AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
Annual Review of Anthropology
Deaf Anthropology
Michele Friedner1 and Annelies Kusters2
1
Department of Comparative Human Development, University of Chicago, Chicago,
Illinois 60637, USA; email: michelefriedner@uchicago.edu
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
2
Department of Languages and Intercultural Studies, School of Social Sciences, Heriot-Watt
University, Edinburgh EH14 4AS, Scotland, United Kingdom; email: a.kusters@hw.ac.uk
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020. 49:31–47
Keywords
The Annual Review of Anthropology is online at
anthro.annualreviews.org
deaf, Deaf, sociality, mobility, space, modalities, technology
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-010220034545
Abstract
Copyright © 2020 by Annual Reviews.
All rights reserved
·.•�,
Deaf anthropology is a field that exists in conversation with but is not reducible to the interdisciplinary field of deaf studies. Deaf anthropology is
predicated upon a commitment to understanding deafnesses across time and
space while holding on to “deaf” as a category that does something socially,
politically, morally, and methodologically. In doing so, deaf anthropology
moves beyond compartmentalizing the body, the senses, and disciplinary
boundaries. We analyze the close relationship between anthropology writ
large and deaf studies: Deaf studies scholars have found analytics and categories from anthropology, such as the concept of culture, to be productive in analyzing deaf peoples’ experiences and the sociocultural meanings
of deafness. As we note, however, scholarship on deaf peoples’ experiences is
increasingly variegated. This review is arranged into four overlapping sections titled Socialities and Similitudes; Mobilities, Spaces, and Networks;
Modalities and the Sensorium; and Technologies and Futures.
31
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
INTRODUCTION: WHAT IS DEAF ANTHROPOLOGY?
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
In this Annual Review article, we identify an emerging field of deaf anthropology that exists in
conversation with but is not reducible to the interdisciplinary field of deaf studies.1 Deaf anthropology, parallel to disability anthropology (Ginsburg & Rapp 2013, Hartblay 2020),2 calls on anthropologists to explore what it means to see, hear, listen, communicate, and inhabit the world
through differential sensory configurations. Drawing from Hartblay’s (2020) argument about disability anthropology’s orientations, we believe that deaf anthropology “is work that engages the
distinctive theoretical concerns and methodological approaches of transdisciplinary [deaf] studies, enacted through a citational politics that foregrounds [deaf] studies texts and scholars.” Deaf
anthropology is predicated on a commitment to understanding “deafnesses” (Mills 2015) across
time and space while holding on to “deaf” as a category that does something socially, politically,
morally, and methodologically. In doing so, deaf anthropology moves beyond compartmentalizing
the body, the senses, and disciplinary boundaries (Burch & Kafer 2010, Friedner & Block 2017,
Friedner & Helmreich 2012).
In the first Annual Review of Anthropology article devoted to deafness, Senghas & Monaghan
(2002, p. 69) succinctly wrote, “Deafness is not merely the absence of hearing[,]” and they argued
for disassociating deaf peoples’ experiences from being seen (solely) through a lens of lack and
deficiency. Further complicating the category, Mills (2015) argued for a “deaf spectrum” or for
deafnesses in the plural to give nuance to a diversity of deaf experiences that are made up of different cultural, social, and (bio)medical components. We consider it important to track the many
meanings and articulations of “deaf,” “Deaf,” “deafness,” and “deafnesses” as concepts in different
fields. Even within the discipline of anthropology, medical anthropologists and linguistic anthropologists often approach deafness with varying methodological and theoretical lenses. We also
note that “deafness” has been rejected in favor of “deaf” by many deaf studies scholars because
of its association with medicalized/deficit perspectives on deafness. Avoiding a debate about suffixes, we focus on “deaf” and not “deafness.” We also draw from Woodward & Horejes (2016),
who argued that a sharp split between deafness as impairment (deaf ) and deafness as identity and
linguistic category (Deaf ) was not the original intention in making a distinction between deaf and
Deaf; we thus follow increasing common conventions and do not capitalize “deaf.”
There have historically been productive overlaps between anthropology and deaf studies, an
international and transdisciplinary field with roots in the emergence of sign language linguistics
in the 1950s and 1960s in the United States and European countries (Murray 2017). Deaf studies
scholars have found analytics and categories from anthropology productive in analyzing deaf peoples’ experiences and the sociocultural meanings of deafness. Indeed, deaf studies’ foundational
concept “Deaf culture” (Padden 1980) derived from an engagement with one of anthropology’s
most important contributions and has been instrumental in carving out deaf peoples’ often described “sense of difference” (Ginsburg & Rapp 2013, p. 59). “Deaf culture” has been used as
an umbrella term for sign language use, collectivity and identity, deaf values, deaf behavior, deaf
uses of technology, and deaf arts and aesthetics. However, just as anthropologists have cautioned
about the use of “culture” as an analytic, scholars have asked important questions about what “culture” does analytically and experientially for understanding deafness (Baynton 2008, Bechter 2008,
Humphries 2008, Kusters 2015b, Kusters et al. 2017a, Turner 1994). Humphries (2008) argued
1 We write “scholars” rather than “anthropologists” when we refer to scholarship from outside of anthropology.
2 Deaf anthropology and disability anthropology share overlaps, and anthropologists working on deafness often
·.•�,
attend the Disability Research Interest Group of the Society for Medical Anthropology. This is not the case,
however, with deaf studies and disability studies, and tensions remain between the two fields (Burch & Kafer
2010).
32
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
that deaf studies scholars must spend less time talking about culture and more time studying discourses of culture, with which we concur. We think “deaf culture” overdetermines deaf difference
from hearing people and assumes similitude between diverse deaf people (Friedner & Kusters
2015).
Scholarship on deaf peoples’ experiences is increasingly variegated, and categories and binaries are blurred: One needs only to consider the proliferation, critique, and pragmatic, ambivalent, and/or enthusiastic embrace of technologies such as cochlear implants (Blume 2009, Mauldin
2016, Mills 2010, Valente 2011; S. Lloyd & C. Bonventre, submitted manuscript, “When the Artificial Is Natural: Reconsidering What Bionics and Sensoria Do”). Recent scholarship has also
attended to deaf peoples’ multimodal language use and linguistic repertoires that do and do not
include national sign languages (De Meulder et al. 2019a), as well as usage of modes of signed
communication that do not fall neatly into the category of a conventional national or local sign
language, such as “family homesign” or “family sign language” (Haviland 2013), “family sign languages” (Hou 2016), “natural sign” and “local sign” (Green 2014b), International Sign (Green
2014a), and “cross-signing” (Zeshan 2015), among other developments. These scholarly interventions complicate what we mean when we say “a deaf person,” “a disabled deaf person,” “a deaf
native signer,” a “deaf person who hears” or even a “sign(ed) language.” Thinking through these
categories also denaturalizes the concept of “a hearing person” and a “spoken language” (Haualand
2008). These are all analytic categories to be expanded upon.
The number of deaf scholars in the field of deaf anthropology is increasing. This growth is resulting in experimentation with diverse modes of anthropological analysis and dissemination such
as video blogs, documentary films, and sign-based academic discussion platforms.3 Additionally,
there is much scholarly interest in International Sign, discussed below, as a means of academic engagement and dissemination, as well as an object of inquiry. We do not argue that only deaf people
can “do” deaf anthropology. Indeed, deaf anthropology avoids trafficking in essentialist ideas of
deaf and hearing people’s biological and sensory practices, arguably often found in deaf studies
works. Scholars engaging in deaf anthropology negotiate the tenuous ground of supporting deaf
studies’ and deaf activist claims of deaf exceptionalism while also critically engaging and recognizing that deaf people are like everyone else: They are multilevel marketing business participants
(Friedner 2015), backpackers (Moriarty Harrelson 2015), commuters (Kusters 2017), churchgoers (Monaghan 2012), entrepreneurs (Cooper 2017), and cafe workers (Hoffman-Dilloway 2016).
Discourses of deaf exceptionalism are discourses to be taken seriously and to be analyzed, like
other descriptive and prescriptive discourses on what it means to be deaf.
This review is arranged into four overlapping sections, and many of the works included are from
deaf studies, applied linguistics, and communication studies, in addition to anthropology. These
works, regardless of disciplinary background, engage key anthropological questions around the
subjects of each of the four sections: Socialities and Similitudes; Mobilities, Spaces, and Networks;
Modalities and the Sensorium; and Technologies and Futures.
SOCIALITIES AND SIMILITUDES
Early social science work on deafness examined the relationship between deaf and hearing people (as distinct identity and sensory categories of people) to develop theories about what binds
deaf people to each other and how deaf people have negotiated difference in relation to hearing
3 Bechter (2008, p. 69) notes that “Deaf Studies faces a discursive landscape that was not designed for it” and
points out that the spoken and written platforms available for research dissemination are at odds with how
many deaf people exercise voice.
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
33
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
people. Higgins (1980, pp. 5–6) argued that membership in deaf communities is not ascribed but
must be achieved through identification with other deaf people and participation in deaf community events. Whereas Higgins foregrounded that deaf people are “outsiders in a hearing world”
and writes of “deaf community,” Padden (1980) and Padden & Humphries (1988) ushered in a
“cultural turn.” Padden (1980, p. 93) defined deaf culture as such: “Members of the Deaf culture
behave as Deaf people do, use the language of Deaf people, and share the beliefs of Deaf people
towards themselves and other people who are not Deaf.” What is interesting is how people acquire
these “learned behaviors,” especially in the absence of intergenerational deaf families and/or a low
density of deaf people in a location. How do people come to identify as or to be deaf? Scholarship
has taken up this question, and both formal and informal education settings have become critical
sites for anthropological research on deaf experiences.
Also considering deaf and hearing relations, Becker (1983) argued that deaf senior citizens
adapt better to entering senior housing institutions because of their previous childhood experiences of being removed from hearing families and placed in deaf schools. Preston (1994) shifted
the anthropological lens from deaf people themselves to hearing family members, namely hearing
children of deaf adults (CODAs), who he argued exist between and negotiate deaf and hearing
cultures. Preston analyzed whether hearing people can belong to deaf communities and cultures,
something still debated. These earlier foundational studies and the concepts of deaf community
and deaf culture paved the way for further analysis of deaf peoples’ experiences as being apart from
and existing alongside and overlapping with those of hearing people.
These studies focused largely on social experiences and deaf peoples’ negotiation of and adaptation to social barriers during a time when there was no comprehensive disability legislation.
One exception is Groce’s (1985) historical study of Martha’s Vineyard, where deaf people ostensibly used Martha’s Vineyard Sign Language with each other and with hearing people. Groce argued that in a world where “everyone. . .spoke sign language,” deaf people are not disadvantaged.4
Whereas Groce’s work on Martha’s Vineyard can be considered an analysis of disability studies
scholars’ theorizing of the social model (Shakespeare 2006) in action, Ladd (2003) proposed a
“culturo-linguistic model” that stresses the importance of culture and language for understanding deaf experiences. Issues of language—and language endangerment, language ideologies, and
language education—loom large in thinking about the creation and maintenance of deaf social
practices and communities. It is impossible to disentangle discussions of deaf sociality from discussions of (sign) language.
In 2003, the groundbreaking Many Ways to Be Deaf (Monaghan et al. 2003) foregrounded research on specific deaf communities and sign languages around the world and the ways that deafness articulates with and in specific contexts. Essays also attended to the role of international
nongovernmental programs working on development issues relating to deafness and the codification and spread of national sign languages; deafness increasingly became a “development issue”
around the world. The volume by Monaghan et al. and subsequent work analyzed how deaf socialities exist in specific times and places and are influenced by (post- and neo-) colonial encounters
and international development funding and discourses. More recently, there is an emphasis among
anthropologists and deaf studies scholars on studying deaf experiences in places in what might be
glossed as the Global South (Friedner 2017).5 The categories of Global South and Global North
·.•�,
4 Groce’s historical anthropological work raises productive questions about whose perspectives are foregrounded in the absence of deaf interlocutors, since the island’s deaf inhabitants had died or moved out.
5 Much of this work does not explicitly address the etiology of deafness (but see Kisch 2008 on intergenerational
deafness among the Al-Sayyid Bedouins and Kusters 2015b on folk discussions of causes of deafness) or the
use of technology such as hearing aids.
34
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
both have purchase and are problematic when analyzing deaf worlds that feature significant mobility of people and ideas and exist in relation to distinct political economic structures.
Scholarship has grappled with how (not) to apply analytics and theories developed in relation to
deaf experiences in the Global North to deaf experiences in the South (Moriarty Harrelson 2017).
To remedy this situation, scholars have utilized emic concepts for deaf and hearing, deaf social
practices, and language development. For example, Friedner (2015) discusses emic concepts such
as “sign butter” and “deaf development.” Green (2014b) discusses two different “hearing” ways
of calling someone deaf in Nepal—laato and bahaara—and the stakes of using these terms: laato
is associated with being senseless and not having the capability to engage in meaning-making,
whereas bahaara does not have these connotations. These concepts create worlds of their own.
Emic concepts reveal what deafness means in specific places and how deafness articulates with
other categories of being in the world. Researchers thus attend to what Paul & Moores (2012)
have titled deaf epistemologies and how specific ways of knowing the world as a deaf person—and
knowing deafness itself—emerge in context. One might also worry, however, about valorization
of the local and the refusal to attend to (global) flows, circulations, and mobilities of people, languages, and concepts (Friedner & Kusters 2015).
A recent analytic move is to consider “deaf ontologies” or “deaf ways of being” (Kusters et al.
2017a, p. 1) and how being a deaf researcher enables (or does not enable) access to deaf research interlocutors and situations. In thinking through epistemology and ontology, deaf studies
researchers have proposed new concepts for understanding deaf experiences, such as Deafhood
(Ladd 2003), Deaf Gain (Bauman & Murray 2014), Deafnicity (Eckert 2010), deaf space (Bauman
2014, Gulliver & Kitzel 2016, Kusters 2015a, Mathews 2007), deaf socialities (Friedner 2015),
deaf sensory orientations (Bahan 2014, Dye 2014), deaf society (Green 2014b), and deaf citizenship (Cooper & Rashid 2015, Emery 2009). These concepts are meant either as a replacement or
as a friendly amendment or addition to the deaf studies concepts of DEAF-WORLD6 and DEAF
CULTURE (Lane et al. 1996), which promote holism and root deaf experiences in secular and
liberal realms (Friedner 2019). This proliferation of analytics demonstrates that much is at stake
for scholars theorizing deaf similitude (or DEAF SAME, as signed) within and across national
boundaries and borders.7 In response to a focus on similitude, scholars have turned to considering intersectionality (sometimes without explicitly using the term intersectionality) (Ahmad et al.
2002, Brueggemann & Burch 2006, Chapple 2019, David & Cruz 2018, Dunn 2008, Foster &
Kinuthia 2003, James & Woll 2004, Kusters 2019a, Leigh 2009, Leigh & O’Brien 2019, Moges
2017, Moreman & Briones 2018, Morgan 2008, Morgan & Kaneko 2017, Ruiz-Williams et al.
2015). Some of this explicitly intersectional work is meant as a corrective to earlier deaf studies scholarship that did not explicitly attend to race, gender, sexuality, ability, ethnicity, and sign
language exposure (Fernandes & Myers 2010). Nakamura (2006, p. 11), for example, argues that
“[b]eing deaf is a hybrid and intersectional identity” and carefully traces how this identity changes
over time in Japan for deaf people of different ages and educational backgrounds.
MOBILITIES, SPACES, AND NETWORKS
The study of deaf social and communicative practices has been explicitly spatialized in the past few
years. Scholars analyzed how mobile deaf signers seek out and produce deaf spaces in diverse places
6 Capitalization is how American Sign Language, and sign language in general, is “glossed”; see Senghas (2016)
on the fraught issues of translation between signed and written languages.
7 Anthropologists should study how these disciplinary concepts have found traction in everyday deaf lives, even
if they do not use said concepts analytically. “Deaf culture” and “deaf world” are widely used in many contexts
internationally.
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
35
ARjats.cls
·.•�,
April 14, 2020
11:34
to engage in deaf sociality and signed communication. The concept of “deaf space” emerged in
the 2000s, around the time that a spatial turn was initiated in the social sciences in general, and at
first scholars used the concept largely independently of one another (e.g., Kusters 2015a, Mathews
2007, Valentine & Skelton 2008). These scholars paid attention to the spatial formation of deaf socialities, the material surrounds or locations where they take place, and how they are related to and
encapsulated within mobilities. Mobilities include deaf urban mobilities (Friedner 2015, Kusters
2019a); mobilities within a country, including rural–urban migration (Graif 2018, Green 2014b,
Hoffmann-Dilloway 2016, Moriarty Harrelson 2017); international mobilities (Breivik et al. 2002,
Friedner & Kusters 2015); forced mobility (Beckmann 2020); and the relationship between literacies and mobilities (İlkbaşaran 2015). Some of this work exists at an intersection between a deaf
anthropology that attends explicitly to space and a new field called deaf geographies (Gulliver &
Fekete 2017, Gulliver & Kitzel 2016).
Deaf spaces are conceptualized as institutional, (semi)public, and virtual spaces (discussed below in the section titled Technologies and Futures). Institutional deaf spaces are most notably deaf
(residential) schools and deaf clubs, but also organizations and events, forming the foci of earlier
works in deaf studies (Ladd 2003, Van Cleve & Crouch 1992). As mentioned above, early deaf
studies and deaf anthropology approached traditional deaf spaces as if they constituted a separate
“world” (Lane et al. 1996). Institutional deaf spaces have declined with the closure of deaf schools,
deaf clubs, and deaf organizations (Padden & Humphries 2005); such closures are the source of
nostalgia and mourning (O’Brien et al. 2017). This decline is often blamed on educational mainstreaming and technological advances (see the section titled Technologies and Futures), although
this notion has been nuanced by Padden (2008) who argued that increasing diversity of deaf peoples’ class backgrounds and employment opportunities led to fragmentation before these developments. Also related to demographic shifts and the decline in deaf communities, Moges (2017)
and Monaghan (2020) consider the impact of HIV/AIDS on the US deaf community in the 1980s
and 1990s.
In attending to the production of deaf spaces in (semi)public places such as parks, pubs, tea stalls,
and public transit, as examples, scholars rejected the trend of analyzing deaf people as existing in a
bounded deaf world. For example, in Mumbai, deaf people produce deaf space by boarding specific
compartments on particular commuter trains (Kusters 2019a); and in Adamorobe, Ghana, deaf
people gather on paths in front of each other’s homes (Kusters 2015a). Deaf people intentionally
seek out deaf experiences when traveling (Moriarty Harrelson 2015) and in refugee camps and
reception centers (Sivunen 2019). These recent works on deaf space production demonstrate that
deaf people are strategic in deciding which spaces they move through, in modifying and adapting
material environments to accommodate deaf spaces, and in approaching people; this work also
attends to communication strategies in various contexts (see the section titled Modalities and the
Sensorium). And as these spaces are (semi)public, they involve being seen by hearing people. Deaf
uses of space have implications for how deaf people are seen in public (Breivik et al. 2002).
In addition to deaf space as a concept, there is the DeafSpace movement, headquartered at
Gallaudet University (Bauman 2014, Edwards 2018, Pérez Liebergesell et al. 2019, Sirvage 2015),
which has been instrumental in envisioning infrastructure modeled on deaf peoples’ social, moral,
political, and economic practices. This project is concerned with a desire to create more liveable
worlds for deaf and deafblind people; and a central goal in these worlds is to maximize opportunities to communicate in sign languages. Extending beyond architecture and the built environment (here within and surrounding Gallaudet’s campus in Washington, DC), attention to deaf
space also means attention to the affordances of everyday objects, such as deaf peoples’ problems in navigating the two-dimensionality of a standard government intake form necessary to
receive state benefits (versus the three-dimensionality of deaf peoples’ sign language practices)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
36
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
(Fagan Robinson 2019). The DeafSpace project is not concerned with disability or deaf “access”
but rather with deaf “being”; it is a moral project.
Deaf spaces and mobilities are connected to national and international networks and institutions. Scholarly work on deaf transnationalism was catalyzed by a multisited ethnographic project
in which Breivik et al. (2002) studied international deaf conferences and sporting events (such as
the Deaflympics). They argued that deaf signers often see themselves as part of a transnational
deaf community before they identify with their national or familial communities (see also Breivik
2005). The term pilgrimage has been used to describe deaf signers’ participation in these “sacred occasions,” in which signers temporarily constitute a majority (Haualand 2007). Similarly,
Gallaudet University, the only liberal arts university in the world for deaf people, is regarded as a
“deaf Mecca” (Lane et al. 1996). Deaf travelers experience such places as inspiring and recharging
(De Clerck 2007). These studies resonate with historical work on deaf transnationalism (Gulliver
2015, Murray 2008) and set the stage for the study of contemporary deaf transnationalism in an
age of increased funding opportunities for deaf development initiatives and increased deaf mobility (Friedner & Kusters 2015). An important factor in the globalization of deaf space is the ability
to communicate through International Sign and/or American Sign Language. Additionally, during longer-term stays abroad, deaf signers learn other sign languages typically in a short time span
(see the section titled Modalities and the Sensorium).
In addition to analyzing deaf mobilities and networks, scholars have asked whether the emergence of deaf communities results from a lack of integration in wider surrounding spatially demarcated communities. Communities such as Martha’s Vineyard (Groce 1985) are appealing to
researchers, lay deaf people, and tourists (Kusters 2010). Kisch (2008) calls these communities
“shared signing communities” (p. 284); they are villages, islands, towns, or groups where, because
of the historical presence of a hereditary form of deafness that exists because of endogamous
marriages, a relatively high number of deaf people have lived together with hearing people for
decades or even centuries. Shared signing communities appear to exist mostly in the Global South
and initially attracted linguists and geneticists; however in the past ten years, they also received
considerable attention from anthropologists. Examples include the Al-Sayyid Bedouin in Israel
(Kisch 2008, 2012), Ban Khor in Thailand (Nonaka 2009, 2014), Bengkala (aka Desa Kolok) in
Bali (Marsaja 2008), and Adamorobe in Ghana (Kusters 2015a). Over the years, communication
within the dense sociocultural networks of these communities has led to the emergence of local sign languages, called “shared sign languages” by Nyst (2012), used by both deaf and hearing
people. Anthropologists have disproved linguists’ earlier claims that deaf people are perfectly integrated (or even assimilated) in these communities simply because hearing people can sign (cf.
Groce 1985). They demonstrated that deaf people are excluded in certain contexts; sometimes
produce deaf-only or deaf-centered social spaces in these communities; connect to people outside
the community by using a different (national rather than shared) sign language with them; and
experience the often significant impact of “outsiders” (such as tourists, charities, and researchers)
on social patterns in these locations.
MODALITIES AND THE SENSORIUM
Sign languages exist in linguistically diverse environments where deaf people not only interact
with one or more sign languages, but also encounter and use one or more spoken and written
language(s). Linguistic anthropologists and other linguistic ethnographers have provided finegrained explorations of deaf peoples’ everyday language practices, as well as insights into language
emergence, language socialization, language contact, language shift, and language ideologies
(Green 2014b; Haviland 2016; Horton 2018; Hou 2016; Keating & Mirus 2003a,b; Pfister 2017).
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
37
ARjats.cls
·.•�,
April 14, 2020
11:34
As with other themes in this article where schools are central sites of exploration and theorizing, educational settings are also important sites of research on modalities (Bagga-Gupta 1999,
Hayashi & Tobin 2015, Holmström et al. 2015, Holmström & Schönström 2018, Tapio 2019,
Valente 2016). This focus on schools is unsurprising, given that education, whether in mainstreamed schools without interpreters, in separate residential schools for deaf children, and in
higher education, is a critical node for deaf activism, hearing-controlled policies, and competing
visions of a good deaf life. Yet, research by linguistic ethnographers and linguistic anthropologists has gone beyond the context of the school and the home, focusing on the unique ways in
which different forms of signed communication, gesture, writing, and mouthing are used and
combined by deaf or deafblind signers when they communicate with other deaf or deafblind signers or with hearing nonsigners. They also analyzed how different forms of signed communication are produced in various language ecologies (Horton 2018, Reed 2020), documenting how
people experience the affordances and limitations of these forms of communication (Edwards
2015, Hoffman-Dilloway 2016, Keating & Mirus 2003b). By focusing on naturally occurring
communication in different sociolinguistic contexts, linguistic ethnography and linguistic anthropology expanded understandings of the actual diversity of deaf communicative practices in everyday sociolinguistic and sociocultural contexts (Hou & Kusters 2020). In addition, they have
focused on languaging in contexts of international mobility which frequently entail (sign) language
learning or translanguaging, multilingual sign interactions, International Sign, and gesture-based
interactions.
Linguistic ethnographers, linguistic anthropologists, and other scholars have questioned neat
divides between “sign language” and “not sign language” (sometimes glossed as gesture) (Green
2014b, Hou 2016, Kusters & Sahasrabudhe 2018). Some sign linguists have organized different forms of signing on a developmental cline: gesture—homesign—communal/rural/family
homesign—village/rural/shared sign language—national/urban sign language (Meir et al. 2010,
de Vos & Zeshan 2012). A distinction is thus made between sign and/or gesture as “system” and
“sign language,” with gesture and International Sign often put in the category of “not language”
(Goldin-Meadow & Brentari 2017). These categories are political as much as they are empirical
(Coppola & Senghas 2017).
Anthropologists have investigated what it means linguistically and communicatively for deaf
people to be immobile, particularly in locales that are not sites of “deaf communities,” such as remote rural locations. They and other scholars have addressed the question of what it means to have
language, the difference that having language makes, and how important it is both to acknowledge
the power of shared language and to investigate practices and experiences of people who do not
communicate in conventional (signed or spoken) languages or who learn to do so later in life. National sign language users may consider it a moral imperative to try to bring such deaf people into
“deaf society” (Green 2014b). In this respect, there is a tension between work in psycholinguistics
and linguistics arguing persuasively that there are critical and often measurable consequences to
learning a conventional sign language early in life (Humphries et al. 2014, Mayberry 2007) and
the abovementioned work by anthropologists and anthropologically oriented linguists arguing
compellingly for recognizing the competence, creativity, and even artistry of deaf people who do
not acquire a conventional sign language early on (or ever) (Green 2014b, Hou 2016, Moriarty
Harrelson 2019, Reed 2020). What is at stake is whether analysts focus on cognition or interaction.
We argue that research must balance attention to both vulnerabilities and competencies.
Recent linguistic anthropology scholarship has also demonstrated the importance of thinking
about how people understand each other in and across modalities not only in relation to knowledge or even familiarity with the modality and/or specific language(s), but also in relation to
moralities, affect, desire, willingness, and commitment (or lack thereof ) to communication and its
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
38
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
very possibility (Graif 2018; Green 2014a,b, 2015). There is growing attention to how language
practices and the related desires and commitments shape and are shaped by language ideologies
(Kusters et al. 2020). This work demonstrates that sign languages are a complex terrain on and in
which different interests and agendas are produced and negotiated. Ideological frictions can be
found for example between deaf and hearing people, sighted deaf and deafblind people, different
generations of deaf persons (Kisch 2012, Kusters 2019b, Nakamura 2006), people in villages
or in towns (Hou 2016, Kusters 2019b), or within a single family that comprises its own micro
community of signers (Haviland 2016). Scholars have described responses to the use of different
sign languages, which are hierarchized by their users. For example, Schmaling (2003), Cooper
(2015), Moges (2015), and Parks (2014) identified conflicts where imported sign languages such
as American Sign Language were not ideally suited to local contexts in the Global South. Hofer
& Sagli (2017) analyzed how Tibetan and Mongolian deaf children are forced to learn written
and spoken Chinese under China’s “civilizing project”; deaf concerns can articulate (or not) with
nationalist projects (Cooper 2017).
The use of different modalities in deaf communication is inextricably linked to the senses. One
of deaf studies’ central projects has been to foreground visuality. Consider, for example, the titles of
the books Open Your Eyes: Deaf People Talking (Bauman 2008) and The People of the Eye: Deaf Ethnicity
and Ancestry (Lane et al. 2011). According to these texts, the eyes discern important sound-based
information and are crucial to engage language that is produced with the face and hands and to
savor deaf art forms such as signed poetry; additionally, the themes of light, transparency, and
visuality are prominent in deaf art forms (Bahan 2014). This foregrounding of vision has been
empowering in backgrounding the paradigm of deaf people as deficient because of their lack of
hearing8 and was central in conceptualizing “Deaf Gain” (Bauman & Murray 2014). Bauman &
Murray (2014, p. xv) want the concept of Deaf Gain “to counter the frame of hearing loss as it
refers to the unique cognitive, creative, and cultural gains manifested through deaf ways of being
in the world.”
In this focus on vision, there has been a tendency by deaf studies scholars to ignore or, at best,
sideline diverse deaf experiences such as those of deafblind people, including those involved in the
protactile movement. Edwards (2015, 2018) has argued that the unmarked phrases “sign language”
or “sign” almost always refer to visual signed communication. To assume that sign languages are
necessarily visual is as problematic as the (hopefully extinct) assumption that languages are necessarily spoken—something that was explicitly theorized as recently as 1960. This work also shows
that the use of a specific channel (here, tactile) for the exchange of linguistic signs is not sufficient
for building a life-world and that a shift in the kinds of social roles and social norms inhabited
within that channel—e.g., roles of authority, the possibility of overhearing by touching people
who continue to sign rather than freezing awkwardly—is necessary. Although tactile reception of
ASL signs existed prior to the protactile movement, it is only since the protactile movement that a
distinct protactile language is emerging. A focus on the eyes and visible language also sidelines the
central importance of touch, vibration, and sound in deaf communication (Friedner & Helmreich
2012, Kusters 2017, Mills 2015, Napoli 2014).
Related to the deaf experience of the sensorium, linguistic anthropologists have studied how
sign languages are experienced and described in qualic ways, or the feeling of doing language. One
8 It
is ironic that anthropologists continue to advance the view of deafness as deficiency through metaphoric
practices of writing of “deaf ears” or “so and so was deaf to X” to indicate that someone is not listening or
paying attention. Deafness has nothing to do with lack of attention or not listening. Similarly, phrases such as
“silent world” or “a silent life” do not resonate with deaf people, either because many deaf people do access
sound in some way or because they experience their lives as “visual,” “tactile,” or “gestural” ones rather than
“silent” ones.
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
39
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
example is SignWriting, a system of writing sign languages, drawing symbols that represent the
perspective of the signer rather than of the receiver, i.e., the expressive rather than the receptive
viewpoint (Hoffman-Dilloway 2018). Another example is deaf signers describing particular registers as “strong,” “hard,” or “soft” (Kusters 2019b). Edwards (2018) has paid attention to protactile
people’s observations that alignment and integration of language with tactile modes of orientation
to the environment motivate language, making it capable of conjuring tactile experience in ways
that feel immediate.
TECHNOLOGIES AND FUTURES
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
In Deaf Gain: Raising the Stakes for Human Diversity, Bauman & Murray (2014) argued that deaf
people and sign languages have much to contribute to the world. Notably, award-winning author Andrew Solomon wrote the book’s foreword in which he discussed how unique and valuable
deaf communities and sign languages are. However, Solomon (2012, p. 114) elsewhere argued
that deafness will eventually disappear as a result of technological developments such as cochlear
implants and that deaf people who refuse such advances are similar to the Amish. To be sure,
deaf people have had long engagements with technology and have been involved in technological
developments such as the emergence of the modern telephone (Mills 2010), often only to be denied access to the same product after it was completed. More recently, and directly related to the
disabling effects of telephone technology, scholars have examined how innovations such as an Internet connection and Internet-based video calling, as well as other platforms such as Skype, have
impacted both social and language practices (Hjulstad 2016, İlkbaşaran 2015, Keating & Mirus
2003a, Valentine & Skelton 2008). Haualand (2014) examined video interpreting, when a sign language interpreter interprets conversations between sign language users and nonsigners by way of
a videophone, an Internet-based platform, and a regular phone in the United States, Sweden, and
Norway to think about new inclusions and exclusions brought about by technology even when providing so-called access. The frame of access, however, is one ambivalently accepted and negotiated
in many deaf worlds because “direct communication” is preferred (Edwards 2018, Green 2014a).
Although sign language interpreters are not explicitly considered a technology, anthropologists
have recently analyzed their role in creating conditions of possibility for deaf peoples’ inclusion
and participation via technologies of signed and spoken voice. Scholars have examined the personal
and professional roles that interpreters play and how they negotiate interdependence on and with
deaf people as well as their roles within deaf communities (Marie 2019). The labor of both deaf
people and interpreters in intersubjectively working together is increasingly a topic of research, as
are deaf peoples’ preferences for different kinds of designated and preferred interpreters (Burke
2017, Feyne 2018). Deaf and deafblind people also work as informal interpreters for and with each
other (Green 2015), brokering language in order to maximize participation and understanding.
There is a difference between technology that assists in providing access and accommodation
and technology that seeks to remediate deafness itself: This tension is at the heart of what Mills
(2010) called “deaf futurism.” As Valente (2011) noted, “cyborgization” through cochlear implants
is often perceived as a threat to deaf communities and ways of life; elsewhere, Valente et al. (2002,
p. 246) located debates over cochlear implants within the terrain of sensory politics, which they
argue “looks at the intersection of biological perception and cultural mediation and interpretation
within a field of power.” Unfortunately, the concept of the cyborg (here turned into a trope) has
not done much to actively engage with deaf peoples’ creative practices with cochlear implants and
other technologies (cf. Mills 2010). More studies on deaf peoples’ ambivalent, excited, and chosen
trajectories of working with and through hearing technology are needed. Today’s implanted babies
and children, and their families, are pioneers on uncharted paths.
·.•�,
40
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
It is not only the implanted individual who has something at stake but also the family: Mauldin
(2016) explored how US-based parents make decisions to implant their children and engage in
processes of “ambivalent medicalization” and the role that mothers play after implantation as part
of the habilitation process. Friedner (2018) discussed what happens when educators and medical
professionals frame sign language as a virus and cochlear implants as presumed cures and then
wonders what the vaccine might be. The figure of the deaf infant and child looms large in these
discussions about what kinds of sensory and linguistic futures are available (Bosteels & Blume
2014, Fjord 1999, Kafer 2013). These debates around deaf futures have recently been formulated
in terms of language deprivation in the case of deaf children (Hall et al. 2019) and the ability of
parents to actively choose sign languages. Snoddon (2014) identified a paradox in that hearing
babies in Western countries are taught baby sign, whereas deaf babies are encouraged not to sign.
In an attempt to remedy negative ideologies around sign languages, deaf studies scholars
and anthropologists have labored to demonstrate that flourishing (De Clerck 2017) occurs and
that value (albeit sometimes ambivalent) (Friedner 2015) can be found in deaf worlds. To this
end, scholars have imagined futures that include sign language recognition on various scales (De
Meulder et al. 2019b), co-equality with hearing people (Murray 2008), deaf citizenship and belonging (Hiddinga & De Langen 2019, Emery 2009), and the possibility of biologically bringing
deaf people into the world in light of increasing genetic screening and editing (Emery et al. 2010,
Johnston 2006). Concerns over deaf futures attend not only to hearing and listening status but
also to communication and language paths, and even the right to exist.
We argue for a future for deaf anthropology in close conversation with anthropology writ large.
By placing “deaf” central in explorations of socialities, mobilities, modalities, and technologies, a
deaf anthropology that exists in partial overlap with the transdisciplinary field of deaf studies has
illuminated different ways of being deaf. Deaf anthropology offers up diverse analyses of what it
means to be a sensing, communicating, and social person in the world. It also argues for deafnesses
as providing ontologies and epistemologies that are valuable and worth preserving.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors are not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that
might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Thanks go to Mara Green, Terra Edwards, Hilde Haualand, and Erin Moriarty for their detailed
comments on earlier drafts of the article and to the multiple other colleagues with whom we discussed this article, including Leila Monaghan, Richard Senghas, Amandine le Maire, Sanchayeeta
Iyer, Alan Rumsey, Lauren Reed, Stephanie Lloyd, and Bambi Schieffelin.
LITERATURE CITED
Ahmad WIU, Atkin K, Jones L. 2002. Being deaf and being other things: young Asian people negotiating
identities. Soc. Sci. Med. 55(10):1757–69
Bagga-Gupta S. 1999. Visual language environments: exploring everyday life and literacies in Swedish deaf
bilingual schools. Vis. Anthropol. Rev. 15:95–120
Bahan B. 2014. Senses and culture: exploring sensory orientations. See Bauman & Murray 2014, pp. 233–54
Bauman H. 2014. DeafSpace: an architecture toward a more livable and sustainable world. See Bauman &
Murray 2014, pp. 375–401
Bauman HDL, ed. 2008. Open Your Eyes: Deaf Studies Talking. Minneapolis: Univ. Minn. Press
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
41
ARjats.cls
·.•�,
April 14, 2020
11:34
Bauman HDL, Murray JJ, eds. 2014. Deaf Gain: Raising the Stakes for Human Diversity. Minneapolis: Univ.
Minn. Press
Baynton DC. 2008. Beyond culture: deaf studies and the deaf body. See Bauman 2008, pp. 293–313
Bechter F. 2008. The deaf convert culture and its lessons for deaf theory. See Bauman 2008, pp. 60–
82
Becker G. 1983. Growing Old in Silence: Deaf People in Old Age. Berkeley: Univ Calif. Press
Beckmann G. 2020. Competence for citizenship: Deaf people’s (re)creation of politics and claim-making possibilities in
Northern Uganda. PhD Thesis, Univ. Zurich
Blume S. 2009. The Artificial Ear: Cochlear Implants and the Culture of Deafness. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
Univ. Press
Bosteels S, Blume S. 2014. The making and unmaking of deaf children. In The Human Enhancement Debate and
Disability: New Bodies for a Better Life, ed. EK Grüber, C Rehmann-Sutter, pp. 81–100. London: Palgrave
Macmillan
Breivik J-K. 2005. Deaf Identities in the Making: Local Lives, Transnational Connections. Washington, DC:
Gallaudet Univ. Press
Breivik JK, Haualand H, Solvang P. 2002. Rome—a temporary Deaf city! Deaflympics 2001. Work. Pap., Stein
Rokkan Cent. Soc. Stud., Bergen Univ. Res. Found., Bergen, Nor. http://bora.uib.no/handle/1956/
1434
Brueggemann BJ, Burch S, eds. 2006. Women and Deafness: Double Visions. Washington, DC: Gallaudet Univ.
Press
Burch S, Kafer A, eds. 2010. Deaf and Disability Studies: Interdisciplinary Perspectives. Washington, DC: Gallaudet
Univ. Press
Burke TB. 2017. Choosing accommodations: signed language interpreting and the absence of choice. Kennedy
Inst. Ethics J. 27(2):267–99
Chapple RL. 2019. Toward a theory of black deaf feminism: the quiet invisibility of a population. Affilia
34(2):186–98
Cooper AC. 2015. Signed language sovereignties in Viê.t Nam: deaf community responses to ASL-based
tourism. See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 95–111
Cooper AC. 2017. Deaf to the Marrow: Deaf Social Organizing and Active Citizenship in Viet Nam. Washington,
DC: Gallaudet Univ. Press
Cooper AC, Rashid KK, eds. 2015. Citizenship, Politics, Difference: Perspectives from Sub-Saharan Signed Language
Communities. Washington, DC: Gallaudet Univ. Press
Coppola M, Senghas A. 2017. Is it language (yet)? The allure of the gesture-language binary. Behav. Brain Sci.
40:e50
David E, Cruz CCJ. 2018. Deaf turns, Beki turns, transformations: towards new forms of deaf sociality. Fem.
Form. 30(1):91–116
De Clerck GAM. 2007. Meeting global deaf peers, visiting ideal deaf places: deaf ways of education leading to
empowerment, an exploratory case study. Am. Ann. Deaf. 152(1):5–19
De Clerck GAM. 2017. Deaf Epistemologies, Identity, and Learning: A Comparative Perspective. Washington, DC:
Gallaudet Univ. Press
De Meulder M, Kusters A, Moriarty E, Murray JJ. 2019a. Describe, don’t prescribe. The practice and politics
of translanguaging in the context of deaf signers. J. Multiling. Multicult. Dev. 40(10):892–906
De Meulder M, Murray JJ, McKee RL, eds. 2019b. The Legal Recognition of Sign Languages: Advocacy and Outcomes Around the World. Clevedon, UK: Multiling. Matters
de Vos C, Zeshan U. 2012. Introduction: demographic, sociocultural, and linguistic variation across rural
signing communities. See Zeshan & de Vos 2012, pp. 2–23
Dunn L. 2008. The burden of racism and audism. See Bauman 2008, pp. 235–50
Dye M. 2014. Seeing the world through deaf eyes. See Bauman & Murray 2014, pp. 193–210
Eckert RC. 2010. Toward a theory of deaf ethnos: Deafnicity ≈ D/deaf (Hómaemon . Homóglosson .
Homóthreskon). J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 15(4):317–33
Edwards T. 2015. Bridging the gap between DeafBlind minds: interactional and social foundations of intention
attribution in the Seattle DeafBlind community. Front. Psychol. 6:1497
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
42
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
Edwards T. 2018. Re-channeling language: the mutual restructuring of language and infrastructure among
DeafBlind people at Gallaudet University. J. Linguist. Anthropol. 28(3):278–92
Emery SD. 2009. In space no one can see you waving your hands: making citizenship meaningful to Deaf
worlds. Citizsh. Stud. 13(1):31–44
Emery SD, Middleton A, Turner GH. 2010. Whose deaf genes are they anyway?: The deaf community’s
challenge to legislation on embryo selection. Sign Lang. Stud. 10(2):155–69
Fagan Robinson K. 2019. The form that flattens. In Medical Materialities: Towards a Material Culture of Medical
Anthropology, ed. A Parkhurst, T Carroll, pp. 126–43. Oxon, UK/New York: Routledge
Fernandes JK, Myers SS. 2010. Inclusive Deaf Studies: barriers and pathways. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 15(1):17–
29
Feyne S. 2018. Variation in perception of the identity of interpreted Deaf lecturers. In Interpreting and the
Politics of Recognition: The IATIS Yearbook, ed. C Stone, L Leeson, pp. 119–37. London: Routledge
Fjord L. 1999. “Voices offstage:” how vision has become a symbol to resist in an audiology lab in the U.S. Vis.
Anthropol. Rev. 15:121–38
Foster S, Kinuthia W. 2003. Deaf persons of Asian American, Hispanic American, and African American backgrounds: a study of intraindividual diversity and identity. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 8(3):271–90
Friedner M. 2015. Valuing Deaf Worlds in Urban India. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Univ. Press
Friedner M. 2017. Doing deaf studies in the Global South. See Kusters et al. 2017b, pp. 129–50
Friedner M. 2018. (Sign) language as virus: stigma and relationality in urban India. Med. Anthropol. 37(5):359–
72
Friedner M. 2019. Praying for rights: cultivating deaf worldings in Urban India. Anthropol. Q. 92(2):403–26
Friedner M, Block P. 2017. Deaf studies meets autistic studies. Senses Soc. 12(3):282–300
Friedner M, Helmreich S. 2012. Sound studies meets Deaf studies. Senses Soc. 7(1):72–86
Friedner M, Kusters A, eds. 2015. It’s a Small World: International Deaf Spaces and Encounters. Washington, DC:
Gallaudet Univ. Press
Gertz G, Boudreault P, eds. 2016. The SAGE Deaf Studies Encyclopedia. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Ginsburg F, Rapp R. 2013. Disability worlds. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 42:53–68
Goldin-Meadow S, Brentari D. 2017. Gesture, sign and language: the coming of age of sign language and
gesture studies. Behav. Brain Sci. 40:e46
Graif P. 2018. Being and Hearing: Making Intelligible Worlds in Deaf Kathmandu. Chicago: HAU
Green EM. 2014a. Building the tower of Babel: International Sign, linguistic commensuration, and moral
orientation. Lang Soc. 43:445–65
Green EM. 2014b. The nature of signs: Nepal’s deaf society, local sign, and the production of communicative sociality.
PhD Thesis, Univ. Calif., Berkeley
Green EM. 2015. One language, or maybe two: direct communication, understanding, and informal interpreting in international deaf encounters. See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 70–82
Groce N. 1985. Everyone Here Spoke Sign Language: Hereditary Deafness on Martha’s Vineyard. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard Univ. Press
Gulliver M. 2015. The emergence of international deaf spaces in France from Desloges 1779 to the Paris
Congress of 1900. See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 3–14
Gulliver M, Fekete E. 2017. Themed section: deaf geographies - an emerging field. J. Cult. Geogr. 34(2):121–30
Gulliver M, Kitzel MB. 2016. Deaf geographies. See Gertz & Boudreault 2016, pp. 450–53
Hall ML, Hall WC, Caselli NK. 2019. Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Lang. 39(4):367–
95
Hartblay C. 2020. Disability expertise: claiming disability anthropology. Curr. Anthropol. 61:S21, S26–36
Haualand H. 2007. The two-week village: the significance of sacred occasions for the Deaf community. In
Disability in Local and Global Worlds, ed. B Ingstad, SR Whyte, pp. 33–55. Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press
Haualand H. 2008. Sound and belonging: What is a community? See Bauman 2008, pp. 111–26
Haualand H. 2014. Video interpreting services: calls for inclusion or redialling exclusion? Ethnos 79(2):287–
305
Haviland J. 2016. “But you said ‘four sheep’ . . . !”: (sign) language, ideology, and self (esteem) across generations
in a Mayan family. Lang. Comm. 46:62–94
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
43
ARjats.cls
·.•�,
April 14, 2020
11:34
Haviland JB. 2013. The emerging grammar of nouns in a first generation sign language: specification, iconicity,
and syntax. Gesture 13(3):309–53
Hayashi A, Tobin J. 2015. Contesting visions at a Japanese school for the Deaf. Anthropol. Educ. Q. 46(4):380–
96
Hiddinga A, De Langen M. 2019. Practices of belonging: claiming elderly care through deaf citizenship.
Citizsh. Stud. 23(7):669–85
Higgins PC. 1980. Outsiders in a Hearing World: A Sociology of Deafness. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Hjulstad J. 2016. Practices of organizing built space in videoconference-mediated interactions. Res. Lang. Soc.
Interact. 49(4):325–41
Hofer T, Sagli G. 2017. ‘Civilising’ Deaf people in Tibet and Inner Mongolia: governing linguistic, ethnic
and bodily difference in China. Disabil. Soc. 32(4):443–66
Hoffmann-Dilloway E. 2016. Signing and Belonging in Nepal. Washington, DC: Gallaudet Univ. Press
Hoffmann-Dilloway E. 2018. Feeling your own (or someone else’s) face: writing signs from the expressive
viewpoint. Lang. Comm. 61:88–101
Holmström I, Bagga-Gupta S, Jonsson R. 2015. Communicating and hand(ling) technologies. Everyday life in
educational settings where pupils with cochlear implants are mainstreamed. J. Linguist. Anthropol. 25:256–
84
Holmström I, Schönström K. 2018. Deaf lecturers’ translanguaging in a higher education setting. A multimodal multilingual perspective. Appl. Linguist. Rev. 9(1):90–111
Horton LA. 2018. Conventionalization of shared homesign systems in Guatemala: social, lexical, and morphophonological dimensions. PhD Thesis, Univ. Chicago
Hou L, Kusters A. 2020. Signed languages. In The Routledge Handbook of Linguistic Ethnography, ed. K Tusting,
pp. 340–55. London: Routledge
Hou LY-S. 2016. “Making hands”: family sign languages in the San Juan Quiahije community. PhD Thesis, Univ.
Tex., Austin
Humphries T. 2008. Talking culture and culture talking. See Bauman 2008, pp. 35–41
Humphries T, Kushalnagar P, Mathur G, Napoli DJ, Padden C, Rathmann C. 2014. Ensuring language acquisition for deaf children: what linguists can do. Language 90(2):31–52
İlkbaşaran D. 2015. Social media practices of deaf youth in Turkey: emerging mobilities and language choices.
See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 112–26
James M, Woll B. 2004. Black deaf or deaf black? Being black and deaf in Britain. In Negotiation of Identities in
Multilingual Contexts, ed. A Pavlenko, A Blackledge, pp. 125–60. Clevedon, UK: Multiling. Matters
Johnston T. 2006. W(h)ither the deaf community? Population, genetics, and the future of Australian Sign
Language. Sign Lang. Stud. 6(2):137–73
Kafer A. 2013. Feminist, Queer, Crip. Bloomington: Indiana Univ. Press
Keating E, Mirus G. 2003a. American Sign Language in virtual space: interactions between deaf users of
computer-mediated video communication and the impact of technology on language practices. Lang.
Soc. 32(5):693–714
Keating E, Mirus G. 2003b. Examining interactions across language modalities: deaf children and hearing
peers at school. Anthropol. Educ. Q. 34:115–35
Kisch S. 2008. “Deaf discourse”: the social construction of Deafness in a Bedouin community. Med. Anthropol.
27(3):283–313
Kisch S. 2012. Demarcating generations of signers in the dynamic sociolinguistic landscape of a shared signlanguage: the case of the Al-Sayyid Bedouin. See Zeshan & de Vos 2012, pp. 87–126
Kusters A. 2010. Deaf utopias? Reviewing the sociocultural literature on the world’s “Martha’s Vineyard Situations.” J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 15(1):3–16
Kusters A. 2015a. Deaf Space in Adamorobe: An Ethnographic Study in a Village in Ghana. Washington, DC:
Gallaudet Univ. Press
Kusters A. 2015b. Peasants, warriors, and the streams: language games and etiologies of deafness in
Adamorobe, Ghana. Med. Anthropol. Q. 29(3):418–36
Kusters A. 2017. “Our hands must be connected”: visible gestures, tactile gestures and objects in interactions
featuring a deafblind customer in Mumbai. Soc. Semiotics 27(4):394–410
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
44
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
Kusters A. 2019a. Boarding Mumbai trains: the mutual shaping of intersectionality and mobility. Mobilities
14(6):841–58
Kusters A. 2019b. One village, two sign languages: qualia, intergenerational relationships and the language ideological assemblage in Adamorobe, Ghana. J. Linguist. Anthropol. https://doi.org/10.1111/jola.12254
Kusters A, De Meulder M, O’Brien D. 2017a. Innovations in Deaf studies: critically mapping the field. See
Kusters et al. 2017b, pp. 1–53
Kusters A, De Meulder M, O’Brien D, eds. 2017b. Innovations in Deaf Studies: The Role of Deaf Scholars. Oxford,
UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Kusters A, Green M, Moriarty Harrelson E, Snoddon K, eds. 2020. Sign Language Ideologies in Practice. Berlin:
Mouton De Gruyter & Ishara Press
Kusters A, Sahasrabudhe S. 2018. Language ideologies on the difference between gesture and sign. Lang.
Comm. 60:44–63
Ladd P. 2003. Understanding Deaf Culture: In Search of Deafhood. Clevedon, UK: Multiling. Matters
Lane H, Hoffmeister R, Bahan B. 1996. A Journey Into the Deaf-World. San Diego, CA: Dawn Sign Press
Lane H, Pillard RC, Hedberg U. 2011. The People of the Eye: Deaf Ethnicity and Ancestry. Oxford, UK: Oxford
Univ. Press
Leigh IW. 2009. A Lens on Deaf Identities. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Leigh IW, O’Brien CA, eds. 2019. Deaf Identities: Exploring New Frontiers. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Marie SS. 2019. Enacting dependence. Somatosphere, Febr. 19. http://somatosphere.net/2019/enactingdependence.html/
Marsaja IG. 2008. Desa Kolok. A Deaf Village and its Sign Language in Bali, Indonesia. Nijmegen, Neth.: Ishara
Press
Mathews ES. 2007. Place, space and identity—using geography in deaf studies. In Proceedings of the Deaf Studies
Today! 2006: Simply Complex, pp. 215–26. Oram: Utah Valley Univ.
Mauldin L. 2016. Made to Hear: Cochlear Implants and Raising Deaf Children. Minneapolis: Univ. Minn. Press
Mayberry RI. 2007. When timing is everything: age of first-language acquisition effects on second-language
learning. Appl. Psycholinguist. 28(3):537–49
Meir I, Sandler W, Padden C, Aronoff M. 2010. Emerging sign languages. In Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies,
Language, and Education, Vol. 2, ed. M Marschark, PE Spencer, pp. 267–80. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ.
Press
Mills M. 2010. Deaf jam: from inscription to reproduction to information. Soc. Text. 28(1):35–58
Mills M. 2015. Deafness. In Keywords in Sound, ed. D Novak, M Sakakeeny, pp. 45–54. Durham, NC: Duke
Univ. Press
Moges R. 2017. Cripping Deaf studies and Deaf literature: Deaf Queer ontologies and intersectionality. See
Kusters et al. 2017b, pp. 215–40
Moges RT. 2015. Challenging sign language lineages and geographies: the case of Eritrean, Finnish, and
Swedish Sign Languages. See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 83–94
Monaghan L. 2012. Founding of two deaf churches: the interplay of Deaf and Christian identities. In A Cultural Approach to Interpersonal Communication, ed. L Monaghan, JE Goodman, JM Robinson, pp. 438–54.
Malden, MA/Oxford, UK: Blackwell
Monaghan L. 2020. Laughter, lament, and stigma: the making and breaking of sign language communities.
In The Routledge Handbook of Language and Emotion, ed. SE Pritzker, J Fenigsen, JM Wilce, pp. 363–80.
London: Routledge
Monaghan L, Schmaling C, Nakamura K, Turner GH, eds. 2003. Many Ways to Be Deaf: International Variation
in Deaf Communities. Washington, DC: Gallaudet Univ. Press
Moreman ST, Briones SR. 2018. Deaf Queer world-making: a thick intersectional analysis of the mediated
cultural body. J. Int. Intercult. Commun. 11(3):216–32
Morgan R. 2008. “Deaf Me Normal”: Deaf South Africans Tell Their Life Stories. Muckleneuk, Pretoria, S. Afr.:
UNISA
Morgan R, Kaneko M. 2017. Being and belonging as Deaf South Africans: multiple identities in SASL poetry.
Afr. Stud. 76(3):320–36
Moriarty Harrelson E. 2015. SAME-SAME but different: tourism and the deaf global circuit in Cambodia.
See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 199–211
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
45
ARjats.cls
·.•�,
April 14, 2020
11:34
Moriarty Harrelson E. 2017. Authenticating ownership: claims to deaf ontologies in the Global South. See
Kusters et al. 2017b, pp. 361–84
Moriarty Harrelson E. 2019. Deaf people with “no language”: mobility and flexible accumulation in languaging practices of deaf people in Cambodia. Appl. Linguist. Rev. 10(1):55–72
Murray JJ. 2008. Coequality and transnational studies: understanding deaf lives. See Bauman 2008, pp. 100–10
Murray JJ. 2017. Academic and community interactions in the formation of Deaf Studies in the United States.
See Kusters et al. 2017b, pp. 77–100
Nakamura K. 2006. Deaf in Japan. Signing and the Politics of Identity. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Univ. Press
Napoli DJ. 2014. A magic touch: deaf gain and the benefits of tactile sensation. See Bauman & Murray 2014,
pp. 211–32
Nonaka AM. 2009. Estimating size, scope, and membership of the speech/sign communities of undocumented
indigenous/village sign languages: the Ban Khor case study. Lang. Comm. 29(3):210–29
Nonaka AM. 2014. (Almost) everyone here spoke Ban Khor sign language—until they started using TSL:
language shift and endangerment of a Thai village sign language. Lang. Comm. 38:54–72
Nyst VAS. 2012. Shared sign languages. In Sign Language: An International Handbook, ed. R Pfau, M Steinbach,
B Woll, pp. 552–73. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter
O’Brien D, Stead L, Nourse N. 2017. Bristol Deaf memories: archives, nostalgia and the loss of community
space in the Deaf community in Bristol. Soc. Cult. Geogr. 20(7):899–917
Padden C. 1980. The Deaf community and the culture of Deaf people. In Sign Language and the Deaf Community, ed. C Baker, R Battison, pp. 89–103. Silver Spring, MD: Natl. Assoc. Deaf
Padden C. 2008. The decline of Deaf Clubs in the United States: a treatise on the problem of place. See
Bauman 2008, pp. 169–76
Padden C, Humphries T. 1988. Deaf in America: Voices from a Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
Padden C, Humphries T. 2005. Inside Deaf Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
Parks ES. 2014. Constructing national and international Deaf identity: perceived use of American Sign Language. In Language, Borders and Identity, ed. D Watt, C Llamas, pp. 206–17. Edinburgh: Edinburgh Univ.
Press
Paul PV, Moores DF, eds. 2012. Deaf Epistemologies: Multiple Perspectives on the Acquisition of Knowledge.
Washington, DC: Gallaudet Univ. Press
Pérez Liebergesell N, Vermeersch P-W, Heylighen A. 2019. Through the eyes of a deaf architect: reconsidering conventional critiques of vision-centered architecture. Senses Soc. 14(1):46–62
Pfister AE. 2017. Forbidden signs: deafness and language socialization in Mexico City. Ethos 45(1):139–61
Preston P. 1994. Mother Father Deaf: Living Between Sound and Silence. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
Reed L. 2020. Switching caps: two ways of communicating in sign in the Port Moresby deaf community, Papua
New Guinea. Asia-Pac. Lang. Var. 6:1
Ruiz-Williams E, Burke M, Chong VY, Chainarong N. 2015. “My deaf is not your deaf”: realizing intersectional realities at Gallaudet University. See Friedner & Kusters 2015, pp. 262–73
Schmaling C. 2003. A for apple: the impact of Western education and ASL on the Deaf community in Kano
State, Northern Nigeria. See Monaghan et al. 2003, pp. 302–10
Senghas RJ. 2016. Sign languages and communicative practices. In The Routledge Handbook of Linguistic Anthropology, ed. N Bonvillain, pp. 247–61. New York: Routledge
Senghas RJ, Monaghan L. 2002. Signs of their times: Deaf communities and the culture of language. Annu.
Rev. Anthropol. 31:69–97
Shakespeare T. 2006. The social model of disability: an outdated ideology? In The Disability Studies Reader, ed.
L Davis, pp. 197–204. New York: Routledge. 2nd ed.
Sirvage RT. 2015. Measuring the immeasurable: the legacy of atomization and dorsality as a pathway in making
Deaf epistemology quantifiable—an insight from DeafSpace. Presented at TEDxGallaudet, Gallaudet Univ.,
Washington, DC, March 6
Sivunen N. 2019. An ethnographic study of deaf refugees seeking asylum in Finland. Societies 9(1):2
Snoddon K. 2014. Baby sign as deaf gain. See Bauman & Murray 2014, pp. 146–58
Solomon A. 2012. Far from the Tree: Parents, Children, and the Search for Identity. New York: Scribner
Tapio E. 2019. The patterned ways of interlinking linguistic and multimodal elements in visually oriented
communities. Deafness Educ. Int. 21(2–3):133–50
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
46
Friedner
•
Kusters
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2020.49. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 82.1.240.13 on 05/12/20. For personal use only.
AN49CH03_Friedner
ARjats.cls
April 14, 2020
11:34
Turner GH. 1994. How is Deaf culture? Another perspective on a fundamental concept. Sign Lang. Stud.
83(1):103–26
Valente JM. 2011. Cyborgization: Deaf education for young children in the cochlear implantation era. Qual.
Inq. 17(7):639–52
Valente JM. 2016. “Your American Sign Language interpreters are hurting our education”: toward a relational
understanding of inclusive classroom pedagogy. Transformations 25(2):20–36
Valente JM, Bahan B, Bauman HDL. 2002. Sensory politics and the cochlear implant debates. In Cochlear
Implants: Evolving Perspectives, ed. R Paludneviciene, IW Leigh, pp. 245–58. Washington, DC: Gallaudet
Univ. Press
Valentine G, Skelton T. 2008. Changing spaces: the role of the internet in shaping Deaf geographies. Soc. Cult.
Geogr. 9(5):469–86
Van Cleve JV, Crouch BA. 1992. A Place of Their Own: Creating the Deaf Community in America. Washington,
DC: Gallaudet Univ. Press
Woodward J, Horejes T. 2016. deaf/Deaf: origins and usage. See Gertz & Boudreault 2016, pp. 284–87
Zeshan U. 2015. “Making meaning”: communication between sign language users without a shared language.
Cogn. Linguist. 26:211–60
Zeshan U, de Vos C, eds. 2012. Sign Languages in Village Communities: Anthropological and Linguistic Insights.
Sign Lang. Typol. 4. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton & Ishara Press
·.•�,
www.annualreviews.org
Review in Advance first posted
on April 21, 2020. (Changes may
still occur before final publication.)
•
Deaf Anthropology
47