Bouchard, Familial Studies of Intelligence A Review
Bouchard, Familial Studies of Intelligence A Review
Bouchard, Familial Studies of Intelligence A Review
discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/16004169
CITATIONS
DOWNLOADS
VIEWS
513
364
361
2 AUTHORS:
Thomas J. Bouchard
Matt Mcgue
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE
Reprint Series
29 May 1981, Volume 212, pp. 1055-1059
SCIENCE
displ.ays the correlations between relatives, biological and adoptive, in the 111
studies. The median correlation in each
distribution is indicated by a vertical bar.
The small arrow indicates the correlation
that would be predicted by a genetic
model with no dominance, no assortative
mating, and no environmental effects.
Researchers do not subscribe to such a
simple model, but it pro'fides a noncontroversial pattern against which to compare the results of various familial groupings. Different investigators will undoubtedly fit different models to the
data.
In general, the pattern of average correlations in Fig. 1 is consistent with the
pattern of correlations predicted on the
basis of polygenic 'inheritance. That is,
the higher the proportion of genes two
family members have in common the
higher the average correlation between
their IQ's.
The data set contains considerable
heterogeneity, as indicated by the x2
statistics. In an attempt to identify the
factors contributing to the heterogeneity,
we subdivided the familial groupings into
opposite-sex and same-sex pairings (Fig.
2) and male and female pairings (Fig. 3).
Among dizygotic twins the IQ's of samesex twins are more similar than those of
opposite-sex twins. This may reflect a
social-environmental effect (parents may
treat same-sex twins more similarly than
opposite-sex twins). The difference between non twin same-sex and oppositesex siblings and between same-sex and
opposite-sex parent-offspring pairings is
trivial. The male-female comparison
does not yield consistent trends. For
example, the average correlations are
larger in male twins than in female twins,
but the reverse is true for other siblings.
The absence of any demonstrable sex
effect is consistent with a polygenic theory of inheritance that does not posit the
existence of sex linkage. Environmental
theories that emphasize the importance
of sex-role effects on general cognitive
development are not supported by these
results (13).
Another possible source of heterogeneity is the intelligence test used. There
are many tests that purport to measure
intelligence, and they may not be highly
interrelated. We found great diversity in
test selection. For example, the 34 corre-
0036-8075/8110529-1055$01.00/0
1055
lations for monozygotic twins reared together were based upon results from 22
different tests, the 41 dizygotic twin correlations upon results from 25. We do
not have sufficient data to determine
whether the magnitude of the familial
correlation is moderated by the specific
test used. We did investigate whether
individually administered tests and
group-administered tests produced different correlations. For the monozygotic
twins reared together the 24 correlations
calculated on group tests produced a
weighted average of .86, and the 10 calculated on individual tests a weighted
average of .84. For the dizygotic twins
reared together, the weighted average of
32 correlations based on group tests is
.60 and of 9 correlations based on individual tests is .61. In neither case did the
distinction between group and individual
test produce an appreciable effect.
The 34 correlations reported on 4672
monozygotic twin pairs reared together
produce a weighted average correlation
of .86. This value is very close to those
reported in earlier reviews and is approximately the same for male and female
pairs. Although the test of homogeneity
yields a significant X2 value (P < .02),
the degree of heterogeneity is not ex-
0.0
0.10
0.20
0.40
0.30
0.50
0.60
MONOZYGOTIC TWINS
REAREO TOGETHER
0.70
0.80
..
0.90
.. "!!l:'!.:
MIDPARENT-MIDOFFSPRING
REAREO TOGETHER
L
)
MIOPARENT-OFFSPRING
REAREO TOGETHER
1.00
.
.
.. . 'f
. .
...:l
.1
.l.. 1m . . ..
. :i
ND.
WEIGHT
DF
MEDIAN
EO
ND.
CORREl
OF
CDRREl AVER
ATIONS PAIRINGS ATION
AGE
34
3
4672
65
.85
.67
2.46
.72
0.92
(Z)
0.46
1.33
410
.73
.72
Z.66
(2)
992
.475
.50
8.11
(7)
1.16
2.36
6.31
41
5546
.58
.60
94.5
(40)
69
26,473
.45
.47
403.6
(64)
203
.24
.24
32
8433
.385
.42
211.0
(31)
6.81
.22
9.61
(3)
3.20
1.55
II '1..11
.1.
I
SINGLE PARENT-OFFSPRING
REAREO TOGETHER
....
SINGLE PARENT-OFFSPRING
REARED APART
I.
ill'"
.. ..... .. . .
I
I
HAlFSI8LiNGS
COUSINS
"I' .
..
II
..
ADOPTING PARENT-OFFSPRING
.
0.0
0.10
I
I
..
0.20
" 'j
0.30
..
0.40
..
..
0.50
0.60
0.70
0.90
.02
200
.35
.31
1,176
.145
.15
1.0Z
(2)
0.51
0.48
345
.29
.29
1.93
(4)
369
.31
.34
10.5
(5)
2.10
758
.19
.24
6.8
(5)
1.36
.19
6.64
(5)
1.33
.33
96.1
(15)
6.41
16
0.80
.oz
(1)
1.55
(1)
6
I
.. :
.22
814
I
I
..
...
.
ASSORTATIVE MATING
d.f.
.86
I.I!. '1
x2 .;.
81.29
(33)
.1
DIZYGOTIC TWINS
REARED TOGETHER
x2 (d.f.)
1397
3817
.18
.365
1.00
Fig. 1. Familial correlations for IQ. The vertical bar in each distribution indicates the median correlation; the arrow, the correlation predicted by a
simple polygenic model.
1056
0.10
0.0
0,20
0.30
-'--
.~
.: .. L:
. ..
0,10
O,D
.:
I
1
0,20
F~milial
0,30
0.50
0.40
0.60
0.70
.57
19
6098
.45
,48
58,6
3,45
(17)
84.65
4,98
(17)
16
5127
.445
,49
70,4
(14)
14
4648
.41
.40
55.4
(13)
4.26
12
4476
,40
.39
70.1
(11)
6,37
460
,18
,IB
461
,12
,12
0.10
0.20
0.3D
0.40
0,6D
0.50
0,70
..
..
.: ...
..
FATHER-DFFSPR1NG REARED
TDGETHER
FATHER-SDN REARED
TDGETHER
ADOPTING MOTHER-OFFSPRING
. . . 1. .:
II
869
,835
.86
26,35
(8)
3.29
12
1,013
.86
.B6
24.4
110)
2.44
10
730
.58
.61
14.73
(8)
1.84
.65
20.32
(9)
2,26
3.15
.64
11
1,986
.42
.50
12
2,321
.38
.47
54.69
1101
5.47
,41
119.02
1241
4.96
,43
36.80
191
4.09
3.50
5,660
1,804
.38
.44
12
2,802
,37
.39
38,52
(11)
22
5,497
.43
,41
141.17
(21)
6.72
,46
,39
33.08
(9)
3,68
,38
36.86
(13)
2.B4
,195
,20
4.28
(5)
0.86
10.1
(5)
2.02
10
964
28.34
(9)
25
::
j .
FATHER-DAUGHTER REARED
TDGETHER
X2 "
dJ.
10
10
.1
1
. .. .
WEIGHT
NO.
ED
OF
NO.
MEDIAN
CDRREl
DF
CDRREL AVER
ATIDNS PAIRINGS ATION
AGE
x 2 Id.f.)
11
.. " . . .
.: I
.. ..
MDTHER-SDN REARED
TDGETHER
..
MOTHER-DAUGHTER REARED
TDGETHER
1.00
I
.1
MDTHER-OFFSPRING REARED
TDGETHER
"j,
I
.!
1
:I~
. ..
I
I
...
.1
.:
0.90
0.80
14
1,658
2,843
.40
1,393
212
.10
.10
247
.22
.22
1,279
.155
.18
214
,00
.00
248
,25
,25
I
1
. I.
,
j
I
D.l0
0,20
0.30
0.40
0.50
0.60
0.70
0,80
0,9D
1.00
5,03
1.00
0,90
0.80
...
0,0
,565
2.43
ADDPTlNG FATHER-SDN
1592
18
68.14
(28)
Fig. 2.
ADDPTlNG FATHER-OFFSPRING
,62
X2 _
d.f.
0,0
,61
3,610
x2 IdJ .)
ADDPTlNG MDTHER-SDN
29
.:
"
..
WEIGHT
NO.
EO
OF
NO.
MEOIAN
CORREL
OF
CORREl AVER
ATiONS PAIRINGS AT10N
AGE
ADOPTING FATHER-DAUGHTER
1.00
..:
"
ADDPTlNG MOTHER-DAUGHTER
0.90
0.80
... .:.I.
0,70
..... :.In.i.
PARENT-OFFSPRING PAIRINGS
0,60
0.50
0.40
1057
offspring is .42 based upon 32 correlations. There is a marked degree of heterogeneity in the distribution, as evidenced not only by a significant X2 value
(P < .01), but also by the broad range of
the correlations. Their extreme heterogeneity cannot be attributed to a sex
effect, inasmuch as opposite-sex and
same-sex pairings yield equivalent averages, or to a maternal effect, the average
correlation of mother and offspring being
the same as that of father and offspring.
Although the large discrepancies between expected and observed correlations for parent and offspring reared in
the parental home (' 'reared together")
may be easily interpreted as a result of a
generational (social-environmental) effect, one should not hastily discount the
possibility of biological factors. Characteristics that are affected very little by
the social environment, such as height
and total fingerprint ridge count, show
similar generational differences. For example, in one large study of height Wingerd et al. (21) found a midparent-offspring correlation of .51. Two large studies of total fingerprint ridge count (22)
yield a Z-weighted mean midparent-offspring correlation of .63. The single parent-offspring correlations for height and
total fingerprint ridge count are .42 and
.42 (21-23).
As with parent and offspring reared
together, correlations for parent and separated offspring are quite heterogeneous.
The weighted average is .22, much less
than the simple expectation of .50. As
suggested by McAskie and Clarke (24),
one possible explanation could be that
parents and offspring are not given the
same test. In fact, roughly 50 percent of
our intergenerational correlations were
based upon data from cases in which
parents were given different tests from
those given their offspring. The lowerthan-expected morphological correlations found in these studies, however,
suggest that scaling may not be the only
problem.
Two familial pairings that are rarely
studied are half-siblings and cousins.
Two half-sibling correlations, both reported by Nichols (16), produce a
weighted average of .31. The four reported correlations for cousins are quite homogeneous; their average, .15, closely
approximates the simple genetic expectation.
A number of recent adoption studies
have added considerable knowledge.
Enough studies are available to permit
comparison of two sets of nonbiological
sibling
pairs-adopted/natural
and
adopted/adopted. Other things being
1058
9.
10.
II.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
1059