Understanding The Psychology of Bullying
Understanding The Psychology of Bullying
Understanding The Psychology of Bullying
With growing recognition that bullying is a complex phenomenon, influenced by multiple factors, research findings
to date have been understood within a social-ecological
framework. Consistent with this model, we review research
on the known correlates and contributing factors in bullying/victimization within the individual, family, peer group,
school and community. Recognizing the fluid and dynamic
nature of involvement in bullying, we then expand on this
model and consider research on the consequences of bullying involvement, as either victim or bully or both, and
propose a social-ecological, diathesisstress model for understanding the bullying dynamic and its impact. Specifically, we frame involvement in bullying as a stressful life
event for both children who bully and those who are
victimized, serving as a catalyst for a diathesisstress
connection between bullying, victimization, and psychosocial difficulties. Against this backdrop, we suggest that
effective bullying prevention and intervention efforts must
take into account the complexities of the human experience,
addressing both individual characteristics and history of
involvement in bullying, risk and protective factors, and the
contexts in which bullying occurs, in order to promote
healthier social relationships.
Keywords: bullying,
social-ecological
victimization,
diathesisstress,
ullying is a unique but complex form of interpersonal aggression, which takes many forms, serves
different functions, and is manifested in different
patterns of relationships. Bullying is not simply a dyadic
problem between a bully and a victim, but is recognized as
a group phenomenon, occurring in a social context in
which various factors serve to promote, maintain, or suppress such behavior (e.g., Olweus, 2001; Rodkin &
Hodges, 2003; Salmivalli, 2001). Accordingly, researchers
have argued for the utility of a social-ecological framework
in understanding school bullying (Espelage, Rao, & de la
Rue, 2013; Espelage & Swearer, 2010; Hong & Garbarino,
2012; Swearer & Espelage, 2004; Swearer et al., 2012).
Social ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) conceptualizes human development as a bidirectional interaction
between individuals and the multiple systems in which they
operate home, neighborhood, school, community, and
society. Thus, bullying behavior is not just the result of
individual characteristics, but is influenced by multiple
relationships with peers, families, teachers, neighbors, and
344
interactions with societal influences (e.g., media, technology). Peer witnesses to bullying are also at risk for negative
outcomes (Rivers, Poteat, Noret, & Ashurst, 2009), even
after controlling for involvement as bullies or victim (Bonanno & Hymel, 2006).
Complicating our understanding of the consequences
of bullying and victimization is recent research documenting the dynamic and fluid nature of childrens involvement
in bullying across roles and over time. Among youth who
are involved in bullying, Ryoo, Wang, and Swearer (2014)
found that frequent victims and frequent perpetrators were
the least stable subgroups, and that students assumed different roles in bullying across school years. Indeed, youth
can observe bullying (i.e., bystanders), experience bullying
(i.e., victims), and perpetrate bullying (i.e., bullies) across
different situations and/or over time. Across contexts, for
instance, a student may be victimized by classmates at
school but bully his or her siblings at home. Longitudinal
studies by Haltigan and Vaillancourt (2014) and Barker,
Arseneault, Brendgen, Fontaine, and Maughan (2008) explored the joint trajectories of involvement in bullying and
victimization over time among 9- to 12-year-old and 11- to
16-year-olds, respectively, with similar results. Most students (73% and 75%, respectively) showed low levels of
Editors note. This article is one of six in the School Bullying and
Victimization special issue of the American Psychologist (MayJune
2015). Susan M. Swearer and Shelley Hymel provided the scholarly lead
for the special issue.
Authors note. Susan M. Swearer, Department of Educational Psychology, University of NebraskaLincoln; Shelley Hymel, Faculty of Education, Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology and Special
Education, University of British Columbia. Susan M. Swearer and Shelley
Hymel are Co-Directors of the Bullying Research Network (http://brnet.unl.edu).
The authors wish to acknowledge the support received for this work,
including support to the first author from the Andrew Gomez Dream
Foundation, the Woods Charitable Fund, and the College of Education
and Human Sciences at the University of Nebraska at Lincoln, and support
to the second author from the Edith Lando Charitable Foundation, the
University of British Columbia Faculty of Education Infrastructure Grant,
and the Canadian Prevention Science Cluster, funded through the Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Susan
M. Swearer, 40 Teachers College Hall, Department of Educational Psychology, University of NebraskaLincoln, Lincoln, NE, 68588-0345; or
Shelley Hymel, Faculty of Education, University of British Columbia,
2125 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, V6T 1Z4. E-mail: sswearer@unl.edu or
shelley.hymel@ubc.ca
ecology. In this article, we summarize some of these complexities in support of a social-ecological perspective on
bullying, and then expand our lens to propose the application of a diathesisstress model that can further our understanding of the dynamics of bullying among children and
youth.
Susan M.
Swearer
bullying and victimization over time (low/uninvolved students), and 11% (both studies) showed trajectories that
would identify them as bullies. Another 10% and 3% of
students, respectively, would be classified as victims and
2% (Barker et al. only) as bully-victims. However, 6% and
3% of students, respectively, showed a pattern of declining
victimization and increased bullying over time (victim to
bully subgroup), a trajectory that was more likely than one
in which bullies are increasingly victimized. Importantly,
these distinct patterns of involvement are associated with
different mental health outcomes.
Researchers have long demonstrated that being involved as both a perpetrator and victim seems to compound
the impact of bullying, with bully-victims experiencing
worse outcomes than either bullies or victims, being at
greater risk for anxiety, depression, low self-esteem, selfharm, suicidal ideation and suicidality, physical injury,
substance abuse, negative attitudes toward school, absenteeism, poor perceptions of school safety, aggression, and
delinquency (e.g., Berkowitz & Benbenishty, 2012; Copeland, Wolke, Angold, & Costello, 2013; Kumpulainen,
Rsnen, & Puura, 2001; Srabstein & Piazza, 2008). In
their trajectory analysis, Haltigan and Vaillancourt (2014)
further demonstrated that, relative to low-involvement students and after controlling for initial psychopathology,
stable victims showed elevated levels of depression, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and anxiety, whereas
stable bullies reported higher levels of anxiety, and those
who shifted from victimization to bullying reported more
anxiety, depression, and somatization. Such findings underscore the importance of considering a childs history of
involvement in bullying over time, and to move beyond the
dyadic bias (Espelage & Swearer, 2003) and view bullying as a dynamic experience, influenced by the social
MayJune 2015 American Psychologist
Shelley Hymel
Consequences of
Bullying/Victimization
Although it is widely understood that involvement in bullying causes problems for victims (see McDougall & Vaillancourt, 2015), children and youth who bully are also at
risk for many of the same problems. Studies addressing
MayJune 2015 American Psychologist
A Social-Ecological DiathesisStress
Model of Bullying: Applications
and Limitations
According to diathesisstress models, the development of
psychological difficulties occurs through the interaction of
an individuals biological and cognitive vulnerabilities and
stressful life experiences. Involvement in bullying is conceptualized as a stressful life event, influenced by multiple
social stressors. However, the presence of social stressors
does not fully explain the development of psychological
difficulties like depression, anxiety, and aggression. Rather,
stressful life events can be exacerbated by biological vulnerabilities and can activate cognitive vulnerabilities, leading to more significant, negative outcomes. Cognitive diathesis is conceptualized as a distorted lens through which
individuals interpret life events (Chango et al., 2012; Hammen & Rudolph, 2003). If negative events are attributed to
global, stable, and internal cognitive schemas, and negative
beliefs about self, world, and future, individuals are at
increased risk for internalizing and externalizing problems.
In one study that supports the utility of a social-ecological,
diathesisstress model of peer victimization, Bonanno and
Hymel (2010) explored why some victimized youth are
more vulnerable to suicidal ideation than others, finding
more suicidal ideation among victims who felt more soMayJune 2015 American Psychologist
REFERENCES
Bacchini, E., Esposito, G., & Affuso, G. (2009). School experience and
school bullying. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,
19, 1732. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/casp.975
Baldry, A. C. (2003). Bullying in schools and exposure to domestic
violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 713732. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0145-2134(03)00114-5
Baldry, A. C. (2004). The impact of direct and indirect bullying on the
mental and physical health of Italian youngsters. Aggressive Behavior,
30, 343355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20043
Baldry, A. C., & Farrington, D. P. (1999). Brief report: Types of bullying
among Italian school children. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 423 426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jado.1999.0234
Ball, H. A., Arseneault, L., Taylor, A., Maughan, B., Caspi, A., & Moffitt,
T. E. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on victims, bullies
and bully-victims in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 49, 104 112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007
.01821.x
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193209. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3
Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of
moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101119. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/0305724022014322
Barboza, G. E., Schiamberg, L. B., Oehmke, J., Korzeniewski, S. J., Post,
L. A., & Heraux, C. G. (2009). Individual characteristics and the
multiple contexts of adolescent bullying: An ecological perspective.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 101121. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10964-008-9271-1
Barker, E. D., Arseneault, L., Brendgen, M., Fontaine, N., & Maughan, B.
(2008). Joint development of bullying and victimization in adolescence:
Relations to delinquency and self-harm. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 1030 1038.
Bauman, S., & Del Rio, A. (2006). Preservice teachers responses to
bullying scenarios: Comparing physical, verbal, and relational bullying.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 219 231. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.219
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety disorders and
phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bender, D., & Lsel, F. (2011). Bullying at school as a predictor of
delinquency, violence and other anti-social behaviour in adulthood.
Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 21, 99 106. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/cbm.799
Benjet, C., Thompson, R. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). 5-HTTLPR moderates
the effect of relational peer victimization on depressive symptoms in
adolescent girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51,
173179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02149.x
Berkowitz, R., & Benbenishty, R. (2012). Perceptions of teachers support, safety, and absence from school because of fear among victims,
bullies, and bully-victims. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 82,
6774. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.2011.01132.x
Bernstein, A., Zvolensky, M. J., Feldner, M. T., Lewis, S. F., Fauber,
A. L., Leen-Feldner, E. W., & Vujanovic, A. A. (2005). Anxiety
sensitivity taxon and trauma: Discriminant associations for posttraumatic stress and panic symptomatology among young adults. Depression and Anxiety, 22, 138 149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20091
Bjrkqvist, K., sterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (2000). Social intelligenceEmpathy aggression? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5,
191200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00029-9
Bonanno, R., & Hymel, S. (2006, July). Exposure to school violence: The
impact of bullying on witnesses. Paper presented at the biennial meeting
of the International Society for the Study of Behavior Development,
Melbourne, Australia.
Bonanno, R., & Hymel, S. (2010). Beyond hurt feelings: Investigating
why some victims of bullying are at greater risk for suicidal ideation.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 56, 420 440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/mpq
.0.0051
Bowes, L., Arseneault, L., Maughan, B., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt,
T. E. (2009). School, neighborhood, and family factors are associated
with childrens bullying involvement: A nationally representative
longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
350
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Alto, G. (2007). Does empathy
predict adolescents bullying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467 476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20204
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Alto, G. (2008). Determinants of
adolescents active defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 93105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.adolescence.2007.05.002
Gini, G., & Pozzoli, T. (2006). The role of masculinity in childrens
bullying. Sex Roles, 54, 585588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199006-9015-1
Gini, G., & Pozzoli, T. (2013). Bullied children and psychosomatic
problems: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 132, 720 729. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1542/peds.2013-0614
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hymel, S. (2014). Moral disengagement among
children and youth: A meta-analytic review of links to aggressive
behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 56 68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
ab.21502
Graham, S., Bellmore, A., & Juvonen, J. (2007). Peer victimization in
middle school: When self- and peer views diverge. In J. E. Zins, M. J.
Elias, & C. A. Maher (Eds.), Bullying, victimization, and peer harassment: A handbook of prevention and intervention (pp. 121141). New
York, NY: Haworth Press.
Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (1998a). A social-cognitive perspective on peer
aggression and victimization. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development (pp. 2370). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley.
Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (1998b). Self-blame and peer victimization in
middle school: An attributional analysis. Developmental Psychology,
34, 587599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.3.587
Haltigan, J. D., & Vaillancourt, T. (2014). Joint trajectories of bullying
and peer victimization across elementary and middle school and associations with symptoms of psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2426 2436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038030
Hammen, C. (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar
depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 555561. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.555
Hammen, C., & Rudolph, K. D. (2003). Childhood mood disorders. In
E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (2nd ed., pp.
233278). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2000). Predictors of peer victimization
among urban youth. Social Development, 9, 521543. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/1467-9507.00141
Hazler, R. J. (1996). Bystanders: An overlooked factor in peer on peer
abuse. Journal for the Professional Counselor, 11, 1121.
Hong, J. S., & Garbarino, J. (2012). Risk and protective factors for
homophobic bullying in schools: An application of the social-ecological framework. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 271285. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9194-y
Hymel, S., & Bonanno, R. (2014). Moral disengagement processes in
bullying. Theory into Practice, 53, 278 285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
00405841.2014.947219
Hymel, S., Rocke Henderson, N., & Bonanno, R. A. (2005). Moral
disengagement: A framework for understanding bullying among adolescents. Journal of Social Sciences, 8, 111.
Hymel, S., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Bonanno, R. A., Vaillancourt, T., &
Rocke Henderson, N. (2010). Bullying and morality: Understanding
how good kids can behave badly. In S. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, &
D. L. Espelage (Eds.), The handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective (pp. 101118). New York, NY: Routledge.
Iyer, P. A., Dougall, A. L., & Jensen-Campbell, L. A. (2013). Are some
adolescents differentially susceptible to the influence of bullying on
depression? Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 272281. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.02.004
Janssen, I., Boyce, W. F., & Pickett, W. (2012). Screen time and physical
violence in 10- to 16-year-old Canadian youth. International Journal
of Public Health, 57, 325331. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00038-0100221-9
Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2000). Peer harassment, psychological adjustment, and school functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 349 359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-0663.92.2.349
Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpel, M., Marttunen, M., Rimpel, A., & Rantanen, P. (1999). Bullying, depression, and suicidal ideation in Finnish
351
352
Salmivalli, C., Krn, A., & Poskiparta, E. (2010). From peer putdowns to
peer support: A theoretical model and how it translated into a national
anti-bullying program. In S. Jimerson, S. Swearer, & D. Espelage
(Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective
(pp. 441 454). New York, NY: Routledge.
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and
their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behavior,
22, 115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:11::
AID-AB13.0.CO;2-T
Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2004). Connections between attitudes,
group norms, and behaviour in bullying situations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 246 258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
01650250344000488
Schmidt, L. A., Polak, C. P., & Spooner, A. L. (2001). Biological and
environmental contributions to childhood shyness: A diathesisstress
model. In W. Crozier & L. E. Alden (Eds.), International handbook of
social anxiety: Concepts, research and interventions relating to the self
and shyness (pp. 29 51). New York, NY: Wiley.
Slee, P. T., & Rigby, K. (1993). Australian school childrens self appraisal
of interpersonal relations: The bullying experience. Child Psychiatry
and Human Development, 23, 273282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
BF00707680
Smith, P. K., Talamelli, L., Cowie, H., Naylor, P., & Chauhan, P. (2004).
Profiles of non-victims, escaped victims, continuing victims and new
victims of school bullying. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
74, 565581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/0007099042376427
Sokol, N., Bussey, K., & Rapee, R. (2014). The effect of victims responses to overt bullying on peer bystander reactions. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Sontag, L. M., & Graber, J. A. (2010). Coping with perceived peer stress:
Gender-specific and common pathways to symptoms of psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 46, 16051620. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0020617
Spriggs, A. L., Iannotti, R. J., Nansel, T. R., & Haynie, D. L. (2007).
Adolescent bullying involvement and perceived family, peer and school
relations: Commonalities and differences across race/ethnicity. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 41, 283293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth
.2007.04.009
Srabstein, J., & Piazza, T. (2008). Public health, safety and educational
risks associated with bullying behaviors in American adolescents. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 20, 223233.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/IJAMH.2008.20.2.223
Stark, K. D., Napolitano, S., Swearer, S., Schmidt, K., Jaramillo, D., &
Hoyle, J. (1996). Issues in the treatment of depressed children. Applied
& Preventive Psychology, 5, 59 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S09621849(96)80001-1
Sugden, K., Arseneault, L., Harrington, H., Moffitt, T. E., Williams, B., &
Caspi, A. (2010). Serotonin transporter gene moderates the development of emotional problems among children following bullying victimization. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
49, 830 840. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.01.024
Sutton, J., & Keogh, E. (2001). Components of Machiavellian beliefs in
children Relationships with personality. Personality and Individual
Differences, 30, 137148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)
00017-9
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and theory
of mind: A critique of the social skills deficit view of anti-social
behavior. Social Development, 8, 117127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
1467-9507.00083
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition and
bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of
353