Optica Applicata, Vol. XXXIX, No. 2, 2009
What characteristics a clinical CSF system
has to have?
ELISA COLOMBO1, 2, LUIS ISSOLIO1, 2*, JAVIER SANTILLÁN1, ROLANDO AGUIRRE1, 2
1
Departamento de Luminotecnia, Luz y Visión, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Tecnología,
Universidad Nacional de Tucumán. Avda. Independencia 1800,
San Miguel de Tucumán (4000), Tucumán, Argentina
2
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas – CONICET, Argentina
*Corresponding
author: lissolio@herrera.unt.edu.ar
We discuss the characteristics a system to measure the contrast sensitivity function (CSF) in
the ophthalmologic clinic has to have. We propose that this system should be computer based
in order to assure flexibility and precision. Besides the original calibration, this equipment needs
an auxiliary system to keep the working conditions allowing periodical gamma corrections. We
try a calibration method based on visual comparisons and show that it is valid and simple.
We propose to use an adaptive psychophysical method to obtain contrast thresholds that
assure a good compromise between precision and duration of the whole test. Finally, we propose
that the system has to have its own normality curves for the different age ranges allowing
the practitioner to perform clinical evaluations. Summing up, we can say that taking into account
the above issues, the fidelity of the stimuli will be guaranteed and the challenges entailed in its
transference to the clinic will be overcome.
Keywords: contrast sensitivity, ophthalmologic clinic, standards.
1. Introduction
Human spatial vision has the ability to detect and process stimuli of varying size and
contrast; this ability can be evaluated through contrast sensitivity function (CSF). This
function is obtained from the threshold contrasts that the visual system requires to
detect or to discriminate a sinusoidal stimulus of any spatial frequency (the inverse of
a grating period). The inverse of the threshold contrast is the contrast sensitivity (CS).
A complete CSF curve results from the threshold contrast measurement for the entire
range of spatial frequencies to which the system is sensitive.
The clinic utility of CSF is determined because this function gives information
about the optic part as well as the neuronal part of the visual system, thus allowing
the ophthalmologists to attain an evaluation of functional vision [1]. From the clinical
standpoint, the CSF test can detect vision loss and can give way to get a better diagnosis
416
E. COLOMBO et al.
of early-stage diseases [2], such as early retina and optic nerve problems, in which
visual acuity does not present alterations in its first stages [3]. In this way, CSF provides
information not only about the ability to detect objects in a vast range of situations [4],
but also to detect anomalies [5, 6].
There has been a renewed interest in CSF for clinical use [7, 8]. Studies show that
it can be a helpful tool to be part of a functional visual assessment, especially when
a more thorough evaluation is necessary other than the one given by Visual Acuity
[9 – 11]. It is being used successfully as an aid in making decisions about cataract
surgery [12]. It has likewise shown to be effective in the diagnosis of glaucoma
[13 – 16], optic neuritis and multiple sclerosis [17, 18], and in the following up of visual
performance changes after refractive surgeries [19 – 23]. It can also help assess visual
performance in the elderly [5, 24 – 27] or in people with low vision [28, 29]. Studies
on patients with congenital nistagmus have also shown the usefulness of CSF [30].
Moreover, it is considered advisable to incorporate these kinds of visual tests to
determine a person’s eligibility to drive [31]. This is even more so considering that
elderly people – whose population percentage is on the rise – undergo an ageing
process of their visual capacities [32, 33] and are more sensitive to factors such as
glare, all of which are aggravated by the use of glasses when compared to the use of
contact lenses [34].
There are several commercial tests, some of them are based on printed or
translucent cards and they use letters [35 – 37] or sinusoidal gratings [2, 38, 39].
The printed cards specify the illuminance levels that must be measured on them in
order to achieve measurement (FACT, for instance), or they are loaded onto a system
that ensures the fulfilment of these conditions (CST Digital). The translucent ones
incorporate the lighting system from behind (CSV-1000). Research shows that
the contrast sensitivity curve provided by sine-wave grating tests is more sensitive and
informative than the results obtained from low-contrast letter acuity systems [2]. Tests
based on printed cards have a limited ability to discriminate the subject’s answer since
the stimuli have a fixed number of contrast values [40]; the result obtained is only one
because the process of determination is carried out only once; the control of illumination is not always easy to get; and reference ranges are not discriminated by age.
As a counterpart to the possibilities mentioned, there is computer-based equipment
that has sophisticated and expensive graphic cards as the VSG (Cambridge Research
Systems). There are also other systems that need libraries for visual stimuli generation,
such as the VideoToolbox and the PsychophysicsToolbox, both of which require
expensive software (a C compiler or MatLab, respectively). These alternatives have
neither the facilities needed to be adapted to clinical measurement nor the elements to
carry out diagnostic tests because they are oriented towards the research on visual
sciences. However, even though the usefulness of this CS function in clinic is well
known and there exist a variety of commercial systems, its use has not extended so
much in the ophthalmology.
Our goal was to evaluate different methods and technologies available at present
to propose the main characteristics a CSF system has to have to be useful in the clinical
What characteristics a clinical CSF system has to have?
417
practice, in order to achieve reliability, accuracy and flexibility. For this purpose we
were checking each one with our own system developed. First of all the system has to
be computer based and to have an easy procedure to periodically calibrate it. Besides,
it is necessary to select a psychophysical method to determine the CSF and to
incorporate ranges by age to compare the measurements from the patients.
2. Hardware components and calibration aspects
The development of a CSF system implies having equipment capable of presenting
low contrast sinusoidal gratings in the entire range that the visual system is sensitive
to. The use of a system based on a PC is the more appropriate alternative to get
the grey scale resolution guaranteeing the fidelity of the stimuli. The minimum
requirements of the PC are a Pentium processor, 512 MB of RAM and a graphic card
that allows two video cards (an AGP port in our case) to be controlled.
2.1. Display selection
The display should be able to reproduce the attributes of an image with such a precision
that the separation of the responses from the normal conditions could be attributed to
visual problems. Nevertheless, issues such as size cost and power consumption are
factors that should not go unwatched once the main objective has been achieved. It
was necessary to choose a given technology from among the three existing ones on
the market. At present, plasma displays are not considered a valid choice since they
do not have the versatility required in ophthalmology and they are highly expensive.
In order to decide between LCD (liquid crystal display) or CRT (cathode rays tube)
displays we compared the most important parameters for reproducing sinusoidal
stimuli of very small and highly controlled contrast:
1. Dynamic range (DR) of luminance: generally, LCD screens have greater
maximum luminance than CRTs, however, minimum luminance is also higher, which
leads to greater contrast proportions for CRTs.
2. Gray level resolution: rather than the absolute value of this number, what is
important is the relationship between luminance and DAC voltage. While CRT
presents an area of greater saturation towards high values of DAC voltage, LCD
screens tend to show a flat area not only at high digital levels but also at low levels.
In this sense, the advantage of working with CRT is remarkable.
This means that nowadays only CRT monitors have DR characteristics and gray
level resolution which make it possible to show low contrast sinusoidal gratings.
However, in terms of size, LCD is at an advantage of CRT monitors, but also this
advantage is compensated by the price of CRTs. Another disadvantage of LCD
screens is that emerging light is not homogeneous in all space directions, as is the case
of CRT, although this deficiency is not very important because the task is foveal. We
know that in a near future the CRT screens will replace the selected CRT ones.
Two monitors of the same brand were compared, one LCD and the other CRT.
They bore similar dimensions, pixel size, and response time and rest consumption.
418
E. COLOMBO et al.
Although maximum luminance in LCD was slightly more than double, contrast
proportion for CRT is 6 times higher; horizontal and vertical vision angles are slightly
higher in CRTs, as well as maximum resolution and colours borne. In the end, a 19''
CRT display was chosen, with a screen size of 35.2 cm × 26.4 cm (Samsung
SyncMaster 955 DF). The video card resolution was set at 1600 × 1200 pixels with
32 bits for colour representation. A monitor of 15'' or 17'' can be used if no spatial
2AFC paradigm is implemented.
2.2. Gray level resolution of the system
The gray scale resolution of the standard VGA video card is insufficient to reproduce
the lowest contrast and by this reason it is necessary to consider auxiliary tools. In
order to expand it we incline toward the option of a video attenuator [41] and a module
for the adaptation of the monitor to the video signal. Both auxiliary systems enable
the color resolution of the monitor jumps from the 8-bit to more than 13 bits.
The video attenuator integrates the output signals r, g and b of the graphic card
into one unique output by means of a passive network that assigns each input signal
with a different attenuation level. This implementation was originally designed to be
used with achromatic monitors due to the only output attenuator; meanwhile,
the authors proposed the use of two graphic cards for colour monitors with an attenuator
that integrates channels r, g and b of both [41]. Instead of this solution, in our system,
we connect the monochromatic attenuator to the three joint inputs of the colour monitor
and modify their input impedance so as to adapt it to the output impedance (75 ohms)
of the monochromatic attenuator.
The beat-stealing algorithm [42] instead of a video attenuator could be considered
but it allows around 10.8 bits, an insufficient number of gray levels to achieve
a precise reproduction of a sinusoidal stimulus of as lower a contrast as 0.2% used in
the measurement of the greatest sensitivities. Besides, the beat-stealing technique
requires accurate information about the effect of the radiant energy on the human
eye, which could vary substantially for individual observers reducing the accuracy of
the process.
2.3. Calibration
The system has to have an original (or initial) careful process of calibration to correct
the different nonlinearities in the video-monitor card system and so asserting the fidelity of the stimuli (for a revision, see [43]).
The gamma correction parameters should be determined by means of a photometric
procedure measuring the absolute luminance with a luminance meter and computing
the parameters k and γ of the relation between the luminance and the DAC voltage:
L = k Dγ
(1)
However, the user needs update this calibration by a periodical procedure. One
alternative is by means of a psychophysical method based on visual comparison [44]
419
What characteristics a clinical CSF system has to have?
a
b
Fig. 1. The bipartite field used to perform the brightness match task in the visual calibration process:
without diffuser (a), with diffuser (b).
which does not require a luminance meter. In this work, we show that this method
could be successfully implemented on a standard PC. We checked it as follows:
a 400-pixel diameter circle is presented on the screen, in one of its halves (A),
the maximum DAC signal is applied (a value of 255) to a previously established pixel
fraction; and in the other semi-circle (B), the signal applied has a varying value of 0
and 255 on all pixels, thus achieving a given luminance level (Fig. 1). This signal is
controlled by the subject that calibrates the equipment, and his task consists in varying
the luminance in B until it matches the brightness of half A (brightness matching task).
These matching values give the levels of voltage in B associated to the luminance value
in A, that is, in the proportion of lit pixels. Once the process for pixels lit for ten
different rates is over we fit the data to obtain the parameters γ and k. Due to the fact
that the different luminance percentages generated in the upper field present a striped
pattern that makes the matching task difficult, a diffuser glass was necessary to be
introduced in the system that covers both fields of the stimulus, which facilitates
the subject’s task.
In order to evaluate the reliability of the visual calibration we compare it with
a photometric one in the equipment proposed. The experiment was carried out with
7 observers, each of whom repeated the test 4 times. Among the observers, some had
no experience in this kind of psychophysical determination, while others did. There is
high repeatability in each observer and among observers, as shown in Figs. 2a and 2b,
respectively. The values obtained were γ = 1.72±0.04 and k = 0.98±0.01, for the visual
calibration, while photometric calibration had the values of γ = 1.733 ± 0.004 and
k = 0.992 ± 0.001. The conclusion is that the parameters are the same within experimental errors, thus validating the visual calibration technique. The results indicate
reliability of the method, a reduced measuring time of approximately 10 minutes and
ease of performance, all the features being sought by the practitioners.
Another aspect of the monitor behaviour to be taken into account by an initial
calibration is the MTF of the display. In spite of the MTF being determined only for
vertical gratings because of the interaction between neighbouring pixels provoking
a contrast decreasing [45], the MTF of the display in the direction of the luminance
420
E. COLOMBO et al.
a
b
Fig. 2. Luminance vs. voltage: data obtained for subject AS (a), data for the seven intervenient subjects
in the experiment, each point is the mean of four measurements (b).
variation of our quasi horizontal gratings has to be determined [46] since we also found
in this case contrast reduction. We measured the maximal and minimal luminance of
gratings and then computed the effective contrast in the screen. This measurement was
done for all the spatial frequencies used in the system (characterized in pixels per cycle)
and for seven low contrasts in the same range used for the determination of contrast
sensitivity. To this end, we used an LMT 1009 luminance meter with approximation
lenses that allows measuring a single pixel. The results are plotted in Fig. 3 showing
the slopes of the lines to be fitted to the data decreasing as the spatial frequency grows.
These slopes are plotted as a function of spatial frequency in Fig. 4 and represent
Fig. 3. Contrast measured in grating counterclockwise direction (+7°) as a function of nominal contrast
for seven spatial frequencies characterized by the number of pixels per cycle. The lines represent linear
fits that show the different slopes obtained for each spatial frequency.
What characteristics a clinical CSF system has to have?
421
Fig. 4. Modulation transfer functions obtained for stimuli of both orientations. In the abscissa axis,
the spatial frequency is represented as the inverse of the number of pixels.
the MTF of the display. From the operative point of view, when sinusoidal gratings
are generated, compensation has to be introduced incrementing the nominal contrast
by a coefficient in inverse proportion to the modulation factor for the corresponding
spatial frequency.
Both gamma correction and the monitor’s MTF measurement have to be carried
out taking into account the fact that the stimuli appear in the central area of the monitor,
thus avoiding misconvergence in the peripheral area that translates onto the image as
an unwanted blur. On the other hand, calibration stimuli have to be small enough to
assure that the mean luminance drives the value of the display current supply
approximately constant; this mean luminance has to be the same when taking measurements as when performing some calibration. In this way, the calibration parameters
are guaranteed to be valid in the measuring process.
3. CSF measurement: software, methodology
and experimental set-up
Special purpose software for measuring CSF has to be developed. The choice of
methodology is crucial for providing a transferable system for the clinic. Once
the stimulus fidelity is assured and good precision in visual function determination is
achieved, the measurement should be done in relatively short periods of time.
3.1. Stimuli
We propose static sinusoidal gratings to be used as stimuli [2]; they were slanted 7°
in relation to the horizontal one, both clockwise and counterclockwise (Fig. 1),
maintaining the same response property, since both orientations belong to a unique
orientation column in the striate cortex [47]. The presentation time for each stimulus
in its nominal contrast could be around 500 ms. Times below this value produce
422
E. COLOMBO et al.
an insufficient detection of the pattern while longer times unnecessarily extend
the measurement [48], the visual size has to be slightly greater than the foveal size –
around 6° or 7° is good enough. The spatial frequency values used have to cover
the low, medium and high ranges of spatial frequencies [32, 33, 39, 49] – from 1 to
24 c/deg.
The maximum luminance of a CRT monitor is approximately 150 cd/m2. The grating
mean luminance should be nearly in the middle of the monitor range of luminance as
well as the immediate surrounding luminance, which is not a relevant variable
[50, 51]. Although the normal room lighting conditions do not change the visual
performance [52], a dark surrounding would be better to avoid distractions. For that
reason, the subject should be advised to look at the display every time.
3.2. Distance of vision
From an optometric point of view, it is convenient to perform the measurement as far
away as possible from the visualization device [53] so as to minimize the demand for
accommodation/convergence, taking for granted a person who is normal or corrected
ametropic. Considering limitation of space a compromising solution could be a distance
of 1.5 m, which considerably lowers the demands.
3.3. The method and the task
We propose an adaptive method because it has the advantage of shortening
measurement times significantly, controlling precision. In this way, we have used
the QUEST [54] adaptive method whose algorithm makes a Bayesian inference of each
response the observer gives, and hence the following contrast to be presented is
established. The observer’s task consists in discerning between the two possible
Fig. 5. Contrast sensitivity obtained for 20, 30 and 40 trials with spatial frequencies of 1, 4 and 12 c/deg.
Each point is the mean of 5 measurements and the bars represents a standard deviation.
What characteristics a clinical CSF system has to have?
423
positions, thus being forced to pick one of them – even if he could no longer see
the stimulus – by pressing the appropriate button.
According to a simple experiment we determined the number of trails needed to
obtain a good estimation of the threshold. We have carried out measurements
considering 20, 30 and 40 trials with spatial frequencies of 1, 4 and 12 c/deg in naïve
and experimented subjects. In Fig. 5, the data obtained for one observer (32 years old)
are plotted. For frequencies of 4 and 12 c/deg, there are no differences among
the number of trials, instead for 1 c/deg, there can be seen differences between
measurements with 20 trials and those performed with 30 and 40 trials. These
observations were confirmed by means of an ANOVA with p < 0.05 for 1 c/deg and
p > 0.05 for 4 and 12 c/deg. Moreover, if we compare the set of data collected for each
number of trials there are no differences between 30 and 40 trials (p > 0.05) although
it is not the case if we consider 20 and 30 trials ( p < 0.05). From these results it was
possible to establish that for 20 trials, there can be an overestimation of the threshold,
while with 30 or 40 measurements, equivalent results were obtained. However, because
time is a very important factor – as in clinics – the option to go with was the one with
30 trials.
4. Reference curves by age range
Finally, probably one of the most important characteristics, is that a CSF clinical system
has to incorporate normality ranges according to age. In this way, the system gets
sensitivity for the diagnosis and follow-ups of visual pathologies. The methodology
used to determine the normal curves must be the same as the one suggested for
the measurement in clinics.
We have determined reference curves with 55 subjects, 11 per age range (20 – 29,
30 – 39, 40 – 49, 50 – 59, 60 – 69 years old), all of whom agreed to an ophthalmologic
check-up designed according to APA (American Psychological Association) regulations for the work performed on human beings, and to international norms on bioethics
and the ones stated in the Helsinki Declaration. Since the goal is to establish curves
that will help detect vision anomalies, a visual acuity unit (AV = 1) was required.
The latter would be corrected in situ by an ophthalmologist for the distance of the test
during measurement sessions; an exam of the ocular means (cornea and crystalline
biomicroscopy) and the fondus, as well as an adequate control of intraocular pressure
(contact tonometry). The group of people included in the measurement was free from
ocular pathologies, though some needed refractive correction. Each observer did two
series of measurements with each eye: the first was deemed part of the learning process
and only the data from the second round were considered. Measurement time for
the six spatial frequencies in each eye was approximately 10 minutes, four times less
than when using a constant stimuli method, which certifies the efficiency of clinical
measurements based on adaptive methods.
Of the 5 curves obtained, and according to the standard mean error – which
indicates the degree of precision estimated in the “centre” of the distribution of
424
E. COLOMBO et al.
a
b
Fig. 6. Normality interval for the age ranges of 20 to 49 (a) and 50 to 69 (b) years. Each point represents
the mean of the sample, vertical bars indicate a standard deviation and the shadowed area represents
the two standard deviations.
means – a separation of the curves in the age ranges 20 – 49 and 50 – 69 years old is
noticed. In clinical practice, it is common to set normality intervals according to
the following criterion [55, 56]
Normality interval = μ ± 2σ
(2)
where μ is the mean for the distribution of means and σ is the standard deviation. This
implies that, if we measure a new subject, there is a 95.45% probability that his
sensitivity curve will drop within that strip. Applying such criterion, we obtain graphs
illustrated in Fig. 6a for the 20 – 49 age range and in Fig. 6b for the 50 – 69 age range.
Considering our results and those from the quoted papers we think that these two
ranges of normality are enough for the system to get a good ability to discriminate
a separation from normality. In addition, the equipment should have the alternative to
visualize the course of the measurement through another monitor and the possibility
of showing the results obtained and to contrast them with the reference strips as well
as to print a report with test results.
5. Discussion
The present challenge in the CSF clinical measurement is to be able to count on
reliable equipment with an appropriate measuring methodology, taking into consideration two basic requisites. The first one is for the results obtained to have enough
accuracy and reliability. The second one is for the time taken in measuring not to extend
too much.
Our decision of choosing a computer based system is in good agreement with earlier
works. For instance, RUBIN [57] showed more reliability on CS measurements done
with CRT-based equipment with respect to printed-card tests through a test-retest
What characteristics a clinical CSF system has to have?
425
analysis, and other works have shown the sensitivity of electronic equipment-based
tests for discriminating the normal case from the pathological one [13, 58].
In relation to the methodology we propose an adaptive method because its
superiority as regards the times involved is well known. Although some authors [59]
had already noticed the inefficiency of the constant stimuli method for some
applications, it has continued to be widely used due to its simplicity for experimental
implementation. Our results showed that an adaptive method like QUEST allows more
reliable results than those attained with constant stimuli and in notably less time
consuming. Furthermore, while the QUEST method immediately estimated threshold
contrast, the constant stimuli method requires further processing, consisting in fitting
the data into some mathematical model that describes the psychometric function from
which the threshold contrast can be obtained.
Furthermore, the clinical measurements will be performed in less time than those
obtained when all the spatial frequencies are presented, which is about 10 minutes.
For instance, evaluating a smaller number of spatial frequencies since the practitioner
will not always need information on the entire range. CS measurement in each spatial
frequency takes approximately one and a half minute, which means that in order to
determine the threshold contrast for just 2 spatial frequencies the practitioner needs
only 3 minutes.
On the other hand, the adaptive method-based measurement could be quicker if
the estimated CS value were basically within the area of normality. In this case,
the trial sequence can be interrupted thus reducing measurement time to less than one
minute, since less precision is required. The same criterion may be considered when
encountering the system with CS estimates which are much lower than normal. Only
in those cases where CS estimation is near the inferior limit of the area of normality
will the system demand a greater number of trials which will take up more time to
allow a more precise determination in order to adequately discern the normal case from
the pathological one.
6. Conclusions
We have established the characteristics a CSF system has to have to be used in the
ophthalmologic clinic. First of all we discuss how to generate very low contrast
sinusoidal gratings with high accuracy. Then we propose that for the practitioner to be
able to realize a clinical evaluation of CSF measurement, the system has to have
normality curves for different age ranges. At the same time, we demonstrate that a PC
to which components and calibration procedures are added in order to use it as
a measuring tool, allowed the presentation of grating with contrast lower than 0.2% in
establishing threshold contrasts. Apart from using a monitor to present sinusoidal
gratings, the illumination level to which the patient’s visual system adapts itself is
determined by monitor parameters and by the luminance intended by the equipment
operator taking the measurements. Another important advantage is the incorporation
426
E. COLOMBO et al.
of a module of visual calibration, allowing it to keep the working conditions
independent of a photometer. Finally, we suggest that this kind of system has to have
at least two ranges by age.
All these characteristics are necessary if the equipment has to be used in clinic,
because: i) measurement can be repeated as often as necessary because the stimuli are
presented randomly, ii ) it can be periodically calibrated, iii) the adaptation luminance
is set mainly by monitor luminance, which makes the system very strong for complex
environments, iv) measurements can be compared with normal ones according to age
of each patient.
In this way, a computerized system for contrast sensitivity tests with characteristics
mentioned in this paper can be used in the clinic as well as in research laboratories, in
driving permit controls and in screening of population like that of school-age children.
Acknowledgements – The authors wish to acknowledge the support of Consejo Nacional de
Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (Fellowship to RA and Grant PIP No. 4948), Consejo de
Investigaciones de la Universidad Nacional de Tucumán (Grant 26/E112 to EC), and Agencia Nacional
de Promoción Científica y Tecnológica (Grant PICT13-15190 to EC).
References
[1] GINSBURG A.P., Contrast sensitivity and functional vision, International Ophthalmology Clinics
43(2), 2003, pp. 5– 15.
[2] GINSBURG A.P., Next Generation Contrast Sensitivity Testing in Functional Assessment of Low
Vision, [Eds.] B. Rosenthal, R. Cole, Mosby-Year Book, Inc., St. Louis, 1996, pp. 77 – 88.
[3] ARDEN G.B., Testing contrast sensitivity in clinical practice, Clinical Vision Sciences 2(3), 1988,
pp. 213–224.
[4] CAMPBELL F.W., Why do we measure contrast sensitivity?, Behavioural Brain Research 10(1), 1983,
pp. 87– 97.
[5] WOO G.C., Contrast sensitivity function as a diagnostic tool in low vision, American Journal of
Optometry and Physiological Optics 62(9), 1985, pp. 648 –651.
[6] BARBUR J.L., Spatial frequency specific measurements and their use in clinical psychophysics,
Clinical Vision Sciences 2(3), 1988, pp. 225– 233.
[7] OWSLEY C., Contrast sensitivity, Ophthalmology Clinics of North America 16(2), 2003, pp. 171–177.
[8] PESUDOVS K., HAZEL C.A., DORAN R.M., ELLIOTT D.B., The usefulness of Vistech and FACT contrast
sensitivity charts for cataract and refractive surgery outcomes research, British Journal of
Ophthalmology 88(1), 2004, pp. 11 – 16.
[9] JINDRA L.F., ZEMON V., Contrast sensitivity testing: a more complete assessment of vision, Journal
of Cataract and Refractive Surgery 15(2), 1989, pp. 141 – 148.
[10] FISCHER M.L., COLE R.G., Functional evaluation of the adult: optometric and ophthalmologic
evaluation, [In] Lighthouse Handbook on Vision Impairment and Vision Rehabilitation,
[Eds.] B. Silverstone, M.A. Long, B.P. Rosenthal, E.E. Faye, Vol. 2., Oxford University Press,
New York, 2000, pp. 833– 853.
[11] STEINERT R.F., Visual function versus vision testing, Ophthalmology 113(8), 2006, pp. 1255 – 1256.
[12] FROST N.A., SPARROW J.M., Use of vision tests in clinical decision making about cataract surgery:
results of a national survey, British Journal of Ophthalmology 84(4), 2000, pp. 432 – 434.
[13] ROSS J.E., BRON A.J., CLARKE D.D., Contrast sensitivity and visual disability in chronic simple
glaucoma, British Journal of Ophthalmology 68(11), 1984, pp. 821 –827.
What characteristics a clinical CSF system has to have?
427
[14] ROSS J.E., Clinical detection of abnormalities in central vision in chronic simple glaucoma using
contrast sensitivity, International Ophthalmology 8(3), 1985, pp. 167 –177.
[15] BLANCHARD D.L., Contrast sensitivity: A useful tool in glaucoma, Glaucoma 10, 1988, pp. 151–153.
[16] TOCHEL C.M., MORTON J.S., JAY J.L., MORRISON J.D., Relationship between visual field loss and
contrast threshold elevation in glaucoma, BMC Ophthalmology 5, 2005, p. 22; doi:10.1186/14712415-5-22.
[17] REGAN D., RAYMOND J., GINSBURG A.P., MURRAY T.J., Contrast sensitivity, visual acuity and
the discrimination of Snellen letters in multiple sclerosis, Brain 104(2), 1981, pp. 333 – 350.
[18] REGAN D., Low-contrast letter charts and sinewave grating tests in ophthalmological and
neurological disorders, Clinical Vision Sciences 2(3), 1988, pp. 235–250.
[19] MONTES-MICO R., CHARMAN W.N., Mesopic contrast sensitivity function after excimer laser
photorefractive keratectomy, Journal of Refractive Surgery 18(1), 2002, pp. 9 – 13.
[20] KAISERMAN I., HAZARBASSANOV R., VARSSANO D., GRINBAUM A., Contrast sensitivity after wave
front-guided LASIK, Ophthalmology 111(3), 2004, pp. 454 – 457.
[21] QUESNEL N.M., LOVASIK J.V., FERREMI C., BOILEAU M., IERACI C., Laser in situ keratomileusis
for myopia and the contrast sensitivity function, Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery 30(6),
2004, pp. 1209 – 1218.
[22] GAMBATO C., GHIRLANDO A., MORETTO E., BUSATO F., MIDENA E., Mitomycin C modulation of
corneal wound healing after photorecfractive keratectomy in highly myopic eyes, Ophthalmology
112(2), 2005, pp. 208 – 219.
[23] GINSBURG A.P., Contrast sensitivity: determining the visual quality and function of cataract,
intraocular lenses and refractive surgery, Current Opinion in Ophthalmology 17(1), 2006,
pp. 19– 26.
[24] OWSLEY C., SEKULER R., BOLDT C., Aging and low-contrast vision: face perception, Investigative
Ophthalmology and Visual Science 21(2), 1981, pp. 362–365.
[25] OWSLEY C., SLOANE M.E., Contrast sensitivity, acuity, and the perception of “real world” targets,
British Journal of Ophthalmology 71(10), 1987, pp. 791 – 796.
[26] NASANEN R., Spatial frequency bandwidth used in the recognition of facial images, Vision
Research 39(23), 1999, pp. 3824–3833.
[27] LOTT L.A., HAEGERSTROM-PORTNOY G., SCHNECK M.E., BRABYN J.A., Face recognition in the elderly,
Optometry and Vision Science 82(10), 2005, pp. 874 –881.
[28] ELDRED K.B., JOSE R.T., Binocular contrast sensitivity testing for low vision patients, Journal of
the American Optometric Association 62(10), 1991, pp. 766– 769.
[29] LEAT S.J., LEGGE G.E., BULLIMORE M.A., What is low vision? A re-evaluation of definitions,
Optometry and Vision Science 76(4), 1999, pp. 198 – 211.
[30] SHOKIDA F., CIANCIA A., Contrast sensitivity is improved following four muscle recessions in
idiopathic congenital nystagmus, American Orthoptic Journal 49, 1999, pp. 90– 96.
[31] MANTYJARVI M., TUPPURAINEN K., Cataract in traffic, Graefes Archive for Clinical and Experimental
Ophthalmology 237(4), 1999, pp. 278 – 282.
[32] OWSLEY C., SEKULER R., SIEMSEN D., Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood, Vision Research
23(7), 1983, pp. 689– 699.
[33] HIGGINS K.E., JAFFE M.J., CARUSO R.C., DEMONASTERIO F.C., Spatial contrast sensitivity: effects
of age, test-retest, and psychophysical method, Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics
and Image Science 5, 1988, pp. 2173 – 2180.
[34] ISSOLIO L., LÓPEZ GIL N., COLOMBO E., MIRÓ I., Brightness perception under transient glare
conditions with myopic observers wearing contact lens and spectacles, Proceedings of SPIE 4419,
2001, pp. 557 – 560.
[35] REGAN D., NEIMA D., Low-contrast letter charts as a test of visual function, Ophthalmology 90(10),
1983, pp. 1192 – 1200.
[36] BAILEY I.L. LOVIE-KITCHIN J.E., The design and use of a new near-vision chart, American Journal
of Optometry and Physiological Optics 57(6), 1980, pp. 378–387.
428
E. COLOMBO et al.
[37] PELLI D.G., ROBSON J.G., WILKINS A.J., The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast
sensitivity, Clinical Vision Sciences 2(3), 1988, pp. 187–199.
[38] ARDEN G.B., The importance of measuring contrast sensitivity in cases of visual disturbance,
British Journal of Ophthalmology 62(4), 1978, pp. 198–209.
[39] GINSBURG A.P., A new contrast sensitivity vision test chart, American Journal of Optometry and
Physiological Optics 61(6), 1984, pp. 403–407.
[40] LEAT S.J., WEGMANN D., Clinical testing of contrast sensitivity in children: age-related norms and
validity, Optometry and Vision Science 81(4), 2004, pp. 245 – 254.
[41] PELLI D.G., ZHANG L., Accurate control of contrast on microcomputer displays, Vision Research
31(7 – 8), 1991, pp. 1337 –1350.
[42] TYLER C.W., Colour bit-stealing to enhance the luminance resolution of digital displays on a single
pixel basis, Spatial Vision 10(4), 1997, pp. 369 – 377.
[43] TRAVIS D., Effective Color Displays: Theory and Practice, Academic Press, 1991.
[44] COLOMBO E., DERRINGTON A., Visual calibration of CRT monitors, Displays 22(3), 2001, pp. 87– 95.
[45] NAIMAN A.C., MAKOUS W., Spatial nonlinearities of gray-scale CRT pixels, Proceedings of SPIE
1666, 1992, pp. 41 – 56.
[46] GARCÍA-PEREZ M.A., PELI E., Luminance artifacts of cathode-ray tube displays for vision research,
Spatial Vision 14(2), 2001, pp. 201–215.
[47] BONHOEFFER T., GRINVALD A., Iso-orientation domains in cat visual cortex are arranged in pinwheel-like patterns, Nature 353(6343), 1991, pp. 429 – 431.
[48] AREND L.E., Response of the human eye to spatially sinusoidal gratings at various exposure
durations, Vision Research 16(11), 1976, pp. 1311 –1315.
[49] GINSBURG A.P., EVANS D.W., CANNON M.W., OWSLEY C., MULVANNY P., Large-sample norms
for contrast sensitivity, American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 61(2), 1984,
pp. 80– 84.
[50] COX M.J., NORMAN J.H., NORMAN P., The effect of surround luminance on measurements of contrast
sensitivity, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 19(5), 1999, pp. 401 –414.
[51] VIZMANOS J.G., DE LA FUENTE I., MATESANZ B.M., APARICIO J.A., Influence of surround
illumination on pupil size and contrast sensitivity, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 24(5),
2004, pp. 464 – 468.
[52] COLOMBO E., SANTILLÁN J., AGUIRRE R., ISSOLIO L., Sensitivity of a new CSF system as a screening
tool in a non clinical population, ARVO 2007, Fort Lauderdale, USA, May 2007.
[53] SHEEDY J.E., SHAW-MCMINN P.G., Diagnosing and Treating Computer-Related Vision Problems,
Butterworth-Heinemann, Amsterdam, 2003.
[54] WATSON A.B., PELLI D.G., QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method, Perception and
Psychophysics 33(2), 1983, pp. 113 –120.
[55] ELLIOTT D.B., GILCHRIST J., WHITAKER D., Contrast sensitivity and glare sensitivity changes with
three types of cataract morphology: Are these techniques necessary in a clinical evaluation of
cataract?, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 9(1), 1989, pp. 25 – 30.
[56] SEGUÍ M.M., PIŃERO D.P., VIQUEIRA V., Evaluación de diferentes métodos psicofísicos para la
medida de la CSF en monitores CRT, Ver y oir, September, 1998, pp. 443 – 451.
[57] RUBIN G.S., Reliability and sensitivity of clinical contrast sensitivity test, Clinical Vision Sciences
2(3), 1988, pp. 169– 177.
[58] BROWN V.A., DORAN R.M., WOODHOUSE J.M., The use of computerized contrast sensitivity, Arden
gratings and low contrast letter charts in the assessment of amblyopia, Ophthalmic and Physiological
Optics 7(1), 1987, pp. 43– 51.
[59] WATSON A.B., FITZHUGH A., The method of constant stimuli is inefficient, Perception and
Psychophysics 47(1), 1990, pp. 87– 91.
Received July 18, 2008
in revised form October 11, 2008