Review article
Violence in sleep
Francesca Siclaria, Ramin Khatamia, c, Frank Urbaniokd, Claudio L. Bassettia, b
Depar tment of Neurology, University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland
Neurocenter of Southern Switzerland
c
Barmelweid Sleep Clinic, Switzerland
d
Psychiatric-Psychological Ser vice, Zürich Justice Depar tment, Switzerland
a
b
There is no actual or potential conflict of interest in relation to this ar ticle.
Summary
Sleep-related violence occurs in 2% of the adult population and can result
from various conditions, including parasomnias (such as arousal disorders
and rapid eye movement behavior disorder), epilepsy (in particular nocturnal
frontal lobe epilepsy) and psychiatric diseases (including delirium, dissociative states and factitious disorders). Its occurrence is probably favored by
hypoactivity or dysfunction of the prefrontal cerebral areas, and selective
activation of pathways involved in complex emotional and motor behavior.
The present review outlines the different sleep disorders associated with
violence, and aims to provide information on diagnosis, therapy and forensic issues. It also discusses current physiopathological models, establishing a
link between sleep-related violence and violence observed in other settings.
Key words: sleep; parasomnias; violence; epilepsy
Introduction
Although generally considered as mutually exclusive, sleep
and violence can coexist. What appears to be one of the first
of such reports dates back to medieval times and relates to
a Silesian woodcutter, who after a few hours of sleep woke
up abruptly, aimed his axe at an imaginary intruder and
killed his wife instead (cited by Gastaut and Broughton [1]).
Another early case was reported by Yellowless in 1878 [2]
and describes a young man with a history of sleep terrors
who killed his 18-month old son by smashing him against
the wall during the night, taking him for a wild beast that
was about to attack his family. Nowadays, dramatic reports
of somnambulistic homicide still gain considerable attention
in the media [3, 4].
The most widely known condition in this respect is
probably sleepwalking, but numerous other disorders with
a potential for sleep-related violence exist, including other
parasomnias, epilepsy and psychiatric diseases. Violence in
the course of these disorders occurs when boundaries defining wakefulness and sleep disrupt, resulting in a dissociation
between “mind sleep” and “body sleep” [5].
Neurologists and sleep specialists should be familiar with
these conditions, not least because sleep-related violence is
more frequent than generally assumed, occurring in up to
2% of the adult population [6]. Important advances in the
fields of genetics, neuroimaging and behavioral neurology
have expanded the understanding of the mechanisms underlying violence and its particular relation to sleep. Along with
this increasing knowledge, sleep specialists assume a growing
role in legal issues related to violent acts committed during
sleep. Lastly, most sleep disorders associated with violence
are treatable; making the correct diagnosis thus constitutes
the first step in the prevention of further violence.
The present review outlines the different sleep disorders
associated with violence, and aims at providing information
on diagnosis, therapy and forensic issues. It also discusses
current physiopathological models, establishing a link between sleep-related violence and violence observed in other
settings.
Definitions
For the purpose of this review, violence is defined as an aggressive act that inflicts unwarranted physical harm on others
[7]. It is a subset of aggression, a broader term encompassing
both mental and physical damage. Unlike violence, aggression may be considered legitimate in certain circumstances.
In the light of current physiopathological models, two types
of aggression can be distinguished [8–10]. Premeditated aggression (also referred to as instrumental, predatory or proactive
aggression) is purposeful and goal-directed. It commonly
occurs in psychopaths and is thought to result from of a
failure of moral socialization, involving the mechanisms of
aversive conditioning and instrumental learning. Premeditated aggression may develop as a lack of formative learning experience, or because the underlying neuro-cognitive
architecture is dysfunctional [8]. Impulsive aggression (also
termed affective, reactive or hostile aggression) constitutes
a response to a frustrating or threatening event that induces
anger, and occurs without regard for any potential goal.
Impulsive aggression has a particular relevance with regard
to sleep-related violence.
Disorders underlying sleep-related violence
Correspondence:
Prof. Dr. med. Claudio L. Bassetti
Neurocenter of Southern Switzerland
Neurology Department, University of Zürich
Via Tesserete 46
CH-6903 Lugano
claudio.bassetti©eoc.ch
Parasomnias
On the basis of electrophysiological variables (eye movements, muscle tone, EEG activity), one can distinguish three
different states of vigilance, namely wakefulness, rapid eye
movement (REM) sleep and Non REM (NREM) sleep. In
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
322
Review article
some circumstances however, boundaries between these
states disrupt, resulting in conditions that share features
of more than one state. Examples of such a state dissociation include, amongst others, disorders of arousal (dissociation between NREM sleep and wakefulness), rapideye-movement behavior disorder, lucid dreaming, peduncular hallucinosis, cataplexy, sleep-related hallucinations
and sleep paralysis (dissociation between REM sleep and
wakefulness). An extreme form of dissociation, referred
to as “status dissociatus”, with the simultaneous presence of features of each one of the three states, has also
been reported [11]. It is in the context of state dissociation, when motor activity becomes dissociated from wakefulness, that sleep-related violence occurs in the course of
parasomnias.
Disorders of arousal
Disorders of arousal encompass sleepwalking, confusional
arousals and sleep terrors. They consist of an incomplete
awakening from NREM sleep characterized by reduced vigilance, impaired cognition, retrograde amnesia for the event
and variable motor activity, ranging from repetitive and
purposeless movements to more complex behaviors such
as eating, drinking, driving, sexual intercourse and aggression [12].
Disorders of arousal are common during childhood (with
a prevalence of 10% for sleepwalking, 17% for confusional
arousals and 1–6.5% for sleep terrors), but may persist or
arise de novo during adulthood in 2–4% of cases [12]. It is
generally assumed that they result from the interplay between predisposing, priming and precipitating factors [13].
Predisposition is based on genetic susceptibility, which in the
case of sleepwalking, is suggested by the 10-fold increased
prevalence among first degree relatives of sleepwalkers [14].
The genes that confer the risk of sleepwalking remain essentially unknown. The only established marker is the presence
of the HLA DQ1B allele found in 35% of sleepwalkers, compared to only 13% of normal subjects [15]. Priming factors
act by either increasing slow wave sleep or by heightening
the threshold for arousal during NREM sleep and include the
acute effect of alcohol, fever, stress, a large variety of psychotropic medications, reviewed elsewhere [13], and probably
sleep deprivation, although studies yield conflicting results in
this respect [16–18]. Among precipitating factors, sleep disordered breathing, periodic leg movements as well as noise
and touch have been identified [13].
Confusional arousals consist of mental confusion or confusional behavior upon awaking from sleep, most often from
slow wave sleep in the first part of the night. If an individual
leaves the bed and starts walking in the course of a confusional arousal, the disorder is referred to as sleepwalking. In
sleep terrors, the arousal is characterized by intense autonomic
activation and typical behavioral features such as sitting up
in bed and screaming. Different arousal disorders may coexist in the same individual, and, not infrequently, an episode
may start as one arousal disorder and evolve into another
(i.e., sleep terror evolving into sleepwalking). They normally
occur in the first half of the night and tend not to recur during the same night.
Disorders of arousal have a potential for sleep related
violence. Homicide, attempted homicide, filicide, suicide and
inappropriate sexual behaviors have been reported in this
setting [19]. Pressman [20] reviewed 32 legal and medical
case reports of violence associated with disorders of arousal,
and found that aggressive behavior occurred in different
ways in confusional arousals, sleepwalking and sleep terrors. In confusional arousals, violence was usually elicited
when individuals were awakened from sleep by someone
else. Bonkalo [21], for instance, reported the case of a night
shift supervisor who fell asleep in the office, was awakened
by an employee and pulled the gun in confusion, killing the
employee. Violent behavior during sleepwalking, in contrast,
tended to occur when the sleepwalking episode was already
underway, and the individual was approached by another
person or incidentally encountered someone else. As an
example, Broughton [22] examined the case of a patient who
during a presumed episode of somnambulism, completed
a 15–20 minute drive to his parents in-law’s house, beat
his father-in-law unconscious and killed his mother-in-law
with a knife from her own kitchen. Violence related to sleep
terrors appears to be a reaction to a concrete, frightening
image that the individual can subsequently describe. In this
context, Howard and D’Orban [23] reported the case of a
man who fell asleep next to his friend, and upon awakening,
found this friend severely injured. He described a frightening image of three figures trying to attack him, and recalled
hitting and punching them.
In the same series of cases, Pressman examined the role
of physical contact and proximity as triggering factors of
sleep-related violence, and found that 100% of confusional
arousals, 81% of sleep terrors and 40–90% of sleepwalking cases were associated with provocations including noise,
touch and/or close proximity.
Two studies tried to further identify risk factors for the
occurrence of violence in disorders of arousal. Moldofsky and
colleagues [24] who recruited 64 consecutive patients with
sleepwalking behaviour or sleep terrors, found that serious
violent behavior directed towards other people occurred
more frequently in men, and was significantly associated
with more stressors, excessive caffeinated beverages, drug
abuse and less stage four sleep. Guilleminault and coworkers
[25] retrospectively reviewed a series of 41 adult individuals
with nocturnal wandering of different etiologies. Compared
to the non-violent individuals, the 29 violent patients were
predominantly male (65% versus 42%), and comprised of
the only two subjects with temporal lobe epilepsy.
Among the complex behaviors that can be observed
during disorders of arousal, sleep-related sexual activity
is frequently associated with violence. Sexual behaviors
during sleep (also termed sleepsex or sexsomnia) encompass
fondling another person, sexual intercourse, masturbation,
and sexual vocalizations. In over 90% of cases, sleepsex
occurs during confusional arousals; the remainder of cases
are related to sleepwalking, REM behavior disorder, seizures,
Kleine-Levin Syndrome, severe chronic insomnia, restless
leg syndrome, narcolepsy and psychiatric diseases [26].
Among the 31 patients with parasomnia-related sleepsex
reviewed by Schenck and coworkers [26], 45% displayed
assaultive behavior, 29% had sex with minors, and 36%
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
323
Review article
Figure 1
82-year-old man with a 2-year history of symmetrical extrapyramidal symptoms and cognitive dysfunction, attributed to vascular
leucencephalopathy. According to his wife he displayed frequent vocalizations and violent behavior during sleep. Polysomnographic
recordings show loss of normal muscle atonia during REM-sleep and increased phasic muscle activity as well as acting out of dreams
with kicking (Fig. a), hitting (Fig. b) and menacing gestures. Based on these features the diagnosis of rapid eye movement disorder
was established.
sustained legal consequences from their sexual sleep-related
behavior. More than half of the patients induced physical
harm to others, and 6% to themselves. Men were clearly
overrepresented (80%) and were the only ones to initiate
sexual intercourse, whereas women preferentially engaged
in masturbation or displayed sexual vocalizations. None of
the patients recalled their sexual behavior the next morning. Of the seven patients with seizure-related sleepsex, only
one patient displayed assaultive behavior, and two patients
injured themselves, while none had sex with minors, injured
others or sustained legal consequences. Males and females
were equally distributed in this group, and only one third
were amnestic for sleepsex.
REM sleep Behavior disorder
REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD) is characterized by a loss
of normal muscle atonia and an increase of phasic muscle
activity during REM sleep, and is associated with altered
dream content and acting out of dreams. It is disruptive
and causes injury to the patient himself or the bed partner.
RBD is more prevalent in men and after the age of 50. In
45% of cases, it represents the first manifestation of a
neurodegenerative disorder including Parkinson’s disease,
multisystem atrophy and dementia with Lewy bodies [27].
Sleep related injuries to the patient himself or to the bed
partner have been reported in 32–69% of cases [28–30] and
often lead the patient to seek medical advice. They generally occur when the patient hits the furniture or walls, or
falls out of bed. Violent acts can be complex, such as firing
an unloaded gun, or setting fire to the bed, and can result
in serious injuries, including subdural hematomas and bone
fractures [28, 29, 31]. Attempted assault of sleep partners
has been reported to occur in 64% of cases, with injuries
in 3% [28]. Unlike violence related to disorders of arousal,
in RBD the individual is readily oriented upon awakening
and can generally recall vivid dream imagery related to the
violent act.
Of note, dreams by patients with idiopathic and secondary RBD have a more aggressive content compared to dreams
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
324
Review article
Table 1
Distinguishing features between nocturnal seizures in nocturnal
frontal lobe epilepsy (NFLE) and arousal disorders.
Features
Disorders of arousal
NFLE
Age of onset
Childhood
Childhood or adolescence
Persistence into adulthood Rarely
Frequent
Motor features
Variable, not highly stereoHighly stereotyped, often
typed, no dystonic posturing hyperkinetic
Amnesia for event
Generally present
Generally present
Postictal confusion
Frequent
Generally absent
Duration
Generally >30 seconds
Seconds to 3 minutes
Same-night recurrence
Low
High
Timing
First third of the night
Any time of the night, often
in clusters
Ictal EEG
Slow waves
Clear-cut epileptiform
discharges in <10%
of normal subjects, despite lower levels of daytime aggressiveness [32]. Likewise, among patients with Parkinson’s
disease, those affected by RBD have more aggressive dreams
compared to patients without RBD, irrespective of gender
[33]. A recent study has shown that in RBD associated with
Parkinson’s disease, motor activity may be vigorous and fast
compared to the hypokinesia observed during wakefulness
[34]. This restored motor control during REM-sleep (or a
dissociated state between REM sleep and wakefulness), in
association with loss of REM sleep atonia and the actionloaded dreams probably accounts for the injury potential of
this disorder. For a practical example see figure 1.
Epilepsy
In nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy (NFLE), seizures can occur
exclusively during sleep and be associated with violence.
There are three different forms of NFLE that frequently coexist in a single patient. Paroxysmal arousals consist of abrupt
arousals from sleep with stereotyped motor phenomena,
including head movements, frightened expressions and
dystonic limb posturing. They are short, lasting less than
20 seconds, and tend to recur frequently during the night.
Episodes of so called nocturnal paroxysmal dystonia also begin with a sudden arousal but involve more complex motor
activity such as bipedal automatisms, rhythmic movements
of the trunk and limbs, as well as tonic and dystonic posturing. They generally last less than two minutes. Epileptic
nocturnal wanderings also start with an abrupt arousal, proceed through the stage of paroxysmal dystonia, and eventually culminate in deambulation. This condition generally
lasts less than three minutes. The clinical manifestations of
seizures are often very similar to the motor activity observed
during disorders of arousal, making a distinction between the
two conditions difficult. In addition, in approximately half
the patients with NFLE, EEG fails to show ictal or interictal
abnormalities [35]. However, establishing the correct diagnosis is crucial, as these entities differ in terms of therapy.
Distinguishing features between nocturnal seizures and disorders of arousal have been reviewed by Derry [36], Nobili
[37] and Tinuper [38] and are summarized in table 1. A
clinical scale has been developed with the aim to distinguish
between these conditions [39]; although some items seem
to limit its sensitivity [40]. Nocturnal hypermotor seizures
[41, 42], including epileptic nocturnal wanderings [43], can
occasionally originate from the temporal lobe.
Injury resulting from nocturnal seizures can be accidental
and related to hyperkinetic features [44] (figure 2), although
compared to seizures during wakefulness, the injury potential of seizures occurring solely during sleep is probably
lower, as the bed represents a relatively safe environment
[45]. Alternatively, injury might result from aggressive, and
more directed behavior as documented during episodic nocturnal wandering [25, 44, 46, 47].
More generally, violent behavior can be related to the
ictal, peri-ictal and interictal period. Ictal aggression is exceptional [48, 49–54]. It can take the form of biting, grasping,
hitting, threatening, screaming, facial expressions of anger,
pushing, shoving, and spitting [48, 52]. In these circumstances the violent act is not directed towards others and does
not involve intricate skills or purposeful movements. However, more directed violence can occur as a reaction to stimuli
of the patient’s environment and can for instance be elicited
by the act of restraining the patient [48, 49]. Seizure-related
violent behavior is more common in men [48, 49, 51, 54]
and is associated with bilateral amygdala/hippocampal and/
or ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction [48]. Peri-ictal
aggression occurs in the pre-ictal, or more commonly in the
postictal period, in a setting of confusion and abnormal mood
(depression, psychosis or delirium). Again, behavior is recurrent, out of character and stereotyped for a given patient, and
is frequently associated with amnesia. It may also occur in a
setting of postictal psychosis that typically follows a seizure
after a lucid interval of hours to days. Compared to postictal confusion, postictal psychosis bears a greater potential
for well-directed violent behavior [55]. Interictal violence
is generally related to cognitive or behavioral disturbances
secondary to a brain dysfunction that may also underlie epileptogenesis [56].
Nocturnal dissociative disorders
Dissociative disorders are defined as a disruption of the usually integrated functions of consciousness, memory, identity,
or perception of the environment [57]. They occur without the conscious awareness on the part of the individual,
are sometimes associated with violence [58] and can arise
exclusively or predominantly from the sleep period [59]. In
contrast to parasomnias of NREM or REM sleep, dissociative
disorders occur during well established EEG wakefulness,
either at the transition from wakefulness to sleep or within
several minutes after awakening from stages 1 or 2 of NREM
or from REM sleep [12]. Schenck and coworkers [59] determined that in 5% of patients referred for sleep-related injury,
nocturnal dissociative disorder was the cause of the injuries.
Patients were predominantly female (87%), had a history of
sexual abuse or posttraumatic stress disorder and additional
dissociative episodes during daytime (87%). Violent behavior
reported in this setting included breaking windows, shredding clothes, thrashing movements, approaching a person
with a knife and automutilation. One patient exclusively
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
325
Review article
Figure 2
36-year-old woman with nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy since the age of 13. Seizures occurred up to 10 times a night and were characterized by an abrupt awakening from sleep with a feeling of chest tightness and dyspnea, and progressed to tonic posturing of the limbs
and hyperkinetic features, including elevation and extension of the left arm (Fig. b) and pedaling (Fig. a and c). The patient was fully
aware during the seizure but was unable to speak. She had sustained several injuries including fractures of her fingers when hitting
objects during her seizures. The arrow on top of the polysomnographic recording shows the beginning of a seizure arising from slow
wave sleep. Intracranial EEG recordings indicated that seizures originated from a cortical dysplasia in the left frontal cingulum.
displayed nocturnal animalistic behavior (quadrupedalism,
growling, hissing) linked to a dream of being a jungle tiger.
Patients were either amnestic for the episodes or recalled a
dream which matched the documented behavior.
Factitious disorder
In factitious disorder (also named Münchhausen syndrome),
an individual presents with an illness that is deliberately
produced or falsified for the purpose of assuming the sick
role. In a special form, factitious syndrome by proxy (or
Münchhausen syndrome by proxy), disease is produced or
feigned in another person [57]. Both disorders may occur
during the sleep period and can be associated with violence.
Griffith and coworkers [60] published a case of apparent
nocturnal apneas, diarrhea and vomiting in a 2-month old
child. Repeated polysomnographic recordings were normal.
Eventually, during a hospital stay, the mother was seen administrating an enema (first rectally and then orally) to the
child, and the diagnosis of Münchhausen syndrome by proxy
was established.
Malingering
In contrast to factitious disorder, malingering is not a mental illness, and intentionally produced symptoms or signs
are motivated by external incentives for the behavior, (i.e.,
economic gain, avoiding legal responsibility, improving
physical well-being) [57]. Again, symptoms can be referred
to the sleep period, but when observed, always occur during
well-established EEG-wakefulness.
Sundowning
In patients with dementia, confusion and wandering frequently increase during late afternoon and evening and
improve during the day (“sundowning”). This phenomenon
occurs in approximately half of the patients with Alzheimer’s
disease, has been shown to predict cognitive decline and can
be associated with disruptive behavior [61].
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
326
Review article
Physiopathology
Anatomic substrates for violence
Time and again, attempts have been made to develop biological explanatory models for violent and sexual offenses.
With the availability of modern imaging techniques, neurobiological phenomena have shifted more and more towards
the center of attention. In this context, various researchers
have supported the theory that offenses, in general, result
from neurobiological deficits [62–64]. Particularly for violent
behavior, emphasis has been placed on prefrontal lesions
or disorders of emotional processing [65–67]. Following
the neurobiological causality theory of criminal behavior,
the authors came to the conclusion that criminal behavior
is – in principle – neurobiologically determined and that the
entire individual responsibility is therefore detracted from
the offender [62–64]. However, it has been demonstrated
that both the theory of neurobiological causality and the
theory of neurobiological determinism can be traced back to
several methodical misunderstandings and cannot be perpetuated in this form [68].
It is important to consider that there are very diverse
forms and mechanisms of violent and sexual offenses, which
differ in regard to development and etiology. In order to
demonstrate relationships between individual subgroups, it
would be necessary to examine a multitude of variables for
very large and homogeneous populations, as – from experience – numerous interdependencies exist, especially in the
areas of forensic research. Such studies are not available.
Generally, for most forms of violent behavior, specific personality traits are important. These can be displayed in the
form of prognostic syndromes; as risk dispositions individual
to a personality. In the sense of relative determinism, these
risk dispositions are associated with a certain probability for
specific forms of violent behavior. The question arises, however, as to whether any of the biological parameters actually
play an etiological role or whether they are simply more
likely the result of a risk-relevant personality trait. Sleeprelated violence is distinctly different from any other form
of violent and sexual delinquency, in as much that biological
and physiopathological mechanisms are of particular importance. For this reason, these parameters will be described
in the following section, despite their limited explanatory
power in general.
Among the anatomic substrates of violence, the prefrontal
and temporal lobes play a major role. Involvement of the
anterior temporal lobe in aggressive behavior was initially
suggested by Klüver and Bucy [79], who showed that bilateral amygdala ablation in monkeys resulted in placidity; an
observation that was later described in humans with bilateral
anterior temporal lesions [80]. Another study identified an
increased occurrence of lesions in the anterior inferior temporal lobe in violent patients with organic mental syndrome
compared to non violent patients of the same group [81].
As discussed above, aggressive behavior may be a feature of
temporolimbic epilepsy [82].
Within the prefrontal cortex, the medial prefrontal and
orbitofrontal regions seem to be selectively implicated in the
control of aggressive behavior. Dysfunction in these areas is
indicated by impaired emotional recognition of faces, errors
in odour identification and disadvantageous decisions in
gambling tests, all of which have been observed in patients
with impulsive aggressive disorder; while working memory,
an indicator of dorsolateral prefrontal dysfunction, was comparable to control subjects [83]. Olfactory dysfunction as a
measure of orbitofrontal dysfunction has also been shown to
correlate with impulsivity and physical aggression in patients
with PTSD [84]. Further evidence for the involvement of
these areas is suggested by the finding that head injuries in
Vietnam veterans were associated with violence if localized
to the frontal ventromedial or orbitofrontal regions [85].
Likewise, individuals with intermittent explosive disorder exhibit exaggerated amygdala activity and diminished
activation of the orbitofrontal cortex in response to faces
expressing anger, and fail to demonstrate coupling between
these two structures [86].
According to functional models [8], the amygdala and
the orbitofrontal lobe act on subcortical systems mediating reactive aggression. While the role of the amygdala is
to up- or downregulate their responsiveness to threat, the
orbitofrontal cortex exerts its modulating activity in response
to social clues, including frustration, expressions of anger,
staring and social disapproval.
Neurotransmitters
Testosterone and genetics
Epidemiological studies indicate that male gender is the most
consistent risk factor for violence [69], suggesting the involvement of testosterone as a causal factor. While a direct
link between aggressive behavior and testosterone has been
demonstrated in animals [70, 71], this link is very weak
and inconsistent in humans [72]. Other variables associated
with violence include unemployment, alcohol abuse, a lower
educational level and access to firearms [69, 73]. A genetic
background of violence is suggested by the higher concordance of violent behavior in monozygotic twins compared
to dizygotic twins [74–77], and this has been confirmed by
studies evaluating twins reared apart [78].
Despite a number of candidate molecules that have been
identified with regard to violent behavior, the most consistent evidence focuses on serotonin and catecholamines.
While serotonin seems to have an inhibitory effect on impulsive aggression, catecholamines are thought to act as a facilitating factor [10]. Impulsive violent offenders, for instance,
have been shown to have lower cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of serotonin metabolites compared to non impulsive
violent offenders [87], and these findings were later supported by other measures of serotonin function [10]. Additionally, fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor,
is associated with an improvement in anger and aggression,
both towards oneself and others [88]. Another study showed
that induced hostile behavior was associated with an increase in basal norepinephrine levels in a group of violent
individuals [89]. Moreover, reductions in assaultive behavior
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
327
Review article
have been observed after the administration of propanolol, a
non selective beta-blocker antagonizing the action of epinephrine, in a double blind, placebo controlled crossover study
in patients with organic brain disease [90]. Elevated levels of
aggression have been shown in mice lacking monoamineoxidase A and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) [91,
92], two enzymes involved in degradation of norepinephrine
and dopamine. In humans, polymorphisms in the MAO-A
gene seem to be associated with violent behavior and linked
to changes in brain structure and function. A low expression of MAO-A variant has been shown to increase the risk
of violent behavior and to be associated with volume reductions in the limbic system and hyperresponsiveness of
the amygdala during emotional arousal. In the same study,
regulatory prefrontal areas showed diminished reactivity
compared to the high expression allele group [93]. Likewise,
violence seems to be associated with low COMT activity in
violent schizophrenic patients [94]. Additional evidence for
the role of dopamine in violent behavior comes from a study
that found increased levels of homovanillic acid, a dopamine
metabolite, in the CSF of violent offenders [95]. Another
study documented a rise in dopamine efflux and a decrease
of serotonin efflux in the nucleus accumbens in rats when
anticipating an aggressive confrontation [96].
Automatisms arising from central pattern generators
In the aforementioned parasomnias and epileptic disorders, highly complex motor activity occurs in the absence of
awareness, suggesting a deactivation of cortical structures
crucial for judgment and consciousness. In fact, a series of
complex behavior sequences can be generated by brainstem
structures alone, without the involvement of cortical regions
[97–99]. These brainstem centers are called central pattern
generators and control stereotyped innate motor behavior
necessary for survival, such as locomotion, swimming, sexual activity and other rhythmic motor sequences. Aggressive
behavior probably represents an innate action pattern aimed
at the defense of the peripersonal space [48].
Activation of central pattern generators probably underlies a series of behaviors observed in the course of parasomnias (such as sleepsex, nocturnal eating disorder, bruxism,
sleepwalking and sleep-related violence) and seizures (ictal
biting, grasping, oro-mandibular automatisms, locomotor
activity, facial expressions of fear, and vocalizations). The
involvement of central pattern generators in both conditions
might result from a final common pathway involving the
cingulum, as suggested by imaging studies [100, 101], which
could also explain their similar semiology [102].
Occurrence of violence during sleep
As outlined above, aggression is modulated by the temporal
(i.e., the amygdala) and frontal lobes (i.e., the orbito-frontal
and medial prefrontal areas) and lesions in these areas might
lead to unwarranted aggressive behavior. Imaging studies
have shown that during NREM sleep, there is hypoactivity of the prefrontal lobe that is particularly marked in the
dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal regions, and less consistently in the associative cortices of the temporal and insular
lobes [103]. This frontal, and in a lesser extent temporal,
deactivation and its resultant impairment in judgment,
purposeful behavior and inhibition of emotional responses
might account for the propensity of sleep to generate violent
behavior.
Additionally, for violence to occur during sleep, features
of both sleep and wakefulness must be present. If the frontal
hypoactivity arises in the context of “mind sleep”, what is
the origin of the “body wakefulness”? Insight comes from
a study, in which SPECT imaging was performed during an
episode of sleepwalking, and demonstrated hyperperfusion
of the posterior cingular cortex and cerebellar vermis and
decreased cerebral blood flow in the frontal and parietal
association cortices [100]. It is thus conceivable that this
“dissociation of states” results from the selective activation
of thalamo-cingulo-pathways implicated in the control of
complex motor and emotional behavior, and from hypoactivation of other thalamocortical pathways, including
those projecting to the frontal lobes.
In conclusion, reactive violence probably represents a
genetically programmed innate behavior arising from central
pattern generators located in the brainstem. Its occurrence is
modulated by the prefrontal and temporal lobes. The association with sleep-related disorders is favored by hypoactivity
of the prefrontal areas, and selective activation of pathways
involved in complex emotional and motor behavior.
Diagnostic procedures
The first step in diagnosing a sleep disorder associated with
violence is obtaining a complete history, if possible, from
both the patient and the bed partner. Particular emphasis
should be placed on identifying the key features that allow
a distinction between the different sleep disorders that have
been discussed earlier (see table 2). Information on sleeping
and waking habits, comorbidities (particularly neurological
and psychiatric conditions) and drug intake should also be
obtained. Depending on the suspected diagnosis, the history
might be complemented by clinical scales, such as the REM
Sleep Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire [104] or
the Nocturnal Frontal Lobe Epilepsy and Parasomnia (FLEP)
Scale [39]. History should be followed by a general physical, neurological and psychiatric examination, and in case
of suspected cerebral lesions, by imaging investigations of
the brain and neuropsychological testing. Documentation of
nocturnal episodes with home videos using a camera with
infrared night vision function might be helpful, as various
sleep disorders such as sleepwalking often fail to occur in a
laboratory setting.
An extensive polygraphic study with a multichannel
scalp EEG, electromyographic monitoring of all four extremities and continuous audiovisual recording is however
necessary for diagnosis. Video-EEG-Polysomnography is
superior to standard polysomnography for the evaluation of
parasomnias because of the increased capability to identify
and localize EEG abnormalities and to correlate behavior
with EEG and polysomnography [105]. Even if they fail to
capture the event, long-term recordings are useful to identify
typical or specific interictal EEG markers. Sleep architecture
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
328
Review article
Table 2
Characteristics of disorders with potential for sleep related violence.
Disorder
State of occurrence
Clinical features
Normal occurrence of violence
Confusional arousals
Dissociation wake/NREM sleep
Incomplete awakening, reduced
vigilance, impaired cognition and
amnesia for the event
When being awakened from sleep
Sleepwalking
Dissociation wake/NREM sleep
Similar to confusional arousals
but with deambulation
On an incidental encounter
or when approached by another
person
Sleep terror
Dissociation wake/NREM sleep
Incomplete awakening from
NREM-sleep with manifestations
of fear
Linked to a frightening dream
image
Rapid eye movement disorder
Dissociation wake/REM sleep
Acting out of dreams
In relation to a dream that is
being acted out
Nocturnal paroxysmal dystonia
Possible in all sleep stages,
preferentially stage NREM2
Bipedal automatisms, twisting
of trunk and pelvis, vocalizations,
dystonic posturing of head/limbs
Accidental or in relation to
hyperkinetic features of seizures
Epileptic nocturnal wandering
Possible in all sleep stages,
normally in stage NREM2
Similar to sleepwalking, more
directed, violence possible
Accidental or when approached
or restrained by another person
Confusional states
Wake
Variable
Variable
Psychiatric dissociative states
Wake or wake/sleep transition
Variable, most frequent manifestation is wandering, generally
amnesia for the event
Often automutilation, trashing
movements, assaults
Malingering
Wake
Variable; associated with primary
or secondary gain
Variable
is essentially normal in patients with arousal disorders
[106–108] but may show increased numbers of arousals or
fragmentation of slow wave sleep, in particular in the first
NREM-REM sleep episode [109, 110]. Hypersynchronous
delta activity (HSD), consisting of bilateral rhythmic, deltawaves occurring for 10–20 seconds during slow wave sleep,
is considered as a typical but non-specific EEG marker for
arousal disorders. The low specificity of HSD is due to the fact
that it also occurs in up to 66% of normal arousals [107, 108].
Documentation of epileptic potential is diagnostic and specific in patients with suspected nocturnal seizures. Unfortunately, certain areas of the frontal lobe (e.g., the medial and
orbital cortex) are not accessible to surface EEG recordings,
so that interictal epileptic potentials cannot be documented in up to 60% of cases [111, 112]. Even ictal recordings
remain inconclusive in 20–40% of cases due to overlapping
motor artifacts [113–115]. Polysomnographic recordings are
also useful in identifying coexistent sleep disorders, which
may act as trigger for parasomnias and require specific treatment (i.e., sleep apnea as a trigger for sleepwalking). Prior
sleep deprivation has been shown to increase the diagnostic
yield of these studies [16, 17]. Technicians are often of great
help, as they can give information on possible precipitating
factors of events (e.g., noise) and can interact with the patient after the event in order to assess postictal confusion or
dream recall. Additional daytime EEG, preferentially after
sleep deprivation, should be carried out in patients with
suspected seizures.
floor or in the basement, securing doors and windows, and
removing potentially dangerous objects from the bedrooms.
In disorders of arousal, any underlying trigger or precipitating factor should be treated or avoided, (i.e., coexisting
disorders such as sleep apnea, but also touching or waking up the patient). The first choice of drug in this setting
is clonazepam, which should be started at a dosage of
0.5 mg at bedtime, and progressively increased up to 2–3
mg. Other benzodiazepines (including triazolam, diazepam
and flurazepam), antiepileptics (including carbamazepine,
phenytoin and gabapentin) and antidepressants (including
imipramine, trazodone and paroxetin) as well as melatonin
have shown to be effective, although data relies on small
case series or single cases [5]. Behavioral treatments such as
hypnosis can also be effective [116].
In the case of RBD, clonazepam is usually very effective,
with reported success rates between 87 and 90% [28, 117].
Although it is generally regarded as safe, this drug should
be used with caution in elderly patients because of the risk
of falling and developing a confusional state. It is also associated with a worsening of obstructive sleep apnea syndrome.
Alternatively, melatonin is an option, especially in patients
with multiple comorbidities.
Nocturnal seizures require specific antiepileptic treatment. In case of NFLE, carbamazepine is the treatment of
choice.
Forensic issues
Treatment
Treatment is aimed at the specific etiology. In all cases, the
environment of the sleeping patient should be made safe.
This can be accomplished by advising sleeping on the first
When assessing criminal responsibility, it is necessary to
establish whether relevant psychological dysfunctions were
present at the time of the offense. There are, however, neither mental disorders nor psychological symptomatologies
that mandatorily lead to a diminished criminal responsibility.
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
329
Review article
Table 3
Criteria for establishing the role of an underlying sleep disorder
in a violent act.
Presence of an underlying sleep disorder
– presence of solid evidence supporting the diagnosis
– previous occurrence of similar episodes
Characteristics of the act
– occurs on awakening or immediately after falling asleep
– abrupt onset and brief duration (lasting minutes)
– impulsive, senseless, without apparent motivation
– lack of awareness of individual during event
– victim: coincidentally present, possible arousal stimulus
On return of consciousness:
– perplexity, horror, no attempt to escape
– amnesia for event
Presence of precipitating factors
– attempts to awaken the subject
– intake or alcohol or sedative/hypnotic drugs
– prior sleep deprivation
Existing symptoms and associated psychological dysfunctions must always be assessed in relation to the timing and
characteristics of the offense. Possible deficits in psychological functions contrast with perceivable abilities in the areas
of will-formation, decision-making, thought, perception, as
well as steering and control of behavior. In this context,
the spectrum of possible actions available to the offender,
along with a precise behavioral analysis, is at the center
of attention when assessing criminal responsibility. Here,
behavior is examined using both intra-individual and interindividual comparison studies. In doing so, the entire life
history of the offender, his personality and in particular the
characteristics of the offence itself are taken into account
[68]. Sleep-related automatisms that occur in the course
of parasomnias or seizures usually qualify for diminished
responsibility because the person is not conscious of his
act, its consequences and of the fact that it is wrong. How
-ever, applying this concept to a particular act can be
problematic for a variety of reasons. Firstly, there are several degrees of consciousness, with numerous transitions
between normal and pathological sleep [118], and establishing the exact level of consciousness retrospectively with
regard to a particular act is sometimes impossible. Secondly,
there is currently no diagnostic tool that enables the diagnosis of an underlying sleep disorder with absolute certainty,
and even if there was one, the presence of a sleep disorder
does not necessarily establish a causal link to the committed act.
Criteria for establishing the putative role of an underlying
sleep disorder in a specific violent act have been proposed by
Mahowald and coworkers (table 3). They include [119]: 1)
good evidence for an underlying sleep disorder, with similar
episodes having occurred previously, 2) a brief duration of
the act (lasting minutes), 3) that the behavior has an abrupt
onset, is immediate, impulsive, senseless, without apparent
motivation, inappropriate, out of character for the individual, and without evidence of premeditation 4) the almost
coincidental presence of the victim, who may have acted as
a stimulus for the arousal, 5) the behavior after the return
of consciousness (perplexity, horror, no attempt to escape)
and the lack of awareness on the part of the individual during the event 6) the presence of some degree of amnesia for
the event 7) in the case of disorders of arousal, the timing
of the act in relation to sleep (on awakening or immediately
after falling asleep) and the presence of precipitating factors
(attempts to awaken the subject, alcohol ingestion, sedative/
hypnotic administration, prior sleep deprivation).
The presence of a sleep-related violent offense must be
distinguished from a simulation. Along with a thorough
analysis of the behavioral pattern, one must examine whether the offender possesses a personality profile relevant
to risk, from which a plausible offense mechanism can be
derived. As soon as an offense can plausibly be explained
through the offender’s risk disposition, the assumption of a
sleep-related violent offense becomes more unlikely. From a
forensic-psychiatric perspective, a robust offense hypothesis
must (1) reconcile all the important findings with as many
aspects of the offense as possible and be able to explain them
consistently (explanatory value), (2) be consistent within
itself and plausible from a professional point of view, regarding forensic-psychiatric criteria (plausibility) and (3) be
corroborated by the evidence at hand (e.g., the results of
the preliminary investigation, witness accounts, findings of
the psychiatric evaluation) in such a way that all relevant
information has been considered and the hypothesis can be
sustainably deduced from the available evidence (corroboration). Against this background, it is advisable, in case of
sleep-related violent offenses, not to base an assessment of
criminal responsibility solely on the medical findings of the
offenders sleep patterns. Rather, an additional, extensive
forensic-psychiatric evaluation is required.
References
1 Gastaut H, Broughton R. A clinical and polygraphic study
of episodic phenomena during sleep – the Sakel Lecture.
Recent Adv Biol Psychiatry. 1965;7:197–221.
2 Yellowless D. Homicide by a somnambulist.
J Ment Sci. 1878;24:451–8.
3 Cartwright R. Sleepwalking violence: a sleep disorder,
a legal dilemma, and a psychological challenge.
Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1149–58.
4 Broughton RJ, Billings R, Cartwright R, Doucette D,
Edmeads J, Edwardh M, et al. Homicidal sonnambulism:
a case report. Sleep. 1994;17:253–64.
5 Bassetti CL. Sleepwalking (somnambulism). In: Laureys S,
Tononi G (eds) The neurology of consciousness: cognitive
neuroscience and neuropathology. Elsevier, 2009.
6 Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Violent parasomnias:
forensic medicine issues. In: Kryger MH, Roth T,
Dement WC (eds) Principle and practice of sleep medicine.
Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders; 2005; pp. 960–8.
7 Filley CM, Price BH, Nell V, Antoinette T, Morgan AS,
Bresnahan JF, et al. Toward an understanding of violence:
neurobehavioral aspects of unwarranted physical
aggression: Aspen Neurobehavioral Conference consensus
statement. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol.
2001;14:1–14.
8 Blair RJR. The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:198–208.
9 Siever LJ. Neurobiology of aggression and violence.
Am J Psychiatry 2008;165:429–42.
10 Volavka J. The neurobiology of violence: an update.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;11:307–14.
11 Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Status dissociatus –
a perspective on states of being. Sleep. 1991; 14:69–79.
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
330
Review article
12 American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International
classification of sleep disorders: diagnostic and coding
manual. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep
Medicine; 2005.
13 Pressman MR. Factors that predispose, prime and precipitate NREM parasomnias in adults: clinical and forensic
implications. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:5–30.
14 Kales A, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Ladda RL, Charney DS,
Weber G, et al. Hereditary factors in sleepwalking and night
terrors. Br J Psychiatry. 1980;137:111–8.
15 Lecendreux M, Bassetti C, Dauvilliers Y, Mayer G,
Neidhart E, Tafti M. HLA and genetic susceptibility to sleepwalking. Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8:114–7.
16 Joncas S, Zadra A, Paquet J, Montplaisir J. The value of
sleep deprivation as a diagnostic tool in adult sleepwalkers.
Neurology. 2002;58:936–40.
17 Pilon M, Montplaisir J, Zadra A. Precipitating factors
of somnambulism: impact of sleep deprivation and forced
arousals. Neurology. 2008;70:2284–90.
18 Guilleminault C, Leger D, Philip P, Ohayon MM. Nocturnal
wandering and violence: review of a sleep clinic population.
J Forensic Sci. 1998;43:158–63.
19 Mahowald MW, Cramer-Bornemann MA. NREM sleep
parasomnias. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement W (eds) Principles and practice of sleep medicine. Philadelphia: Elsevier/
Saunders; 2005; pp 889–96.
20 Pressman MR. Disorders of arousal from sleep and violent
behavior: the role of physical contact and proximity.
Sleep. 2007;30:1039–47.
21 Bonkalo A. Impulsive acts and confusional states during
incomplete arousal from sleep: crinimological and forensic
implications. Psychiatr Q. 1974;48:400–9.
22 Broughton R, Billings R, Cartwright R, Doucette D,
Edmeads J, Edwardh M, et al. Homicidal somnambulism:
a case report. Sleep. 1994;17:253–64.
23 Howard C, D’Orban PT. Violence in sleep: medico-legal issues
and two case reports. Psychol Med. 1987;17:915–25.
24 Moldofsky H, Gilbert R, Lue FA, MacLean AW. Forensic sleep
medicine: sleep-related violence. Sleep. 1995;18:731–9.
25 Guilleminault C, Moscovitch A, Leger D. Forensic sleep medicine: nocturnal wandering and violence.
Sleep. 1995;18:740–8.
26 Schenck CH, Arnulf I, Mahowald M. Sleep and sex: what
can go wrong? A review of the literature on sleep related
disorders and abnormal sexual behaviors and experiences.
Sleep. 2007;30:683–702.
27 Iranzo A, Molinuevo JL, Santamaria J, Serradell M, Marti MJ,
Valldeoriola F, et al. Rapid-eye-movement sleep behaviour
disorder as an early marker for a neurodegenerative
disorder: a descriptive study. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:572–7.
28 Olson EJ, Bradley FB, Boeve BF, Silber MH. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: demographic, clinical and
laboratory findings in 93 cases. Brain. 2000;123:331–9.
29 Comella CL, Nardine TM, Diederich NJ, Stebbins GT.
Sleep-related violence, injury, and REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 1998;51:526–9.
30 Scaglione C, Vignatelll L, Plazzi G, Marchese R, Negrotti A,
Rizzo G, et al. REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s
disease: a questionnaire-based study.
Neurol Sci. 2005; 25:316–21.
31 Dyken ME, Lin-Dyken DC, Seaba P, Yamada T. Violent
sleep-related behavior leading to subdural hemorrhage.
Arch Neurol. 1995;52:318–21.
32 Fantini ML, Corona A, Clerici S, Ferini-Strambi L. Aggressive dream content without daytime aggressiveness in REM
sleep behavior disorder. Neurology. 2005;65:1010–5.
33 Borek LL, Kohn RK, Friedman JH. Phenomenology of dreams
in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2007;15:411–4.
34 De Cock VC, Vidailhet M, Leu S, Texeira A, Apartis E, Elbaz A,
et al. Restoration of normal motor control in Parkinson’s
disease during REM sleep. Brain. 2007;130:450–6.
35 Provini F, Plazzi G, Tinuper P, Vandi S, Lugaresi E,
Montagna P. Nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy: clinical and
polygraphic overview of 100 consecutive cases.
Brain. 1999; 122:1017–31.
36 Derry C, Duncan JS, Berkovic SF. Paroxysmal motor
disorders of sleep: the clinical spectrum and differentiation
from epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2006;47:1775–91.
37 Nobili L. Nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy and non-rapid eye
movement sleep parasomnias: differences and similarities.
Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:251–4.
38 Tinuper P, Provini F, Bisulli L, Vignatelli L, Plazzi G,
Vertugno R, et al. Movement disorders in sleep: guidelines
for differentiating epileptic from non-epileptic motor phenomena arising from sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:255–67.
39 Derry CP, Davey M, Johns M, Kron K, Glencross D, Marini C,
et al. Distinguishing sleep disorders from seizures: diagnosing bumps in the night. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:705–9.
40 Manni R, Terzaghi M, Repetto A. The FLEP scale in diagnosing nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy, NREM and REM
parasomnias: data from a tertiary sleep and epilepsy unit.
Epilepsia. 2008;49:1581–5.
41 Nobili L, Cossu M, Mai R, Tassi L, Cardinale F, Castana L,
et al. Sleep-related hyperkinetic seizures of temporal lobe
origin. Neurology. 2004; 62:482–5.
42 Ryvlin P. Avoid falling into the depths of the insular trap.
Epileptic Disord. 2006;8(Suppl 2):S37–56.
43 Nobili L, Francione S, Cardinale F, Lo Russo G.
Epileptic nocturnal wanderings with a temporal lobe origin:
a stereo-electroencephalographic study. Sleep. 2002;25:
669–71.
44 Schenck CH, Milner DM, Hurwitz TD, Bundlie SR,
Mahowald MW. A polysomnographic and clinical report on
sleep-related injury in 100 adult patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146:1166–73.
45 Wirrel EC. Epilepsy-related injuries.
Epilepsia. 2006; 47:79–86.
46 Maselli RA, Rosenberg RS, Spire JP. Episodic nocturnal
wanderings in non-epileptic young patients.
Sleep. 1988;11:156–61.
47 Plazzi G, Tinuper P, Montagna P, Provini F, Lugaresi E.
Epileptic nocturnal wanderings. Sleep. 1995;18:749–56.
48 Tassinari CA, Tassi L, Calandra-Buonaura G, StanzaniMaserati M, Fini N, Pizza F, et al. Biting behavior, aggression
and seizures. Epilepsia. 2005;46:654–63.
49 Delgado-Escueta AV, Mattson RH, King L, Goldensohn ES,
Spiegel H, Madsen J, et al. Special report.
The nature of aggression during epileptic seizures.
N Engl J Med. 1981;305:711–6.
50 Ramani V, Gumnit RJ. Intensive monitoring of epileptic
patients with a history of episodic aggression.
Arch Neurol. 1981;38:570–1.
51 Rodin EA. Psychomotor epilepsy and aggressive behavior.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1973;28:210–3.
52 Marsh L, Krauss GL. Aggression and Violence in Patients
with Epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2000;1:160–8.
53 Reuber M, Mackay RD. Epileptic automatisms in the criminal
courts: 13 cases tried in England and Wales between 1975
and 2001. Epilepsia. 2008;49:138–45.
54 Marsh L, Krauss GL. Aggression and violence in patients
with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2000;1:160–8.
55 Kanemoto K, Kawasaki J, Mori E. Violence and epilepsy:
a close relation between violence and postictal psychosis.
Epilepsia. 1999; 40:107–9.
56 Devinsky O, Ronsaville D, Cox C, Witt E, Fedio P,
Theodore WH. Interictal aggression in epilepsy: the BussDurkee Hostility Inventory. Epilepsia. 1994;35:585–90.
57 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington DC: American
Psychiatry Association; 2000.
58 McCaldon RJ. Automatism. Can Med Assoc J. 1964;
91:914–20.
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
331
Review article
59 Schenck CH, Milner DM, Hurwitz TD, Bundlie SR,
Mahowald MW. Dissociative disorders presenting as
somnambulism. Polysomnographic, video and clinical documentation (8 cases). Dissociation. 1989;11:194–204.
60 Griffith JL, Slovik LS. Munchausen syndrome by proxy and
sleep disorders medicine. Sleep. 1989;12:178–83.
61 Scarmeas N, Brandt J, Blacker D, Albert M, Hadjigeorgiou G,
Dubois B, et al. Disruptive behavior as a predictor
in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2007; 64:1755–61.
62 Roth G. Fühlen, Denken, Handeln. Frankfurt am Main:
Suhrkamp; 2003.
63 Markowitsch HJ. Warum wir keinen freien Willen haben.
Der sogenannte freie Wille aus Sicht der Hirnforschung.
Psychologische Rundschau. 2004;55:163–8.
64 Singer W. Über Bewusstsein und unsere Grenzen: Ein neurobiologischer Erklärungsversuch. In: Becker A, Mehr C,
Nau HH (eds) Gene, Meme und Gehirne. Geist und
Gesellschaft als Natur. Eine Debatte. Frankfurt am Main:
Suhrkamp; 2003; pp 279–305.
65 Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL. Dysfunction in the
neural circuitry of emotion regulation – a possible prelude
to violence. Science. 2000;289:591–4.
66 Blair RJ, Cipolotti L. Impaired social response reversal. A case
of “acquired sociopathy”. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 6):1122–41.
67 Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the
possible functions of the prefrontal cortex.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1413–20.
68 Urbaniok F, Hardegger J, Rossegger A, Endrass J.
Neurobiologischer Determinismus: Fragwürdige Schlussfolgerungen über menschliche Entscheidungsmöglichkeiten
und forensische Schuldfähigkeit. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr.
2006;431–41.
69 Stanton B, Baldwin RM, Rachuba L. A quarter century of
violence in the United States. An epidemiologic assessment.
Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1997; 20:269–82.
70 Allee WC, Collias NC, Lutherman CZ. Modification of the
social order in flocks of hens by injection of testosterone
propionate. Physiol Zool. 1939; 12:412–40.
71 Beeman AE. The effect of the interval between castration
and first encounter to the aggressive behavior of mice.
Anat Rec. 1947;99:570–1.
72 Archer J. The influence of testosterone on human aggression.
Br J Psychol. 1991;82:1–28.
73 Hastings JE, Hamberger LK. Sociodemographic predictors
of violence. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1997;20:323–35.
74 Raine A. Genetics and Crime. In: The psychopathology
of crime. San Diego: Academic Press; pp 47–79, 1993.
75 Coccaro EF, Bergeman CS, Kavoussi RJ, Seroczynski AD.
Heritability of aggression and irritability: a twin study of
the Buss-Durkee aggression scales in adult male subjects.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41:273–84.
76 Gelhorn H, Stallings M, Young S, Corley R, Rhee SH,
Christian H, et al. Common and specific genetic influences
on aggressive and non aggressive conduct disorder domains.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006; 45:570–7.
77 Seroczynski AD, Bergeman CS, Coccaro EF. Etiology of
impulsivity/aggression relationship: genes or environment?
Psychiatr Res. 1999;86:41–57.
78 Grove WM, Eckert ED, Heston L, Bouchard TJJ, Segal N,
Lykken DT. Heritability of substance abuse and antisocial
behavior: a study of monozygotic twins reared apart.
Biol Psychiatry. 1990;27:1293–304.
79 Klüver H, Bucy PC. Preliminary analysis of the functions
of temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch Neurol Psychiat. 1939;
42:979–1000.
80 Lilly R, Cummings JL, Benson JF, Frankel M. The human
Klüver-Bucy syndrome. Neurology. 1983; 33:1141–5.
81 Tonkonogy JM. Violence and temporal lobe lesions:
head CT and MRI data. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci.
1991; 3:189–96.
82 Devinsky O, Bear D. Varieties of aggressive behavior in
temporal lobe epilepsy. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141:651–6.
83 Best M, Williams JM, Coccaro EF. Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:8448–53.
84 Dileo JF, Brewer WJ, Hopwood M, Anderson V, Creamer M.
Olfactory identification dysfunction, aggression and impulsivity in war veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder.
Psychol Med. 2008;38:523–31.
85 Grafman J, Schwab K, Warden D, Pridgen A, Brown HR,
Salazar AM. Frontal lobe injuries, violence and aggression.
Neurology. 1996; 46:1231–8.
86 Coccaro EF, McCloskey MS, Fitzgerald DA, Luan Phan K.
Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat
in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry.
2007; 62:168–78.
87 Linnoila M, Virkkunen M, Scheinin M, Nuutila A, Rimon R,
Goodwin FK. Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic
acid concentrations differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Sci. 1983; 33:2609–14.
88 Walsh MT, Dinan TG. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
and violence: a review of the available evidence.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2001;104:84–91.
89 Gerra G, Zaimovic A, Avanzinni P, Guicastro G, Caccavari
R, Palladino M, et al. Neurotransmitter-neuroendocrine
responses to experimentally induced aggression in humans:
influence of personality variable. Psychiatr Res. 1997; 66:
33–43.
90 Greendyke RM, Kanter DR, Schuster dB, Verstreate S,
Wotton J. Propanolol treatment of assaultive patients with
organic brain disease. A double-blind, crossover, placebocontrolled study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1986; 174:290–4.
91 Cases O, Seif I, Gaspar P, Chen K, Pournin S, Müller U,
et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain
serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA.
Science. 1995; 268:1763–6.
92 Gogos JA, Morgan MK, Luine V, Santha M, Ogawa R,
Pfaff D, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient
mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine
levels and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:
9991–6.
93 Meyer-Lindenberg A, Buckholtz JW, Kolachana B, A RH,
Pezawas L, Blasi G, et al. Neural mechanisms of genetic
risk for impulsivity and violence in humans.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:6269–74.
94 Lachman HM, Nolan NA, Mohr P, Saito T, Volavka J.
Association between catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype
and violence in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:835–7.
94 Soderstrom H, Blennow K, Manhem A, Forsman A.
CSF studies in violent offenders. I. 5-HIAA as a negative
and HVA as a positive predictor of psychopathy.
J Neural Transm. 2001;108:869–78.
96 Ferrari PF, van Erp AM, Tornatzky W, Miczek KA. Accumbal
dopamine and serotonin in anticipation of the next aggressive episode in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:371–8.
97 Siegel A, Pott CB. Neural substrates of aggression and flight
in the cat. Prog Neurobiol. 1988;31:261–83.
98 Berntson GG, Micco DJ. Organization of brainstem
behavioral systems. Brain Res Bull. 1976;1:471–83.
99 Grillner S, Dubuc R. Control of locomotion in vertebrates:
spinal and supraspinal mechanisms. Adv Neurol. 1988;
47:425–53.
100 Bassetti C, Vella S, Donati F, Wielepp P, Weder B.
SPECT during sleepwalking. Lancet. 2000; 356:484–5.
101 Schindler K, Gast H, Bassetti C, Wiest R, Fritschi J, Meyer K,
et al. Hyperperfusion of anterior cingulate gyrus in a case of
paroxysmal nocturnal dystonia. Neurology. 2001; 57:917–20.
102 Tassinari CA, Rubboli G, Gardella E, Cantalupo G, CalandraBuonaura G, Vedovello M, et al. Central pattern generators
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
332
Review article
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
for a common semiology in fronto-limbic seizures and in
parasomnias. A neuroethologic approach.
Neurol Sci. 2005;26(Suppl 3):s225–32.
Dang-Vu T, Desseilles M, Petit D, Mazza S, Montplaisir J,
Maquet P. Neuroimaging in sleep medicine.
Sleep Med. 2007;8:349–72.
Stiasny-Kolster K, Mayer G, Schafer S, Moller JC, HeinzelGutenbrunner M, Oertel WH. The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire--a new diagnostic instrument.
Mov Disord. 2007;22:2386–93.
Aldrich MS, Jahnke B. Diagnostic value of video-EEG polysomnography. Neurology. 1991; 41:1060–6.
Schenck CH, Pareja JA, Patterson AL, Mahowald MW.
Analysis of polysomnographic events surrounding 252
slow-wave sleep arousals in thirty-eight adults with injurious
sleepwalking and sleep terrors. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;
15:159–66.
Zadra A, Pilon M, Joncas S, Rompre S, Montplaisir J.
Analysis of postarousal EEG activity during somnambulistic
episodes. J Sleep Res. 2004; 13:279–84.
Pressman MR. Hypersynchronous delta sleep EEG activity
and sudden arousals from slow-wave sleep in adults without
a history of parasomnias: clinical and forensic implications.
Sleep. 2004; 27:706–10.
Guilleminault C, Poyares D, Aftab FA, Palombini L. Sleep and
wakefulness in somnambulism: a spectral analysis study.
J Psychosom Res. 2001; 51:411–6.
Espa F, Ondze B, Deglise P, Billiard M, Besset A.
Sleep architecture, slow wave activity, and sleep spindles
in adult patients with sleepwalking and sleep terrors.
Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:929–39.
111 Bautista RE, Spencer DD, Spencer SS. EEG findings in
frontal lobe epilepsies. Neurology. 1998;50:1765–71.
112 Kellinghaus C, Luders HO. Frontal lobe epilepsy.
Epileptic Disord. 2004;6:223–39.
113 Laskowitz DT, Sperling MR, French JA, O’Connor MJ.
The syndrome of frontal lobe epilepsy: characteristics and
surgical management. Neurology. 1995; 45:780–7.
114 Williamson PD, Spencer SS. Clinical and EEG features of
complex partial seizures of extratemporal origin. Epilepsia.
1986; 27(Suppl 2):S46–63.
115 Morris HH, 3rd, Dinner DS, Luders H, Wyllie E, Kramer R.
Supplementary motor seizures: clinical and electroencephalographic findings. Neurology. 1988;38:1075–82.
116 Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Non-rapid eye movement sleep
parasomnias. Neurol Clin. 2005; 23:1077–106, vii.
117 Schenck CH, Hurwitz TD, Mahowald MW. Symposium: Normal and abnormal REM sleep regulation: REM sleep behaviour disorder: an update on a series of 96 patients and a
review of the world literature. J Sleep Res. 1993; 2:224–31.
118 Broughton RJ, Shimizu T. Sleep-related violence: a medical
and forensic challenge. Sleep. 1995;18:727–30.
119 Mahowald MW, Bundlie SR, Hurwitz TD, Schenck CH. Sleep
violence – forensic science implications: polygraphic and
video documentation. J Forensic Sci. 1990;35:413–32.
S C H W E I Z E R A R C H I V F Ü R N E U R O L O G I E U N D P S Y C H I A T R I E 2009;160(8):322–33
www.sanp.ch | www.asnp.ch
333