JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
ARTICLE NO.
34, 182–201 (1998)
JS971348
Attitude Dissimulation and Persuasion
Gregory R. Maio
University of Wales at Cardiff, UK
and
James M. Olson
University of Western Ontario, Canada
Received: March 12, 1997; revised: July 25, 1997; accepted: November 17, 1997
Because people’s true attitudes toward an object become more accessible immediately
after indicating an attitude that they know to be false, we predicted that such attitude
dissimulation might paradoxically cause the true attitude to have a stronger effect on
subsequent judgments. In two experiments, participants were randomly assigned to express
false, true, or no attitudes toward Albert Einstein. Next, in an ostensibly separate study,
participants read a persuasive message describing Einstein’s dislike for a particular
technology and then rated their attitude toward this technology. As expected, results
indicated that participants who had previously indicated false or true attitudes toward
Einstein indicated more dislike for the technology than participants who had not previously
expressed attitudes toward him. A second experiment replicated the effect of attitude
dissimulation using a manipulation that was similar to that employed in cognitive
dissonance experiments. Possible effects of falsely indicating ‘‘politically correct’’ attitudes are discussed. r 1998 Academic Press
Attitudes can be defined as associations in memory between objects and
evaluations (Fazio, 1990; Olson & Zanna, 1993). For example, opera fans who
like Luciano Pavarotti have formed an internal association between the evaluation
‘‘like’’ and the attitude object ‘‘Luciano Pavarotti.’’ Consequently, if asked about
Pavarotti, fans can recall and express liking for him.
The research reported in this paper was funded by a research grant to the second author and was
conducted while the first author was supported by a doctoral fellowship, both from the Social Sciences
and Humanities Research Council of Canada. We thank Russell Fazio, Richard Petty, Yaacov Trope,
Mark Zanna, and several anonymous reviewers for their comments on a previous version of this paper.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Greg Maio, School of Psychology, Cardiff
University of Wales, Cardiff, Wales, UK CF1 3YG.
182
0022-1031/98 $25.00
Copyright r 1998 by Academic Press
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
183
In everyday life, however, people sometimes express attitudes that they know
are false. Research has examined numerous issues related to such dissimulation
(e.g., DePaulo, Stone, & Lassiter, 1985; Olson, 1988; Olson & Ross, 1988;
Zuckerman & Driver, 1985), but the aspect of attitude dissimulation that has
received the most attention has been its effect on the deceivers’ own attitudes.
Specifically, in past research on cognitive dissonance and self-perception processes (see Cooper & Fazio, 1984; Cooper & Scher, 1994), it has been found that
people who are asked to express a false attitude toward an attitude object will
sometimes change their attitude to make it more congruent with their lie. This
attitude change occurs primarily when people freely express false attitudes in a
manner that produces aversive consequences (Cooper & Fazio, 1984; Cooper &
Scher, 1984; cf. Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, Simon, & Nelson, 1996).
Although attitude dissimulation that occurs without choice and without aversive consequences does not typically produce dissonance-motivated or selfperception-mediated attitude change, it might nonetheless have important psychological consequences that have not yet been recognized. Specifically, in the
present set of studies, we tested the paradoxical prediction that such attitude
dissimulation can cause people’s original attitudes to have stronger effects on
subsequent attitude-relevant judgments.
The Effect of Attitude Dissimulation on Attitude Accessibility
Consistent with a model of attitude expression (Judd & Brauer, 1995), we
suggest that a variety of pressures can induce people to express attitudes that are
different from their true attitudes, while leaving the true attitudes unchanged. We
propose that an indirect consequence of such dissimulation is increased accessibility of the true attitude from memory. For example, at a business luncheon, a
person may be asked about his or her attitude toward the Republican party. In
response, the business person might access and compute a negative attitude, but
then recall that the question was asked by a staunch Republican. If the query came
from a Republican who happened to be a valuable client, the respondent might
quickly choose to indicate a neutral or positive attitude, while being aware of his
or her true negative attitude. As a result of this dissimulation, the person might
increase his or her chances of getting a valuable contract or promotion, assuaging
any concerns about lying. Nevertheless, this dissimulation might remind the
person of his or her true attitude, which could increase the attitude’s accessibility.
We recently examined the effect of attitude dissimulation on attitude accessibility in two experiments (Maio & Olson, 1995). In these studies, we constructed a
situation where attitude dissimulation was unlikely to cause attitude change. In
particular, similar to the example above, we provided a strong external justification for attitude dissimulation, one which would override any potential dissonancemotivated attitude change. Specifically, participants were simply instructed to
express false attitudes—no choice was made salient—and the dissimulation
produced no aversive consequences.
We also distinguished between attitude dissimulation, which involves indicat-
184
MAIO AND OLSON
ing an attitude that one knows to be false, and uttering simple falsehoods: People
can utter falsehoods without being aware of the truth, but dissimulation involves
making a false statement while being aware of the truth. Thus, to elicit a
laboratory analog of attitude dissimulation, we forced participants to access their
true attitudes while expressing false attitudes. This was achieved by asking them
to indicate the opposite of their true attitudes. We also included a condition that
elicited attitude falsehoods, by asking participants to make specific statements
that we knew were false (e.g., ‘‘horses are bad’’). In this condition, participants
did not have to access their true attitudes in order to express false attitudes.
Finally, we included a condition where participants indicated their true attitudes.
After participants completed the experimental manipulation, we measured
attitude accessibility by asking participants to express quickly and accurately their
true attitudes toward (a) the objects for which they had expressed the opposite of
their true attitudes, (b) the objects for which they had given specific false
attitudes, (c) the objects that they had evaluated truthfully, and (d) other objects
that they had not evaluated at all. Before and after the manipulation and attitude
accessibility measure, participants rated their attitudes toward all of the attitude
objects.
As expected, results indicated that neither attitude dissimulation, nor indicating
specific false attitudes, nor truthful expression caused attitude change from the
pre- to posttest. Nonetheless, both attitude dissimulation and expression caused
attitudes to become more accessible (i.e., more quickly recalled) than if specific
false attitudes or no attitudes had been expressed.
The Potential Effect of Attitude Dissimulation on Attitude-Relevant Judgments
The effect of attitude dissimulation on attitude accessibility is interesting
because, in general, highly accessible attitudes have larger effects on judgments
and behavior than inaccessible attitudes (Fazio, 1990). Therefore, by making true
attitudes more accessible, attitude dissimulation might paradoxically cause the
true attitudes to have larger effects on subsequent judgments and behavior.
Although this hypothesis is plausible, other perspectives suggest that attitude
dissimulation might increase the impact on judgments and behavior of the false,
rather than the true, attitude (even though the true attitude may become more
accessible). For example, to the extent that expressing a false attitude is rewarded,
individuals may be more likely to repeat such behavior in the future. Also, the
mere expression of an assertion (such as a false attitude) is sometimes sufficient to
increase the perceived plausibility of that assertion, especially when perceivers
are too ‘‘cognitively busy’’ to monitor their judgments carefully (Arkes, Boehm,
& Xu, 1991; Gilbert, Krull, & Malone, 1990).
Finally, it is possible that attitude dissimulation makes both true and false
attitudes more accessible, which might eliminate any effect of dissimulation on
subsequent judgments and behavior. For example, recent conceptualizations of
attitudes have suggested that positive and negative evaluations of an object can
coexist and can even be simultaneously accessible (Cacioppo, Gardner, &
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
185
Berntson, 1997; Maio, Bell, & Esses, 1996). Perhaps, then, attitude dissimulation
might increase the accessibility of the true and false evaluations, creating
attitudinal ambivalence and eliminating effects of dissimulation on judgments.
Given these diverse possibilities, it is important to conduct an empirical test of the
effects of attitude dissimulation on subsequent judgments. The present experiments provide such a test.
Overview
The present experiments build on our previous research and on an experiment
by Roskos-Ewoldsen and Fazio (1992). Roskos-Ewoldsen and Fazio (1992)
manipulated whether or not participants repeatedly expressed their attitudes
toward Jacques Cousteau, who was a popular oceanographer. After completing
this manipulation, participants were asked to rate their agreement with an essay
ostensibly written by Cousteau. It was predicted that participants who repeatedly
expressed attitudes toward Cousteau would agree more with his essay than
participants who had not previously expressed attitudes toward him, because
repeated expression should make more accessible participants’ positive attitudes
toward Cousteau. Results confirmed this hypothesis. (In addition, manipulation
checks showed that repeated attitude expression did not change participants’
attitudes toward Cousteau.)
Using similar procedures, we explored whether people will also be more likely
to accept persuasive messages from a likeable source when they have repeatedly
expressed false attitudes toward him or her (i.e., indicated dislike toward the
source), while being aware of their true attitudes. We also tested whether attitude
dissimulation affected cognitive responses to the message. According to contemporary models of persuasion (e.g., Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989; Petty &
Cacioppo, 1986), variables can sometimes affect yielding to a message without
affecting cognitive reactions to the message. For example, various heuristic cues
(e.g., source likeability) can sometimes affect yielding without affecting people’s
message-relevant thoughts (see Chaiken et al., 1989; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986).
Roskos-Ewoldsen and Fazio (1992) did not examine the effect of attitude
expression on cognitive responses in their experiments, but suggested that this
issue should be examined. Consequently, we included a measure of participants’
message-relevant thoughts in our experiment (see, e.g., Chaiken, 1980; Millar &
Millar, 1990; Petty, Schumann, Richman, & Strathman, 1993).
EXPERIMENT 1
In our first experiment, attitude dissimulation was manipulated using a technique similar to the one employed in our previous research (Maio & Olson, 1995).
In particular, we manipulated whether participants expressed the opposite of their
true attitudes, true attitudes, or no attitudes toward Albert Einstein. Albert Einstein
was chosen as the target because we expected that people strongly like him,
making it unlikely that participants would change their attitudes toward him as a
result of attitude dissimulation or expression. Participants were then presented
186
MAIO AND OLSON
with an essay describing nanotechnology, which is a technology that might
someday enable the creation of devices far smaller than a microchip. The essay
indicated that Albert Einstein was opposed to research on nanotechnology.
We then measured attitudes toward stopping nanotechnology research and
cognitive reactions to the message. Finally, to ensure that attitudes toward
Einstein were unaffected by the manipulation, we measured these attitudes and
three characteristics of these attitudes: attitude certainty, importance, and intensity
or strength. Attitude certainty is the extent to which people are confident that their
attitude is correct; attitude importance refers to the degree to which people care
deeply about their attitude; and attitude intensity is the strength of the emotional
reaction elicited by an attitude object (Krosnick, Boninger, Chuang, Berent, &
Carnot, 1993). Recent findings indicate that these three attitude characteristics
produce effects similar to those of attitude accessibility (see Krosnick et al., 1993;
cf. Bassili, 1996), so it seemed worthwhile to test whether these variables might
mediate the effects of our manipulation.
Method
Participants
Participants were 52 psychology undergraduates (7 men, 43 women, and 2 who did not indicate
their sex) at the University of Western Ontario, who participated for course credit. Data from five
additional participants were omitted because of failure to follow instructions (N 5 3), prior knowledge
about nanotechnology (N 5 1), or suspicion (N 5 1).
Overview
One to four participants took part in each session. To ensure that the instructions were understood,
we delivered them verbally as well as on paper. Thus, all participants in the same session were
assigned to the same experimental condition.
Participants were told that they would be participating in a number of different ‘‘surveys.’’ In this
series of ‘‘surveys,’’ the second measured initial attitudes toward Albert Einstein; the seventh
contained a manipulation of whether participants expressed true, false, or no attitudes toward Albert
Einstein; and the eighth contained a persuasive message that (falsely) cited Albert Einstein as
advocating a ban on nanotechnology. After reading this message, participants indicated (a) their
attitudes toward banning nanotechnology, (b) their thoughts while reading the message, (c) their
attitudes toward Einstein, and (d) the strength of their attitudes toward Einstein. The other ‘‘surveys’’
were included for unrelated research and served as fillers. After completing all of the materials,
participants were probed for suspicion and debriefed.
Premanipulation Attitudes toward the Source
Premanipulation attitudes toward Einstein were measured within a ‘‘survey of students’ attitudes
toward famous people from the past and present.’’ The experimenter stated that this survey was a pilot
questionnaire that would enable him to choose some famous people as topics for a questionnaire to be
used later in the year. Participants then rated their attitudes toward 26 famous people, using a 9-point,
skewed, semantic-differential scale. This scale contained one negative value, 21 (unfavorable), a
neutral point, 0, and seven positive values from 11 (slightly favorable) to 17 (extremely favorable).
The seventeenth famous person rated in the survey was Albert Einstein.
Experimental Manipulation
Control condition. The experimenter stated that the purpose of the seventh ‘‘survey’’ was to
examine perceptions of famous individuals from all walks of life. The experimenter stated that he was
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
187
interested in discovering which famous individuals students liked the most, because he wanted to use
those famous individuals as topics for a study later in the year. He then distributed a booklet containing
the names of five different individuals who were well-known in Canada: Wayne Gretzky, Jean
Chretien (the Canadian Prime Minister), Whitney Houston, Bill Clinton, and Donald Trump.
Participants rated each individual using five 6-point (23 to 13) semantic-differential scales, which
were anchored by awful/nice, worthless/valuable, unpleasant/pleasant, bad/good, and unfavorable/
favorable. (No neutral point was presented.) Each scale was presented twice for each famous
individual. Thus, a total of ten scales (5 3 2) were presented for each famous individual. These scales
were presented in random order, except that adjacent scales were never used to rate the same person.
Attitude expression. The instructions in this condition were identical to those in the control
condition, except that participants in this condition were asked to rate Albert Einstein instead of Jean
Chretien. Participants rated Einstein favorably (M 5 1.78).
Attitude dissimulation. The targets in this condition were identical to those in the attitude expression
condition; thus, Albert Einstein was rated in both conditions. However, participants in the attitude
dissimulation condition were asked to indicate the opposite of their true attitudes toward each famous
individual. For example, if their true rating was 12, they were asked to indicate 22. To justify this
request, the experimenter stated that this manner of responding was part of a new procedure for
detecting attitudes. As instructed, participants rated Einstein unfavorably (M 5 22.18).
Persuasive Message
In the eighth ‘‘survey,’’ participants were told that we were interested in measuring their attitudes
toward a wide variety of new social issues. The experimenter stated that he did not have time to ask
each participant questions about all of the issues, so he was asking each person to draw the name of an
issue from a box. He stated that they would be presented with a photocopy of an article describing the
selected issue, because most people are unfamiliar with the issues contained in the box.
Through appropriate contrivances, participants always drew the topic of ‘‘research on nanotechnology.’’ They were then given a booklet containing a (fictitious) article about research on nanotechnology. The article looked like a photocopy of an editorial page from a scientific magazine. The top of the
page was entitled ‘‘Opinion,’’ and the bottom right showed the name and institutional affiliation of the
(fictitious) author. Below the author’s name, a hydrogen atom was depicted above a caption indicating
that the hydrogen atom is a basic building block of nanotechnology. The remaining two-thirds of the
page displayed the text of the message. A portion of the text introduced and described nanotechnology
(parts of the text were inaccurate):
Despite the incredible achievements that were facilitated by the invention of the microchip,
a new technology is emerging that will make the microchip look like a relic. It is expected
that, in the next 30 years, this new technology will begin to create complicated ‘‘minifactories’’ that are one millionth the size of a microchip. This new technology is called
nanotechnology, because its goal is the creation of ‘‘mini-factories’’ from individual atoms,
which are approximately one nanometer in size (i.e., one millionth of a millimeter). In fact,
these ‘‘atom factories’’ are nicknamed ‘‘nanites,’’ which is a name that is borrowed from a
Star Trek episode that presented possible harmful consequences of these ‘‘mini-factories.’’
Because nanites would be built from atoms, nanites could take atoms from specific
objects and recombine the atoms to form different objects. The objects that would be created
would depend on the design of the nanite. For example, nanites could be designed to create
and repair DNA molecules, which are the basic building blocks of all life. Nanites could
even be programmed to create copies of themselves.
After describing some benefits of nanotechnology, the text then (falsely) stated that Einstein foresaw
its invention.
Interestingly, Albert Einstein foresaw the invention of nanites. He was excited by the
possible benefits of creating nanites, but he was also concerned that nanites could be
188
MAIO AND OLSON
programmed to have destructive consequences. Specifically, Einstein was concerned that
nanites could be designed to tear atoms from all objects in the environment, and not just
specific objects, such as viruses. Such nanites could easily consume organic tissue and,
consequently, kill people. Because of their destructive capabilities, nanites could be
designed as weapons of war. For this reason and other reasons, Einstein felt that nanotechnology should not be explored.
Despite Einstein’s concerns, research on nanotechnology has moved forward, because the
scientific community now feels that nanotechnology can be researched safely. Nevertheless,
in my opinion, it is only a matter of time before Einstein’s vision becomes reality.
Eventually, we will be able to design nanites for use as deadly weapons. Thus, even though
there are tremendous potential benefits of this new technology, the risks are too great. We
should not explore nanotechnology.
Dependent Measures
Attitudes toward nanotechnology. After reading the persuasive message, participants rated their
attitudes toward stopping the development of nanotechnology. Participants rated their attitudes in
three different ways. First, they evaluated the idea of stopping research on nanotechnology using five
7-point (23 to 13) semantic-differential scales, which were anchored by bad/good, harmful/
beneficial, foolish/wise, negative/positive, and worthless/valuable. Responses to these scales were
then averaged (a 5 .89). Next, using a 7-point scale from 23 (extremely unfavorable) to 13
(extremely favorable), participants rated their general favorability toward stopping research on
nanotechnology. Finally, using a 9-point scale from 24 (disagree very strongly) to 14 (agree very
strongly), they rated the extent to which they agreed with banning research on nanotechnology.
Scores on the three attitude measures were significantly correlated (lowest r (45) 5 .76, p , .001).
Thus, we created an aggregate index of these measures, weighting each measure equally. Because
there were different response scales for the three measures, we first calculated z-scores for each
measure, and then these z-scores were averaged to form an overall index of attitudes.
Message-relevant thoughts. After indicating their attitudes toward stopping research on nanotechnology, participants were asked to write down any thoughts that occurred to them while they read the
article. The instructions and coding procedures for the thought-listing task were adapted from
Cacioppo and Petty (1981). Participants were given 18 lines to list their thoughts, and they were asked
to number each thought.
After listing their thoughts, participants were asked to go back and rate the extent to which each
thought was favorable toward stopping the development of nanotechnology. They rated their thoughts
using a 7-point scale from 23 (extremely unfavorable) to 13 (extremely favorable). The ratings for
each thought were averaged to form an index of the extent to which the thoughts reflected favorability
toward stopping the development of nanotechnology.
Postmessage attitudes toward the source. After completing the thought-listing measure, participants
were told that previous research has found that people’s opinions of an individual can influence their
attitudes toward the person’s views. The experimenter stated that, in his analysis of attitudes toward
nanotechnology, he wanted to statistically control for attitudes toward Albert Einstein. Participants
were then asked to rate their attitudes toward Einstein. These ratings were performed using the same
semantic-differential scale as was used to assess premanipulation attitudes toward Einstein.
Confidence, importance, and strength of attitudes toward the source. Using 9-point scales from 0
(not at all) to 8 (extremely), participants rated their confidence about their attitude toward Einstein, the
importance of their attitude, and the strength of their attitude.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Premanipulation and postmanipulation attitudes toward the source. Premanipulation and postmessage attitudes toward Einstein were submitted to a 3 3 2
189
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
TABLE 1
EXPERIMENT 1: ATTITUDES TOWARD NANOTECHNOLOGY AMONG PARTICIPANTS WHO HAD EXPRESSED
FALSE ATTITUDES, TRUE ATTITUDES, OR NO ATTITUDES TOWARD ALBERT EINSTEIN
Attitude measure
Attitude dissimulation
Attitude expression
Control
Composite
Differential scales
Favorability
Agreement
0.27
0.72
0.53
1.07
0.26
0.79
0.53
0.82
20.50
20.20
20.63
20.75
Note. The table presents the mean raw scores for each attitude measure, except for the composite
measure, which produced z-scores. Higher scores indicate greater favorability toward nanotechnology.
(condition 3 trial) mixed-model ANOVA, with trial (pre- vs postmessage) as the
within-subjects factor. As expected, results indicated no significant effects. Thus,
the manipulation did not affect attitudes toward Einstein. Overall, attitudes toward
Einstein were positive (M 5 4.45; 5 5 very favorable), with no participants
indicating a negative attitude toward him.
Confidence, importance, and strength of attitudes toward the source. Threelevel one-way ANOVAs on participants’ ratings of attitude confidence, importance, and strength indicated no significant effects. Attitudes toward Einstein were
characterized by high confidence (M 5 4.58; 4 5 moderately confident), mild
importance (M 5 2.50; 2 5 mildly important) and mild to moderate strength
(M 5 3.09; 4 5 moderately strong).
Attitudes toward Nanotechnology
A 3-level one-way ANOVA on participants’ attitudes toward nanotechnology
indicated an effect of the experimental manipulation, F (2, 44) 5 3.90, p , .03.
Examination of this effect confirmed our predictions. First, participants who were
asked to repeatedly express false attitudes toward Albert Einstein were more
favorable toward his recommendation that research on nanotechnology be stopped
(M 5 0.27) than were participants who had not indicated true or false attitudes
toward him (M 5 20.50), t (44) 5 2.41, p , .02. In addition, participants who
were asked to repeatedly express true attitudes toward Albert Einstein were more
favorable toward a ban on nanotechnology (M 5 0.26) than were participants
who had not indicated true or false attitudes toward him, t (44) 5 2.42, p , .02.
The attitudes of participants who had expressed false attitudes toward Einstein did
not significantly differ from the attitudes of participants who had expressed true
attitudes toward Einstein, t (44) 5 0.03, ns. (See Table 1.)
Message-Relevant Thoughts
A 3-level one-way ANOVA on thought favorability scores (average rating of
the thoughts) indicated a significant effect of the experimental manipulation, F (2,
40) 5 3.20, p 5 .05. As expected, participants who were asked to repeatedly
express false attitudes toward Albert Einstein indicated thoughts that were more
190
MAIO AND OLSON
favorable toward stopping nanotechnology research (M 5 0.49) than did participants who had not indicated true or false attitudes toward him (M 5 20.44), t
(40) 5 1.78, p , .05 (one-tailed). In addition, participants who were asked to
repeatedly express true attitudes toward Albert Einstein indicated thoughts that
were more favorable toward stopping nanotechnology (M 5 0.81) than did
participants who had not indicated true or false attitudes toward him, t (40) 5
2.46, p , .02. Participants who expressed false attitudes toward Einstein did not
indicate thoughts that were significantly different from the thoughts indicated by
participants who expressed true attitudes toward him, t (40) 5 20.64, ns.
We tested whether the effects of attitude dissimulation and attitude expression
on thoughts mediated the effects on attitudes toward nanotechnology. To test
whether thoughts played a mediating role, we conducted three different regression
analyses for both the attitude expression and dissimulation conditions. First,
experimental condition was used to predict attitudes toward stopping nanotechnology (control 5 0, dissimulation or expression 5 1). Second, experimental condition was used to predict the favorability of thoughts regarding stopping nanotechnology. Third, experimental condition and thought favorability were used to
predict attitudes toward stopping nanotechnology.
The analyses comparing the attitude dissimulation and control conditions were
consistent with what would be expected if thoughts mediated the effect of
dissimulation on attitudes. Specifically, the effects of attitude dissimulation on
attitudes, B 5 .39, t (29) 5 2.25, p , .04, and on thought favorability, B 5 .32, t
(25) 5 1.72, p , .05 (one-tailed) were significant in the first two regression
analyses, and the effect of thought favorability on attitudes was significant in the
third analysis, B 5 .66, t(24) 5 4.60, p , .001. The effect of attitude dissimulation
was not significant in the third regression analysis, B 5 .19, t (24) 5 1.29, ns,
where favorability of thoughts was simultaneously entered as a predictor.
Similarly, the results in the attitude expression condition supported the idea that
thoughts mediated the effect of repeated expression on attitudes. Specifically, the
effects of attitude expression on attitudes, B 5 .40, t (30) 5 2.42, p , .03, and on
thought favorability, B 5 .43, t (27) 5 2.49, p , .02, were significant in the first
two regression analyses, and the effect of thought favorability on attitudes was
significant in the third regression analysis, B 5 .73, t (25) 5 5.12, p , .001. The
effect of attitude expression was not significant in the third regression analysis,
B 5 .08, t (25) 5 0.55, ns, where favorability of thoughts was simultaneously
entered as a predictor.
Although these results are consistent with the hypothesis that thoughts about
nanotechnology mediated the impact of attitude dissimulation and attitude expression on attitudes toward nanotechnology, their correlational nature means that
they are not necessarily inconsistent with the possibility that attitude dissimulation and expression directly affected attitudes, which may, in turn, have affected
thoughts about nanotechnology. That is, participants’ listed thoughts might have
also served to rationalize their expressed attitudes. To examine this possibility, we
used regression analyses to test whether the effects of attitude dissimulation and
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
191
expression on thoughts were mediated by attitudes toward nanotechnology.
Results indicated that the effect of attitude dissimulation on thoughts was not
significant, B 5 .04, t (24) 5 0.27, ns, when attitudes were simultaneously entered
as a predictor, B 5 .71, t (24) 5 4.60, p , .001. Similarly, the effect of attitude
expression on thoughts was not significant, B 5 .16, t (25) 5 1.15, ns, when
attitudes were simultaneously entered as a predictor, B 5 .70, t (25) 5 5.12, p ,
.001. These results indicate that attitudes toward nanotechnology may have
mediated the effects of attitude dissimulation and expression on thoughts about
nanotechnology. Overall, then, participants’ thoughts might have affected their
attitudes and vice versa (see below).1
Discussion
Expressing true or false evaluations of Albert Einstein did not alter attitudes
toward him, but did affect attitudes toward his alleged point of view regarding
nanotechnology. Specifically, participants were more favorable toward stopping
nanotechnology when they had repeatedly expressed either true or false attitudes
toward Einstein. Presumably, attitude expression and attitude dissimulation each
made participants’ positive attitudes toward him more accessible, which caused
them to agree more with his point of view.
The effect of truthful attitude expression on yielding to the message replicated
past findings (Roskos-Ewoldsen & Fazio, 1992), whereas the finding that lying
about attitudes can increase the influence of true attitudes on yielding is novel.
The possibility that dissimulation would increase the impact of the false attitude
was not confirmed. Thus, the experiment showed that expression of an attitude
does not necessarily cause subsequent attitude-relevant judgments to become
more congruent with the expressed attitude (e.g., Houston & Fazio, 1989;
Roskos-Ewoldsen & Fazio, 1992). That is, expression of dislike for something
might not always cause subsequent judgments of it to be less favorable; if the
expressed dislike was knowingly false, subsequent judgments could actually
become more favorable. Also, by showing that attitude dissimulation caused true
1 For reasons of space, we have not included description of another study that was very similar to
Experiment 1, with five notable differences. First, a premanipulation measure of attitudes toward
Einstein was not included; only a postmanipulation measure was administered. Second, the experiment did not contain measures of the strength of attitudes toward Einstein. Third, participants in the
control condition rated their attitudes toward David Suzuki, a popular Canadian scientist, rather than
their attitudes toward Jean Chretien. Fourth, participants were asked to list fewer than six thoughts in
their thought-listing. Fifth, the favorability of participants’ thoughts was rated by two trained coders
and not by the participants themselves, possibly causing the scoring of favorability to be less accurate
(see Bell, Esses, & Maio, 1996; Esses, Haddock, & Zanna, 1993; Maio et al., 1996).
Despite these different procedures, the principal results were similar to the findings for Experiment
1. As expected, participants were significantly more favorable toward stopping nanotechnology when
they had repeatedly expressed either true or false attitudes toward Einstein. Expressing true or false
evaluations of Einstein did not alter attitudes toward him. Unlike the findings for Experiment 1, the
experimental manipulation did not significantly affect responses to the thought-listing measure,
perhaps because coders’ classifications of participants’ thoughts are less accurate than participants’
own ratings.
192
MAIO AND OLSON
attitudes to have a larger effect on attitude-relevant judgments, the results
indicated that the effects of attitude dissimulation on attitude accessibility (Maio
& Olson, 1995) have practical significance.
Mediational analyses suggested that the effects of attitude dissimulation and
attitude expression on attitudes were mediated by message-relevant thoughts.
However, mediational analyses also suggested that the effects of attitude dissimulation and attitude expression on thoughts were mediated by attitudes. These
paradoxical findings might indicate that the effects of the manipulations on
attitudes and thoughts were reciprocally interdependent. In other words, participants’ thoughts might have biased their attitudes, which, in turn, might have
further biased their thoughts. To date, most persuasion researchers have not tested
for such reciprocal interdependence of thoughts and attitudes (Eagly & Chaiken,
1993). Our data suggest that such reciprocal relations might occur and should be
tested.
Importantly, the results of Experiment 1 might also shed new light on previous
research that has occurred under the rubric of cognitive dissonance theory
(Festinger, 1957). A primary goal of past cognitive dissonance research has been
the identification of conditions that are necessary for attitude dissimulation to
elicit attitude change. Results have indicated that attitude dissimulation tends to
elicit attitude change only when the dissimulator freely chooses to dissimulate in a
context that produces aversive consequences for which the dissimulator feels
personally responsible (Cooper & Fazio, 1984). Researchers did not appear to
anticipate interesting consequences of attitude dissimulation when such conditions were absent. Experiment 1 demonstrates that there may be measurable
effects of attitude dissimulation even when it does not elicit attitude change.
Indeed, it is possible that, in past cognitive dissonance research, similar effects
occurred undetected in the conditions where attitude dissimulation did not
produce attitude change.
Nevertheless, this implication would be strengthened if we found similar
results using a different method to manipulate dissimulation. Our manipulation in
Experiment 1 elicited attitude dissimulation in a heavy-handed fashion: Participants were asked to circle the opposite of their true attitudes. Participants might
have felt that they were simply translating their responses into opposites, rather
than dissimulating. A new procedure requiring more creative dissimulation would
rectify this ambiguity.
EXPERIMENT 2
Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1, except that Experiment 2 focused
solely on the effects of attitude dissimulation, manipulating it using a context like
that of the prototypical ‘‘no attitude change’’ condition in a cognitive dissonance
experiment. As in many past dissonance studies (e.g., Scher & Cooper, 1989;
Steele & Liu, 1983), attitude dissimulation was elicited by asking participants to
write counterattitudinal essays. Specifically, we asked participants to write
negative, untrue essays about Albert Einstein. Also, paralleling past cognitive
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
193
dissonance studies, participants were told that their essays would be presented to
others, whose attitudes might be affected by the essays. To minimize the
likelihood that attitude dissimulation would produce attitude change, we gave
participants no choice about engaging in attitude dissimulation and told them that
we did not expect their stories about Albert Einstein to affect future participants’
perceptions of him (thus reducing possible aversive consequences). We should
also note that, because we eliminated choice in our dissimulation condition,
attitude change would not be expected from a self-perception perspective either
(Bem, 1972; Olson, 1990, 1992; Olson & Roese, 1995).
Method
Participants
Participants were 57 psychology undergraduates (12 men and 45 women) at the University of
Western Ontario, who participated for course credit. Data from three additional participants were
omitted because of failure to follow instructions (N 5 2), or suspicion (N 5 1). Two additional
participants failed to complete the experimental manipulation.
Overview
Participation occurred individually. As in Experiment 1, participants were told that they would be
completing several different ‘‘surveys.’’ The experimenter stated that all of the surveys were pretests
that would help him develop questionnaires for studies later in the year. He also emphasized that
responses were anonymous and confidential, stating that participants should not write their name on
the booklets and that they should place their completed booklets inside a box in the room; a research
assistant would collect the box at the end of the day and enter the data into a computer.
Participants were then given the first ‘‘survey,’’ which measured their initial attitudes toward Albert
Einstein; the third portion manipulated whether or not participants expressed false attitudes toward
(i.e., wrote a negative story about) Albert Einstein; and the fourth portion contained the same
persuasive message and dependent measures that were used in Experiment 1. The second ‘‘survey’’
was included for an unrelated study and served as a filler between the initial attitude measure and the
experimental manipulation. After completing all of the ‘‘surveys,’’ participants were probed for
suspicion and debriefed.
Premanipulation Attitudes toward the Source
Premanipulation attitudes toward Einstein were measured in the same manner as in Experiment 1.
Experimental Manipulation
Control condition. The experimenter stated that the focus of the third ‘‘survey’’ was tabloid
newspapers and magazines (e.g., Globe, National Enquirer). Participants were told that many
celebrities have been winning lawsuits against tabloid newspapers, because the tabloids frequently
publish negative, untrue stories about celebrities. The experimenter stated that he was interested in this
issue because celebrities allege that these stories negatively affect the public’s perceptions of them. He
then claimed to be skeptical that the tabloids actually affect people’s perceptions of celebrities. He
described a study that he claimed to have conducted in the previous summer, wherein he asked
participants to write negative, untrue stories about two famous celebrities. He stated that, later in the
year, other participants were asked to read the ‘‘tabloid-like’’ stories and then indicate their perceptions
of the two celebrities. He said that there was no effect of these stories on the participants’ perceptions
of the celebrities, supporting his theory.
Nevertheless, the experimenter pointed out that he should have examined the effects of negative,
194
MAIO AND OLSON
untrue stories about a variety of celebrities, rather than just two celebrities. He stated that he might
discover effects for some celebrities but not for others, and, consequently, he was asking current
participants to write stories about a variety of famous people. He told participants that he had different
booklets containing the names of three different famous people and that he was asking each participant
to write negative, untrue stories about the people named in one, randomly selected booklet. He then
retrieved one of the booklets from another room.
The booklet that he retrieved gave space for participants to write stories about John F. Kennedy,
Harrison Ford, and Oprah Winfrey (in that order). For each celebrity, participants were given
three-quarters of a page to write a negative, untrue story about the celebrity. In the bottom-quarter of
each page, there were three six-point (23 to 13) semantic-differential scales, which were anchored by
worthless/valuable, unpleasant/pleasant, and stupid/smart. The experimenter stated that he wanted
future participants to believe that whoever wrote the story did not like the person described therein and
that the story explained why the person was disliked. Participants were asked to indicate unfavorable
attitudes using the semantic-differential scales.
Attitude dissimulation. The procedure in the attitude dissimulation condition was identical to the
procedure in the control condition, except that participants were given a booklet that contained Albert
Einstein as the second celebrity, rather than Harrison Ford. As instructed, all participants indicated
negative stories about Einstein (e.g., he was a fraud; he was a terrorist). They also rated him
unfavorably (M 5 2.88).
Dependent Measures
Attitudes toward nanotechnology. Attitudes toward nanotechnology were measured using the same
three attitude measures as in Experiment 1. As in Experiment 1, the three attitude measures were
significantly correlated (lowest r (55) 5 .55, p , .001). Thus, we calculated z-scores for each
measure, and these z-scores were averaged to form an overall index of attitudes.
Message-relevant thoughts. After indicating their attitudes toward stopping research on nanotechnology, participants completed the same thought-listing task as in Experiment 1.
Postmessage attitudes toward the source. Attitudes toward Einstein were measured using the same
procedure and scale as in Experiment 1.
Confidence, importance, and strength of attitudes toward the source. These measures were identical
to those employed in Experiment 1.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Premanipulation and postmessage attitudes toward the source. Premanipulation and postmessage attitudes toward Einstein were submitted to a 2 3 2
(condition 3 trial) mixed-model ANOVA, with trial (pre- vs postmessage) as the
within-subjects factor. As expected, results indicated no significant effects. Thus,
the manipulation did not affect attitudes toward Einstein. Overall, attitudes toward
Einstein were very positive (M 5 4.86; 5 5 very favorable), with no participants
indicating a negative attitude toward him.
Confidence, importance, and strength of attitudes toward the source. T-tests on
participants’ ratings of attitude confidence, importance, and strength indicated no
significant effects. As in Experiment 1, attitudes toward Einstein were characterized by high confidence (M 5 5.23; 4 5 moderately confident), mild to moderate
importance (M 5 3.53; 4 5 moderately important), and mild to moderate strength
(M 5 3.68; 4 5 moderately strong).
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
195
TABLE 2
EXPERIMENT 2: ATTITUDES TOWARD NANOTECHNOLOGY AMONG PARTICIPANTS WHO HAD EXPRESSED
FALSE ATTITUDES OR NO ATTITUDES TOWARD ALBERT EINSTEIN
Attitude measure
Attitude dissimulation
Control
Composite
Differential scales
Favorability
Agreement
0.24
0.34
20.11
0.11
20.22
20.60
20.57
20.60
Note. The table presents the mean raw scores for each attitude measure, except for the composite
measure, which produced z-scores. Higher scores indicate greater favorability toward nanotechnology.
Attitudes toward Nanotechnology
As expected, participants who a wrote negative, untrue story about Albert
Einstein were subsequently more favorable toward his recommendation that
research on nanotechnology be stopped (M 5 0.24) than were participants who
did not write stories about him (M 5 20.22), t (55) 5 1.99, p 5 .05. This result
replicated the principal finding in Experiment 1.
Message-Relevant Thoughts
Participants who wrote negative, untrue stories about Albert Einstein tended to
indicate thoughts that were more favorable toward stopping nanotechnology
research (M 5 0.09) than did participants who did not write stories about him
(M 5 20.40), t (53) 5 1.63, p , .06 (one-tailed).
As in Experiment 1, we conducted regression analyses that tested whether the
favorability of participants’ thoughts about nanotechnology mediated the effect of
attitude dissimulation on attitudes toward nanotechnology. Results indicated that
the effects of attitude dissimulation on attitudes, B 5 .26, t (55) 5 1.99, p 5 .05,
and on thought favorability, B 5 .22, t (53) 5 1.63, p , .06 (one-tailed) were
significant. When attitude dissimulation and thought favorability were entered as
simultaneous predictors of attitudes, the effect of thought favorability on attitudes
was significant, B 5 .67, t (52) 5 6.60, p , .001, whereas the effect of attitude
dissimulation was not significant, B 5 .09, t (52) 5 0.90, ns. Thus, the effect of attitude
dissimulation on attitudes may have been mediated by message-relevant thoughts. Yet,
when attitude dissimulation and attitudes were entered as simultaneous predictors of
thoughts, the effect of attitudes was significant, B 5 .68, t (52) 5 6.60, p , .001,
whereas the effect of attitude dissimulation was not significant, B 5 .06, t (52) 5
0.55, ns. Thus, the effect of attitude dissimulation on message-relevant thoughts
may have been mediated by attitudes. Put simply, as in Experiment 1, participants’
thoughts appear to have affected their attitudes and vice versa. (See Table 2.)
Discussion
As in Experiment 1, expressing false evaluations of Albert Einstein did not
affect participants’ attitudes toward him. In addition, despite substantial changes
196
MAIO AND OLSON
in our procedure for eliciting attitude dissimulation, participants who expressed
false attitudes toward Einstein again indicated attitudes and thoughts that were
more favorable toward stopping nanotechnology than participants who did not
express false attitudes toward him. Also, mediational analyses suggested that the
effect of attitude dissimulation on attitudes was mediated by message-relevant
thoughts and that the effect of attitude dissimulation on thoughts was mediated by
attitudes. These findings replicated the mediational analyses in Experiment 1,
lending further support to the hypothesis that the effects on attitudes and thoughts
were reciprocally interdependent.
Importantly, these results add to the construct validity of our conclusions
because they revealed effects of attitude dissimulation using a procedure that was
very different from our previous experiment. Specifically, we elicited false
attitudes in a manner similar to low choice conditions in past cognitive dissonance
experiments, wherein participants were asked to write counterattitudinal essays.
These essays were written in a context that did not produce attitude change (low
choice and no aversive consequences). Nonetheless, such attitude dissimulation
caused participants’ true attitudes to have a larger effect on subsequent judgements than if no dissimulation occurred. Therefore, attitude dissimulation produced interesting consequences even though the circumstances that generate
attitude change were absent. It is also important to note that, unlike the first
experiment, participants in the attitude dissimulation condition believed that their
characterization of Einstein would be taken as their true attitude by someone;
therefore, this procedure created a closer analogue to real-world attitude dissimulation.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The principal novel finding in both experiments was that falsely evaluating the
source of a persuasive message can increase the influence of one’s true attitude on
yielding to a message. These results provide evidence for the practical importance
of our previous finding that lying about attitudes can make true attitudes more
accessible from memory (Maio & Olson, 1995). In addition, it is interesting that
attitude dissimulation affected message-relevant thoughts and attitudes simultaneously. These findings suggest that attitude dissimulation caused participants to
process the message in a biased manner, which affected their message-relevant
thoughts. Participants’ message-relevant thoughts and attitudes may then have
reciprocally affected one another (see Smith, 1982, for evidence of similar
reciprocal effects).
We should also note that supplementary analyses revealed that only 5.39% of
the total number of thoughts listed by participants referred to Albert Einstein.
Thus, Einstein did not appear to be a salient aspect of the article on nanotechnology. In fact, during the debriefing, many participants indicated that they felt that
their attitudes toward Einstein did not influence their agreement with his point of
view. This belief was inconsistent with the finding that the expression of false or
true attitudes toward Einstein affected attitudes toward banning nanotechnology.
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
197
Thus, it appears that participants may have been unaware of the effect of their
attitudes toward Einstein on their attitudes toward banning nanotechnology.
This lack of awareness is consistent with what would be expected if attitudes
toward Einstein were being spontaneously activated from memory during perusal
of the persuasive message. After spontaneous activation from memory, attitudes
can bias perception of attitude-relevant information without any conscious
awareness of the attitude (Fazio, 1990). In contrast, if attitudes are deliberately
accessed from memory, people should be more cognizant of their attitude. Given
that participants’ thoughts rarely referred to Einstein, it seems unlikely that
participants in our experiments deliberately accessed their attitudes toward
Einstein as a means of calculating their attitudes toward banning nanotechnology.
Instead, attitudes toward Einstein may have been activated spontaneously by the
citation of him in the persuasive message, particularly among participants who
expressed false (or true) attitudes toward him.
To investigate this hypothesized process more closely, it would be necessary to
test whether attitude dissimulation results in the spontaneous activation of true
attitudes from memory and whether this spontaneous activation mediates the
effects of attitude dissimulation. In the present experiments, it would have been
ideal to measure the activation of attitudes toward Einstein prior to message
exposure, but we did not do so because we were concerned that such a measure
might indirectly activate participants’ attitudes toward Einstein prior to presentation of the persuasive message, causing these attitudes to be accessible in all of
the experimental conditions. We hope in future research to include a direct
assessment of the mediating role of attitude accessibility.
In addition to raising such theoretical issues, the counterintuitive effect of
attitude dissimulation has interesting implications for understanding some realworld behavior. These implications are particularly evident in situations where
social norms put pressure on people to lie. In such situations, people might feel no
choice but to lie and think that their dissimulation helps them to avoid unnecessary social friction. Thus, the prerequisites for attitude dissimulation to cause
attitude change, namely freedom of choice and aversive consequences (see
Cooper & Fazio, 1984), might not be present.
An example of such a situation could be a party, where topics like politics are
often discussed. During such conversations, one individual might express strong
dislike for a particular political candidate. Politeness norms might then cause
another individual, who privately admires the candidate, to echo a few negative
things about him or her, while being aware of the numerous positive things that
could be said. Consequently, the person who said a few negative things about the
politician might become more aware of his or her liking for the politician. Our
findings suggest that one consequence of this increased awareness could be
increased agreement with the politician’s subsequent speeches (assuming that the
increased accessibility of the original attitude endured for some period of
time—an assumption not tested in our experiments).
The above example represents a real-world instance where attitude dissimula-
198
MAIO AND OLSON
tion might increase the impact of true attitudes. We expect that more serious
consequences of attitude dissimulation could occur in other contexts. For example, so-called ‘‘political correctness’’ pressures may cause some people who
strongly dislike particular minority groups to falsely express positive attitudes
toward the groups. Such attitude dissimulation would be designed to minimize
aversive consequences for the prejudiced individual, which means that it should
not induce attitude change. Instead, such dissimulation might make the true,
negative attitudes more accessible than if no attitude had been expressed. As a
result, the prejudiced person might be more likely to discriminate against
members of the minority groups, when they can do so anonymously or with
impunity, relative to having not expressed positive attitudes.
Interestingly, recent research has suggested similar paradoxical effects of
stereotype suppression. That is, when people are asked to not think about
particular negative stereotypes, these stereotypes tend to become more accessible
from memory and exert stronger effects on subsequent judgments and behavior
(e.g., Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, & Jetten, 1994). In general, trying to suppress
a thought tends to make it easier to retrieve from memory (e.g., Wegner, 1994). It
has been suggested that this effect occurs because of an ‘‘ironic’’ monitoring
process, wherein people must be vigilant against appearances of the suppressed
stereotype in consciousness. Paradoxically, such a process requires repeated
awareness of the stereotype in order to suppress it. Thus, the stereotype is made
more accessible.
In contrast, we expect that attitude dissimulation does not involve a vigilant
search process. Once it is decided that it is desirable to express a particular
attitude, people might express that attitude without worrying that their true
attitude will enter their awareness. That is, the true attitude might be primed only
in the first moments of attitude dissimulation, where it is automatically activated
from memory. On occasion, various psychological processes might extend the
duration of this priming, but these processes might not involve a vigilant search
process. For example, pressures to dissimulate might occasionally elicit psychological reactance (Brehm, 1966), which would cause recurrent contemplation of
one’s attitude. Future research could examine the extent to which suppression and
dissimulation have similar underlying mechanisms and effects.
Caveats and Conclusions
Our findings suggest that attitude dissimulation might produce the same effects
as truthful attitude expression, but there are some important caveats to this
conclusion. One important point is that attitude dissimulation can occur in a
variety of contexts, including face-to-face interaction, telephone conversations,
and written messages. Thus, the effects of dissimulation and expression need to be
compared across these contexts. We included a truthful attitude expression
condition only in Experiment 1 and not in Experiment 2, because our focus was
on the effects of attitude dissimulation. Future research comparing the effects of
attitude dissimulation and attitude expression across a variety of contexts might
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
199
yield more information about the processes underlying the effects of these
variables.
In addition, we suspect that there are many conditions under which attitude
dissimulation and attitude expression will produce dissimilar effects. First,
dissimilar effects could occur when attitude dissimulation produces attitude
change. Previous research has found that attitude change can result when people
decide to tailor their attitude expressions to suit their audience (e.g., Higgins &
Rholes, 1978; McCann, Higgins, & Fondacaro, 1991). Specifically, people’s true
attitudes become similar in valence to the previously false attitudes. When this
attitude change occurs, subsequent attitude-relevant judgments might become
consistent with the changed attitudes, rather than with the previously held
attitudes. (Such effects might be preempted if people initially hold strong,
unambiguous attitudes, which are less susceptible to change.)
Second, the frequency with which dissimulation has occurred might be an
important factor. Perhaps false attitudes become strongly associated in memory
with an attitude object after frequent dissimulations over an extended period of
time. After a large number of dissimulations, the strength of the association
between the attitude object and the false attitude might be strong enough to
override the impact of the true attitude on judgments. Our experiments did not
elicit repeated dissimulation over periods of days or weeks. This possibility could
be examined in future research.
In sum, our findings indicate that the effects of lying on subsequent judgments
and behavior may be more complex than has been documented previously. Social
psychologists have focused on the implications of lying for cognitive dissonance
and self-perception processes. Our studies show that lying has diverse possible
consequences. Given the ubiquity of deception in social behavior, it is important
that these consequences be understood.
REFERENCES
Arkes, H. R., Boehm, L. E., & Xu, G. (1991). Determinants of judged validity. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 576–605.
Bassili, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative indices of psychological attributes: The case
of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 637–653.
Bell, D. W., Esses, V. M., & Maio, G. R. (1996). The utility of open-ended measures to assess
intergroup ambivalence. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Sciences, 28, 12–18.
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). San Diego: Academic Press.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. San Diego: Academic Press.
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and
measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
1, 3–25.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1981). Social psychological procedures for cognitive response
assessment: The thought-listing technique. In T. V. Merluzzi, C. R. Glass, & M. Genest, Cognitive
assessment (pp. 309–342). New York: Guilford.
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus
message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.
200
MAIO AND OLSON
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and
beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp.
212–252). New York: Guilford.
Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–266). San Diego: Academic Press.
Cooper, J., & Scher, S. J. (1994). When do our actions affect our attitudes? In S. Shavitt & T. Brock
(Eds.), Persuasion: Psychological insights and perspectives (pp. 95–111). Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.
DePaulo, B. M., Stone, J. I., & Lassiter, G. D. (1985). Deceiving and detecting deceit. In B. R.
Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 323–370). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Values, stereotypes, and emotions as determinants
of intergroup attitudes. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and
stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 137–166). San Diego: Academic
Press.
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). New York: Academic Press.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, Il: Row, Peterson.
Gilbert, D. T., Krull, D. S., & Malone, P. S. (1990). Unbelieving the unbelievable: Some problems in
the rejection of false information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 601–613.
Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., & Nelson, D. E. (1996). Evidence that the
production of aversive consequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 5–16.
Higgins, E. T., & Rholes, W. J. (1978). ‘‘Saying is believing’’: Effects of message modification on
memory and liking for the person described. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14,
363–378.
Houston, D. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1989). Biased processing as a function of attitude accessibility:
Making objective judgments subjectively. Social Cognition, 7, 51–66.
Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (1995). Repetition and evaluative extremity. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick
(Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 43–71). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Krosnick, J. A., Boninger, D. S., Chuang, Y. C., Berent, M. K., & Carnot, C. G. (1993). Attitude
strength: One construct or many related constructs? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1132–1151.
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight:
Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–817.
Maio, G. R., Bell, D. W., & Esses, V. M. (1996). Ambivalence and persuasion: The processing of
persuasive messages about immigrant groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32,
513–536.
Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (1995). The effect of attitude dissimulation on attitude accessibility. Social
Cognition, 13, 127–144.
McCann, C. D., Higgins, E. T., & Fondacaro, R. A. (1991). Primacy and recency in communication
and self-persuasion: How successive audiences and multiple encodings influence subsequent
evaluative judgments. Social Cognition, 9, 47–66.
Millar, M. G., & Millar, K. U. (1990). Attitude change as a function of attitude type and argument type.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 217–228.
Olson, J. M. (1988). Misattribution, preparatory information, and speech anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 758–767.
Olson, J. M. (1990). Self-inference processes in emotion. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.),
Self-inference processes: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 6, pp. 17–41). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Olson, J. M. (1992). Self-perception of humor. Evidence for discounting and augmentation effects.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 369–377.
Olson, J. M., & Roese, N. J. (1995). The perceived funniness of humorous stimuli. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 908–913.
ATTITUDE DISSIMULATION
201
Olson, J. M., & Ross, M. (1988). False feedback about placebo effectiveness: Consequences for the
misattribution of speech anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 275–291.
Olson, J. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 44,
117–154.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). San Diego: Academic
Press.
Petty, R. E., Schumann, D. W., Richman, S. A., & Strathman, A. J. (1993). Positive mood and
persuasion: Different roles for affect under high- and low-elaboration conditions. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5–20.
Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). The accessibility of source likeability as a
determinant of persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(1), 19–25.
Scher, S. J., & Cooper, J. (1989). Motivational basis of dissonance: The singular role of behavioral
consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 899–906.
Smith, E. R. (1982). Beliefs, attributions, and evaluations: Nonhierarchical models of mediation in
social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 248–259.
Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 45, 5–19.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.
Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. E. (1985). Telling lies: Verbal and nonverbal correlates of deception. In
A. W. Siegman & J. S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp.
129–147). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.