Comparing Experiential Approaches: Structured Language
Learning Experiences versus Conversation Partners for
Changing Pre-Service Teacher Beliefs
Shannon McCrocklin1a
Abstract
ARTICLE HISTORY:
Received December 2019
Received in revised form February 2020
Accepted February 2020
Available online March 2020
KEYWORDS:
Teacher education
Experiential learning
Second language acquisition
Beliefs
Classroom-based research
Research has shown that language teachers’ beliefs are often
difficult to change through education. Experiential learning
may help, but more research is needed to understand how
experiential approaches shape perceptions. This study
compares two approaches, conversation partners (CONV)
and structured language learning experiences (SLLE),
integrated into a course in language acquisition. Participants
(n = 32) completed a pre- and post-questionnaire that
included: demographic questions, Likert scales on beliefs,
ranking of second language acquisition (SLA) factors, and
open-ended questions. Results showed differences from preto post-questionnaire for both groups for four Likert scale
items (e.g., the importance of exact pronunciation) and six
language learning factors (e.g., motivation). Further, both
groups grew to recognize more factors. Slight differences
emerged between groups on two items, the importance of
intelligence and dominance in SLA. Qualitative analysis
showed that, while CONV reported no changes in beliefs,
SLLE reported finding language learning to be more difficult
than believed. Further, SLLE wished to have more of the
project, while CONV did not.
© 2020 IJSCL. All rights reserved.
1
Assistant Professor, Email: shannon.mccrocklin@siu.edu
Tel:+1-618-4533428
a
Southern Illinois University, USA
S. McCrocklin/ International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 8(1), 2020
1. Introduction
I
n recent years, there has been increasing
interest in teacher cognition (Borg,
2015). Although there is a complex
relationship between teacher beliefs and teacher
actions, beliefs are foundational to actions and,
as such, are important to explore (Borg, 2011).
One strand of research in teacher cognition has
focused on how teacher beliefs are affected by
pre-service education. Although beliefs are
often resistant to change in pre-service teacher
education, experiential learning seems to be a
promising approach to changing problematic
beliefs. Despite research into the impact of
experiential approaches as part of teacher
education, only a few empirical studies have
examined structured language learning
experiences and even less have examined
conversation partners. Further, previous
research on conversation partners and
structured language learning experiences
focused on a single approach and did not make
comparisons. This study seeks to examine the
ways, if any, that these experiential approaches
affect pre-service teacher beliefs and compares
the impact of each.
2. Theoretical Framework
2.1. Language Teacher Education
Education for effective language teachers must
include not only information about the
language of instruction, principles of L2
development, and approaches to teaching
(Senior, 2006) but also must increase awareness
of teaching practices and beliefs through
reflection and critical evaluation (Farrell,
2018). Student-teacher beliefs, however, are
often ingrained and difficult to change through
education programs (Moodie, 2016; Tatto,
1998; Wideen, Mayer-Smith, & Moon, 1998;
Wubbels, 1992). Research has shown that
language teachers are often heavily influenced
by their own language-learning experiences
(Borg, 2003, 2011; Ellis, 2016; Kagan, 1992;
Moodie, 2016), and student teachers often
comb through received information to support
and strengthen pre-existing notions, instead of
challenging and re-evaluating beliefs (Kagan,
1992).
Yet, work to examine and improve teacher
education shows that programs can affect
beliefs (Blume, Gerlach, Roters, & Schmidt,
ISSN 2329-2210
2019; Busch, 2010; Kavanoz, Yüksel, & Varol,
2017; Mattheodakis, 2007; Mighani &
Moghadam, 2019). Mattheodakis (2007)
conducted a longitudinal study following
students through a four-year teacher-education
program for prospective English as a foreign
language (EFL) instructors and found that
several pre-service teacher beliefs did change.
Mattheodakis noted that education influenced
about half of the examined beliefs. Students’
beliefs about the importance of grammar and
pronunciation in language learning tended to
decrease as students encountered evidence and
re-examined their original beliefs. Half of the
beliefs were resistant to change, however, such
as beliefs about the relative difficulty of
language learning. Busch (2010) also
conducted a longitudinal study over the course
of three years, examining the beliefs of preservice teachers (n = 381) enrolled in a teachereducation program. She found that participants
reported numerous changed beliefs. Further,
while initial beliefs were often reported to be
based on previous language learning
experiences, changes of beliefs were attributed
to experiential learning programs.
2.2. Experiential Learning
Given the challenges in encouraging
prospective teachers to evaluate their beliefs,
researchers should continue to explore possible
improvements to teacher-preparation programs.
One path to improving language-teacher
education may involve experiential learning, in
which learning is viewed as something to be
shaped (and re-shaped) through experience
(Kolb & Kolb, 2005; Moon, 2004). An
important element of the experiential learning
design is reflection (Blume et al., 2019; Busch,
2010; Wright, 2010). “Teachers do not simply
reproduce their own experience in the
classroom. However, if they are to transcend its
effects, it is reflection on the experience that is
critical” (Ellis, 2006, p. 2). Matic (2011) noted
that student teachers often lacked the ability to
reflect critically on their own learning
experiences, being unable, for example, to
identify specific reasons why they enjoyed
communicative activities more than grammartranslation, which prevented them from
applying those realizations to their own
teaching. Experiential learning allows the
learner’s beliefs to be examined through new
educational experiences, with reflection
71
72
Comparing Experiential Approaches
leading to more refined ideas that may be better
aligned to new theories or methods of teaching
(Kolb, 1984). Reflection can, therefore, help
resolve tensions or competitions between
beliefs and new information (Farrell, 2006).
The most common channel for introducing
experiential learning and reflection in L2
teacher education is teaching practicums (Borg,
2009; Crandall, 2000). Additional methods to
enhance experiential learning have included
conversation partners and structured language
learning experiences (SLLEs). Although
conversation partners are recommended for L2
learners as a strategy for promoting acquisition
(Oxford, 2002), there seems to be limited
research into the use of conversational partners
as part of ESL teacher education, specifically.
One key study, Biondo Salomão (2011),
explored the use of tandem conversation
partners, in which people with two different
native languages serve as conversation partners
for the other, finding that student teachers
found the experience useful for learning to
describe their language in contrast with another,
leading the author to recommend conversation
partners be a part of teacher-education
programs. Additionally, Keengwe (2010)
explored the use of conversational partners as
part of a multicultural education course for
elementary education majors, finding that preservice teachers partnered with ESL students
from a local language institute valued the
experience and reported greater appreciation of
linguistic and cultural diversity after the
experience.
Another approach to integrating experiential
learning in language-teacher education is for
pre-service teachers to re-enter the role of
language learners through what Ellis (2006)
terms a structured language learning experience
(SLLE). Ellis notes that SLLEs may range from
a single short lesson to a semester of study, but
a key element is using the experience to
encourage reflection on language teaching from
the student perspective. Weed (1993)
advocated for speaking in an unknown
language to prospective teachers in short
stretches and then encouraging the student
teachers to reflect, both exploring emotional
responses as well as critical reflections on the
ways language modifications can affect
comprehension. The ability of SLLEs to evoke
emotional reactions, specifically frustration,
was also noted in Washburn (2008) and WrightMaley and Green (2015). Further, SLLEs can
help learners connect received information
(content information presented in the
classroom) with experiential knowledge and
provide prospective teachers a chance to test
out their ideas about second language learning
(Flowerdew, 1998).
While it is clear that teacher educators and
researchers have explored various experiential
approaches to ESL teacher education, no
studies were identified that worked to compare
two experiential projects. More research is
needed to understand the ways that different
experiential approaches may impact and shape
perceptions of second language acquisition
(SLA). Additionally, some of the findings have
been based on anecdotal evidence or small case
studies (e.g., Washburn, 2008; Weed, 1993).
More work is needed that seeks to empirically
examine changes to pre-service teachers’
beliefs as a result of particular interventions or
experiences. Therefore, this study compares
two experiential approaches, conversation
partners, and structured language-learning
experiences, as part of teacher education,
exploring the ways each approach affects
changes in beliefs. Specifically, this research
study seeks to answer the following questions:
1. For students participating in a course in
language acquisition, in what ways do
structured language learning experiences
(SLLE)
and
conversation
partner
experiences (CONV):
a. affect beliefs about second language
learning
b. increase recognition of a number of
factors that affect SLA?
2. What are the students’ reactions to the two
experiential projects?
3. Methodology
The present study was conducted at a university
in the South of Texas in the United States of
America. It was conducted in two sections of an
advanced undergraduate course in language
acquisition that covered first language
acquisition, SLA, and bilingualism. The course
counted for credit in degree plans, including
majors and minors, in English as a Second
Language, English, English Education, and
Bilingual Education. All students enrolled in
the course participated in the activities, but
S. McCrocklin/ International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 8(1), 2020
students were provided informed consent and
could decline to contribute their data to the
study.
3.1. Participants
After receiving information about the study, 32
students provided consent for their data to be
included in the study. The participants included
ISSN 2329-2210
66% females and 34% males with an average
age of 24. The majority of participants (62.5%)
were English (which includes English
Education) or Education (Elementary or
Bilingual) majors. An additional four students
were Spanish majors, while a variety of other
majors were also represented including
Psychology, Biology, and Kinesiology. Table 1
shows the data for each group.
Table 1
Participant Characteristics by Group
Number of Participants
Average Age
% Bilingual in Spanish/English
% Studied/Learned an L2 after age 12
Gender
University Major
Notably, the majority of participants, 69%,
reported being bilingual in Spanish and
English. This includes students who began
learning both languages before the age of six
and those who reported advanced proficiency in
both languages. Bilingualism is common in
South Texas and families are often able to
maintain bilingualism into their fourth and fifth
generations (Anderson-Mejías, 2005). Research
suggests that bilinguals may have greater
insight into second language (L2) learning than
monolinguals in explaining how English
functions to create meaning (Ellis, 2004), better
understanding the challenges of identity
formation across two cultures (Ellis, 2006), and
experience trying to communicate in an L2
(Ellis, 2006). However, the researcher noted
that in previous semesters of the course,
bilinguals were also sometimes judgmental of
monolinguals and adult L2 learners, thinking
them lazy or unintelligent not to have acquired
an L2. Such judgmental comments, uttered with
concerning frequency, formed much of the
impetus for re-envisioning and reformatting the
language acquisition course and creating this
study.
3.2. Experiential Treatments
Each section, offered in different semesters,
experienced one of the experiential learning
CONV
11
23.5
64%
36%
Male= 5
Female= 6
Education= 1
English= 7
Spanish= 1
Other= 2
SLLE
21
25
71%
29%
Male= 6
Female= 15
Education = 10
English= 3
Spanish= 3
Other= 5
projects, either conversation partners (CONV)
or the structured language learning experience
(SLLE). The project was embedded into the
language acquisition course as a major project
relating to the SLA unit and goals. However, as
SLA was only one portion of the course, usually
comprising about 40% of the semester, it was
important to keep the in-class time for both
projects reasonable and limited.
3.2.1. Structured Language Learning Experience
(SLLE)
Participants in the structured language learning
experience (SLLE) worked through the
semester to learn French. Half of the work for
the SLLE was completed through Duolingo, a
web-based language-learning platform also
available as an application for Android and
iPhone. Although Duolingo has been criticized
for inauthentic language and a strict curriculum
(Cunningham, 2015), it has been shown to be
effective in helping students improve their
language abilities (Vesselinov & Grego, 2012)
and is free and convenient. Students completed
11 units of French Lessons within Duolingo,
estimated to total three to four hours of practice.
After completing the basic units, students
progressed through units such as food, animals,
adjectives, plurals, colors, and possessives.
73
74
Comparing Experiential Approaches
Further, students took part in four 50-minute
lessons in class. The in-class lessons built upon
material that students learned through
Duolingo, such as greetings, which students
were first introduced to during the basic units.
Each in-class lesson focused on creating
communicative experiences; for example,
students introduced themselves to each other in
the greetings lesson and completed a restaurant
role-play to practice food vocabulary. In-class
lessons also included activities from additional
methods, such as Total Physical Response,
which was used as a warm-up in the first lesson.
Throughout the semester, students were asked
to respond in short paragraphs (guidance given
to submit four to five sentences) to six different
reflective prompts, such as “After a few basic
lessons in French, how would you feel trying
the language with a native speaker? What do
you feel like you have mastered at this point?”
and “What was your favorite activity to learn
French? Why?” Often, these were completed at
the end of the in-class French lessons, but due
to time constraints some had to be assigned for
homework.
3.2.2. Conversation Partner Experience (CONV)
Participants in the conversation partner
experience (CONV) were matched with an ESL
student at the campus-affiliated language
institute at the beginning of the semester.
Students were provided contact information for
their partner match and were expected to
arrange times for meeting that worked for both
parties. Students were required to meet with
their partner at least four times during the
semester, each for 50-minute sessions.
Students were required to write a report and
reflective essay at the conclusion of the four
meetings. The essay assignment provided
guidance on length (1000-1500 words) along
with reflective prompts, such as, “What do you
think went well during your meetings? Is there
anything that did not go well? Why?” and,
“What do you feel that you learned about
second language acquisition from this
conversation partner experience?” Students
submitted the reflective reports online through
the course management website.
As this project depended upon the
responsiveness and reliability of their ESL
partner matches, who voluntarily signed up to
take part in the project to attain more English
practice, some students struggled to arrange
meetings and persistence was sometimes
required to ensure partner follow-through.
Students who continued to struggle to arrange a
meeting with their initial partner were rematched in the second half of the semester.
However, two students continued to face issues
in arranging a meeting. As such, it was
necessary to create a comparable alternative
assignment that would still foster exposure to
L2 learners. For these cases, students prepared
a list of interview questions and identified two
L2 learners (of any language) to interview.
Their essay, then, included less reflection
(although this was still a required component)
to
make
space
for
the
increased
research/journalistic expectations for their
findings.
3.3. Instruments
To measure changes in beliefs, participants
completed a pre-questionnaire during the first
week of the semester and a post-questionnaire
in the final week. The pre-questionnaire
included demographic questions (including
age, gender, major and information about
languages learned/known), 10 Likert scale
ratings on beliefs about language learning, and
identification and ranking of 13 factors that
affect SLA.
Similar to Mattheoudakis (2007) and Horwitz
(1985), students were presented in the pre- and
post-questionnaire with 10 Likert scale items in
which participants ranked their agreement with
popular beliefs about language learning on a
six-point scale, ranging from 1- Strongly
Disagree to 6- Strongly Agree. Nine of the
items were chosen from Lightbrown and Spada
(2013). In Chapter 9 of How Languages are
Learned, the authors work to introduce 18
popular ideas about L2 learning and to clarify
the truth of the issue. The popular ideas had
varying levels of truth and substance, ranging
from mostly false to somewhat true. Although
there was not a single true/false or right/wrong
answer to these statements, they reflect ideas
that are popular outside of the field and had the
potential to illuminate beliefs of participants.
The final item included in the Likert scales was
chosen based on researcher experiences with
bilinguals in the region, which hinted that
another popular belief may be, “Learning a
second language is easy”.
S. McCrocklin/ International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 8(1), 2020
ISSN 2329-2210
within-subjects factor of TIME (pre-/post-) and
a between-subjects factor of GROUP
(CONV/SLLE) with effect sizes calculated as
partial eta squared (ηp²). For survey items
identified as having potential significant
differences when examining TIME * GROUP,
a post-hoc paired samples t-test was used to
compare pre- and post-responses for each
group, with effect sizes calculated as Cohen’s d.
Participants were also provided with a list of
thirteen factors that affect language learning.
The factors were selected from textbooks
introducing language acquisition: Brown
(2014), Lightbrown and Spada (2013), and
Ortega (2008). Participants were asked to circle
any factors on the list that they believed
affected SLA and then, for those circled, rank
the factors in importance with one being the
most important factor. For example, if a student
selected three factors, such as self-esteem,
motivation, and learning style, they were then
asked to rank those three for relative
importance. If a feature was not circled, it
received a ranking of 14 (one step below 13,
which would have been the lowest ranking
possible, if a participant did circle all factors).
For the open-ended responses, the researcher
examined each written response identifying
themes mentioned by two or more participants.
An additional reviewer also examined the
responses to identify themes and provided
feedback that informed subsequent analyses.
After a second round of review and analysis,
responses were coded and tallied to provide
counts and percentages for themes.
The post-questionnaire included the same
Likert scale ratings and ranking of factors tasks
from the pre-questionnaire, along with openended questions about the project, such as, “In
what ways did this project further your
understanding of adult second language
acquisition?” and, “If I do this project again in
another semester, what are two suggestions you
have to make the project more useful for other
students?”.
4. Results
4.1. Changing Beliefs about L2 Learning
In order to measure changes in beliefs about
language learning, students ranked their
strength of agreement with 10 statements. Table
2 shows each of the statements and, then, for
each group, the average pre- and post-course
ratings, and the difference calculated by
subtracting pre from post. In the comparisons
section, results from the mixed ANOVA,
including the significance and effect sizes of
TIME and TIME * GROUP are included.
3.4. Data Analysis
To avoid running numerous t-tests, which can
increase the chances of a Type 1 error, results
were examined using a mixed ANOVA with the
Table 2
Pre- and Post- Likert Scale Statement Rankings by Group with Comparison Results from Mixed ANOVA
SLLE
Comparisons
Pre-
Post-
Difference
Pre-
Post-
Difference
Sign. TIME
ηp² TIME
Sign.
TIME
GROUP
ηp² TIME * GROUP
*
CONV
1.
People learn languages by imitating
native speakers.
4.55
3.64
-0.91
3.9
3.67
-0.23
.103
.086
.262
.042
2.
Highly intelligent people are good
language learners.
3.36
3.36
0.00
3.48
2.14
-1.34
.014
.187
.067
.107
3.
The best predictor of success in
language learning is motivation.
4.54
5.45
0.91
4.57
5.0
0.43
.011
.197
.306
.035
4.
The earlier a L2 is learned the greater
the likelihood of success.
5.27
5.0
-0.27
5.62
5.67
-0.05
.690
.005
.335
.031
75
76
Comparing Experiential Approaches
5.
Most of the mistakes that L2 learners
make are due to interference from their
first language.
The best way to learn new vocabulary
is through reading.
4.27
4.09
-0.18
4.52
4.28
-0.24
.429
.021
.923
.000
4.72
5.18
0.46
4.14
4.14
0.00
.493
.016
.346
.030
It is essential for learners to be able to
pronounce all the individual sounds in
the L2
8. L2
teachers
should
present
grammatical rules one at a time and
have learners practice before moving
on to the next rule.
9. Learners' errors should be corrected as
soon as they are made in order to
prevent the formation of bad habits.
10. Learning a second language is easy.
4.36
4.18
-0.18
4.33
4.19
-0.14
.027
.017
.932
.000
4.36
3.91
-0.45
5.00
4.29
-0.71
.027
.153
.649
.007
4.09
4.27
0.18
5.19
4.76
-0.43
.376
.026
.243
.045
2.36
2.36
0.00
2.67
2.19
-0.48
.160
.065
.306
.035
6.
7.
Results showed statistically significant
differences from pre- to post-questionnaire for
four Likert scale items: 2. highly intelligent
people are good language learners (p = .014,
ηp² = .187), 3. the best predictor of success in
language learning is motivation (p = .011, ηp² =
.197), 7. it is essential for learners to be able to
pronounce all the individual sounds in the L2 (p
= .027, ηp² = .017), and 8. L2 teachers should
present grammatical rules one at a time and
have learners practice before moving on to the
next rule (p = .027, ηp² = .153). With the
exception of the pronunciation item, which
featured a small effect size, the other three
items displayed a medium effect size when
examining the effect of TIME.
At the post-questionnaire, groups showed
weakened agreement to the statements asserting
the exact pronunciation of L2 sounds was
essential and grammar rules should be
presented one at a time. The waning emphasis
on grammar and pronunciation instruction
parallels Mattheoudakis (2007), which showed
that strong beliefs regarding grammar and
pronunciation instruction are susceptible to
change as students encounter evidence to
challenge preconceived notions. Both groups
were more likely to agree, though, that
motivation is critical in language learning.
While motivation is complex, it is generally
accepted as critical for successful adult SLA
(Lightbrown & Spada, 2013) and both groups
gained a greater understanding of its
importance.
The only difference to emerge between the
groups within the Likert scale data was that
SLLE reported declining agreement that highly
intelligent people are more likely to be good
language learners at the post-questionnaire
(#2). CONV showed no difference on this item.
This is also the only Likert item approaching
significance among the interactions of TIME *
GROUP (p = 0.067, ηp² = .107), although the
effect size was medium. As a post-hoc test, a
paired samples t-test was used to compare the
pre- to post-questionnaire scores on this item.
While CONV showed no difference (0.00),
SLLE showed a lessening agreement (-1.34)
which was statistically significant (p = .006)
with a medium effect size (as calculated by
Cohen’s d) of .65. This change of belief is likely
connected to SLLE experiencing the challenges
of learning an L2; students may have felt their
ego threatened as they struggled with the task,
perhaps realizing that their own intelligence
only took them so far in learning an L2.
4.2. Recognizing Factors that Affect SLA
To measure awareness of factors influencing
SLA on the pre- and post-questionnaire,
participants were provided with a list of thirteen
factors known to affect language learning.
Participants circled any factors they believed
affected SLA and then, for those circled, ranked
the factors in importance. Table 3 below shows
each factor that was included along with the
number of factors circled. For each group, the
pre- and post-scores are included, along with
the difference subtracting pre- from postquestionnaire. For the number of factors
circled, a positive difference number indicates
more factors recognized, while in remaining
rows a positive difference indicates the factor
became less important (was ranked lower in
importance). In the comparisons section, results
from the mixed ANOVA, including the
significance and effect sizes of TIME and
TIME * GROUP are included.
S. McCrocklin/ International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 8(1), 2020
ISSN 2329-2210
Table 3
Pre- and Post- Recognition of SLA Factors by Group with Comparison Results from Mixed ANOVA
Comparisons
Pre-
Post-
Difference
Pre-
Post-
Difference
Sign. TIME
ηp² TIME
Sign. TIME
GROUP
ηp² TIME
GROUP
*
SLLE
*
CONV
Number of Factors Circled
7.64
8.64
1.00
5.52
7.86
2.34
.001
.322
.212
.051
Age
Motivation
Intelligence
Learning Style
Introversion/Extroversion
Inhibition
Anxiety
Willingness to Communicate
Self-Esteem
Empathy
Dominance
Ethnic Group Affiliation
Attitudes toward L2 Cultural
Group
4.09
3.82
11.09
7.27
10.91
11.91
12.18
3.36
9.18
13.09
10.82
9.09
6.54
5.91
1.36
12.00
9.18
7.18
10.82
6.81
3.55
6.27
11.36
12.82
11.27
4.91
1.82
-1.46
0.91
1.91
-3.73
-1.09
-5.37
0.19
-2.91
-1.73
2.00
2.18
-1.63
3.86
3.05
9.24
5.67
10.38
13.19
11.19
7.52
11.43
14.00
13.29
11.86
9.86
3.29
2.42
12.57
6.42
8.52
12.48
8.14
5.81
6.38
12.67
12.52
11.10
6.57
-0.57
-0.63
3.33
0.75
-1.86
-0.71
-3.05
-1.71
-5.05
-1.33
-0.77
-0.76
-3.29
.762
.119
.017
.255
.007
.220
.000
.360
.000
.016
.798
.787
.006
.003
.079
.174
.043
.218
.050
.338
.028
.442
.179
.002
.002
.222
.175
.272
.256
.588
.312
.792
.271
.437
.260
.747
.080
.137
.405
.060
.040
.043
.010
.034
.002
.040
.020
.042
.004
.099
.072
.023
When examining the effect of TIME, the
number of factors identified as impacting SLA
increased, a statistically significant difference
(p = .001), with a large effect size (ηp²=.322).
Both groups became increasingly aware of the
importance of five factors that affect SLA:
Introversion/Extroversion (p=.007, ηp² = .218),
Anxiety (p = .000, ηp² = .338), Self-Esteem (p
= .000, ηp² = .442), Empathy (p = .016, ηp² =
.179), and Attitudes toward the L2 Cultural
Group (p = .006, ηp² = .222). Effect sizes
ranged from medium for introversion/
extroversion, empathy, and attitudes towards
L2 cultural group to large for anxiety and selfesteem. The increased recognition of factors
suggests both groups did develop a deeper
perspective on language learning. Additionally,
despite differences on the Likert scale item on
intelligence, both groups downgraded the
importance of intelligence in the postquestionnaire (p = .017, ηp² = .174) with a
medium effect size.
Only one SLA factor, dominance, approached
significance when exploring the interaction of
TIME * GROUP (p = 0.080). While CONV
found dominance to be less important on the
post-test (+ 2.00) SLLE showed a greater
recognition of the impact of dominance in L2
learning (-0.77). Although the result is
somewhat surprising, given that dominance
should be most noticeable in the context of
interaction, with which CONV had greater
experience, SLLE interacted with classmates
through communicative activities which may
have led to recognition of the role of dominance
for learners in taking turns, holding the floor,
and otherwise obtaining opportunities for
practice. As a post-hoc test, a paired samples ttest was used to compare the pre- to postquestionnaire responses to this item. For CONV
and SLLE the pre- to post-questionnaire
differences were not statistically significant (p
= .155 & .391, respectively), however, their
opposite movement is appreciable.
4.3. Student Perceptions
Despite limited differences between groups
quantitatively, the qualitative analysis of the
open-ended responses showed noticeable
differences. When asked how the project helped
further understanding of SLA, CONV focused
on age as a factor, while SLLE regularly
mentioned the importance of motivation. Over
a third of CONV (36%) described learning the
importance of age in language learning, while
only 5% of SLLE noted age. In detailing how
his understanding had grown, one CONV
student wrote, “The older you get, the harder it
becomes to develop a second language”. Age
was the only factor mentioned by more than one
77
78
Comparing Experiential Approaches
student in CONV. On the other hand, over a
third of SLLE commented on the importance of
motivation in language learning. One
participant wrote, “Learning a second language
helped me understand that it takes a lot of
practice, motivation, and determination to
succeed in learning a second language”. The
only other factor mentioned by more than one
SLLE participant was anxiety, commented on
by two participants (10%). One participant
wrote, “This project helped me understand the
challenges faced by second language learners
… The fear and anxiety of not being able to
‘ace’ second language learning hit me pretty
hard. But, in that way, it helped me
understand”.
More importantly, when asked how the course
changed student beliefs about L2 learning, the
most common response (36%) from CONV
was that it did not change beliefs. One
participant wrote, “Honestly, my beliefs
haven’t changed much. The course has simply
solidified and refreshed me on materials
regarding [second language learning].” This
solidification of beliefs echoes findings of
Kagan (1992) who found that students sift
through received information to find support
for, instead of challenges to, pre-existing
notions. On the other hand, around a quarter of
SLLE (24%) pointed out that, at the beginning
of the semester, they thought language learning
would not be particularly difficult, a belief that
changed during the course. One participant
said, “I honestly didn’t think that second
language learning would be as hard as it really
was”. Similarly, another student said, “I had
read about potential challenges and how
difficult it may be, but I really thought it would
be a piece of cake until I actually did it myself”.
Some students assumed that similarities
between French and Spanish would help them.
One participant wrote, “I thought it would be
easy to learn French due to its similarities to
Spanish. I was completely wrong. More work
has to be put into it”. The SLLE was surprising
to students, reproducing the goals of Weed
(1993) and Washburn (2008) and did allow
students to test out notions about language
learning similar to Flowerdew (1998). These
findings may also indicate that SLLEs have the
potential to change a resistant idea identified by
Mattheoudakis (2007), the relative difficulty of
language learning, although no statistically
significant changes were identified for the
Likert item, “Learning a second language is
easy”.
Despite finding the SLLE project challenging,
48% of SLLE participants specifically
mentioned wanting more of the project. When
making recommendations for future semesters,
students mentioned wanting more language
days, songs, and practice. They wanted it to be
“more of a main component of the course”. On
the other hand, only one participant in CONV
(9%) mentioned wanting to have more of the
project, perhaps partially due to initial
frustrations contacting and setting up meetings
with their ESL partners. In describing potential
changes to the project in future semesters,
several participants in CONV voiced
frustrations about arranging meetings. One
participant recommended connecting students
with their conversation partners through
WhatsApp to get people in contact more easily,
a strategy that had worked well for her.
However, students also indicated that working
to add other elements to the project may have
made it more successful, such as using it to
gather data about learner errors or including
more reflection in class about the conversation
partner sessions. The CONV responses did not
align with previous studies that suggested
students may find value in the chance to explain
aspects of the language or may grow to value
cultural diversity (Biondo Salomão, 2011;
Keengwe, 2010). However, the lack of
comments regarding diversity may be due to
CONV partners being primarily native Spanish
speakers from Mexico, partially sharing a
cultural background with many of the CONV
students.
5. Concluding Remarks
Following theories of experiential learning,
which suggest that learner growth is most likely
to occur through experiences that allow
students to re-examine their beliefs (Kolb,
1984), this study explored the use of
experiential projects to enhance an upper-level
course in language acquisition. Although
previous studies have examined SLLEs as part
of experiential learning for language teachers in
education programs (Ellis, 2006; Flowerdew,
1998; Wright-Maley & Green, 2015), few
studies have examined conversation partners
(primarily Biondo Salomão, 2011 and
Keengwe, 2010), and no studies were identified
that worked to compare the two approaches.
S. McCrocklin/ International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 8(1), 2020
ISSN 2329-2210
This study compared SLLE and conversation
partner projects, each embedded into onecourse section, using pre- and postquestionnaires to track changes in beliefs.
into a teaching methods course. Alternatively,
the project could fit into a course devoted to
specific language skills, such as a course in
teaching L2 speaking and listening.
When examining the quantitative data, CONV
and SLLE responded similarly to their differing
experiential projects. According to the Likert
scale agreement ratings, both groups became
more aware of the importance of motivation in
language learning, while decreasing the
emphasis put on ordered, careful grammar
instruction and learners’ exact pronunciation of
the L2 sound inventory. Both groups also
became increasingly aware of the many factors
that can affect SLA, circling on average one
(CONV) to two (SLLE) more recognized
factors in the post-questionnaire. Both
increasingly recognized the importance of
introversion/extroversion, anxiety, self-esteem,
empathy, and attitudes toward the L2 cultural
group. The numerous changes across groups,
along with Mattheoudakis (2007) and
Kavaonoz, Yüksel, and Varol (2017), further
support that education can be effective in
changing many pre-service teacher beliefs
about language learning.
Further, both projects were reasonably easy for
the instructor to accomplish. For the SLLE, the
researcher managed homework through an
online dashboard and facilitated a limited
number of in-class lessons. The researcher was
able to utilize a language she had previously
studied. For the conversation partners, the
instructor arranged sign-up sheets to be
disbursed at the intensive language institute and
simply matched the volunteers to a student.
However, given the challenges that arose in this
project, instructors should explore additional
formats for conversation partner implementation,
addressing the logistics carefully to prevent
frustration. Further, instructors may wish to
examine other aspects of the project
arrangements, such as the nature and timing of
the reflections completed.
However, differences emerged on two of the
quantitative measures, primarily the importance
of intelligence and dominance in SLA. When
examining
the
open-ended
responses,
additional differences emerged. In addition to
diverging foci (CONV: age, SLLE: motivation),
students showed dramatic differences in
perceptions of the value of the projects. Perhaps
most importantly, students in CONV did not
feel that the project substantially changed their
views of SLA, were frustrated by the logistics
of the project, and did not report wanting more
of the project. On the other hand, students in
SLLE found the project challenging, opening
their eyes to the difficulty of learning a second
language, and wanted more time and activities
dedicated to the project.
For teacher education programs interested in
implementing more experiential learning, both
projects may be useful considering both did
change prospective teachers’ beliefs. However,
these results suggest that SLLEs may be more
suitable as part of language-teacher education
as students reported valuing the SLLE project
more. An SLLE project could reasonably fit
into numerous courses; in addition to a
language acquisition course, it may fit easily
It is important to recognize the limitations of
the study, however. First, it was challenging to
control for time on task in the two different
groups. Both groups completed the same
number of readings on SLA and had roughly
the same amount of time for interaction (four
50-minute sessions in which CONV met with
their language partner or SLLE tried activities
with classmates during course time). To try to
match the time on task for SLLE, which also
had significant work in Duolingo outside of
class, CONV had more class time allotted to
discussing the readings. In the end, the overall
time on task for the SLA portion of the course
was reasonably equal, but CONV had slightly
less time for the experiential project.
Additionally, the CONV group faced greater
difficulties in getting started in their project
because they depended on their conversation
partner to set up meetings. This frustration
affected students’ reactions to the project.
Further, although rates of participation in the
study across both sections were similar, the
CONV group was about half the size of the
SLLE due to lower enrollment in the CONV
semester. Small numbers of participants in
CONV may have made it more difficult to
identify differing trends in the data. Finally, the
composition of the groups differed in
proportion of varying majors, which may have
affected students’ learning goals for the course.
79
80
Comparing Experiential Approaches
However, the attempts to compare two
approaches identified some key differences.
Future research is needed to continue to
compare various forms of experiential learning.
For example, researchers could compare oneon-one experiences (e.g., conversation partners
and tutoring experiences) or compare various
teaching experiences (e.g., tutoring versus
whole class teaching practicums) to continue to
find the ways that each experiential learning
approach affects pre-service teacher beliefs.
This information could help programs deploy
pedagogical interventions to address particular
student needs. Teacher education programs
could, potentially, assess prospective teacher
beliefs at the beginning of a program and utilize
a specific experiential project (or a combination
of experiential projects) to address problematic
beliefs or specific needs.
References
Anderson-Mejias, P. L. (2005). Generation and
Spanish language use in the lower Rio
Grande Valley of Texas. Southwest
Journal of Linguistics, 24(1), 1-12.
Biondo Salomão, A. C. (2011). Collaborative
language learning in Teletandem: A
resource for pre-service teacher
education. Profile Issues in Teachers
Professional Development, 13(1), 139156.
Blume, C., Gerlach, D., Roters, B., & Schmidt,
T. (2019). The ABCs of inclusive
English teacher education: A quantitative
and qualitative study examining the
attitudes, beliefs and (reflective)
competence of pre-service foreign
language teachers. TESL-EJ, 22(4), 1-18.
Borg, S. (2015). Teacher cognition and
language education (2nd ed.). New York,
NY: Bloomsbury Academic.
Borg, S. (2011). The impact of in-service
teacher education on language teachers'
beliefs. System, 39(3), 370-380.
Borg, S. (2009). Language teacher cognition. In
A. Burns, & J. C. Richards (Eds.), The
Cambridge guide to second language
teacher education (pp. 163-171). New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Borg, S. (2003). Teacher cognition in language
teaching: A review of research on what
language teachers think, know, believe,
do. Language Teaching, 36, 81-109.
Brown, H. D. (2014). Principles of language
learning and teaching: A course in L2
acquisition (6th ed.). White Plains, NY:
Pearson.
Busch, D. (2010). Pre-service teacher beliefs
about language learning: The second
language acquisition course as an agent
for change. Language Teaching Research,
14(3), 318-337.
Crandall, J. (2000). Language teacher
education. Annual Review of Applied
Linguistics, 20, 34-55.
Cunningham, K. J. (2015). Duolingo [Review].
TESL-EJ, 19(1), 1-9.
Ellis, E. M. (2016)."I may be a native speaker
but I'm not monolingual": Reimagining
all teachers' linguistic identities in
TESOL. TESOL Quarterly, 50(3), 597630.
Ellis, E. M. (2006). Language learning
experiences as a contributor to ESOL
teacher cognition. TESL-EJ, 10(1), 1-20.
Ellis, E. M. (2004). The invisible multilingual
teacher: The contribution of language
background to Australian ESL teachers’
professional knowledge and beliefs. The
International Journal of Multilingualism,
1(2), 90-108.
Farrell, T. S. C. (2018). Reflective language
teaching: Practical applications for
TESOL teachers (2nd ed.). New York,
NY: Bloomsbury Academic.
Farrell, T. S. C. (2006). The teacher is an
octopus: Uncovering pre-service language
teachers’ beliefs through metaphor
analysis. RELC Journal, 37(2), 326–248.
Flowerdew, J. (1998). Language learning
experience in L2 teacher education.
TESOL Quarterly, 32(3), 529 - 535.
Horwitz, E. K. (1985). Using student beliefs
about language learning and teaching in
the foreign language methods course.
Foreign Language Annals, 18(4), 333340.
Kagan, D. M. (1992). Implications of research
on
teacher
belief.
Educational
Psychologist, 27(1), 65-90.
Kavanoz, S., Yüksel, H. G., & Varol, B. (2017).
Evolvement of pre-service language
teachers’ beliefs through teacher
education. International Journal of
Progressive Education, 13(1), 119-135.
Keengwe, J. (2010). Fostering cross cultural
competence in preservice teachers
through
multicultural
education
S. McCrocklin/ International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 8(1), 2020
experiences. Early Childhood Education
Journal, 38(3), 197-204.
Kolb, A.Y., & Kolb, D. A. (2005). Learning
styles and learning spaces: enhancing
experiential learning in higher education.
Academy of Management Learning &
Education, 4(2), 193-212.
Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning:
Experience as the source of learning and
development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Lightbrown, P., & Spada, N. (2013). How
languages are learned (4th ed.). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Matic, M. (2011). The English language
learning experience and its impact on the
future English language teachers’ career.
Procedia Social and Behavioral
Sciences, 11, 132-135.
Mattheoudakis, M. (2007). Tracking changes in
pre-service EFL teacher beliefs in
Greece: A longitudinal study. Teaching
and Teacher Education, 23, 1272-1288.
Mighani, M. G., & Moghadam, M. Y. (2019).
Building intercultural sensitivity in preservice EFL teachers through interactive
culture-focused
speaking
tasks.
International Journal of Society,
Culture, & Language, 7(2), 27-39.
Moodie, I. (2016). The anti-apprenticeship of
observation: How negative prior
language learning experience influences
English language teachers’ beliefs and
practices. System, 60, 29-41.
Moon, J. (2004). A handbook of reflective and
experiential learning: Theory and
practice. London, England: Routledge
Farmer.
Ortega, L. (2008). Understanding L2 acquisition.
London, England: Hodder Education.
Oxford, R. L. (2002). Language learning
strategies in a nutshell: Update and ESL
suggestions. In J. C. Richards, & W. A.
Renandyn (Eds.), Methodology in
language teaching: An anthology of
current
practice
(pp.
124-132).
Cambridge,
England:
Cambridge
University Press.
ISSN 2329-2210
Senior, R. (2006). The experience of language
teaching.
Cambridge,
England:
Cambridge University Press.
Tatto, M. T. (1998). The influence of teacher
education on teachers’ beliefs about
purposes of education, roles, and
practice. Journal of Teacher Education,
49(1), 66-77.
Vesselinov, R., & Grego, J. (2012). Duolingo
effectiveness study: A final report.
Retrieved from: https://s3.amazonaws.
com/duolingo-papers/other/vesselinovgrego.duolingo12.pdf.
Washburn, G. N. (2008). Alone, confused, and
frustrated: Developing empathy and
strategies for working with English
language learners. The Clearing House,
81(6), 247-250.
Weed, K.Z. (1993). Teacher awareness of the
language learners’ situation. In D.
Freeman & S. Cornwell (Eds.), New
ways in teacher education (pp. 191-141).
Alexandria, VA: TESOL Publications.
Wideen, M., Mayer-Smith, J., & Moon, B.
(1998). A critical analysis of the research
on learning to teach: Making the case for
an ecological perspective on inquiry.
Review of Educational Research, 68(2),
130–178.
Wright, T. (2010). L2 teacher education:
Review of recent research on practice.
Language Teaching, 43(3), 259-296.
Wright-Maley, C., & Green, J.D. (2015).
Experiencing the needs and challenges of
ELLs: Improving knowledge and
efficacy of pre-service teachers through
the use of a language immersion
simulation. Cogent Education, 2(1), 1-17.
Wubbels, T. (1992). Taking account of student
teachers’ preconceptions. Teaching and
Teacher Education, 8(2), 137-149.
81