Cognition: Virginie Crollen, Marie-Pascale Noël, Xavier Seron, Pierre Mahau, Franco Lepore, Olivier Collignon
Cognition: Virginie Crollen, Marie-Pascale Noël, Xavier Seron, Pierre Mahau, Franco Lepore, Olivier Collignon
Cognition: Virginie Crollen, Marie-Pascale Noël, Xavier Seron, Pierre Mahau, Franco Lepore, Olivier Collignon
Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT
Brief article
Institut de Recherche en Sciences Psychologiques (IPSY), Centre de Neuroscience Systme et Cognition (NeuroCS), Universit Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC), Universit de Montral, Canada
c
Centre for Mind/Brain Science, University of Trento, Italy
b
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 16 September 2013
Revised 13 May 2014
Accepted 7 June 2014
Available online 2 July 2014
Keywords:
Finger-counting
Blindness
Grounded cognition
a b s t r a c t
Though a clear interaction between nger and number representations has been demonstrated, what drives the development of this intertwining remains unclear. Here we tested
early blind, late blind and sighted control participants in two counting tasks, each performed under three different conditions: a resting condition, a condition requiring hands
movements and a condition requiring feet movements. In the resting condition, every
sighted and late blind spontaneously used their ngers, while the majority of early blind
did not. Sighted controls and late blind were moreover selectively disrupted by the interfering hand condition, while the early blind who did not use the nger-counting strategy
remained unaffected by the interference conditions. These results therefore demonstrate
that visual experience plays an important role in implementing the sensori-motor habits
that drive the development of ngernumber interactions.
2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
The nger-based representation of numbers has often
been advocated as an instance of grounded cognition
(e.g., Fischer & Brugger, 2011; Wilson 2002). Since performance on nger discrimination tasks was shown to be a
good predictor of arithmetic abilities (Fayol, Barrouillet, &
Marinthe, 1998; Nol, 2005), it has indeed been argued
that ngers may be the missing tool (Andres, Di Luca, &
Pesenti, 2008) that sustains the assimilation of basic
Corresponding authors. Address: Institut de Recherche en Sciences
Psychologiques (IPSY), Centre de Neuroscience Systme et Cognition,
Universit Catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, B-1348
Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. Tel.: +32 10 47 40 89 (V. Crollen). Address:
CIMeC Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, via delle
Regole 101, Mattarello, TN, Italy. Tel.: +39 0461 282778; fax: +39 0461
883066 (O. Collignon).
E-mail addresses: virginie.crollen@uclouvain.be (V. Crollen), olivier.
collignon@unitn.it (O. Collignon).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.002
0010-0277/ 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
92
numerical processing and nger movements) relies on sensori-motor habits that are driven by vision. In this paper,
we examined the impact of hand interference on the
counting performance of blind adults. This experiment will
therefore allow us to exclude the idea that nger-counting
develops later in blind people on the basis of non-visual
cues (e.g., kinematic/proprioceptive). It will also allow us
to exclude the idea that nger-counting was present in
blind children but that it did not manifest by an explicit
motor behavior (e.g., absence of voluntary motor activity
but increased cortico-spinal activity of hand muscles;
Andres, Seron, & Olivier, 2007). If nger and number representations actually share common cognitive and/or brain
resources, a motor interference task involving the ngers
should disrupt counting abilities by adding noise in the
shared system.
In the present research, early blind (EB), late blind (LB)
and sighted control adults (SC) were tested with 2 counting tasks and 1 memory task carried out under 3 different
conditions: (1) a control resting condition; (2) a condition requiring the realization of hand movements unrelated to nger-counting; and (3) a condition requiring
the realization of feet movements. If early vision does
not shape the interaction between ngers and the symbolic representation of numbers, all participants should
spontaneously use their ngers to count and should manifest a hand interference effect (i.e., the hand interfering
condition should be more disrupting than the feet condition). In contrast, if early vision is important for the
development of the ngernumber interactions, early
blind individuals should less use their ngers and the
hand interfering condition should not be more disrupting
than the feet condition in this population. Moreover, as
participants were also involved in a working memory task
(listening span test) under the same control and sensorimotor interference conditions, our experiment allowed us
to test whether hand interference effects (Imbo,
Vandierendonck, & Fias, 2011; Michaux, Masson, Pesenti,
& Andres, 2013) would disrupt participants counting performance more than their performance in the listening
span test.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
One group of 15 sighted and two groups of blind participants (11 early and 14 late blinds) took part in the study
(see supplemental Table 1 for a detailed description of
the different groups). In terms of age, the SC did not statistically differ from the EB (p > .2) and LB (p > .1) groups.
Unlike the EB, all LB participants had experienced functional vision before sight loss. At the time of testing, the
participants in both blind groups were totally blind or
had, at the utmost, only rudimentary sensitivity for brightness differences and no patterned vision. In all cases, blindness was attributed to peripheral decits with no
additional neurological problems. Procedures were
approved by the Research Ethics Boards of the University
of Montreal. Experiments were undertaken with the
3. Results
While all SC and LB participants spontaneously used
their ngers to complete the control and foot conditions
of the enumeration and ordered series manipulation tasks,
only 4 EB did so (see supplemental videos 1 and 2). A Chisquared test demonstrated that the EB distribution into
nger-counter and non-nger counter was signicantly
different from the distribution observed in the SC and LB
groups, ps < .001. Two subgroups of EB were therefore
identied: EB who never used their ngers (EB ) and EB
93
94
Fig. 1. Results of the enumeration task (maximum score = 10). Error bars denote standard error of the mean. EB
ngers; EB+, the early blind who used their ngers; LB, the late blind and SC, the sighted controls.
Fig. 2. Results of the ordered series manipulation task. Error bars denote standard error of the mean. EB
EB+, the early blind who used their ngers; LB, the late blind and SC, the sighted controls.
4. Discussion
The study of blind individuals offers the unique opportunity to examine how visual experience shapes cognition in
the context of extreme changes in the environmental input
(Bedny & Saxe, 2012; Crollen, Dormal, Seron, Lepore, &
Collignon, 2013). Here, we studied visually deprived individuals in order to obtain new insights into the origins of
the interactions between ngers and symbolic numbers.
are the early blind who did not use their ngers;
Fig. 3. Results of the listening span test (maximum score = 27). Error bars denote standard error of the mean. EB
ngers; EB+, the early blind who used their ngers; LB, the late blind and SC, the sighted controls.
95
3
While some studies did not involve any explicit nger movements
(Tschentscher et al., 2012), many of the above mentioned research used
tasks which are spatial in nature by involving movements of ngers
(Harrington et al., 2000), pointing/grasping (Simon, Mangin, Cohen, Le
Bihan, & Dehaene, 2002), or mapping nger locations to a spatial position
(Andres et al., 2012). It is therefore difcult to strongly argue that the
parietal cortex is involved in nger representation per s.
Acknowledgments
Authors wish to thank Giulia Dormal for her help in preliminary data collection and Samuel Di Luca for his help in
editing the videos. This research and the authors were supported by the Canada Research Chair Program (FL), the
96
Dormal, G., Lepore, F., & Collignon, O. (2012). Plasticity of the dorsal
spatial stream in visually deprived individuals. Neural Plasticity,
659687.
Fayol, M., Barrouillet, P., & Marinthe, C. (1998). Predicting mathematical
achievement from neuropsychological performance: A longitudinal
study. Cognition, 68, 6370.
Fayol, M., & Seron, X. (2005). About numerical representations: Insights
from neuropsychological, experimental and developmental studies.
In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition
(pp. 322). New York: Psychology Press.
Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: Spatial
numerical associations point to nger counting as prime example of
embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology. http://dx.doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260.
Gerstmann, J. (1930). Zur symptomatologie der hirnlsionen im
bergangsgebiet
der
unteren
parietal-und
mittleren
occipitalwindung. Nervenarzt, 3, 691695.
Harrington, D. L., Rao, S. M., Haaland, K. Y., Bobholz, J. A., Mayer, A. R.,
Binderx, J. R., et al. (2000). Specialized neural systems underlying
representations of sequential movements. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 12, 5677.
Imbo, I., Vandierendonck, A., & Fias, W. (2011). Passive hand movements
disrupt adults counting strategies. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 201.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00201.
Masson, M. E. J. (2011). A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to
null-hypothesis signicance testing. Behavioral Research, 43, 679690.
Michaux, N., Masson, N., Pesenti, M., & Andres, M. (2013). Selective
interference of nger movements on basic addition and subtraction
problem solving. Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 197205.
Nol, M. P. (2005). Finger gnosia: A predictor of numerical abilities in
children? Child Neuropsychology, 11(5), 413430.
Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2013). The relation between nger
gnosis and mathematical ability: Why redeployment of neural
circuits best explains the nding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 877.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00877.
Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2008). An alternative view of the
relation between nger gnosis and math ability: Redeployment of
nger representations for the representation of numbers. In B. C. Love,
K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual
meeting of the cognitive science society, Austin, TX, July 23 26, 2008.
Cognitive Science Society, pp. 16471652.
Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning
curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus.
Neuron, 44, 547555.
Proske, U., & Gandevia, S. C. (2012). The proprioceptive senses: Their roles
in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle
force. Physiological Reviews, 92(4), 16511697.
Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. In P. V.
Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 111196). Cambridge:
Blackwell.
Simon, O., Mangin, J. F., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2002).
Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related
areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron, 33(3), 475487.
Thevenot, C., Castel, C., Danjon, J., Renaud, O., Ballaz, C., Baggioni, L., et al.
(2014). Numerical abilities in children with congenital hemiplegia:
An investigation of the role of nger use in number processing.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(2), 88100.
Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in
simple processing systems. In N. J. Castellan & F. Restle (Eds.),
Cognitive theory (pp. 199239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermller, F. (2012). You
can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modulate
motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. NeuroImage, 59,
31393148.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin
and Review, 9, 625636.
Withagen, A., Kappers, A. M., Vervloed, M. P., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L.
(2013). Short term memory and working memory in blind versus
sighted children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(7),
21612172.
Zago, L., Pesenti, M., Mellet, E., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., & TzourioMazoyer, N. (2001). Neural correlates of simple and complex mental
calculation. NeuroImage, 13, 314327.