Colarusso - Typological Parallels Between PIE and NWC (1981)
Colarusso - Typological Parallels Between PIE and NWC (1981)
Colarusso - Typological Parallels Between PIE and NWC (1981)
McMaster University
1.
Introduction:
476
JOHN COLARUSSO
477
3.1. Some recent work (Hopper 1973, 1977a, and 1977b; Gamkrelidze
1976; Szemerenyi 1967) has viewed the PIE stop system as typologically
improbable. The three-way contrast (using the dentals as examples)
*dh ~ *d ~ *t is known in no living language. We should note here,
however, that a similar system is necessary in the reconstruction of
Proto-Chinese (cf. Karlgren 1940, 1915-26). Karlgren (1940) posits a
velar series *k ~ *kh ~ *g ~ *gh ~ *D ~ *h (= *x ?). Cowan (1971:22-3,
JOHN COLARUSSO
478
85-9) eliminates the voiced series and presents such sets as those in
(1).
(1)
item
gloss
Canton
Shanghai
Peking
Proto-Chinese
24
2
help
/pol)/
/pal)/
/pal)/
all
/phou/
/phu/
/phu/
*p
*ph
41
plate
/phOn/
/be/
/phan/
*bh
479
Later PIE
*dh (lldhll)
Early PIE
Unstable
*d
*dh
*t'
*t'
*d
*t/t h
*t/t h
*t
*t h (lit hll)
The unstable system would have driven the language over into its
11 Classic 11 form, with the addition of voiceless aspirates, while a few
marginal dialect areas, notably Germanic and Armenian, would have retained the early PIE form with only slight modifications. The shift
of *t' to *d (Hopper 1973, 1977a:43, and 1977b; Gamkrelidze 1976; Gamkrelidze and Ivanov 1972 and 1973) does have a precedent in NWC (cf.
Colarusso 1975:.82-3). In Kabardian, /p', t', ... /have 11 Knarrstimme 11
or creaky voicing (cf. Kuipers 1960:19-20). In some Abaza dialects,
this has given rise to voiced segments in certain positions. Contrast
the standard Abaza forms in {3) (Serdjucenko 1956:633) with the Anatolian dialect forms in (4) (Allen 1956 and 1965b). In (4), the standard stative present /-p'/ and the active present /-y-t'/ are shifted
to /-b/ and /-y-d/ respectively.
(3)
Standard Abaza:
a.
/s-c'vwa-p'/
11
I sit, I am sitting 11
b.
/s-~W-y-t'/
11
I write, I am writing 11
------------------------------------
480
(4)
JOHN COLARUSSO
Anatolian Abaza:
a.
/s-c'vwa-b/
(same as 3a)
b.
/s-1w-y-d/
(same as 3b)
Voiced aspirates are absent from the Caucasus with the exception of
the Eastern Armenian dialect already mentioned. Thus, while the Caucasian languages in general can provide no typological grounds for
understanding of the PIE voiced aspirated series, they do make the
occurrence of such sounds in Armenian all the more remarkable and
suggest strongly that this is a survival of an old PIE feature rather
than some sort of late areal innovation.
3.2. The Hopper-Gamkrelidze-Ivanov {H-G-I) system was posited,
in part, to account for the absence, or great rarity, of PIE *b.
Typologically, in languages with a glottal ejective series, the /p'/
is often missing. Absence of a /b/ in a language with labial stops
is much rarer, if indeed it occurs at all. Therefore, the rarity of
PIE *b was better explained if *b were traced back to *p' instead.
Typologically this reasoning is sound. /p'/ is indeed often missing
from the Northeast Caucasian languages. It is always present, however,
in the NWC ones and in the Kartvelian languages as well. While the
NWC and Kartvelian facts are not typologically significant, they should
be kept in mind in any efforts to relate PIE and Caucasian features in
areal terms. Typologically significant is the absence of a voiced
uvular stop from both NWC and Kartvelian groups {also from most Northeast languages as well). While PIE 11 classical 11 *ghw, construed in the
11 new 11 PIE as a possible uvular {Hopper 1977a:48-9), seems to have been
the rarest of the labiovelars (ibid. 47), it is still common enough
to have been an original segment no matter how far back we wish to
push our PIE reconstructions. This too is distinctly at odds with the
NWC areal picture and should be borne in mind in any future work.
4.
481
----~--
---
JOHN COLARUSSO
482
the Ubykh.
4.2. Thus, though the developments are complex, we may note the
following pattern. The Circassian languages show the clearest developments, with PNWC */r-/ going to /d-/ and /-A-/, the latter when prefixed. Ubykh shows preservation in prefixed forms and perhaps /1-/
initially, though there seems to be a confusion of possible forms.
Abkhaz and Abaza show simple preservation. This is a north-to-south
gradient with the most thoroughgoing elimination of PNWC */r-/ in the
north among the Circassians. This is noteworthy for if the PIE homeland was to the north of the Caucasus, then it may have been contiguous
to the Circassian speaking area. Thus the north-to-south gradient may
reflect an archaic phonotactic areal feature shared by PIE and the
northern NWC languages, with the northernmost, the Circassian languages,
showing the most PIE influence, the middle, Ubykh, showing some influence along with a good deal of native innovation and dialect mixture,
giving rise to the confusing picture we see today, and the southernmost area, the Abkhaz-Abaza languages, showing no PIE influence. Certainly more thorough inv~stigation of PIE and NWC initial */r/ would
be rewarding.
5.
PIE Velars:
*k
r'
*g
*gh
(plain velars)
*g"'
*ghW
The plain velars are in some way palatalized in the satem languages,
483
PIE *k
*g
*gh:
ooA.Lx6s,
Others assume that the forms in (.6) reflect the true PIE velar series,
while the velars of (5), as in Lat. centum 100 Av. satam, PIE
*kytom, go back to a palatal series, usually written *k, *g, *gh, but
which I shall write as in (7) in order to follow traditional caucasiological analysis.
11
(7)
11
*kY
*gY
*ghY
(palatals)
*k
*kW
*g
*gw
*gh
*ghW
(plain velars)
(labiovelars)
Some workers, for example Hamp (1970), use the system in (7) to account
for differences in the reflexes of palatals and velars within Indo-Iranian. There is no general consensus even now, however, as to whether
the system in (5) or that in (7) is to be preferred.
5.2. Turning to typological st~dies, Hopper (1977a) takes the
system in (5) and reinterprets the plain velars as palatals and the
labiovelars as uvulars, to obtain the system in (8).
(8)
Hopper's PIE:
*khY
*k' Y
*qhw
*q'w
*gY
------~--
~--~
-----
JOHN COLARUSSO
484
The system in (8) also shows Hoppers revisions of the PIE source
features (cf. 3 above).
5.3. Unfortunately I know of no language with the system shown
in (8), neither within nor without the Caucasus. The system in (9) is
found along with several uvular series in East Circassian and the West
Circassian dialects Shapsugh, Hakuchi, and Old Chemgwi, spilling over
into Ubykh (Colarusso 1977:89-92), with contrast of voiceless aspiration only in some West Circassian dialects.
(9)
khY
kY
gY
k'Y
The contrasts in (9) are not unusual and may be found among some of
the American Indian languages of the Northwest Coast. The AbkhazAbaza languages add to the system in (9) a series of plain velars,
along with 3.or 5 uvular series, to produce a typologically unusual
system. The simplest set of contrasts (using unmarked voiceless segments as exemplars), /kW/ ~ /q/ ~ /qw/, is found, apparently as arecent development, in Kabardian, Bzhedukh, and New Chemgwi, but such a
set is a very non-Indo-European looking one. In effect, therefore,
the NWC languages are much richer in the palatal, velar, and uvular
areas than is any recoverable stage of PIE and as a consequence can
provide little typological guidance for the analysis of the PIE 11 gutturals11. The issue of choosing between such systems as (5), (7), or
(8) will have to be decided on the basis of internal PIE considerations.
6.
Epenthesis (Anaptyxis):
6.1. Anaptyxis or vowel epenthesis in PIE is quite complex (Polom~ 1965:29, fn. 126): *~ 3 p 0 kwto- ~Iranian *puxta, or the syllabic
-b- in Avestan debenao- (Skt. dabhnoti), or the Greek forms ~AnTos,
-----------------------
--~-
485
~ETO~, 6o<o~,
0 + a % [-voice]
b.
[+voice]
II
~---------------------------
JOHN COLARUSSO
486
Abaza:
/d-e-1-ba-d/
"him/her-she-see-past" = "she saw him/her"
b .. /d-1-z-ba-d/ ~ /d-1-a-z-ba-d/
"him/her-she-can-see-past" = "she was able to see him/her"
c. /d-gY-1-m-ba-d/ ~ /d-a-gY-1-a-m-ba-d/
"him/her-neg(ative)-she-neg-see-past" = "she did not see
a.
/d-1-ba-d/
him/her"
d.
/d-gY-1-z-m-ba-d/
/d-gY-a-1-z-a-m-ba-d/
"him/her-neg-she-can-neg-see-past" = "she could not see
~
him/her"
-----~-----
487
.
. {[+syllabic]}
[-syllab1c][-syllab1c]
#
Rule (12) is complicated by the role of glides; there is some indication that post-tonically they do not count as segments for (12).
6.5. Whatever the exact form of (12), what is of interest to us
is that it is a rule which inserts syllables starting from the end of
a word. This will cause the syllabification of a word to alter radically as it is inflected. Similar variability occurs in the PIEgrade (cf. Anttila 1969:30-3; Schmitt-Brandt 1967). Sometimes there is
evidence of inflectional differences (cf. Polome 1965:29, fn. 126):
Gk. E:rropov : rrrrpwwL "it has been fated", "with [p e ry] before thematic endings and [pry]
before athematic endings". In other cases, no
e
clear difference in inflection is apparent (cf. the examples cited in
---
488
JOHN COLARUSSO
Ablaut (Metaphony):
~--------------
~------
- -
--
..
-------------
489
b.
c.
JOHN COLAF.l!SSO
490
f.
g.
Abadzakh
a.
W~st
b.
c.
Circassian Ablaut:
Kabardian:
a.
lzal
"once"
Bzhedukh:
b.
Is' We/ "good" in Is' We-7\ayw -e-ney I "good-see-trans-deverbative" = "love" (noun) vs. (c)
c.
d.
(16)
Circassian Ablaut:
Bzhedukh:
a.
----
"I
---------------------------
b.
c.
491
= "I
Forms in (17) are most likely due to final vowel devoicing and deletion,
rather than true ablaut.
(17)
7.4. The other NWC languages show true ablaut but not in a
productive form. In Abkhaz-Abaza, it exists but has no clear morphological significance and is probably a historical relic. Thus, we
find forms such as Bzyb Abkhaz /a-bayw;, /a-beyw/, /a-bywa/, all meaning "bone" (cf. Colarusso 1977:106). Such variants most likely reflect
retentions of different ablaut grades of a root from a period when ablaut was still an active process in Abkhaz-Abaza. In Ubykh, leveling
seems to have taken place, and much less ablaut variation has survived,
though pairs such as /lasxwa/ "hazel nut", root */Ia-/ "nut" (cf. 4.1,
above) with /-sxwe/, vs. /sxwa/ "herbs" (earlier "*greens", cf. Bzh.
/sxwaant'a/ "grey, blue, green") can be found.
7.5. We thus have a situation in which morphologically conditioned
true ablaut seems to have been a characteristic of PNWC and has survived
in only one branch, leaving relics elsewhere. Some forms, such as the
Bzyb variants of "bone", the Ubykh forms, and such inflectional variants
as the root for "son" in {15d) in Bzhedukh, strongly suggest that some
original ablaut may have been morphologically conditioned but not seman-
----
~-----
~-~~
~~---
JOHN COLAHUSSO
492
Schwebeablaut:
11
11 ) ,
11
493
11
cvc-cv
a.
*pan-t ( i )
pi ntis
-----------------------------
----
494
JOHN COLJIP.lJSSO
eve-e-ve0
b.
*pon-t-e~2-
ee-e-c-ve
c.
*pn-t-a2-es
0
,..
495
present
aorist
dr-ek'dr-ik'-e
Intransitive
dr-k'-V
der-k'0
496
JOHN COLAHUSSO
NWC Etymologies:
497
*/base/
*/bse/
b.
*/base/
*/bse/
This is about as simple as NWC etymologies come. There is no affixation of grammatical class prefixes or other types of suffixes. Of immediate concern to our purposes is the obvious vowel deletion due to
the influence of what must have been a strong stress accent, still
preserved in Ubykh and, to a lesser extent, Abkhaz-Abaza. More complex histories may be seen in (21).
(21)
*/?aqa/
b.
*/?qa/
Ubykh /qa-n/
*/ba-?aqa/ + */ba?qa/
/-pq/ "bone, frame"
d.
e.
Abzhwi
*/ba-?aqa-w/ + */ba?qu/
Bzyb /a-beyw; id.
*/be?qwa;
*/a-baq'wa;
*/a-be)w/
g.
*/be-?eqa-w/
/be)W/ id.
*/be?qu/
*/ba?qwa;
*/beq'wa;
Abaza
h.
*/ba-?eqa-w/ + */ba?qo/
b)Wa/ + Bzyb /a-bywa/
*/ba?qwa/
*/a-beq'wa;
i.
*/be-?eqa-w/ + */ba?qu/
Abaza /b)wa; id.
*/ba?qwe;
*/baq'we/
*/be)We/
f.
*/baqe/
*/bqe/
*/bqa/
Bzh.
*/a+
Some of the etymologies (21c-i) might be replaced with analogical extensions of various vowel grades, but the conditions for this are not
yet clear and so the histories have been recorded in detail. It should
498
JOHN COLARUSSO
be noted that the history of the Ubykh form (21d), for example, from
*/be-?eqa/ to */be?qa/ produces a form reminiscent of a PIE theme I,
CeC-C-. A further shift from */be?qa/ through */beqa/ to /bya/ produces a form reminiscent of a PIE theme II, CC-eC-, but without the
final consonant. An intermediate form */b?eqa/, though it would look
more like an IE theme II, is ruled out as it would produce an early
Ubykh */~ya/, not found. Similarly, an intermediate form */b?qa/,
though resembling an IE zero-grade stem, would be expected to yield
only Ubykh /~ya/ and never /bya/, though the latter is the earlier,
unassimilated form. It is through such recurrent processes of syncope
that the NWC languages have acquired the remarkable consonant clusters
and general phonotactic patterns that set them apart from the Northeast Caucasian languages, with whom they may be remotely related, and
give them their resemblance to PIE.
8.5. Thus, although NWC etymologies only approximately follow
PIE Schwebeablaut patterns, it is tempting to suggest that PIE itself
may have derived its Schwebeablaut from an earlier full-grade stage,
CVC-VC-, reminiscent of PNWC. This would give the earliest PIE a look
which, though currently not popular, has been propounded before (see,
for example, Kerns and Schwartz 1940). Phonologically, and typologically, it is simpler to assume an early form *pJZ-ew-, for example,
yielding *pJl-w-, theme I (Benveniste 1935:54), (Goth. filu), and
another *pol-ew- yielding *pol-w- (Gk. *n6\u), while a *pel-ow- yielded a theme II *pl-ow- (Olat. plous, Lat. plus), and a *pel-~- yielded
*pl-Jw- (Hom. Gk. n\o~). This theory reduces Schwebeablaut to an earlier process of syncope. While this theory may be somewhat unesthetic
in that it may occasionally require the reconstruction of a vowel whose
color is completely lost (one might prefer to have *pJl-Vw- rather than
*pel-ew-), it has the advantage of accounting in a straightforward way
for the difference between PIE *e and the anaptyctic vowel: PIE *e is
no longer an anaptyctic vowel, but a simple vowel in its own right, and
the anaptyctic vowel need have no connection with it. Furthermore, the
499
500
JOHN COLARUSSO
often falling together with the reflex of *a, suggests that *o may
have stood in opposition to *a through the feature [+round]. This
would make *o's rounding a redundant feature that became significant
only after the emergence of *a. Moreover, an interpretation of PIE
*o as [+low] would make the vowel system look much more like the NWC
one, making it typologically unusual, but comprehensible. The similarities between the two systems are strong enough to warrant further
study. There are two further considerations that are worth looking at.
9.2. The reinterpretation of the PIE *e ~ *o system as */a/~
*/a/ presents one problem. It is quite easy to imagine that at the
time when the old morphological patterns were breaking down and syllabic */y/ and */w/ were being reinterpreted as */i/ and */u/, the
old */a/ was reinterpreted in its least marked allophone, probably
*[a] originally, leading to *a in Indo-Iranian and *e or some other
front vowel in most of the other dialects. The shift of */a/ (apophonic, i.e., classic *o) to */o/ presents difficulties. It is possible
that */a/ was rephonemicized on the basis of a rounded allophone, perhaps the product of coloring due to labiovelars and o-coloring laryngeal(s). Here the confusion of the reflexes of *o with the /a/ reflexes of nonapophonic *a may be traces of the earlier form of *o as
*/a/ (apophonic). It is also possible that PIE simply realized an
earlier */a/ as *[o] or*[~]. Of interest in this regard is the persistence until nearly the end of the first millennium A. D. of an
areal feature in the Ukraine in which /a/ was realized with rounding.
Traces of this can be found in the history of Ossetic and to the present day in many Hungarian dialects, Hungarian having been contiguous to
Ossetic prior to its final migration into Europe. Thus, although it
is perhaps unlikely, PIE may reflect an ancient southern Russian areal
feature. However one may wish to construe the shift of hypothetical
PIE apophonic */a/ to *o, one must find a way of distinguishing this
apophonic */a/ (*o) from nonapophonic *a. We shall return to nonapophonic *a in the section on laryngeals (13.5). For now, it should be
501
11
11
11
9.3. In the NWC languages, the anaptyctic vowel [a] coincides with
the systematic phoneme /a/. This makes good sense since what an anaptyctic vowel is supposed to do is add a feature of [+syllabic] to break
up consonant clusters, and this is just what the distinctive specification of /a/ is. Epenthetic [a] and systematic /e/ behave the same way
phonetically, with the exception that the systematic one tends to persist in most languages even when unstressed, It is this persistence
that is probably a major aid to the NWC language learner in his task of
sifting through the welter of schwas in his language to find underlying
ones and is the chief reason why no dialect has totally lost this phoneme. In this regard, the PIE neutral syllabic vowel, *e, differs from
the anaptyctic vowel and, as has been noted (8.2), is a strong reason
not to regard the PIE Schwebeablaut system as due to anaptyctic processes. Yet if we are really to compare the PIE vowel system to that of
the NWC, then we must account for this difference between the treatment
of the neutral vov1el and the anaptyctic vov1el in the two groups. Two
possible explanations have come to mind. First, one could readily imagine a NWC language eventually setting up some sort of phonetic difference
in the treatment of systematic /e/ as opposed to epenthetic [e]. This
would be a great aid to the child in learning one of these languages and
502
JOHN COLARPSSO
would promote the preservation of /e/ which frequently carries an important semantic load. If this happened in PIE, such a difference,
perhaps degree of fronting, would be established by superficial phonetic rules that would later be reinterpreted in the diverging IE dialects
as phonemic differences, thus giving rise to the different reflexes of
IE *e as opposed to the anaptyctic vowel. Second, it is possible that
PIE did not have well established anaptyctic processes and that these
arose only during the period of dialect differentiation. In favor of
this interpretation of PIE anaptyxis as late, it should be noted that
even Greek, which is, of course, highly conservative in its treatment
of PIE vocalism, shows varrying reflexes of the PIE anaptyctic vowel.
Sometimes it resembles schwa secundum, *dekC- ~ Gk. 6L6aoxw (cf. 8.2),
but at other times turns up as an -L-, cf. Gk. X~E~ without anaptyxis
vs. x-\JLI:;0~ yesterday" with anaptyxis, perhaps showinq i-coloring from
a *-y- (~ *dhghe-y6-s, Skt. hy&s), or with some other coloring (cf.
6.1), or may even have been absent altogether, cf. Gk. on(n)To~ ~
*~ 3 p-kw-t6- (8.1).
If a careful sifting of the PIE material provides
evidence for one or more anaptyctic processes in the parent language,
then we may construe this to mean that PIE distinguished */e/ from
anaptyctic *[e] by phonetically stabilizing the former and allowing
the latter to undergo coloring by adjacent consonants and glides. If
the reflexes of anaptyxis in the IE dialects show tremendous and irreconcilable differences in syllabic form and position, then we may assume
that PIE reinterpreted */e/ as *e and that anaptyxis was a late, dialectal IE innovation. Only further consideration of the difficult question
of PIE anaptyxis will permit a choice between these two alternatives.
With the aid of NWC, we have at least been able to frame these two
theories.
11
10.
10.1. PIE laryngeal theory is still in a state of flux (cf. vJinter 1965a), largely b~cause of (1) doubt as to the reflexes of laryn-
-----------------------------
-------
--------
TYPOLOGICAL PARALLELS
RETt~EEN
503
,.,...---.
10.2. To elucidate not merely the behavior but also the inherent
phonological nature of the PIE laryngeals (i.e., what were they?), we
must turn to typological comparisons. There have been many efforts
along this line (Barnhard 1980; Colarusso 1975:394-8; Keiler 1970; Lehmann 1952; Sapir 1938). The task is in principle simple. We must find
segments in other languages that behave like PIE laryngeals. Here \'Je
must rea.lize that we know not merely the phonotactics of the PIE laryngeals, but their history as well. Roughly, the laryngeals went through
three stages: (1) an early period when they persisted in all positions
and did not color contiguous vowels, (2) a later stage when they persisted in some positions, \'Jere possibly lost in others \'lith compensatory
lengthening of preceding vowel, and colored contiguous vowels (attested
in the Anatolian languages, with relics elsewhere, cf. Gk. ~6wp + *2wd-or or *~ew-d-or, Alb. hap, Gk. aRo + *~4ep- [Hamp 1965a:125, 4.1.1]),
JOHN
504
COLA~USSO
and (3) a late, dialectal stage in which they color contiguous vowels,
are lost with compensatory lengthening of a precedin9 vowel and vanish
elsewhere (attested in the non-Anatolian branches with exception of
the relict forms mentioned in [2]). To a phonologist, such a history
tells a great deal. We must find typologically parallel sounds, i.e.,
they must have the phonological or historical ability to be stable and
phonologically inactive, and then be able to undergo changes that preserve their stability while enabling them to color vo~tJels, and finally
undergo yet further changes which lead to total instability and loss.
This is a considerable amount of information when coupled with the actual vowel-coloring properties which we know the laryngeals to have had,
particularly if we construe such vowel-coloring as due to simple phonological assimilation, and I see little reason not to. With all this as
a starting point, we shall see that the NWC lanauages provide a complex,
but remarkably detailed ami convincing account of what the PIE laryngeals
may have been like and how they vanished.
10.3. First, we must note that there have been some false or misleading parallels dra\'m beb1een NWC and PIE with respect to .the laryngeals. Kuipers (1960:105) compares Kabardian /w/ and /y/ with the PIE
laryngeals (though we shall see that his comparison of /h/ with them is
more to the point), cf. (22).
(22)
r--
/ey/
+ [
/aw/
T]
[u]
/ay/
+ [
eJ
/aw/+[6]
The NWC glides exhibit loss, with compensatory lengthening of a preceding vowel after coloring thereof. Effects like this due to glides have
been postulated in limited conditions for PIE (cf. Benveniste 1935:63,
with *rey-y- + *rey-, Ved. ray-, vs. *rey-ey- + *rey-i-s, Ved. rap{-h:,
and Schmitt-Brandt [1967:48, 48], with *gwyewo- + Gk. 13Lo~, vs. *gu'eywo+ Lat. v~vus, and *bhweti- + Gk. ~uoL~ vs. *bhewti- + Skt. bhut{-~),
505
but such processes are controversial and are overshadowed by the effects
of the laryngeals. The trouble with Kabardian glide effects is that /w/,
for example, produces [6] only when the preceding vowel is already low,
i.e., when it is an /a/. A preceding /a/ is raised, backed and rounded
to [u], the /w/ combining with this [u] as length. This coloring is
basically different from that observed for the PIE laryngeals. For PIE
For *e2 and *84 both
*~1 no coloring is observed, merely lengthening.
*e and *o are mapped into *a, for *e3 into *o. Thus,
\'/hen coloring oc.
curs in PIE, it is without regard to the original character of the vowel affected. This is not the case with the NWC glides, nor do we find
simple lengthening without coloring. The NWC glides do not, therefore,
afford a true parallel with the PIE laryngeals.
~
10.4. Vowel coloring and lengthening involve only one main process in NWC: occasional lowering of a /a/ to /a/, and then lowering of
this and any original /a/'s below their usual "low'' articulatory target
by a prolongation of their articulatory gesture, i.e., by lengthening.
There are three sources for this process (Colarusso in press; 1975:31229; Kuipers 1960:32-9).
10.5. In the Circassian languages, /-a-a-/ yields [a] or [Q], depending upon consonantal environment, both being long as though two morae were involved (cf. [13b,c], in 7.2). The same is true in Ubykh,
(cf. [13d], in 7.2). This is the first source of long /a/ in NWC languages. This process does not occur in the Abkhaz-Abaza branch.
10.6. There is strong evidence that /h/ in /ah/ sequences can have
the same effect as the second /a/ in /aa/ (Colarusso in press; 1975:3238, 390-3; Kuipers 1960:33-4, 37-8). This effect is largely confined to
the Ci rcass ian 1anguages, cf. (23).
(23)
JOHN COLARUSSO
506
Bzhedukh:
a.
b.
c.
d.
b.
c.
d.
e.
(24b) shows vowel-lengthening due to /h/ in Ubykh. (?4c) shows a variant without initial /h/, and (24d, e) show forms in which initial /h/
does not undergo metathesis and vowel-lengthening consequently does not
occur. /h/, at least in Circassian, seems to be a glide, ([-consonantal]), and is subject to inversion as is /y/. Thus one has Bzh. /s-yawana/ ''my-possessive prefix-house" = "my house" (alienable possession)
-+ /s-ay-wana/ -+ [slwuna]. For /h/ vwrd boundary, /#/, can act (optionally) as a consonant. For non-initial /h/ Kabardian presents clear evidence in the form of its nominal plural marker /-ha/ (Kuipers ibid.:
37-8), cf. West Circ. /-xa/, Bzh. and Shapsugh /-xha/. Thus, one has
Kab. /7:.'a-r/ "man-definite"= "the man", but /7r'a-ha-r/ "man-pl-def" =
"the men". Here no metathesis takes place, this being true even when
the stem ends in a consonant, cf. /7i.'a-z-ha-r/ "man-old-pl-def" = "the
old men''. Morphological conditioning is at play, both here and in the
examples in (25) where /-ha-/ is suffixed to a pronominal or possessive
suffix.
-----------------
- - - - - - - -
(25)
507
/-ha-/ in Prefixes:
/0-ya-tx-a-0/ "it-he-write-trans-pres" = "he is writing it'!
'
/0-ya-ha-tx-a-0/ + /0-y-ha-tx-a-0/ + /0-y-ah-tx-a-0/ + [yatxL
J
Kabardian
a.
b.
/0-ya-tx-a-ns/
it"
d.
e.
f.
JOHN COLARUSSO
508
no data that suggest otherwise, the learner will dispose of A and use
B along with the rule B ~A to organize his grammar. Thus, a Bzhedukh
child hearing the word [natref] "maize" will posit the underlying form
/nahtraf/ .. Given such learning strategies and their crucial role in
determining the underlying forms of a language, the "original'' long vowels of PIE are highly suspect. Since PIE had the well-developed lengthening processes due to loss of laryngeals and possibly glides (cf. 10.
3), it would be very likely that the original long vm>~els would have
been due to loss of earlier laryngeals or other compensatory lenthening
processes. Certainly original long vowels would have been possible,
but PIE indirectly attests to generations of speakers interpreting long
vowels as due to the loss of some segment. This makes it highly likely
that the original long vowels had a similar basis. We shall return to
the possibility of an "older generation" of laryngeals below (cf. 13).
10.8. The arguments used for non-initial /-ha-/ in Circassian
have limited application to Ubykh (Colarusso 1975:325-8), largely because [a]'s persist in Ubykh regardless of stress. Most Ubykh [a]'s
appear to be /-a-a-/ sequences (cf. [13d]). In old compounds this
morphology is moribund and often [a]'s are shortened, thus [yabe] (~
/i{aaba/) alternates with /ijaba/ "strong, hard" (Vogt 1963:34). Historically, a sequence of vowel plus /h/ seems to have been involved in the
plural affixes, cf. (27).
(27)
/sa-wa-n-tw~n/
b.
/sa-w8-na-tw~n/ +- */sa-wa-n-ah-tw~n/
c.
11
me-you-he-give-pres"
/sYa-wa-n-tW-a-n/
+-
e.
r-
f.
g.
*/sYa-wa-n-tW-h-n/ or */sYa-wa-n-tW-eh-n/
"us-you-he-give-pl-pres"
d.
509
---------------
-------~
- -
~--
JOHN COLARUSSO
510
(28)
before a laryngeal
/yy/
/y?/
/i?/
/e?/
/ww/
/w?/
/u?/
/o?!
/??/
/a?/
/a?/ (older
speakers)
[eJ
[6]
[a]
(younger)
511
ference as well.
We saw that Bella Coola /?/ acted as a glide and vocalized
as [a]. But whether a given laryngeal is treated as a glide or a consonant or is vocalised as a low or a mid vowel seems to be language
specific. Hence, we spoke of consonantal articulations in the laryngeal region, contrary to most accepted theory. In the Circassian languages we saw that the laryngeal /h/ acted like a glide, undegoin9 metathesis as did /y/. West Circassian /?/ and /?W/ never behave as
glides, but pattern exactly like consonants, thus Bzh. /?a/ hand
never */a?/ or *[6.], /t'?Wa/ tW0 never */t'a?w/. In East Circassian,
however, the last word gives evidence that in this branch Proto-Circassian */?W/ was treated as a glide, thus Kab. /t'aw/ bl0 +- */t'a?w/ +*/t'?wa/, with glide-metathesis as with /h/ and /y/. Note also, that
the Kabardian form shows no evidence of lowering before */?w/, i.e., we
do not find */t'aw/. No lowering is found before the glottal stops in
West Circassian either, cf. Bzh. /a-?Wa/ ''his lips -+ /a-?w/, never
*/a-?w/, and /a-?a/ his hand -+/a-?/, never */a-?/. Therefore, /?/
is one segment that we can expect to vocalize as either [e] or [a],
just as the evidence in Greek and Hittite_suggest. This stands as a
well-attested typological tendency. In effect we are dealing with a
laryngeal that is [-high, -back, -low, +consonantal], a kind of consonantal // or, within the NWC languages, the consonantal counterpart to
/a/ since this is also [-high, -back, -low]. Note that, if a laryngeal
were to be consonantal and non-low, it would have to be non-back, otherwise it would be some sort of velar or uvular occlusive. Therefore,
our theory of laryngeals (cf. Colarusso forthcoming c for further details) provides an elegant explanation for the fact that consonantal,
non-low/?/ in the Circassian languages shows no coloring effects, i.e.,
its feature matrix consists of all negative feature specifications as
far as point of articulation features are concerned. In this sense,
the lucky choice of the notation o at least for *a1,
in PIE is quite
,...
accurate: *~1 was apparently a consonantal form of a schwa-like segment
10.11.
11
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11
11
11 ,
11
11 ,
512
JOHN COLAP.USSO
(at least /a/ in the NWC sense as a mere syllabic peak with no inherent
coloring), though of course there need not have been a true schwa vowel
counterpart in the PIE phonemic inventory. Such a PIE*/?/, or perhaps
better */?E/ or */?a/ would have behaved like the other laryngeals, syllabifying or vanishing with compensatory lengthening of a preceding vowel, but having no vowel-coloring effects. Only in one dialect branch,
Anatolian, did PIE *21 have a syllabic allophone that was [+low]. When
it was syllabified in Hellenic, it yielded the expected front, mid vowel form /E/, Gk. "E".
10.12. From the NWC evidence we may conclude that/?/ is a prime
candidate for PIE *21, an equation often made (Barnhard 1980; Lehmann
1952:108; Sturtevant 1951). /h/ is an excellent candidate for *~4, if
one wishes to posit another a-coloring laryngeal. Barnhard (1980), in
an interesting typological study, and following Hopper (1977a:49-50),
Lehmann (op. cit. :108) and Sturtevant (op. cit. :76), has equated *24
with /h/, but *2 2 with /x/, a voiceless velar or uvular spirant. Clearly if one posits two a-coloring laryngeals, there must be a difference
between them, but we will argue later against either velar or uvular
interpretations of any laryngeal (cf. 15). Finally, *~3, the most
enigmatic laryngeal, is often interpreted as a voiced velar or uvular
spirant with non-distinctive rounding, /y/ 4 [yw]. This too we shall
argue against later. Although there is no precise parallel to this
laryngeal among the NWC languages, there is much data 0n rounding: Kab.
/c'ak'w/ "little" 4 [c't~k'w], Bzh. /a-?we; "his lips" 4 [eS??w], /she-w/
"horse-predicative = being on horseback/a horseman~:
[shu]. That a
feature [+round] was part of *23 is indicated by the fact that it left
an intervocalic /-w-/ in a number of languages when it was syllabified
but followed by a vowel, cf. *de~3-m- (Attic <'iLounJL), *d2 3 -en- (Cypr.
ooFEvaL, Skt. &ivane) (t1artinet 1953). To find evidence for a laryngeal
segment with o-color, we have to turn briefly to the North ~merican Indian language Tonkawa (Hoijer 1946), which has a system of Schwebeablaut
(based on deletion processes, cf. 8.5), and traces of three vowel-col11
11
~------------------------------
-----------------
-----
513
/yamaxa-/ root:
b.
c.
d.
/yakap-/ root:
e.
f.
/yaka-kap-o?/
g.
/hepa-/ root:
h.
/xe-pa-co?/
i.
/hap i -/ root:
j.
/ka-pi-lo?/
k.
/ho?oxaw-/ root:
1.
/ko-?oxaw-o?/
+-
+-
"to hit"
/yaka-ykap-o?/ "he hits him repeatedly",
CVCV- reduplication and glide ~ 0/V___C.
+-
+-
------------
-----------~
514
JOHN COLARUSSO
Types of Palatalization:
a.
velar
-+
palatal:
+high
+back
b.
velar
-+
-+
+high
-back
kY
+high
+high
-back
-back
palata-alveolar affricate:
k
l+high
+back
-+
+high
+high
-back
-back
l+high
-back
+coronal
+delayed release
r--
Thus, while (30a) may be in some abstract sense the simpler phonological
change, (30b) is actually far more common due, apparently, to ease of
articulation. Note that the pharyngeals in NvJC fail to lower vo~tJels
(Colarusso in press; 1975:338-9), contrary to some claims (Kuipers 1960:
22-3), cf. Kabardian where both /a/, /a/ and [a] are found next to/~/,
/da~an/ "to carry something with someone's help", /da~an/ "to enter (a
515
11.1. The Nt~C languages show that pharyngeals can give rise to
laryngeals. It seems that pharyngeals are the_ only articulation that
can combine with [+low] to produce multiply articulated consonants, all
other zones of articulation not permitting a radical constriction of
the or.al cavity without conflicting with the open oral cavity characteristic of [+low] (Colarusso 1975:405-8; 1978). Furthermore, we should
expect laryngeals, if they are non-syllabic vowel-like segments, to occur with any of the modifications which we find with vowels. For example, since we find nasalized vowels, we might expect to find nasalized
laryngeals and in fact in Nenets Samoyed we find a nasalized /?n/ op- .
posed to a plain /?/ (Tere~enko 1966a:377). Since we find pharyngealized vowels in some Salishan languages (Mattina 1979) and a few Northeast Caucasian languages (cf. 10.13, 11.5), we should also expect to
JOHN COLARPSSO
516
find pharyngealized laryngeals, which we do. Such laryngeals are usually [+low], perhaps as a result of the synergistic effects between
pharyngeals and low vowels which we discussed in 10.13. Unlike nasalization, however, pharyngealization is not merely a secondary resonance
feature, but a1so represents a di sti net articulatory zone. Thus, pharyngealized laryngeals have the status of multiply articulated consonants, much like African /kP/ or Georgian /t'k'/ or /t'q'/ (Colarusso
in press). Such multiply articulated segments are found in a number of
languages. The Northeast Caucasian language Chechen (Nakh sub-group)
has many pharyngealized consonants, among which is a pharyngealized
glottal stop,/~/, opposed to/?/ (Catford 1970:2). The North American
Wakashan family has a similar contrast, cf. Nitinat with /~?/ vs. plain
/h/ and/?/, and the closely related Nootka with both/~/ and/~/ vs.
/h/ and /?/ (Jacobsen 1969:125-7) (it is not clear whether these are
[+low] pharyngeals or pharyngealized laryngeals, in which case they
would be better written as/~/ and/~/ respectively). In the NWC languages a similar tendency may be seen in the West Circassian tendency
to realize pharyngeals with a distinct laryngeal component, /~/ ~ [~],
/~/ (only in Arabic loans)~[~] or [n].
Similar tendencies occur in
the Ashkharwa dialect of Abkhaz (Colarusso in press, 1975:181; Catford
1970, 1972; Allen 1965a:119; Lomtatidze 1954:11ff. ), /~/ ~ [~], [ShJ or
[h], /~/ ~ [~], [~n] or [n]. The Ashkharwa tendencies seem to have been
generalized in the other Abkhaz dialects to more or less obligatory phonological rules. The pharyngeals, therefore, seem to have been the
source of the laryngeals in the other Abkhaz dialects.
..._____
..._____
..._____
11.2. In an Anatolian dialect of Abkhaz (Dumezil 1967:10), ProtoAbkhaz-Abaza (PAA) */~/has yielded /h/. Elsewhere, */~/ is preserved,
along with its rounded counterpart */~w; which persists even in Anatolian Abkhaz. The fate of */1/ has been different. Although it is preserved as an underlying, systematic phoneme,/~/, this/~/ is realized
in the Bzyb, Abzhwi(-Samurzakan) and Anatolian dialects as[~], perhaps
[n], rendered in the national orthography as aa. This laryngeal real-
- - - - - - - - - - -
517
ization of/\/, just like the laryngeal /h/ in Circassian, causes lml/ering and lengthening of a vowel~ There are two facts that point to /I:J./
as underlying aa. First, there is a rule spread throughout the N\1C
group that voices a verbal index when it occurs immediately before a
transitive verb root that begins with a voiced segment. Thus, /-s-/
"I" becomes 1-z-/ in the Abkhaz form /y-s-ba-wyt'/ ''it/them-I-see-pres"
= "I see it/them". Hith /-~-/ "we" in this position, one would expect
1-I:J.-/. One finds instead a lowered and lengthened vowel (ByazWba 1971:
76), cf. (31).
(31)
b.
lyasbawyt'l a-epenthesis
c.
d.
e.
lying form)
f.
lya~bawyt'l
g.
lya\bawyt'l index-voicing
h.
lyahbawyt'l \-lowering
i.
j.
a-epenthesis
~-
The derivation in (31e-j) is the only natural way to account for an [a]
where one would have expected *fa\/. Note that in (31i) the rule of
vowel-coloring treats /h/(~ /I:J./) just like a glide. So too in (31j),
both /h/ and /w/ vanish, with compensatory lengthening of the preceding
vowel. /h/ in Abkhaz, once produced, is obviously treated as a glide.
He may note that the form /w-ah-b6t.'/(= /w-h-ba-wt'/ ?) "we see you
(masc.)" in Anatolian Abkhaz (Dumezil op. cit.:31, 30) is either an
error or an icon elicited in its underlying form. The following assimilations show further evidence that /I:J./ underlies aa (Bgazba 1964:110):
518
JOHN COLARUSSO
Abzhwi ataacwa (/a..,ta)cWa/) -+ Bzyb /a-tahcwa; family Abzhwi axWaaxwtra (/a-xw)axwt-ra/ or /a-xwa)xwt-ra/) -+ Bzyb /a-xw~axwt-ra/ tO trade;
trade Bzyb /a-k'al ~a-ra/-+ Abzhwi ak'alaara (/a-k'al)a-ra/) l!opening
Finally, Abaza and Ashkhar\'Ja Jl.bkhaz /)a/ corresponds to Bzyb and Abzhwi
aa, cf. Abaza, Ashkharwa /-r)a/ agentive suffix, Bzyb and Abzhwi -raa,
Abaza and Ashkharwa I -)a-/ d0\'/n, back, beneath (prefix on verbs),
Bzyb and Abzhwi -aa-, Abaza /-)a-/ prefix on verbs denoting either
hither direction of activity or activity distant from the speaker,
Ashkharwa I -)a-/ hither Bzyb and Abzhwi -aa- hither a11 prefixes
on verbs. Abaza forms with /)/ are even rendered into Abkhaz linguistic works by means of aa (Bgazba op. cit. :145), Abaza ;~wsasa-r1a/
maiden written as ~Wsasa-raa.
11
11 ,
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11
11
11
11
11 ,
11
11 ,
11
11 ,
11 ,
11
11
11
r--
11
11
11 ,
11
11.4. The preceding sections have shown that one of the most likely antecedents to a laryngeal is an earlier pharyngeal. Of course,
there are numerous instances of PIE *p and *s becoming /h/ in a number
of daughter languages, but these segments cannot be of concern to us
here because they appear to be preserved within the mother language at
all recoverable stages of its history. Therefore, to gain a further
understanding of how the PIE laryngeals may have evolved, we must look
closely at the behavior of pharyngeals, in particular how they differ
from laryngeals. It is in this \'lay that we may find evidence that earlier pharyngeals underly some of the PIE laryngeals.
519
Pharyngeal s and pharyngea 1i zed segments have a rather surprising property: they front things, vowels especially. This is the
emphati c softening of Trubetzkoy (1931: 10-2 ~ 1958:124) (cf. Colarusso
1975:336-45; 1978). This is a phonetic effect. Pharyngealization produces a low frequency, formant-like noise of considerable power, this
being responsible for the muddy
thick or dark quality often
attributed to such sounds. t~hen combined with other formants, however,
such a low formant mimics the low formant characteristic of front vowels and other sonorants made in the front of the mouth, the results being often interpreted as fronting. Such effects are surprisingly widespread. In some Northeast Caucasian languages pharyngealized vowels
(v) occur. In certain dialects, these have been reinterpreted as front
vowels, Tsakhur /t'ox/-+ dial. /t'ox/ rope Rutulian /gat'/-+ dial.
/gat'/ Cat" (!) (Jeiranisvili 1959:343). The same effect is apparent
in some Interior Salishan languages, cf. Columbian /~acam/-+ [~~cam]
(Kinkade 1967), and in the history of Semitic, cf. (32) (Colarusso 1978).
11.5.
11
11
11
11 ,
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11 ,
11
(32)
a.
/ipte/ +- /ipte/ +-
b.
c.
Hebrew:
d.
/he~slr/
e.
+-
/~aser/
inherently pharyngealized)
Handaic:
f.
g.
The NWC 1anguages fo 11 ow suit. Ubykh, \'lith its numerous pharyngea 1i zed
uvulars and labials, offers excellent examples. In (33) spectrographic
data has been used (Colarusso 1975:219-92, 341; 1978) to determine the
520
JOHN COLPRUSSO
quantitative shift in the first two formants of the target vowel, /e/.
The effect is one of fronting.
(33)
/xama/
/xema/
environment
target
850Hz
1,700Hz
-x
m
---
550Hz
1,200Hz
Thus we have /xama/ 11 Stranger 11 -+ [xema], but /xfl!!}_a/ 11 harvest 11 -+ [xc:( !!}_a]
where the [~] almost gives the impression of [E]. In Abzhwi and Bzyb
Abkhaz this same effect has worked within a pharyngeal segment itself.
Thus, we have PAA */)W/ realized as [yw], i.e., a pharyngealized [4] in
Abzhwi and reinterpreted as /yw/ in Bzyb, cf. Ashkharwa /a-mrtwa/ 11 road 11 ,
Abaza /m)wa/, Abzhwi /a-m)We/-+ [amywa], Bzyb /a-mywo/. It is evident
that Bzyb has reinterpreted this pharyngeal as a rounded /y/ because
in certain Bzyb forms dissimilation has unrounded an earlier j)w/, and
when this happens we get /y/ rather than the/)/, aa, that we should
expect if this segment were still an underlying pharyngeal, cf. Bzyb
(a-i.Wyan/ vs. Abzhwi /a-zw)wan/ 11 sky 11 , Bzyb /a-t' waya/ vs. Abzhwi /at'We)Wa/ 11 horn 11 (Bgazba 1964:112-4). There are a few Bzyb forms in
which /yw/ has syllabified to produce front vowels, cf. Bzyb [wu] ~
/wyw; ~ */w)w/ 11 race 11 , [au]~ /ayw/ ~ */a)W/ 11 clothes moth 11 , [ozwa]
~ /aywzwa/(?) ~ */rtwazwa/ 11 twenty 11 . Thus, we may conclude that the
pharyngeals have the rather remarkable property of being the only segments that can be intimately associated both with low vowels and with
front vowels, high, mid and low. This unique combination of properties
will have important consequences for our theories about the PIE laryngeals.
A final point about the N\:JC pharyngeals is their tendency
toward skewing in both their synchronic and dia~hronic development. In
(34) we see how certain members of the PAA pharyngeal system are preserved while others tend toward lo\'t laryngeals or toward fronted glides.
11. 6.
521
PAA
Abaza
Ashkharwa
Bzyb
Abzhwi
Anatolian
*I~/
/~/
/~/([ ~] [ ~] [ h])
/I)!
/~/
/h/
*Ill/
/11/
/1/([ fi])
/\'I ([ fi])
/\'I ([ fi])
*/~W/
;~w;
;~w;
;~w;
;~w;
;~w;
*/flW/
/f!W/
;r;w;
/yW/
/f!W I ([ Lj])
/)WI ([ Lj] [ Ll ])
522
JOHN COLARUSSO
------------------------
- -
__
..
_,_ _ _ _
,
,_,
m.;rc
523
basis of internal evidence alone. Some workers have recognized the limits of Anatolian and have posited more than 3 laryngeals on the basis
of patterns within PIE (Puhvel 1965:92). One of these laryngeals, lost
in Anatolian, has even gained some currency, viz., *24 (Polom~ 1965:13ff;
Kurytowicz 1935:28-30). Thus, we have *22 in Hitt. ~anti "in front,
before, separately", ~anza "foreside, front", Lat. ante "before", Gk.
avT~ "over, against" (PIE *~2ent-), but *24 in Hitt. ap(p)a ''afterwards",
Lye. epn, Hier. Luw. apan, Gk. an:o "from", Clq, "backwards, back, again'!,
Lat. ab "from", absque "without", Alb. hap (Vaccarizzo /yap-/, Sophiko
/hap-/) "open" (PIE *2 4 ep-[o]). The Albanian forms may even have preserved *24 (Hamp 1965a:125, 4.1.1).
12.3. There is a tendency in the literature to attribute phonological behavior to a single laryngeal rather than to a phonological feature characterizing a class of laryngeals. Reconstructing a feature,
rather than a segment, though strange, is phonologically quite sound
and natural. For example, Winter (1965b:109-11, 113) argues for *g21
~ *gh in Indo-Iranian, being skeptical of *g~ 3 in Lat. ego, Gk. tyw(v),
Skt. aham. He takes Skt. maha, mahi-, Gk. wrya as reflecting *meg21-~2,
with *-22 an old collective. This is probably correct as Tokh. B maka,
makats (gen.) is inflected as a plural. Gk. wryEBos probably reflects
the simple stem in *meg2 1-dh-o-s. He chooses, however, to ignore the
vocalic evidence for *22 in the Greek forms yv&Bos, yvaBuos in favor of
a stem *gen-~1-. One expects the syllabic form of *~2 to yield Gk. a,
that of *21 Gk. E. He also ignores the -a- in Gk. Buy&Tnp and takes
the -a- in Tokharian from words for "mother" and "father", but the simple fact is that if *21 had been involved, one would have expected Tokh.
A *tkecar or *ckecar, B *tkecer, rather than A ckacar, B tkacer (cf.
also Hamp 1970 for further efforts to find *81 here). This is to ignore
attested facts, however, for analogical speculations, something to be
done only with the utmost caution. Furthermore, it is not necessary.
The evidence indicates that *~1, *~2, and *~3 all aspirate a preceding
voiced stop in Indo-Iranian. The proper conclusion is not that only
------~----
--------
JOHN COLARUSSO
524
one of these really does this, the others being illusory, but rather
that all three share some feature that is responsible for this phenomenon. Though a form *~ueg-21-, *21g-o21- may underly the forms for 11 I 11 ,
it is better to posit *81eg-83- and variants as *e3 is defined on the
basis of non-alternating *o and there are no known forms *ege, etc.
(Goth. ik is from the unstressed PGmc .. *ika(n), the stressed *eka(n)-+
Oice. ek [Hempel 1966:51-2], and not from *ike(n)). Thus, we have
*21g-e23- (theme II)-+ Gk. Eyw(v), *21eg-23- (theme I)-+ Skt. ah&m,
Arm. es, and probably Hitt. uqqa (/ekwkwa/ or /egwa/ + *2 1ekw-2 3-, with
*23 [+round]). Theme I would have given Lat. *eg and II Lat. *go, so
that leveling or analogical change here would be likely, cf. *21eg-e23
-+Lat. *ego-+ ego, via iambic shortening (Buck 1933:95), a form with a
restored full-grade in the root. Therefore, we seem to have a *83 which
aspirated voiced stops in Indo-Iranian. Various grades of *meg-21-(:22-)
seem to account for the daughter forms: *meg-21--+ Skt. mah (can occur
as an independent word, cf. Lindeman 1970:82; Grassmann 1964:1019), Gk.
~EYE~o~, Hitt. mekkis (with -kk- or -g- + *-k21- and perhaps -i- due to
the front nature of *21), *meg-21-22--+ Skt. mahi (also an independent
word, cf. Grassmann 1964:1012; MacDonell 1910:108), Gk. ~Eya, Tokh. B
maka, makats (gen.), *meg-21-e22- (with analogically restored full-grade
in the root)-+ Skt. maha, *meg-e~1- (anal. + *mg-e~1-)-+ Arm. mec. He,
therefore, seem to have a *21 that has the same effects in Indo-Iranian
as *23 Finally, *dhug-22-ter-+ Gk. ~uychnp, Skt. duhit~-. Av. dug8oar,
Tokh. B tkacer, Arm. dustr (where some idiosyncratic cluster developments seem to have led to early loss of laryngeal, cf. Hamp 1970; tlinter
1965b:112-3, for dissenting views). Thus, we can add *22 to the other
two laryngeals, all three sharing some phonological feature. Given the
phonological effects in Indo-Iranian, it is likely that we are dealing
with clusters of the form *g-h-, i.e., that these 3 laryngeals were spirants (but cf. 12.4, 16.3). What is of interest is that there is one
bit of striking evidence that there was another form of *\23 with quite
different phonological effects.
~
525
12.4.
------------------------.
-----
526
JOHN COLARUSSO
~.
527
528
JOHN COLARl!SSO
+*/?ph/, cf. 12.4 where */?w/ has the same effects), while at the
same time explaining the lack of vowel-coloring, with Lith. duobe, duobti going back to an earlier */de?wph-j with vowel-coloring. This explanation receives further unexpected support when it is seen that the
only other form known to me which seems to have PIE *b may also have
this rare segment explained by means of a similar confusion between
*/?W/' */w?/' etc.: thus Gk. oA.LBpb\! "smooth" (Hesych.) (*/I?W-ph-j'
*/?wl-ph-j), Lat. Ziibricus (*/l'f{?-ph-j), OE. slipor, Norw. slipra ''to
slide" (*/[s-] I?W-[e]ph-j), OHG. sUfan (*/[s-] le?[w]-ph-j ?), with
leveling among these forms leading to generalization of */p'/ (classic
*b), and further Lat. levis "smooth" (*/le?w-y-s/), Gk. :\d:o~ (*/l?ewyo-s/), and perhaps OE. Um "lime, birdlime" (*/le?[w]-m-/ ?). Our
theory has an unexpected and highly attractive bonus: we can explain
nearly all the difficult details of the three forms that clearly contain a PIE *b while at the same time showing that this *b is itself due
to details of a laryngeal, */?W/ or*/?/, in combination with some other labial stop, which we have chosen in most cases to represent as */ph/
(classic *p) where evidence as to its exact nature is lacking. In other words, the three clear instances of PIE */p'/ (*b) are the automatic
result of laryngeal effects that are also needed to explain the difficult details of vocalism in the forms in question. PIE */p'/ (*b) can,
therefore, be eliminated altogether. ~1ore information may yet be gleaned
from these *o rv *aw forms. The forms for "grow" indicate that a laryngeal in Anlaut must be overshadowed by a laryngeal in Auslaut, cf. Lat.
aug-e-o+ *~2ew-g- with a-coloring, but Lith. uoga + *~2e~3-g- \Jith the
laryngeal in Auslaut dominating. The Sanskrit form ug-ra-~ \'lould be
quite naturally *~ 2 w-g-. Furthemore, the peculiar Hittite form for
"mouth", ais, may have a similar explanation, also involving varying
vowel grades. Lindeman's development, *~3e81-os
~ *ahas ~ *ahes ~ *aes
"" ,..
~ Hitt. aii5, via a sel'ies of hypothetical .Proto-.Anatolian forms, though
perhaps plausible, is quite ad hoc. Furthermore, it goes against the
evidence that Auslaut laryngeals dominate in VO\'Jel-coloring, as well as
against the tendency for well-established cases of *~3 (of whatever sort)
529
11
11
11
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11
11 ,
JOHN
530
COLA~USSO
ments may block this deletion, cf., for example, Hitt. da-i "he places"
vs. te7:!,-7:!,i "I place", with variable retention of *~1, Gk. d~nllL. Not
all laryngeals, however, vanish in Auslaut, cf. Hitt. we~-zi, wa~-zi
"he turns", weh-anzi, wah-anzi "they turn" (no clear cognates outside
Anatolian), pa7:!,7:!,ur "fire", Toch. P.por (+ *paur), Goth. fon (with -n
generalized to the nominative from the oblique stem, cf. funins gen.,
Hitt. pa1J1Jweni dat. sg. )(~Jinter 1965c:192) -- though this form may have
undergone metathesis from *paw1J1Jr, cf. Toch. B puwar, Gk. nup, Arm. hur,
Oice. furr (a zero-grade?) -- Hitt. sebur "urine", Oice. saurr "male
semen; impurity, filth" (with metathesis in the full-grade, cf. OHG.
sou, souwes, OE. seaw, Oice. soggr [Lehmann 1965:213], and in the zerograde, cf. Oice. surr "sour", OCS. syr?J "moist, raw", Gk. VEL, Toch. B
suwam. "it rains"), Hitt. mehur "time", Goth. mel "time, hour, season",
OHG. mal "time", OE. mffil "mark, sign, measure, fixed time", Lat. metior
"to measure, mark off distance (perhaps on a sundial or some other early
instrument for measuring time)" (Puhvel 1965:89; Gamkrelidze 1968:92),
the last form with no evidence of metathesis. Winter S efforts to see
a *~3 in Hitt. me~ur by tentatively comparing it to Lat. moveo "move",
momentv~ is semantically less convincing than Puhvel S identification
and would seem to be motivated by an effort to reduce, unnecessarily,
all these persistent forms to some type of *~3 The coloring of the
vowel in Goth. mel points to an e-coloring laryngeal, and this accords
well with Lat. metior. The coloring effects of the laryngeal in Hitt.
pa~~ur are unclear.
Goth. fon suggests a-coloring, while OHG .. fiur
suggests e-coloring. Winter (ibid.) points to the Hittite dissimilation
of *o before a rounded segment: Hitt. nekuz "evening", Lat. nox; Hitt.
nekumanza "naked", Goth. naqaps. Thus, he would expect an earlier *sabur (+ *so~wr) behind Hitt. sebur. These dissimilating *o S in Hittite
are said to be those arising from laryngeal coloring, apophonic *o being stable, cf. Hitt. sakki "he sees", sekkweni "we see", Goth. sahw,
sehwum. On a phonological level, however, an /o/, regardless of where
it came from, should behave consistently. Rather than posit a dissimilation here for se~ur, nekuz, nekumanza, it may be simplest to accept
~
-----------------------------
---------
M~D
NWC
531
these as e-grades, regardless of the vocalism of their cognates elsewhere. The laryngeal in seGur, therefore, would be some type of *~1,
and the Germanic cognates, Oice. saurr, soggr, OHG. sou, etc., would be
rare cases of indubitable o-grade before a *~1 of some sort. If this
is correct, we may then readily add Lat. semen to this series of cognates. Whatever their coloring effects, and this is by no means a
settled issue, the laryngeals in these three words -- note too that
Hittite orthography indicates two kinds, -~~- and -~- -- persist in
Auslaut and this sets them quite apart from the usual reflexes of PIE
laryngeals discussed at the outset of this section. I shall examine
these words, Hitt. pahhur,
sehur,
mehur,
in more detail in 16.4, where
..........
.....
......
I shall suggest a radically different solution to their persistent laryngeals based upon further NWC parallels.
12.9. Though Hittite writes ~u- in certain words, it is noteworthy
that the reflexes of all forms of *~3 in initial position in Anatolian
are simple~-, cf. the scanty, but striking, forms Hitt. ~astcd "bone",
Luw. ~assa-, Gk. ooTsov, Arm. oskr (all with what is apparently */?w;,
cf. 13.5 for details), vs. Luw. ~aZJi-, Hier. Luw. haZJis "sheep", Lat.
ovis, but Arm. hoviZJ "shepherd" (with Arm. h- apparently pointing to
either */~w; or */~w;), cf. again 13.5 (data from Puhvel 1965:88; Winter 1965b:102). One might have expected some indication of rounding in
the Anatolian reflexes. All the more so as there is the initial GUand this is in contrast with ZJ- in some words, cf; Hitt. Guekzi "he conjures; he slaughters" vs. ZJekzi "he demands" (Puhvel op. cit.:87, fn.
21). Some instances of Hitt. hu- clearly reflect the h- reflex of one
of the *:;p s of Luw. ~au)i- or Hitt. ljast'Cd, cf. Hitt. ~ulana-, ~uliya
"wool" (~ *e 3ZJ-l-), Gk. oiS.\oc::; "wooly, woolen" (~ *~3eu)-l-), and with
metathesis Lith. vllna, Lat. lana, Skt. Ji1~na, Goth. umlla (PGmc. *uwlno
[~ *ZJ~~ 3 -n-e~ 2 ]) -- but cf. Dor . .\~voc::; with *~ 2 -- (Walde and Hofmann
1965:2.756-7). Other forms show a *e,.., 2 , cf. Hitt. huu,ant- "wind", Lat.
ventus (~ *~ 2 ZJ-en-t-), Gk. anJJL, "breathe hard, blow" (~ *~2ZJ-e~1-),
Epic aw (~ *e2eZJ?). Other forms suggest that a type of rounded laryn,..,
~
,..,
532
JOHN COLARUSSO
11 ,
11
11
12.10. There is another slight indication that rounded laryngeals may have kept their rounding in some Anatolian dialects despite
the Luwian and Hittite~- reflexes of classic *~3, though here too the
evidence is frustratingly slight. This rounded laryngeal may be represented by Lycian q (Puhvel 1965:84-5). Lycian q corresponds to
Hieroglyphic Luwian ~u, Hittite ~. and in forms borrowed outside of
Anatolian to a rounded velar, cf. Lye. qastti, qanuwetti punish
Hitt. aaskizzi, aannai judge Lye. qla Chief Hitt. ~alanta head
Lye. trqqas (nom. sg.), trqqnti (dat. sg.), Milyan {Lye. dial.) trqqiz
(nom. sg.) god'' or Lyciarch (?), Hier. Luw. Tarhu(nt)- name of
storm god, Anatolian onomastics TapHov6a~, Etruscan Tarxu-, Lat. Tarquinius, cf. Hitt. tar~-zi he conquers (*~2 ?). Therefore, it is
not impossible that some instances of Luwian au-, -~u- may reoresent
a rounded laryngeal, distinct from the h- reflex of classic *e3. The
speculations here and in 12.7, 12.8 might be lightly dismissed if
it were not for the fact that there is sound evidence for two forms of
*~ 3 elsewhere (12.2, 12.3, 12.7, 12.8, 12.9, i.e., */?w/, */lw/ and
perhaps */bw/). The worth of these hypotheses can only be borne out
by careful future research.
11 ,
11
11
11
11 ,
11
13.
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11
11
533
ments, the pharyngeals, that not merely resembled the laryngeals, but
could also offer an historical source for them. We noted in particular
that these pharyngeals could readily vocalize either as a low vowel or
a front, often high vowel, a unique pattern of vocalization alternatives. Such a distinctive vocalization pattern would be a powerful
tool for arguing for earlier pharyngeal forms of laryngeals in PIE if
variations between front and low vowel forms for laryngeals in syllabic
position could be found. We have already commented upon the peculiar
behavior of the so-called schwa primum (12.5): *meg-~1-~2 ~ Skt. mahi,
with i, but Gk. ~Eya, ~Eya~, with a, where a *~ 2 is involved. Such variable syllabic reflexes may underly other problems of IE philology.
Polome (1965:31, 43 and fn. 195) is reluctant to equate
Hitt. daluki-, Gk. 6oALX6~, Skt. dirgha-, precisely because of the odd
equation Hitt. -u- ~ Gk. -L-. If we attribute schwa primum vocalism
to *~2 (perhaps instances of syllabified */~/ or*!'/), then we might
posit here a form with a distinct pharyngeal: */dJ1wgh-j. Its rounding would account for the a-color of the syllabic *l in both Hittite
and Greek, as well as the -u- vocalism in Hittite, while its pharyngeal
nature would account for the high, front vowel, -L-, in Greek. Cf. also Hitt. kaluti- line, list (+- */k!\'wdhy-/), Gk. xAwcrL~ Spinning,
line, list (+- */kle1wdhy-/), xAwBw Spin (+- */klef!wdh-j) (Puhvel
1965:90). We have already made use of a similar pharyngeal fronting
in 12. 7 to account for the -i- (/y/) in Hitt. ai5 mouth +- *awys +13.2.
11
11
11
11
11
11
11
11
*~~+ew1-s.
13.3. While certain front vowels or glides may be due to pharyngeal fronting effects, it is clear that the /-y-/ in the diphthongal
stems of the Hittite -~i conjugation are not as a class exuded from
the frequent root final laryngeal (contra Puhvel 1965:91-2; Risch 1955).
This -i-, -iy- can be from a *-y(e/o)- present that occurs elsewhere in
IE, cf. Hitt. dai he places tiyanzi they place Lat. (con-)diunt
(10.1, 12.8). Furthermore, this *-y- of the present has been shown
11
11 ,
11
11 ,
JOHN COLARPSSO
534
11 ,
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11
11
11
11
11
---------------------~.~.--
- -
535
13.5. We have argued that the PIE laryngeals indeed behaved like
true laryngeals, [low]. We have also seen that there is some evidence,
some of it strong, that there were several variants of at least two of
the laryngeals, *~2 and *~3 Furthermore, some of the peculiarities of
PIE vocalism that have been tentatively linked to laryngeals find plausible explanations if viewed as alternate low or fronted vowels or
glides arising from pharyngeals. We obtain a simple, yet powerful,
picture of the PIE laryngeals that have survived into Anatolian, if we
assume, therefore, that they go back to pharyngeals. Moreover, we may
now account for those PIE laryngeals that did not survive into Anatolian (with one exception) as being reflexes of original laryngeal segments. Thus, the second a-coloring laryngeal, *~4, would go back to
an original */h/, *81 back to*/?/. Given the apophonic parallels between NWC and PIE (9.2), such typologically troublesome and unlikely
JOHN COLARUSSO
536
~~
13.6.
537
closely resemble, therefore, that of the same sounds in the NWC languages. Though complex and admittedly speculative, these proposals offer a wealth of phonologically and typologically sound mechanisms for
explaining many of the more obscure details of laryngeal theory in PIE.
These proposals try to do for PIE and its daughters what a generative
phonologist would try to do for a living language: come up with a rich
enough underlying structure (proto-forms) and a set of natural rules
that will be rich enough to account for all the data that cannot be
morphologically explained away. These goals have led quite naturally
to a proliferation of PIE laryngeals, quite in contrast to most modern
efforts in historical work to restrict the number of proto-segments,
laryngeals in particular, that are. posited for PIE. Thus, the present
proposals may seem quite odd to most Indo-Europeanists. There is some
typological evidence, however, that suggests strongly that PIE should
have more segments than present reconstructions give it and that this
is particularly true of the PIE laryngeals.
14.
538
JOHN COLARUSSO
---------'----------
---~--~-
-----~--
539
p'
Pharyngealized
-p
p'
t
tW
d
dW
t'
t'W
c'
Rounded
c
cw
)w
c'w
Apicalized
c'
c_v
~y
c'v
c'
Alveolars
Rounded
Affricates, Spirants (Laminal)
Alveo-palatals (Rounded)
Palata-alveolars (Laminal)
Retroflexed
Laterals
s
sw
z
zw
s
sW
z
.zw
sv
zY
?\ '
z
v
kY
gY
k'Y
Velars (Rounded)
kW
gw
k'w
xw
qY
q'Y
xY
yY
Plain
q
qW
q'
q'W
xw
y
yw
Pharyngealized
q'
-y
Pharyngealized, Rounded
qw
q'W
xw
yw
Laryngeal
Palatals
Rounded
14.3. In other words, the PIE consonantal system is very impoverished when compared to its vowel system if the NWC and Ndu languages are
a reliable typological guide, as they seem to be. The historical processes that lead to vertical vowel systems will not create consonantal
series at new points of articulation, but they will create numerous series with secondary articulations. It is these that are missing from
PIE as it now stands, with the sole exception of the rounded ("labio-")
velars. I do not wish to suggest that people should go hunting for
rounded alveolars, etc., in PIE, but rather that any laryngeal or pharyngeal consonants that are proposed should be quite naturally expected
---------------------
JOHN COLARUSSO
540
Lapials
p'
Alveolars
t'
f'
Rounded
c'
(:_w
~W
c'W SW
zW
SW
zW
Alveo-palatals, Rounded
Palate-alveolars (Laminal)
c'v 5v
Retroflexed
c'
Retroflexed, Rounded
7<'
Laterals
Palatals
Velars
k'
Rounded
kW
gW
k'W XW
q'Y xY
yY
Plain
q'
Rounded
Pharyngealized
Rounded
Pharyngeals
Rounded
15.
-------
-----------
541
15.2. This frustrating mixture of forms occurs right within Anatolian itself: (data from Puhvel) Hitt. kilamni : Gilammar "gate building", is~isaza : iskisaza (neut. nom. iskisa- "back"). These may be
signs of dialect mixture, but this k ~ Gconfusion is a common one
throughout this area, cf. OBabyl. Gissatum : kissatum "forced servi ce
NAssyr. ~anasu : kanasu Subject oneself", LBabyl. tamaku : tama~u (no
gloss), Hurrian kesk/~i, ~e3k/~i at Boghazkoy, gs~-p at Ras Shamra,
1
',
11
JOHN COLARUSSO
542
"chair" (cf. the Hurrian loan into Ugaritic, k~i3, and Hittite, kislji[ta]-), Hattie Kat(t)a~~as, lj_ata~~as, Kata~gas, lj,ataggas, etc., "Queen''
(name of a goddess). Such fluctuations over space and through time
strongly suggests unsettled scribal conventions for representing sounds
alien to the cuneiform syllabary, originally taken from Sumerian, a language poor in gutturals. Taken together with the Greek transcriptions,
any hope of deducing the phonological nature of the Anatolian laryngeals
from the orthographic evidence seems fairly remote.
15.3. Abandoning orthographic concerns, we must ask ourselves if
it is typologically possible that velar or uvular spirants could have
been associated with the pharyngeals and laryngeals that we have seen
are likely to underly the PIE laryngeals. Unfortunately, to complicate
the picture yet further, the answer is ''yes''. Fortunately, however,
the relationship between all these sounds is well defined. The velars,
uvulars and laryngeals do not seem to be phonologically interrelated in
any way in the NWC languages. On the other hand, the PNWC uvulars have
in Abkhaz-Abaza become pharyngeals, while the original pharyngeals, again in Abkhaz-Abaza and in part in Ubykh, have become pharyngealized
uvulars (Colarusso 1978), cf. (37) and (38).
(37)
PNWC Uvular
a.
~/pa-w-xa/ ~
b.
*/pa-xa/
c.
'~~/pa-y-xa-/ ~
d.
"~~/pa-xa/ ~
PNWC
e.
<J-qh(e/a)-/ "pear"
'~~/w-qhe-/ ~ Bzh. /qhwe<Z.e)/ "pear"
f.
*/y-q~a/ ~ ubykh
g.
*/qha/
~ Shapsugh WCir.
/pxa/ id.
Ubykh /pxYa(dak'W)/ "young girln
;xv'a;
~ Abkhaz /a-~a/
543
b.
PNWC
'~~/li(e/a)~a/
c.
*/7\e~a/
-+
"mountain"
*/-'1\~a/
-+ Bzh.
/qwes~a/
forest"
d.
*/7\e~a/-+ */1-i.~a/-+
e.
*/1-i.a~a/
544
JOHN COLARUSSO
545
*-e-e,., ,., +-aka-, with velar stops emerging through dissimilation before
a spirant, in this case another laryngeal, much as with the dissimilation before (*)s in Latin and Armenian. Clearly, we must be prepared
to accept velar allophones of some laryngeals. A careful consideration
of the data, however, shows that this velar, though arising from a laryngeal, does not itself have laryngeal effects. The Latin forms are
crucial here, because these are the only reflexes that simply and unambiguously preserve laryngeal coloring and lengthening. It is clear
that in the shift *sen-ee2-to-s
+ senatus the *e2
,.,
,., has exhibited its
normal behavior. In *sen-ee2-s
+ senex, on the other hand, neither
,.,
coloring nor lengthening is apparent, yet this is just where one clearly has a velar allophone of *e2.
Therefore, if there was something a,.,
bout a velar that caused laryngeal effects, i.e., if this laryngeal
were actually a velar, then we would expect the form *senax or *senax,
which, of course, we do not find. The length in -trix is not a forceful counterexample because it could easily represent an earlier *-tr~x
with a long vowel restored from a *-tri (Skt. -tri). In short, rather
than pointing to a velar laryngeal, the Latin evidence merely points to
a velar allophone for a laryngeal which, because of its velar nature,
lacks any laryngeal properties. We have two strong arguments, Watkins'
and, unintentionally, Martinet's, that the PIE laryngeals could not
have been velar spirants.
15.6. The velar allophone of some of the laryngeals remains to be
accounted for. This is where the pharyngeal to uvular shift which we
observed in Abkhaz-Abaza and in Ubykh is useful. \~e can well picture
a shift, taking the predecessor of Lat. senex as an example, of the following sort: */sen-e~-s/ + */sen-ex-s/ + */sen-ex-s/ + /seneks/ (senex),
where the shift of */~/ + */x/ (possibly via */q/) is found in the shift
from PNWC to PAA, and the shift, */x/ + /x/, is found in going from PAA
to the Abzhwi dialect (cf. 39b, d). Note, however, that *~2 cannot be
/x/. The only coloring effect that a /x/ can have is a fronting one,
as we saw with Ubykh (cf. 11.5.[34]), no lowering effects are possible.
------------------------~
------
546
JOHN
COLA~USSO
While fbi can have fronting effects, it can, in its allophonic possibilitites as [bh] and [h], have lowering effects as well. *~2 cannot,
therefore, be underlyingly either /x/ or /x/. If we take it to be a
pharyngeal, however, we can explain its lowering effects as well as its
tendency to form uvular or velar spirants. The same arguments hold for
the rounded laryngeal, *~3 The tendency to shift either way between
pharyngeals and pharyngealized uvulars is apparently due to co-articulatory effects, the tongue being somewhat backed with the retraction of
the tongue root characteristic of pharyngealization. A pharyngealized
uvular being a complex segment with a difficult articulation, it is
natural that it be prone to simplification, either to a plain uvular,
as in Abzhwi Abkhaz, or to a plain velar, as apparently in some dialects
of PIE.
~
16.
-------------------------
-----------
------------
547
11 ,
11
11
11
11 ,
11 ,
11
11
11 ,
11
11 :
11
11
11
11
11
11 ,
11
11
11 ,
11
11
11
11 ,
11 ,
11
11
548
JOHN COLARUSSO
Voiceless varieties of these types of laryngeals may have existed, */~/ (*~2) and */~w;
(*:~p); for*/~/, */sthe~-/, */sth~-/ "stand": Dor. L:-aTiiJJL, onho~,
Lat. status, Skt. sthita-~; for */~w;, perhaps (if we follow Hittite
orthographic evidence), */s~-~w-/, */sen-~w-/ "strive, gain": Gk. *&viJJJL (*/snhW-m-i/), Skt. san6ti, Hitt. sanhanzi (both */snhW-en-t"-i/),
Hitt. san~zi (*/sen~W-th-i/) (Puhvel 1965:91-2). It is also possible
that */~w; may underlie some of the following forms, where Sanskrit
shows a rounded realization of a *~ in one case and an unexplained -uin another: Skt. sphurj- "burst", Gk. a<papay(o)JaL "to burst, crackle",
Lith. sp~rgas (with -~r- possibly pointing to *-t~-, cf. Watkins 1965a:
117), Lat. spargo (+- *sparago ?) "to scatter, sprinkle" (Burrow 1965:701, 3; Meillet 1964:124), where we may posit */sp"[~wg-/, */sph~w[g-/
variants of a stem; Skt. sphurati "throbs, quivers", Lith. spiri~. Gk.
anaCpw "to gasp (of a dying fish, hence to flop, thrash about)'', Lat.
sperno "to remove, reject", and perhaps Gk. a<pal:pa "ball" (Buck 1949:
907), pointing to a stem */sph~w(e)r(-y)-/, perhaps a different vocalic
grade of the preceding stem. Thus, */~/ and */~w; may be considered to
lie behind at least some of the voiceless aspirates that have developed
in Indo-Iranian, Greek and Armenian. Their spirant h-like character
would be responsible for preservation or addition of aspiration in the
preceding stop. These pharyngeals survive in Anlaut in Anatolian,
though it is not possible in most cases of Anlaut and never in initial
position to distinguish between voiced and voiceless counterparts, cf.
*/ther('i'/h)-/ "conquer": Hitt. tarh(h)-, Lat. -trCire, trans, Skt. tiras,
Av. taro, Oir. tar, Welsh tra-, trauJ (Hamp 1965b:231; Benveniste 1935:
151); */('i'/~)er-k'-/ "shining, v1hite": Hitt. ~arkis, Gk. apyn~. Lat.
araentwn, Arm. arcat' (*l:arcat', with an unstable h- from *82.,
cf. t1in,...
ter 1965b:102, 2.1); *j(c;w;~w)ew-/ "sheep": Luw. ~aui-, Hier. Luw.
hau;is, Arm. hoviu; "shepherd", Lat. ovis "sheep". ~le should recall that
Armenian data are crucial for distinguishing between the rounded pharyngeals, as in the last word, and */?W/ in initial position, as the latter
seems also to have been preserved in initial position in Anatolian, thus
0.
0.
549
*/?west'-/ "branch 11 :
550
JOHN COLARUSSO
(*~1)
-y
pharyngeals
(*~2)
rounded
()'c2 3)
~w
C[W
(*24)
rounded
(*21)
(*23)
?W
Typologically (39) may seem odd in the absence of plain uvular or velar
stops and spirants. but it should be noted that this system is very
similar to that in Proto-Semitic, where one has only two plain velars,
*/k/ and */g/, 1t1hile the other uvulars are all pharyngealized (actually
pharyngealized velars underlyingly) (Moscati et aZ. 1964:24, 8.3), cf.
(40). I have modified the traditional Semitic transcription.
(40)
Proto-Semitic "Gutturals":
velar
-q
pharyngeals
laryngeals
g
X
-y
C[
In the history of PIE the system in (39) would have been successively
collapsed down toward the laryngeal zone of articulation, at which the
segments would have become unstable and subsequently vanished. As to
an earlier, though entirely conjectural, stage of PIE lying behind (39),
one could envision a system such as that in (41), where the numerous
pharyngeals of (39) are now spirants in the velar and labio-velar (uvular?) series, while the pharyngealized uvulars are now simple pharyn-
551
geals.
(41)
k'
k'W
velars
rounded (labio-velars)
pharyngeals
?
?W
laryngeals
rounded
xw
y
yw
The system in ( 4-1) is very Caucasian 1ooki ng. Moreover, the shift from
(41) to (39) has its parallel in the PNWC shift to PAA. It is tempting
to view all this as an IE-Caucasian areal feature. Subsequent modification and loss of the laryngeals in PIE could then be viewed as due to
spread of the mother tongue beyond this area and consequent simplification by new speakers of what must have seemed a very difficult and
strange system.
11
11
16.6. The proposals in this paper have been suggestions with which
Indo-Europeanists may work. Whether or not most of these will bear
fruit can only be determined by careful comparative work. Surely, in
many etymologies involving laryngeals insufficient information has survived to permit any sure determination of the exact nature of the underlying laryngeal and it is in these cases that the traditional cover symbols will be used, now and for the forseeable future. It is time, however, that more confidence be placed in typological and phonological
theory and that we view as highly 1i kely, if not assured, that the PIE
laryngeals were some form of pharyngeal or laryngeal consonant, and
perhaps even pharyngealized uvulars. The set of possible can.didates
for the PIE larync;~eals may be confidently viewed as limited and well
defined, and the phonological and historical behavior of these forms as
well understood, albeit complex. The identification of a particular
reflex within an etymology _as a given segment, *!QI, */l'/, *f?wj, etc.
will only rest on detailed etymological work. Major shifts such as
""""'""----------------------~
- - - - -..
---------
---
------------
- -
-- --
---~--
552
JOHN COLAPUSSO
that from the system in (41) to the one in (39) may only be recoverable,
if at all, through careful systematic considerations or even external
comparison (cf. Barnhard this volume; 1977). Despite such reservations,
Indo-Europeanists should now have greater confidence in using the phonological behavior of pharyngeals, laryngeals and pharyngealized uvulars
to unravel the difficult and complex patterns found in the reflexes of
the PIE laryngeals.
REFERENCES:
Allen, J.D. and Hurd, P. ~J. 1972. "}fanambu Phonemes," Te Reo 15.
Allen, W. S. 1965a. "On One Vowel Systems", Linqua 13.
Allen, W. S. 1965b. "An Abaza Text':, Bedi kartlisa: Revue de kartvelologie 19.
Allen, W. S. 1956. "Structure and System in the Abaza Verbal Complex'",
Amsterdam.
Bomhard, A. R. 1979. "The Indo-European Phonological System:. New
Thoughts about its "Reconstruction and Development," 01~bis XXVIII/ 1.
Barnhard, A. R. 1977. "The 'Indo-European/Semitic' Hypothesis P.eexamined", JIES 5/1.
Buck, C. D. 1949. A Dictionary of Selected Synonyms in the Principal
Indo-European Languages. Chicago.
Buck, C. D. 1933. Comparative Grammar of Greek and Latin. Chicago.
Burrow, T. 1965. The Sanskrit Language. London.
Catford, J. C. 1977. "Nountain of Tongues: the Languages of the Caucasus", Annual Review of Anthropology 6.
- - -
---------
TYPOLOGICAL PAFALLELS
BET~ffiEN
553
Catford, J. C. 1972. "Labialization in Caucasian Languages Hith Special Reference to Abkhaz", in Proceedings of the Seventh International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. The Hague.
Catford, J. C. 1970. Report on Fieldtrip to the U.S.S.R.
(Mimeographed.) Ann Arbor.
Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New
York.
Colarusso, J. Forthcoming(a). "Methodological Considerations in Historical Reconstruction: The Case of Proto-Northwest Caucasian",
International RevieUJ of Slavic Linguistics. (Special volume on
the non-Slavic languages of the U.S.S.R., ed. by Bernard Comrie.)
Colarusso, J. Forthcoming (b). "The Origin of Greek Favcon-".
Colarusso, J. Forthcoming(c). "On the Nature of the So-called 'Laryngeal' Segments".
Colarusso, J. In Press. "Phonemic Contrasts and Distinctive Features:
Caucasian Examples", in The Elements: A Parasession on Linguistic
Units and Levels_, Including Papers from the Conference on Non-Slavic Languages of the USSR. Chicago. (Appeared in 1979.)
Colarusso, J. 1978. The Typology of Pharyngeals and Pharyngealization:
Caucasian Examples. Paper presented at the 6th North American Conference on Afro-Asiatic Linguistics, April 9-10, Toronto.
Colarusso, J. 1977. "Languages of the Northwest Caucasus 11 , in Tl:e Lan-
42.
Gamkrelidze, T. V. and Ivanov, V. V. 1973. "Sprachtypologie und die
Rekonstruktion der gemeinindogermanischen Verschliisse", Phonetica
27.
---------------------
554
JOHN COLMl'SSO
Garnkrelidze, T. V. and Ivanov, V. V. 1972. "Lingvisticeskaja tipologij a i rekonstrukcij a sistemu indoevropej skix smycnyx", in vlorking
555
Kerns, J. A. and Schwartz, B. 1940. "The Laryngeal Hypothesis and IndoHittite, Indo-European Vocalism", JAOS 60.
Kinkade, M. D. 1967. "Uvular-Pharyngeal Resonants in Interior Salish'',
IJAL 33.
Klimov, G. V. 1969. Die kaukasischen Sprachen. Translated from the
Russian by W. Boeder. Hamburg.
Kuipers, A. H. 1976. "Typologically Salient Features of Some Northwest Caucasian Languages", Studia Caucasica 3.
Kuipers, A. H. 1975. A Dictionary of Proto-Circassian Roots. Lisse.
Kuipers, A. H. 1968. "Unique Types and Typological l!niversals", in
Pratidanam. The Hague.
Kuipers, A. H. 1963. "Proto-Circassian Phonology';, Studia Caucasica 2.
Kuipers, A. H. 1960. Phoneme and Morpheme in Kabardian. The Hague.
Kury{owicz, J. 1935. Etudes indoeuropeennes. Vol. I. Cracow.
Lehmann, W. P. 1965. "Germanic Evidence", in Evidence for Iaryngeals.
The Hague.
Lehmann, W. P. 1952. Proto-Indo-B~ropean Phonology. Austin.
Lehmann, W. P. 1951. "The Distribution of Proto-Indo-European /r/",
Language 27.
Lindeman, F. 0. 1970. Einfuhrung in die Lar~wtgaltlzem~ie. Berlin.
Lindeman, F. 0. 1967. "Indo-europeen *os 'bouche'", in To Honor Roman
Jakobson. Vol. II. The Hague.
Lomtadze, E. A. 1963. Ginuxskij dialekt didojskogo ja:::yka. Tbilisi.
Lomtatidze, K. V. 1954. Asxaruli dialekt'i da misi adgili sxva apxazur-abazur dialekt'ta soris [The Ashkharwa Dialect and its Place
among the Other Abkhaz-Abaza Dialects]. (In Georgian.) Tbilisi.
HacDonell, A. A. 1910. Vedic Grammar. Strassburg.
Martinet, A. 1957a. "Phonologie et 'laryngales'", Phonetica 1.
Martinet, A. 1957b. "Les 'laryngales' indo-europeennes", in Proceed&ngs of the VIIIth International Congress of Linguists. Oslo.
Martinet, A. 1956. "Some cases of -k-/-ul- Alternation in Indo-European", Word 12.
Martinet, A. 1955a. "Le couple senex-senatus et le 'suffixe' -k-",
BSL 51/1.
Martinet, A. 1955b. "L'analyse en traits distinctifs et la reconstruction: le vocalisme o non-apophonique en indo-europeen", in Econo-
Traitc de pl:onologEe
diacl~ronique.
Bern.
Martinet, A. 1953. "Non-apophonic a-vocalism in Indo-European", r/ord 9.
Mattina, A. 1979. "Pharyngeal Hovement in Colville and Related Phenomena in the Interior Salishan Languages", IJAL 45/1.
Meillet, A. 1964. Introduction a l'etude comparative des langues indoeuropeennes. Reprint of 8th edition. University.
556
JOHN COLARUSSO
557
geals.