An Assessment of The Motor Ability of Leaners
An Assessment of The Motor Ability of Leaners
An Assessment of The Motor Ability of Leaners
E
112
11
1 0 8 w ~ ~
7YEARS
6MONTHS
?YEARS
7MONTHS
?YEARS
SMONTHS
7YEARS
9MONTHS
7YEARS
10MQNTHS
7YEARS
11MONTHS
Figure 4.2 Mean score for age group differences in running speed and agility.
7YEARS ?YEARS ?YEARS 7YE.A,RS
6MONTHS ?MONTHS 8MONTHS 9MONTHS
Figure 4.3 Mean score for age group differences in balance.
56
?YEARS
10MONTHS
TYEARS
11MONTHS
3.2
"
3
8
2.9
E
2.8
2.7
lYEA.RS
6MONTHS
7YEARS
7MONTHS
?YEARS
aMONTHS
?YEARS
9MONTHS
7YEARS
10MONTHS
?YEARS
11MONTHS
Figure 4.4 Mean score for age group differences in bilateral coordination.
6_21---_
7YEARS
6MONTHS
7YEARS
7MONTHS
?YEARS
8MONTHS
lYEARS
9MONTH$
7YEARS
10MONTH$
?YEARS
11MONTH$
Figure 4.5 Mean score for age group differences in strength.
57
4.5
4
3.5
3
2.5
c
2
E
1.5
?YEARS
6MONTHS
?YEARS
7MONTHS
?YEARS
8MONTHS
?YEARS
9MONTHS
?YEARS
1QMONTHS
?YEARS
11MONTHS
Figure 4.6 Mean score for agegroup differences in upper-limb coordination.
This pattern of higher scores and better performances found in the older children
confers with literature. As children grow older, they will show improved motor
proficiency (Govatos, 1959; Rarick, 1980; Krombholz 1997).
4.2 COMPARISON OF MOTOR PROFICIENCY SCORES FOR THE
LEARNERS FROM THE PIETERMARITZBURG AND EMPANGENI
REGIONS.
4.2.1 Campositive result scores (Empangeni versus Pietermarit2burg)
Compositive score results for boys and girls represent the differences in the five
areas of motor proficiency between the Empangeni and the Pietermaritzburg
areas of KwaZulu- Natal. The differences between these two regions in all five of
58
the areas of motor proficiency represented by skills for running speed and agility
(t=1.60), balance (t=0.66), bilateral coordination (t=0.18), strength (t=3.55) and
upper limb coordination (t=1.69), are significant at p< .05 (Table 4.2).
Table 4.2 Mean scores, standard deviations and t values of sub-test items for
all subjects tested in Empangeni and Pietermaritzburg
Sub Test
Items
Empangeni Pietermaritzburg t values
Compositive Compositive
scores (n=123) scores (n=366) Empangeni
r.'-:'-'--'-'-'--'''-r-='=:=..L..--+:'-:'''''-=-'''--''-'-r::c::'-'---j vs PMB
Mean SD Mean SD
Omega
squared
Running 11.22 1.93 11.54 1.85 -1.60. 0.002
Balance 7.75 2.39 7.58 2.56 0.66 -0.001
Bilateral
2.99 0.84 3.01 0.94 -0.18. -0.002
Coordination
Strength 7.29 1.44 6.73 1.72 3.55
0.023
Upper limb
3.63 1.10 3.43
1
1
.
32 1.69
0.004
Coordination
w = omega squared
.significant at p<.05
4
2
OCOMPOSITIVE SCORES
Empangeni
OCOMPOSIT1VE SCORES PMB
Running
BalanCe Bi-lateral Coordination Strength Upper--limb Coordination
Figure 4.7 Mean score of motor proficiency for all the learners from Empangeni
and Pietermaritzburg.
59
These results show that the greatest significant differences fall in the area of
strength ability. Here, learners from the Empangeni region scored 2.3% (w
2
=
0.023) higher than the learners from Pietermaritzburg. This difference in strength
scores can be attributed to the differences in the constructive nature of play
within the two regions. Children in the small town of Empangeni might find
themselves more inclined to engage in physical activities outside (farm-like
activities) which would possibly include actions such and moving, pushing and
carrying heavy objects. Children from the Empangeni region also scored higher
in tests for balance, strength and upper limb coordination. These superior scores
may also be attributed to the difference in weather conditions experienced in the
separate towns. Empangeni experiences a considerably milder winter compared
to Pietermaritzburg. These findings are in agreement with literature by Kohll and
Hobbs (1998) and the National Children Youth and Fitness Study (1987). These
studies show that activity levels are higher during the warmer months and drop
during the colder months. As Empangeni does not experience a terribly cold
wintry season, children in this region are able to play outside for a much longer
duration of the year, thus, increasing their accessibility to physical activity.
4.2.2 Comparison of boys' motor proficiency scores between the two
regions
Scores for the five areas of motor proficiency in Table 4.3 represent the
differences between the scores for male learners from Empangeni and
Pietermaritzburg. The male learners from Pietermaritzburg performed better in
60
the running speed and agility tests (Cl} =0.0015), balance (cl =0.0004) and upper
limb coordination (0)2 =0.0032), while the male learners from Empangeni scored
higher on the bilateral coordination (Cl} =0.0038) and strength tests (Cl} =0.0051).
However, none of the differences in scores for the learners from the two regions
were significant. Figure 4.8 represents the mean scores of the sub-tests
between the male learner from Empangeni and Pietermarilzburg
Table 4.3
Mean scores, standard deviation and t values of sub test items for
boys from Empangeni and Pietermarilzburg
Sub Test
Empangeni Pietermarilzburg tvalues
Omega
Bo s (n=58) Bo s (n=180) Empangeni
Items
Mean SO Mean SO vsPMB
squared
Running
11.88 1.87 12.19 1.54 -1.16 0.0015
Balance
6.95 2.53 7.31
1
2
.
53 -0.95 -0.0004
,
Bilateral
2.97 0.94 2.92 0.92 0.31 -0.0038
Coordination
Strength
7.60 1.49 7.26
1
1
.
63 1.49 0.0051
Upper limb
3.67 1.15 3.76 i 1.26 -0.50 -0.0032
Coordination
I
w = omega squared
61
Running Balance Bilateral Coordination Strength Upper..jimb Coordination
Figure 4.8 Mean score of motor proficiency for boys from Empangeni and
Pietennarilzburg.
4.2.3 Comparison of girls' motor proficiency scores between the two
regions
Motor skill ability scores for female learners from Empangeni and
Pietermaritzburg presented significant differences at p<0.05 for four of the five
areas of gross motor proficiency. These skills included balance (t=1.97), bilateral
coordination (t=0.65), strength (t=3.84) and upper limb coordination (t=3.00).
Scores for balance and strength tests were performed 1.13%, 5.19% higher
respectively, while upper limb coordination was performed 3.08% better by the
female learners from Empangeni. Running speed and agility as well as bilateral
coordination test scores for girls presented more proficient in the
Pietermaritzburg area.
62
Table 4.4 Mean scores, standard deviation and t values of sub test items for
girls from Empangeni and Pietermaritzburg
Sub Test
Empangeni Pietermaritzburg tvalues
Omega
Items
Girls (n=65) Girls (n=186) Empangeni
squared
Mean SD Mean ,SD vsPMB
Running 10.63 1.79 10.90 1.92 -1.04 0.0003
Balance 8.46 2.02 7.84
1
2
.
56 1.97 0.0113
Bilateral
3.02 0.76 3.09 0.96 -0.65. -0.0023
Coordination
Strength 7.02 1.35 6.22
1
1
.
65 3.84
0.0519
Upper limb
3.6 1.07 3.12
1
1
.
31 3.00
0.0308
Coordination
w = omega squared
significant at p<.05
12
10
a
0
u
6
0
E
4
2
0
Running Balance Bilateral Coordination Strength Upper4imb Coordination
Figure 4.9 Mean score of motor proficiency scores for girls from Empangeni and
Pietermaritzburg.
63
The differences seen in their test scores of balance, strength and upper-limb
coordination may be attributed to the possibility that girls in the Empangeni area
may have an increased exposure to physical activity and that a significant total
time of this play takes place outdoors, where balance, strength and upper-limb
coordination have a greater prevalence to develop as opposed to their
Pietermaritzburg counterparts who may not be inclined to play outdoors as often.
Although girls from Pietermaritzburg excelled at bilateral coordination which is
associated more with games, dance and specific activities completed indoors.
The composite scores discussed in 4.2.1 rely highly on the significant differences
identified in the girls' scores between the two regions. Four of the five areas of
motor proficiency were proven significant in the girls scores compared to only
one area in the scores of the boys. Thus, the girls in the Empangeni and
Pietermaritzburg region display noteworthy differences in their motor proficiency.
The girls from Empangeni show better overall performances in the physical skills
compared to the Pietermaritzburg subjects tested. The possible cause of such
findings may be as a result of the girls in the Empangeni region possibly being
more inclined to play outdoors and participate in more of the "tom-boy" like
activities compared to the girls from the city.
4.3 MOTOR PROFICIENCY SCORES AND GENDER DIFFERENCES
Table 4.5 and its corresponding Figure 4.10, represent the results of the
comparative scores of girls versus boys. The results are compositive scores of
both the Empangeni and the Pietermaritzburg subjects tested. The t scores for
all five areas of motor proficiency have a significant difference at p<.05. The
boys performed significantly better on their sub-tests of running speed and agility
(t=8.08), strength (t=6.31) and upper-limb coordination (t=4.47). The girls scored
favourable results on their tests of balance (t=-3.47) and bilateral coordination
(t=-1.67). The biggest differences in scores were seen on the test of running
speed and agility and strength where the boys scored 11.6% and 7.4% better
than the girls respectively.
Table 4.5 Mean scores, standard deviation and t values of sub test items for
boys versus girls (compositive scores of the two regions tested)
Sub Test
BOYS GIRLS tvalues
Omega
I
(n=238) (n=251) Boys vs
Items
Mean SO Mean Iso Girls
squared i
Et:>;:'" <",,)<; <,;'i ,,:;d ;,,) ..:: ' .. ';'i :.i;i;'!;:;';;! .. ir,;':::::....(,: I
1.63
!
i
Running
12.12 10.83 ! 1.89 8.08 0.116
1
,
..
Balance 7.22 2.53 8.00
I
-3.47. 0.022
I
1
2
.45
I
..
Bilateral
I
!
Coordination
2.93 0.93 3.07
! 0.91
-1.67. 0.004
i
7.34 1.60 6.43
i
6.31
i
Strength ! 1.62
0.074
i
Upper limb
3.74
I
! 3.24 i 1.27 4.47 10.037
I
! 1.23
I Coordination
I I I
w'= omega squared
significant at p<.05
65
Running Balance Bilateral Coordination Strength Upper-limb Coordination
Figure 4.10 Mean score of motor proficiency for boys versus girls (Compositive
scores from Empangeni and Pietermarilzburg).
Although it is generally expected that little difference in motor proficiency scores
between genders will be seen in the early elementary school years, some doubt
has been cast on this generalization. Numerous research by Rarick (1980);
Butterfield et al. (1993) and Krombholz (1997) has shown the differences in
motor proficiency between girls and boys to be significant, where girls exceed on
some items and boys on others. Reasons for these gender differences, may be
attributed to the fact that parents, peers, teachers, and coaches, who provide
opportunities, encourage girls and boys toward different activities. Girls are
generally encouraged to play quietly and practise fine motor skills such as
drawing and colouring in, whilst boys are encouraged to participate in more
vigorous movement activities such as running, chasing and jumping (Bouchard et
aI., 1994).
66
4.4 MOTOR PROFICIENCY AND THREE DEMOGRAPHIC SCHOOL
CATEGORIES
4.4.1 Differences in motor proficiency between the three demographic
classes of schools tested in the Pietermaritzburg region
ANOVA was completed to determine the differences between motor proficiency
levels within the three demographic groups of the schools tested in
Pietermaritzburg. These groups included independent schools (n=75),
government schools (n=270) and low socio-economic govemment schools
(n=21).
This ANOVA analysis was completed for each of the five areas of motor
proficiency assessed and the meaningfulness of these results was determined by
omega squared. The results show significant differences between the categories
of schools for performances in all of the five areas assessed, and can be
expressed as follows (Table 4.6). F is significant for running speed and agility
and may be described as, F (2,807) = 560.10052p<O.5. F is significant for
balance skills between the 3 demographic school categories F (2,807) =
333.2691 p<0.5 as well as for bilateral coordination F (2,807) = 381.9516p<0.5.
Lastly, significant differences are also seen between the three school categories
on their tests of strength and upper-limb coordination, F may be described as F
(2,807) = 462.4185p<0.5 and F (2,807) = 304.0145p<0.5 respectively.
67
Table 4.6 Degree of freedom and F values of the sub test items for the 3
different demographic categories of schools in Pietermaritzburg
Subtest Item F-Value
Running 2 807 560.10_
Balance 2 807 333.27-
Bilateral
2 807 381.95-
Coordination
Strength 2 807 462.42_
Upper-limb
2 807 304.01-
Coordination
- significant at p<.05
14
12
10
8
6
4
o
Running Balance Bilateral Coordination Strength Upper-limb Coordination
Figure 4.11 Mean score of motor proficiency for the different demographic
categories of schools tested in PietermarilZburg.
68
4.4.2 Motor proficiency scores for independent versus low socio-
economic schools
Interesting results are shown in the comparison of the independent school and
the iow socio-economic school, motor proficiency scores in the Pietermaritzburg
region. Three of the sub-eategories for motor proficiency show favourable results
by low socio-economic schools. These skills include balance, bilateral
coordination and upper-limb coordination. The other two remaining areas of
motor proficiency running and strength exhibited superior scores performed by
independent school learners.
The most prevalent and significant difference in scores, was seen in the scores
related to upper-limb coordination (t=5.17), which was performed 24.8% better by
the learners from a low socio-economic school than the learners from an
independent institution. On the test of running speed and agility independent
school learners narrowly beat their lower socio-economic counterparts by 1%.
The tests of balance and bilateral coordination expressed 4% and 5% differences
respectively, favouring the low socio-economic school learners.
independent school learners performed 3% better on their test of strength.
69
Lastly
Table 4.7 Mean scores, standard deviation and t values of sub test items for
learners from Independent and Low socio-economic schools
Independent Low Socio- tvalues
School Scores Economic School Independent
Sub Test (n=75) Scores (n=21) Vs Low Omega
Items Socio- squared
Mean SO Mean SO Ecomonic
Schools
Running 11.94 2.04 11.90 1.26 0.12 -0.01
Balance 8.25 2.23 9.29 1.62 -2.36 0.05
Bilateral
3.13 0.92 3.62 0.74 -2.51 0.05
Coordination
Strength 7.25 1.59 6.57 1.25 2.07 0.03
Upper limb
I
Coordination
3.55 1.29 4.71 0.64 i -5.17
0.25
w = omega squared
significant at p<0.05
14
12
10
B
6
4
2
o
Running speed and
agility
Balance Bilateral coordination strength Upperlimb coordination
Figure 4.12 Differences in mean scores of motor performance between
independent school leamers, and low socio-economic government
school learners from Pietermaritzburg.
70
Results of this nature have not been readily written in literature to-date, only an
article by Krombholz (1997) showed contradictory findings to these, stating that
children of a higher socio-economic status performed better than children of a
lower status. A possible reasoning for findings of such a study would be the
effect of leisure activities during the pre-primary years (3-5years) of development.
Children attending low socio-economic schools may not be exposed to as strict a
physical activity program as children attending independent schools, or have
state of the art equipment, facilities and trained educators. Yet on some tasks
these children scored higher than those attending school with such amenities.
There are two possible explanations for such findings; which involves previous
exposure to physical activities and leisure activities during pre-school and
foundation school years. According Krombholz (1997) and Graft et al., (2004),
an analysis of children's leisure behaviour showed that children who are more
active-either in organized extramural activities and/or on a regular basis do have
better gross motor development, and that, children with a higher weekly
Television viewing frequency tended to demonstrate poorer gross motor
development.
Children attending independent schools are often exposed to a busy afternoon of
extramural activities which may possibly take away from their outdoor play time.
The parents of these children are also more conservative in terms of safety, and
often restrict playing outdoors and interacting in neighbourhood activities. On the
71
other end of the scale children attending the low socia-economic schools are
most likely to grow up out of town, perhaps on a farm in the hills where toys are
not much other than a ball and rope, or possibly a home-made bat, and what-
ever else their imagination can come up with. These children have limited
access to things like a television or computer, they experience hours of
unstructured play outside, where imagination is the only educator and the
environment their endless room of equipment Isn't this the time these subjects
invest in developing gross motor skills? This rural world has been compared to
the world of a child attending an independent school, with many amenities plus a
television, a DVD player, and a computer. A media world at their finger tips
(many from a young age), with little will to experience real play or encouragement
to experience the outdoors. This comparison agrees with research by Loucaides
et aI., (2004) which shows that children who attend rural schools or grow up in a
rural environment, spend more time outside than those children who attend
urban schools.
Finally, the task which tested strength was scored higher by those attending
independent schools. This may possibly be attributed to malnutrition and a lack
of protein in the diet of the children attending a low socio-economic school, who
currently mayor may not have a feeding scheme in place.
72
4.5 MOTOR PROFICIENCY AND PHYSICAL EDUCATION
The discussion in 4.4 relates to the three demographic categories of schools
assessed, to which various patterns regarding their physical education class
structure can be assigned. The independent schools encompass a high
standard of physical education where classes are presented twice per week for
learners (Grade 1 &2) for a duration of 45 minutes each, and are constructively
planned and coordinated by an adequately qualified teacher. The classes mostly
include the learning and practising of basic gross motor skills required for
particular sporting games. These schools also include compulsory afternoon
sport sessions where these learners engage in learning, practicing and playing a
particular sport code. It was established that physical education classes in the
government and low socio-economic government schools, take place on average
only once per week in the majority of these schools. Most often these classes
are presented by the class teachers themselves and often involve the children
playing specific sports, where most of the learners are unable to perform basic
gross motor skills required for the different sports. At these schools there is no
regular compulsory afternoon sporting activities. These activities are offered on
a voluntary basis only. This set-up, where after-school sport is not compulsory
for the learners, allows those who are less confident to shy away from sport at a
young age, and most often never to participate in sport again possibly not even in
their high school career.
73
Possible causes of such findings may again be further explained in literature by
Saakslahti et aI., (1999); Thomas (1999); Rudisill et aI., (2002) and Graft et aI.,
(2004) who have discussed the negative influences of the lack of physical activity
and, the fact that children, who are more active, -either in organized extramural
activities and/or on a regular basis- have better gross motor development. These
observations made may also be attributed to how ~ m is spent during the
different physical education classes, to what extent the refining of motor skills
has been expressed and the extent of the quality of the feedback given; a
phenomenon for low motor skill acquisition (Graham, 1987). For example,
physical education lessons completed at an independent school take place more
often than at a government school, and there is also compulsory after-school
sport at these schools which most often the government schools don't inflict. The
physical education lessons at such schools include extreme refining of motor
skills, positive specific feedback and limited time for games. Whereas, at a
government school, and this is possibly also associated with an increase in the
number of children per class, most classes are more often games and specific
sports codes with relative feedback comments, little refinement of basic motor
skills and no individualized attention. This influence of a physical education trend
shows remarkably in the results.
74
Findings from this study show that motor proficiency trends in todays children
vary according to age and gender. The type and amount of physical education
children are exposed to before and during their elementary school years also
influences motor performance.
5.1 CONCLUSIONS
1. Gender differences in motor proficiency scores were significant. Boys are
no longer significantly stronger than girls in all motor skills. Girls show
strength in skills such as balance and bilateral coordination.
2. An increase in chronological age is associated with an improved
performance of motor skills.
3. Learners attending independent schools scored superior levels of motor
proficiency, in contrast to government school learners who scored lower.
75
4. Physical education structures between the schools shows an influence on
motor performance, as well as a child's previous exposure to physical
activity during pre-primary years.
5.1.1 Gender and motor proficiency
The results of this stUdy agreed with research by Krombholz (1997). who stated
that girls fare better in some skills while boys perform better in others. In this
specific study the differences are statistically significant. Girls out performed
boys in tests for balance and bilateral coordination, while boys were significantly
stronger in tests of running speed and strength. Upper-limb coordination was
closely contested with the girls being narrowly defeated. Since physical
characteristics of girls and boys are very similar prior to puberty, it would seem
probable that differences in motor performance may be environmentally
encouraged and attributed to the preferential motor activities of girls and boys
Krombholz (1997). Also, as a result of our modern society, girls are brought-up
and encouraged to participate in physical activity more now than ever before.
With such gender equality, in respect to physical activity, gender differences in
motor skill performance will soon be closely marginalized.
5.1.2 Age and motor proficiency
Motor proficiency commonly improves with age, but there are some skills in
which older learners, possibly because of a proportionate increase in physical
growth will be outperformed by a younger counter-part. These outcomes are in
76
agreement with research by Govatas (1959); Rarick (1980) and Krombholz
(1997). Results demonstrated in the study, show improved motor performance
by the older learners in four of the five areas of motor proficiency. Balance being
the only task which is performed better by the younger learner. This result is
possibly because older learners have experienced an unbalanced growth in
stature and seem to be clumsy in terms of body control. A stronger, core stability
may counteract the awkward body control associated with a disproportionate
growth experienced during child development. However. further investigations of
this topic would be necessary.
5.1.3 Motor proficiency and the three demographic classes of schools
assessed
Children who are growing up in a rural environment and who attend a Low soda-
economic government school, showed better acquisition in three of the five areas
of motor skill performance (balance. bilateral and upper-limb coordination)
possibly as a result of leisure activities they were exposed to during the important
(pre-primary) fundamental development years. Such scores also verify the
negative effects modern society and technology is initiating with regards to
movement patterns and basic gross motor skills, which should be developed
during childhood.
5.1.4 Physical education and motor proficiency
Participation in a physical education program can result in significant
improvements in motor proficiency (Light et al.. 2000). The structure and number
77
of these physical education classes contribute to overall motor performance. The
independent schools curriculum allows for physical education classes to be
presented twice per week as well as a structured compulsory after-school
sporting program which is in place. In contrast, government schools only allow
for on average one class per week, without any compulsory after school activity
plan in place. The structure and nature of these physical education classes,
regardless of number, is also very important. At this young developing age
(under 8 years), fundamental motor skills need to be practised and mastered in
order for these skills to become autonomous in sports specific situations. It has
been the experience of this author that when physical education is assigned to a
time period during the school day in a government organization, that children
often spend the time in an unstructured environment or are "taught" to play
specific sports such as soccer, hockey, etc. Again this is in contrast to the
independent school set-up, where these lessons are prepared and the aim is for
the learners to refine and master fundamental tasks in dynamic situations.
In despair to low motor proficiency scores by learners, literature by Smith and
O'Keefe (1999), shows that low motor performance levels can be improved
through appropriate and disciplined Physical Education lessons and learners up
to the age of 12 years can catch up to achieve full development. Sufficient time
in the elementary school curricula should be devoted to physical activity, as it has
been stated by Shepard (1997) that added time for physical activity, has shown
78
no reduction in grades, many learners contrarily demonstrated improvement in
their grades.
The relationship between gross motor performance and previous exposure to
movement programs during the elementary school years of a child (4 -7years) -
which are most prevalent for initial fundamental motor skill development (Burton
1992) - is a topic that requires further investigation.
5.2 RECOMMENDATIONS
The results of this study also lead to recommendation of topics for further
research.
A continuation of such research is necessary to establish norm values of
gross motor skills for South African children at various developmental
stages of childhood. Gross motor skills should be concentrated on, as
these are skills required to develop movement patterns for every day living
and promote better social interactions.
The issue of pre-primary exposure to physical activity should also be
taken into consideration in a study of a child's experience in motor
learning at pre-school, or what is now known as grade R. This may be
where the learning phase is most predominant and experience will be
most beneficial.
79
The importance of physical education in the South African school curricula
should be reconsidered.
Finally, it would be interesting to continue to compare and investigate the
difference in motor competency of higher socio-economic and lower socio-
economic learners.
80
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders. Washington DC.
ARNOLD, P.J. (1979). Meaning in Movement, Sport and Physical Education.
London: Heineman
BLACK, S. (1995). Just do it. Executive Educator, 17(4): 33-36.
BOUCHARD, C., SHEHARD, R.J., &STEPHENS., (eds) 1994). Physical activity,
fitness and health: lntemational proceedings and consensus statement.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
BRUININKS, R.H. (1978). Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency,
Manual. Circle Pines, Minnesota: AGS.
BUNKER, l.K. (1981). Elementary Physical Education and Youth Sport. Journal
Physical Education and Recreation, 52(2): 26-28.
BURTON, AW. (1992). The Development of Movement Skills. Early Report,
vol. 19. no. 2.
BURTON, A.w., &DAVIS, W.E. (1992). Assessing balance in Adapted Physical
Education: Fundamental concepts and applications. Adapted Physical Activity
Quarterly, 9: 14-46.
BUTTERFIELD, S.A & LOOVIS, E.M. (1993). Influence of Age, Sex, Balance
and Sport Participation on Development of Throwing by Children in Grade k-8.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76: 459-464
CARSON, L.M. (2001). The I am learning Curriculum. Teaching Elementary
Physcial Education, 12 (5): 9-13.
CINTAS, H.L. (1995). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in development
and the impact of parental expectations on motor behaviour. Pediatric Physical
Therapy, 7, 103-111.
81
CORBIN; C.B.; PANGRAZI, RB. & LE MASURIER, G.C. (2004). Physical
Activity for Children: Current Patterns and Guidelines. Presidents Council of
Fitness and Sports, Research Digest, 5(2).
CORSO, M. (1993). Is developmentally appropriate physical education the
answer to children's school readiness? Journal of Health, Physical Education,
Recreation and Dance, 19(2): 6-7.
CROWE, T.K. (1989). Pediatric assessments: A survey of their use by
occupational therapists in Northwestern School systems. The Occupational
Therapy Journal of Research. 9: 273-286.
DAUER, V.P., & PANGRAZI, RP. (1989). Dynamic Physical Education for
Elementary school children. (9
th
ed) New York: Macmillian Publishing Company
DE JAGER M. (2001). Breingim. Kaapstad: Human & Rousseau. In Fredericks,
C.R, Kokot, S.J. & Krog, S. (2006). Using a Developmental Movement
Programme to Enhance Academic Skills in Grade 1 Learners. SA Journal for
Research in sport, Physical Education and Recreation, 28(1): 29-42.
Department of Education. 2002. Revised National curriculum statement Grade
R-9 (Schools) Policy: Ufe Orientation. In HENDRICKS. P.C. (2004). The Role of
Physical Education in South African Primary Schools. Unpublished Med. Western
Cape. University of the Western Cape.
DU TOIT, D. & PIENAAR, A.E. (2001). Current status and assessment of
quantitative and qualitative one leg balance ability in 3-6 year old children. S.A.
Journal for Research in Sport, Physical Education and Recreation, 23(2): 51-62.
EDWARD, F.l. & FINN-STEVENSEN, M. (1987). Children Development and
Social Issues. Lexington, MA &Toronto: D.C. Heath and Company.
Enotes. Gross motor skills. Encyclopedia of nursing and allied health. Retrieved
July 06, 2006 from the World Wide Web: http://health.enotes.com/nursing-
encyclopedia/gross -motor-skills
FREDERICKS, C.R, KOKOT, S.J. & KROG, S. (2006). Using a Developmental
Movement Programme to Enhance Academic Skills in Grade 1 Learners. SA
Journal for Research in Sport, Physical Education and Recreation, 28(1): 29-42.
82
GABBARD, C. (2000). Physical Education: should it be in the core curriculum?
Principle, 79(3): 29-31.
GALLAHUE, D.L. (1982). Motor development and movement experience for
young children. In Butterfield, S.A & Loovis, E.M. (1993). Influence of Age, Sex,
Balance and Sport Participation on Development of Throwing by Children in
Grade k-8. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76: 459-464
GALLAHUE, D.L. & OZMUN, J.C. (1998). Understanding Motor Development in
Infants and Children (4
th
ed.) Boston, Mass: McGrow-Hill.
GODDARD-BLYTHE, S. (2000). Early Learning in the Balance: priming the first
ABC. Support for learning, 15(4): 154-158.
GOODWAY, J.D. & BRANTA, C.F. (2003). Influence of motor skill intervention
on fundamental motor skill development of disadvantaged pre-school children. In
GOODWAY, J.D. & ROBINSON, L.E. (2006). SKIPing toward an Active Start
Promoting Physical Activity in Preschoolers. Retrieved May 29, 2006 from the
World Wide Web: www.journal.naeyc.org/aboutlpermissions.asp.
GOODWAY, J.D. & ROBINSON, L.E. (2006). SKIPing toward an Active Start
Promoting Physical Activity in Preschoo/ers. Retrieved May 29, 2006 from the
World Wide Web: www.journal.naeyc.org/aboutlpermissions.asp.
GOVATOS, L.A. (1959). Relationship and Age Differences in Growth Measures
and Motor Skills. Child Development, 30: 333-340.
GOWLAND, C., KING, G., KING, S., LAW, M., LEnS, L., MACKINNON, L.,
ROSENBAUM, P., & RUSSELL, D. (1991). Review of selected measures in
neuro-developmental rehabilitation. Hamilton: Ontario: Neuron-developmental
Clinical Research Unit.
GRAFT, C; KOCH, B; KRETSCHMANN-KANDEL, E; FALKOWSKI, G; CHRIST,
H; COBURGER, S; LEHMACHER, W; BJARNASON-WEHRENS, B; PLATEN, P;
TOKARSKI, W; PREDEL, H.G. & DORDEL, S. (2004). Correlation between
BMI, Leisure habits and motor abilities in childhood. International journal of
Obesity 28: 22-26.
83
GRAHAM, G. (1987). Motor skill Acquisition- An Essential Goal of Physical
Education Programs. Journal Physical Education Recreation and Dance, 58(7):
44-48.
HAGER, T. (2000) The struggling Learner. Retrieved May 27, 2006 from the
World Wide Web: wwwkidscanleam.neUleam.htm.
HENDRICKS. P.C. (2004). The Role of Physical Education in South African
Primary Schools. Unpublished Med. Western Cape. University of the Western
Cape.
htlpllwww.highscope.org/ELEMENTARY/EL-7.htm. Author unknown. Date
retrieved 23 June 2006.
HOUSNER, L.; CARSON, L.; HAWKINS, A. &WIEGAND, R. (2006). The
predicatability of middle school motor skill and fitness from k-2 measures: a five
year study. Paper presented at the AIESEP World Congress July 2006,
Jyvaskyla, Finland.
JANZ, K.F., DAWSON, J.D. & MAHONEY, L.T. (2000). Tracking physical
fitness and physical activity from childhood to adolescence: The Muscatine study.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 32(7): 1250-1257.
KATZENELLENBOGEN. E.H. (1994). The Physical education curriculum in
changing times: relevances, needs and innovative strategies. Paper presented at
the 2
nd
Multidisciplinary Congress of the South African Federation for Movement
and Leisure Sciences. Free State: 1994.
KNIGHT. D. & RIZZUTO, T. (1993). Relations for Children in Grades 2. 3. and 4
Between Balance Skills and Academic Achievement. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 76: 1296-1298.
KOHL Ill, HW. & HOBBS. K. E. (1998). Development of Physical Activity
Behaviours among children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 101(3): 549-554.
KROMBHOLZ. H. (1997). Physical Performance in Relation to Age. Sex. Social
Class and Sports Activities in Kindergarten and Elementary school. Perceptual
and Motor Skills. 84: 1168-1170
84
LOUCAIDES, CA, CHRDZOY, S.M. & BENNETT, N. (2004). Differences in
physical activity levels between urban and rural school children in Cyprus. Health
Education Research, 19 (2); 138-147.
LOUW, DA (1995). (2
nd
ed). Human Development. Pretoria: Kagiso Tertiary.
LUEBKE, L.L. (1981). Physical Education in Early childhood. Joumal Physical
Education and Recreation, 52(2): 29-30.
McPHERSON, S.L., & THOMAS, J.R. (1989). Relation of knowledge and
performance in boys tennis:-Age and expertise. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology. 48, 190-211.
MILLER, I. (1995). Factors influencing adults' participation in physical activity:
does physical education and school sport play a role? Proceedings of the i
h
International Rainbow Week Symposium. Cape Town: 1995.
NASPE. (2004). Physical Activity for Children: A Statement of Guidelines for
Children Ages 5-12. In CORBIN; C.B.; PANGRAZI, R.B. & LE MASURIER, G.C.
(2004). Physical Activity for Children: Current Patterns and Guidelines.
Presidents Council of Fitness and Sports, Research Digest, 5(2).
NEWELL, K. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In In
Goodway, J.D. & Robinson, L.E. (2006). SKIPing toward an Active Start
Promoting Physical Activity in Preschoolers. Retrieved May 29, 2006 from the
World Wide Web: www.journal.naeyc.org/aboutlpermissions.asp.
OLDS, A.R. (1994). From cartwheels to caterpillars: children's need to move
indoors and out. Child care information exchange, May/June 32-36.
PICA, R. (1998). Movement and the Brain: moving and learning in early
childhood. In Fredericks, C.R, Kokot, S.J. & Krog, S. (2006). Using a
Developmental Movement Programme to Enhance Academic Skills in Grade 1
Learners. SA Journal for Research in Sport, Physical Education and
Recreation, 28(1): 29-42.
PIENMR, A.E. (1994). Die voorkoms en remediering van groot motoriese
agterstande by 6 - 9 jarige kinders in die junior primere fase. Potchefstroom.
PU for CHO. Unpublished Doctoral thesis.
85
PILLAY, C.M. & OOSTHUIZEN, V. (1990). An investigation into the status and
role of Physical Education in Ciskeian Junior and Senior Secondary Schools.
Paper at SASSSPER Congress, University of Port Elizabeth: 1990.
PORSTEINSDOTIIR, M.H & BOGADOTIIR, B.G.H. Motor Proficiency of 6-
7year-old Icelandic children. Retrieved June 23, 2004 from the World Wide Web:
www.physio.hi.is/resteach.htm.
Private schools, South Africa. Retrieved on July 25, 2006 from World Wide Web:
http://www.privateschooling.co.za
RARICK, G.L. (1980). Motor Development - Its Growing Knowledge Base.
Journal of Physical Education and Recreation, 51 (7): 26-27,56-61.
RODGER, S. (1994). A survey of assessments used by pediatric occupational
therapists. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal. 41: 137-142.
ROSE, B.; LARKIN. D. & BERGER. B.G. (1998). The importance of Motor
Coordination for Children's Motivational Orientation in Sport. Adapted Physical
Activity Quarterly, 15: 316-327.
ROSS, J.G. & GILBERT. G.G. (1985). The national children's and youth fitness
study. In GRAHAM, G. (1987). Motor skill Acquisition- An Essential Goal of
Physical Education Programs. Journal Physical Education Recreation and
Dance, 58(7): 44-48.
RUDISILL, M.E; LAWRENCE, M.B; GOODWAY, J.D &WALL. S.J. (2002).
Effect of pre.-school physical activity and motor skill development program on the
loco motor skill performance of underserved children. Retrieved June 23, 2006
from the World Wide Web:
http://aalperd.confex.com/aahperd/2002Ifinalprogram/paper2201.htm
SAAKSLAHTI. A; NUMMINEN. P; NIINIKOSKI. H; RASK-NISSILA, L; VIIKARI. J;
TUOMINEN, J. &VALlMAKI. I. (1999). Is physical activity related to body size,
fundamental motor skills and CHD risk factors in early childhood? Pediatric
Exercise Science. 11: 327-340.
86
SABATINO, R. (1987). Review of Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency.
In J.Y. Mitchell, (ed). The ninth mental measurements yearbook. (1) (pp 235-
236). Lincoln, NE: The Buros Institute of Mental Measurements of the University
of Nebraska.
SANDERS, SW. (1992). Designing Preschool Movement Programs.
Champaign, 111. Human Kinetic Publishers.
SATTLER, J.M. (1992). Assessment of children. San Diego: Jerome Sattler,
Publisher.
SEEFELDT, V. (1984) Physical Fitness in Preschool and Elementary School-
aged Children. Journal Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 55(9): 33-40.
SHEPARD, R.J. (1997). Curricular physical activity and academic performance.
Pediatric Exercise Science, 9: 113-126.
SHERRILL, C. (1986). Adapted physical education and recreation: A
multidisiciplinary approach. (3ro ed). Dubuque. IAWm. C. Brown.
SHERRILL, C. (1993). Adapted physical education, recreation and sport: Cross
disciplinary and lifespan approah. (4
th
ed). Dubuque lA: Wm.C.Brown.
SITZER, J. (2003). Life Orientation: a learning area in South African schooling
curriculum. In HENDRICKS. P.C. (2004). The Role of Physical Education in
South African Primary Schools. Unpublished Med. Western Cape. University of
the Western Cape.
SLOAN, W. (1955). The Lincoln-Oseretsky Motor Development Scale. Genetic
Psychology Monographs, 51 :183-252.
SMITH, P.J. & O'KEEFE, S. (1999). Fundamental Motor Skill Development. The
Irish Scientist Year Book. Retrieved June 30, 2006 from the World Wide Web:
http://www.irishscientist.ie/p187ahtm
http://www. the teachersport.com Author unknown. Date retrieved 11 October,
2006.
87
THOMAS, J.R. (1999). McCloy research lecture: children's control, learning, and
performance of motor skills. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(1): 1
-9.
THOMAS, J.R. (2001). Children's control, learning, and performance of motor
skills. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 1-9.
THOMSA, JR., &THOMAS, K.T. (1988). Development of gender differences in
physical activity. Quest, 40, 219-229.
WADE, M.G. (1992). Motor Skills, Play and Child Development: an Introduction.
Early Report, 19(2)
WILSON, B.N., KAPLAN, B.J., CRAWFORD, S.G., & DEWEY, D. (2000).
Interrupter reliability of the Bruininks Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency- Long
form. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 17 (1): 95-110.
Yack, E. (1989). Sensory Information: a survey of its use in the clinical setting.
Canadian Journal Of Occupational Therapy, 56: 229-235.
88
APPENDIX A
89
Letter to Department of Education -KZN: Requesting Permission to
Conduct Research
University of Zululand
Private Bag X 1001
3886
7 March 2006
Superintendent-General
Department of Education
Private Bag X9137
Pietermaritzburg
3200
Attention: Or R C Lubisi.
RE: REQUEST FOR PERMISSION TO CONDUCT RESEARCH
IN PRIMARY SCHOOLS IN THE PIETERMARITZBURG
REGION
Background
I, Nicola Portela, am currently completing my Masters in Human Movement
Science through the University of Zululand and have chosen to complete my
project in the area of physical education and the proficiency of gross motor skills
in Primary School learners.
Purpose
The purpose of my stUdy is to determine the average gross motor capabilities of
primary school learners and to compare these values with others, as well as to
identify the importance of a movement program on a regular basis in the class
time table. Value of this research too lies in turn, with the end results being
forwarded to a collection of data necessary for the development of a National
Data-base of Motor Proficiency in Children.
Research methodology
The sample of participants will be selected from 17 Primary Schools in the
Pietermaritzburg region of Kwazulu-Natal, which is regarded as the educational
core of this respective province, and includes schools of a wide demographic
range. Within each of the 17 schools all the learners of Grade One and Grade
Two aged seven years and six months to 8 years at the time of the arranged
assessment date will undergo motor proficiency assessments.
The Participation of both the school and the leamer in this project would be on a
voluntary basis. I, the researcher will personally confer with the school to
negotiate a date and time, which would not compromise the learning schedule.
Confidentiality of the school as well as the learner will be respected. Each child
selected for assessment will need to have a consent form completed by his or
her guardian prior to testing.
90
Key elements of the Bruininks-oseretsky test (BOMPT) of motor proficiency will
be used to assess the development of gross motor skills of learners. This test
battery is comprised of 8 Subtests; these include an assessment of Running
Speed and Agility, Balance, Bilateral Coordination, Strength and Upper Limb
Coordination. The SUbjects will be encouraged at all times to complete each task
to the best of his/her capability.
On Completion of this study both the Department of Education as well as each of
the participating Schools will have a copy of the research available to them.
I will greatly appreciate if you could grant me the opportunity to carry out my
study at schools within the Pietermaritzburg region as this would help me to
further my education and knowledge of the related subject as well as facilitate
research in the Motor Proficiency field
Thanking You,
Nicola Portela
Contact Details:
nportela901@yahoo.com
0823302718
033 394 3662 (fax)
91
PllOVIHCEOFKWAZUUI-HATAl
ISlFUIIDAZWE SAKWAZlIllJ.NATAU
P1lOV1NSlE KWAZlItWI4TAl.
DEPAJmIEHT OF EDUCATION
U_AllGOWEIFlJNDO
DEPARlEIEIIT VAN DNlJERWYS
Telephone: 033 4&512S
Fax: onM65188
Private Bag )(9137
PfetermarilZlMg
3200
iNhLOKOHHQV1SI HEAD OFFICE ?IETERMARITZBU'RG
Ms Nk:oIa PorteIa
Univmily 01 Zululand
Private Bag .1001
3886
Dear Ms Portela,
REQUEST fOil PERMISSION TO CONDUCT RESEARCH IN PIlIMARY
SCHOOlS INTHE PIETERMARlTZBURG REGION
Your request for permission to COnduct researdl in the PietermarilZburg Region in
the area of physical education and the profICiency of gross motor skills in Primary
Sdloolleamel3 is glallled subject to the following ccn<li!ions'
o Pmopa15 and educators are under 00 obligation to assist you in yew
lnveatigatitm.
c Principals, edUcators and: 5Chools should not be identifiable "In any way from the
results of the mvestigation.
c YDU malte all the arrangl!l'Tlents concerning your investigation.
c TeachinQ and Learning ~ o g a m S 3IB oolro be in1&nl..'pled.
c A photocopy of this letter ia submilted to the princrpal of the sctloolflllmtioo
where the intendBd resaacd'l is to be conciud:ed
o Yoor research WI" be _ to the identified _earn....
, A brief s_oftne content, lin<ingS ana Il!COfl1mendations .re provided to the
Supemt_of Il1is Depar1ll'lent
, 'The Department .-a copy of Ille campIele<l reportldissertBti_
It is hoped that you will find the above in order.
Best Wisnes
RCASSIUS WBlst, PhD
SUPERItlTENOENT-GENERA!-
92
Letter to all the schools involved in the assessments
Date
To The Principle of '" '" .
I, Nicola Portela, am currently completing my masters in Human Movement
Science at the University of Zululand and have chosen to complete my project in
the area of physical education, and the proficiency of gross motor skills in
primary school children.
The purpose of my study is to determine the average gross motor capabilities of
the children and to compare these values with those I collected from a study in
Empangeni during my honours year [2004]. This study will help to identify the
importance of a movement program on a regular basis in the class timetable.
Through this study I will be assessing 17 schools in the Pietermaritzburg region
and will be able to provide you with feedback regarding the motor skill abilities of
your learners.
I hereby ask permission to conduct a gross motor skill proficiency assessment on
the Foundation Phase learners [between the ages of 7 years 6months and 8
years], of your School. The test will be individually administered and 4 children
can be accommodated every half hour.
93
The assessment procedure will G0nsist of eight sub-tests related to running
speed and agility, bilateral coordination, balance, strength, and upper limb
coordination, and will be conducted by myself. In order to avoid variance in the
availability of the subjects I would appreciate it if the tests could be completed
during school time and in an indoor facility at your school. All information used in
this regard will remain confidential, and neither the name of the school or any of
the subjects tested will be mentioned in the document.
I would appreciate it if you could grant me the opportunity to carry out my study
at your school as this will help me successfully complete my masters project and
thus further my education and knowledge of the related subject.
Your positive confirmation in this regard will be highly appreciated.
Thanking you,
Nicola Portela
94
Letter to the parents of the learners selected for testing
Date
Dear Parent! Guardian,
I Nicola Portela am currently completing my Masters in Human Movement Science at the
University at Zululand. I have chosen to direct my research in the field of motor
development in children and the project is entitled "Motor skIll proficiency levels of
foundation phase primary school children in the Pietermaritzburg area".
I require subjects both boys and girls currently completing Grade 1 or 2 within the age
bracket 7 years and 6 months to 8 years. The test battery to be completed consists of 8
subtests aimed at evaluating gross motor skills, from which my data will be recorded.
These tests include activities such as running, jumping, throWing, kicking and balancing,
which the child will be encouraged to complete to the best of his/her ability.
All testing will be administered dUring school hours allocated by the school and at the
school premises. Children will be required to wear PE kit during the testing period.
Please be assured that neither the name of the school nor the candidates, being
assessed will be mentioned in the document.
SCHEDULED TESTING DATE:
Any further questions may be directed to the respective class teacher who will confer
with me.
With this I ask you to kindly complete the attached consent form and return it to your
child's class teacher as soon as possible prior to the testing.
Your cooperation in this regard is greatly appreciated,
Thank you,
Nicola Portela
95
INFORMED CONSENT FORM
I, '" .. , , having been fully informed of the nature of
the research entitled Investigation of the proficiency of motor skills in primary
school children in Pietermaritzburg, do hereby give my consent for my
child , to act as a subject in the above mentioned research.
I agree to any relevant finding being passed on to members of the research
team, and to the use of this information in publication for research purposes with
the name of the participant remaining confidential.
I realize that it is necessary for my child to promptly report any signs or
symptoms indicating any abnormalities to the researcher.
I am aware that I may withdraw my consent and that my child can withdraw from
participation in the research at any time.
I have read the above and understand it, any questions which may have
occurred have been answered to my satisfaction.
[Print name] [Signature]
PARENT OR GUARDIAN OF THE SUBJECT
[Print name] [Signature]
PERSON ADMINISTRATING INFORMED CONSENT
96
[Date]
[Date]
APPENDIX B
97
Evaluation sheet for each assessment
0
""
~
:r
c
0
~
0
0
p::>
-] ]
-
"-
0
]
,
z
~
"
.t '1
g
%
i
6
0 ,
~
g .5 ,
c -
, ,
,
~ :;
"
j
-
.
~
f
<
~
f.
I
i
:i::
=
0
,
~
-
"
0
'-
~
i:
w .;,j 'E
W
~ 0
1
oc oc
~
0 0
a
~ ~ e
0 0
,
::
~ ~ '?
g --
~ ,
0
g-
i
"
1
j
l
,
i
. ~
: ~
~
~
1;
'.!::
i
~
: ~
~
>
.01
.
"
: ~ ~ ~
0
11
" "'
~ < :i ~
~
<
~
'.
~
<
~
~
i
<
0 ;g
0
~
; ~
"
~
c
f
0
,,;,.
;;
u
i
g ~ ~ 0
,
~
~
,
z
..
0
~
J;
"
0
UJ
~
~
"
!
~
-
.
:.f
c
~
0
0
~
-
0
~ .
, N
!
: ;.,
c
"
"
!.
=
,
~
-
'"
1
-
c
~
" ~
~
5
-
<
,
~
0
I
~
=
~
; ~
<
~
"
c
c
Z
~ r ~ r
~
<'
98
Point Score Table
Table 1: Point scores for test 1 Running speed and agility
RAW SCORE POINT SCORE
(seconds)
10.9 -11 1
10.5 -10.8 2
9.9 - 10.4 3
9.5- 9.8 4
9.4-8.9 5
8.8-8.5 6
8.4- 7.9 7
7.8-7.5 8
7.4-6.9 9
6.8-6.7 10
6.6-6.3 11
6.2 - 6.1 12
6.0 -5.7 13
5.6-5.5 14
<5.5 15
Table 2: Point scores for test 2 Balance (standing on the preferred leg on a
balance beam)
RAW SCORE IPOINT SCORE
(secondsl
1-2
! 1
3-4 2
5-6 3
7-8 4
99
Table 3: Point scores for test 2 Balance (walking forward heel-to-toe on a
balance beam)
RAW SCORE POINT SCORE
(steps)
0 0
1-3 1
4 2
5 3
6 4
Table 4: Point scores for test 3 Bilateral Coordination (tapping feet
alternately while making circle with fingers)
RAW SCORE POINT SCORE
Pass 1
Fail 0
Table 5: Point scores for test 3 Bilateral Coordination (jumping up and
clapping hands)
RAW SCORE ! POINT SCORE
0 10
,
I
1 11
2 12
3 3
4 4
i
>4 5
lOO
Table 6: Point scores for test 4 Strength
RAW SCORE POINT SCORE j
1 1
I
2 2
3 3
4 4
5 5
6 6
7 7
8 8
9 9
10 10
11 11
12 12
13 13
14 14
15 15
Table 7: Point scores for test 5 Upper-Limb Coordination (catching a
tossed ball with the preferred hand)
RAW SCORE POINT SCORE
0 0
1-2 1
I
3-4 2
5 3
Table 8: Point scores for test 5 Upper-Limb Coordination (throwing a ball at
a target with the preferred hand)
! 3-4 12
i 5 ! 3
101