Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only €10,99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment
The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment
The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment
Ebook1,092 pages11 hours

The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment

Rating: 5 out of 5 stars

5/5

()

Read preview

About this ebook

The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment presents the state-of-the-art in the field of personality assessment, providing a perspective on emerging trends, and placing these in the context of research advances in the associated fields.
  • Explores emerging trends and perspectives in personality assessment, building on current knowledge and looking ahead to the future landscape of the field
  • Discusses emerging technologies and how these can be combined with psychological theories in order to enhance the real-world practice of assessing personality
  • Comprehensive sections address gaps in current knowledge and collate contributions and advances from diverse areas and perspectives
  • The chapter authors are eminent scholars from across the globe who bring together new research from many different countries and cultures
LanguageEnglish
PublisherWiley
Release dateJan 19, 2016
ISBN9781119173496
The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment

Related to The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment

Related ebooks

Psychology For You

View More

Reviews for The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment

Rating: 5 out of 5 stars
5/5

1 rating0 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

    Book preview

    The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment - Updesh Kumar

    Preface

    Beginning with the earliest attempts to assess and explain personality by the physiologists like Hippocrates, Canon, Sheldon, and their many predecessors, ranging through the initial psychological descriptions and measurements by Freud, Allport, Cattell, and the contemporary psychologists, the field of personality assessment has evolved and grown enormously to arrive at our present-day comprehensive understanding of varied aspects of personality from different perspectives. Integral to clinical diagnosis and personnel selection, the field of personality assessment today forms a huge and multifarious domain that continues to evolve through a symbiotic relationship between research and practice, and thrives through the dedicated efforts of researchers and advances of techniques facilitated by technological innovations. There is no dearth of literature on personality assessment, ranging from lay psychology tools promising grand results, to theoretically grounded research perspectives in journal articles and comprehensive volumes. However, proceeding further in a structured manner requires an elaborative critical perspective on the research advances and future developments in the concomitant fields. The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment proposes to address this requirement by systematically delineating the recent advances and the ways forward in the area of personality assessment, along with varied modern theoretical underpinnings and emerging empirical evidences.

    Providing a comprehensive perspective on the emerging issues and recent trends in personality assessment, the volume has been divided into two sections, encapsulating Emerging Conceptual Trends in the first section, followed by a look into Emerging Assessment Perspectives and Methodological Issues in the second. Opening in the first chapter with discussion on Integrating Trait and Process Approaches to Personality, Robert R. McCrae brings forth the differential utility of trait and process approaches in diagnosis and selection versus interventions. Citing the better relevance of process approach in effective interventions but a relative lack of a unified framework, the author discusses conceptual and empirical work to link processes to traits and further deliberates on some ways in which such research might lead to psychological interventions. Discussing a related issue of Personality Assessment and Theory, Corr and Poropat throw light on the issue of underlying personality traits impacting the validity of personality assessment. Making a call for application of sophisticated generalizability theory, they delineate the interplay between surface traits and source processes and different sources of variance in personality assessment. Moving further, Horstmann and Ziegler attempt to elaborate on the links of personality with Situational Perception in context with Lewin’s person–situation interaction formula. Highlighting the role of subjective and individualistic situation perception in one’s personality, the authors challenge the perspective of looking at perceptual individual differences and call for inclusion of a pre-operationalized psychological situation for personality assessment.

    Victor and his colleagues in the fourth chapter discuss the Temperamental Components of the Developing Personality. Highlighting the neural networks between genes, temperament, and personality, the authors describe a model conjoining personality as well as temperament scales to locate inherently determined temperament components within a broad personality structure, and they cite certain important empirical findings in the matter of context. Just like temperamental traits, another important dimension of personality is discussed by Viswesvaran and Ones in Chapter 5, wherein they very ably describe Integrity Tests: A Review of Alternate Conceptualizations and Some Measurement and Practical Issues. Making a call for comprehensive explanations for the construct of integrity as a component of personality, the authors provide a literary synthesis of the nomological net of integrity construct and an existing prominent theoretical model conceptualizing this trait. Further, an attempt has been made to present elaborative theoretical conceptualizations of the integrity construct, review some major measurement issues, summarize some recent controversies, and present some future research directions.

    Slowly widening the scope of the volume, Costantini and Perugini put forth Network Analysis as a new way to think about personality in Chapter 6. Highlighting the utility of network analysis in conceptualization and measurement of personality, they elaboratively present the fundamental concepts of network analysis and review recent studies conducted by applying the network methodology. They also review the methods and indices used to define different networks and summarize their important properties. Extending the scope of the subject matter further in the next chapter, Petrides, Siegling and Saklofske present Theory and Measurement of Trait Emotional Intelligence. The authors provide an overview of the operationalization of emotional intelligence as a personality trait; and, covering the key aspects of theory and measurement, they also present a brief critique of popular measures of the construct and highlight varied areas of its application.

    Taking the emotional intelligence related discourse to the next step, Reuven Bar-On makes a call for looking Beyond IQ and EQ and presents the Bar-On Multifactor Model of Performance to the reader. Along with the introduction of the model, Bar-On also introduces the reader to his proposed assessment tool: the Multifactor Measure of Performance™ (MMP™). Explaining the need for this model, he cites the methodology adopted to develop it, its potential applicability and current limitations, and the ways in which this model can represent a significant paradigm shift in assessment. Progressing further, a similar paradigm shift approach has been described by Southard and Zeigler-Hill wherein they put an emphasis on the significance of Measuring the Dark Side of Personality. The authors review the existing empirical literature in the area of assessment of dark and aversive personality features, largely concerned with antagonism and manipulation, measurement instruments tapping such features, and the recent attempts made to broaden the conceptualization of dark personality features. Diversity and Assessment is the focal point of Chapter 10 authored by Chu and colleagues. Citing the relatively little attention received by cultural and diversity issues in assessment, they accentuate the need for having culturally competent practice of assessment and testing by putting forth a comprehensive literature analysis reviewing the main domains of culture-informed assessment, including culturally-adjusted differential norms, culture-specific validity of the constructs and measures, varied reporting styles, the cultural and technical context of the testing process, and culturally-informed responses to assessment.

    Shaping deductively from general to particular, Laher presents Future Directions for Personality Assessment: An African Perspective. Putting forth the need for cross-cultural validation of the five-factor model of personality, the author discusses African research contributions in the form of development of new assessment tools, based on NEO-Personality Inventory Revised, focused towards informing cross-cultural developments on the understanding of personality structure globally. Some future directions for the five-factor personality theory and assessment have been presented using the findings from the South African projects. As far as in-depth psychopathological personality assessment is concerned, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) has been a widely accepted and quite credible instrument and has been revised and improved from time to time. Certain Advances in the Use and Interpretation of the MMPI-2 have been discussed and called for by Salazar, Tonetti, Sommers, and Greene in Chapter 12. They look into three so-far overlooked topics and analytically examine (a) whether the MMPI-2 code types are actually categories; (b) whether the MMPI-2 somatization scales are categories or dimensions; and (iii) whether it is possible to use item response theory to develop subscales within the MMPI-2 item pool that could be administered independently in computer adaptive testing. The issues have been well addressed by the authors, and crucial future directions have been provided for the MMPI-2 users.

    Widening the latitude of coverage and carrying the discussion forward towards applied fields, the second section of the volume delves into Emerging Assessment Perspectives and Methodological Issues, and incorporates chapters on theoretical and methodological issues involved in various assessment techniques. The section opens with Chapter 13 authored by James N. Butcher, which delineates Objective Personality Assessment with the MMPI-2. Showing the MMPI-2 as an internationally adaptable measure, the author herein provides a description of test use in clinical, forensic, and personnel evaluations, and addresses the issues of the influence of culture on the personality and mental health problems, the cross-cultural effectiveness of the MMPI-2, the extent of adaptation of the test in international settings, and computer-based personality assessment using the MMPI-2. The utility of MMPI-2 in detecting personality factors influencing job performance in high-risk occupations has been well illustrated by the help of a case study. In the next chapter, Roche and Pincus deliberate about Precision Assessment: An Individualized and Temporally Dynamic Approach to Understanding Patients in their Daily Lives by asserting for a person-specific approach to personality assessment by using longitudinal data of daily life. They describe the person-specific paradigm and its potential contribution to the advancement of personality assessment in light of the existing personality theories. They further present concise research evidence on person-specific assessment using an interpersonal framework, and discuss the barriers in the way of its wide entry into the personality assessment domain.

    Are Situational Judgment Tests (SJTs) Better Assessments of Personality than Traditional Personality Tests in High-Stakes Testing? This question has been raised and aptly addressed by Whetzel and McDaniel in Chapter 15 by highlighting the distinction between self-reports of behavioral tendency and knowledge, and reviewing the importance of this distinction for faking and the construct validity of SJTs. They also review the ways and basis of why SJTs predict job performance and then provide certain recommendations for future research. Moving ahead in discussing assessment methodologies, Galić and colleagues analyze certain limitations of self-report measures and evaluate relatively non-conventional assessment methods of Conditional Reasoning Problems and Implicit Association Test (IAT) Based Tasks as alternatives to self-reports for personality assessment. Providing descriptions of both approaches and the general logic behind their use in personality assessment, the authors illustrate their application for measuring trait aggressiveness. In Chapter 17, Krishnamurthy, Finn, and Aschieri talk about Therapeutic Assessment in Clinical and Counseling Psychology Practice. Emphasizing the unique applicability of therapeutic assessment, the authors provide an overview of its core principles and methods, review the supportive empirical evidence, describe the scope of its applicability, and discuss future directions.

    A recent innovation focusing on Personality Assessment in Ecological Settings by Means of Virtual Reality is the focal point of Chapter 18, wherein Cipresso and Riva cite the biases associated with questionnaires and self-report measures and propose a protocol of using virtual reality aimed at parsing a structured way to assess the five factors of personality. They present the reader with the Vpers test that includes five virtual scenarios to assess each dimension of personality through an interactive environment, thereby assessing personality in a more ecological way and avoiding most of the self-report biases. Taking the volume to more specifics, Gacono and associates explore The Use of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-R) and Rorschach Inkblot Method (RIM) in Forensic Psychological Assessment. Giving consideration to the role of comprehensive multimethod personality assessment in forensic psychology, the authors discuss the distinct role of the forensic psychologist and specific forensic considerations, in addition to traditional clinical considerations of accuracy. The incremental clinical validity of psychological testing has been reviewed, and the authors have called for the integration of the PCL-R and the Rorschach Inkblot Method, demonstrating their joint effectiveness by means of a case study.

    Proceeding further from the Rorschach Inkblots, Darolia describes the Holtzman Inkblot Technique and Personality Assessment and discusses the distinctive advantages of the Holtzman Inkbolt Technique over the widely accepted RIM. Highlighting the robust psychometric properties, and much more objective administration and computerized scoring system, the author recommends the use of Holtzman Inkblots for more precise and detailed personality assessment. In the next chapter, Edwards, Holleran, and Beutler advocate the use of Systematic Treatment Selection (STS) to match individuals seeking mental health treatment with a particular required therapeutic technique. The authors explicitly describe the STS process and explain its theories and principles along with literary evidences. Further, they assess and analyze the use of integrative assessment and introduce Innerlife – a web-based STS measure. Following the ongoing discussions, in Chapter 22 Parkash and I attempt to present a brief meta-analytical synthesis of various Personality Assessment Paradigms: Issues and Challenges. We attempt to review the relative advantages and disadvantages of different assessment methodologies, and put forth the challenging assessment issues that need to be kept in mind and addressed adequately when starting any kind of personality assessment.

    Addressing specific assessment issues in a particular behavioral domain, Hummel, Sottile, Spangler, and Bongar elucidate The Identification and Assessment of the Correlates of Military Suicide. Citing the latest supportive research evidence from military and veteran populations in the US and from around the world, the authors endeavor to explore various situational, psychological, and personality factors contributing to suicidality in military and veteran populations, and discuss their assessment means. Chapter 24 addresses Integrative Clinical Assessment of Sexual and Gender Minority Clients, wherein Whitney Bliss and her associates supply an overview of minority stress and its mental health effects, and individual factors that may be associated with discrimination-related stressors. They further present a five-phased model of clinical assessment, and discuss some assessment measures highlighting the Cultural Assessment of Risk of Suicide – Minority Stress Scale (CMSS).

    Addressing the basic issues of test construction with a specific approach, Burak Tunca proposes Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests: A Proposed New Method and Practical Guidelines for Data Analysis. Arguing for the need to ensure that the items are not endorsed differently across groups and the statistical results are not confounded, the author provides a review of the existing methods for assessing item bias in personality measures and presents some practical guidelines to the interested researchers for detecting item bias by using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure (e.g., in SPSS and LISREL syntaxes). The volume progresses further with the deliberations of Fluckinger and Snell about User Reactions to Personality Assessment: Implications for Assessment Credibility, Utility, and Practicality. Mentioning the relative paucity of research on participants’ reactions to personality assessment, the authors attempt to address the effects of user reactions as a relatively unexplored dimension of personality assessment, and try to analyze the ways in which consideration of these reactions offers the potential of increased credibility, general interest, utility and practicality.

    Highlighting another significant issue of response distortion, Kurtz and colleagues offer Novel Approaches to Adjusting for Positive Response Distortion (PRD) with the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI). They review the existing research efforts to detect and correct for PRD in major personality inventories, and thoroughly explore the Personality Assessment Inventory for its novel approaches of having Positive Impression Management predicted deviation scores and specific scores. The authors also present their empirical findings showing the utility and limitations of these two novel approaches of PAI to adjust for PRD. Chapter 28 also addresses the related issue of Applicant Faking Behavior, wherein Griffith and colleagues discuss this phenomenon and provide some insight regarding the findings at different levels of analysis (i.e., scale level, item level, composite level), propose a number of research questions that could be addressed at these levels of analysis, and discuss the implications of this research for the understanding of personality measurement. Towards the end of the volume, Johar and I have tried to make some explorative comments about Transforming Assessment: New Pedagogies for the Digital Age. We endeavor to explore the technological challenges and try to address the technical advances that can provide newer future directions to psychological assessment. Keeping the utility of varied technological innovations in focus, we attempt to envisage a vision for the diverse applications of newly emerging technologies in the realm of psychological assessment. After discussion of the technological challenges, Mukherjee and I sum up the volume in Chapter 30 with a comprehensive note on Ethical Issues in Personality Assessment. Ethical concerns always remain at the heart of psychological assessment, and therefore as an ethical editorial duty we discuss the ethical issues involved in personality assessment under various contexts in the light of the ethical guidelines established by different professional associations. Emphasis has been given to the crucial need to progressively evolve and refine the professional and ethical standards of personality assessment, to ensure ideal professional practices and to establish psychological assessment as an accurate and methodical discipline.

    The volume includes chapters authored by internationally acclaimed researcher/scholars in their respective fields, and thus attempts to provide a comprehensive, multidirectional perspective on personality assessment. I am privileged to have worked with some of the world’s leading researchers/academicians in editing this handbook, which would not have been possible at all without the unconditional support, diligence, commitment, and patience of the contributors. With its focus on the emerging trends and perspectives in personality assessment, the volume builds upon the knowledge base encapsulated by a plethora of existing research in the area, and provides many systematic directions and future possibilities in personality assessment. The volume also breaks new ground by exploring the possibility of using emerging technologies to find ever-expanding applications of personality assessment and enhance its predictive validity. The volume will provide a platform for discussion and a starting point for a fruitful endeavor towards developing a deeper understanding of the multifarious issues involved in personality assessment, and for providing a valuable input for structuring future developments in the area.

    Editing a handbook on such a vast area of research like personality assessment is indubitably a gruelling task, and I convey my gratitude to everyone who contributed to this extensive effort and provided me with steady support. I am deeply indebted to the authors for the time and effort they have poured into the project. Their exceptional scholarly work significantly contributed to this eminent and enlightening product. I hope this handbook will create a common platform of interaction for researchers, academics, psychometricians, human resource practitioners, clinical psychologists, forensic psychologists, social workers, and other professionals in the field. It will serve as a pivotal reference point for crucial issues, and will function to generate new ideas for research and the continual advancement of the field of personality assessment.

    UPDESH KUMAR

    Section I

    Emerging Conceptual Trends

    1

    Integrating Trait and Process Approaches to Personality: A Sketch of an Agenda

    Robert R. McCrae

    Gloucester, MA, USA

    Since its inception as a separate section of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Personality Processes and Individual Differences has implicitly defined the field of personality psychology. Great progress has been made in understanding individual differences in personality traits and related characteristics such as interests and values. But despite insightful and fruitful programs of research on such processes as self-regulation (Hoyle, 2010), attachment (Cassidy & Shaver, 2008), and identity formation (McAdams, 2003), there is nothing like a single, coherent account of personality processes as a whole. Perhaps this is because there are simply too many different processes at different levels of abstraction (and operating over different timescales) to allow an integration in the foreseeable future. But some commentators might have said the same about personality traits 40 years ago – and yet a reasonable understanding of the scope and hierarchical structure of traits has been achieved (John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008). The same will not happen for personality processes until the challenge is accepted and the task undertaken. Some theorists have begun this project (Fleeson & Jayawickreme, 2015; Hampson, 2012; Hooker & McAdams, 2003; Poropat & Corr, 2015); in this chapter I will sketch out another possible agenda that might structure efforts to understand the full scope and operation of personality processes. It builds on the greatest strength of contemporary personality psychology: our understanding of traits.

    Individual Differences: The Five-Factor Model

    The hierarchical structure of personality traits

    The story of how the welter of trait constructs, labels, and scales was made manageable by the rise of the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality has been told many times (Digman, 1990; McCrae & John, 1992). Today most psychologists recognize that, at a minimum, a comprehensive description of personality must include information on five very broad factors: Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Openness to Experience (O), Agreeableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C). Some measure of these five factors is now routinely included in studies of individual difference variables, even if only to show that the variable of chief interest to the researchers offers some incremental validity. It usually does, because there are many important individual differences that are not wholly explained by the topmost level of the FFM. Often, these variables can be construed as narrower traits, or facets. The NEO Personality Inventory-3 (NEO-PI-3: McCrae & Costa, 2010) assesses 30 facets, six for each factor. Facets were chosen to represent the most important constructs in the psychological literature, but they clearly do not exhaust the possibilities – for example, punctuality appears to be a facet of C (Roberts, Bogg, Walton, Chernyshenko, & Stark, 2004) not included in the NEO-PI-3.

    Some writers (Goldberg, 1993) have long argued that the trait hierarchy extends below the level of facets, to include distinct ways in which facet-level traits may be expressed. Roughly, these correspond to individual items in a trait scale. McCrae (2015) has called these lower-level traits nuances, and argued that they, like higher-level traits, are consensually valid, longitudinally stable, and heritable. Such claims can be examined by analyzing individual items from which the facet-level variance has been statistically removed, leaving only the item-specific variance. Research to date generally supports the hypothesis that nuances form a separate level of the trait hierarchy (Mõttus, McCrae, Allik, & Realo, 2014). What this means is that individuals are characterized over long periods of their lifespan not simply by five broad factors, but by a huge array of distinguishable tendencies. The person brings much more to each encounter with a situation than many of us had imagined.

    Considerations for assessment

    The hierarchical view of the FFM has three important implications for personality assessment. First, although assessment of the five factors is a useful addition to most research on individual differences – and essential to any understanding of the individual in clinical and other applied contexts – it is ultimately not sufficient. The assessment of facet-level traits (or at least a judicious selection of them) should become routine.

    Second, the scales used to assess higher order traits must be sufficiently long to ensure that they assess the trait of interest. Single-item scales suffer not only from low reliability and narrow content that limits their predictive validity (Credé, Harms, Niehorster, & Gaye-Valentine, 2012), but also from a confounding of higher-level variance with facet- and nuance-level variance (McCrae, 2015). A substantial amount of the variance in any single item is specific to that item; the higher-order trait the item is supposed to indicate can be separated from this incidental variance only by aggregating across many items from a longer scale.

    Third, researchers need to consider the use of individual items as predictors. On the one hand, they include valid trait variance that may prove particularly useful in understanding a specific criterion; researchers might formulate and test hypotheses about specific items (e.g., Terracciano et al., 2009). On the other hand, individual items are low in reliability, and the sheer number of items in a long inventory means that statistical tests, and thus opportunities for Type 1 errors, are multiplied in exploratory analyses. In addition, single items, unlike the scales they compose, have rarely been examined for construct validity. To understand the association of a particular nuance with some criterion, the researcher must rely on rational interpretation and, ideally, conduct corroborating research to demonstrate that the proposed interpretation is plausible.

    Nature and properties of FFM traits

    Armed with a comprehensive model and a variety of measures (De Raad & Perugini, 2002), personality psychologists have conducted systematic research and discovered a great deal about the nature of traits. These studies have illuminated the intrinsic nature of personality traits as well as documenting such properties as stability, developmental change, and universality.

    Traits have sometimes been construed narrowly as patterns of behavior, suggesting that they are something akin to well-established habits. But a careful consideration of their correlates shows that each of the five factors has motivational (Costa & McCrae, 1988), emotional (Costa & McCrae, 1996), and interpersonal (Costa & McCrae, 2010) manifestations. Traits at all levels of the hierarchy are psychologically deeper than mere habitual behaviors (Costa & McCrae, in ).

    They are deeper as well in the sense that they form a kind of core of personality in the broadest sense. FFM traits (Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Riemann, & Livesley, 1998) and their structure (Yamagata et al., 2006) are heritable; they endure over decades (Terracciano, Costa, & McCrae, 2006) despite the vicissitudes of life experience; and they emerge in strikingly similar form in widely different cultures (McCrae, Terracciano, & 78 Members, 2005). FFM traits are a central part of human nature, and each individual’s traits are central parts of his or her identity (McCrae & Costa, 1988a; Sheldon, Ryan, Rawsthorne, & Ilardi, 1997).

    The FFM in a framework for personality processes

    Cervone (2005) noted that there are two distinct meanings of the term personality structure: one refers to the organization of personality variables in a population (for example, the FFM), whereas the other refers to the organization and operation of personality mechanisms within an individual (most famously, Freud’s id, ego, and superego). Cervone argued that the FFM cannot refer to both; although it is a useful model of individual differences, it cannot represent the intrapsychic structure of personality. Conceptually, a dimension of individual differences cannot be a mechanism inside an individual’s head; emprically, the observed covariation of behaviors or states of any particular person need not, and often does not, mirror the covariation of traits in the population. For example, characteristic levels of cheerfulness and sadness are essentially independent in groups of people, but at any given moment, the degree of cheerfulness in an individual is inversely related to the degree of sadness (Diener & Emmons, 1984).

    So far, so good. But Cervone (2005) then concluded that this means that traits of the FFM cannot be causes for, or provide explanations of, the behavior and experience of individuals. This conclusion does not follow, and stems, it appears, from a failure to recognize two distinct meanings of the term trait. In one sense, trait refers to a characteristic of an individual (e.g., Mary’s curly hair or her trustworthiness); in a second sense, trait refers to a dimension of individual differences (e.g., curliness of hair or degree of trustworthiness). Tellegen (1991) distinguished the latter by calling it a trait dimension, and would say that the FFM is a structural model of personality trait dimensions. Of course, trait dimensions do not cause behavior in individuals, but intrapsychic traits may. Mary’s trustworthiness may be a (partial) explanation of why she followed through on her commitment to walk the dog.

    For most psychologists (and most laypersons), the idea that there are enduring dispositions within people that help explain their actions is so obvious that it hardly requires a defense. It is also intuitively clear that differences across people help to identify the nature of dispositions within them. We know that the A disposition is not the same as the E disposition because some people are agreeable but not extraverted, and some are extraverted but not agreeable. Readers interested in the arguments that have been offered against this common-sense position and the rebuttals to those arguments can consult McCrae and Costa (2008a; see also McCrae & Costa, 1995). For the present purpose, it suffices to say that it is legitimate to use FFM traits within the person when constructing an account of personality processes.

    Intrapsychic Structure and Personality Processes

    Needs and the need for traits

    Personality processes refer to the ways in which intrapsychic structures interact with each other and the world, so the study of processes or mechanisms must begin by identifying these internal structures – mental contents that account for how people act and feel. The variables that have received the most attention from recent personality process theorists are those that account for the purposeful direction of behavior: needs, motives, goals, strivings. Although important distinctions can be drawn among these constructs (Sheldon, 2011), they share an emphasis on selecting and guiding behavior. Dweck (1996) argued that our most meaningful affect, cognition, and behavior occur and cohere in relation to our goals (p. 349), and believed that they formed the most natural level of analysis for personality psychology. Cervone (2005) included goals along with beliefs and standards (or values) as the major intentional mental structures; each has associated appraisal processes. For example, we might evaluate political candidates in terms of our standards of public duty; interpret the results of a political poll as vindication of our belief that the public is ignorant; and decide to contribute to the underdog as a way of furthering our political agenda. Evaluating, interpreting, and planning to achieve our goals are personality processes.

    Cervone’s (2005) knowledge-and-appraisal personality architecture (KAPA) is based on rather abstract philosophical distinctions about intentional mental contents, and might serve as the basis for a computer simulation of personality. In particular, KAPA does not emphasize the human origin of goals and standards. In contrast, Deci and Ryan’s (2000) self-determination theory (SDT) also sees goals as central personality variables, but grounds them in basic human needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Goals themselves are relatively arbitrary – I may aspire to lose weight, or finish reading War and Peace, or join a cult – but according to SDT, goal-related pursuits will be rewarding and sustained if and only if they satisfy needs for autonomy, competence, or relatedness.

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are central concepts in SDT. Behavior that is performed because individuals feel free to follow their inner interests (Deci & Ryan, 2000, p. 234) is intrinsically motivated, and has the potential to satisfy needs for autonomy and competence. If, however, external influences undermine the sense of free behavior (e.g., by imposing a deadline or providing a monetary incentive), the same behavior is no longer as satisfying, and the enjoyment and quality of the performance decline. The mechanism here is a shift toward a more external perceived locus of causality (p. 234), a kind of reappraisal that occurs at the level of a specific behavior. Behavior that is performed ultimately because it is required by external forces (e.g., cultural mores) has extrinsic motivation, but the individual can derive some satisfaction of basic needs from these behaviors if he or she internalizes the values embodied in the behavior, ideally integrating them with other values and standards with which the person has identified. Internalization is a process that presumably occurs over a long period of time, and results in a change not simply in behavior, but in the inner nature of the person – one’s identity.

    In SDT, needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness are thought to be universal, so the only individual difference variables of interest are measures of the degree to which these needs are satisfied. That, in turn, is thought to be determined by the life experience of the individual, and by the individual’s reactions (including defenses) to frustrations of these needs. This emphasis on the environmental determinants of individual differences is understandable, given that much of the research upon which SDT is based was experimental. However, it seems odd for a theory of personality (especially one that values self-determination!) to ignore so completely the contribution of the person. There are at least two reasons why an account of intrapsychic structures should move beyond those universals emphasized by SDT to include innate individual differences, especially personality traits.

    First, there is empirical evidence that need satisfaction is related in part to personality traits. Understandably, agreeable extraverts are more satisfied with their relatedness than are antagonistic introverts; adjusted and conscientious extraverts have a greater sense of competence; and open and adjusted extraverts perceive higher levels of autonomy (Sabol, 2005; Weinstein, Pryzbylski, & Ryan, 2012). It is particularly noteworthy that need satisfaction can be thwarted by one’s own personality traits. Lamenting his unmet need for relatedness, the defiantly disagreeable Calvin once remarked to Hobbes, I wish I had more friends, but people are such jerks. (Watterson, 2012). Like SDT, Calvin blames the environment for his condition, although external observers might attribute the problem to his characteristic way of evaluating others.

    Second, a consideration of traits could supplement SDT’s account of intrinsic motivation. What is it that makes a particular kind of activity interesting for its own sake? There is a substantial literature trying traits to occupational and recreational interests (De Fruyt & Mervielde, 1997; Kandler, Bleidorn, Riemann, Angleitner, & Spinath, 2011). Charitable work may appeal to one high in A3: Altruism; organizing a messy desk may be more engaging to one high in C2: Order. Indeed, the whole concept of intrinsic motivation would seem most applicable to behaviors that are congruent with one’s trait profile. Personality processes cannot be fully understood without a consideration of traits as part of the intrapsychic structure. And as Hampson (2012) noted, studies demonstrate that the addition of personality traits increases the explanatory power of processes involving social-cognitive constructs (p. 329).

    Dynamic processes in Five-Factor Theory

    Five-Factor Theory (FFT; McCrae & Costa, 1996, 2008b) is a general theory of personality that was developed to account for the results of research on the FFM. The most striking research finding was that traits seemed to be relatively independent of the particular contexts in which they were expressed. They demonstrated continuity across much of the lifespan (Terracciano et al., 2006), showed little impact of childrearing practices and childhood environments (McCrae & Costa, 1988b), and were found in similar form in the most diverse cultures (McCrae & Costa, 1997). This was odd, because habits and interests certainly change with age; children learn language, religion, and food preferences from their parents; and values and customs vary widely by culture. It appeared to us that there must be a profound difference between personality traits and such things as habits, religion, and customs. FFT described traits as basic tendencies (BTs) and distinguishes the other phenomena as characteristic adaptations (CAs), because they are the unique adaptations that a particular person makes to specific life circumstances.

    Nowadays most personality theories are formulated as systems (Cervone, 2005), and FFT can be summarized in a system diagram. Figure 1.1 shows that the five broad personality factors are classed as BTs, but so are narrower facets and nuances of personality (as well as other characteristics such as intelligence, musical ability, and universal needs for air, food, and perhaps autonomy, competence, and relatedness). All these are postulated to have biological bases. However, many of the phenomena that psychologists are interested in (including beliefs, goals, skills, habits, roles, and relationships) are classed as CAs; the self-concept is singled out as a particularly important CA.

    Image described by caption and surrounding text.

    Figure 1.1 A representation of the Five-Factor Theory personality system, with examples of the contents of each component. Intrapsychic components are in rectangles; extrapsychic components are in ellipses.

    Adapted from McCrae and Costa (2008b).

    For the present purposes, the most relevant part of Figure 1.1 is the set of arrows marked dynamic processes, which represent the chief causal pathways between components of the system. Across a long time-frame, the crucial paths show the influences (a) of personality traits and (b) of external influences (EIs) on the development of CAs. For example, an individual high in Openness to Aesthetics who is given an opportunity to take piano lessons may develop technical skill and a musical repertoire. Across a short time-frame, Figure 1.1 shows that the individual behaviors that cumulate into the objective biography (OB) of the individual are influenced (c) by the habits, beliefs, and so on, of the person and (d) by the immediate demands and affordances of the situation. If asked to perform at a party, our pianist may draw on her skills and musical memory to play a popular tune.

    Although it is not obvious from the figure, FFT holds that these causal influences work jointly: adaptations are not simply the sum of trait influences plus life experience influences, but mental structures that evolve as individuals with particular traits interact over time with particular life experiences. Similarly, behaviors reflect the response to a given situation by a person with specific CAs.

    These two interactive mechanisms might be distinguished by the Piagetian terms accommodation (the creation of new mental structures, namely, CAs) and assimilation (the generation of acts and reactions consistent with existing CAs in each situation). These can be represented symbolically by the following formulae:

    These are the classes of psychological processes of central interest to personality psychologists; other arrows in Figure 1.1 refer to specialized instances of assimilation or accommodation (e.g., modifying the self-concept) or to interactions between the core and periphery of the personality system. The dynamic processes that generate BTs from biological bases are the province of neuroscience; those that account for the influence of the individual’s behavior on his or her environment are best studied by social psychologists or sociologists.

    The agenda

    How might one go about systematically studying personality processes? The pivotal role of CAs in the formulae above suggests a scheme: identify the major classes of CAs, and, for each, consider the accommodative processes that lead to the development of the CA, and the assimilative processes that lead to its expression in actions and reactions. Knowledge, for example, is a central CA in Cervone’s (2005) intrapsychic architecture, and knowledge is acquired through processes of learning and expressed through processes of problem-solving. Internalized values are crucial CAs for Deci and Ryan’s (2000) account of extrinsic motivation; they are developed through internalization and they lead to satisfying behavior when they are activated as guides to conduct.

    Unfortunately, FFT does not yet offer a comprehensive taxonomy of characteristic adaptations, but the examples it provides (see McCrae & Costa, 1996) show that it has a very broad scope. For example, interpersonal relationships – viewed intrapsychically – are CAs: configurations of beliefs, values, affects, and behavioral routines oriented toward a particular person. The processes that regulate the development and operation of relationships generally fall beyond the scope of goal models of personality processes – falling in love or coming to hate one’s boss are not usually goal-directed – but they are certainly processes in which personality psychologists (e.g., attachment theorists) have a strong interest. Table 1.1 lists some examples of CAs and their associated processes.

    Table 1.1 Some examples of personality processes associated with characteristic adaptations

    Note: BTs (basic tendencies) interacting with EIs (external influences) lead to the development of CAs (characteristic adaptations) through accommodative processes. The CAs lead to actions and reactions, that is, the OB (objective biography), through assimilative processes. Table notes document associations between traits and accommodative processes. BTs may also affect assimilative processes as moderator variables, although that is not represented in this table.

    aKaufman et al. (2010); bBickle (1996); cMcCrae and Löckenhoff (2010); dRyder, Alden, and Paulhus (2000); eEinstein and Lanning (1998); fOlver and Mooradian (2003); gNettle (2005); hShaver and Brennan (1992); iJohnson and Morgeson (2005); jTesch and Cameron (1987).

    Table 1.1 does not, however, explain how BTs, and especially FFM traits, are involved in creating CAs. Postulate 6b of FFT, Differential dynamics, states that some dynamic processes are affected differentially by basic tendencies of the individual, including personality traits (McCrae & Costa, 1996, p. 75). Take learning, for example. Clearly, the most relevant BT is general intelligence, sometimes defined as the capacity for learning. However, personality traits also affect the learning process, through learning styles or strategies (Bickle, 1996). Costa and Piedmont (2003) described styles of learning based on the two factors of O and C. Open individuals are intrinsically interested in new knowledge, although their interests may not be well focused; conscientious people are diligent scholars who learn well because they apply themselves. Combinations of these two factors (or their lack) lead to different styles of learning. All five factors affect the development of CAs. For example, N predisposes individual to form anxious attachments (Shaver & Brennan, 1992), and E leads people to develop social and enterprising vocational interests (Costa, McCrae, & Holland, 1984).

    Note that many of the characteristics that we normally class as correlates of personality traits are acquired through time and experience. No one is born with an innate fondness for skydiving, but almost everyone encounters situations with an element of danger. For some people these are unpleasant encounters, and they learn to avoid them. For others – extraverted excitement-seekers – they provide a special rush that the individual relishes. From peers, movies, and video games, excitement seekers learn the options available in their culture to satisfy the need for thrills, and, given the opportunity, they may try them and find that they greatly enjoy them. Skydiving may become a hobby, the source of a new social circle, and a salient part of the extravert’s identity.

    McCrae and Costa (1991) distinguished between temperamental and instrumental links between traits and psychological wellbeing, and Hampson (2012) pointed out that these roughly parallel the distinction between moderator and mediator effects. One might further argue that assimilative processes sometimes show the moderating effects of traits on the expression of CAs, whereas accommodative processes explain how trait manifestation is mediated by CAs. To return to the example of skydiving, a reaction to a first experience of risk-taking is moderated by level of E: everyone may experience some degree of pleasant excitement, but the extravert feels much more than the introvert. We say that such people are temperamentally suited to thrill-seeking. In contrast, becoming a skydiver is a lengthy process that requires training, financial investment, socialization into a subculture, perhaps the development of a new life narrative. All of this is instrumental, with skydiving mediating the satisfaction of the extravert’s need for excitement.

    Table 1.1 illustrates that multiple processes may be associated with any given CA, and the example of learning shows that different personality factors may affect the same process. In some ideal future, Table 1.1 might be an exhaustive catalog, with all relevant processes listed for each CA, each process classified by the factor (or facet) that affects it. Imagine, then, sorting the rows by personality factor; the result would be a compendium of the ways in which personality traits get outside the skin (Hampson, 2012).

    McCrae and Löckenhoff (2010) provided a conceptual analysis of how and why individuals high in C come to show high levels of self-control. McCrae (1976) had proposed a number of techniques for self-control, and McCrae and Löckenhoff argued that most of them would be easier for individuals high in C. For example, balance of interest is a technique in which people delay or deny gratification by focusing on long-term goals. Because conscientious people have better formulated life goals than undirected people, the balance tips toward self-control more frequently for them. Again, commitment is a technique in which people make resolutions and wager their self-esteem on the success with which they keep them; high C individuals have a strong need for achievement which motivates such a wager. Self-control requires the expenditure of effort (Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, 1998), and conscientious people have more of the purposeful energy that can fuel efforts.

    These are, of course, interpretations rather than established facts, but they illustrate how testable hypotheses can be generated by considering the ways in which personality traits affect the processes that produce relevant outcomes. Process research fills in the causal gaps between assessed traits and their established correlates.

    Complexities

    The tidy organization of Table 1.1 belies the true complexity of understanding processes, and anyone considering work on the proposed agenda needs to be forewarned. Here are a few of the issues that pose complications:

    Personality traits affect some CAs more than others. English speakers growing up in Boston acquire an accent that probably has little or nothing to do with their traits. Not all psychological processes are personality processes.

    Some CAs are more durable than others, and the theoretical status of shortlived phenomena is not always clear. The lifelong goal of becoming a great novelist is surely a CA, but what about the personal project (Little, Lecci, & Watkinson, 1992) of completing an assigned essay by Wednesday? Is that a CA, or better regarded as an outcome, an instance of behavior that is to be explained rather than an intrapsychic structure used to provide explanations?

    Like all causal chains, personality processes are indefinitely divisible into subprocesses. Achieving a goal may involve perceiving a situation, appraising it as an opportunity to meet a need, formulating a plan, making an effort, using a skill. In turn, formulating a plan may involve remembering learned strategies, weighing the alternatives, coordinating with other planned or ongoing activities, and so on. An exhaustive explanation is impossible; researchers must select a segment of the causal chain that is convenient for research or useful in applications.

    Processes may be multidetermined. Solving a problem may involve divergent thinking moderated by O, repeated efforts requiring high levels of C, and cooperation with others working on the problem (facilitated by A). Different environments can shape or constrain the operation of a psychological process. Further, outcomes often show equifinality, the same result emerging from different processes. The path from traits to behaviors is through personality processes, but there are many alternate routes.

    These considerations are not intended to discourage research on personality processes, but they do suggest that no single individual or research team will be able to provide a comprehensive account of them. Perhaps personality psychologists should approach this task like the Human Genome Project, a grand collective effort with enormous potential payoff. (Or perhaps, given the realities of funding in the social sciences, crowd sourcing is a better model.) The researchers cited in the notes to Table 1.1 are pioneers in this project.

    Assessing Personality Traits and Processes

    An agenda with some similarity to that offered here was proposed by Hooker and McAdams (2003) in what they called the Six-Foci Model. In place of BTs, CAs, and the self-concept, it specifies traits, goals, and life narratives as structures. A class of processes is associated with each structure. In an ambitious study, Hooker, Choun, Mejía, Pham, and Metoyer (2013) assessed the five factors in 99 older adults, and then used internet technology to survey daily stress and progress toward health and social goals over 100 days. Using multilevel analysis, they showed that N hindered, and E and C facilitated, progress toward goals. Sophisticated analyses also revealed interactions between perceived daily stress and traits; in particular, stress interfered with goal progress chiefly among those high in N.

    Studies such as this, which trace patterns of action and reaction over time and analyze them in conjunction with enduring traits, will play an important role in research on personality processes. However, many research designs and assessment strategies can advance our understanding of processes without requiring such a commitment of time and participant effort.

    The simplest way to build up a body of information about traits and processes is routinely to include measures of traits – at least the five broad factors – in experimental studies. If a given trait facilitates the operation of a process, then those individuals with higher levels of that trait should show more effects in the experimental conditions that activate the process. Historically, these trait moderator effects were called Trait × Treatment interactions, because they were usually concerned with psychotherapy. Clearly, this design has broader applicability. Ideally, specific hypotheses would be tested, but systematic exploratory analyses are also worth reporting. Future meta-analyses could make sense of even small and occasional findings. Social, educational, and clinical psychologists could – and should – contribute to this literature.

    One of the advantages of experimental studies is that the participant need not understand which processes are activated; many of them are below the level of awareness. But in some cases, people know full well what is going on in their heads and can report about it if asked. Surely the most widely researched personality processes are ways of coping (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985). McCrae and Costa (1986) asked respondents to recall a particular stressful event and then to indicate which of a series of coping responses they made (and whether it helped them solve the problem or feel better). Carver, Scheier, and Weintraub (1989) created a dispositional coping measure, presumably tapping recurrent coping strategies. Different ways of coping are meaningfully related to basic personality traits (Watson & Hubbard, 1996); in particular, individuals high in N tend to use immature and ineffective mechanisms.

    The scope of personality traits is reasonably circumscribed; one can measure most traits of interest with a few hundred items. It does not seem likely that researchers will create comparable omnibus personality process measures, because there are too many discrete processes – McCrae (1982), for example, identified 28 different ways of coping. Instead, assessments of process variables are likely to be specialized and domain-specific; tools that are useful for particular research topics or for educational, clinical, or other applications. Global personality trait assessments will remain useful because they summarize the outcomes of a myriad of internal processes. Regardless of the how or why, extraverts will usually end up acting and reacting like extraverts, neurotics like neurotics. For those trying to understand people, this is very valuable

    Enjoying the preview?
    Page 1 of 1