Sleep related injuries to the patient himself or to the bed partner have been reported in 32–69% of cases [28–30] and often lead the patient to seek medical advice. They generally occur when the patient hits the furniture or walls, or falls out of bed. Violent acts can be complex, such as firing an unloaded gun, or setting fire to the bed, and can result in serious injuries, including subdural hematomas and bone fractures [28, 29, 31]. Attempted assault of sleep partners has been reported to occur in 64% of cases, with injuries in 3% [28]. Unlike violence related to disorders of arousal, in RBD the individual is readily oriented upon awakening and can generally recall vivid dream imagery related to the violent act. Of note, dreams by patients with idiopathic and secondary RBD have a more aggressive content compared to dreams S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 324 Review article Table 1 Distinguishing features between nocturnal seizures in nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy (NFLE) and arousal disorders. Features Disorders of arousal NFLE Age of onset Childhood Childhood or adolescence Persistence into adulthood Rarely Frequent Motor features Variable, not highly stereoHighly stereotyped, often typed, no dystonic posturing hyperkinetic Amnesia for event Generally present Generally present Postictal confusion Frequent Generally absent Duration Generally >30 seconds Seconds to 3 minutes Same-night recurrence Low High Timing First third of the night Any time of the night, often in clusters Ictal EEG Slow waves Clear-cut epileptiform discharges in <10% of normal subjects, despite lower levels of daytime aggressiveness [32]. Likewise, among patients with Parkinson’s disease, those affected by RBD have more aggressive dreams compared to patients without RBD, irrespective of gender [33]. A recent study has shown that in RBD associated with Parkinson’s disease, motor activity may be vigorous and fast compared to the hypokinesia observed during wakefulness [34]. This restored motor control during REM-sleep (or a dissociated state between REM sleep and wakefulness), in association with loss of REM sleep atonia and the actionloaded dreams probably accounts for the injury potential of this disorder. For a practical example see figure 1. Epilepsy In nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy (NFLE), seizures can occur exclusively during sleep and be associated with violence. There are three different forms of NFLE that frequently coexist in a single patient. Paroxysmal arousals consist of abrupt arousals from sleep with stereotyped motor phenomena, including head movements, frightened expressions and dystonic limb posturing. They are short, lasting less than 20 seconds, and tend to recur frequently during the night. Episodes of so called nocturnal paroxysmal dystonia also begin with a sudden arousal but involve more complex motor activity such as bipedal automatisms, rhythmic movements of the trunk and limbs, as well as tonic and dystonic posturing. They generally last less than two minutes. Epileptic nocturnal wanderings also start with an abrupt arousal, proceed through the stage of paroxysmal dystonia, and eventually culminate in deambulation. This condition generally lasts less than three minutes. The clinical manifestations of seizures are often very similar to the motor activity observed during disorders of arousal, making a distinction between the two conditions difficult. In addition, in approximately half the patients with NFLE, EEG fails to show ictal or interictal abnormalities [35]. However, establishing the correct diagnosis is crucial, as these entities differ in terms of therapy. Distinguishing features between nocturnal seizures and disorders of arousal have been reviewed by Derry [36], Nobili [37] and Tinuper [38] and are summarized in table 1. A clinical scale has been developed with the aim to distinguish between these conditions [39]; although some items seem to limit its sensitivity [40]. Nocturnal hypermotor seizures [41, 42], including epileptic nocturnal wanderings [43], can occasionally originate from the temporal lobe. Injury resulting from nocturnal seizures can be accidental and related to hyperkinetic features [44] (figure 2), although compared to seizures during wakefulness, the injury potential of seizures occurring solely during sleep is probably lower, as the bed represents a relatively safe environment [45]. Alternatively, injury might result from aggressive, and more directed behavior as documented during episodic nocturnal wandering [25, 44, 46, 47]. More generally, violent behavior can be related to the ictal, peri-ictal and interictal period. Ictal aggression is exceptional [48, 49–54]. It can take the form of biting, grasping, hitting, threatening, screaming, facial expressions of anger, pushing, shoving, and spitting [48, 52]. In these circumstances the violent act is not directed towards others and does not involve intricate skills or purposeful movements. However, more directed violence can occur as a reaction to stimuli of the patient’s environment and can for instance be elicited by the act of restraining the patient [48, 49]. Seizure-related violent behavior is more common in men [48, 49, 51, 54] and is associated with bilateral amygdala/hippocampal and/ or ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction [48]. Peri-ictal aggression occurs in the pre-ictal, or more commonly in the postictal period, in a setting of confusion and abnormal mood (depression, psychosis or delirium). Again, behavior is recurrent, out of character and stereotyped for a given patient, and is frequently associated with amnesia. It may also occur in a setting of postictal psychosis that typically follows a seizure after a lucid interval of hours to days. Compared to postictal confusion, postictal psychosis bears a greater potential for well-directed violent behavior [55]. Interictal violence is generally related to cognitive or behavioral disturbances secondary to a brain dysfunction that may also underlie epileptogenesis [56]. Nocturnal dissociative disorders Dissociative disorders are defined as a disruption of the usually integrated functions of consciousness, memory, identity, or perception of the environment [57]. They occur without the conscious awareness on the part of the individual, are sometimes associated with violence [58] and can arise exclusively or predominantly from the sleep period [59]. In contrast to parasomnias of NREM or REM sleep, dissociative disorders occur during well established EEG wakefulness, either at the transition from wakefulness to sleep or within several minutes after awakening from stages 1 or 2 of NREM or from REM sleep [12]. Schenck and coworkers [59] determined that in 5% of patients referred for sleep-related injury, nocturnal dissociative disorder was the cause of the injuries. Patients were predominantly female (87%), had a history of sexual abuse or posttraumatic stress disorder and additional dissociative episodes during daytime (87%). Violent behavior reported in this setting included breaking windows, shredding clothes, thrashing movements, approaching a person with a knife and automutilation. One patient exclusively S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 325 Review article Figure 2 36-year-old woman with nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy since the age of 13. Seizures occurred up to 10 times a night and were characterized by an abrupt awakening from sleep with a feeling of chest tightness and dyspnea, and progressed to tonic posturing of the limbs and hyperkinetic features, including elevation and extension of the left arm (Fig. b) and pedaling (Fig. a and c). The patient was fully aware during the seizure but was unable to speak. She had sustained several injuries including fractures of her fingers when hitting objects during her seizures. The arrow on top of the polysomnographic recording shows the beginning of a seizure arising from slow wave sleep. Intracranial EEG recordings indicated that seizures originated from a cortical dysplasia in the left frontal cingulum. displayed nocturnal animalistic behavior (quadrupedalism, growling, hissing) linked to a dream of being a jungle tiger. Patients were either amnestic for the episodes or recalled a dream which matched the documented behavior. Factitious disorder In factitious disorder (also named Münchhausen syndrome), an individual presents with an illness that is deliberately produced or falsified for the purpose of assuming the sick role. In a special form, factitious syndrome by proxy (or Münchhausen syndrome by proxy), disease is produced or feigned in another person [57]. Both disorders may occur during the sleep period and can be associated with violence. Griffith and coworkers [60] published a case of apparent nocturnal apneas, diarrhea and vomiting in a 2-month old child. Repeated polysomnographic recordings were normal. Eventually, during a hospital stay, the mother was seen administrating an enema (first rectally and then orally) to the child, and the diagnosis of Münchhausen syndrome by proxy was established. Malingering In contrast to factitious disorder, malingering is not a mental illness, and intentionally produced symptoms or signs are motivated by external incentives for the behavior, (i.e., economic gain, avoiding legal responsibility, improving physical well-being) [57]. Again, symptoms can be referred to the sleep period, but when observed, always occur during well-established EEG-wakefulness. Sundowning In patients with dementia, confusion and wandering frequently increase during late afternoon and evening and improve during the day (“sundowning”). This phenomenon occurs in approximately half of the patients with Alzheimer’s disease, has been shown to predict cognitive decline and can be associated with disruptive behavior [61]. S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 326 Review article Physiopathology Anatomic substrates for violence Time and again, attempts have been made to develop biological explanatory models for violent and sexual offenses. With the availability of modern imaging techniques, neurobiological phenomena have shifted more and more towards the center of attention. In this context, various researchers have supported the theory that offenses, in general, result from neurobiological deficits [62–64]. Particularly for violent behavior, emphasis has been placed on prefrontal lesions or disorders of emotional processing [65–67]. Following the neurobiological causality theory of criminal behavior, the authors came to the conclusion that criminal behavior is – in principle – neurobiologically determined and that the entire individual responsibility is therefore detracted from the offender [62–64]. However, it has been demonstrated that both the theory of neurobiological causality and the theory of neurobiological determinism can be traced back to several methodical misunderstandings and cannot be perpetuated in this form [68]. It is important to consider that there are very diverse forms and mechanisms of violent and sexual offenses, which differ in regard to development and etiology. In order to demonstrate relationships between individual subgroups, it would be necessary to examine a multitude of variables for very large and homogeneous populations, as – from experience – numerous interdependencies exist, especially in the areas of forensic research. Such studies are not available. Generally, for most forms of violent behavior, specific personality traits are important. These can be displayed in the form of prognostic syndromes; as risk dispositions individual to a personality. In the sense of relative determinism, these risk dispositions are associated with a certain probability for specific forms of violent behavior. The question arises, however, as to whether any of the biological parameters actually play an etiological role or whether they are simply more likely the result of a risk-relevant personality trait. Sleeprelated violence is distinctly different from any other form of violent and sexual delinquency, in as much that biological and physiopathological mechanisms are of particular importance. For this reason, these parameters will be described in the following section, despite their limited explanatory power in general. Among the anatomic substrates of violence, the prefrontal and temporal lobes play a major role. Involvement of the anterior temporal lobe in aggressive behavior was initially suggested by Klüver and Bucy [79], who showed that bilateral amygdala ablation in monkeys resulted in placidity; an observation that was later described in humans with bilateral anterior temporal lesions [80]. Another study identified an increased occurrence of lesions in the anterior inferior temporal lobe in violent patients with organic mental syndrome compared to non violent patients of the same group [81]. As discussed above, aggressive behavior may be a feature of temporolimbic epilepsy [82]. Within the prefrontal cortex, the medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal regions seem to be selectively implicated in the control of aggressive behavior. Dysfunction in these areas is indicated by impaired emotional recognition of faces, errors in odour identification and disadvantageous decisions in gambling tests, all of which have been observed in patients with impulsive aggressive disorder; while working memory, an indicator of dorsolateral prefrontal dysfunction, was comparable to control subjects [83]. Olfactory dysfunction as a measure of orbitofrontal dysfunction has also been shown to correlate with impulsivity and physical aggression in patients with PTSD [84]. Further evidence for the involvement of these areas is suggested by the finding that head injuries in Vietnam veterans were associated with violence if localized to the frontal ventromedial or orbitofrontal regions [85]. Likewise, individuals with intermittent explosive disorder exhibit exaggerated amygdala activity and diminished activation of the orbitofrontal cortex in response to faces expressing anger, and fail to demonstrate coupling between these two structures [86]. According to functional models [8], the amygdala and the orbitofrontal lobe act on subcortical systems mediating reactive aggression. While the role of the amygdala is to up- or downregulate their responsiveness to threat, the orbitofrontal cortex exerts its modulating activity in response to social clues, including frustration, expressions of anger, staring and social disapproval. Neurotransmitters Testosterone and genetics Epidemiological studies indicate that male gender is the most consistent risk factor for violence [69], suggesting the involvement of testosterone as a causal factor. While a direct link between aggressive behavior and testosterone has been demonstrated in animals [70, 71], this link is very weak and inconsistent in humans [72]. Other variables associated with violence include unemployment, alcohol abuse, a lower educational level and access to firearms [69, 73]. A genetic background of violence is suggested by the higher concordance of violent behavior in monozygotic twins compared to dizygotic twins [74–77], and this has been confirmed by studies evaluating twins reared apart [78]. Despite a number of candidate molecules that have been identified with regard to violent behavior, the most consistent evidence focuses on serotonin and catecholamines. While serotonin seems to have an inhibitory effect on impulsive aggression, catecholamines are thought to act as a facilitating factor [10]. Impulsive violent offenders, for instance, have been shown to have lower cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of serotonin metabolites compared to non impulsive violent offenders [87], and these findings were later supported by other measures of serotonin function [10]. Additionally, fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, is associated with an improvement in anger and aggression, both towards oneself and others [88]. Another study showed that induced hostile behavior was associated with an increase in basal norepinephrine levels in a group of violent individuals [89]. Moreover, reductions in assaultive behavior S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 327 Review article have been observed after the administration of propanolol, a non selective beta-blocker antagonizing the action of epinephrine, in a double blind, placebo controlled crossover study in patients with organic brain disease [90]. Elevated levels of aggression have been shown in mice lacking monoamineoxidase A and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) [91, 92], two enzymes involved in degradation of norepinephrine and dopamine. In humans, polymorphisms in the MAO-A gene seem to be associated with violent behavior and linked to changes in brain structure and function. A low expression of MAO-A variant has been shown to increase the risk of violent behavior and to be associated with volume reductions in the limbic system and hyperresponsiveness of the amygdala during emotional arousal. In the same study, regulatory prefrontal areas showed diminished reactivity compared to the high expression allele group [93]. Likewise, violence seems to be associated with low COMT activity in violent schizophrenic patients [94]. Additional evidence for the role of dopamine in violent behavior comes from a study that found increased levels of homovanillic acid, a dopamine metabolite, in the CSF of violent offenders [95]. Another study documented a rise in dopamine efflux and a decrease of serotonin efflux in the nucleus accumbens in rats when anticipating an aggressive confrontation [96]. Automatisms arising from central pattern generators In the aforementioned parasomnias and epileptic disorders, highly complex motor activity occurs in the absence of awareness, suggesting a deactivation of cortical structures crucial for judgment and consciousness. In fact, a series of complex behavior sequences can be generated by brainstem structures alone, without the involvement of cortical regions [97–99]. These brainstem centers are called central pattern generators and control stereotyped innate motor behavior necessary for survival, such as locomotion, swimming, sexual activity and other rhythmic motor sequences. Aggressive behavior probably represents an innate action pattern aimed at the defense of the peripersonal space [48]. Activation of central pattern generators probably underlies a series of behaviors observed in the course of parasomnias (such as sleepsex, nocturnal eating disorder, bruxism, sleepwalking and sleep-related violence) and seizures (ictal biting, grasping, oro-mandibular automatisms, locomotor activity, facial expressions of fear, and vocalizations). The involvement of central pattern generators in both conditions might result from a final common pathway involving the cingulum, as suggested by imaging studies [100, 101], which could also explain their similar semiology [102]. Occurrence of violence during sleep As outlined above, aggression is modulated by the temporal (i.e., the amygdala) and frontal lobes (i.e., the orbito-frontal and medial prefrontal areas) and lesions in these areas might lead to unwarranted aggressive behavior. Imaging studies have shown that during NREM sleep, there is hypoactivity of the prefrontal lobe that is particularly marked in the dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal regions, and less consistently in the associative cortices of the temporal and insular lobes [103]. This frontal, and in a lesser extent temporal, deactivation and its resultant impairment in judgment, purposeful behavior and inhibition of emotional responses might account for the propensity of sleep to generate violent behavior. Additionally, for violence to occur during sleep, features of both sleep and wakefulness must be present. If the frontal hypoactivity arises in the context of “mind sleep”, what is the origin of the “body wakefulness”? Insight comes from a study, in which SPECT imaging was performed during an episode of sleepwalking, and demonstrated hyperperfusion of the posterior cingular cortex and cerebellar vermis and decreased cerebral blood flow in the frontal and parietal association cortices [100]. It is thus conceivable that this “dissociation of states” results from the selective activation of thalamo-cingulo-pathways implicated in the control of complex motor and emotional behavior, and from hypoactivation of other thalamocortical pathways, including those projecting to the frontal lobes. In conclusion, reactive violence probably represents a genetically programmed innate behavior arising from central pattern generators located in the brainstem. Its occurrence is modulated by the prefrontal and temporal lobes. The association with sleep-related disorders is favored by hypoactivity of the prefrontal areas, and selective activation of pathways involved in complex emotional and motor behavior. Diagnostic procedures The first step in diagnosing a sleep disorder associated with violence is obtaining a complete history, if possible, from both the patient and the bed partner. Particular emphasis should be placed on identifying the key features that allow a distinction between the different sleep disorders that have been discussed earlier (see table 2). Information on sleeping and waking habits, comorbidities (particularly neurological and psychiatric conditions) and drug intake should also be obtained. Depending on the suspected diagnosis, the history might be complemented by clinical scales, such as the REM Sleep Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire [104] or the Nocturnal Frontal Lobe Epilepsy and Parasomnia (FLEP) Scale [39]. History should be followed by a general physical, neurological and psychiatric examination, and in case of suspected cerebral lesions, by imaging investigations of the brain and neuropsychological testing. Documentation of nocturnal episodes with home videos using a camera with infrared night vision function might be helpful, as various sleep disorders such as sleepwalking often fail to occur in a laboratory setting. An extensive polygraphic study with a multichannel scalp EEG, electromyographic monitoring of all four extremities and continuous audiovisual recording is however necessary for diagnosis. Video-EEG-Polysomnography is superior to standard polysomnography for the evaluation of parasomnias because of the increased capability to identify and localize EEG abnormalities and to correlate behavior with EEG and polysomnography [105]. Even if they fail to capture the event, long-term recordings are useful to identify typical or specific interictal EEG markers. Sleep architecture S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 328 Review article Table 2 Characteristics of disorders with potential for sleep related violence. Disorder State of occurrence Clinical features Normal occurrence of violence Confusional arousals Dissociation wake/NREM sleep Incomplete awakening, reduced vigilance, impaired cognition and amnesia for the event When being awakened from sleep Sleepwalking Dissociation wake/NREM sleep Similar to confusional arousals but with deambulation On an incidental encounter or when approached by another person Sleep terror Dissociation wake/NREM sleep Incomplete awakening from NREM-sleep with manifestations of fear Linked to a frightening dream image Rapid eye movement disorder Dissociation wake/REM sleep Acting out of dreams In relation to a dream that is being acted out Nocturnal paroxysmal dystonia Possible in all sleep stages, preferentially stage NREM2 Bipedal automatisms, twisting of trunk and pelvis, vocalizations, dystonic posturing of head/limbs Accidental or in relation to hyperkinetic features of seizures Epileptic nocturnal wandering Possible in all sleep stages, normally in stage NREM2 Similar to sleepwalking, more directed, violence possible Accidental or when approached or restrained by another person Confusional states Wake Variable Variable Psychiatric dissociative states Wake or wake/sleep transition Variable, most frequent manifestation is wandering, generally amnesia for the event Often automutilation, trashing movements, assaults Malingering Wake Variable; associated with primary or secondary gain Variable is essentially normal in patients with arousal disorders [106–108] but may show increased numbers of arousals or fragmentation of slow wave sleep, in particular in the first NREM-REM sleep episode [109, 110]. Hypersynchronous delta activity (HSD), consisting of bilateral rhythmic, deltawaves occurring for 10–20 seconds during slow wave sleep, is considered as a typical but non-specific EEG marker for arousal disorders. The low specificity of HSD is due to the fact that it also occurs in up to 66% of normal arousals [107, 108]. Documentation of epileptic potential is diagnostic and specific in patients with suspected nocturnal seizures. Unfortunately, certain areas of the frontal lobe (e.g., the medial and orbital cortex) are not accessible to surface EEG recordings, so that interictal epileptic potentials cannot be documented in up to 60% of cases [111, 112]. Even ictal recordings remain inconclusive in 20–40% of cases due to overlapping motor artifacts [113–115]. Polysomnographic recordings are also useful in identifying coexistent sleep disorders, which may act as trigger for parasomnias and require specific treatment (i.e., sleep apnea as a trigger for sleepwalking). Prior sleep deprivation has been shown to increase the diagnostic yield of these studies [16, 17]. Technicians are often of great help, as they can give information on possible precipitating factors of events (e.g., noise) and can interact with the patient after the event in order to assess postictal confusion or dream recall. Additional daytime EEG, preferentially after sleep deprivation, should be carried out in patients with suspected seizures. floor or in the basement, securing doors and windows, and removing potentially dangerous objects from the bedrooms. In disorders of arousal, any underlying trigger or precipitating factor should be treated or avoided, (i.e., coexisting disorders such as sleep apnea, but also touching or waking up the patient). The first choice of drug in this setting is clonazepam, which should be started at a dosage of 0.5 mg at bedtime, and progressively increased up to 2–3 mg. Other benzodiazepines (including triazolam, diazepam and flurazepam), antiepileptics (including carbamazepine, phenytoin and gabapentin) and antidepressants (including imipramine, trazodone and paroxetin) as well as melatonin have shown to be effective, although data relies on small case series or single cases [5]. Behavioral treatments such as hypnosis can also be effective [116]. In the case of RBD, clonazepam is usually very effective, with reported success rates between 87 and 90% [28, 117]. Although it is generally regarded as safe, this drug should be used with caution in elderly patients because of the risk of falling and developing a confusional state. It is also associated with a worsening of obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Alternatively, melatonin is an option, especially in patients with multiple comorbidities. Nocturnal seizures require specific antiepileptic treatment. In case of NFLE, carbamazepine is the treatment of choice. Forensic issues Treatment Treatment is aimed at the specific etiology. In all cases, the environment of the sleeping patient should be made safe. This can be accomplished by advising sleeping on the first When assessing criminal responsibility, it is necessary to establish whether relevant psychological dysfunctions were present at the time of the offense. There are, however, neither mental disorders nor psychological symptomatologies that mandatorily lead to a diminished criminal responsibility. S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 329 Review article Table 3 Criteria for establishing the role of an underlying sleep disorder in a violent act. Presence of an underlying sleep disorder – presence of solid evidence supporting the diagnosis – previous occurrence of similar episodes Characteristics of the act – occurs on awakening or immediately after falling asleep – abrupt onset and brief duration (lasting minutes) – impulsive, senseless, without apparent motivation – lack of awareness of individual during event – victim: coincidentally present, possible arousal stimulus On return of consciousness: – perplexity, horror, no attempt to escape – amnesia for event Presence of precipitating factors – attempts to awaken the subject – intake or alcohol or sedative/hypnotic drugs – prior sleep deprivation Existing symptoms and associated psychological dysfunctions must always be assessed in relation to the timing and characteristics of the offense. Possible deficits in psychological functions contrast with perceivable abilities in the areas of will-formation, decision-making, thought, perception, as well as steering and control of behavior. In this context, the spectrum of possible actions available to the offender, along with a precise behavioral analysis, is at the center of attention when assessing criminal responsibility. Here, behavior is examined using both intra-individual and interindividual comparison studies. In doing so, the entire life history of the offender, his personality and in particular the characteristics of the offence itself are taken into account [68]. Sleep-related automatisms that occur in the course of parasomnias or seizures usually qualify for diminished responsibility because the person is not conscious of his act, its consequences and of the fact that it is wrong. How -ever, applying this concept to a particular act can be problematic for a variety of reasons. Firstly, there are several degrees of consciousness, with numerous transitions between normal and pathological sleep [118], and establishing the exact level of consciousness retrospectively with regard to a particular act is sometimes impossible. Secondly, there is currently no diagnostic tool that enables the diagnosis of an underlying sleep disorder with absolute certainty, and even if there was one, the presence of a sleep disorder does not necessarily establish a causal link to the committed act. Criteria for establishing the putative role of an underlying sleep disorder in a specific violent act have been proposed by Mahowald and coworkers (table 3). They include [119]: 1) good evidence for an underlying sleep disorder, with similar episodes having occurred previously, 2) a brief duration of the act (lasting minutes), 3) that the behavior has an abrupt onset, is immediate, impulsive, senseless, without apparent motivation, inappropriate, out of character for the individual, and without evidence of premeditation 4) the almost coincidental presence of the victim, who may have acted as a stimulus for the arousal, 5) the behavior after the return of consciousness (perplexity, horror, no attempt to escape) and the lack of awareness on the part of the individual during the event 6) the presence of some degree of amnesia for the event 7) in the case of disorders of arousal, the timing of the act in relation to sleep (on awakening or immediately after falling asleep) and the presence of precipitating factors (attempts to awaken the subject, alcohol ingestion, sedative/ hypnotic administration, prior sleep deprivation). The presence of a sleep-related violent offense must be distinguished from a simulation. Along with a thorough analysis of the behavioral pattern, one must examine whether the offender possesses a personality profile relevant to risk, from which a plausible offense mechanism can be derived. As soon as an offense can plausibly be explained through the offender’s risk disposition, the assumption of a sleep-related violent offense becomes more unlikely. From a forensic-psychiatric perspective, a robust offense hypothesis must (1) reconcile all the important findings with as many aspects of the offense as possible and be able to explain them consistently (explanatory value), (2) be consistent within itself and plausible from a professional point of view, regarding forensic-psychiatric criteria (plausibility) and (3) be corroborated by the evidence at hand (e.g., the results of the preliminary investigation, witness accounts, findings of the psychiatric evaluation) in such a way that all relevant information has been considered and the hypothesis can be sustainably deduced from the available evidence (corroboration). Against this background, it is advisable, in case of sleep-related violent offenses, not to base an assessment of criminal responsibility solely on the medical findings of the offenders sleep patterns. Rather, an additional, extensive forensic-psychiatric evaluation is required. References 1 Gastaut H, Broughton R. A clinical and polygraphic study of episodic phenomena during sleep – the Sakel Lecture. Recent Adv Biol Psychiatry. 1965;7:197–221. 2 Yellowless D. Homicide by a somnambulist. J Ment Sci. 1878;24:451–8. 3 Cartwright R. Sleepwalking violence: a sleep disorder, a legal dilemma, and a psychological challenge. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1149–58. 4 Broughton RJ, Billings R, Cartwright R, Doucette D, Edmeads J, Edwardh M, et al. Homicidal sonnambulism: a case report. Sleep. 1994;17:253–64. 5 Bassetti CL. Sleepwalking (somnambulism). In: Laureys S, Tononi G (eds) The neurology of consciousness: cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology. Elsevier, 2009. 6 Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Violent parasomnias: forensic medicine issues. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC (eds) Principle and practice of sleep medicine. Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders; 2005; pp. 960–8. 7 Filley CM, Price BH, Nell V, Antoinette T, Morgan AS, Bresnahan JF, et al. Toward an understanding of violence: neurobehavioral aspects of unwarranted physical aggression: Aspen Neurobehavioral Conference consensus statement. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001;14:1–14. 8 Blair RJR. The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:198–208. 9 Siever LJ. Neurobiology of aggression and violence. Am J Psychiatry 2008;165:429–42. 10 Volavka J. The neurobiology of violence: an update. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;11:307–14. 11 Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Status dissociatus – a perspective on states of being. Sleep. 1991; 14:69–79. S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 330 Review article 12 American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International classification of sleep disorders: diagnostic and coding manual. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005. 13 Pressman MR. Factors that predispose, prime and precipitate NREM parasomnias in adults: clinical and forensic implications. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:5–30. 14 Kales A, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Ladda RL, Charney DS, Weber G, et al. Hereditary factors in sleepwalking and night terrors. Br J Psychiatry. 1980;137:111–8. 15 Lecendreux M, Bassetti C, Dauvilliers Y, Mayer G, Neidhart E, Tafti M. HLA and genetic susceptibility to sleepwalking. Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8:114–7. 16 Joncas S, Zadra A, Paquet J, Montplaisir J. The value of sleep deprivation as a diagnostic tool in adult sleepwalkers. Neurology. 2002;58:936–40. 17 Pilon M, Montplaisir J, Zadra A. Precipitating factors of somnambulism: impact of sleep deprivation and forced arousals. Neurology. 2008;70:2284–90. 18 Guilleminault C, Leger D, Philip P, Ohayon MM. Nocturnal wandering and violence: review of a sleep clinic population. J Forensic Sci. 1998;43:158–63. 19 Mahowald MW, Cramer-Bornemann MA. NREM sleep parasomnias. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement W (eds) Principles and practice of sleep medicine. Philadelphia: Elsevier/ Saunders; 2005; pp 889–96. 20 Pressman MR. Disorders of arousal from sleep and violent behavior: the role of physical contact and proximity. Sleep. 2007;30:1039–47. 21 Bonkalo A. Impulsive acts and confusional states during incomplete arousal from sleep: crinimological and forensic implications. Psychiatr Q. 1974;48:400–9. 22 Broughton R, Billings R, Cartwright R, Doucette D, Edmeads J, Edwardh M, et al. Homicidal somnambulism: a case report. Sleep. 1994;17:253–64. 23 Howard C, D’Orban PT. Violence in sleep: medico-legal issues and two case reports. Psychol Med. 1987;17:915–25. 24 Moldofsky H, Gilbert R, Lue FA, MacLean AW. Forensic sleep medicine: sleep-related violence. Sleep. 1995;18:731–9. 25 Guilleminault C, Moscovitch A, Leger D. Forensic sleep medicine: nocturnal wandering and violence. Sleep. 1995;18:740–8. 26 Schenck CH, Arnulf I, Mahowald M. Sleep and sex: what can go wrong? A review of the literature on sleep related disorders and abnormal sexual behaviors and experiences. Sleep. 2007;30:683–702. 27 Iranzo A, Molinuevo JL, Santamaria J, Serradell M, Marti MJ, Valldeoriola F, et al. Rapid-eye-movement sleep behaviour disorder as an early marker for a neurodegenerative disorder: a descriptive study. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:572–7. 28 Olson EJ, Bradley FB, Boeve BF, Silber MH. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: demographic, clinical and laboratory findings in 93 cases. Brain. 2000;123:331–9. 29 Comella CL, Nardine TM, Diederich NJ, Stebbins GT. Sleep-related violence, injury, and REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 1998;51:526–9. 30 Scaglione C, Vignatelll L, Plazzi G, Marchese R, Negrotti A, Rizzo G, et al. REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s disease: a questionnaire-based study. Neurol Sci. 2005; 25:316–21. 31 Dyken ME, Lin-Dyken DC, Seaba P, Yamada T. Violent sleep-related behavior leading to subdural hemorrhage. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:318–21. 32 Fantini ML, Corona A, Clerici S, Ferini-Strambi L. Aggressive dream content without daytime aggressiveness in REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology. 2005;65:1010–5. 33 Borek LL, Kohn RK, Friedman JH. Phenomenology of dreams in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2007;15:411–4. 34 De Cock VC, Vidailhet M, Leu S, Texeira A, Apartis E, Elbaz A, et al. Restoration of normal motor control in Parkinson’s disease during REM sleep. Brain. 2007;130:450–6. 35 Provini F, Plazzi G, Tinuper P, Vandi S, Lugaresi E, Montagna P. Nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy: clinical and polygraphic overview of 100 consecutive cases. Brain. 1999; 122:1017–31. 36 Derry C, Duncan JS, Berkovic SF. Paroxysmal motor disorders of sleep: the clinical spectrum and differentiation from epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2006;47:1775–91. 37 Nobili L. Nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy and non-rapid eye movement sleep parasomnias: differences and similarities. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:251–4. 38 Tinuper P, Provini F, Bisulli L, Vignatelli L, Plazzi G, Vertugno R, et al. Movement disorders in sleep: guidelines for differentiating epileptic from non-epileptic motor phenomena arising from sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:255–67. 39 Derry CP, Davey M, Johns M, Kron K, Glencross D, Marini C, et al. Distinguishing sleep disorders from seizures: diagnosing bumps in the night. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:705–9. 40 Manni R, Terzaghi M, Repetto A. The FLEP scale in diagnosing nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy, NREM and REM parasomnias: data from a tertiary sleep and epilepsy unit. Epilepsia. 2008;49:1581–5. 41 Nobili L, Cossu M, Mai R, Tassi L, Cardinale F, Castana L, et al. Sleep-related hyperkinetic seizures of temporal lobe origin. Neurology. 2004; 62:482–5. 42 Ryvlin P. Avoid falling into the depths of the insular trap. Epileptic Disord. 2006;8(Suppl 2):S37–56. 43 Nobili L, Francione S, Cardinale F, Lo Russo G. Epileptic nocturnal wanderings with a temporal lobe origin: a stereo-electroencephalographic study. Sleep. 2002;25: 669–71. 44 Schenck CH, Milner DM, Hurwitz TD, Bundlie SR, Mahowald MW. A polysomnographic and clinical report on sleep-related injury in 100 adult patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146:1166–73. 45 Wirrel EC. Epilepsy-related injuries. Epilepsia. 2006; 47:79–86. 46 Maselli RA, Rosenberg RS, Spire JP. Episodic nocturnal wanderings in non-epileptic young patients. Sleep. 1988;11:156–61. 47 Plazzi G, Tinuper P, Montagna P, Provini F, Lugaresi E. Epileptic nocturnal wanderings. Sleep. 1995;18:749–56. 48 Tassinari CA, Tassi L, Calandra-Buonaura G, StanzaniMaserati M, Fini N, Pizza F, et al. Biting behavior, aggression and seizures. Epilepsia. 2005;46:654–63. 49 Delgado-Escueta AV, Mattson RH, King L, Goldensohn ES, Spiegel H, Madsen J, et al. Special report. The nature of aggression during epileptic seizures. N Engl J Med. 1981;305:711–6. 50 Ramani V, Gumnit RJ. Intensive monitoring of epileptic patients with a history of episodic aggression. Arch Neurol. 1981;38:570–1. 51 Rodin EA. Psychomotor epilepsy and aggressive behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1973;28:210–3. 52 Marsh L, Krauss GL. Aggression and Violence in Patients with Epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2000;1:160–8. 53 Reuber M, Mackay RD. Epileptic automatisms in the criminal courts: 13 cases tried in England and Wales between 1975 and 2001. Epilepsia. 2008;49:138–45. 54 Marsh L, Krauss GL. Aggression and violence in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2000;1:160–8. 55 Kanemoto K, Kawasaki J, Mori E. Violence and epilepsy: a close relation between violence and postictal psychosis. Epilepsia. 1999; 40:107–9. 56 Devinsky O, Ronsaville D, Cox C, Witt E, Fedio P, Theodore WH. Interictal aggression in epilepsy: the BussDurkee Hostility Inventory. Epilepsia. 1994;35:585–90. 57 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington DC: American Psychiatry Association; 2000. 58 McCaldon RJ. Automatism. Can Med Assoc J. 1964; 91:914–20. S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 331 Review article 59 Schenck CH, Milner DM, Hurwitz TD, Bundlie SR, Mahowald MW. Dissociative disorders presenting as somnambulism. Polysomnographic, video and clinical documentation (8 cases). Dissociation. 1989;11:194–204. 60 Griffith JL, Slovik LS. Munchausen syndrome by proxy and sleep disorders medicine. Sleep. 1989;12:178–83. 61 Scarmeas N, Brandt J, Blacker D, Albert M, Hadjigeorgiou G, Dubois B, et al. Disruptive behavior as a predictor in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2007; 64:1755–61. 62 Roth G. Fühlen, Denken, Handeln. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp; 2003. 63 Markowitsch HJ. Warum wir keinen freien Willen haben. Der sogenannte freie Wille aus Sicht der Hirnforschung. Psychologische Rundschau. 2004;55:163–8. 64 Singer W. Über Bewusstsein und unsere Grenzen: Ein neurobiologischer Erklärungsversuch. In: Becker A, Mehr C, Nau HH (eds) Gene, Meme und Gehirne. Geist und Gesellschaft als Natur. Eine Debatte. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp; 2003; pp 279–305. 65 Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL. Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation – a possible prelude to violence. Science. 2000;289:591–4. 66 Blair RJ, Cipolotti L. Impaired social response reversal. A case of “acquired sociopathy”. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 6):1122–41. 67 Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1413–20. 68 Urbaniok F, Hardegger J, Rossegger A, Endrass J. Neurobiologischer Determinismus: Fragwürdige Schlussfolgerungen über menschliche Entscheidungsmöglichkeiten und forensische Schuldfähigkeit. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 2006;431–41. 69 Stanton B, Baldwin RM, Rachuba L. A quarter century of violence in the United States. An epidemiologic assessment. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1997; 20:269–82. 70 Allee WC, Collias NC, Lutherman CZ. Modification of the social order in flocks of hens by injection of testosterone propionate. Physiol Zool. 1939; 12:412–40. 71 Beeman AE. The effect of the interval between castration and first encounter to the aggressive behavior of mice. Anat Rec. 1947;99:570–1. 72 Archer J. The influence of testosterone on human aggression. Br J Psychol. 1991;82:1–28. 73 Hastings JE, Hamberger LK. Sociodemographic predictors of violence. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1997;20:323–35. 74 Raine A. Genetics and Crime. In: The psychopathology of crime. San Diego: Academic Press; pp 47–79, 1993. 75 Coccaro EF, Bergeman CS, Kavoussi RJ, Seroczynski AD. Heritability of aggression and irritability: a twin study of the Buss-Durkee aggression scales in adult male subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41:273–84. 76 Gelhorn H, Stallings M, Young S, Corley R, Rhee SH, Christian H, et al. Common and specific genetic influences on aggressive and non aggressive conduct disorder domains. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006; 45:570–7. 77 Seroczynski AD, Bergeman CS, Coccaro EF. Etiology of impulsivity/aggression relationship: genes or environment? Psychiatr Res. 1999;86:41–57. 78 Grove WM, Eckert ED, Heston L, Bouchard TJJ, Segal N, Lykken DT. Heritability of substance abuse and antisocial behavior: a study of monozygotic twins reared apart. Biol Psychiatry. 1990;27:1293–304. 79 Klüver H, Bucy PC. Preliminary analysis of the functions of temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch Neurol Psychiat. 1939; 42:979–1000. 80 Lilly R, Cummings JL, Benson JF, Frankel M. The human Klüver-Bucy syndrome. Neurology. 1983; 33:1141–5. 81 Tonkonogy JM. Violence and temporal lobe lesions: head CT and MRI data. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1991; 3:189–96. 82 Devinsky O, Bear D. Varieties of aggressive behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141:651–6. 83 Best M, Williams JM, Coccaro EF. Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:8448–53. 84 Dileo JF, Brewer WJ, Hopwood M, Anderson V, Creamer M. Olfactory identification dysfunction, aggression and impulsivity in war veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2008;38:523–31. 85 Grafman J, Schwab K, Warden D, Pridgen A, Brown HR, Salazar AM. Frontal lobe injuries, violence and aggression. Neurology. 1996; 46:1231–8. 86 Coccaro EF, McCloskey MS, Fitzgerald DA, Luan Phan K. Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 62:168–78. 87 Linnoila M, Virkkunen M, Scheinin M, Nuutila A, Rimon R, Goodwin FK. Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentrations differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Sci. 1983; 33:2609–14. 88 Walsh MT, Dinan TG. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and violence: a review of the available evidence. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2001;104:84–91. 89 Gerra G, Zaimovic A, Avanzinni P, Guicastro G, Caccavari R, Palladino M, et al. Neurotransmitter-neuroendocrine responses to experimentally induced aggression in humans: influence of personality variable. Psychiatr Res. 1997; 66: 33–43. 90 Greendyke RM, Kanter DR, Schuster dB, Verstreate S, Wotton J. Propanolol treatment of assaultive patients with organic brain disease. A double-blind, crossover, placebocontrolled study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1986; 174:290–4. 91 Cases O, Seif I, Gaspar P, Chen K, Pournin S, Müller U, et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science. 1995; 268:1763–6. 92 Gogos JA, Morgan MK, Luine V, Santha M, Ogawa R, Pfaff D, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95: 9991–6. 93 Meyer-Lindenberg A, Buckholtz JW, Kolachana B, A RH, Pezawas L, Blasi G, et al. Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:6269–74. 94 Lachman HM, Nolan NA, Mohr P, Saito T, Volavka J. Association between catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype and violence in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:835–7. 94 Soderstrom H, Blennow K, Manhem A, Forsman A. CSF studies in violent offenders. I. 5-HIAA as a negative and HVA as a positive predictor of psychopathy. J Neural Transm. 2001;108:869–78. 96 Ferrari PF, van Erp AM, Tornatzky W, Miczek KA. Accumbal dopamine and serotonin in anticipation of the next aggressive episode in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:371–8. 97 Siegel A, Pott CB. Neural substrates of aggression and flight in the cat. Prog Neurobiol. 1988;31:261–83. 98 Berntson GG, Micco DJ. Organization of brainstem behavioral systems. Brain Res Bull. 1976;1:471–83. 99 Grillner S, Dubuc R. Control of locomotion in vertebrates: spinal and supraspinal mechanisms. Adv Neurol. 1988; 47:425–53. 100 Bassetti C, Vella S, Donati F, Wielepp P, Weder B. SPECT during sleepwalking. Lancet. 2000; 356:484–5. 101 Schindler K, Gast H, Bassetti C, Wiest R, Fritschi J, Meyer K, et al. Hyperperfusion of anterior cingulate gyrus in a case of paroxysmal nocturnal dystonia. Neurology. 2001; 57:917–20. 102 Tassinari CA, Rubboli G, Gardella E, Cantalupo G, CalandraBuonaura G, Vedovello M, et al. Central pattern generators S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 332 Review article 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 for a common semiology in fronto-limbic seizures and in parasomnias. A neuroethologic approach. Neurol Sci. 2005;26(Suppl 3):s225–32. Dang-Vu T, Desseilles M, Petit D, Mazza S, Montplaisir J, Maquet P. Neuroimaging in sleep medicine. Sleep Med. 2007;8:349–72. Stiasny-Kolster K, Mayer G, Schafer S, Moller JC, HeinzelGutenbrunner M, Oertel WH. The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire--a new diagnostic instrument. Mov Disord. 2007;22:2386–93. Aldrich MS, Jahnke B. Diagnostic value of video-EEG polysomnography. Neurology. 1991; 41:1060–6. Schenck CH, Pareja JA, Patterson AL, Mahowald MW. Analysis of polysomnographic events surrounding 252 slow-wave sleep arousals in thirty-eight adults with injurious sleepwalking and sleep terrors. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1998; 15:159–66. Zadra A, Pilon M, Joncas S, Rompre S, Montplaisir J. Analysis of postarousal EEG activity during somnambulistic episodes. J Sleep Res. 2004; 13:279–84. Pressman MR. Hypersynchronous delta sleep EEG activity and sudden arousals from slow-wave sleep in adults without a history of parasomnias: clinical and forensic implications. Sleep. 2004; 27:706–10. Guilleminault C, Poyares D, Aftab FA, Palombini L. Sleep and wakefulness in somnambulism: a spectral analysis study. J Psychosom Res. 2001; 51:411–6. Espa F, Ondze B, Deglise P, Billiard M, Besset A. Sleep architecture, slow wave activity, and sleep spindles in adult patients with sleepwalking and sleep terrors. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:929–39. 111 Bautista RE, Spencer DD, Spencer SS. EEG findings in frontal lobe epilepsies. Neurology. 1998;50:1765–71. 112 Kellinghaus C, Luders HO. Frontal lobe epilepsy. Epileptic Disord. 2004;6:223–39. 113 Laskowitz DT, Sperling MR, French JA, O’Connor MJ. The syndrome of frontal lobe epilepsy: characteristics and surgical management. Neurology. 1995; 45:780–7. 114 Williamson PD, Spencer SS. Clinical and EEG features of complex partial seizures of extratemporal origin. Epilepsia. 1986; 27(Suppl 2):S46–63. 115 Morris HH, 3rd, Dinner DS, Luders H, Wyllie E, Kramer R. Supplementary motor seizures: clinical and electroencephalographic findings. Neurology. 1988;38:1075–82. 116 Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Non-rapid eye movement sleep parasomnias. Neurol Clin. 2005; 23:1077–106, vii. 117 Schenck CH, Hurwitz TD, Mahowald MW. Symposium: Normal and abnormal REM sleep regulation: REM sleep behaviour disorder: an update on a series of 96 patients and a review of the world literature. J Sleep Res. 1993; 2:224–31. 118 Broughton RJ, Shimizu T. Sleep-related violence: a medical and forensic challenge. Sleep. 1995;18:727–30. 119 Mahowald MW, Bundlie SR, Hurwitz TD, Schenck CH. Sleep violence – forensic science implications: polygraphic and video documentation. J Forensic Sci. 1990;35:413–32. S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33 www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch 333