Analysis of Turn-Taking in A Scripted Conversation
Analysis of Turn-Taking in A Scripted Conversation
Analysis of Turn-Taking in A Scripted Conversation
2014
1. INTRODUCTION
This paper is a discourse analysis of a food and drink dyad from a student
course book (appendix 1). The analytical approach used is Conversation Analysis
(CA). The study focuses on turn-taking, and shows how a scripted audio recording is
an unnatural representation of what happens in natural conversational discourse.
In order to put the study in context, a summary of the history of CA is given
and a brief definition of the approach is outlined in 1.2. Next an overview of the
discourse being analysed in the study is given in 2.0. The discourse is then considered
with reference to turn-taking and a more detailed look at adjacency pairs is discussed
in 2.1 and 2.2. In 3.0 contextual and cultural factors are analysed leading to a brief
discussion about scripted discourse and authenticity.
\Due to the nature of the scripted dyad, and word count limitations, the
analysis focuses primarily on adjacency pairs and omits direct discussion about presequences, insertion sequences and turn keeping.
2. CRITICAL FRAMEWORK
2.1 What is Conversation Analysis?
CA is the study of natural talk in interaction. Its aim is to see how people
communicate, whether casual or institutional (in the school, government office or any
other establishment). CA developed from ethnomethodology in the late 1960s and
early 1970s principally from work by the sociologist Harold Garfinkel, who was
interested in the question of how people interpreted daily life and developed a sense
of social order. In 1974 work by Harvey Sacks and his close associates Emanuel A.
Schegloff and Gail Jefferson on turn-taking in conversation further developed what is
known today as CA. In short CA,
Focuses on detailed recorded conversations, analysing them for specific
features of their moment by moment production, and interpreting the
significance of the utterances in the light of their environments of action.
(Heritage, 1984.p.292)
natural conversation overlap occurs. This is when an interactant starts speaking before
the turn of the other speaker has finished. Ervin-Tripp (1979) observes that overlap,
(interruptions or simultaneous speech), in turns happens in approximately 5 per cent
of conversation. This suggests that speakers usually know, through complex verbal
and nonverbal signalling, when a turn is ending.
McCarthy (1991) notes overlap is significant in conversation as it normally
signals, urgency, annoyance, or the desire to correct what is being said. The food and
drink dyad contains no interruptions or overlaps. This implies the turn-taking is
cooperative rather than competitive. Overlap in conversation can also be a sign of
heightened emotional involvement. Tannen (1989) defines co-operative overlap as a
sign of a high-involvement style in which interactants show concord, enthusiasm and
interest in each others talk. An example of this is shown in turns (13) and (14) at the
end of the food and drink dyad. However, the lack of involvement shown by the
speakers in the preceding turns makes the sudden interest sound forced and unnatural.
Further indicators for turn taking are highlighted by Wardhaugh (2002) who points
out that a change in pitch level often signals closure or the end of a turn. This happens
in the food and drink dyad. Pitch changes can be heard when listening to the last
words of turns (4), (9), (11) and (12) (see appendix 1 and CD enclosed). The words
often, tea, mayonnaise and though have fall in tone. Pauses are also used to indicate
the end of a turn in conversations. This is illustrated in the turn change between turns
(9) and (10). Here the short pause, of approximately 0.8 of a second, after the word
mayonnaise signals the end of the turn.
In conversation listeners often like to show the speaker they are interested,
understanding, agreeing or simply attending to the message. This is usually done by
inserting a word such as sure, right, or yeah . In addition back- channel vocalisations
such as hmm, ahh, and eh-eh are commonly used by the listener as non-interrupting
signals to the speaker.
Other utterances that typically signal the end of a turn are phrases like you know,
or something and do you know what I mean? Appendix 1 has no examples of backchannel or any short words or phrases to show the speaker they are attending to the
message. As McCarthy notes,
Natural conversational data can often seem chaotic because of back
channel, utterance-completions and overlaps (1991.p127)
The food and drink dyad lacks many natural conversation elements. This paper
now looks atthe adjacency pairs in the dyad to determine whether they are authentic
representations of adjacency pairs that occur in natural conversation.
3.2 Adjacency Pairs
Adjacency pairs allow speakers to allocate and give up turns. Sacks et al
(1974) identify adjacency pairs as major turn types. As with the example above
the most common is the question/answer adjacency pair. The food and drink dyad
contains six question/answer adjacency pairs. There is no talk (or pre-sequence)
prior to the opening sequence and no insertion sequences Pre-sequences and
insertion sequences are linguistic elements ofnatural conversations that are not
included in the scripted dyad.
Different adjacency pairs occur in spoken discourse. For example:
(1) offer/accept: A) Ill make dinner (2) request/grant: A) Can I sit here?
B) Thanks
B) Yeah, no problem
The adjacency pairs above show that different utterances are likely to be followed
by particular responses. An offer is followed by acceptance and so on. Writers
such as Guy Cook identify further patterns,
In an adjacency pair there is often a choice of two likely responses. A
request is most likely to be followed by an acceptance or a refusal. (1989
p.53)
if the speaker is responding to a questionnaire or does not care that the speaker
might like some of the things that s/he dislikes.
In authentic conversation, interruptions and overlaps are part of the
negotiation of turns. Smooth speaker switches are negotiated with no simultaneous
speech or perceptive pauses. This signifies the speakers are negotiating turns
cooperatively, rather than competitively where interruptions would feature more.
Apart from the speaker switch between Selina and Finn (shown in turns (9) and
(10) of appendix 1) where a perceptive pause occurs between mayonnaise and what
the other speaker switches are smooth.
However, the smooth speaker switches intuitively sound odd and unnatural. If
a person likes coffee and somebody says to them, I dont like coffee, a probable
response might be an interruption to find out why the person does not like coffee.
Alternatively a person might seek to defend their position of liking coffee and
interrupt the speaker to hail the benefits of an early morning cuppa. Either way,
for someone to remain quiet and listen attentively to someone listing their likes and
dislikes without any reasons or linguistic softening for their choices appears
unusual.
3.4 Scripted Discourse and Authenticity
Turn-taking signifiers are complex and range from drops in intonation, to the
response expectations inherent in adjacency pairs. Even in casual conversation
speakers have different roles whether they are friends, co-workers or strangers.
These different roles determine how people interact with each other. Guy Cook
illustrates this point saying, students fall silent when the professor speaks - in the
bar as well as the seminar (1989.p53). Without a context a layer of meaning is
removed from the message and the listener may not personalise the language, and
misunderstand its grammatical function.
A context is not provided for the food and drink dyad. The listener is given no
clues as to where the conversation is taking place or what the relationship between
the speakers might be. The lack of context provides an unnatural situation for the
listener. Different roles and settings generate different structures, without any
context or any idea of the relationship between the two speakers an interpreter of
the conversation is left only with the structures which provide few clues to how,
when and with who the language might be used. Even the pace of the conversation
is uniform and in received pronunciation without the variability of authentic
conversation. As Porter and Roberts (1981) note regarding scripted dyads in ELT,
the intonation has sounds like a mother talking to a child with unusually wide and
unusually frequent pitch movement. (p37)
Appendix 1
Appendix 2
REFERENCES
Burns, A. and Coffin,S. (2001) Analysing English in a Global Context: A Reader,
London
& New York: Routledge.
Cook, G. (1989), Discourse, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Eggins, S. and Slade, D. (1997), Analysing casual conversation, London: Cassell
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1979). Children's verbal turn-taking. In Ochs, E. and Schieffelin, B.
(Eds.), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, pp. 391-414
Greenhall, S. (1997), Reward Starter Students Book CD1, London: Macmillan
Heinemann
Guariento, G and Morley, J. (2001) Text and task authenticity in the EFL Classroom.
ELT Journal 55/4. Oxford University Press. England
Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology. Oxford: Blackwell.
McCarthy, M. (1991). Discourse Analysis for language Teachers, Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Porter, D. and Roberts, J. (1981). Authentic Listening Activities ELT Journal Volume
36/1. Oxford University Press
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A. and Jefferson, G. (1974), A simplest systematics for the
organisation of turn-taking in conversation,
Language 50 (4), 696-735.
Tannen, D (1989), Talking Voices, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Wardhaugh, R. (2002, 4th ed.), An Introduction to Sociolinguistics, Oxford: Blackwell.