ISPS2009 Proceedings PDF
ISPS2009 Proceedings PDF
ISPS2009 Proceedings PDF
Proceedings of
ISPS 2009
002 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 003
Proceedings of the
International Symposium on
Performance Science 2009
Edited by
AARON WILLIAMON
Royal College of Music, London
SHARMAN PRETTY
The University of Melbourne
and
RALPH BUCK
The University of Auckland
004 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or
transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of the publisher, or
as expressly permitted by law, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics rights
organization. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to
Aaron Williamon, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort Road,
London SW7 2BS, United Kingdom.
Disclaimer. Statements of fact and opinion in the articles in the Proceedings of ISPS 2009 are those
of the respective authors and contributors and not of the editors or the AEC. Neither the AEC nor the
editors make any representation, express or implied, in respect of the accuracy of the material in this
volume and cannot accept any legal responsibility or liability for any errors or omissions that may be
made. The reader should make her or his own evaluation as to the appropriateness or otherwise of
any experimental technique described.
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4
Aaron Williamon
Sharman Pretty
Ralph Buck
006 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 007
Contents
Keynote paper
Performance, science, and society page 5
Robert Winston
Keynote paper
Discovering deliberate practice activities that overcome
plateaus and limits on improvement of performance page 11
K. Anders Ericsson
Symposium
Physical and psychological vulnerabilities
in music and dance students page 23
Thematic sessions
The vocabulary of performance page 43
Performance analysis page 63
Symposium
Performance science: Implications for
educational and professional practice page 83
Thematic sessions
Expression and interpretation I page 105
Expertise development page 125
Expression and interpretation II page 145
Memory and performance page 165
Graduate award paper
Fast feedforward error-detection mechanisms in
highly skilled performance page 187
Mara Herrojo Ruiz
008 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Keynote paper
A balanced approach to excellence: Life skill
intervention and elite performance page 203
Deidre Anderson
Poster session page 215
Symposium
Musicians dystonia: New aspects in
pathophysiology and treatment page 373
Thematic sessions
Understanding performance page 401
The perception of technique page 421
Emotion in performance page 437
Workshops page 457
Symposium
Student musicians motivation, learning, and performance page 467
Thematic sessions
Performers health page 493
Performance and life factors page 513
Physicality of performance page 529
Defining performance page 545
Psychology of performance page 565
Perceiving performance page 581
Keynote paper
The dominant artistic discourse as a health determinant page 599
Sylvie Fortin
Scientific committee
Robert Winston
Robert Winston, Imperial College London, Faculty Building, South Kensington Campus,
London SW7 2AZ, UK; Email: r.winston@imperial.ac.uk
Wednesday
16 December 2009
Keynote paper
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
K. Anders Ericsson
With more experience in the associative phase (grey segment in Figure 1),
large mistakes become increasingly rare, performance appears smoother, and
learners no longer need to concentrate as hard to perform the task. After a
limited period of training and experiencefrequently less than 50 hours for
most recreational activitiesan acceptable standard of performance is at-
tained. Once performance is autonomous (white segment in Figure 1), indi-
viduals no longer attempt further modifications and improvements, and this
typically leads to a stable plateau of performance, which is consistent with
Galtons (1869/1979) description. Galton, however, would argue that these
stable plateaus exist not only for the acquisition of acceptable performance in
casual, recreational activities, but also for the acquisition of exceptional abil-
ity in most domains.
This view of acquisition of skill is consistent with findings for amateurs
performance in tennis and golf, where they improve initially but then stay at a
stable of performance level for decades of regular engagement. Performance
in everyday skills, such as driving and typing (Keith and Ericsson 2007), also
are remarkably stable across decades of regular activity. Finally, professional
performance does not improve with experience beyond the first few years of
initial experience (Ericsson 2004, Ericsson and Lehmann 1996, Ericsson et
al. 2007). In fact, sometimes job performance decreases as function of job
experience since graduation (Choudhry et al. 2005).
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
performance. In everyday life, beginners are searching for quick fixes that
allow them to rapidly reach an acceptable performance. In contrast, teachers
are focused on future performance and on the student acquiring the appro-
priate fundamentals, so that the student can master more complex techniques
and reach higher levels of performance. In many domains, such as music,
ballet, and gymnastics, teachers over time have developed a curriculum where
there is consensus on the best ordering of techniques to be mastered in order
to reach the highest levels of performance (Bloom 1985). For many domains
of expertise, such as scientific research, writing, and art, there is currently
less consensus on the preferred curriculum and even if a curriculum would
benefit the development of creative performance.
Deliberate practice should be scheduled when the performers are rested
and maximally alert, and the duration of training must be adapted so they
avoid fatigue and thus are able to maintain their highest level of concentra-
tion and performance during the entire training session.
Deliberate practice requires a close connection between the actual per-
formance and the training tasks. It is essential that some aspect of current
performance is taken as the focus and that training tasks are intermittently
exchanged for the real-world context so that transfer of improvements during
training is successfully incorporated in the corresponding real-world per-
formance. It is possible that the training tasks lead to performance improve-
ments that depend on the crutches and the scaffolding during training and
thus cannot be connected to the aspects of the real-world performance that
were the original stimuli for the design of the practice activities.
In the rest of the paper I will discuss how these ideas have allowed re-
searchers to identify the deliberate practice activities that have a high corre-
lation with attained performance in numerous domains (Ericsson 2006).
If you were a chess player in a chess club in the 1950s and were able to beat
all the other players, could you, and if so how would you be able to improve
your performance? At that time there were no chess computers that played
chess at a world class level, and it was not that common that people travelled
to tournaments to play the best players in the state, country, or around the
world. Based on some informal interviews and analyses, Ericsson et al. (1993)
proposed that it was essential for effective learning that one encountered
challenging situations, where the probability of making mistakes and failure
was relatively high. We proposed that one method for doing this in chess
would be to buy books and chess magazines with published games between
16 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
international level and world-class chess players. The aspiring chess expert
would then simulate playing against these players by analyzing each position
in the chess game and by trying to find the best move for every position in the
game. If the chess player selected a move that matched the one picked by
chess master then the chess player did as well as the master. More impor-
tantly, if the chess players picked a different move from the master then there
is an opportunity for learning and improvement. Through further analysis,
the chess player could attempt to figure out why the chess masters move was
superior. The next step would be to think through how the chess players
processes involving search and move selection should be changed to be able
to find this move as well as similar moves in other chess positions. A few as-
piring chess experts reported spending 3-5 hours every day engaging in this
type of solitary analysis along with studies of variants of chess openings. Sub-
sequent studies have found that serious chess players spend as much as four
hours every day engaged in this type of solitary study (Charness et al. 2005).
Most important, these studies show that the accumulated hours of solitary
analysis of chess playing is the best predictor of someones chess skill. Some-
what surprisingly the amount chess playing with friends is not associated
with increased chess-playing skill. Furthermore, these studies show that the
size of someones chess library, that is the number of chess books and chess
magazines, is correlated with better chess skillperhaps because they were
necessary for solitary chess study before the emergence of chess playing com-
puter programs and internet data bases. By spending additional time analyz-
ing the consequences of moves for a chess position, players can increase the
quality of their selections of moves. With more study, individuals refine their
representations and can access or generate the same information faster. As a
result, chess masters can typically recognize a superior move virtually imme-
diately, whereas a competent club player requires much longer to find the
same move by successive planning and evaluation rather than recognition (de
Groot 1966). With additional time the master can often generate even better
moves.
In the classical model of skill acquisition (Fitts and Posner 1967), more
experience allows the person to generate the same move faster through auto-
mation. In contrast, the nature of the improvement in chess concerns the
ability to generate different and better moves based on refined acquired rep-
resentations and associated analysis and search (de Groot 1946/1978). The
same type of improvement, based on deliberate practice and the acquisition
of complex representations supporting planning, evaluation, and online
monitoring of performance (cf. Ericsson and Kintsch 1995) can explain grad-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 17
Most adults are able to type, yet there are often large individual differences in
their style and efficiency. Adults typically do not think about their typing and
simply do it, thus typing would seem to meet the criterion of low effort or
even autonomous performance. A recent review (Keith and Ericsson 2007)
has found that estimates of how much someone has been typing appear to be
a poor predictor of measurable typing speed under standardized conditions.
How could someone improve their typing performance after having typed at a
similar speed for decades? The answer is clearly supported by a number of
training studies. The key to improved speed in typing is to find some time
during the day (Dvorak et al. 1936) when one is able to fully concentrate for
15-25 minutes. During that time, one finds something to copy and increases
ones comfortable typing speed to a speed 15-30% faster than normal. During
this faster speed it becomes apparent that some key strokes or key-stroke
combinations are slower and associated with hesitations. One then works on
mastering these weaknesses by focused practice and then by interweaving
them into speeded typing. In an interactive fashion, we speed up ones typing
to find the problems causing slower typing and then practice until the speed
of typing has increased and then repeat the process. The general approach of
this type of deliberate practice is to find methods to push performance be-
yond its normal speedeven if that performance can be maintained only for a
short time. This methodology offers the potential for identifying and correct-
ing weaker components that will improve performance.
How is it possible to improve the speed of habitual and effortless behav-
ior? Researchers have studied the individual differences in typing speed of
skilled typists and unskilled participants by having them type passages from a
collection of unfamiliar texts as fast as they can without making errors. High-
speed films of finger movements show that the faster typists start moving
their fingers toward their desired locations on the keyboard well before the
keys are struck. The superior typists speed advantage is linked to their per-
ceptual processing of the text beyond the word that they are currently typing
(Salthouse 1984). By looking ahead in the text to identify letters to be typed,
they can prepare future keystrokes in advance. This evidence for anticipation
has been confirmed by experimental studies where expert typists have been
restricted from looking ahead. Under such conditions their typing speed is
dramatically reduced and approaches the speed of less skilled typists.
18 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
and me, and most importantly SF, that he could still improve his perform-
ance. SF later reached a digit span of 82 digits.
For the last few decades, I have been searching for documented instances
where motivated healthy individuals have reached unmodifiable plateaus that
constrain their performance in a given domain of expertise. So far I have
mostly encountered people who gave up their efforts because they did not
think that they could reach the performance of other people, rather than that
they had reached their own limits.
IMPLICATIONS
For a long time, the belief that individuals innate capacity limits their attain-
able level of performance has been accepted based on indirect and weak evi-
dence. In contrast, the theoretical framework of deliberate practice asserts
that improvement in performance of aspiring experts does not result from
automation due to further experience. By increasing the challenge of training,
individuals can remain in the cognitive phase (see the upper curve in Figure
1) and keep engaging in deliberate practice to acquire and refine complex
cognitive mechanisms that mediate how the brain and nervous system control
performance.
The time has come to seek out detailed information on performance pla-
teaus encountered by individuals motivated to improve. Scientists should
document their existence and examine their structure with experiments and
analyze the past training and current performance. This evidence on per-
formance limits will allow us to evaluate different theories of the determi-
nants of expert level performance, as well as motivate the development of new
training curricula and associated deliberate practice activities in order to as-
sess whether these plateaus reflect unmodifiable limits to the development of
performance.
Acknowledgments
I want to thank Len Hill for comments on an earlier draft of this paper.
References
Ericsson K. A., Krampe R. T., and Tesch-Rmer C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice
in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, pp. 363-406.
Ericsson K. A. and Lehmann A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evi-
dence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology,
47, pp. 273-305.
Ericsson K. A., Whyte J., and Ward P. (2007). Expert performance in nursing: Review-
ing research on expertise in nursing within the framework of the expert-perform-
ance approach. Advances in Nursing Science, 30, pp. E58-E71.
Fitts P. and Posner M. I. (1967). Human Performance. Belmont, California, USA:
Brooks/Cole.
Galton F. Sir (1869/1979). Hereditary Genius. London: Julian Friedman Publishers.
(Original work published in 1869.)
Keith N. and Ericsson K. A. (2007). A deliberate practice account of typing proficiency
in everyday typists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, pp. 135-145.
Salthouse T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: General, 113, pp. 345-371.
Williams A. M., Ward P., Knowles J. M., and Smeeton N. J. (2002). Anticipation skill in
a real-world task: Measurement, training, and transfer in tennis. Journal of Ex-
perimental Psychology: Applied, 8, pp. 259-270.
Symposium:
Physical and psychological vulnerabilities
in music and dance students
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Musicians and dancers (secondary, tertiary, and professional) are at high risk
of strain and injury in the execution of their art (Barton et al. 2008). Ap-
proximately half of professional musicians and music students experience
significant symptoms (Norris 1993, Zaza 1998) with a point prevalence of
performance related musculoskeletal disorders (PRMD) ranging from 39% to
87% in adult musicians and from 34% and 47% in secondary students (Zaza
1998). Musculoskeletal injury is also the most frequent medical problem
among classical and modern dancers (McBryde et al. 2007), with 97% of all
dancers surveyed sustaining injuries over an eight month period (Ostwald et
al. 1994). Overuse injuries account for 60-76% of all dance injuries (Bronner
et al. 2003). Potential risk factors associated with developing PRMD in musi-
cians include gender (females at greater risk; Barton et al. 2008), years of
playing, instrument played (string and keyboard players at greater risk;
Bruno et al. 2008), playing-related physical (long hours, over-practicing) and
psychological stressors (self-pressure/academic), lack of preventative well-
ness behaviors (taking breaks), and previous trauma (Wu 2007). This study
reports the results of a PRMD questionnaire to provide information regarding
the point prevalence of PRMD and pain experienced by tertiary level per-
forming arts students in music and dance as a basis for subsequent targeted
preventive action.
METHOD
Participants
The sample comprised 151 students, aged from 17-50 years, with a mean age
of 21.44 years (SD=5.9); 109 (72%) music and 42 (28%) dance students54
(36%) males and 97 (64%) femalesattending the National Institute of Crea-
tive Arts and Industries, University of Auckland, in March 2009. These num-
bers represented 48% (music) and 79.2% (dance) of the total cohort. Students
completed a comprehensive questionnaire at the commencement of the aca-
demic year.
Materials
Body Mass Index (BMI): BMI is a measure of body fat based on height and
weight that applies to adult men and women. People may be categorized into
four weight groups based on their BMI as follows: underweight: <18.5, nor-
mal weight: 18.5-24.9, overweight: 25.0-29.9, obese: 30.0. Students were
assessed for BMI to ascertain whether there were any differences in PRMD
and pain according to weight.
Procedure
Ethics approval for the study was granted by the University of Auckland Hu-
man Participants Ethics Committee. The study was introduced to students in
the first lectures of the semester through brief presentations to class groups
by the lecturers and researchers. Participation information sheets were dis-
tributed to all eligible students, and those who were interested were invited to
attend classes set aside for the completion of the surveys.
RESULTS
Demographics: There were 151 students [109 (72%) music and 42 (28%)
dance students; 54 (36%) males and 97 (64%) females; mean age=21.44 years
(SD=5.9), range 17-50 years]; 52 students (34%) were in their first year, 54
(36%) in second year, 27 (18%) in third year, 1 in fourth year (0.7%), and 17
(11%) were postgraduate students; piano students (24%), guitar (13%), violin
(9%), saxophone (9%), cello (6%), drums (6%), trumpet (6%), viola (6%),
flute (5%), and clarinet (5%). The remainder was studying double bass,
French horn, trombone, tuba, and bassoon. The majority of dance students
were studying contemporary (38%) or hip hop (29%), then jazz (14%), classi-
cal (7%), cultural (7%), tap (2%), and ballroom (2%). Of the total group, 63
(40%) reported having received a medical diagnosis for a condition related to
playing their instrument or performing their dance style. Of the 109 music
students, 37 (33%) reported one or more diagnoses from medical practitio-
ners that comprised mainly repetitive sprains and strains (tennis elbow, car-
28 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 1. PRMD and pain factors and descriptive and F-statistics by group.
Table 2. PRMD and pain factors and descriptive and F-statistics by gender.
pain, 11.3% of whom reported suffering daily or almost daily pain. Fifty-three
percent (n=40) said that their current pain was caused by playing their in-
strument/dancing. Seventy-two percent (72%; n=54) indicated that their
current pain had a negative impact on their ability to play their instru-
ment/dance.
Subgroup analyses: Subgroup analyses were conducted to ascertain
whether PRMD and pain patterns were related to group (music or dance),
gender, or body mass index (BMI<18.50 or >18.50). Dance students recorded
higher mean ratings than music students for the most severe pain ever ex-
perienced (see Table 1). With the exception of current pain severity, females
reported higher mean ratings on all the other PRMD and pain factors (see
Table 2). Fifteen percent (n=22) of students were underweight, 70% (n=104)
were of normal weight, 12% (n=18) were overweight, and 3% (n=5) were
obese. Chi square analysis indicated that there were no differences in the
proportions of underweight, overweight, or obese students by group or sex,
and no relationship between BMI and any PRMD or pain measure.
30 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
DISCUSSION
Dianna Kenny, University of Sydney, PO Box 170, Lidcombe, New South Wales 1825,
Australia; Email: d.kenny@usyd.edu.au
References
Barton R., Killian C., Bushee M. et al. (2008). Occupational performance issues and
predictors of dysfunction in college instrumentalists. Medical Problems of Per-
forming Artists, 23, pp. 72-78.
Bronner S., Ojofeitimi, S., and Rose D. (2003). Injuries in a modern dance company.
American Journal of Sports Medicine, 31, pp. 365-373.
Bruno S., Lorusso A., and LAbbate N. (2008). Playing-related disabling musculoskele-
tal disorders in young and adult classical piano students. International Archives of
Occupational and Environmental Health, 81, pp. 855-860.
McBryde A. M., Rodriguez R. F., and Dugas J. R. (2007). Dance injuries. Sports Medi-
cine Update, Sept/Oct, pp. 2-6.
Norris R. (1993). The Musicians Survival Manual. St. Louis, USA: MMB Music.
Wynn Parry C. B. (2004). Managing the physical demands of musical performance. In
A. Williamon (ed.), Musical Excellence (pp. 41-60). Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.
Ostwald P. F., Baron B. C., Byi N. M., and Wilson F. R. (1994). Performing arts medi-
cine. Western Journal of Medicine, 160, pp. 48-52.
Wu S. J. (2007). Occupational risk factors for musculoskeletal disorders in musicians: A
systematic review. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 22, pp. 43-51.
Zaza C. (1998) Playing-related musculoskeletal disorders in musicians: A systematic
review of incidence and prevalence. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 158,
pp. 1019-1025.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Tertiary students in music and dance practice for many hours per day over
many years, often commencing in early childhood, to perfect their art, with
the hope that the years of practice will gain them a place in a leading orches-
tra or dance company. To achieve this goal, dancers and musicians place their
bodies under tremendous stress on a regular basis, with pain and injury often
being considered an occupational hazard (Brandfonbrener 2003, Bruno et al.
2008, Garrick and Lewis 2001, Hoppmann 2001, Hoppmann and Reid 1995,
Kelman 2000). However, despite the many physical demands involved in
practicing music and dance, little attention has been paid to the impact of
practice on the musculoskeletal system. Systematic reviews of literature
(Bragge et al. 2006, Wu 2007) have identified practice as a risk factor, citing
long hours and over-practicing as contributors to injury, in particular the
development of overuse syndromes. Hoppman and Patrone (1989) showed
that longer daily practice time was significantly related to development of
musculoskeletal problems, and Zetterberg et al. (1998) found that practice
hours were related to problems in the neck and upper extremity of musicians.
Bejjani et al. (1996) observed that overuse syndrome occurs in up to 50%
professional symphony orchestra musicians and 21% music students. Morse
et al. (2000) found that the relationship of hours played to pain is complex,
and may well be affected by risks from non-music jobs, which are very com-
mon among amateur musicians (p. 85). Musculoskeletal injury is the most
frequent medical problem among classical and modern dancers (Milan 1994,
Ostwald et al. 1994, McBryde et al. 2007). Ostwald et al. (1994) revealed that
over an eight month period, 97% of all dancers surveyed had sustained inju-
ries. Overuse injuries account for the majority (60-76%) of all dance injuries
and these are most likely to occur when fatigued or overworked (Bronner et
al. 2003). However, there are no studies assessing the relationship between
musculoskeletal disorders and practice in dance. This study assessed whether
amount of daily practice and practice and rehearsal routines of tertiary music
and dance students were related to frequency, severity, and duration of re-
ported PRMDs.
METHOD
Participants
The sample comprised music and dance students in all years who were at-
tending the National Institute of Creative Arts and Industries, University of
Auckland, in March 2009. The sample comprised 151 students, 109 (72%)
music and 42 (28%) dance students. These numbers represented 48% (mu-
sic) and 79.2% (dance) of the total cohort. There were 54 (36%) males and 97
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 33
(64%) females. They ranged in age from 17-50 years, with a mean age of 21.44
years (SD=5.9). Students completed a comprehensive questionnaire at the
commencement of the academic year.
Materials
Procedure
Ethics approval for the study was granted by the University of Auckland Hu-
man Participants Ethics Committee. The study was introduced to students in
the first lectures of the semester through brief presentations to class groups
by lecturers and researchers. Participation information sheets were distrib-
uted to all eligible students and those who were interested were invited to
attend classes set aside for the completion of the surveys.
RESULTS
DISCUSSION
Dianna Kenny, University of Sydney, PO Box 170, Lidcombe, New South Wales 1825,
Australia; Email: d.kenny@usyd.edu.au
References
Bejjani F. J., Kaye G. M., and Benham M. (1996). Musculoskeletal and neuromuscular
conditions of instrumental musicians. Archives of Physical Medicine and Reha-
bilitation, 77, pp. 406-413.
Bragge P., Bialocerkowski A., and McMeeken J. (2006). A systematic review of preva-
lence and risk factors associated with playing-related musculoskeletal disorders in
pianists. Occupational Medicine, 56, pp. 28-38.
36 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Dianna T. Kenny1,2
This study assessed the factor structure of the revised Kenny Music Per-
formance Anxiety Inventory (KMPAI) using a sample (n=151) of tertiary
music and dance students attending the National Institute of Creative
Arts and Industries, University of Auckland. The scale consists of 40
items that assess the factor structure of the revised, expanded KMPAI to
ascertain whether it captured the latent etiological factors identified by
emotion theory underlying performance anxiety. Students completed the
(revised) Kenny Music Performance Anxiety Inventory at the com-
mencement of the 2009 academic year. Principal component analysis
(with varimax rotation) of the KMPAI revealed three latent factors and 12
underlying factors, as follows: early relationship context comprising gen-
erational transmission of anxiety and parental empathy; psychological
vulnerability comprising controllability, depression, hopelessness, and
trust; and proximal performance concerns comprising somatic anxiety,
pre- and post-performance rumination, self/other scrutiny, performance
outcome concerns, memory reliability, and commitment to performance.
These results provide initial evidence of the complex structure of music
performance anxiety, particularly in its severe form, and indicate that
management and treatment of the condition will need to be tailored to
the individuals pattern of contributing causal features.
take account of the etiological complexity of the condition (Kenny and Os-
borne 2006). Kenny (2004, 2009) developed the Kenny Music Performance
Anxiety Inventory (KMPAI) to include the assessment of underlying psycho-
logical vulnerabilities according to Barlows emotion based theory of the eti-
ology of anxiety disorders, as well as pre-performance experiences, as a step
in aiding the development of a more comprehensive conceptualization of the
condition and to provide a more appropriate focus for comprehensive treat-
ments for performing artists suffering from performance anxiety. The aim of
this study was to assess the factor structure of the revised, expanded KMPAI
to ascertain whether it captured the latent etiological factors identified by
emotion theory (Barlow 2000).
METHOD
Participants
The sample comprised music and dance students in all years who were at-
tending the National Institute of Creative Arts and Industries, University of
Auckland, in March 2009. The sample comprised 151 students: 109 (72%)
music and 42 (28%) dance students. These numbers represented 48% (mu-
sic) and 79.2% (dance) of the total cohort. There were 54 (36%) males and 97
(64%) females. They ranged in age from 17-50 years, with a mean age of 21.44
years (SD=5.9).
Materials
Procedure
Ethics approval for the study was granted by the University of Auckland Hu-
man Participants Ethics Committee. The study was introduced to students in
the first lectures of the semester through brief presentations to class groups
by lecturers and researchers. Participation information sheets were distrib-
uted to all eligible students and those who were interested were invited to
attend classes set aside for the completion of the surveys.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 39
Table 1. Rotated factor structure of the revised 40 item Kenny Music Performance Anxi-
ety Inventory.
Table 1 (cont.)
RESULTS
DISCUSSION
These results provide initial evidence of a complex structure for music per-
formance anxiety, particularly in its severe form, that is consistent with the
emotion based theory of the anxiety disorders. These data indicate that man-
agement and treatment of music performance anxiety will need to take ac-
count of multiple factors in its etiology and maintenance, with a broader
focus than proximal performance concerns. Results of this study will need to
be replicated on other samples and tested on professional musicians to cross-
validate the factor structure of the revised KMPAI beyond student popula-
tions.
Dianna Kenny, University of Sydney, PO Box 170, Lidcombe, New South Wales 1825,
Australia; Email: d.kenny@usyd.edu.au
References
Barlow H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the per-
spective of emotion theory. American Psychologist, 55, pp. 1245-1263.
Kenny D. T. and Osborne M. S. (2006). Music performance anxiety: New insights from
young musicians. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, pp. 103-112.
Kenny D. T. (2009). Kenny Music Performance Anxiety Inventory-Revised. Unpub-
lished manuscript.
Kenny D. T., Davis P. and Oates J. (2004). Music performance anxiety and occupational
stress amongst opera chorus artists and their relationship with state and trait anxi-
ety and perfectionism. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, pp. 757-777.
Thematic session:
The vocabulary of performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Verbal overshadowing (VO) can occur when we use words to describe sensory
experiences (such as sight, taste, or hearing). Numerous studies have exam-
ined this phenomenon in face, taste, and sound recognition and report that
verbal description, or verbal encoding, impairs later recall of the respective
experience. Individuals were less likely to identify the target stimuli from a
line-up (Parr et al. 2002, Perfect et al. 2002, Schooler and Engstler-Schooler
1990). The perception and description of music performance presents a
similar sensory challenge. Can words adequately capture sound quality in a
music performance? And if so, which terms or group of terms are most effec-
tive in expressing sound quality?
The VO effect has been most apparent in face recognition, where wit-
nesses to crimes have to describe the perpetrator of a robbery in detail and
46 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
later select their face from a photographic line-up. The act of verbal descrip-
tion has negatively influenced individuals ability to recognize the face they
described in words (Schooler and Engstler-Schooler 1990). Empirical studies
have confirmed this VO effect in facial recognition by comparing verbal de-
scription groups with memory-only control groups. More recent studies have
demonstrated that there is a similar VO effect on listeners ability to recognize
spoken voices following verbal description while no-description control
groups were able to isolate the voice from a line-up (Perfect et al. 2002, Va-
nags et al. 2005). Interestingly, expert wine-tasters were more likely than
novices to recognize wine-relevant odors by their verbal labels (Parr et al.
2002) despite the fact all tasters had knowledge of language used to describe
wine. Novices may have lacked the ability to separate perceptual and linguis-
tic skills while experts superior perceptual skills protected them from lin-
guistic interference to taste recognition.
In music, the modality of the stimuli (aural) does not match the task (ver-
bal description), yet we rely on verbal descriptors to explain and communi-
cate our perception of sound quality in training and assessment. There is
growing interest in the way listeners perceive, rate, and describe music per-
formances. Like the witnesses above, listeners form a global impression of the
performance or performer and use a limited selection of verbal descriptors to
explain the reasons for their judgments. They focus on the more easily ver-
balized technical and performance components rather than describing overall
quality (Davidson and Coimbra 2001, Stanley et al. 2002). In fact, examiners
have even noted candidates dress and stage manner to facilitate later recall of
individual singers performances for discussion and assessment (Davidson
and Coimbra 2001). It is more natural to conceptualize sound as belonging to
a sound-producing object, rather than analyze the components that make up
the sound (Ekholm et al. 1998). The use of language may effectively hinder
our recall of performers and performances.
While there is a growing body of knowledge focused on categorizing ter-
minology used in describing vocal quality, there is still much to be learned
about verbal descriptors in listeners perception of music performance. Com-
munication may focus on the most easily verbalized characteristics in the
sound rather than individual composition of these characteristics that makes
a sound unique (Kenny and Mitchell 2006, Mitchell and Kenny 2006). Ver-
balization, rather than enhancing interpretation of sound qualities, may have
a disruptive effect on listeners recall and communication of music perform-
ance. The main objective of this project is to examine impact of verbal de-
scription on the recall of music performances and, specifically, singers. It
investigates whether verbal overshadowing (VO), or putting a sound into
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 47
words, distorts listeners memory and subsequent recall of the original per-
formance or performer. This paper presents work in progress, the full details
of which will be reported in subsequent publications.
METHOD
Participants
Singers and listeners were recruited from the staff and students at the Sydney
Conservatorium of Music (SCM).
Materials
Procedure
Listeners (n=24) were asked to attend a single listening session at SCM and
were informed that the purpose of the study was to investigate the effect of
visual and verbal tasks on the ability to recognize aural stimuli. Audio sam-
ples were played to listeners from a CD player via circum-aural closed-back
stereo monitoring headphones (Sennheiser HD 270) to ensure that each lis-
tener heard the same quality of sample. Listener participants were assigned to
either a verbal description group or a non-description control group.
Listeners were presented with a short sample of a single singer perform-
ing song 1 (for encoding) on CD. Singer presentation was randomized from
the six singers. Listeners were then asked to perform a visual maze task on
paper (a filler-task) for 10 mins. In the following 5 mins, the experimental
group was asked to write a detailed description of the voice they heard at the
start of the test, while the control group completed an anagram puzzle. Fi-
nally, both groups were presented a line-up of six voices singing a short ex-
cerpt from song 2 and were asked to identify the original voice they heard and
to rate their confidence in their response on a scale of 1-10 (1=not confident,
10=very confident).
RESULTS
Table 1. Correct and incorrect voice line-up identifications, and mean confidence scores
for the verbal description and non-description groups.
correctly identified their target voice from the line-up. Table 1 shows the dis-
tribution of responses by experimental group. Fifty percent of the control
group and 33% of the verbal description group correctly identified their target
voice.
Verbal descriptions
For the written descriptions of target voices, listeners were instructed to focus
on the characteristics of the voice singing the target song rather than the song
the singer sang. Most listeners identified that the singer was a soprano, al-
though some listeners debated whether their target singer might be a mezzo
soprano, with comments such as: slightly darker sound than for soprano
and voice in a lower timbre to that of a soprano. Eight listeners mentioned
their target singers use (or lack) of vibratofor instance, singer does not use
an abundance of vibrato, smooth even vibrato on long notes but none on
shorter notes, and vibrato enlarged at the end of phrase; vibrato change
may be for emotional purpose?.
Seven listeners mentioned the singers diction or articulation of language:
good diction, the articulation of the words was extremely clear, and when
singing, the words are clearly understandable. Listeners made general judg-
ments about the quality of their target voices without expressing the reasons
that motivated their statements: not very expressive at all, after thinking
about it; quite boring, not the style of music I listen to but singer is good,
quality of the instrument has potential, needs refinement of technique to
bring out the individual voice, but she is still young, and sings in an un-
emotional way (possibly she doesnt know/understand what shes saying).
Some listeners mentioned words usually associated with classical singing
voice, such as rich and smooth, bell-like, round, and covered while
others used technical descriptors like support and projection. For two
listeners, the sound they heard evoked a mental picture of the target voice
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 49
DISCUSSION
This is the first study to investigate the effect of VO in the perception of music
performers. Verbal description decreased listeners likelihood of identifying
the target voice but only showed a slight reduction in listeners confidence in
their identification choice. Listeners were less likely to pick the correct voice
but were not aware that verbalizing their perception of the target voice re-
duced their ability to achieve correct identification.
Listeners responses in this study follow a similar test of spoken voice rec-
ognition (Perfect et al. 2002), where only 50% of listeners in the non-verbal
group and 21.4% of the verbal description group correctly identified the target
voice from a line-up. This project will confirm if music listeners are similarly
susceptible to the effects of VO when they describe singing voices.
Listeners descriptions of the target voices illustrated the limits of lan-
guage to communicate music perception. Examination of the comments fur-
ther confirmed that vocal quality is difficult to articulate or itemize (Davidson
and Coimbra 2001, Ekholm et al. 1998). While verbalizing sound quality in
this context proved an unusual challenge, listeners still resorted to descrip-
tions of the song, or indeed of the person singing, rather than attempt to de-
scribe the idiosyncrasies of each voice.
These preliminary results form part of a more extensive study investigat-
ing the nature of VO effects in music listeners and the extent to which the
results from VO studies in other domains generalize to the musical context.
Moreover, this ongoing work is also highlighting the complexities and ambi-
guities in describing singing voices linguistically.
Acknowledgments
The first author was funded by an early career researcher grant from the Sydney Con-
servatorium of Music, University of Sydney. We wish to thank the singers and listeners
who took part in this study.
References
2000). Recently, however, Collister and Huron (2008) compared the intelli-
gibility of solo sung versus spoken whole words, investigating consonant
confusions as well as vowels. Their results showed many more identification
errors for sung words.
Intelligibility is clearly an issue, however, for groups of singers such as
choirs: Fisher (1991) developed a research-informed articulatory diction
development method in response to the finding that approaches to choral
dictionare based primarily on tradition and personal preference (p. 270);
Racette et al. (2000) suggest that choral singing may be more effective than
solo singing for improving word intelligibility (for aphasics) since it may
entrain more than one auditory-vocal interface (p. 2571), while Emmons and
Chase (2006) argue that, in the interests of better diction, consonants should
be approximated and vowels modified.
Two years ago, we reported the results of a survey of listeners views on
factors that might affect the intelligibility of sung text (Fine and Ginsborg
2007a). Almost 400 open-ended statements were provided by 94 respon-
dents, most of whom devoted much of their music listening to vocal and cho-
ral music, and stated that the intelligibility of sung text was important to
them when listening to lyrics in their own, or another language. We had sug-
gested four possible broad categories of factor, relating to performer(s), envi-
ronment, listener, and music and/or lyrics. As expected from the research on
intelligibility cited above, a third of all statements concerned performer-re-
lated factors including articulation, diction and enunciation, breathing and
phrasing, communicating text, expression and stage presence, voice quality,
and range. One additional factor suggested was choral ensemble.
We then explored the effect of expertise. Our respondents were roughly
equally divided between experts (professional, semi-professional, and student
singers, and some singing teachers) and non-experts (amateur singers and
non-singers) (Fine and Ginsborg 2007b). The singing teachersperhaps as
they are in the business of improving singers skills, including intelligibility
made the highest proportion of performer-related statements.
We are now investigating some of the factors nominated by our survey re-
spondents in subsequent empirical research, in order to increase our under-
standing of how to enhance the intelligibility of sung text and improve
singers diction through more effective vocal pedagogy. The present study
investigates two factors: (1) number of singers and (2) listeners experience of
singing. More singers are likely to cause more variability and noise in the
acoustic signal, thus making its decoding and understanding more difficult;
indeed, some questionnaire respondents reported that choirs are generally
harder to understand than solo singers. If listeners are themselves experi-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 53
enced singers, they are more likely to be attuned to factors affecting singers
diction, and they may be better than non-singers at resolving the acoustic
signal into recognizable words.
Our aims were, therefore, twofold. First, we asked if sung text is harder to
understand when performed by a group of singers rather than a soloist. We
predicted that a single singer would be easier to understand than multiple
singers, even when singing in unison. Second, we investigated the extent to
which the listeners singing experience affects his or her ability to understand
sung text, even when the piece of music is unfamiliar. It was hypothesized
that expert singers would be better at comprehending sung text because of
their own experience of singing words.
METHOD
Participants
There were 48 participants (15 M, 23 F), with a mean age of 36.2 years (one
participant did not give her age). Half were self-reported singers (mean
age=36.9 years) and half self-reported non-singers (mean age=35.6 years).
Materials
Procedure
the eight melodies, two solo and two ensemble. The exact melodies used were
counterbalanced, so each stimulus was heard by 24 participants.
Each test stimulus was presented twice and the participants were in-
structed to write down the words they heard sung. On the first hearing of each
song, a pause was given after each line to allow the participant to catch up (as
the focus of the study was perception rather than memorization ability). The
second time through, the stimulus was played without breaks. Different pens
were used on each hearing so that writing differences by hearing were appar-
ent during subsequent scoring.
The productions were then scored as follows. An error was counted for
every word missing or incorrectly heard, and for each additional word. Incor-
rect order of words or numbers was not counted as an error, as memory was
not being tested. Errors were then subtracted from the total number of words
in the song (varying from 22 to 27), and then the performance score was
transformed into a percentage, used in the analyses below.
RESULTS
The participants were asked for their singing experience on a 5-point Likert
scale (1=non-singer, 2=occasional singer, 3=keen amateur singer, 4=semi-
professional singer, 5=professional singer). They were split into two groups
(24 had answered 1-2, 24 answered 3-5). On average, the singers had sung for
22.8 years, tended to rehearse for 3.7 hours per week, and reported listening
to sung music 6.9 hours per week. On average, the non-singers had sung for
0.9 years, did not rehearse at all, and reported listening to sung music for
10.5 hours per week.
Mean performance data (understanding sung text) are shown in Table 1.
A mixed Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was carried out. The within-subject
variables were number of singers (solo vs. ensemble) and hearing (first vs.
second). The between-subject variable was singing experience (singer vs. non-
singer). The dependent variable was % performance score. Hearing was
highly significant (F1,46=82.13, p<0.001), with better performance on the sec-
ond hearing (94.8%) than the first (90.1%). Singing experience was also sig-
nificant (F1,46=5.95, p<0.02), with singers scoring 94.1% and non-singers
scoring 90.8%. However, number of singers was not significant (92.9% solo,
92.0% ensemble).
The relationships between experience and performance factors were in-
vestigated. Overall performance significantly correlated with singing experi-
ence (r48=0.31, p<0.05), number of years singing (r48=0.33, p<0.05), hours of
singing per week (r48=0.45, p<0.01), and number of hours listening to sung
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 55
Solo Ensemble
First hearing Second hearing First hearing Second hearing
Singers 92.1 96.4 91.3 96.6
Non-singers 89.3 93.9 87.6 92.5
music per week (r48=-0.31, p=0.05). Not surprisingly, singing experience also
correlated with years singing (r48=0.84, p<0.001) and number of hours sing-
ing per week (r48=0.77, p<0.001). When the singers and non-singers were
investigated separately, the only significant correlations were, for singers, age
and years singing (r23=0.84, p<0.001) and, for non-singers, overall perform-
ance and hours listening to sung music (r24=-0.53, p< 0.01), which was unex-
pectedly a negative correlation.
It was notable that certain words were more often misheard than others.
A preliminary survey shows the following words were misheard or omitted by
more than a quarter of the participants (n=12): run, fields, still, no,
the, and are. In particular, non-numeric words were often missed in the
number stimuli, perhaps due to the lack of semantic context. The patterns of
words misheard varied slightly between the solo and the ensemble stimuli.
DISCUSSION
Overall, participants could understand the sung words well (92% accuracy).
The hypothesis that experienced singers would find it easier to understand
sung text than inexperienced or non-singers was supported, both by more
accurate text identification for singers than for non-singers and a significant
correlation between singing experience and overall performance. This may be
because singers are used to producing sung text themselves, and therefore
better attuned to understanding text sung by others. They will also have ex-
perience of being in rehearsals and performances, and hearing others sing
while knowing the words themselves, forging a stronger connection between
the words and their sung acoustic signals.
However, the results do not support the hypothesis that ensemble singing
is harder to understand than solo singing, with no significant difference in
performance evident between the two conditions. This may be because the
three singers involved all concentrated on making diction as clear as possible
so there were few substantial differences between the stimuli in the acoustic
signal. Another possible reason is that ensemble effects only really become
apparent when there are many singers (even small choirs normally contain at
56 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
least eight singers). These results are considered inconclusive and are not
sufficient to reject the hypothesis; further study will be undertaken with lar-
ger ensembles, and also consider the effect of harmony versus unison singing.
Hearing the song for a second time significantly improved intelligibility and
underlines the importance of familiarity and repeated hearings on listeners
ability to understand sung text. However, there were certain words that were
still hard to understand on the second hearing. In future studies, it would be
interesting to investigate the acoustic properties of these words and also the
effects of the presence or absence of semantic context on intelligibility.
References
Performance cues (PCs) are the landmarks in a piece of music that a musician
thinks about during performance. They provide a mental map of the piece
that allows the performer to monitor the music as it unfolds and to recover
from mistakes and memory lapses. PCs are prepared during practice so that
they come to mind automatically on stage, giving the musician the ability to
focus on each aspect of the piece at the right moment, providing the flexibility
needed for musical spontaneity and to recover from disruptions.
We know that musicians use PCs from longitudinal case studies in which
experienced performers recorded their practice as they prepared new pieces
for performance and then reported the PCs that they used (Chaffin 2006,
Ginsborg et al. 2006, Chaffin et al. 2002, in press). The validity of the reports
was supported by behavioral evidence from practice, polished performance,
and written recall.
58 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
Here, we describe a survey of PC use designed to find out how the number
and type of PCs that a musician uses is affected by experience, instrument,
musical style, and type of performance. We plan to ask musicians to report
PCs for two pieces (one easy, one harder to prepare) that they have already
prepared or are in the process of preparing for performance. Although we call
it a survey, each musicians contribution is more like a case-study, but with
no recording of practice. Colleagues and their graduate students at a variety
of institutions will conduct an open-ended series of independent but related
studies, each composed of several of these case studies and each contributing
to a central database. In this way, we will build up a more comprehensive
picture of PC use. Meanwhile, the local investigators will answer specific
questions about factors affecting PC use such as, for example, effects of con-
servatory training.
We will describe the procedure to be used in the survey and then report
results from longitudinal case studies conducted to date to provide an indica-
tion of the kinds of results that we expect to obtain.
Types of PC
We have found it useful to distinguish five main types of PC: structure, ex-
pression, interpretation, basic technique, and shared. Structural PCs are criti-
cal places in the formal structure such as harmonic and melodic boundaries.
Expressive PCs represent turning points in the musical feeling (e.g. excited or
sad). Interpretive PCs represent the changes in tempo, dynamics, timbre, or
color that accomplish these expressive effects. Basic PCs represent details of
technique that must be implemented in order to be able to produce these
musical gestures as planned, such as a fingering required to set up the hand
for what follows. Shared PCs coordinate ensemble playing.
Basic PCs vary considerably across instruments. Many instruments re-
quire attention to fingering, while a singer might think instead about breath
control. String players must attend to left hand shifts and to right hand
changes in bowing direction. Some kinds of interpretive PCs appear to be
common across instruments (e.g. phrasing, dynamics, and tempo), while
others are instrument specific (e.g. pedaling on the piano, intonation for
strings, word meaning for singers).
Several PCs may refer to a single place in the music (see Figure 1). For ex-
ample, basic, interpretive, and expressive PCs at the same spot might indicate
a pianists decision to use the 1st finger (basic) in order to play forte (inter-
pretation) because this is the emotional climax (expression). The presence
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 59
Figure 1. One page of a PC report by pianist Gabriela Imreh for Claude Debussys Clair
de Lune showing expressive, interpretive, and basic PCs.
Figure 2. Excerpts from PC reports made by cellist Tnia Lisboa for J. S. Bachs Cello
Suite VI (Prelude) on four separate copies of the score for (1) structure (top left: har-
monic (H), melodic (M), and lower level (L3) boundaries), (2) expression and interpre-
tation (top right, interpretive PCs in parentheses), (3) hand position and intonation
(bottom left, red and black respectively), and (4) bowing and fingering (bottom right,
red and black respectively). (See full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
60 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
of the three different PCs would indicate that the pianist was prepared to
think of any or all of these aspects at this point.
The musician does not necessarily think of every PC in very single per-
formance. In reporting the presence of a PC, the musician is saying that s/he
is prepared to think about this feature of the music during performance, if
necessary. On a good day, the pianist in the above example may decide to
leave the fingering and the forte to take care of themselves while focusing on
the climax. On a bad day, the same pianist may be fighting to get the notes
right and let the forte and climax take care of themselves in order to focus on
the fingering. One benefit of well prepared PCs is that they allow the musician
to give very similar performances under very different conditions, including
changes in their own mental and emotional states.
Reporting PCs
In the initial research on PCs, pianist Gabriela Imreh reported PCs on a spe-
cially prepared version of the score (see Figure 1, Chaffin 2006). Subse-
quently, most musicians have marked PCs on multiple copies of a published
score. For example, cellist Tnia Lisboa used four separate copies of the score
(see Figure 2, Chaffin et al. in press).
To show how PCs can be compared across musicians and pieces, we have
summarized PC reports from nine longitudinal case studies, using previously
published and unpublished data from our laboratory. Three pianists provided
reports for the Italian Concerto (Presto): a professional (Imreh) and two uni-
versity student piano performance majors, one MA and one BA-level. Imreh
also provided reports for a second piece, Clair de Lune by Claude Debussy.
We also have reports by three other professional soloists for cello (Lisboa),
voice (Ginsborg), and piano (Silva), and by two students: a BA-level trumpet
performance major and a 14-year old piano student.
Figure 3 shows the number of PCs of each type for each piece. To allow
comparison across pieces, frequencies were normalized by dividing by the
number of beats in each piece. Despite the small sample, there are suggestive
differences. The trumpet and grade-school students reported fewer PCs than
other musicians by an order of magnitude. For the Presto, the two students
reported fewer PCs than the professional. The professional pianist (Imreh)
reported fewer PCs for the easier (for her) Clair de Lune than for the chal-
lenging Presto. With the exception of the Presto, the four professionals all
reported similar numbers of PCs.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 61
Figure 3. Number of performance cues per beat for different musicians and pieces. (See
full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
Figure 4 shows the number of PCs of each type as a percentage of the total
reported (normalized by number of beats). Experience mattered; the profes-
sionals used more basic PCs, the college students more interpretive PCs, the
grade school student (and trumpeter) more structural PCs.
62 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
IMPLICATIONS
All the longitudinal case studies of experienced soloists preparing new works
conducted by our laboratory have found that the musicians engaged in ex-
tended practice of PCs. This suggests that PCs are necessary for reliable per-
formance, perhaps because motor memory is unreliable (Chaffin et al. in
press). While the proposed survey is unlikely to discover whether some musi-
cians do not use PCs, it should identify the factors that affect PC use and es-
tablish the range of variability in the use of PCs.
We hope that PC surveys will become commonplace in music conservato-
ries and departments. We believe that the self-study involved provides musi-
cians with insight into their own learning and memorization. All of the
musicians who have participated in the longitudinal case studies that form
the basis for this work report that they found the process of self-study to be
beneficial (Chaffin et al. 2002, pp. 266-268; Chaffin et al. in press). In addi-
tion, we expect the conclusions will be of value to music pedagogy and also to
the psychology of music performance.
Materials for conducting PC surveys can be found on our website at www.
htfdcc.uconn.edu/psyclabs/musiclab.html.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Alexandra Paberty Clarke and Kristen Begosh for contributing data for the
grade school student and trumpeter, respectively.
References
Chaffin R. (2007). Learning Clair de Lune: Retrieval practice and expert memorization.
Music Perception, 24, pp. 377-393.
Chaffin R., Imreh G., and Crawford M. (2002). Practicing Perfection. Mahwah, New
Jersey, USA: Erlbaum Associates.
Chaffin R., Lisboa T., Logan T., and Begosh K. T. (in press). Preparing for memorized
cello performance: The role of performance cues. Psychology of Music.
Ginsborg J., Chaffin R., and Nicholson G. (2006). Shared performance cues in singing
and conducting: A content analysis of talk during practice. Psychology of Music, 34,
pp. 167-194.
Thematic session:
Performance analysis
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
One isochronous melody (the fast melody) was created that was easy to
perform with the right hand and could be continuously repeated (see notation
in Figure 2); it was designed to be performed at very fast tempi (tempo con-
ditions were 7, 8.4, 9.6, 10.7, 11, 7, 12.3, 14 and 16 tones/s, presented on dif-
ferent trials); the pianists decided at what tempo they stopped performing
(open-ended design). The tempo was indicated by a metronome in a synchro-
nization-continuation paradigm. For comparison, we include data from two
moderate melodies that contained 16 tones and were performed at moder-
ate to medium fast tempi (2, 4, 6, and 7 tones/s), as reported earlier (Goebl
and Palmer 2008).
Procedure
A passive motion capture system (Vicon 460) equipped with six infrared
cameras tracked the movements of 4 mm reflective markers glued on pianists
finger joints, hand, and wrist at a sampling rate of 250 frames/s. The motion
trajectories of the five finger-tip markers were smoothed with functional data
analysis techniques (Ramsay and Silverman 2005) and analyzed in the verti-
cal dimension (height above piano key surface).
Kinematic landmarks were extracted prior to each keystroke (see also
Figure 2): the key-bottom landmark (KB, the finger is stopped by the keybed)
and the maximum finger height (mxH, to be interpreted as the beginning of
the finger movement) for all keystrokes. An additional finger-key landmark
sometimes occurs when fingers strike the keys from a distance above the key
surface (struck touch, see Askenfelt and Jansson 1990) and the acceleration
peak is larger than a threshold of 10 m/s2 (for details, see Goebl and Palmer
2009). A pressed touch does not feature such a landmark. In addition, we
determined the peak velocity at which the finger arrives at the key surface.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 67
17 24
1 17 FK
24
17
0.5 24 Time of FK
Figure 1. Time of maximum height (mxH, upper graphs) and time of finger-key contact
(FK, lower graphs) in number of events prior to note onset (key-bottom, KB) by per-
formed tempo. Thick line is the mean of 12 pianists; Pianists 17 and 24 are plotted sepa-
rately. (See full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
RESULTS
The open-ended design of the fast melody generated the following results:
all pianists were able to perform up to a rate of 11.7 tones/s (sixteenth notes at
176 bpm, beat=quarter note); 8 played up to 12.3, 7 up to 13.3, 6 up to 14.0,
and only 3 up to 16.0 tones per second (considerably faster, for example, than
the metronome markings of Chopins Op.25/11). The fast pianists had
similar amounts of piano lessons and years of playing, compared to the
slower pianists; the only difference was the weekly practice, significantly
higher for the fast players (25 vs. 13 hours). The following analyses attempt
to identify kinematic properties that distinguish the fast players from the slow
players.
Figure 1 shows the average timing values of the identified landmarks by
tempo condition for the moderate melodies (2-7 tones/s) and the fast melody
(7 tones/s and faster). Timing has been normalized on an event-to-event basis
relative to KB contacts to allow comparisons across tempo conditions. At
about 8 tones/s, the movement initiation (time of maximum height) toward
the current keystroke occurs more than one keystroke before (starts to over-
lap with) the previous keystroke (KB). Furthermore, the time of FK contact
approaches the time of the previous keystroke at the fastest rates. This over-
lap in movement landmarks between current and previous keystrokes may be
68 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
60
Medium fast (7 tones/s)
50
mxH
40
4
30 2
3
FK
Height above keys (mm)
20
10 4
3
KB 2
0
5.6 5.8 6 6.2 6.4 6.6 6.8 7 7.2
60
30
3: FK
4
20
2 3
10 4 2: KB
3 2
0
4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 4.9 5
Time (s)
Figure 2. Finger trajectories of index (2), middle (3), and ring finger (4) of Pianist 24
playing one cycle of the fast melody at a medium fast tempo (upper panel) and a very
fast tempo (lower panel). Three kinematic landmarks are labeled in the finger trajecto-
ries before each keystroke: the maximum height (mxH), the finger-key contact (FK),
and the key-bottom contact (KB). Vertical lines denote MIDI onset times. (See full color
version at www.performancescience.org.)
Pianist 17 Pianist 24
1 1
0.6 0.6
0.2 0.2
moderate melodies moderate melodies
0 0
2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16
Rate (tones/s)
Figure 3. Finger velocity in meters per second at finger-key contact (FK) against per-
formance rate, separately for Pianist 17 (left) and Pianist 24 (right). Proportional scaling
with rate is plotted with solid lines.
performs occasionally in church. Pianist 24 was able to produce the very fast
tempo conditions; he played the piano for 20 years and still studies piano
actively; he practices 21 hours per week and performs in public 4-5 times per
year. The individual landmark timing for these two pianists is shown in Fig-
ure 1. Pianist 17 needed longer than Pianist 24 to perform a keystroke at a
given tempo. Thus, her keystroke landmark timing overlaps with those of
previous keystrokes at slower tempi than do Pianist 24s keystroke land-
marks. Moreover at each pianists fastest performed tempo condition, the FK
timing occurred close to the arrival (KB) of the previous keystroke
Goebl and Palmer (2008) identified two groups of pianists who differed in
their playing behavior across tempo conditions: a low-FK group who
showed a positive relation between FK landmark proportion and tempo (low
at slow tempo conditions) and a high-FK group with high (close to 1) FK
proportions for all tempo conditions. These two groups differed also at the
velocity with which their fingers arrived at the key surface (FK velocity): the
velocities of the low-FK group scaled proportionately with the tempo condi-
tions, but not those of the high-FK group. The fingers of the low-FK group
arrived at the key surface with twice the velocity when the performance
tempo doubled.
We analyzed the same FK measures for the pianists performing the fast
melody in this study; they all showed ceiling effects in their FK proportions at
all tempi faster than 7 tones/s and no proportionality overall in the FK finger
velocities. The FK measures were also contrasted for the two exemplary pi-
anists; their mean finger velocities at finger-key contact are plotted against
the full range of performed tempi in Figure 3 for the moderate melodies (2-7
tones/s) and the fast melody (7 tones/s and faster). The slow pianist (17)
70 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Austrian Research Fund (J2526) to the first author and
by the Canada Research Chairs program and NSERC Grant 298173 to the second.
References
Askenfelt A. and Jansson E. V. (1990). From touch to string vibrations. Timing in the
grand piano action. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, pp.52-63.
Goebl W. and Palmer C. (2008). Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano per-
formance. Experimental Brain Research, 186, pp. 471-479.
Goebl W. and Palmer C. (2009). Synchronization of timing and motion among per-
forming musicians. Music Perception, 26, pp. 427-438.
Ramsay J. O. and Silverman B. W. (2005). Functional Data Analysis (2e). New York:
Springer.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Paul Molin
tract the qualities and flaws of a pianist, and summarize them by synthetic
graphs and synthetic variables.
METHOD
Participants
Eighteen pianists (mean age 22.5 years, ranging from 12-59), from middle
level music conservatories to professional level participated in the experi-
ment. They had played piano for a mean of 14.5 years (range=4-52) and re-
ceived either course credit or payment for their participation.
Materials
The pianists performed on a classical upright piano (Yamaha U2) with MIDI
interface. MIDI data were collected on a DELL laptop PC, by using the
Cubase SX program. All statistic programs were written in the Matlab pro-
gramming language by members of the LEAD-CNRS laboratory.
Procedure
Fingering was verified by the operator to be 1-2-3 for the right hand and
1-4-2 for the left hand for the natural arpeggio state, 1-2-4 for right hand
and 1-3-2 for left hand in the upward direction, and the reverse for the
down direction.
RESULTS
For all exercises, the first indices of quality are a high tempo and few false
notes. But these are not the only qualities of a good piano technique; for
scales and arpeggios, the main word characterizing the quality of playing is
homogeneity. This means rhythmic homogeneity for each hand, rhythmic
homogeneity between hands, homogeneity of velocity (intensity of key de-
pressing force) for each hand, velocity homogeneity between hands, and
overlap homogeneity (legato homogeneity). Of course, in an artistic perform-
ance of a piece of music, this homogeneity is not observed; all the discrepan-
cies in an artistic performance are voluntary acts, guided by the artistic
sensibility. But the homogeneity required in the exercises is the way to gain
the control of the movements, in order to play with expression.
We then retained seven variables: (1) tempo (TPO), (2) tempo coefficient
of variation (CVTPO), (3) number of false notes (FN), (4) intra-hand velocity
standard deviation (STDVEL), (5) intra-hand overlap standard deviation
(STDOVL), (6) lag time between hands (LAGLR), and (7) velocity difference
between hands (DVELLR). For trills, only the first five variables were used,
since the exercise was performed hand-by-hand.
In the experiments of Jabusch et al. (2004, 2007, 2009), the authors fixed
the tempo of the exercises (scales) and allowed the subjects to repeat them
10-15 times. They then computed a mean scale for each subject. Our point
of view is slightly different. An important part of the technical level is the
pianists awareness of his own skille.g. I will not begin a scale at a metro-
nomic tempo of 160 if I know that I will not finish the scale. We then left the
subjects free to choose their tempos and considered the tempo variable as a
level variable among others.
We present complete results and graphs for the Eb major scale. Table 1 shows
the correlation matrix of the seven variables for the scale. It is not surprising
74 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 1. Correlation matrix between the seven quality variables for Eb major scale.
Tempo False Inter hand Intra hand CV tempo Overlap Time lag
notes velocity velocity STD between
variations STD hands
(TPO) (FN) (DVELLR) (STDVEL) (CVTPO) (STDOVL) (LAGLR)
TPO 1 -0.179 -0.610 -0.165 -0.468 -0.503 -0.198
FN -0.179 1 -0.024 0.422 0.682 0.187 0.686
DVELLR -0.610 -0.024 1 0.291 0.128 0.516 0.134
STDVEL -0.165 0.422 0.291 1 0.220 0.242 0.724
CVTPO -0.468 0.682 0.128 0.220 1 0.540 0.432
STDOVL -0.503 0.187 0.516 0.242 0.540 1 0.159
LAGLR -0.198 0.686 0.134 0.724 0.432 0.159 1
that the tempo variable is negatively correlated with all other variables: intui-
tively, tempo is a positive index of quality, while the other variables are nega-
tives. Some other correlations, intuitively expected, can be noticed: for
example, the high correlation (0.682) between the false notes and CV. Tempo
variables can be understood as a false note induces a stress for the pianist,
then an irregularity of tempo. In the same way, the high correlation between
the CV tempo and the time lag can be understood as a false note in one hand
does not affect the other hand, and then increases the time lag between
hands.
Figure 1 shows the graphic results of the Eb major PCA, which is consis-
tent with respect to the correlation matrix. The left part of the figure is the
correlation circle, which represents the correlations between variables and
principal components. The first two principal components account for about
70% of the total variance of the data, and thus the bi-dimensional represen-
tation is reliable. As expected, the tempo variable is opposed to all others. It is
very interesting to see that the correlation between tempo and false notes is
low (-0.179). A good pianist can do many wrong notesa known joke of Al-
fred Cortot: if you picked all my false notes in a concerto, it could be a sec-
ond concerto.
In order to interpret the graph of subjects, let us describe some of them:
pianists 3, 4, and 9 are beginning the second cycle of studies in a regional
conservatory, pianist 18 is a concert pianist, pianist 2 is a piano teacher, and
pianist 1 is an amateur (the author). All other pianists are second or third
cycle students in a regional conservatory. Then the first principal component
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 75
Table 2. Metronomic and adjusted tempos (scores) for the Eb major scale.
Subject 1 2 3 4 5 7 9 10 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Tempo 86.7 79.2 65.9 31.0 36.7 71.7 54.5 41.8 35.6 64.8 85.5 94.6 71.7 64.2 143.3
Score 63.0 59.8 26.7 1.7 16.6 50.0 13.8 25.1 7.1 30.1 62.7 64.0 49.2 38.7 125.0
Figure 1. Principal component analysis of the Eb major scale. The right panel represents
the variables and the left panel the subjects.
can be easily interpreted as the technical level axis. The second axis can de-
scribe more precisely the flaws of the pianist: for example, a pianist far above
the second axis is characterized by many false notes, a bad synchronism be-
tween hands, but a good legato (little overlap STD).
DISCUSSION
The PCA analysis appears to be a good visualization tool for a synthetic pres-
entation of the technical level of a pianist. The results for trills and arpeggios
are similar to those of scales. All the default variables are positively corre-
lated, and all negatively correlated with the tempo variable. As we stated in
the section on PCA above, the principal components cannot be used as they
are, but they suggest an overall evaluation in a form opposing the tempo with
all the other variables. For example, for the Eb major scale, the first principal
component has the following equation:
PC1=-0.3595*TPO+0.3991*FN+0.2860*DVELLR+
0.3682*STDVEL+0.4331*CVTPO+0.3668*STDOVL+ 0.4147*LAGLH
76 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
All the coefficients have the same magnitude. The idea is then to give the
results to the musician in the form of an adjusted tempo, very understandable
by musicians:
TPOADJ=PC1/-.3595=TPO-1.1102*FN-0.7956*DVELLR
-1.0244*STDVEL-1.2049*CVTPO-1.0205*STDOVL -1.1537*LAGLH
This formula gives the results in Table 2. Under this system, a pianist who
plays fast and has many flaws gets the same score as a pianist who plays
slower but with good legato, good rhythmic homogeneity, few false notes, etc.
Acknowledgments
I wish to thank Konrad Maria Engel for kindly accepting to lend his talent to our ex-
periments, to Eckart Altenmller and Hans Christian Jabusch for fruitful help and dis-
cussions, to Chlo Fourier and Michel Cara the indefatigable operators, and to
Vronique Assadas for her stylistic help.
Paul Molin, LEAD CNRS, University of Burgundy, Esplanade Erasme, Dijon, Cte dOr
21000, France; Email: paul.molin@u-bourgogne.fr
References
Bach C. P .E. (1762). Essai sur la Vritable Manire de Jouer les Instruments Clavier.
Paris: CNRS Editions 2002.
Clarke E. F. (2004). Empirical methods in the study of performance. In E. F. Clarke and
N. Cook (eds), Empirical Musicology (pp. 77-102).Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cortot A. (1928). Principes Rationnels de la Technique Pianistique. Paris: Salabert.
Hotelling H. (1933). Analysis of a complex of statistical variables with principal compo-
nents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 24, pp. 417-441.
Jabusch H.C., Alpers H., Kopiez R., and Altenmller E. (2009). The influence of practice
on development of motor skills in pianists: A longitudinal study in a select motor
task. Human Movement Science, 28, pp. 74-84.
Jabusch H.C., Vauth H., and Altenmller E. (2004). Quantification of focal dystonia in
pianists using scale analysis. Movement Disorders, 19, pp. 171-180.
Jabusch H.C., Yong R., and Altenmller E. (2007). Biographical predictors of music-
related motor skills in children pianists. In A. Williamon and D. Coimbra (eds.),
Proceedings of ISPS 2007 (pp.363-368). Utrecht, The Netherlands: European As-
sociation of Conservatoires (AEC).
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
In recent years, a lot of people who have wanted to learn how to play the pi-
ano have been able to create easily an environment in which to learn due to
the development of the MIDI keyboard. However, most people who start of-
ten give up practicing the piano. There are many reasons for this: (1) special-
ists instructions cannot be given due to temporal and spatial restrictions, (2)
the piano performance cannot be objectively evaluated, (3) beginners cannot
plan how to teach themselves the piano, (4) beginners tire of practicing if they
only use textbooks, and (5) it takes a lot of time to master the piano. To over-
come these difficulties, many systems have been developed for supporting
novice players and/or beginner pianists. However, even though these systems
record and use a set of performance data, they cannot appropriately evaluate
a performance with the deviations from a metronome in artistic terms. To
solve this problem, we have developed a method to evaluate automatically the
skill level and proficiency for performing a one octave scale (e.g. Akinaga
2006).
METHOD
Materials
This study uses the following evaluation models: spline curves and regression
curves. The spline curve used here is the interpolated curve that passes
through the representative points. The representative points are determined
by finger crossing in three ways. Our previous study aimed to evaluate a per-
formance containing artistic expressions such as intonation. Therefore, spline
curves were introduced as a standard for evaluating performance proficiency.
An example of a spline curve is shown in Figure 1. Regression curves used in
this study are n-dimensional polynomial (1n10). When n=1, the model
means a straight line, or when n=15, the model means the actual performance.
When n increases, obtained regression curves become closer to the actual
performance.
Procedure
Condition 1. Fifteen inputted notes are categorized into four clusters. Centers
of each cluster are assumed as representative points. In these conditions, the
clusters are comprised of C-D-E-F, F-G-A-B-C, C-B-A-G-F, and F-E-D-
C, respectively. Representative points of condition 1 are shown in Figure 2.
Condition 2. Fifteen inputted notes are categorized into four clusters.
Centers of each cluster are assumed as representative points. In these condi-
tions, the clusters are comprised of C-D-E, F-G-A-B-C, C-B-A-G-F, and
E-D-C, respectively. Here, overlapping notes for crossing fingers belong to a
cluster, whereas in condition 1 they belong to both clusters. Representative
points of condition 2 are shown in Figure 3.
Condition 3. Fifteen inputted notes are categorized into three clusters.
Centers of each cluster are assumed as representative points. In these condi-
tions, the clusters are comprised of C-D-E, F-G-A-B-C-B-A-G-F, and E-
D-C, respectively. Unlike condition 2, here two clusters around fingerings of
returning are categorized into a cluster. Representative points of condition 3
are shown in Figure 4.
RESULTS
Determination coefficient
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
This study was partly supported by the High-Tech Research Center project for private
universities, matching fund subsidy from the Japanese Ministry of Education, Culture,
Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT) and Research for Promoting Technological
Seeds from the Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST).
Shinya Morita, Graduate School of Science and Technology, Ryukoku University, 1-5
Yokotani, Seta Oe-cho, Otsu Shiga 520-2194, Japan; Email: t08m079@mail.ryukoku.
ac.jp
References
Akinaga S., Miura M., Emura N., and Yanagida M. (2006). Toward realizing automatic
evaluation of playing scales on the piano. In M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina,
and M. Costa (eds), Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Music
Perception and Cognition [CD-ROM] (pp. 1843-1847). Bologna, Italy: Bologna Uni-
versity Press.
Dannenberg R., Sanchez M., Joseph A. et al. (1990). A computer-based multi-media
tutor for beginning piano students. Interface, 19, pp. 155-73.
Dannenberg R., Sanchez M., Joseph A. et al. (1993). Results from the Piano Tutor Pro-
ject. In D. Smalley, N. Zahler, and P. Galvani (eds), Proceedings of the Fourth Bien-
nial Arts and Technology Symposium (pp. 143-150). New London, Connecticut,
USA: Connecticut College.
Symposium:
Performance science: Implications for
educational and professional practice
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
This study profiles music students physical and mental fitness for per-
formance. Participants were recruited from the Royal College of Music
(RCM, n=59) and Royal Northern College of Music (RNCM, n=32), and
standardized measures of health promoting behaviors, anxiety, perfec-
tionism, cardiovascular fitness, and physical strength and flexibility were
employed to assess students performance-related health and wellbeing.
The resulting profile indicates that (1) students tend to fall outside of
their target BMI, with more students falling below their target than above
it, (2) cardiovascular fitness is most frequently below average or average,
with under 40% of students achieving above average cardiovascular fit-
ness, (3) student fatigue correlates variously with aspects of perfection-
ism, trait anxiety, health promotion, and self-regulated learning, and (4)
pain that is reported to stop performance is most often linked to the up-
per arm/elbow, left and right hands, and the back. The value of such
profiling exercises in educational contexts is discussed, with examples of
implementation drawn from a UK conservatoire.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Music students fitness for performance was profiled through the following
self-report measures:
Procedure
The profiling procedure was divided into three stages. Stage 1 introduced the
participant to the profile study, secured written consent, and screened health
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 87
suitability for the physical assessment. Each participants height (cm), weight
(kg), hand spans (cm), and finger spans (angle) were recorded.
Stage 2 was conducted online using the environment provided by Sur-
veymonkey, to which the self-report measures above were adapted. The
questionnaire was identical at both institutions, and students were able to ask
a member of the research team for clarification on the questions presented as
necessary.
Stage 3 comprised the physical assessment, conducted by an exercise sci-
entist. The assessment included measures of balance (dominant balance),
grip strength (kg), core flexibility (cm), body fat/lean percentages, body fat/
lean mass (kg), body mass index (BMI), shoulder flexibility internal and ex-
ternal (dominant balance), and sub-maximal cardiovascular fitness (heart
rate recovery, in bpm). Body composition measurements were taken using a
bioelectrical impedance meter and sub-maximal fitness by a 3-min step test
with bpm measured using a Polar heart rate monitor. Participants were given
a printed summary of their results and were fully debriefed.
Data analysis
RESULTS
100
80
60
Percentage
men
women
40
20
0
Below target Target Above target
100
80
60
Percentage
men
women
40
20
0
Below average Average Above average
Figure 2. Cardiovascular fitness. Below average includes below average, poor, and
very poor. Above average includes above average, good, and excellent (n=84).
DISCUSSION
The profile arising from these results can be summarized as follows: (1) stu-
dents tend to fall outside of their target BMI, with more students falling below
their target than above it, (2) cardiovascular fitness is most frequently below
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 89
Table 1. Significant Pearson correlations (p<0.05) between ratings of pain in the elbows,
hands, spine, mouth and feelings of fatigue with other self-report measures and BMI,
where + indicates a significant positive correlation and - a negative correlation.
average or average, with under 40% of students achieving above average car-
diovascular fitness, (3) student fatigue correlates variously with aspects of
perfectionism, trait anxiety, health promotion, and self-regulated learning,
and (4) pain that is reported to stop performance is most often linked to the
upper arm/elbow, left and right hands, and the back.
Given the physicality of musicians work, such a trend toward poor fitness
is concerning. With the injury and pain problems frequently reported in con-
servatoriesand in light of profiling exercises such as reported herethere is
clearly a need for educational programs to address this issue. At the RCM, for
example, the profile is used to inform a compulsory seminar series for all
incoming undergraduate students, designed to raise awareness of the impor-
tance of musicians fitness and wellbeing. To enable students to follow this up
with practical steps, poor fitness demonstrated in the profile is targeted
through a fitness awareness scheme, and aspects of student fatigue and injury
addressed through Peak Performance Workshops, introducing students to
Alexander technique, yoga, Pilates, and tai chi, among other interventions.
Further work to extend the sample is underway, with new intakes at the
RCM and RNCM, and at other UK conservatoires, in order to refine and
streamline the profiling procedure. The tools to emerge from this process will
90 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a grant from the Royal College of Music, London.
Aaron Williamon, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Con-
sort Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: awilliamon@rcm.ac.uk
References
Ginsborg J., Kreutz G., Thomas M., and Williamon A. (2009). Healthy behaviours in
music and non-music performance students. Health Education, 109, pp. 242-258.
Kreutz G., Ginsborg J., and Williamon A. (2009). Health-promoting behaviours in con-
servatoire students. Psychology of Music, 37, pp. 47-60.
Kreutz G., Ginsborg J., and Williamon A. (2008). Music students health problems and
health-promoting behaviors. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 23, pp. 3-11.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Research over the last two decades has examined the physical fitness status of
professional and student dancers (Chatfield et al. 1990, Cohen et al. 1982,
92 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Dahlstrom et al. 1996, Novak et al. 1978, Rimmer et al. 1994), but there is
still debate about how fit dancers should be. One study (Wanke 2001) as-
sessed the effect of an integrated endurance training program for ballet
dancers (n=16) across a ballet season and found positive results. Some stud-
ies have suggested a discrepancy in the intensity level between training, re-
hearsal, and performance (Rist 1994, Wyon and Redding 2005, Redding and
Wyon 2001), and the idea of supplementary fitness training has been de-
bated, albeit untested longitudinally with large groups of dancers.
The applicability of laboratory tests and training regimes from sports is
questionable (Schantz and Astrand 1984, Redding and Wyon 2003), and it is
becoming increasingly necessary to gather relevant data and qualitative ob-
servationsphysiological and psychologicalin order to develop specific
methods of promoting and assessing dance fitness. The purpose of this study,
longitudinal and experimental in design, was to examine the effect of a one-
year dance specific fitness program on undergraduate modern dance students
undertaking full-time training.
METHOD
Participants
Screening information from first year students (n=86) provided initial data
on a series of physiological tests, including the Dance Aerobic Fitness Test
(DAFT; Wyon et al. 2003), the Wingate Anaerobic bike Test (WAnT), and the
vertical jump height test, which were repeated at the end of the one-year in-
tervention. Students also completed an evaluation questionnaire regarding
their personal appreciation of the program. Existing screening information on
the previous student cohort (n=85) provided an opportunity to compare
physiological changes across the year without the fitness intervention.
Procedure
A weekly 90 minute fitness class was developed across the year according to
principles of periodization and specificity (Bompa 1999). The primary aim
was for the structure and content to be responsive to curriculum needs and to
engage in a dialogue with technique teachers and therapists. Intensity and
duration of exercises were considered. The dancers heart rates were regularly
monitored to ensure that the intensity level was appropriate to elicit a train-
ing response. Functional fitness training preceded more dance-based move-
ment that increasingly replicated vocabulary from technique classes. For
example, plyometric training was introduced, initially using basic parallel foot
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 93
250
200
b.min-1
Pre
150
Post
100
50
1 2 3 4 5
DAFT 5 stages
Figure 1. Mean heart rates of dancers undertaking dance specific fitness training before
and after the one year intervention.
positions, and later modified to include turned out positions that more closely
mimicked the type of jumps seen in dance. Upper body strength exercises
gradually progressed to incorporate partner lifting of varying speeds and
complexities. During the final phase, a circuit type structure reflected the
variety of activity and speed of succession that would be encountered in a
dance class.
All data were entered into SPSS (v.12). Means and standard deviations were
calculated for all variables. Dependant t-tests were used to monitor changes
in heart rate and jump height data collected from the pre- and post-testing.
RESULTS
Results from the pre- and post-screening assessments show that, following
the intervention, there was a significant decrease (p<0.05) in mean heart
rates across all five stages of the DAFT test (Figure 1), indicating improve-
ment in the dancers aerobic capabilities. In addition, it appears that this par-
ticular group of students displayed lower heart rates at every stage, compared
with the group of first year students from the previous year who did not en-
gage in regular fitness training (Table 1).
The females in the intervention group improved their jump height
(p<0.05), while the males did not. Participant responses to the evaluation
94 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 1. Mean heart rates across each stage of the DAFT for three different groups of
dance students. There was a significant difference between the two groups at every stage
(p<0.05).
DISCUSSION
Results from the DAFT test show that aerobic fitness improved over the one-
year period, supporting findings of previous studies (Wanke 2001). When
comparing the DAFT heart rate data to that of the dancers who did not par-
ticipate (from the previous Year 1 cohort), there appears to be an overall re-
duction at each stage, indicating that it is the fitness training intervention
that has contributed to this effect.
Comments from the evaluation questionnaire show that students have gained
knowledge and skills that they do not acquire at any other point in their dance
training and are capable of applying this knowledge and understanding to
their wider physical development. The most frequent physiological self-
observed effects were reductions in fatigue, improvement in general energy
levels, and improved capacity in dance classes to sustain technique and
jumping ability. The importance of warm-up and cool down was commonly
cited and the recognition of the relationship between fitness and injury
prevention was highlighted. In psychological terms, preliminary analysis of
the comments reveals that students have found fitness valuable as a relaxa-
tion tool. They explain that they feel more positive and confident in general
and find a reduction in stress, tension, and frustration. Reasons include the
holistic approach, the absence of competition in the fitness class, and the
increased knowledge, awareness, and value of relaxation techniques as an
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 95
References
Redding E. and Wyon M. A. (2003). Strengths and weaknesses of current methods for
evaluating the aerobic power of dancers. Journal of Dance Medicine and Science, 7,
pp. 10-16.
Rimmer J. H., Jay D., and Plowman S. A. (1994). Physiological characteristics of trained
dancers and intensity level of ballet class and rehearsal. Impulse, 2, pp. 97-105.
Rist R. A. (1994). Children and exercise: A dance medicine perspective. Sportcare Jour-
nal, 1, pp. 1-7.
Schantz P. G. and Astrand P. O. (1984). Physiological characteristics of classical ballet.
Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise, 16, pp. 472-476.
Wanke E. M. (2001). Breathless: Results of a long term study of an integrated endur-
ance training in professional dancers. Paper presented at the Eleventh Annual
Meeting of the International Association of Dance Medicine and Science, Madrid,
Spain.
Wyon M. A. and Redding E. (2005). Physiological monitoring of cardiorespiratory ad-
aptations during rehearsal and performance of contemporary dance. Journal of
Strength and Conditioning Research, 19, pp. 611-614.
Wyon M. A., Redding E., Abt G. et al. (2003). Development, reliability and validity of a
multi-stage dance specific aerobic fitness test (DAFT). Journal of Dance Medicine
and Science, 7, pp. 80-84.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Training methods within the performing arts have typically been based upon
tradition and personal experience. In addition to these, within many areas of
performance, a broader range of programs are becoming increasingly em-
ployed with the aim of equipping performers with skills necessary for effec-
tive performance preparation and career management. However, some of
98 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
these programs are developed and implemented with little empirical support
for their efficacy. In dance, for instance, educators are being encouraged to
review and assess how training is provided (Kimmerle and Cte-Laurence
2003). From the viewpoint of researchers, performers, and those who train
performers, it is important that these programs be subjected to empirical
testing in order to provide an unbiased assessment of how they can be em-
ployed to enhance performance, not to mention a level of quality assurance
regarding their efficacy.
This paper highlights methods for empirically developing and testing in-
tervention programs with a focus on evidence-based outcomes. In particular,
a musician-specific mental skills training program is evaluated qualitatively
and quantitatively in terms of its potential to enhance learning and perform-
ing skills.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
state anxiety prior to performing, while trait anxiety was measured using the
trait anxiety index from the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (TAI,
Spielberger et al. 1970). Lastly, each participant gave a 15-minute public per-
formance comprising two contrasting pieces of their choice in order to meas-
ure performance ability.
Qualitative methods involved feedback from the experimental group col-
lected during and following the training phase concerning students views on
the relevance and usefulness of the program, focus groups conducted with the
experimental group following the training and final testing phase, and case
notes kept by the first author throughout the project.
Procedure
Participants from both the experimental and control groups first attended a
session during which they completed most of the questionnaires. Following
this, each participant performed their chosen pieces in a public performance.
Immediately prior to performing, the participants completed the self-efficacy
for performing questionnaire and the CSAI-2R. The performances were
videoed and assessed for quality by two external adjudicators. Upon comple-
tion of the first round of testing, a musician-specific mental skills training
program was delivered to the experimental group. The topics covered fell in
three main categories: (1) motivation and effective practice, (2) relaxation and
arousal control, and (3) performance preparation and enhancement. The
training phase involved one 60-minute group session and one 30-minute
individual session per week for nine weeks. The control group, meanwhile,
received no additional training. Following the training phase, the experi-
mental and control groups again completed the questionnaires and public
performances, using a procedure identical to the first round of testing. De-
scriptive statistics for the questionnaires were initially calculated from the
two rounds of testing, following which repeated-measures analyses of vari-
ance (ANOVAs) were employed to elucidate changes occurring from one
testing phase to the next.
RESULTS
Quantitative results
Table 1. Significant results from repeated-measures ANOVAs. The mean scores (and
standard deviations) for the experimental (EG) and control groups (CG) for both phases
of testing are provided, as are the resulting F values.
Pre-test Post-test F
Self-regulated learning EC 48.69 (6.21) 48.69 (4.51) F1,21=6.22, p<0.01
CG 51.18 (6.34) 51.18 (4.90)
Quantity of practice EC 4.39 (1.15) 4.39 (1.35) F1,21=9.64, p<0.01
CG 4.27 (1.35) 4.27 (1.06)
Technical proficiency EC 4.81 (0.66) 4.81 (0.73) F1,21=5.59, p<0.01
CG 5.18 (0.75) 5.18 (0.83)
Self-efficacy EC 41.94 (5.09) 41.94 (5.03) F1,21=6.88, p<0.01
CG 45.00 (6.18) 45.00 (6.24)
Betts QMI: Total EC 105.44 (26.13) 105.44 (26.22) F1,21=9.26, p<0.01
CG 78.36 (16.63) 78.36 (21.92)
Betts QMI: Sensations EC 16.00 (6.09) 16.00 (4.80) F1,21=5.66, p<0.01
CG 11.09 (3.18) 11.09 (3.89)
Betts QMI: Touch EC 15.63 (4.65) 15.63 (5.43) F1,21=6.75, p<0.01
CG 10.64 (4.06) 10.64 (4.51)
Qualitative feedback
The results in Table 1 were further supported by comments from the partici-
pants revealing that, as a result of taking part in the training program, they
were experiencing improved practice efficiency, greater levels of self-aware-
ness and confidence when performing, facilitative views toward and a height-
ened sense of control over anxiety, and healthier perspectives toward music
making.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 101
Feedback from the participants also provided insight into issues pertain-
ing to the content and delivery of the program. The participants felt that the
training program could have been strengthened with the inclusion of more
case studies and less information on research findings, more examples of
practical application, and a greater use of class discussion and activities to
facilitate learning from one another.
The case notes, as well as information derived from the follow-up focus
group, suggest that people may respond to a training program individually,
particularly in terms of their willingness to engage with some of the content
areas. Given this, prior to program implementation, it would seem instructive
to develop a sense of participants willingness to engage in change so that the
program could best be designed and delivered based on the participants
wants and needs.
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank Christopher Connolly, Caroline Marlow, and Melanie Gregg for their
assistance with the development of the mental skills training program.
Terry Clark, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort
Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: tclark@rcm.ac.uk
References
Cox R., Martens M., and Russell W. (2003). Measuring anxiety in athletics: The Revised
Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
25, pp. 519-533.
Kimmerle M. and Cte-Laurence P. (2003). Teaching Dance Skills. Andover, New Jer-
sey, USA: Michael Ryan Publishing.
Sheehan P. (1967). A shortened form of Betts questionnaire upon mental imagery.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, pp. 386-389.
Spielberger C. D., Gorsuch R. L., and Lushene R. F. (1970). STAI: Manual for the State-
Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, California, USA: Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Zimmerman B. J. (1986). Becoming a self-regulated learner: Which are the key sub-
processes? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, pp. 307-313.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
urements for the whole piece have been made). It divides the fabric of the
Prelude into two rhythmic layers: the solo voice in the right hand and the
sequence of accompanying chords in the left hand each have their own
rhythmic structure (see Figure 2) and are analyzed separately. Finally, the
timing phenomena are related to the score in a speculative process of inverse
interpretation: inspired by Timmers et al. (2000), we assume that there is a
great number of possible ways to analyze the structure of a composition. We
do not analyze the compositional structure first, and map it to the perform-
ance data later, but the other way around. We inspect the timing data and try
to separate expected phenomena from Argerichs individual approach. Then,
the score of the piece is analyzed in order to find the properties that are high-
lighted by Argerichs interpretation. This analysis is done strictly from a lis-
teners point of view: we are not trying to reconstruct Argerichs thoughts or
intentions. But we do try to articulate which sense or meaning weas listen-
ersmight discover in Argerichs interpretation of Chopins piece.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
Method
Our method to secure tone onset times seeks to meet two requirements: it
must allow us to differentiate tone onsets in two separate rhythmic layers
(solo voice and accompaniment), and the accuracy of the measurements must
be within 10 ms. Our approach implies two steps. First, we marked the onsets
roughly in a sonogram (window size=4410 samples, hop size=441 samples);
this places the timing in a range of 20 ms near the attack time. Since the
notes of the accompaniment chords are not necessarily struck at the same
time, only the onset of the loudest note of each chord was measured. In a
second step we adjusted the measurements with the help of filters and two
loudness measurements: a narrow bandpass filter (bandwith=100 Hz) was
employed to isolate the fundamental tone (or, if necessary, one of the first
partials) of a note; the delay caused by the filtering was compensated. Then,
the behavior of two loudness measurements was interpreted in order to de-
tect the precise timing of the onset. It was set to the moment when a fast peak
level (window size=1 ms) overshoots an RMS level (27 ms) which reacts early
but slowly to an energy surge. First tests with monophonic piano examples
suggest that this method places the timing markers within a range of 2-12 ms
after the physical onset; more systematic tests need yet to be conducted in
order to evaluate this (rather conservative) estimate. All measurements and
the visualizations of timing data in Figure 2 were made with the Lucerne Au-
dio Recording Analyzer (software freely available at www.hslu.ch/lara).
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 109
Figure 1. Average distribution of bar duration on the 8 eighth notes in the accompani-
ment in % (calculated with timing data from bars 1-11 and 13-22).
Analysis
In bars 1-11 and 13-22, the accompaniment of Chopins Prelude op. 28/4 con-
sists of a rhythmically uniform sequence of chords, pulsating in eighth notes.
Argerich plays these eighth notes with flexible rhythm; nevertheless, a gen-
eral bar profile is discernible.
Figure 1 shows the mean distribution of bar durations among the 8 eighth
notes of a bar throughout the whole piece (in % of bar duration). Two ritar-
dando zones can be identified: one strong ritardando spans over the bar line,
from eighth note 7 (13.1% of bar time) and the longest 8 (15.2%) to 1 (14.2%).
We would like to call this formation bar line ritardando. Another, much
more subtle lengthening concerns eighth note 5 (12.7%). We call this the
mid-bar ritardando. Besides the performance invariants reported in the
research literature (slowing down on phrase endings, melody lead), this aver-
age bar profile can serve us as a background for identifying Argerichs indi-
vidual reactions to particular configurations within the composition.
Figure 2 presents Argerichs rhythmic disposition of bars 1-4 alongside
the respective excerpt of the score. The rhythm diagram just above the score
(L for left hand) represents the timing of the accompaniment: every vertical
line on the horizontal timeline marks the timing of a detected onset. The
width (and consequently the height) of the squares represent the inter-onset-
intervals (IOI) on different metrical levels of the score. The smallest squares
denote IOIs between neighboring eighth notes, the biggest squares denote
IOIs between successive downbeatsi.e. they represent bar durations. All
IOIs are given as a tiny numerical value (in ms) in the upper part of each
square. The top rhythm diagram in Figure 2 (R for right hand) represents
the timing of the solo melody: the smaller squares stand for the anacrusis, the
dotted half notes, and the quarter notes, respectively. The biggest squares
visualize bar durations analogous to the lower diagram. Both diagrams (R+L)
are adjusted horizontally to the same timeline. The numbers between the
110 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 2. Rhythm diagram and score (according to G. Henle edition, 1968), bars 1-4.
diagrams represent differences between onsets in the right and the left hand,
whichaccording to the scoreoccupy the same metric position. Most of the
time, Argerich plays the solo voices onsets earlier than the accompaniments:
in bars 1-4, the melody leads in seven of nine instances (78%), the average
melody lead is 28 ms.
Argerich takes up the slow tempo of the anacrusis in the accompaniment.
In bar 1, she starts with a quite long first eighth note (807 ms) which takes
17.2% of bar duration; then she speeds up. No mid-bar ritardando is discerni-
ble in bar 1, but the bar line ritardando (eighth notes 7 and 8) is distinct. In
bar 2, Argerich starts fast but slows down considerably in the second half of
the bar. This tendency is even stronger in bar 3: the first half of bar 3 is fast
(28.7 bpm) and takes only 43.6% of bar duration (2092 ms), whereas the
second half is considerably slower (22.2 bpm) and lasts for 56.4% of bar du-
ration (2704 ms).
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 111
IMPLICATIONS
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank John Rink for giving the first idea to do this paper, Claudia
Emmenegger for typesetting the music example, and Richard Beaudoin for his careful
proofreading and inspiring collaboration. Many thanks go to the faculty and the stu-
dents of the Department of Music at Harvard University for their constructive com-
ments on an early draft of this paper.
Olivier Senn, Institute for Music Performance Studies, Lucerne School of Music, Zen-
tralstrasse 18, 6003 Lucerne, Switzerland; Email: olivier.senn@hslu.ch
References
Cristine MacKie
simpler solution, as will certain aspects of the rethink which has taken place
in recent years in musical academic circles, notably Berry (1989) and Rink
(2002) who have looked afresh at the relationship between performance and
analysis. These changes have made possible the informal analysis at the end
of this article, which seeks to demonstrate that the graduated pacing of the
ascent and descent of the melodic curves in Bruyres requires a subtle appli-
cation of retards and accelerations following the principle of the roller
coaster. Informal tools of analysis such as dcoupage, diagrams, and histori-
cal evidence are used to support this approach.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
The initial tempo from which the change starts, the number of steps
though which the change must move, a related tempo to be achieved at
the end of the time series: these elements set the temporal framework
within which an retard/acceleration must be shaped (Epstein 1995, p.
418).
He suggests, also, that the cubic curve, while mathematically complex, can
describe an act of timing such as the retard and acceleration which not only
feels natural and seemingly effortless to effect, but is compatible with our
own musical intuitions (Epstein 1995; see Figure 1 below). From this
mathematical concept, Epstein extrapolates the idea that the shape of the
curve could represent a possible timing trajectory in the moulding of a retard
and accelerando (p. 419).
In this pilot study, Epstein transferred performances of retards by Szell
and Stravinsky and accelerations by Karajan and Herrara de la Fuente onto
an analog audio tape and timed the durations of their successive beats with
the tape measure technique (p. 417). This enabled him to assemble a highly
correlated model of a retard and accelerando performance from which he
observed that their trajectory fitted within the model of the cubic curve with
striking similarity. This, he suggests, means that they all had a good compre-
hension of the boundary tempos that frame the timing path of their re-
tards and accelerations. Finally, a computer programme revealed that the
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 115
y z
w x
Figure 1. A reproduction of Epsteins (1995) diagram which shows: first, two horizontal
lines between w-x and y-z. These lines represent the steady, or predetermined, tempos.
Second, an s-shaped ascending curve links the horizontal line w and x to the horizontal
line at the uppermost point of the curve marked y and z.
Retardation Acceleration
Figure 2. A general representation of the curve of a roller coaster. The arrow on the left
shows the path of the retard of the roller coaster as it ascends toward the peak of the
curve. The arrow on the right shows the path of acceleration of the roller coaster as it
descends on the downward side of the curve.
retards and accelerandos measured in the study consistently fitted within the
cubic curve model with less than 7% variance. Epstein concluded that the
cubic model does about as well as it is possible to do (p. 423).
Since the time constraints on a performers life may discourage the study of
the models described above in detail, it may be more effective for him or her
to observe the similarities between the shape of a melodic curve in Bruyres
and the path of the roller coaster as it rises and falls (this being closely related
to the cubic curve). The energy of the roller coaster, during its fall, is trans-
formed from potential energy to kinetic energy (energy in motion) and, dur-
ing its rise, from kinetic energy to potential energy (stored energy). Thus,
during the ascent of the roller coaster there is a retardation as it draws toward
the top of the curve, and an acceleration as it descends on the other side of
the curve (see Figure 2).
116 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 3. An informal analysis of bars 9-13 of Bruyres by Debussy. (1) Above the score,
the contour of the melodic curve is highlighted with a black line to expose it more
clearly, (2) level 1 shows the prolonged cadential memory device, (3) level 2 shows the
deployment of the cadences irregulires at an intermediate level of hearing, (4) the
dotted lines on both sides below level 2 are an approximation of the area of the prede-
termined tempo, 66 to the quarter note.
Figure 4. The curve represents the path of the roller coaster as it ascends and descends.
The vertical lines below represent the hypothetical cadential points of support.
The analysis
This informal analysis seeks to expose elements that will support the pacing
of the ascent and descent of the melodic curves in Bruyres. Since the aim is
to apply a subtle application of retards and accelerations following the princi-
ple of the roller coaster, it will, first, expose the shape of the melodic curve
between bars 9-13, since curves not angles are the very nature of this compo-
sition (Schmitz 1950, p. 172), and second, demonstrate that the role of the
perfect cadence and the cadences irregulires support the pacing of the
curve. Dcoupage and a diagram will be employed as part of the analytical
process. For example, in bars 9-13, the vertical bar lines have been removed
to expose a complete melodic curve. Its contour is highlighted above with a
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 117
black line (see Figure 3) to reveal the similarity of the rise and fall of the mu-
sical curve with the ascending and descending path of the roller coaster. Thus,
just as the energy of the roller coaster is transformed from kinetic to potential
energy as it draws toward the peak of the curve, bars 9-10 may be performed
with a subtle departure from the predetermined tempo of approx 66 to the
quarter note, and a retardation applied toward the Eb at the top of the me-
lodic curve.
In the same way in which the roller coaster uses kinetic energy during its
descent of the curve to fuel the rest of the ride (Epstein 1995, p. 27), per-
formers may plan their descent of the curve with a graduated accelerative
pacing through bars 10-13 and gradually rejoin the predetermined tempo of
66 to the quarter note smoothly in bar 13.
However, the pacing of the ritardando and accelerando, like the roller
coaster, needs some means of support or stability. This article suggests that
this is provided by Debussys deployment of a hierarchical arrangement of
cadential points (see Figure 4).
At level 1 (see bars 10-13, Figure 3), the function of the perfect cadence
may be seen as a prolonged cadential memory device which serves to main-
tain a sense of unity in a work that otherwise might be heard as a series of
fragmentary and isolated melodic curves. The dominant chord of the cadence
is placed under the highest point of the melodic curve on the first beat of bar
10, and it does not resolve onto the tonic, Ab major, until the first beat of bar
13. This, alongside the added energy of the accacciatura in the base, contrib-
utes stability to the pacing of the retardation, pushing the music up and for-
ward to the top of the melodic curve like the car on the roller coaster. The
positioning of the tonic chord Ab on the first beat of bar 13 enhances the
sense of closure as the car descends to the end of the melodic curve.
At level 2 (see Figure 3), the cadences irregulires are identifiable by the
harmonic progression using the subdominant with an added sixth, which
then resolves onto the tonic. These may be seen first between the third beat of
bar 8 and the first beat of bar 9 (see Figure 3) and similarly between bars 12
and 13. In both instances, their function is to support the pacing of a subtle
retard toward the end of the descent of the melodic curve.
Conclusion
The similarity of the melodic curves in Bruyres with the roller coasters track
and the degree of slope and corresponding retardation and acceleration with
its coefficient of friction at all points along the way (Epstein 1995, p.27),
which in this case is supported by two different types of cadences, is clear to
118 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
see. If the performer plans and controls this recurring accumulation and re-
lease of tension, which together affect the pacing of the retardation and accel-
eration during the ascent and descent of the melodic curve, this will, as
Epstein points out lead the car to its designated end, its motion controlled
throughout (1995, p. 27). If not, the pacing of the ascent and descent of the
melodic curve, like the car of the roller coaster, may be spent too soon and the
intended musical effect, which is to control a graduated pacing of the ascent
and descent of the melodic curve from the predetermined tempo of approxi-
mately 66 to the quarter note, to the same related tempo at the downward
end of curve may be dissipated.
IMPLICATIONS
Future research may consider associating the graduated pacing of retards and
accelerandos in piano performance with the kinetics of body movements. For
example, bodily movements may be organized to reduce any angular move-
ments in performance by planning ideal trajectories of bodily motion that
take into account the demands of the musical score.
Acknowledgments
I wish to acknowledge Euan MacKie and Bruce Paterson for their invaluable comments
and proof reading.
References
Debussy C. (1913). Prelude Book 2, No. 5 Bruyres. London: Boosey and Hawkes.
Epstein D. (1995). Shaping Time. New York: Schirmer Books.
Rink J. (1995). The Practice of Performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schmitz R. (1966). The Piano Works of Claude Debussy. New York: Dover Publications.
Sunberg V. and Verillo V. (1980). On the anatomy of the retard: A study of timing in
music. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 68, pp. 772-779.
Saltzer F. (1982). Structural Hearing (volumes 1 and 2). New York: Dover Publications.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Many renowned pianists perform with great success up to old ages (e.g.
Backhaus played his last concert at 85, Horowitz at 84, Arrau at 88). The de-
mands posed by performing publicly are enormous (motor skills, memory,
physical endurance, stress factors; see Williamon 2004). Theories of human
life-span development identify three factors to be mainly responsible for
successful aging: selection, optimization, and compensation (SOC model,
Baltes and Baltes 1990). Applied to piano performance this would imply that
older pianists play a smaller repertoire (selection), practice these few pieces
more (optimization), and hide technical deficiencies by reducing the tempo of
fast passages while maintaining tempo contrasts between fast and slow pas-
sages (compensation) (Vitouch 2005).
In this study, we examine a unique corpus of Chopin performances by
Nikita Magaloff, recorded on stage at age 77. We test whether Magaloff actu-
ally used strategies identified in the SOC model to master this unprecedented
project. First, we assess his performance by quantifying performance errors.
Second, we analyze recordings of the etudes by other renowned pianists to
test whether Magaloffs performance tempi were slower than those of the
120 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Materials
In Spring 1989, Magaloff performed the entire work of Chopin for solo piano
that was published during Chopins lifetime (Op.1-64) in six public appear-
ances at the Vienna Konzerthaus. These concerts were recorded with a
Bsendorfer computer-controlled grand piano that provides a huge set of
symbolic performance data with highest precision156 pieces over 320,000
performed notes; about 10 hours of performed music.
To put Magaloffs etudes performances into context, recordings of the
etudes by the following performers were also analyzed (a total of 289 per-
formances): Arrau (recorded 1956), Ashkenazy (1975), Backhaus (1928), Biret
(1990), Cortot (1934), Gavrilov (1985), Giusiano (2006), Harasiewicz (1961),
Lortie (1986), Lugansky (1999), Magaloff (1975), Magaloff (1989), Pollini
(1972), Schirmer (2003), Shaboyan (2007), and Sokolov (1985).
The 14 recordings of the Nocturne Op.15 No.1 were by Argerich (1965),
Arrau (1978), Ashkenazy (1985), Barenboim (1981), Harasiewicz (1961),
Horowitz (1957), Leonskaja (1992), Maisenberg (1995), Magaloff (1975),
Perahia (1994), Pires (96), Pollini (68), Richter (68), and Rubinstein (1965).
Procedure
Table 1. Error % by piece category and error type (i.e. insertion, deletion, substitution).
RESULTS
Performance errors
Table 2 shows the tempo modes obtained for all pianists. Each performance is
named by the first two letters of the pianist, followed by the pianists age at
the time of the recording. For the sake of comparison the metronome indica-
tions from the Henle Edition (Zimmermann 1983) were added (HEN). In 12
of the 18 pieces, Magaloffs tempo (MA) is within a 10% range of the Henle
indications. Three pieces are more than 5% slower and three pieces more
than 5% faster compared with the metronome markings. Compared with the
performances of 14 other recordings (including an earlier performance by
Magaloff in 1975) Magaloffs performances of the Op.10 etudes are on average
1.2% slower than the average over all other recordings. The Op.25 etudes are
on average about 5.6% slower than the average performance.
Comparing Magaloffs recordings at the age of 63 and 77, the tempi vary
to a surprising degree, but no systematic tempo decrease in the latter can be
122 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 2. Tempo modes of different pianist for selected pieces from Op.10 and Op.25.
Entries are named by the first two letters of the pianists name and age at recording.
HEN
MA77
1.6 PO26
80 AR75
RU78
1.4
RU78
70 HEN
1.2
ARG24
60 AR75 1
r=0.621* n=14 p=0.0178 r=0.086 n=14 p=0.7707
50 0.8
20 40 60 80 20 40 60 80
Age (years) Age (years)
Figure 1. Nocturne Op.15 No.1 by 14 pianists and Magaloff: basic tempo of middle sec-
tion (left) and tempo ratio between middle and first section (right) against performers
age. Dashed lines indicate given tempo (left) or tempo ratio (right) by Henle edition.
found. On the contrary, in 12 pieces out of 18, the recording at age 77 is faster,
sometimes to a considerable degree (up to 17% in Op.10 No.10). On the
whole, no significant correlation of age and tempo could be established.
DISCUSSION
Based on the fact that Magaloff performed the entire piano works by Chopin,
we can refute the selection part of the SOC model. Due to missing informa-
tion about his practice regime before and during the performance period, we
cannot make a statement about optimization processes. Magaloffs tempi do
not point to compensation processes, which were indeed found with other
famous pianists. However, his relatively high error rates may indicate that
124 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Magaloff aimed at realizing his musical ideas of Chopins work rather than at
error-free performances. In sum, Magaloffs data does not seem to corrobo-
rate the SOC model. This study is the first of its kind to examine a huge cor-
pus of symbolic performance data of the entire work of a composer and to put
it into context of a substantial number of other recordings.
Note
A basic tempo value was estimated by the mode value, the most frequent bin of an in-
terbeat interval histogram with a bin size of 4% of the mean inter-beat interval.
Acknowledgments
Funded by the Austrian National Research Fund (FWF), project no. P19349-N15.
References
Cathy Aggett
METHOD
Data were collected from two sources, the first being a group of n=14 profes-
sional singers from America (2), Australia (7), Canada (1), New Zealand (3),
and Spain (1) in a study from 2006-07. Six were male, seven female, and their
voice types were soprano (6), mezzo soprano (2), tenor (4), baritone (1), and
bass/baritone (1). The professional singers completed a series of three email
questionnaires, the first of which sought responses about the selection and
subsequent preparation of twentieth and twenty-first century art song reper-
toire from a program for a recent performance. The second email question-
naire sought information on learning and preparing one twentieth and
twenty-first century art song contained in the submitted program. Partici-
pants were requested to send a score of the song, which they could annotate
to illustrate their answers. Specific and generic questions (relating to any
twentieth and twenty-first century art song) about learning strategies, proce-
dures (i.e. order of learning), and the use of vocal techniques and challenges
were asked in the third and final email questionnaire. The resulting qualita-
tive data was categorized into a list of either performance or learning strate-
gies. The draft list of strategies (n=50) was informed by a list of strategies
drawn from the initial literature review, combined, and made available to
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 129
Materials
The 34 songs for the recital were chosen as a result of an extensive search of
all the Australian art songs in the Australian Music Centre (submitted from
2005-09) and other sources including the Mitchell (Sydney, New South
Wales) and National (Canberra, Australian Capital Territory) libraries, in an
attempt to select a repertoire of songs suitable for all voice types and ad-
dressing a range of pedagogical aspects.
RESULTS
Eighty-four strategies drawn from the literature review and first group of
participants were categorized and coded as being musical (M), performance
(P), or contextual (C) according to themes evident in the literature. Many of
these strategies can be viewed as performance (self-regulatory) strategies for
the singer and/or learning strategies (for a teacher or singer). Performance
strategies (n=55) included overall strategies to help prepare a piece (10), pitch
strategies (12), rhythm strategies (5), textual strategies (7), vocal techniques
(7), and ensemble and accompaniment strategies (7). Learning strategies
(n=29) included general strategies (14), order of learning (8), pitch strategies
(1), aural strategies (1), and strategies for preparing a whole song cycle or
program (4).
The second group of participants adopted n=118 performance strategies
42 musical, 49 performance, and 27 contextual; a total of n=43 learning
strategies were adopted, with 21 being musical, 11 performance, and 11 con-
textual, with singers introducing 13 new strategies to the list (see Table 1).
130 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 1. Strategy use in relation to recurring musical (M), performance (P), and con-
textual (C) themes existing in the literature on and about twentieth and twenty-first
century solo vocal music and Australian song. The performance and learning strategies
used by professional singers as reported in the study conducted in 2006-07 were sorted
into M, P, and C themes, some of which were adopted by the three professional singers
in a 2008 recital.
M P C M P C
Occurrence in literature 163 70 269
DISCUSSION
mands specific to the repertoire. Both an ensemble and pitch learning strat-
egy used with the sixth song of Peggy Glanville Hicks 13 Ways of Looking at
a Blackbird involved the accompanist playing the notes of each bar as a block
chord to familiarize the singer with the tonality, gradually adding in the
rhythm.
The use and application of performance rather than learning strategies,
drawn from literature and the experience of professional singers, were found
to be effective in striving for performing excellence when preparing twentieth
and twenty-first century Australian art song. Categorizing strategies by the
themes apparent in the literature and cross-referencing them with the experi-
ences of professional singers can offer singers and singing teachers a way of
tackling the challenges offered by twentieth and twenty-first century vocal art
song.
Acknowledgments
This paper was written with the assistance of a Graduate Student International Travel
Assistance Award from the Australian Music and Psychology Society.
Cathy Aggett, School of Communication Arts, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag
1797, Penrith South DC, New South Wales 1797, Australia; Email: c.aggett@uws.edu.au
132 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
References
Aggett C. (2008). Singers strategies for performing and learning 20th Century Austra-
lian art songs. Paper presented at the Twenty-eighth World Conference of the In-
ternational Society for Music Education, Bologna, Italy.
Barry N. H. and Hallam S. (2002). Practice. In R. Parncutt and G. E. McPherson (eds.),
The Science and Psychology of Music Performance (pp. 151-165). Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Emmons S. and Thomas A. (1998). Power Performance for Singers. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Fine P. and Ginsborg J. (2007). How singers influence the understanding of sung text.
In A. Williamon and D. Coimbra (eds.), Proceedings of ISPS 2007 (pp. 253-258).
Utrecht, The Netherlands: European Association of Conservatoires (AEC).
Ginsborg J. and Sloboda J. A. (2007). Singers recall for the words and melody of a new,
unaccompanied song. Psychology of Music, 35, pp. 421-440.
Hallam S. (1997). The development of memorisation strategies in musicians: Implica-
tions for education. British Journal of Music Education, 14, pp. 87-97.
Jrgensen H. (2004). Strategies for individual practice. In A. Williamon (ed.), Musical
Excellence (pp. 85-103). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Killian J. N. and Henry M. L. (2005) A comparison of successful and unsuccessful
strategies in individual sight-singing preparation and performance. Journal of Re-
search in Music Education, 53, pp. 51-65.
Mabry S. (2002). Exploring Twentieth-Century Vocal Music. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
McPherson G. E. and Zimmerman B. J. (2002). Self-regulation of musical learning. A
social cognitive perspective. In R. Cowell and C. Richardson (eds.), The New Hand-
book of Research on Music Teaching and Learning (pp. 327-347). New York: The
National Association for Music Education.
Nielsen S. (1999). Learning strategies in instrumental music practice. British Journal of
Music Education, 16, pp. 275-291.
Rink J. (2002). Analysis and (or?) performance. In J. Rink (ed.), Musical Performance
(pp. 35-58). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sundberg J., Iwarsson J., and Hagegrd H. (1994). A singers expression of emotions in
sung performance. Speech, Music and Hearing Quarterly Progress and Status Re-
port, 35, pp. 81-92.
Williamon A. (2004). Musical excellence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The aim of the present study was to find out if there were significant dif-
ferences between the learning student, the supervising professor, and the
expert performer working in a symphony orchestra regarding the devel-
opment of their ability to perform. Survey results demonstrate that all
groups showed a secure level of understanding of the concepts of deliber-
ate practice and self-awareness, while those of metacognition were prac-
tically unknown. While students reported the most practice of all groups,
they rarely record themselves or use computer technology. All partici-
pants were highly supported by their parents especially at the time when
they decided to become a professional. The instrumental diversity of size,
scordatura, playing postures or bowing schools was seen as an advantage
rather than a disadvantage, overruling statements of previously under-
taken research. The study highlights the importance of early pre-profes-
sional learning for the university student.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Together with the empirical approach adapted to this study, literature was
reviewed comprising the areas of (1) the learner and (2) the instrument. The
first part focused on practice, expertise, task requirements, and biographical
issues. A second part reviewed the history of the double bass, its performer,
and their influences on the development of expertise of the instrument and its
role in the repertoire. Many of the findings that came out of the historical
reviewsuch as the diversity of at least two bow schools, different playing
postures and tuningsare still subject to controversial discussion today.
These concerns were included in the survey to examine the impact of history
and tradition on the present generation. The blueprint of the survey (see Fig-
ure 2) was based on a multidimensional learning model to instrumental
practice conceived by Hallam (1997).
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 135
Procedure
The data collected from the survey were processed through the statistical
program SPSS 17.0, taking into account the most adequate statistical tech-
136 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
nique to the involved variables. The data were organized according to the
nature of variables, most of them qualitative and descriptive techniques.
Principal component analysis was used to reduce the dimensionality and to
identify profiles considering biography, education, etc. To compare the re-
sults, parametric Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) or non parametric Kruskal
Wallis techniques were used. For the other topics, some independent chi-
squared tests were performed to evaluate the association between the differ-
ent groups of PROF, ORCH, and STUD and the correspondent item. The de-
cision rule used to detect significant statistical evidence was a probability (p)
value less than 0.05. In addition, the survey provided 204 individual com-
ments to open-ended questions which were assembled for further discussion
according to the topics and group of population.
RESULTS
Results from the survey revealed that the double bass is still a male domi-
nated instrument. Male participants were dominant in the group of the PROF
(79%), STUD (72%), and ORCH (100%). No female orchestra bassist partici-
pated in the survey. The mean age of PROF was 50 years male and 41 years
female, ORCH 41 years (only male), and STUD 23 years male and 21 years
female. The average age for starting the double bass for all groups was be-
tween 14-15 years. However, the starting age ranged from 3-20 years. A high
percentage of all groups previously gained experience on other instruments,
which they learned for 4-7 years before moving to the double bass at the age
of 13-15. The late and most diverse starting age reveals a relative inconsis-
tency when comparing to other instruments such as the violin. These findings
confirm those from a similar study undertaken by Langner (2003).
The majority of the ORCH group (52%) practices regularly only when they
have important performances, while this is done less by PROF (36%) and
STUD (25%). This may be because PROF and STUD have a different time
frame to build up repertoire in advance. They may also have more freedom in
choosing their repertoire, while the ORCH group normally has to deal with an
intense and pre-given performance schedule. Additional comments were
made on these issues, which confirm these views. The PROF (78%) and
ORCH (76%) reported that they practice between 1-3 hours daily, while this
was assumed by 60% of the STUD; 35% of STUD reported practicing 4-6
hours per day. In all groups, between 54-67% of the participants tended to
maintain a practice free day through the week. STUD (73%), unlike PROF and
ORCH (38%), consider studying etudes as a key point of their practice. The
landscape of practice contents was fairly even and consistent in all groups
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 137
DISCUSSION
Results of this study revealed a strong relation and influence between teach-
ers and their students. The group of the PROF was the most experienced in all
performance domains and had also the highest academic qualification when
compared to the ORCH and the STUD. The latter, however, seemed to follow
their PROF in being active in all domains, although professional opportuni-
ties such as performing in festivals are yet rather more limited for them. Early
instrumental learning on the double bass has been identified as one of the
138 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Fundao para a Cincia e Tecnologia and Programa
Operacional Cincia e Inovao 2010 (POCI 2010), co-funded by the Portuguese Gov-
ernment and European Union by the FEDER Program.
Florian Pertzborn, Escola Superior de Musica ESMAE/IPP, Rua de Alegria 504, 4200
Porto, Portugal; Email: florianpertzborn@esmae-ipp.pt
References
The interest and curiosity about the lives of exceptional scientists, artists, or
athletes has always encouraged researchers to search for the reasons and
determinants of being creative and attaining high levels of performance. In
science and performing arts, pioneering work by Roe (1946), Bloom (1985),
140 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Gardner (1993), and Feist (2006) has inspired many researchers. For in-
stance, Lubinski et al. (2001) tracked intellectually precocious youth over 25
years, reaching conclusions about stimulating early educational experiences,
mentoring relationships, early development of interests, and strong commit-
ment to career and work as significant attributes in developing exceptional
expertise in science. Recently, Feist (2006) conducted and reviewed several
studies on personality and development of scientists, arguing for the need of a
new psychology of science for a complete understanding of scientific
thought and behavior. Conversely, research in dance seems to be still devel-
oping, focusing essentially on classical and non-professional dancers, and
more negative topics, such as eating behavior disorders, injury, stress, or
physical and biomechanical issues (see Krasnow and Kabanni 1999). Still,
some efforts have been developed towards a more complete dance science,
with studies focusing on professional and elite performing artists and psy-
chological issues, such as personality profiles, expertise, or motivation (e.g.
Hays 2002, Kogan 2002, Urea 2004).
Moreover, several theoretical models have been developed to explain the
process of performing excellence in different domains (see Arajo et al.
2007). The theoretical models of Renzulli (2002) or Gagn (2004), explaining
talent development, Ericssons deliberate practice and expert performance
approach (Ericsson and Charness 1994), as well as the Berlin school para-
digm on the study of wisdom (Baltes and Staudinger 2000) represent some of
the strongest approaches that influence current empirical research. Overall,
findings suggest that singularity of exceptional individuals seems to emerge
from a dynamic combination of contextual and personal factors.
Despite the vast amount of research on excellent performance, several is-
sues remain ambiguous. Definitions of excellence are still vague and multiple
criteria are used to identify excellent performance (Weiss et al. 2002, Trost
2000, Urea 2004, Zannotto 2006). Additionally, little is known about the
trajectories of excellent individuals from their own thoughts and meanings.
Quantitative inquiry has been favored in the study of performance excellence,
even if the singularity of successful trajectories and the complexity associated
with performing excellence in adulthood is assumed. Nevertheless, qualitative
methods are being gradually accepted as a valuable way to investigate singu-
lar and complex topics in-depth. Hence, a qualitative case study was con-
ducted aiming to analyze the singularities and commonalities behind talent
and success trajectories. In this paper, we present general findings and dis-
cuss some lessons learned from this research.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 141
METHOD
Participants
Two female dancers (contemporary dance) and two scientists (life sciences,
female and physics, male) were consensually identified by a panel of experts
in each specific domain. Participants were all nominated for revealing excel-
lence in performance and being actively engaged in their respective field. The
ages of participants were 36, 41, 38, and 43, respectively.
Materials
The interview protocol was created after reviewing relevant literature and
interview guides successfully used in previous studies with exceptional indi-
viduals in different domains (e.g. Connaughton et al. 2007, Kiewra and Cres-
well 2000). A semi-structured protocol covering the following main areas in a
fluid and flexible sequence was used: (1) career path, (2) past achievements
and actual performance, (3) expertise acquisition, (4) personal characteris-
tics, (5) role models and significant others, and (6) relationships within each
professional community. In addition, participants academic and professional
pathway and productivity were analyzed through their actualized curricula
vitae.
Procedure
ticipated issues could arise during interviews. Transcripts were sorted into
three main dimensions: (1) contextual factors, (2) personal factors, and (3)
answers to a specific question: what makes you an excellent professional.
Texts were coded and analyzed using Maxqda qualitative analysis software
and following the proposals of Schilling (2006) and Mayring (2000) on
qualitative content analysis. Some validity procedures were used such as tri-
angulation, member checking, and peer-debriefing (Onwuegbuzie and Leech
2007).
RESULTS
DISCUSSION
In this study, the role of positive emotional states and strong psychological
features are clearly associated with performing excellence, in addition to
contextual factors. However, we observed a slight difference between artists
and scientists, mainly on working strategies and career trajectories. Whereas
careers in science are more organized and sequential, trajectories in dance are
irregular and unstable. Scientists seem to plan more, following rigid sched-
ules, while dancers have more flexible agendas and engage in multiple activi-
ties other than physical training, which seems to contribute to their technical
and performance skills (Kogan 2002). Furthermore, this study provided a
rich description of individuals trajectories, showing several important condi-
tions needed to excel in a small and peripheral country such as Portugal. Par-
ticipants critical picture of the Portuguese educational and professional
system questions its role in the identification and encouragement of potential
talents, suggesting the importance of mentorship and awareness of creative,
sometimes unconventional, accomplishments. Not withstanding some well
known limitations of qualitative case studies, we believe that more qualitative
research is needed in order to promote a deeper understanding of performing
excellence. A vast amount of research with scientists exists, but contemporary
dancers are as yet under studied. Hence, the present case study suggests that
scientists and dancers personal wellbeing, as well as their career develop-
ment patterns, are interesting topics for further research efforts.
References
Arajo L. S., Almeida L. S., and Cruz J. F. (2007). Excellence in achievement contexts:
Psychological science applications and future directions. In A. Williamon and D.
Coimbra (eds.), Proceedings of ISPS 2007 (pp. 17-22). Utrecht, The Netherlands:
European Association of Conservatoires (AEC).
Baltes P. B. and Staudinger U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to
orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, pp. 122-
136.
Bloom B. (1985). Developing Talent in Young People. New York: Ballantine Books.
144 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Connaughton D., Wadey R., Hanton S., and Jones G. (2008). The development and
maintenance of mental toughness: Perceptions of elite performers. International
Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, pp. 83-95.
Ericsson K. A. and Charness N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisi-
tion. American Psychologist, 49, pp. 725-747.
Feist, G. (2006). The Psychology of Science and the Origins of Scientific Mind. London:
Yale University Press.
Gagn F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT as a developmental theory.
High Ability Studies, 15, pp. 119-147.
Gardner H. (1993). Creating Minds. New York: Basic Books.
Hays K. F. (2002). The enhancement of performance excellence among performing
artists. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, pp. 299-312.
Kiewra K. A. and Creswell J. W. (2000). Conversations with three highly productive
educational psychologists: Richard Anderson, Richard Mayer, and Michael
Pressley. Educational Psychology Review, 12, pp. 135-161.
Kogan N. (2002). Careers in the performing arts: A psychological perspective. Creativ-
ity Research Journal, 14, pp. 1-16.
Krasnow D. and Kabbani M. (1999). Dance science research and the modern dance.
Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 14, pp. 16-20.
Lubinski D., Benbow C., Shea D. et al. (2001). Men and women at promise for scientific
excellence: Similarity not dissimilarity. Psychological Science, 12, pp. 309-317.
Mayring P. (2000). Qualitative content analysis. Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 1,
available at http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0114-fqs0002204.
Onwuegbuzie A. J. and Leech N. L. (2007). Validity and qualitative research: An oxymo-
ron? Quality and Quantity, 41, pp. 233-249.
Roe A. (1946). Artists and their work. Journal of Personality, 15, pp. 1-40.
Schilling J. (2006). On the pragmatics of qualitative assessment: Designing the process
for content analysis. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22, pp. 28-37.
Shanteau J., Weiss. D. J., Thomas R. P., and Pounds J. C. (2002). Performance-based
assessment of expertise: How to decide if someone is an expert or not. European
Journal of Operational Research, 136, pp. 253-263.
Trost G. (2000). Prediction of excellence in school, higher education and work. In K.
Heller, F. Mnks, R. Sternberg, and R. Subotnik (eds.), International Handbook of
Giftedness and Talent (2e, pp. 317-330). Oxford: Pergamon.
Urea C. (2004). Skill Acquisition in Ballet Dancers: The Relationship between Delib-
erate Practice and Expertise. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Florida State University.
Zanotto E. D. (2006). The scientists pyramid. Scientometrics, 69, pp. 175-181.
Thematic session:
Expression and interpretation II
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Dorottya Fabian
METHOD
Materials
Procedure
Aural analyses of bowing, fingering, and phrasing together with software as-
sisted analyses of vibrato, tempo, and rhythmic flexibility were conducted.
The analysis aimed to distinguish between mainstream and period violin-
ists and examined their techniques and artistic approach. Only general trends
and aspects of the quantitative data are reported.
RESULTS
Table 1. Vibrato rate, width, and frequency of use measured on selected notes in differ-
ent movements and averaged across each selected violinist. (NB. Rate in cycle/second,
width in semitones; frequency refers to occurrence of vibrato on the selected pitches.)
ton-Pine used vibrato the least and Hahn the most. Multiple recordings of
violinists indicate a decreasing use of vibrato. Table 1 provides details.
Overall tempo choices showed no clear trend but varied most in fast and
fugal movements, as well as in the C major Adagio. Table 2 provides an ex-
ample.
Variety was discovered in relation to ornamentation as well. Luca, Podger,
Huggett, Gringolts, and Tognetti added graces and embellishments (e.g. A
minor Andante, E major Gavotte). Their solutions differed radically from
each-other both in terms of quantity and type of added ornaments. Rhythmic
flexibility was more obvious in the most recent and HIP versions, where the
articulation of metrical groups and the inflection of hierarchically important
150 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 2. Tempo choices in selected G minor sonata recordings. Mean metronome values
were calculated from movement duration. SDs marked as negative indicate variance
that is slower than the mean. SD>2 is italicized.
DISCUSSION
The analyses indicate great diversity in approach, along the continuum from
literalistic to flexible. Several mainstream violinists are influenced by current
beliefs about HIP. This can be seen in the declining use of vibrato; the more
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 151
Table 3. Dotting ratios calculated from IOIs in selected D minor Corrente and C major
Adagio recordings. A ratio of 0.75 indicates literal dotting (3:1).
articulated and lifted bowing style, and the use of lower positions and open
strings. Multiple recordings of artists, especially those of Kremer, show this
tendency clearly. Apart from this broad stylistic fashion no real homogeneity
was found. Rather, the examination showed diversity in tempo choices, or-
namentation, and approaches to phrasing and expressive flexibilities. These
qualitative differences do not lend themselves easily to quantitative reporting
and can be better explained through detailed individual comparisons.
152 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This project has been supported by an Australian Research Council discovery grant
(DP0879616). I would like to thank Bridget Kruithof for help with data collection and
Rachel Barton-Pine for her generosity in providing me with recordings of her concerts.
Dorottya Fabian, School of English, Media and Performing Arts, University of New
South Wales, Webster Building, Sydney, New South Wales 2052, Australia; Email: d.
fabian@unsw.edu.au
References
Day T. (2000). A Century of Recorded Music. New Haven, Connecticut, USA: Yale Uni-
versity Press.
Fabian D. (2005). Towards a performance history of Bachs Sonatas and Partitas for
solo violin. In L. Vikrius and V. Lampert (eds.), Essays in Honor of Lszl Somfai
(pp. 87-108). Lanham, Maryland, USA: Scarecrow Press.
Fabian D. and Schubert E. (2008). Musical character and the performance and percep-
tion of dotting, articulation and tempo in recordings of Variation 7 of J. S. Bachs
Goldberg Variations (BWV 988). Musicae Scientiae, 12, pp. 177-203.
Katz M. (2003). Beethoven in the age of mechanical reproduction: The Violin Concerto
on record, Beethoven Forum, 10, pp. 38-54.
Leech-Wilkinson D. (2009). The Changing Sound of Music. London: Centre for the
History and Analysis of Recorded Music (CHARM).
Milson D. (2003). Theory and Practice in Late Nineteenth-Century Violin Perform-
ance. Aldershot,UK: Ashgate.
Ornoy E. (2008). Recording analysis of J. S. Bachs G minor adagio for solo violin (ex-
cerpt): A case study. Journal of Music and Meaning, 6, available online at
www.musicandmeaning.net.
Philip R. (2004). Performing Music in the Age of Recording. London: Yale University
Press.
Repp B. (1990). Patterns of expressive timing in performances of a Beethoven minuet by
nineteen famous pianists. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, pp.
622-641.
Repp B. (1992). Diversity and commonality in music performance: An analysis of timing
microstructure in Schumanns Trumerei. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 92, pp. 2546-2568.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Sally Collyer
Melbourne, Australia
When classical singers and singing teachers are asked what do you need to
sing well? the list almost always starts with good breathing. Yet, despite
hard work on all sides there remains a large gulf between research into
breathing for singing and its influence in the teaching studio. There are three
main reasons for the lack of interaction. First, singing teachers often find the
concepts, techniques, and equipment used in breathing studies to be difficult
to access. This lack of familiarity makes it difficult for teachers to interpret
and apply research findings in the studio. Second, the scope of what is con-
sidered to be the breathing system is often too narrow, neglecting the respi-
ratory contribution of elements not immediately associated with respiration.
Last, conceptualizing breathing as simply air supply for phonation negates its
role as the link between phonation, emotion, and musical line, a role that is
crucial to singing and in teaching singing. This has led some pedagogical ap-
proaches to reject studies as unrealistic or irrelevant.
The article introduces, from a pedagogical perspective, some of the con-
cepts and methods used in the study of breathing. The first section explains
154 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
Breathing studies
Lung volumes
The most common assessment of breathing is lung volume (LV). Key meas-
urements are LV at the start and end of each phrase, with the difference be-
tween these being the amount of air used (to normalize comparisons, LV
measures are expressed as a percentage of the individuals vital capacity
[%VC], measured by having the singer breathe in to full capacity and then
exhale as far as possible). On average, singers generally start phrases at 70-
80%VC and end phrases at 30-50%VC (Hixon 1991). The individual singer is
highly consistent in LV measurements (Thomasson and Sundberg 1999), but
singers are very different (Collyer et al. 2008, Collyer et al. in press). Intra-
singer consistency versus inter-singer variability can be seen in Figure 1. The
question immediately arises whether a particular LV behavior is associated
with better singing, but no study has yet been able to find such a link. The
next question, then, is how is LV measured?
Chest-wall kinematics
100%
Lung volume at the start
Vital Singing (= exhaling)
Capacity
Lung volume at the end
0%
1 2 3
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Figure 1. Lung volumes (LV) used in the messa di voce on B4. At left: consistency of one
singer performing three messe di voce. At right: variability amongst 10 female singers.
Data are taken from studies published in Collyer et al. (2008) and Collyer et al. (in
press). (See full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
1 2 3 4 5
ABDOMEN
6 7 8 9 10
Figure 3. CWK plots for the LV changes shown in Figure 1. (See full color version at
www.performancescience.org.)
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 157
allel to the 100%VC line. Line C-D-E follows the singer performing a messa di
voce. Note that at first (C-D) this singer expands the RC (paradoxical behav-
ior), but greater AB decrease gives net exhalation and then RC also decreases
(D-E).
CWK plots have provided a great deal of information about singers be-
havior. The prephonatory adjustment (B-C) is very common (Thorpe et al.
2001). So too are paradoxical movements (where RC or AB dimension de-
creases during inhalation or increases during singing). But the most striking
feature is that, while a trained singer shows a highly consistent CWK pattern
on the same task, singers are very different from each other. Figure 3 shows
the CWK plots underlying the LV changes in Figure 1.
The second section of the paper addresses the definition of a breathing sys-
tem. Voice is often conceptualized as consisting of three isolated stages: res-
piration, phonation, and articulation. This segmented model is inconsistent
with human anatomy and physiology because it models the larynx as an inde-
pendently functioning entity rather than as an intrinsic and inseparable part
of the respiratory system at all stages of development. For example, the role
of the larynx in modulating airflow for singing derives from its role in modu-
lating airflow for respiratory needs, which in turn originates with modulation
of lung fluid efflux by the larynx, which is essential for lung development in
the womb (Harding et al. 1986). Even when protecting the airway during
swallowing, a role often considered to be the larynxs primary function, the
larynx also assumes an expiratory modulation role to assist aspiration (Ced-
borg et al. 2009). The conference presentation considers the limitations of
the segmented model, which requires that laryngeal adjustments can occur
independently of respiratory adjustments.
The final section of the paper extends this wider breathing system model to
incorporate emotional influences of and on breathing and then to relate these
to the nature of musical line. This pivotal role of breathing as the intermedi-
ary between phonation, emotion, and musicwhich is at the heart of histori-
cal pedagogys reverence for breathingcannot be sustained under a
segmented or larynx-driven paradigm. The conference presentation discusses
how a wider concept of the nature of breathing is consistent with the nature
of singing at all levels: the technical, the interpretative, and the human.
158 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
IMPLICATIONS
LV and CWK data raise many questions for the teacher. What determines a
singers breathing pattern? How susceptible to training is breathing? What
represents an improvement for a singer? How does the teacher match train-
ing with singer? In the conference presentation, findings of the studies are
presented from a pedagogical perspective, including the implications of study
limitations such as technical constraints.
Sally Collyer, PO Box 156, Box Hill, Victoria 3128, Australia; Email: sallycollyer@yahoo.
com.au
References
METHOD
Participants
Materials
The first eight measures of Mozarts Clarinet Concerto in A Major (K. 622),
second movement, were used. The excerpt (Figure 1) was chosen for its high
expressive content and the fact that it is well known. All clarinetists per-
formed on an A clarinet. All clarinetists were familiar with the excerpt before
participating. An active motion capture system (NDI Optotrak Certus) meas-
ured the clarinetists movements at a 250 Hz sampling rate. Markers were
placed on each fingernail tip of the clarinetists hands and on the clarinet: one
marker on the barrel (below the mouthpiece), one centered just above the
bell, and two on the bell. These markers provided information about the ori-
entation of the clarinet. An AKG C414 B-XLS standing microphone, as well as
a microphone integrated into the clarinet barrel, recorded the audio at 44.1
kHz. The clarinet movements are reported only up to the notated ornament in
measure seven, which permitted a variety of temporal interpretations and
was therefore excluded.
Procedure
RESULTS
Figure 1. Mean clarinet bell elevation (degrees) in each expressive condition. Horizontal
lines indicate significance regions (grey and black indicate p<0.05 and p<0.01, respec-
tively); vertical shaded bars indicate locations of rests at phrase boundaries.
Figure 2. Mean timing deviation, bell elevation, and intensity (from standing mic) for
each expressive condition. Gaps indicate locations of ends of phrases.
DISCUSSION
Expressive timing and clarinet bell motion increased across each phrase with
clarinetists expressive intent: performers used more bell motion and more
expressive timing when performing with greater expressive intent. Clarinet-
ists may use the bells elevation to help shape phrases, by physically repre-
senting change in musical features such as tension/relaxation. Together with
previous findings that documented performers reduced expressive timing
when musicians were asked to perform without moving (Wanderley et al.
2005), these findings suggest a bi-directional relationship between expres-
sion and movement in performance. The clarinet elevations and the expres-
sive use of rubato increased from phrase beginnings to endings. Clarinet tone
intensities instead showed an inverted U-shape throughout each phrase.
This study extended several findings on expressive performance to the
study of ancillary gestures in a wind instrument that permits considerable
degrees of motion. Our findings suggest that ancillary bell gestures are rule-
governed; they correlate with some but not all acoustic features of musical
expression, and in particular with phrase structure. Future research may de-
rive musical variables from performers movements by creating a taxonomy of
gestures that correspond to specific musical intentions.
164 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by Canada Research Chair and NSERC Grant 298173 to
the first author, by an NSERC-USRA fellowship to the second author, by a SSHRC fel-
lowship to the third author, and by an NSERC-CGS fellowship to the fourth author.
References
Davidson J. W. (1995). What does the visual information contained in music perform-
ances offer the observer? Some preliminary thoughts. In R. Steinberg (ed.), Music
and the Mind Machine (pp. 105-114). Berlin: Springer.
Davidson J. W. (2007). Qualitative insights into the use of expressive body movement in
solo piano performance: A case study approach. Psychology of Music, 35, pp. 381-
401.
Delalande F. (1988). La gestique de Gould. In L. Courteau (ed.), Glenn Gould Pluriel
(pp. 85-111). Qubec City, Canada: Lousie Courteau Editrice.
Gabrielsson A. (1987). Once again: The theme from Mozarts piano Sonata in A Major
(k.331). In A. Gabrielsson (ed.), Action and Perception in Rhythm and Music (pp.
81-104). Stockholm: Royal Swedish Academy of Music.
Kendall R. A. and Carterette E. C. (1990). The communication of musical expression.
Music Perception, 8, pp. 129-163.
Palmer C. (1989). Mapping musical thought to musical performance. Journal of Ex-
perimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, pp. 331-346.
Ramsay J. O. and Silverman B. W. (2005). Functional Data Analysis (2e). New York:
Springer.
Sundberg J., Friberg A., and Fryden L. (1991). Common secrets of musicians and listen-
ers: An analysis-by-synthesis study of musical performance. In P. Howell, R. West,
and I. Cross (eds.), Representing Musical Structure (pp. 161-197). London: Aca-
demic Press.
Wanderley M. M. (2002). Quantitative analysis of non-obvious performer gestures. In I.
Wachsmuth and T. Sowa (eds.), Gesture and Sign Languages in Human-Computer
Interaction (pp. 241-253). Berlin: Springer Verlag.
Wanderley M. M., Vines B., Middleton N. et al. (2005). The musical significance of
clarinetists ancillary gestures: An exploration of the field. Journal of New Music
Research, 34, pp. 97-113.
Thematic session:
Memory and performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
There were 16 participants, split into two equal groups of expert musicians
(professional, semi-professional, and student) and those whose professional
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 169
life did not involve music. The musicians (5 M, 3 F) were aged between 20
and 48 years (mean=30.8). Some were specialists either in conducting or in
percussion playing. Non-musicians (6 M, 2 F) were aged between 23 and 74
years (mean=49.9), and had no musical training beyond school, though two
had choral singing experience, and one was a self-taught drummer.
Materials
Procedure
There were two tasks. First, participants were asked to clap, synchronizing as
closely as possible with a recorded beat (the beats on the recording disap-
peared and reappeared, see below). In the second task, participants were
asked to reproduce from memory the speeds of the original recording. The
three speeds of 35, 110, and 185 bpm (slow, medium, and fast, respec-
tively) were chosen so as not to be related to each other by any simple pro-
portion. Tempo order was counterbalanced between participants.
In the first section of each test, participants were asked to listen to the re-
cording of beats, to join in after hearing the first four beats, and to continue
clapping until told to stop. Some beats on the metronomic stimulus were ab-
sent and the participants were informed of this but asked to continue clap-
ping nonetheless. In all tests, there were 8 beats present, 4 absent, 4 present,
4 absent, 8 present, 8 absent, 8 present, and 8 absent. Thus, clapping regu-
larity in both the presence of a beat (SMS) and its absence (continuation)
were tested. Participants performed the above test at the three speeds and
then repeated them in the same order. A gap of about 30 s was left between
tests. They were then asked to fill in a questionnaire covering age, gender,
and musical experience. Approximately 4 mins were given for this.
170 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 1. Mean, minimum, and maximum recorded clapping tempi (bpm) as a function
of the given tempo. The target tempo is shown by the solid line. (See full color version at
www.performancescience.org.)
Next, participants memory for the three speeds was tested. They were
asked to clap at the slow, medium, and fast speed, in the same order they had
already been tested on, both beginning and ending when indicated. A total of
about 32 claps were recorded for each trial. After the recall test of each speed,
the first test (clapping with the recorded beats) was repeated, followed, after a
1 min pause, by the test of memory once again. A complete testing session
took approximately half an hour per participant.
RESULTS
The present article focuses on the participants tempo memory data. Overall,
participants were fairly good at recalling the slow tempo but tended to under-
estimate both the medium and fast tempi (i.e. clapping too slowly) quite sub-
stantially (see Figure 1). However, their recall for these tempi was generally
better on the second recall trial, underestimating less (see Table 1). When the
musicians and non-musicians were analyzed separately, the musicians were
generally found to recall the medium and fast tempos more accurately than
the non-musicians, underestimating the speeds less (see Table 2). A 3-way
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with tempo, trial, and musical experience as
factors was conducted on the mean error as a percentage of the target tempo.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 171
Table 2. Clapping recall tempo (bpm) as a function of musical training (SD in brackets).
DISCUSSION
65 or 140), rather than the terms slow, medium, and fast. We will vary
the number of tempi included in tests and accurately control the time elaps-
ing before participants are required to reproduce a tempo from memory. We
also intend to include distracter tasks, which may affect mental rehearsal, and
compare the use of a beat pad with clapping, to reveal the relevance of the
choreographic element of clapping.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Derrick Walford for assistance with the data measurement using Praat.
References
Jazz and classical musicians use knowledge of formal musical structure to aid
retrieval (Chaffin and Imreh 2002, Noice et al. 2008). Motor cues aid actors
174 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
recall of text and contextual cues significantly aid retrieval (Noice and Noice
1997, 2002). Music prompts increased recall for prose and verse, and the
recall of such material tends to begin and end at breath pause locations
(Rubin 1997).
In contemporary dance, there is no set repertoire of steps or verbal asso-
ciates. Contemporary dance and choreographic traditions are passed on not
in words but by showing and doing (Grove 2005). In the absence of notation
and scores the dancers are dynamic archives of works that they have re-
hearsed and performed. Smyth and Pendleton (1994) proposed that move-
ment involving configuration of body parts is encoded kinesthetically. Overby
(1990) has suggested that, while novice dancers prefer a visual mode of
thought, experienced dancers are inclined to use both verbal and visual
modes of thinking.
Margaret Barr had a choreographic career that spanned more than 60 years
and three continents. Barr described her work as dance-drama because she
was interested in both the inherent drama of the body in motion and its abil-
ity to create and sustain the human drama of her primarily narrative based
works. Barrs technique classes were a series of set studies performed to the
driving rhythms of Carl Orffs Carmina Burana. This choreographed material
explored a broad range of movement dynamics including the elegant simplic-
ity of her lyrical studies to the extremes of strong, direct, thrusting sequences.
Throughout her classes great attention was given to the rhythm of the breath
cycle as the dancer moved. For Margaret the dramatic moment in a breath
cycle was the breath pausethe moment of stasis between the inhalation of
breath and its exhalation.
for mental rehearsal and visual and/or motor imagery that should enhance
recall. The body versus mannequin conditions permitted the investigation of
procedural recall in the form of self-motion versus motion of another figure
without use of words. We anticipated greater recall using self-motion.
METHOD
Participants
Four female dancers who had all trained with the same choreographer vol-
unteered for the study (mean age=55.38 years, SD=10.13, range=44.0-68.5).
Two had performed the choreographers dance material within the past three
years, whereas the oldest participant had not performed it for 22 years
(dancer 2, C) and the second youngest participant for 31 years (dancer 3, F).
RESULTS
Table 1. Duration of recall by dancer and seven exercises showing raw scores (in sec-
onds) and as proportions of the duration of the associated musical excerpt. (Proportions
shown in brackets; 0 refers to zero recall.)
0.55 (SD=0.38) in the immediate recall condition and 0.44 (SD=0.31) in the
delayed recall condition. There was no difference between proportion recall
duration when the dancers own body was used (mean=0.50, SD=0.29) com-
pared with when the mannequin was used for recall (mean=0.49, SD=0.40).
Dancers reflected on their recall of each exercise and completed a short
written questionnaire. Their responses are organized below according to in-
dependent variables, cues, memory associates, and images.
Recall using the mannequin: immediate doll condition required thinking
as I moved it, i.e. feeling it in my own body as I moved the doll (K), doll
totally unable to movedisjointedresults lacking flow and staccato posi-
tioning which [is] what all/most dancers try to avoid (C), could not visualize
movement on the doll. Perhaps a real body would have triggered more mem-
ory (F).
Immediate versus delayed conditions: immediate and delayed body
didnt make much difference more that it immediately came to mind or not
(S). W commented that having time to rehearse the exercises in the delayed
recall condition led to some confusion, while F wrote that in the delayed body
recall condition she had a lot of trouble remembering anything first without
moving.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 177
Table 2. Mean duration recall proportions collapsed across seven exercises and three
dancers and shown as a function of recall condition. (SD shown in brackets.)
DISCUSSION
rial. Interference paradigms could also be used effectively to shed light on the
nature of the memory code.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the Australian Research Council (LP0562687) and the
Australia Council for the Arts, Ausdance, and QL2 Centre for Youth Dance. We thank
Mark Gordon and our dancer participants for their generous contribution of time and
energy.
References
Chaffin R., Imreh G., and Crawford M. (2002). Practicing Perfection. Mahwah, New
Jersey, USA: Erlbaum.
Grove R. (2005). Show me what you just did. In R. Grove et al. (eds.), Thinking in Four
Dimensions (pp. 37-49). Melbourne, Australia: Melbourne University Press.
Noice H., Jeffrey J., Noice T., and Chaffin R. (2008). Memorization by a jazz musician:
A case study. Psychology of Music, 36, pp. 63-79.
Noice H. and Noice T. (1997). Long-term retention of theatrical roles. Memory, 7, pp.
357-382.
Noice T. and Noice, H. (2002). Very long-term recall and recognition of well-learned
material. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, pp. 259-272.
Overby L. Y. (1990). A comparison of novice and experienced dancers imagery ability.
Journal of Mental Imagery, 14, pp. 173-184.
Rubin D. C. (1997). Very long-term memory for prose and verse. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, pp. 611-621.
Smyth M. M. and Pendleton L. R. (1994). Memory for movement in professional ballet
dancers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 25, pp. 282-294.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The amount of information and the time available for its retention are two
important aspects when we talk about memorization. According to several
authors (Marteniuk 1976, Perlmutter and Hall 1992, Weiss 1995) Millers
work has established a referential: the short-term memory has an overall
capacity of 72 items. The recovery of more information is dependent on the
use of the long-term memory.
The short-term memory increases considerably with age, so the capacity
to memorize is generally lower in children when compared with adults. This
difference is due to the lack of knowledge and strategies of memory that chil-
dren present (Ille and Cadopi 1999). As children grow older they acquire a
repertoire of strategies that will help to solve problems: from age seven they
begin to use strategies of rehearsal/memorization spontaneously, and until
age eleven, a rapid increase in the number and quality of strategies is evident
(Bouffard and Dun 1993, Weiss and Klint 1987). The acquisition of the label-
ing, repetition, and cluster strategies increases progressively up to eleven
years old, and during the next two years children acquire other strategies.
One problem that arises when studying motor memory is how to measure
itthat is, to find an appropriate unit of measurement to quantify this capac-
ity. It may be easier to identify the number of words in a spoken or written
discourse than the number of movements of a person who is dancing. The
tests that are used to measure the capacity of short-term memory are called
memory span. This type of testing identifies the longest sequence of non-re-
lated items that can be recalled immediately in the same order as they were
presented.
The measure established by Miller (72 chunks of information to the
short term memory capacity) means that the amount of information per
chunk varies. This leads the author to conclude that capacity of short-term
memory is a matter of organization of material to remember in units of
meaningful information, so that maximum information is retained (Mart-
eniuk 1976). But can the amount of recalled words be applied to, or similar to,
the ability to remember movements?
According to Bavelier et al. (2006) the capacity of the short-term memory
depends on the nature of the information, for example 72 items for language
and 41 for visual-spatial items. Furthermore, the ability to organize infor-
mation into chunks seems to depend more on familiarity and experience that
the individual has with the skill or knowledge area, than their pre-existing
capacity to store more information in memory (Rose 1997). This study tries to
identify the largest number of motor actions that subjects with and without
experience in dance are able to recall after the presentation of various se-
quences of motor actions in dance. We wanted to compare children with
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 181
METHOD
Participants
Materials
In the present study we have applied the memory span test. This is a test of
presentation/reproduction of items (motor actions) in which subjects are
classified/categorized by the number of items that they can identify and recall
in the correct order (Bouffard and Dunn 1993). The material to be reproduced
by subjects (motor actions and movement phrases), developed by the authors,
was created for this study taking into account the degree of difficulty of the
motor actions and movement phrases. We aimed to use sequences of motor
actions without complexity, considering that a task with more relationships
between the parts that compose it is more complex than a task with fewer
relationships. For example, a jump with a rotation during the aerial phase is
more complex than a vertical jump from two to two feet.
We created twelve sequences of non-related motor actions and eleven se-
quences of related motor actions. The transition between each sequence is
characterized by adding a motor action. Structure units were taken to be the
motor actions of the classification of movement in dance presented by Rodri-
gues (1999). The motor actions were our unit of measurement: posture, bal-
ance, gesture, turns, steps, displacement, jumps, and falls.
Procedure
The memory span test was done individually; only the participant and the
researcher, responsible for projecting the images and recording the partici-
182 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
pant performance, were in the studio. The test started with the projection of
the first group of non-related motor actions. The subject observed the se-
quence twice and then was asked to repeat it immediately once. The test fol-
lowed the same procedure until the last sequence (12 motor actions). Subjects
were asked to reproduce exactly what they saw, maintaining the overall form
and also keeping the order of the presented movements. Subjects were not
allowed to reproduce/rehearse the film during its projection. After a break of
10-15 minutes the second test was applied. The procedure was the same as the
first, but this time subjects were asked to recall eleven movement phrases
(related motor actions) of 2-12 actions per phrase.
RESULTS
The two types of sequences of our study (non-related and related motor ac-
tions) were studied according to the following variables: maximum number of
motor actions recalled in the correct order, number of sequences recalled in
the correct order, number of sequences recalled independently of order, total
actions recalled in the correct order, and total actions recalled independently
of order.
The maximum number of motor actions recalled in the correct order
ranges from 2-11 and seems to be the most interesting variable in quantifying
the number of actions per sequence either in future studies or in learning
applications. The results range from 3.50 actions in the children group with-
out experience, to 8.00 actions in the group of adults with experience. The
other two groups realized almost 5.00 actions sequentially in the correct or-
der. For this variable, the approximate values obtained for the group of chil-
dren with experience and the group of adults with no experience is consistent.
The number of sequences recalled in the correct order ranges from 0-5,
with values of 3.67 actions for adults with experience, and 2.00 actions in
adults with no experience. This variable focuses on the sequence; the number
of actions executed correctly before the first error. This means that there were
some subjects (2 adults without experience and 1 child with experience) who
failed the first sequence of 2 related motor actions and that there were some
(2 adults and 1 child with experience) who continued accurately until the fifth
motor action sequence, after having also correctly completed the earlier se-
quences. In this variable, children with experience presented results slightly
higher than adults with no experience.
The results obtained from the analysis of the number of sequences re-
called independently of orderthat is 12 and 11 sequences (related and un-
related, respectively)consider all of the sequences that were executed
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 183
without error because one subject can correctly complete 4 actions, not com-
plete 5 actions, and correctly complete 6 actions, etc. For this variable, the
values are between 1 and 6 activities, with averages ranging from 1.17 for chil-
dren with no experience to 4.17 for adults with experience. The other two
groups have similar values with a slight advantage for children with experi-
ence (3.20-2.20 actions) compared with adults without experience (3.17-2.17
actions).
The total of actions recalled in the correct order varies from 15.58% in
children without experience to 76.62% in adults with experience, which is the
equivalent to 12 and 59 actions from the 77 actions presented by the model.
The average values range from 33.76%-69.26%, with the highest values be-
longing to adults with experience and the lowest to children with no experi-
ence. Analysis of the total of actions recalled independently of order showed
mean values generally higher than the values of the previous variable, with
values ranging from 34.19%-70.30%. To study further the age and experience
independent variablesand because of sample size and according the type of
dependent variableswe used the nonparametric Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon
or the t-Student test. To compare the related and non-related data we used
the Wilcoxon test and, for the paired data, the t-Student test. We considered a
result significant if p0.05.
DISCUSSION
The results indicate that there are significant differences when comparing the
groups with respect to experience. The number of sequences recalled in the
correct order depends on this factor as it appears in all possible comparisons
and cross-comparisons. The other four variables in the study also appear of-
ten, which reinforces the idea that years of practice in a particular activity
influence the ability to recall movement, which despite being generic (not
specific to a particular dance technique) have dance as frame of reference of
the motor repertoire. With regard to age, it is confirmed that for the variables
under study (which appear to have consensus in the literature) children are
less able than adults to memorize movements; this disadvantage is more ap-
parent if the sequences are particularly long (more than 5 motor actions),
common in many situations of dance learning. When we associated age and
experience, we found an aspect that seems worthy of development in future
study: that the group of children with experience have similar or sometimes
slightly higher scores than the group of adults without experience. This, and
the size of the sample for different age groups, should be considered in future
studies.
184 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
References
Playing tennis, performing a piece of music from memory, and even leaping
in a rhythmic gymnastics exercise are all complex multimodal tasks which
rely on predictive (feedforward) mechanisms acquired through extensive
training. All these refined sensory-motor tasks depend on time-based se-
188 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
Materials
The stimuli were six sequences extracted from the right-hand parts of the
Preludes V, VI, and X of the Well Tempered Clavier (Book I) by J. S. Bach
and the Piano Sonata No. 52 in E Flat Major by J. Haydn. These pieces were
chosen because their parts for the right hand contain mostly single pitches of
the same value (duration)i.e. sixteenth notes. The number of notes per se-
quence was approximately 200. The tempo for each piece was selected so that
the interonset interval (IOI, the time between onsets of two subsequent
notes) was 125 ms (8 tons/s) in all cases. Participants were instructed to re-
hearse and memorize them before the experimental session.
Procedure
Data analysis
Only isolated pitch errors and correct notes, which were preceded and fol-
lowed by three correct notes, were included in the analysis. Two additional
constraints were set to all pre-selected errors and correct notes in order to
assure their temporal precision and to avoid overlapping of brain responses:
first, the time interval between MIDI note on and off was not accepted above
150 ms. Second, the minimal and maximal IOI prior to and after the error
were set to 100 and 300 ms.
We performed the following types of analysis of the EEG signal: first, the
standard time averaging technique was executed to analyze the ERPs of the
brain responses triggered by actions leading to pitch errors (wrong note was
played) as compared with actions leading to correct pitches. ERPs were de-
rived by averaging the raw epochs for each subject and condition and the
result was baseline-corrected (from 300 to 150 ms prior to the keystroke).
Second, we analyzed the bivariate (pair-wise) phase synchronization between
EEG signals (Lachaux et al. 1999, Varela et al. 2001), as a measure of the
functional interaction and integration between the underlying neural popula-
tions. This latter investigation focused on the electrodes F3-FCz and F4-FCz,
as a measure of the synchronization between areas located over the lPFC and
mPFC. To rule out an alternative explanation of volume conduction effects,
the bivariate synchronization index was additionally calculated between C3-
FCz and C4-FCz: these pairs have a similar distance as pairs F3-FCz and F4-
FCz, but a more posterior location; however, there are no a priori hypotheses
that posit a role of these brain regions in cognitive control. Three frequency
ranges were analyzed: (1) the theta band (4-8 Hz), based upon its modulation
of the ERN (Luu et al. 2004, Cavanagh et al. 2009), (2) the alpha band (8-13
Hz), as an indicator of attention-deficits and precursor of forthcoming
mistakes in monotonous tasks (Mazaheri et al. 2009), and (3) the beta band
(13-30 Hz), due to its sensitivity to movement-related changes in humans
(Pfurtscheller et al. 1997).
All statistical tests were performed by means of nonparametric univariate
permutation tests and multivariate synchronized permutations (Good 2005).
RESULTS
Performance data
Moreover, these data indicated that pianists played with a similar timing and
error rate with or without auditory feedback.
Interestingly, the loudness (assessed by MIDI velocity) of errors was sig-
nificantly reduced as compared with the loudness of the corresponding cor-
rect notes in the same position on the musical score, both in AM (mean=-7,
SD=4, p=0.01) and in M (mean=-5, SD=4, p=0.01). The reduction in loud-
ness was similar in both conditions (p>0.05). These results indicated that a
corrective response had already been initiated by the time the subjects
pressed the erroneous key. Finally, a pre- and post-error slowing was ob-
served both in AM and M [pre-error slowing of 190 ms (SD=60) in AM and
170 ms (SD=60) in M; post-error slowing of 240 ms (SD=60) in AM and 200
ms (SD=60) in M].
The comparison between the waveforms of errors minus correct notes, both
in AM and in M, revealed a negative event-related potential triggered 70 ms
before the onset of errors. This negative component resembled the ERN and
was termed pre-error negativity (pre-ERN, see Figure 1A and 1B). Also, we
observed a final positive deflection between 240 and 280 ms (AM) and be-
tween 180 and 220 ms (M), resembling the Pe. The Pe was larger in AM. The
maximum of the pre-ERN was localized across frontocentral positions (Fz,
FCz, Cz) of the scalp in both conditions. Likewise, the topographic maxima of
the Pe in AM and M were localized across frontocentral electrode positions.
By means of multivariate statistical analysis, we established that the ERP
waveforms of the mesial frontocentral electrodes between errors and correct
notes differed depending on the task condition from 220 to 260 ms (signifi-
cant interaction of the factors event type x condition, p=0.008). A post hoc
univariate permutation test across subjects in AM revealed a significant en-
hanced negativity, the pre-ERN, between -70 and -20 ms before errors as
compared with correct notes (p=0.007) and a significant Pe (p=0.008) be-
tween 240 and 280 ms. A similar result was obtained in M between -50 and 0
ms for the pre-ERN (p=0.008) and between 180 and 220 ms for the Pe. The
comparison between ERPs in AM and M revealed a significantly larger Pe in
AM between 250 and 280 ms (p=0.008). However, the pre-ERN did not dif-
fer between conditions (p=0.3).
The source of activity generating the pre-ERN was localized by sLORETA
in the Brodmann area (BA) 32 of the rostral ACC (Figure 1C). Similarly, the
neural generators of the of the brain activity associated with the Pe were lo-
calized by sLORETA in the BA 24 of the rostral ACC (Figure 1D).
192 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 1. Note-onset ERPs depicted at electrode location FCz for erroneous notes
(dashed line), for correct notes (solid line), and for the difference (errors minus correct
notes, bold line) in the presence (A) and absence of auditory feedback (B). Note the
increased negativity just before the commission of the errors (pre-ERN, arrow) and the
large positivity after errors (Pe, arrow) in both conditions. Note the different scaling in
y-axes (C) sLORETA localized the source of activity associated with the pre-ERN in AM
and M in the BA 32 of the rostral ACC. (D) The neural generator for the Pe in AM and M
was localized by sLORETA in the BA 24 of the rostral ACC. (See full color version at
www.performancescience.org.)
The lower beta band (13-15 Hz) phase coupling between FCz and F4 increased
robustly from -100 to 0 ms before overt errors compared with overt correct
notes (p=0.001, permutation test, Figure 2). Similar statistical tests were run
on the pairs F3-FCz, C4-FCz and C3-FCz, but no significant effects were
found. These findings give evidence of an increased right-lateralized phase
interaction between FCz and F4 preceding errors, which could be related to
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 193
Figure 2. Difference between erroneous and correct trials in the grand-averaged phase
synchrony between channels F4 and FCz. An increase in bivariate phase synchrony can
be observed starting 100 ms prior to the note onset and due to more phase coupling for
error trials. Significant differences are marked by the black contour (p=0.001). The left
panel illustrates the results of the correlation analysis. The right panel depicts the elec-
trodes (dark pink) FCz and F4, in addition to electrodes F3, C3 and C4 (light pink). (See
full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
DISCUSSION
In the present study, the main finding was a negative event-related potential
triggered 70 ms before the onset of errors, the pre-ERN. The pre-ERN sig-
naled error detection in advance and was generated by the rostral ACC. Fur-
thermore, this component was independent of the auditory feedback and had
a correlate at the behavioral level: the loudness of pitch errors was reduced.
This behavioral finding suggests that a corrective response had already been
initiated by the instant of pressing the erroneous key.
194 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by the Center of Systems Neuroscience, Hanover, and the EU
through the Marie Curie Early Stage Training Contract (MEST-CT-2005-021014) to
Mara Herrojo Ruiz.
196 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Mara Herrojo Ruiz, Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians Medicine, Hanover
University of Music and Drama, Hohenzollernstrasse 47, Hanover 30161, Germany;
Email: Maria.Herrojo-Ruiz@charite.de
References
Pfurtscheller G., Stancak A., and Edlinger E. G. (1997). On the existence of different
types of central beta rhythms below 30 Hz. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology, 102, pp. 316-325.
Rushworth M. F., Walton M. E., Kennerley S. W., and Bannerman D. M. (2004). Action
sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, pp.
410-417.
Schmidt R. A. (1975). A schema theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review,
82, pp.225-260.
Wolpert D. M., Ghahramani Z., and Jordan M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensori-
motor integration. Science, 269, pp. 1880-1882.
Thursday
17 December 2009
Keynote paper
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Deidre Anderson
RETIREMENT AS A TRANSITION
from elite sport (Atchley 1991, Lerch 1984, McPherson 1980, Rosenberg 1981,
Freidman and Havighurst 1947, Rose 1962, Kuypers and Bergston 1973).
These approaches can also be applied to a range of other elite performance
environments, including the arts, dance, music, and acting industries.
The social gerontological perspective takes into account the following
theories: Activity Theory, Disengagement Theory, Subculture Theory, Conti-
nuity Theory, Social Breakdown Theory, and Social Exchange Theory. Some
of these will be discussed below.
Activity Theory (Friedman and Havighurst 1954; Havighurst and Albrecht
1953) maintains that as various activities are lost, the individual establishes
new roles, and the maintenance of these contributes positively to self concept
and life satisfaction. An important difficulty with this theory when relating to
elite performance, according to Baillie and Danish (1992), is that it may not
always be possible to duplicate the type of environment created within the
elite performance context.
Disengagement Theory (Cumming and Henry 1961) was developed as a
response to Activity Theory and suggests that the ageing process sees indi-
viduals mutually agreeing to withdraw from society and thereby providing
them with the opportunity to enjoy life in the period after paid employment.
This theory proposes that it is a function of both the individual and society
that naturally occurs. When attempting to draw parallels with the elite per-
formance environment, this concept is a difficult one as it assumes that the
withdrawal is mutually agreeable between both parties, and this may not al-
ways be the case.
Continuity Theory represents a refinement of Activity Theory (Atchley
1977) and posits that, although activities and habits might alter or be replaced
by others throughout our lives, these may not always provide the same
meaningful experience. If individuals are capable of retaining the continuity
of meaning in their activities, retirement is said to be less traumatic (Ander-
son 1999). In relating this theory to elite performance the challenge will be to
find meaning in other activities when for so long many elite performers have
characterized their lives around one specialized activity.
Additional theories worthy of consideration in the present context are So-
cial Breakdown Theory (Kuypers and Bergston 1973) and Exchange Theory
(Dowd 1975). Kuypers and Bergston proposed that with any role loss, such as
retirement, the individual might become the recipient of negative external
labels, leading to a breakdown in their social framework. Therefore, any ma-
jor role loss is seen to have significant potential to impact adversely on self
image. In order to limit the downward spiral that often results, they argued
that a social reconstruction is typically needed.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 205
Q ui ck F ix Wa y SEL F IDENT IT Y
DET OUR
Sel f man ag em e n t
Wh o is o n m y tr ai n?
A tt itu d es
Self Kn o wle dg e
In ter p er s on al s ki lls
C OPING G ROVE De cis io n m ak in g s kil ls
Mo n ey Ch as e
A CCEPT ANCE
Highway
Figure 1. The process of dealing with both voluntary and involuntary retirement from
elite performance. (See full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
Kleber and Brock (1992) found that adjustment difficulties might well be
explained by how the athlete perceives the level to which sport has con-
stricted their development. What the athlete believes could be reflected in
several ways. They might see that engagement in sport prevented serious
orientation toward a career and life beyond. It is also possible that sport may
have promoted degrees of self-detachment and desensitization to the wider
world, thereby leaving the athlete with heightened feelings of vulnerability as
retirement approaches (Anderson 1999).
Whether or not the decision to retire from elite performance is voluntary
or involuntary, qualitative analysis reveals that performers are frequently
disadvantaged by a failure to plan for life outside the performance environ-
ment (McPherson 1980). If young performers can be socialized to perceive
the value of planning for retirement during their elite performance careers,
they might minimize or even avoid the trauma that is currently being experi-
208 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
PERSONAL
BEST
T ERM INAL AVE
RESPONSI BIL IT Y RD
Wh o i s o n m y tr a in
Per so n al
r es po n sib il ity fo r
t he jo u r ne y
F RUST RAT IO N R D
INFOR MA TI ON OVERL OA D
IN TEG RAT ED GO ALS
Wa nt s su cc es s to o
kn o win g wh at b a dly , to o m uc h
in f or m at io n is v ali d p r ess u re o n s elf
PRO FESSI ONA L APPRO ACH
GR EIF WAY
Fr o m : fa ns , m e di a,
fa m ily , f r ien d s, wo r k, c lu b
REL OC AT ION CLO SE
F O CUS C LO SE
Un re so lv ed
Per so n al is su es
Transition F EAR A VE
Highway NO T HRU RO AD
Fe ar o f su cc es s & fa ilu re
Deidre Anderson, Macquarie University, North Ryde, New South Wales 2109, Australia;
Email: deidre.anderson@dvcc.mq.edu.au
References
Alison M. T. and Meyer C. (1988). Career problems and retirements among elite ath-
letes: The female tennis professional. Sociology of Sport Journal, 5, pp. 212-222.
212 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Sarah E. Allen
Sarah E. Allen, Meadows School of the Arts, Southern Methodist University, P.O. Box
750356, Dallas, Texas 75214, USA; Email: sarahallen@smu.edu
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Louise Atkins
METHOD
Participants
The study was carried out across seven UK conservatoires, with members of
staff from each institution aiding with data collection. The study also ques-
tioned a total of 46 student participants from UK conservatoires: 26 male and
20 female participants, with a mean age of 22 years. The students were from
both undergraduate and postgraduate courses and played cello, clarinet, flute,
horn, oboe, percussion, piano (including two accompanists), saxophone (clas-
sical and jazz), trumpet, trombone, tuba, violin, and voice. Twenty-seven
participants had experienced an injury, 19 had not, and 15 were currently
dealing with an injury.
Materials
Procedure
Each institution was approached on an individual basis and case studies were
completed with the cooperation of staff members. Since the personal data of
students was handled in a variety of different ways, procedure for finding
participants and completing the questionnaire was carried out through a vari-
ety of methods. Some questionnaires were filled out over the telephone with
the researcher taking notes, while others were completed by hand or via
email.
RESULTS
This investigation revealed a wide variety of activities, some of which are de-
scribed below, with conservatoires providing support to students on many
levels. One-off workshops were found to be the most common form of health
and wellbeing activity, giving students insight into issues ranging from injury
prevention to performance anxiety and from hearing health awareness to how
to create a healthier lifestyle. Specialist departmental workshops were noted
by 45% of students as being particularly successful, and one student com-
mented that there are fortnightly classes for accompanists where our specific
issues can be addressed; I find this really useful. In three conservatoires,
workshops have developed into a regular series of classes.
Three institutions present health and wellbeing issues as a first-year pro-
ject. As well as addressing practical skills, these classes also focus on aca-
demic knowledge, developing professional skills, and addressing
performance-practice issues. One participant commented: I had health and
wellbeing education when I was in the first year, but Ive forgotten everything
theyve said and I know others have too. Only 43% of participants recognized
that health and wellbeing was part of their curriculum.
In four conservatoires, health and wellbeing issues were also found to be
raised as a part of larger projects, such as outreach projects and teacher
training programs. Conservatoire students are increasingly given the oppor-
tunity to pursue academic study of the subject further, with five conserva-
toires noting an increase in the amount of students choosing health and
wellbeing as a topic for dissertation projects. Masters modules and doctoral
study in the area is also developing within conservatoires.
Conservatoires also showed diverse support for those who become in-
jured, with a wide range of services for psychological and physical wellbeing,
including counseling and physiotherapy and access to information, support,
funding, and treatment. However, only 35% of students suggested that they
had taken advantage of this. All seven conservatoires provide resources to
222 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
DISCUSSION
ries may be prevented, how they might detect the onset of injury, where they
go for treatment of an injury, and how they are supported back to recovery is
essential. Having consistency in comprehensive education and injury support
would allow students to study in an environment comparatively free of con-
cern over injury and leave their institutions with a set of skills central in a
professional career.
Students have expressed within this study their belief in the importance
of education in health and wellbeing matters. Students have also stressed the
importance of raising awareness of topics relating to injury, including both
the prevention and treatment of playing-related disorders. For this kind of
information to be successfully interpreted by music students, the role of the
one-to-one teacher should be carefully evaluated. Although creating effective
learning plans within the curriculum may be very beneficial, it is one-to-one
teachers who have the most influence on the practical usage of techniques.
Without the involvement of these teachers in health and wellbeing education,
the topic may struggle to be translated into useable, practical techniques.
Acknowledgments
This research project has been carried out with the kind support of the Musicians Be-
nevolent Fund and the British Association of Performing Arts Medicine. Both organiza-
tions have provided access to key contacts within the field, along with a range of other
resources and continuing help and support throughout this study.
Louise Atkins, Royal Welsh College of Music and Drama, Castle Grounds, Cathays Park,
Cardiff CF10 3ER, UK; Email: louise.atkins@rwcmd.ac.uk
References
Guptill C., Zaza C., & Paul S. (2000). An occupational study of physical playing-related
injuries in college students. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 15, pp. 86-90.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
One hundred and sixty-two RCM students (86 F, 76 M; mean age=23.7 years,
SD=4.8) participated in the audiogram study over two years, from 2007 to
2009.
Procedure
Participants completed a consent form and then went through standard clini-
cal audiometric testing. They were subsequently asked to respond to a survey
detailing their noise exposure.
Audiogram measurements were made with participants comfortably
seated in a soundproof room. A recently calibrated (June 2006, June 2007,
and June 2008) Kamplex KC 50 audiometer with TDH-39 earphones were
used for the standard audiometric frequencies 125, 250, 500, 1k, 2k, 4k, 6k,
and 8 kHz; high frequency headphones were used to measure 10k, 12.5k, and
16 kHz. A manual Hughson-Westlake procedure was used with 5 dB resolu-
tion for most subjects, but some were done with finer (3 or 1 dB) resolution
around areas of particular interest.
Audiogram corrections
Figure 1. Summary of hearing threshold levels (dB HL) from audiograms taken from
RCM students. Boxes extend from lower quartile to upper quartile with median hearing
level demarked as the center of a notch; whiskers show extent of the contiguous data
(minus outliers). The average of the data is plotted as a thick line. Notches in the inter-
quartile boxes display the variability of the median between samples. The width of a
notch is computed so that box plots whose notches do not overlap have different medi-
ans at the p<0.05 significance level. The 6 kHz threshold notch for the left ear was sta-
tistically significant. No significant notch was found for the right ear.
the two sets agreed in all statistical aspects, indicating that our correction was
successful. (The RAM data will be presented in conjunction with these data in
a subsequent paper.)
RESULTS
Audiogram data (Figure 1) from the young musician group showed a statisti-
cally significant threshold notch at 6 kHz in the left ear, indicative of noise-
induced hearing loss. A Wilcox signed rank test showed the median threshold
at 6 kHz was statistically higher than the median from either the 4 or 8 kHz
tests (Z=-6.37, p<0.001, and Z=-4.52, p<0.001). Further, paired t-tests indi-
cated that the mean threshold at 6 kHz was significantly higher than thresh-
olds taken at 4 kHz or 8 kHz (p<0.001 for both). The right also showed that
the 6 kHz threshold departed significantly from 4 kHz, but there was no sig-
nificant difference between 6 and 8 kHz thresholds and therefore no signifi-
cant notch.
A comparison of two subgroups, trumpet/trombone players versus vio-
lin/viola players, was carried out to see whether noise exposure asymmetry
with violin and viola players assumed to get more exposure in their left ears
could account for the asymmetry we saw in the overall audiogram data. We
found, however, that both the trumpet/trombone subgroup and the vio-
lin/viola subgroup showed the same trends as the entire group data (cf. Fig-
228 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 2. Summary of hearing threshold levels (dB HL) from audiograms taken from 20
trumpet/trombone players (top panel) and 37 violin/viola players (bottom panel), dem-
onstrating that trumpet/trombone players exhibited notches at 6 kHz preferentially in
the left ear. Boxes extend from lower quartile to upper quartile with median hearing
level demarked as the center of a notch; whiskers show extent of the contiguous data
(minus outliers). The average of the data is plotted as a thick line. Notches in the inter-
quartile boxes are centered at the median and display the variability of the median be-
tween samples. The width of a notch is computed so that box plots whose notches do not
overlap have different medians at the p<0.05 significance level.
Table 1. Average hearing threshold levels (dB HL) for 4, 6, and 8 kHz from audiograms
from (1) the entire musician group, (2) 20 trumpet/trombone players, and (3) 37 vio-
lin/viola payers. All groups show similar trends, with higher thresholds in the left ear at
6 kHz and perhaps higher thresholds in the right ear at 8 kHz. These factors combine to
form a more visible notch in left-ear audiogram traces, such as those in Figures 1 and 2.
ures 1 and 2), with the left ear being more likely to display a notch at 6 kHz.
The notch was more likely to be salient in the left because: (1) hearing thresh-
old levels at 6 kHz were generally higher (but not statistically higher, p=0.15
in paired t-test) in the left than in the right and (2) hearing threshold levels at
8 kHz were generally higher (but not statistically significantly higher, p=0.29
in a paired t-test) in the right ear (see Table 1).
DISCUSSION
Our audiogram data reveal a statistically significant notch in the left ear at 6
kHz across the group of young musiciansa potential hallmark of noise-in-
duced hearing lossbut interestingly, no such notch appeared for the right
ear data. We initially suspected that this asymmetry, clearly apparent across
the whole group, may have been caused by a subgroup of violin/viola players
who receive higher levels of sound in their left ears. However, data for this
group alone did not show a preponderance of the asymmetry. An established
body of literature suggests that there may be differences in the physiological
susceptibility of left and right ears to NIHL (Watson 1967). Exactly what type
of differences could account for the asymmetry is still a source of speculation.
For example, it is possible that olivocochlear efferents are stronger on the left
side or that there is a difference in the middle ear reflex between the two ears
(Nageris et al. 2007). It is also interesting to note that many reports indicate
that TEOAEs appear to be stronger in the right than in the left ear in infants
(Keefe et al. 2008). It should be noted that in many of these studies the right
ear is tested first, which could account for the asymmetry by producing an
efferent response prior to the subsequent left ear test.
In conclusion, the earliest audiometric indicator of impending NIHL for
musicians may be a developing notch at 6 kHz in the left ear.
Bradford C. Backus, Ear Institute, University College London, 332 Grays Inn Road,
London WC1X 8EE, UK; Email: b.backus@ucl.ac.uk
References
Keefe D. H., Gorga M. P., Jesteadt W., and Smith L. M. (2008). Ear asymmetries in
middle-ear, cochlear, and brainstem responses in human infants. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 123, pp. 1504-1512.
Lee J., Behar A., Kunov H., and Wong W. (2003). Noise exposure of opera orchestra
players. Canadian Acoustics, 31, pp. 78-79.
Nageris B. I., Raveh E., Zilberberg M., and Attias J. (2007). Asymmetry in noise-in-
duced hearing loss: Relevance of acoustic reflex and left or right handedness. Otology
and Neurotology, 28, pp. 434-437.
Lutman M. E. and Qasem H. Y. N. (1998). A source of notches at 6 kHz. In D. Prasher
and L. (eds.), Advances on Noise Research: Biological Effects of Noise (vol. 1, pp.
170-176). London: Whurr.
Royster J. D., Royster L. H., and Killion M. C. (1991). Sound exposures and hearing
thresholds of symphony orchestra musicians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 89, pp. 2793-2803.
Taylor W., Pearson J., Mair A., and Burns W. (1965). Study of noise and hearing in jute
weaving. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, pp. 113-120.
Watson J. E. (1967). Bilateral asymmetry in noise induced hearing loss. Annals of Otol-
ogy, Rhinology, and Laryngology, 76, pp. 1040-1042.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
When performing, pianists can listen and evaluate only from their position at
the keyboard. As for any further assessment and feedback, one can listen to a
recording after the event. But how faithful is it?
Pianists are often surprised on hearing a playback of their performances.
They frequently remark that the recording does not necessarily match their
perception of certain aspects of their interpretation. Vladimir Ashkenazy once
remarked in an interview:
In a recording studio, the difference is what finally gets onto tape, de-
pending on the properties of the acoustics and given piano. What hap-
pens very often is that what you hear on a playback is not exactly what
you thought you played, so you have to adjust. Then in the process of re-
recording, you have to have the results that will have the meaning that
you have inside you (Noyle 1987, p. 10).
232 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Things that work in performance are sometimes not good on records and
vice versa. Recording has its own lawsthats why I am not very much
for the recording of live performances (Dubal 1997, p. 7).
From the pianists point of view, the dry acoustics often found in studios,
the nature of the piano, the lack of audience, and the recording set up are all
factors that impact upon the way a recorded performance is perceived. Al-
though the set of issues surrounding each performance in a recording session
is very complex, we have to learn to deal with them. But how can we adjust
our technical and musical approaches in order to create a recording that
represents a faithful image of our interpretative ideals?
This paper explores some of the factors that influence the quality of the
artistic product achieved in a recording studio. It considers technical, musi-
cal, and conceptual adjustments that a pianist can make in order to achieve a
recorded performance that represents a faithful image of his or her interpre-
tative ideals. Due to space limitations, only three case studies will be pre-
sented. These have been selected either because they are unlikely to be
encountered in a regular concert performance or because they are intriguing
with respect to the ways in which recording equipment processes certain de-
tails of interpretation.
METHOD
Participants
The project is being conducted with the assistance of Kevin Roper, Senior
Music Producer with the Australian Broadcasting Corporation (ABC) in Ade-
laide. The pianist was Marija Bajalica.
Materials
Recordings used for this research were made at the studios of the ABC in
Adelaide. The piano used was a Steinway Grand, model D, which was one
year old at the time of the first recording.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 233
Procedure
RESULTS
The first case study is the Allegro from Mozarts Sonata in B flat KV570. The
theme in the lower voice in Figure 1 is broad and flowing and needs to be
crafted into a single thought.
The first recording revealed inconsistencies in sound quality within this
phrase (specifically in bars 102 and 106). The sudden disjunction in tone be-
tween the D eighth notes and B quarter note in the lower voice made it sound
as if the theme was being performed on two different instruments.
To achieve the desired unity of expression, it was necessary to suppress
the multiple personalities found in this particular register of the instrument.
In order to achieve this, an unusually lazy legato was used, providing more
time to blend and disguise the different tonal characters. Since no pedal is
used in this section and microphones were positioned reasonably close to the
strings, there was a concern that longer overlapping of the fingers would be-
come noticeable on the recording, yielding distasteful and stylistically inap-
propriate results.
The recording made during the second studio session did not reveal this
to be an issue. Neither the acoustics nor the microphones recognized the lazy
legato technique as an inadequate one. The disjunctions in sound were mini-
234 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 1. Allegro from W.A. Mozarts Sonata Bb Major KV 570 (bars 101-108).
Figure 2. Allegretto from W.A. Mozarts Sonata Bb Major KV 570 (Bars 1 to 5).
Figure 3. Andante dolente section of Juliets Girlhood from Prokofievs Romeo and
Juliet Suite for Piano Op. 75 (bar 82).
mized and the legato that was achieved sounded rather like a cello. With
fewer disturbances in the lower voice, rests in the first voice became trans-
parent, providing a clear entry of the melody in the upper voice.
The second case study is from the third movement, Allegretto, of the
same Sonata, shown in Figure 2. The light-hearted, even irresponsible char-
acter of the theme dominates this movement. To complement its mood, the
pulse unit should be treated in a leisurely fashion, but with no delays.
The initial feeling after the first recording was that it had achieved the
right pulse and that the variety of detached notes were properly felt and exe-
cuted. Subsequent evaluation, however, concluded that the differences be-
tween finger staccato, hand staccato, and all different articulations from
portato to staccato could not be clearly distinguished. This compromised
some of the shades of expression that were being sought. Furthermore, a
certain amount of sostenuto can be heard in the theme that was not obvious
at the time of playing. This can be attributed to the splashing acoustics of the
hall that did not allow staccato notes to clear in time for the next to proceed.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 235
Attempts to hear the end of one tone before producing the next ended in cre-
ating impression of sostenuto, which was undesirable for this movement.
The general conclusion from the first recording of the Allegretto was that
the attempt to engage with the acoustics and large proportions of the hall did
not result in the type of sound that was desired. The recording equipment
captured the initial moment that the sound was produced but very little of its
full journey. Therefore the decision was made that the next recording of the
same piece would explore the accustomed interpretation without any sponta-
neous adjustments provoked by the studio environment. There is a sense in
which this is counterintuitive as pianists routinely make fine adjustments in
response to what they are hearing.
The follow-up session proved that this was an extremely difficult and
awkward task. The second recording did, however, achieve something much
closer to a desired interpretation of the work. Its character is now supported
by an appropriate variety of articulation, and an effective pulse enables de-
tached notes to speak freely yet coherently.
The third case study is the Andante dolente section of Juliets Girlhood
from Prokofievs Romeo and Juliet Suite for Piano Op.75. It implies, for the
first time in the suite, a tragic conclusion to the opus, a shadow forming over
Juliets portrait. While shifting away from the rest of the piece, it speaks
through a dimmed tone of voice in the theme of the left hand (see Figure 3)
and a continuous vibration in the upper register.
In the first recording, the Andante dolente achieved its tranquillo state to
a certain extent. Some mild finger action was employed in executing the
thirty-second notes in the upper voice. The recording revealed that the mi-
crophones picked up every bit of that action so that the mechanics of the pi-
ano (the hammer action in particular) became overwhelmingly present in the
sound. Consequently, the right hand passage sounded more as a work of labor
and less as a continuous vibration. Along with it, the lower voice had lost its
peace. Dynamics in this section were very well balanced, so the main concern
for the re-recording session was the substance of the tone.
To project this section more successfully, the voices had to become more
specific in shades of expression. The lower voice required a somber quality of
tone, while the upper needed to achieve the effect of an inevitable and con-
tinuous occurrence.
Issues regarding the melody were resolved by voicing. The somber color
of tone can be obtained by feeling the key motion with greater sensitivity so
that any fast contact with the bottom of the key is avoided. By making a very
slow and controlled descent to the bottom of the key, the moment and the
236 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
place of the birth of the tone becomes uncertain. Supported with the right
amount of sound, the gloomy and somber effect is achieved.
To eliminate the intrusion of the hammer action in the right hand sound
and to accomplish the effect of vibration, finger action needs to be minimized.
The sound should be produced by a swaying of the weight over the keys, em-
ploying as little finger movement as possible.
This method was easier to implement than in the case of the Mozart work,
as it utilized only recognized pianistic skills. In fact, the acoustics of the hall
worked to the benefit of the piece and were of great help in the delivery of the
final product.
DISCUSSION
This paper addresses a set of issues that has been remarked upon by many
professional pianists but which has escaped serious, structured attention. It
identifies particular technical and artistic approaches that enable the pianist
to make the transition from concert hall to recording studio without com-
promising artistic outcomes. The first case study confirms that subtle changes
in articulation can be made to counteract tonal irregularities between regis-
ters. Such irregularities would go unnoticed in the concert hall but become
magnified in a recording. The second case study established that focusing
attention on the immediate sound feedback received from within the piano
(rather than from the acoustics of the hall) could be a key to a successful re-
cording. The final example concludes that studio recording requires much
greater sensitivity towards the key motion if traces of the hammer action are
to be cleared from the recorded sound. In cases where subtle shades create an
essence of the tone, this proved to be a crucial issue.
References
Noyle L. J. (1987). Pianists on Playing. Metuchen, New Jersey, USA: Scarecrow Press.
Dubal D. (1997). Reflections from the Keyboard. New York: Schirmer Books.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
Two professional gypsy musicians from Ceua, a small village of the Trgu
Mure region, were invited to the INSERM Laboratory of the University of
Burgundy. They are among the best (and last) interpreters of Transylvanian
village music, were born in 1953 and 1951, and have played together since
they were children. Csanyi plays the melody on the violin and Csangalo the
rhythmic-harmonic accompaniment on the contr, a specially prepared viola
with three strings tensed on a bridge with a flattened curve, tuned A-D-G.
Materials
attached to six tripods, 2 m from the ground on each side of the subject, at a
distance of 3 m from each subjects body. In this experiment, the motion of
only two markers was analyzedthat is, the ones located on the top of the
bows. Kinematic parameters in three dimensions (X, Y, and Z) were calcu-
lated from successive frames taken at 10 ms intervals. Kinematic variables
were low-pass filtered using a digital second-order Butterworth filter at a cut-
off frequency of 5 Hz. Sound was recorded separately and the session was
filmed with two additional standard cameras. Synchronization between sound
and image was obtained by using a clapper board equipped with two addi-
tional retroreflective markers.
Procedure
RESULTS
Figure 1 shows the rhythmical pattern obtained by tracking the marker posi-
tioned at the top of the viola bow on the Y dimension. The periodical cycle of
the aksak rhythm is clearly recognizable. The short (S) and long (L) durations
for each period were obtained by measuring the time interval between one
pick and the next. The picks indicate a change in the bow direction, corre-
sponding to the beginning of a short rhythmic unit (superior picks) and of a
long rhythmic unit (inferior picks). This measure was obtained with a margin
of error of 7 ms. For the test melody (Duo14), short and long durations vary
as follows: 1.05 sS1.127 s, and 1.541 sL1.817 s. The mean value of S/L
ratio for Duo14 is 0.7 (corresponding to S/L=2.2/857, if S=2 as in aksak conven-
tion), and varies largely from period to period in the same performance
(2/3.044S/L2/2.448). The mean values of S/L ratio for the three performances of
the same melody are very similar: S/L=0.695 for Duo12 and 0.696 for Duo13.
Finally, for all 8 melodies the proportion S/L respected the formula:
3/4S/L2/3 (see Table 1).
Figure 1. SMART viewer window. Top: Musicians virtual silhouettes (violinist left,
violist right). Bottom: Rhythmical pattern of Duo14, obtained by tracking the marker
positioned at the top of the viola bow on the Y dimension. (See full color version at
www.performancescience.org.)
Table 1. S/L mean values for eights de meseli tunes (Duo12, Duo13, and Duo14 are dif-
ferent performances of the same tune). Aksak (2/3) and 3/4 ratio are included in the table
for comparison.
Figure 2. Top: Superposition of violin and viola bow movements for Duo14 (extract).
Bottom: Transcription of melody (violin bow) and harmonic-rhythmic accompaniment
(viola bow) obtained from changes of bows directions. Lines linking melody to accom-
paniment highlights desynchronizations between the two performers.
DISCUSSION
Motion capture technologies have been used here to study two parameters of
Transylvanian de meseli repertoire in live performances: rhythm and swing,
defined as the desynchronization between melody and harmonic-rhythmical
accompaniment. The tracking of only two markers (positioned at the top of
the bows) allowed us to obtain easy readable representations of (1) aksak
rhythmical patterns and (2) type and degree of desynchronizations. These
gesture-based representations served here both as a measuring tool and as a
support for musicological analysis and transcription.
While measures have been done for Turkish village music (using a sound
analyzer, Cler and Estival 1997), to our knowledge Transylvanian aksak rep-
ertoire has been analyzed only qualitatively (Bout 1997). In relation to a
similar repertoire as the one concerned here, Bout (1997) raised the problem
of determining if musicians conceive the rhythm in terms of an orthodox
aksak (2.3) or of a heterodox aksak (3.4). Results showed that the rhythm
concerned here is between the two: the de meseli repertoire is based on a
local variant of aksak characterized by the function 2/3<S/L<3/4.
Deviations from norm durations, which are generally linked to expressiv-
ity in live performances (Gabrielsson 1995), were found. While in the case of
turkish aksak, Cler and Estival (1997) described a stability of S/L deviations
along the entire performance, our results showed that they vary widely from
period to period. This difference may be attributed to the different social
function of the repertoire studied. Transylvanian listening tunes, as opposed
to Turkish dance tunes, may leave to the performers more liberty in rhythmi-
242 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
The first author has conducted field research in Transylvania from 2004-06 thanks to a
CNRS doctoral grant. This study was realized thanks to a financial support of the CNRS
Music, Cognition, Society network.
References
Mental rehearsal with the score is clearly connected with analytical study,
and analytical insight no doubt interacts with the performance plan. Under-
standing levels of structure and hierarchical organization or categorization
within various musical parameters, for instance, can help the performers
interpretation. Analysis connotes conceptual skills as well as allowing the
improvement of perceptual and motor skills, in particular those related to an
expressive performance.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
Our focus is on exploring how we map the conceptual and perceptual struc-
tures in the music to cognitive mental representations through score reading.
We will also explore the structure of the learning/performing process of a
new score, in connection with the implied cognitive models, to understand
better how it can inform performance through feedback.
Table 1. The stage(s) of the learning/performing process of a new score when perform-
ers find mental practice and musical analysis with the score useful (n=75).
Mental Analysis of
Stage rehearsal the score
Sight-reading the score for the first time 33 25
Exploring and getting to know the music 35 41
Becoming fully familiar with the piece 33 34
Playing the whole piece fluently and musically 26 21
Memorizing the piece 32 22
Trial performances 15 9
Other 2 2
priority to exploring structural aspects in the printed score, the other focusing
on exploring the music while playing, i.e. auditory rather than visual.
An exploratory survey on performers experience of mental rehearsal and
the importance of music analysis in learning and performing a new piece is
currently being conducted. Two questions are pertinent to this paper, and
asked at which stages the performers tended to find these strategies most
useful. Current results (data are still being collected) for mental practice and
analysis are shown in Table 1.
As is apparent from the Table, mental practice is particularly useful right
from before initial sight-reading up to memorizing the piece for performance.
Score analysis was thought to be most useful while the performer is exploring
and becoming fully familiar with the piece, but less so before sight-reading.
Reductive theories (e.g. Schenker 1979, Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983) reduce
the piece to its underlying contrapuntal-harmonic structure. They can explain
how listeners represent what is aurally understood in a piece of music. They
may clarify a pieces structure, reveal how it was composed, and draw atten-
tion to its hierarchical levels. Harmonic region based theories (e.g. Longuet-
Higgins 1987, Lerdahl 2001) evaluate harmonic distance, according to pitch
relationships, and map the harmonic progressions along the entire musical
piece. They account for sequential and hierarchical harmonic tension, voice-
leading, and harmonic attraction. Like reductive theories, they can represent
whole pieces of music graphically and succinctly.
Categorization theories (e.g. Delige and Mlen 1997, Ockelford 2005) ac-
count for surface level events and their similarities, transformations, and
derivations, and how these events can be categorized. Mathematically based
theories (e.g. pitch-class set, see Forte 1973) allow the discovery of rules and
their manipulation, explain permutations and combinations, and account for
levels of structure, particularly in contemporary music. Descriptive theories
explain and describe musical symbols and events along the score in a narra-
tive way. They promote insight into the referential meaning of the piece and
guide the aesthetic experience (e.g. Agawu 2009).
We can suppose that all the above theories, which talk mainly about how
we represent the music we hear, can be applied to how we analyze the printed
notation when we see a score. When expert musicians see a score, the analysis
they do may involve hearing the music internally and understanding its
structure, therefore creating an association between seeing and hearing.
IMPLICATIONS
The usage of mental rehearsal and analysis helps various aspects of perform-
ance practice, including sight-reading, audiation, memorization, and tech-
nique. It also has implications for pedagogy and teaching, by stimulating
intellectual inquiry, creative interpretation, and improvisatory skills in the
student. A good level of analysis implies expertise and helps performers to be
versatile, not least by enabling them to create their own learning strategies,
and to acquire a capacity for evaluation of their own (self-evaluation) and
others performances. It enables musicians to generate and use mental repre-
sentations efficiently, and this aspect is a fundamental requirement to be an
expert performer (Lehmann et al. 2007). Future studies will investigate the
effects of expertise on strategy development.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the survey respondents (from the UK, Portugal, and
Spain), and the Portuguese Government (MCTES, FCT) for financial support.
References
Agawu K. (2009). Music as Discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
248 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Rosie Burt-Perkins1,2
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
The term learning culture is not new to educational writing, yet there are
relatively few attempts to conceptualize the term or to operationalize it as a
theoretical tool. The exception is the work of James et al. (2007), who used
learning culture as a means of accessing the complexity of further (post com-
pulsory) education in the UK. At the center of their thinking is the definition
of learning culture as the social practices through which people learn (p.
23). So defined, it is not simply the contexts within which people learn but
rather the practices through which people learn, practices which promote,
inhibit or rule out certain kinds of learning (p. 28). The key assumption here
is that learning takes place in, and through, a cultural setting (Hodkinson and
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 251
James 2003). That is, if we are to meet the oft-posed challenge of changing
the culture, we need to look beyond the one-to-one lesson, practice studio, or
concert platform to discover the practices that belie what can and is learned
at conservatoires; how different learning cultures enable or disable different
learning possibilities for the people that come into contact with them (James
et al. 2007, p. 28).
As yet, learning culture has not been adapted for theoretical use in higher
education institutions or in music education. While space limits a full con-
ceptualization here, there are two central tenets to the theory as we consider
its application to performance-based institutions:
IMPLICATIONS
This paper has proposed learning culture as a theoretical lens for tackling the
under-researched area of conservatoire culture. At the time of writing, field-
work has been recently completed at a UK conservatoire, limiting this paper
to a discussion of theoretical, rather than empirically-based, implications.
However, the potential for new understandings is, I argue, two-fold. First,
learning culture offers understanding of the hidden skills of performance;
those that accompany the more frequently researched motor skills, psycho-
logical training, and physiological preparation. In assuming that learning
occurs through social practice, learning culture emphasizes the skills involved
in mediating the (often blurred) transition from student to professional,
capturing ways of knowing and doing that are integral to the process of be-
coming a musician but that remain remarkably under-researched. Institu-
tional practices such as criteria for orchestral selection, for example, or social
practices such as peer networking, play a large but often sidelined part in
performers success. Knowledge of these practices has the potential to inform
both student experience and institutional curricula, as conservatoires strive to
offer competitive and world-leading preparation for performance-based ca-
reers.
Second, learning culture answers Jrgensens (2009) call for further re-
search focusing on conservatoires institutional culture. Identified as an area
where research is lacking, refrains such as we need to change the culture are
not uncommon. Yet such statements are notoriously slippery, referring to
practices that are historically located, influenced by wider policy agendas, and
often fraught with divided opinion and inaction. In viewing learning as occur-
ring through cultural practice, learning culture provides a theoretical lens
that grounds cultural discussions in the core task of educating young per-
formers. That is, institutional culture and the learning of performers are con-
sidered as inextricably intertwined and best understood as such. The first step
to transforming culture may well be to understand it; indeed, in establishing
performance science and its associated curriculum development within the
conservatoire landscape, knowledge of the learning culture at play may prove
a valuable tool.
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank Pamela Burnard, University of Cambridge, for her thoughtful,
challenging and supportive supervision of the ongoing PhD on which this paper is
based.
254 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Rosie Burt-Perkins, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince
Consort Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: rperkins@rcm.ac.uk
References
Barratt E. and Moore H. (2005). Researching group assessment: Jazz in the conserva-
toire. British Journal of Music Education, 22, pp. 299-314.
Bloomer M. (2001). Young lives, learning and transformation: Some theoretical consid-
erations. Oxford Review of Education, 27, pp. 429-449.
Bourdieu P. (1984). Distinction. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Harvard University
Press.
Hodkinson P. and James D. (2003). Transforming learning cultures in further educa-
tion. Journal of Vocational Education and Training, 55, pp. 389-406.
James D., Biesta G., Colley H. et al. (2007). Improving Learning Cultures in Further
Education. London: Routledge.
Janesick V. (2000). The choreography of qualitative research design: Minuets,
improvisations and crystallization. In N. Denzin and Y. Lincoln (eds.), Handbook of
Qualitative Research (pp. 379-400). Thousand Oaks, California, USA: Sage.
Jrgensen H. (2004). Strategies for individual practice. In A. Williamon (ed.), Musical
Excellence (pp. 85-104). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Jrgensen H. (2009). Research in and for conservatoires: An overview. Paper presented
at The Reflective Conservatoire, Guildhall School of Music and Drama, London.
Kingsbury H. (2001). Music, Talent, and Performance. Philadelphia: Temple University
Press.
Kreutz G., Ginsborg J., and Williamon A. (2009). Health-promoting behaviours in con-
servatoire students. Psychology of Music, 37, pp. 47-60.
Lebler D. (2008). Popular music pedagogy: Peer learning in practice. Music Education
Research, 10, pp. 193-213.
Nettl B. (1995). Heartland Excursions. Urbana, Illinois, USA: University of Illinois
Press.
Williamon A. (2004). Musical Excellence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The term narrative has been addressed differently in research fields such as
literary studies, linguistics, aesthetics, and anthropology. In musical research,
different approaches to narrative have been discussed (Almn 2008, Klein
2004, Maus 1988, Tarasti 1979), resulting in several reorientations of the
concept: new consensus is developing about musical narrative that is
aware both of the limitations of musical expression and of the rich potential
of music as a narrative medium (Almn 2008, p. 3).
256 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
All these studies shed light on an issue that can be observed through dif-
ferent angles, but common to virtually all approaches to musical narrative is
the recognition of a degree of similarity between musical and literary dis-
course (Almn 2008, p. 11), a similarity that can be extended to theatrical
discourse, since narrative mechanisms native to one mediumfrequently
cross-pollinate with other media, resulting in complex semantic hybrids (p.
38).
In music, the use of theatrical devices can enhance performance. They are
also linked to ritual practices, a powerful idea that has been, and still is, ap-
plied to compositional works. In the context of theatrical and performance
studies, this idea has been developed by authors such as Richard Schechner,
and it remains a key concept in ethnomusicology studies: Rituals are collec-
tive memories encoded into actions. Play gives people a chance to tempo-
rarily experience the taboo, the excessive, and risky. Thus, ritual and play
transform people (Schechner 2007, p. 52). If musical narrative is envisaged
as a sequence of ritual structures, which involve an emotional attachment
from the audience to the musical work, it then becomes one of the effects that
a composer intends to produce in the listener, and the performer, as media-
tor, plays an essential role in this process.
This paper aims to demonstrate that the concepts of musical narrative
and ritual dimension cannot be seen as isolated objects but as entities of
transformation by composers, and how the trilogy composer/performer/
listener (audience) is associated with musical narrative and ritual.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
This research lies on the concepts of ritual, transgression, and musical narra-
tive, as applied in a case study of a musical theatre piece, Sound Bridges. The
study focuses on how musical gesture takes different meanings in the trilogy
composer/performer/listener and discusses different concepts connecting the
role of embodied ritual in performance, and its effects in the listener.
In traditional musical narrative, the focus is on construction, and one
could speak of fields around which hierarchies, systems, and rules of musical
language are built. In this piece, we find transgressive musical gestures, acted
out by performers, combined with conventional narrative procedures, as the
successive suspensions in the piece generate pauses in the discourse and
challenge conventional hierarchical values. An effective non-verbal communi-
cation by the performers can mediate a narrative deconstruction of the lis-
teners expectations, through the connection between musical gestures on the
one hand, and musical narrative and ritual on the other.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 257
Verbal tools
The pieces title and the program notes are verbal communicative tools, which
function as a direct way of transmitting an intention from the composer to the
performer and to the listener, and engage them into a narrative strategy.
Tempo indications, agogic and dynamics, as well as music theatre also func-
tion as means of verbal communication between composer and performer,
and can act indirectly in the listeners perception of narrative.
Non-verbal tools
Mobile ring tone: In concerts, turning off phones is a common ritual. Nev-
ertheless the audience is willing to forgive the transgression.
Score pages out of order: An uncommon, but possible, mishap. One
friend, a member of the audience, comments: Such bad luck! Maybe they
should restart.
Players cough: At this point some members of the audience realize that
the interruptions may be intentional.
Motionless and repetition: The performers stop, motionless, for 15 to 25 s;
then they repeat the same phrase, over and over again. Most of the audi-
ence understands that the first three interruptions were planned and that
the piece includes an intended transgression.
Leaving the stage: One of the performers continues to play while the other
two leave the stage.
IMPLICATIONS
Music theatre can thus function as a relevant field for research of non-verbal
techniques. Pieces written for the theatre can then be seen as a theatrical ac-
tion that is generated and determined by the music; in Sound Bridges these
theatrical actions, previously called blocs of music and interruptions, are gen-
erated by the music itself and allow for the deconstruction of musical narra-
tive through ritualized forms of transgression. Ritual and transgression can
be linked to traditional concepts of musical narrative connecting composing,
performing, and listening activities. The concept of ritual can be successfully
manipulated by the composer and the performer, particularly in the context
of contemporary music. Transgression of ritual, as planned by the composer,
can act as a deconstructive factor; as mediators, performers take a crucial role
in the process, allowing the creation of multiple sensations and experiences in
the listener.
References
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Procedure
RESULTS
Along with the data collection, several other occasions demonstrated that
the interviews, since they implied moments of reporting their repertoire
preparation, functioned as a catalyst for reflection upon students strategies,
their problems, and their conquests. Listed below are some excerpts from the
students comments.
The first semester student regretted his approach toward Bachs Fugue in
the first recall stimulated interview. He said:
This Fugue! I prepared the first page for almost four months. In the
exam, it was clear that the first page was good, and the others were infe-
rior to the first one! This would be unacceptable now! I need to read the
whole piece, even though I cannot play it well. But I have to read it com-
pletely, to realize: Here I will need to spend much time!
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 265
The cases also provided some other interesting testimonials. The first se-
mester student in the third recall stimulated interview mentioned:
For me, it is always worthy to show for other people what I am doing.
And in this way, recording, it became worthier. Now I can watch. I
think that this self-critic is very important. It helps! Sometimes it is more
efficient than any other thing.
The fifth semester student, during the third recall stimulated interview
commented:
DISCUSSION
effective tools for instrumental teaching and learning, since they allow the
students to expose their thoughts and actions regarding preparation in order
to value their own musical experience. During the process of data collection,
piano undergraduate interviewees were active agents in their preparation and
demonstrated awareness about their weaknesses and strengths in music self-
knowledge. Fostering situations, in which the student may be given room to
question and to reflect upon his/her own process and products may be a
valuable strategy in instrumental teaching. Videos of the students perform-
ance may provide opportunity for the piano teacher and student to reflect
upon the instrumental practice and performance.
Regina A. T. dos Santos, Graduate Music Program, Federal University of Rio Grande do
Sul, Rua Prof. Annes Dias 112, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul 90020-090, Brazil;
Email: jhsreg@adufrgs.ufrgs.br
References
Expired air provides the basis for sound production for musicians playing
a wind instrument. This air stream must be controlled and directed into
the mouthpiece of the instrument via a reed mechanism at the mouth. To
be able to do this, firm velopharyngeal closure is required to prevent air
leaking out through the nose from the oral cavity. In some musicians im-
pairment of soft palate function may result in this air leak occurring,
which is known as velopharyngeal insufficiency (VPI). A review of the
functional anatomy of the soft palate and how it relates to wind and brass
playing is discussed with a brief review of VPI as it is reported in musi-
cians. A better understanding of the soft palate anatomy will assist stu-
dents and music teachers to optimize their performance and prevent
performance related medical problems.
the nasopharynx being incomplete, allowing air to escape from the nose when
playing (Ingrams et al. 2000). Other than in musicians, this condition is most
frequently reported as a consequence of a structural cleft palate deformity,
hence is often termed stress VPI when it occurs as a result of the stresses
imposed by playing a wind instrument. It has been observed that this condi-
tion usually occurs in advanced students pursuing a professional career, due
to an increase in physically demanding practice and performance regimen
which imposes undue stress on the soft palate over a prolonged period of time
(Schwab and Schultze-Florey 2004).
The majority of research into VPI has been by speech pathologists or oto-
laryngologists, and the literature deals primarily with cleft palate or speech
disorders. Of only ten articles investigating VPI in musicians, fourteen cases
were reported (see Table 1).
A dissertation thesis presented a case series of clarinetists with VPI (Gib-
son 1995). The author later published two articles summarizing his research
and outlining recent trends in the treatment of VPI (Gibson 1998, 2008).
There have been three questionnaire studies conducted in London, Germany,
and the USA. The authors reported, respectively, that 7%, 31%, and 34% of
the participating student and professional wind musicians have experienced
VPI (Ingrams et al. 2000, Schwab and Schultze-Florey 2004, Malick et al.
2007). Due to the small sample sizes of these studies, these figures do not
accurately represent the significance of this condition, although informal
discussions with fellow musicians and music teachers suggest that this condi-
tion is a well-known phenomenon.
The purpose of this review is to present the functional anatomy of the soft
palate and how this anatomy potentially relates to wind and brass instrument
performance.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
When playing a wind instrument, air passes from the lungs up into the upper
respiratory tract and is eventually channeled into the mouthpiece of the in-
strument via some form of reed mechanism. As the air meets this resistance
when flowing into the instrument there is an increase in pressure in the upper
respiratory cavity, requiring firm control of the soft palate.
The soft palate, also known as the velum, separates the nasopharynx from
the oropharynx. This closure of the two parts is known as velopharyngeal or
palatopharyngeal closure and is important for swallowing, speech, and blow-
ing. The soft palate extends posteriorly from the hard palate, and five muscles
control its movement. Each of the paired muscles can be divided into
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 269
their principal actions on the soft palate. The levator veli palatini and the
musculus uvulae are elevators, the palatoglossus and palatopharyngeus are
depressors, and the tensor veli palatini is a tensor.
The tensor veli palatini is the only muscle of the soft palate that is innervated
by the cranial nerve V (Moore and Dalley 2006). The muscle bellies are at-
tached superiorly to the scaphoid fossa of the medial pterygoid plate, the
spine of the sphenoid bone, and the cartilage of the pharyngotympanic tube.
It is then inferiorly attached to the palatine aponeurosis. The main action of
the tensor is to tense the soft palate which opens the pharyngotympanic tube
during swallowing and yawning (Moore and Dalley 2006). In playing a wind
instrument, the tensor acts with the levator to maintain velopharyngeal clo-
sure which allows the airflow through the mouth.
The levator veli palatini is innervated by the pharyngeal branch of the
vagus cranial nerve X and is superiorly attached to the cartilage of the
pharyngotympanic tube and the temporal bone. Like the tensor, the levator is
attached inferiorly to the palatine aponeurosis (Moore and Dalley 2006). The
levator is the most important muscle in the elevation of the soft palate during
swallowing and yawning. As described above, the action of the levator is im-
270 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
portant in closing the oronasal cavity ensuring airflow is directed through the
mouth.
The palatoglossus and the palatopharyngeus are innervated by the vagus
cranial nerve X (Moore and Dalley 2006). The palatoglossus is attached supe-
riorly to the palatine aponeurosis and is attached inferiorly to the side of the
tongue. The main action of this muscle is to elevate the posterior part of the
tongue and may also depress the soft palate onto the tongue (Moore and
Dalley 2006). If the palatoglossus depresses the soft palateopening the na-
sal cavity and elevating the tongue, thereby closing the oral cavitythen it
may be these actions that assist in the breathing technique known in musical
pedagogy as circular breathing. This technique, occasionally used by wind
instrumentalists, involves the simultaneous expiration of air through the
mouth and inspiration through the nose, which allows the player to blow
continuously through the instrument. Although circular breathing has been
discussed in the music literature, the action of these muscles needs further
research.
The palatopharyngeus is superiorly attached to the hard palate and the
palatine aponeurosis and is inferiorly attached to the lateral wall of the phar-
ynx. The palatopharyngeus tenses the soft palate and pulls the pharynx walls
superiorly, anteriorly, and medially during swallowing (Moore and Dalley
2006). Huang et al. (1998) found that the inferior action of the palatopharyn-
geus acts against the inferior action of the levator. Additionally, upon con-
traction of these muscles the velum stretches to make contact with the
posterior pharyngeal wall, enhancing velopharyngeal closure (Huang et al.
1998).
The musculus uvulae is also innervated by the vagus cranial nerve X
(Moore and Dalley 2006). The muscle is attached to the posterior nasal spine
and the palatine aponeurosis superiorly and is attached to the mucosa of the
uvula inferiorly. The main action of this muscle is to shorten the uvula and
pull it superiorly (Moore and Dalley 2006). It also assists the levator veli
palatini in palatopharyngeal closure by adding bulk to the soft palate, which
seals off the nasopharynx (Kuehn 1979). When the soft palate is elevated, the
uvula can be seen drawing upwards and backwards towards the back of the
throat.
Two other muscles that belong to the pharynx are also associated with the
soft palate. Both the superior pharyngeal constrictor and the salpingo-
pharyngeus are innervated by the pharyngeal branch of the vagus cranial
nerve X (Moore and Dalley 2006). The superior pharyngeal constrictor,
which is attached superiorly to the pterygoid hamulus, and the pterygoman-
dibular raphe, inserts into the pharyngeal aponeurosis forming the sides and
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 271
back walls of the nasopharynx and the back of upper oropharynx. The salpin-
gopharyngeus is attached to the cartilaginous part of the pharyngotympanic
tube and inserts inferiorly to the palatopharyngeus muscle (Moore and Dalley
2006). The superior constrictor, assisted by the salpingopharyngeus which
elevates the pharyngeal wall, pulls the pharyngeal wall forward to help gain
velopharyngeal closure.
IMPLICATIONS
As can be seen from this review, the function of the soft palate is essential for
maintaining upper respiratory tract structure under pressure and hence al-
lowing optimal airflow. It is crucial for wind and brass players to be able to
maintain firm velopharyngeal closure for optimum performance. Much more
research needs to be conducted into the mechanisms behind the performance
related problems, which may be associated with the soft palate. It is impor-
tant for musicians and music teachers to increase their understanding of the
functional anatomy and physiology for optimizing performance and injury
prevention.
Alison Evans, School of Public Health, Edward Ford Building A27, University of Sydney,
Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia; Email: evansa@health.usyd.edu.au
References
Gordon N. A., Astrachan D., and Yanagisawa E. (1994). Videoendoscopic diagnosis and
correction of velopharyngeal stress incompetence in a bassoonist. Annals of Otol-
ogy, Rhinology and Laryngology, 103, pp. 595-600.
Huang M. H. S and Lee S. T. (1998) Anatomic basis of cleft palate and velopharyngeal
surgery: Implications from a fresh cadaveric study. Plastic and Reconstructive Sur-
gery, 101, pp. 613-627.
Ingrams D. R., McFerran D. J., and Graham J. M. (2000). Velopharyngeal incompe-
tence in wind players. Physical and Emotional Hazards of a Performing Career:
Special issue of Music Performance, 2, pp. 105-112.
Klotz D. A., Howard J., Hengerer A. S., and Slupchynskj O. (2001). Lipoinjection aug-
mentation of the soft palate for velopharyngeal stress incompetence. Laryngoscope,
111, pp. 2157-2161.
Kuehn D. P. (1979). Velopharyngeal anatomy and physiology. Ear, Nose and Throat
Journal, 58, pp. 316-321.
Malick D., Moon J., and Canady J. (2007). Stress velopharyngeal incompetence: Preva-
lence, treatment, and management practices. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 44,
pp. 424-433.
Massengill R. and Quinn G. (1974). Adenoidal atrophy, velopharyngeal incompetence
and sucking exercises: A two year follow-up case. Cleft Palate Journal, 11, pp. 196-
199.
McVicar R., Edmonds J., and Kearns D. (2002). Sphincter pharygoplasty for correction
of stress velopharyngeal insufficiency. Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery,
127, pp. 248-250.
Moore K. L. and Dalley A. F. (2006). Clinically Oriented Anatomy (5e). Baltimore,
Maryland, USA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.
Peterson-Falzone S. J. (1985). Velopharyngeal inadequacy in the absence of overt cleft
palate. Journal of Craniofacial Genetics and Developmental Biology, Supplement
1, pp. 97-124.
Schwab B. and Schultze-Florey A. (2004). Velopharyngeal insufficiency in woodwind
and brass players. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 19, pp. 21-25.
Weber J. and Chase R. A. (1970). Stress velopharyngeal incompetence in an oboe player.
Cleft Palate Journal, 7, pp. 858-861.
Wolff (III) E. A. (1995). Medical corner (stress velopharyngeal insufficiency). Interna-
tional Trombone Association Journal, 23, pp. 14-15.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
This paper introduces some aspects of the Vincenzo Vitale piano school;
a twentieth century Italian school generated from the intersection of the
Neapolitan piano school and other European musical realities. Although
the School is recognized to be important by many, there is very limited
specific literature about either the School or Vincenzo Vitale (1908-84).
The thesis entitled The Piano Teaching of Vincenzo Vitale, undertaken
toward the completion of a Bachelors degree and specialization at the
University of Bologna in 2005, is still to my knowledge the only in-depth
scholarly study on the topic so far. The focus of the thesis was on the
principles of Vitales teachings, his theories, the formulation of these
theories, and their background.
Alongside the German, Russian, and French schools of piano playing during
the 1800s in Naples, the Neapolitan piano school, founded by Francesco
Lanza (1783-1862) and subsequently by Sigismund Thalberg (1812-71), was
flourishing. Well into the first part of the 1900s, Naples with its conservatory
was the center of piano playing in Italy. Pianists such as Michelangeli, Cic-
colini, Pollini, and most of the Italian pianists have their roots in this school.
Furthermore, the Neapolitan schools prerogatives of piano playing have been
exported outside of Italyfor example, Beniamino Cesi (1845-1907) taught
piano in St. Petersburg, Vincenzo Scaramuzza (1885-1968) in Buenos Aires,
and Vincenzo Vitale (1908-84) in the US. During the second half of the twen-
tieth century, the Vincenzo Vitale piano school was considered the apex of the
Neapolitan piano school and Vitale one of the most important Italian piano
teachers (e.g. Campanella 1994, Di Benedetto 2004, Valori 1981). Although
Vitale died in 1984, his name appears in many curricula vitae, some of whom,
in many cases, were just auditors to Vitales lectures; it would seem that his
274 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
provided Vitale with an axis that exemplified this concept of pianistic lexicon,
and that axis stands as a referential typology of pianistic sonority. Interest-
ingly enough, Vitale sourced the fundamental ingredients of the specific ty-
pology of sound, the cantabile and the brillante sound, from the brief
introduction in Thalbergs LArt du chant appliqu au piano volume pub-
lished in 1850 by Girard in Naples. As Rattalino (1983) observed, Thalberg
modeled these two sonorities on the singing voice (cantabile) of the bel canto,
specifically on the distinction between cantar di petto and cantar di grazia.
Schematically, Vitales interpretation toward the realization of the two
fundamental ideas of sound on the instrument concerned the amount of
weight of the arm complex (shoulder, forearm, and hand) released on the
keypad. The cantabile sound requires a major amount of weight to be released
on the keypad and the brillante sound requires a major amount of suspension
of the weight to be released on the keypad. The following was deduced in this
line of thought: (1) the more weight released on the keypad, the more the
finger must sustain the weight which limits its percussiveness; and (2) the
more weight that is suspended (in other words, the weight of the arm, fore-
arm, and even hand is sustained by the appropriate muscles that have this
function), the more the finger has the right muscular/articular condition to
solve its percussive action on the keypad. These observations brought Vitale
to assert that sound production on the piano can be categorized from the
point of view of touch mechanisms in only two fundamental techniques: the
weight technique (tecnica di peso) and the percussive technique (tecnica per-
cussiva).
The two different technique typologies were inferred from what Vitale be-
lieved the only two fundamentally different possibilities of physiological ac-
tion of the finger on the keypad. The finger can either sustain, as a point of
support, the weight of the arm (necessary in delivering the expressive effects
in legato, cantabile, and in polyphonic passages), or it can execute a percus-
sive action (what is commonly known as articulation) necessary in the per-
formance of fast passages. In Vitales theory, the two technical categories
(weight and percussive) serve as a referential point of piano playing. In real-
ity, Vitale spoke openly of a compromise of the two mechanisms, as piano
performance (lesecuzione pianistica) necessitates a continuous technical
constructthe hybrid technique.
Vitales necessity to find practical solutions in piano playing made him re-
alize that there is no separation between interpretation and technique of exe-
cution, as one informs the other constantly. In fact, Vitale in his notes
(undated notes transcribed by the author) writes that the Greek terms techne,
which stands for art, and technicos, which means serves art, clarify the
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 277
rhythmic variations (groups of two, three, or four notes). Vitale did not com-
pose the exercises; these were sourced from traditional piano pedagogy and
were assembled by him within a system of rational criteria of determined
aims.
IMPLICATIONS
The underlying problem is: what is, or can be, the use of the Vitale system of
teaching in the larger panorama of piano pedagogy? Possible solutions stand
in presenting the findings to a larger audience, and thus encourage scholarly
attention and debate to a substantial pianistic phenomenon.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to the following professors who have given me, and are giving me, the
skills in conducting the research on the Vitale School: Renato Di Benedetto, Maurizio
Giani, Neryl Jeanneret, and Rosalind Hurworth.
References
1 Independent researcher, UK
2 Department of Psychology, University of Buckingham, UK
This paper investigates the link between practice and level of expertise in
cryptic crossword solvers. A survey was conducted of 241 expert solvers,
which established that, even among crossword experts, the range and
frequency of cryptics solved and the time spent upon this varied greatly.
The link between practice and level of expertise was therefore not
straightforward, but seemed to be connected to the primary focus of the
solverwhether they aimed to speed-solve or solve advanced cryptics
of exceptional difficulty. A brief review of motivational drivers for solvers
of cryptics is also included.
Puzzles of all standards and for all ages are routinely available in newspapers
and magazines, and crazes for particular types of brainteaserssuch as Su-
dokuregularly sweep the world. One particularly long-lived puzzle type is
the crossword, which exists in two basic forms: cryptic and non-cryptic. In the
non-cryptic form, a clue word or phrase prompts the solver to discover a
synonym word which will fit into the grid and intersect with cross-checking
letters. This type of crossword is found throughout the world and is the main
(virtually the only) type of crossword to be found in the USA.
The focus of this study, however, is the cryptic crossword, which may be
found in broadsheet newspapers and quality magazines throughout the UK
and Commonwealth. In these puzzles, the surface reading of the clue is
phrased to be deliberately misleading. The solver learns to ignore this read-
ing, and to identify and crack a grammatical set of coded instructions which,
if executed precisely, will lead to the correct (and only) answer.
Although cryptic crosswords are not audience-oriented, many solvers
strive for self-improvement and recognition within the crossword commu-
280 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Procedure
RESULTS
In total, 256 people responded. Due to missing data, 241 questionnaires were
analysed (209 M, 32 F; mean age=53 years, range=23-83). The mean starting
age for solving cryptics was 17 years; solvers typically spent 10 years on
block-style cryptic crosswords before going on to more advanced ones and
were highly experienced (mean years solving=36, range=2-66). There were
221 Listener Crossword solvers, although the majority of respondents had
first seen the survey advertized on the Crossword Centre website (n=161).
Respondents tended to solve cryptics regularly (mean time spent=8 hours
per week, range=1-30) and from various newspapers (mean different cryptics
regularly attempted=4.5; range=0-28). There were, however, significant dif-
ferences within these ranges at the individual level.
282 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
These groups were first analyzed by the volume of crosswords they tack-
led (number of different crosswords solved by each participant, weighted by
the frequency with which they tackled them; see Table 1 for results). The
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 283
groups were then analyzed by the hours spent tackling cryptic crosswords
each week (Table 2). The average number of years spent solving cryptic
crosswords was also calculated for each sub-category (Table 3).
Respondents were also asked to consider and rate, on a 1-5 scale, a series
of 26 statements compiled specifically for the survey exploring a range of
potential motivational drivers (both extrinsic and intrinsic) behind their
solving behavior. Respondents replies indicated high levels of intrinsic moti-
vation. Primary intrinsic drives were related to (1) the buzz of succeeding in
an intellectual challenge (68% rated this very/highly motivating) and (2)
preserving mental sharpness (61% very/highly motivating). Expert cross-
word solving was seen as a solitary hobby: 88% of respondents claimed that
participation in team solving was not motivating for them. Generally, extrin-
sic motivational drivers such as prizes or public acclaim were also seen as less
important; however, 33% had participated in public speed-solving contests,
and 73% of those who tackled the Listener crossword (n=221) routinely sub-
mitted it for the official personal annual statistics, with 54% aiming to appear
on the Listener roll of honor.
DISCUSSION
Becoming an expert in any domain requires practice, hard work, and strong
motivation, both intrinsic and extrinsic. However, this study highlighted that
even among crossword experts the range and frequency of cryptics solved
varied greatly. Super-Experts in speed solving (alone or in combination
with high Listener expertise) seemed to attempt routinely a much broader
range of crosswords than others. It is possible that the high volume of clues
covered enables more of the problem solving process to become automatic for
these speed experts, leading to an increasingly instantaneous recognition of
the clue architecture. By contrast, those who were solely Listener Super-Ex-
perts solved in a conspicuously more focused range. Despite this, the time
spent on crosswords per week was relatively constant across the Super-Ex-
pert categories, with 77-82% of participants spending 0-10 hours on cross-
word solving, predominantly in the 6-10 hours category: the rapidity of the
Speed experts may have contributed to their ability to cover a wider range
of material in relatively quicker time; whereas the Listener Super-Experts
may have deliberately focused on harder and more time-consuming puzzles.
Those falling into the Ordinary Expert category, however, generally
lagged behind the Super-Expert categories both in the range of crosswords
tackled and in the time spent, with 49% of respondents spending 5 or fewer
284 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
hours per week on solving activities. This suggests that practice may be one
key factor in achieving Super-Expert status.
Expert solvers were highly intrinsically motivated, and pursued cryptic
crossword solving primarily as a source of intellectual stimulation and pleas-
ure. Solving is a solitary pastime, however, and many experts seemed to use
various extrinsic goals, such as the maintenance of annual statistics, as a
source of extra extrinsic incentive to supplement their own internal drive.
Acknowledgments
Many thanks to John Green, Peter Biddlecombe, and Derek Harrison for helping us to
advertize the survey with the annual Listener statistics, at the Times speed-solving com-
petition and on The Crossword Centres message board.
References
METHOD
Participants
Procedure
They were asked to play four beats for three minutes under the tempi denoted
above. The musical score played in this experiment is shown in Figure 1. All
the participants were allowed to rest for a minimum of one minute after each
performance. The flexor carpi ulnaris muscle of both arms, extensor carpi
ulnaris muscle of both arms, metronome signal, and oscillation of drumhead
were all measured. The movement of flexor carpi ulnaris muscle corresponds
to the movement of bending the wrist forward, and the movement of extensor
carpi ulnaris muscle corresponds to the movement of bending the wrist back-
ward.
RESULTS
To analyze the short-term playing strategy, mean EMG of the range of around
500 ms centered at the time a drumstick strikes a drum is called one-stroke
mean EMG and is compared among each measured muscle and each drum-
mer (see Figure 2). The results of the analysis of one-stroke mean EMG are
shown in Tables 1 and 2. Table 1 shows the results of a comparison between
the one-stroke mean EMGs of the flexor carpi ulnaris muscles and those of
extensor carpi ulnaris muscles for all participants. Table 2 shows the styles of
arm movement in terms of muscle for each participant. Table 2 confirms that
significant differences in the one-stroke mean EMG of all participants exten-
sor carpi ulnaris muscles are less/more than those of flexor carpi ulnaris
muscles. Flexor dominance is when the one-stroke mean EMG of the
extensor carpi ulnaris muscle is less than that of the flexor carpi ulnaris mus-
cle. Extensor dominance meanwhile is when the one-stroke mean EMG of
the flexor carpi ulnaris muscle is less than that of the extensor carpi ulnaris
muscle.
288 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 1. Results of comparison between one-stroke mean EMG of flexor carpi ulnaris
muscle and one-stroke mean EMG of extensor carpi ulnaris muscle.
DISCUSSION
Tables 1 and 2 confirm that four players have the same style of motor control
in both arms. Specifically, all trained drummers have this feature, whereas
just one non-drummer has it, indicating that trained drummers use the same
style of motor control in both arms when playing the drums. Conforming
right arm movements to left arm movements is thought to be essential to play
the drums without making mistakes throughout a piece of music. In short,
trained drummers play drums with the same style of motor control in both
arms under the short-term playing strategy.
Table 3 confirms that non-drummers mean EMGs during the last one minute
were significantly higher than those of the first one minute. Such results were
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 289
Table 3. Results of comparison between mean EMGs during first and last one minute.
obtained only for the non-drummers. This demonstrates that only non-drum-
mers mean EMGs during the last one minute were significantly higher than
those of the first one minute, indicating that trained drummers are suffi-
290 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
ciently relaxed when playing the drums throughout a piece of music. That is
to say, trained drummers play the drums with less strain in terms of muscle
control under the long-term playing strategy.
Acknowledgments
This study was partly supported by the High-Tech Research Center Project for Private
Universities matching fund subsidy from the Japanese Ministry of Education, Culture,
Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT) and Research for Promoting Technological
Seeds from the Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST).
References
Veronica Gaspar
Veronica Gaspar, 5-7 Biserica Amzei, Bucharest 010391, Romania; Email: gasparveron
ica@yahoo.com
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Yasuhiro Goto
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
Scarlattis Sonata in C Major, K. 159 was chosen due to its structural propor-
tions and easy accessibility. In 6/8 and with a range of D-d, it is marked
allegro in various manuscripts. This bi-partite sonata is 64 measures long (26
296 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
bars+38 bars) or 128 measures with the repetitions and no first or second
endings. In the overall AB form, the B part consists of B+A (or rounded bi-
nary form with developmental aspects and recapitulation), which may be seen
with repetitions as: [A1] [A2] [B1, A3,] [B2, A4]: A2=repeat of A1, B2=repeat
of B1, A3=variant of A1 in recapitulation, A4=repeat of A3. The crux appears
in measure (ms.) 13 (A section) and in ms. 52 (=ms. 26 of that section or ms.
9 of the recapitulation).
Before measuring the placement of the crux, a one measure discrepancy
was found in various manuscripts and editions in the first half (ms. 16); this
had to be considered with the logical conclusion that Scarlattis copyist
accidentally omitted it because of its repetitious nature. Gilberts Urtext
edition was chosen because of the logical symmetry of the sonatas halves.
The phi or golden section measurement of each half was taken in order to
compare with the crux position (Table 1). Although GS proportion is not
present, a mirrored symmetrical proportion is found: the crux occurs three
measures before phi in the first half and three measures after phi in the
second half. A proportion of +3 measure-differential (almost 4 measures)
occurs when exclusively measuring the crux position of the recapitulation.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 297
Real time Crux Crux End End End End Crux Crux End End
A1 A2 A1 A2 B1 B2 A3 A4 A3 A4
ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms.
13 13 26 26 43 43 52 52 64 64
(39) (52) (95) (107) (88) (116) (90) (128)
Total
duration
Asperen 11,294 35,357 23,713 47,790 65,435 102,401 73,318 110,197 84,748 125,991
MM=128
Crudelli 11,379 35,861 24,403 48,774 66,685 105,434 74,537 113,202 87,077 128,896
MM=126
Nicolson 11,834 36,628 24,883 50,000 68,579 108,02876,761 116,186 87,077 131,788
MM=120
Halton 13,506 42,483 28,587 57,613 79,386 123,419 88,519 132,452102,235 149,432
MM=113
Pogorelich13,304 41,196 27,942 56,550 77,080 122,082 86,063 131,248 101,065 149,316
Model
MM=112
Harper 13,542 42,468 28,742 57,543 78,539 124,424 88,085 134,061103,112 153,162
MM=110
Tilney 14,252 44,176 30,048 59,786 81,062 125,922 90,541 135,561 104,998153,074
MM=110
Performers proportion
Real time Crux Crux End End End End Crux Crux End End
A1 A2 A1 A2 B1 B2 A3 A4 A3 A4
ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms. ms.
13 13 26 26 43 43 52 52 64 64
(39) (52) (95) (107) (88) (116) (90) (128)
Total
duration
Asperen 13,326 41,814 28,166 56,64077,502 121,351 86,779 130,556 100,412 149,316
length
ratio=0,843
Crudelli 13,08641,423 28,26656,665 77,274 122,08286,290131,081 100,861 149,316
ratio=0,863
Nicolson 13,256 41,323 28,156 56,607 77,641 122,370 86,962 131,547 101,000149,316
ratio=0,882
Halton 13,506 42,48328,587 57,613 79,386 123,419 88,519 132,452 102,235 149,316
ratio=1,007
Pogorelich 13,304 41,196 27,942 56,550 77,080122,08286,063131,248 101,065 149,316
MODEL
ratio=1,000
Harper 13,784 41,333 28,00155,983 76,540 121,289 85,861 130,693100,523149,316
ratio=1,0257
Tilney 13,784 42,95029,296 58,32479,053 122,823 88,286132,262 102,393 149,316
ratio=1,0251
Figure 2. Real time and equalized timings; Pogorelich and Halton were the same in real
time; all recordings were adjusted to Pogorelichs model for equalized timings. (See full
color version at www.performancescience.org.)
end of A3, and end of A4. This information gave foreground or surface tempo
measurements. Tempo and real time timings ranged from MM 110 to MM128
and from 125.991 to 153.074. To determine the internal or background
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 299
Table 4. Real time/reference model time: table ratio comparisons at key moments.
Real time/ Crux Crux End End End End Crux Crux End End
reference A1 A2 A1 A2 B1 B2 A3 A4 A3 A4
model time
Asperen 0,84900,8582 0,8486 0,8450 0,8489 0,8387 0,8589 0,8396 0,8385 149,
length >0,843> > > > > > - > 316
ratio=0,843
Crudelli 0,8553 0,8704 0,8733 0,8624 0,8651 0,8636 0,8660 0,8625 0,8615 149,
ratio=0,863 <() > > <() > > > -<() <() 316
Nicolson 0,8895 0,8891 0,8905 0,8841 0,8897 0,8848 0,8919 0,8852 0,8820 149,
ratio=0,882 > > > > > > > > > 316
Halton 1.0157 1,0312 1,0230 1,0187 1,0299 1,0109 1,0285 1,0091 1,0115 149,
ratio=1,007 316
Pogorelich 13,304 41,196 27,942 56,550 77,080 122,082 86,063 131,248 101,065 149,
MODEL 1,000 1,000 1,000 1,000 1,000 1,000 1,000 1,000 1,000 316
ratio=1,000
Harper 1,0178 1,030 1,0286 1,0159 1,0189 1,0191 1,0234 1,0214 1,0214 149,
ratio=1,0257 <! >() >() <! <! <! <! <! <! 316
Tilney 1,0712 1,0723 1,0753 1,0572 1,0516 1,0314 1,0520 1,038 1,0389 149,
ratio=1,0251 >() >() >() >() >() >() >() >() >() 316
and analysis of length ratios reveal that Nicholsons performance has the
highest rate of convergences toward the timings of the model at 100%: As-
peren=88%, Harper=77%, Crudelli=55%, and Tilney=0%.
IMPLICATIONS
It is concluded that: (1) although the crux does not fit into direct GS propor-
tions, the symmetrical mirrored relationship between the two halves of the
sonata of three measures is significant, (2) when repeats are made by the
performers, the length of the A section corresponds to the occurrence of the
crux in the B section (52 measures), making a strong case for the necessity of
the repeats, and (3) regardless of live or commercial recordings or choice of
instruments, there occurred two performances of the same duration: Po-
gorelich (piano, commercial/model performance), Halton (harpsichord, live
performance). We may infer that there is a proportional sense of crux by
composer and by performers, which is evident in both foreground (surface)
and background (structural) tempi.
Nancy Lee Harper, Department of Communication and Art, University of Aveiro, Cam-
pus Santiago, 3810-193 Aveiro, Portugal; Email: harper@ua.pt
References
Halton R. (2002) Domenico Scarlatti and his Cantabile Sonatas. Musicology Australia,
25, pp.22-48.
Harper N. L. (2007) Golden section in the sonatas of Domencio Scarlatti: A re-exami-
nation of Kirkpatricks crux. In A. Williamon and D. Coimbra (eds.), Proceedings
of ISPS 2007 (pp.239-244). Utrecht, The Netherlands: European Association of
Conservatoires (AEC).
Harper N. L. and Henriques T. (2008). Performing musical structure: Crux-phi percep-
tions in Domenico Scarlattis sonata K. 380. Paper presented at the fourth Confer-
ence on Interdisciplinary Musicology (CIM08), Thessaloniki, Greece.
Scarlatti D. (1978). D. Scarlatti. Sonates-vol. IV, K. 156-205, le pupitre, Collection de
Musique Ancienne Publie sous la Direction de Franois Lesure. Paris: Heugel.
Recordings: van Asperen B. (1992). Domenico Scarlatti Sonate. Amersterdam: EMI
Classics; Crudelli M. (2002) Domenico Scarlatti. Rome: Radio Vaticana MK 17112
00; Nicolson L. (2004) Scarlatti Sonatas. Cologne: Capriccio; Pogorelich I.
(1993). Scarlatti Sonatas. Hamburg: DGG B100001GGV; Tilney C. (1988). Scarlatti
Sonatas. Toronto: Dorian Recordings DOR 90103.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Case description
Procedure
RESULTS
Table 1. Mean ratings for eight attributes of RDs piano playing skill.
Figure 1. Coronal MRI T1 images obtained in 2007 (left) and 2008 (right).
smaller for the motor than process scales and suggests that the physical as-
pects of ADL performance were relatively preserved in RD.
Mean ratings for RDs piano playing for both recordings are summarized
in Table 1. While all ratings of piano playing attributes at Year 2 were lower
than at Year 1, none of the changes reached statistical significance.
Structural MRIs showed significant orbitofrontal and anterior temporal
atrophy (see Figure 1), which became worse over 12 months. The estimated
percentage total brain volume loss over one year was 3.17%.
DISCUSSION
are similar to case reports in the literature of patients with Alzheimers dis-
ease who have relative preservation of musical performance skills despite
impairment in other domains (Beatty et al. 1999).
Although physical abilities required for the performance of ADLs re-
mained relatively stable in RD over 12 months, the mental skills necessary for
the same ADLs had worsened. This finding suggests that motor skills were
reasonably preserved in RD and may have supported RDs ongoing capacity
to play familiar musical pieces on the piano.
This case study has some limitations. First, the rating scale for the as-
sessment of musical performance used emphasized the motor aspects of
playing a musical instrument (e.g. accuracy of the notes and rhythm). The
ability to convey the mood and nuances of musical pieces was not studied.
Development of a scale to measure these aspects of musical performance will
be of interest, particularly as deficits in emotion processing are a prominent
feature of patients with frontotemporal dementia. Second, while there was an
increase in atrophy over time, it is unclear where this may have occurred.
Analysis of atrophy in the motor regions of the brain, relative to the frontal
and temporal lobar areas, would also be of interest in the future.
Finally, RD was seen relatively early in the course of FTD. It is unclear
whether there is a resilience of musical skills overall in dementia or whether
different abilities decline at different rates with disease progression. Further
longitudinal assessments of musical performance skills as well as cognition
and activities of daily living should also be examined.
Acknowledgments
This research is supported in part by NHMRC Project Grant (#570106) and ARC Fed-
eration Fellowship (FF0776229). SH is supported by the Australian Postgraduate
Award. We thank Patient RD and his family.
Sharpley Hsieh, Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, PO Box 82, St Pauls, Syd-
ney, New South Wales 2031, Australia; Email: s.hsieh@powmri.edu.au
References
Baird A. and Samson S. (2009). Memory for music in Alzheimers disease: Unforgetta-
ble? Neuropsychology Review, 19, pp. 85-101.
Beatty W. W., Rogers C. L., Rogers R. L. et al. (1999). Piano playing in Alzheimers dis-
ease: Longitudinal study of a single case. Neurocase, 5, pp. 459-469.
306 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Crystal H. A., Grober E., and Masur D. (1989). Preservation of musical memory in Alz-
heimers disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 52, pp.
1415-1416.
Fisher A. G. (2003). Assessment of Motor and Process Skills (vol. 1). Fort Collins, Colo-
rado, USA: Three Stars Press.
Mioshi E., Dawson K., Mitchell J. et al. (2006). The Addenbrookes cognitive examina-
tion revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Inter-
national Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, pp. 1078-1085.
Neary D., Snowden J. S., Gustafson L. et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration:
A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 51, pp. 1546-1554.
Smith S. M., Zhang Y., Jenkinson M. et al. (2002). Accurate, robust and automated
longitudinal and cross-sectional brain change analysis. NeuroImage, 17, pp. 479-
489.
Smith S. M., Jenkinson M., Woolrich M. W. et al. (2004). Advances in functional and
structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23, pp.
208-219.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The aims of the present study were to develop a chinrest that could di-
rectly assess chinrest force during musical performance and to provide
baseline force data. A force transducer-mounted chinrest was designed
and built by the present authors. Data were obtained from 11 elite violin-
ists while they performed scale tasks at different dynamics (p, mf, and f),
tempi (1, 4, and 8 Hz), and hand positions (first and eighth). Data were
also obtained from playing the tasks with vibrato, and the excerpts were
from the Bruch and Dvorak concertos. A mechanical test of the chinrest
confirmed good linearity of the force up to 100 N. During the scale tasks,
chinrest force was around 14 N at p, which increased to 18 N at f. Neither
the playing tempo nor the hand position largely affected the force, but it
was significantly increased during vibrato. During playing the Bruch con-
certo, the force was elevated to 28 N (peak force=39 N; the peak-force
range in all players=26-76 N). Typical chinrest force to stabilize the violin
during ordinary musical performance is thus less than 20 N, but it can be
tripled or more when performing technically demanding musical tasks.
Yuriko Kubota
University of London, UK
The aim of this paper is to explore the traits and roles of professional ac-
companists. This study is based on an internet survey. Professional ac-
companists were asked to complete a questionnaire through an internet
website. The participants (n=96) identify themselves mainly as being
sensitive, thoughtful, logical, and optimistic. At the start of rehearsal, ac-
companists generally try to establish a collaborative tempo and timing
while seeing the big picture of the music. At the end of rehearsal, they
concentrate on mood/feeling, dynamics, and aesthetic goals much more
than at the start. Although some insist that accompanists should follow
the soloists expression, most accompanists enjoy and expect communi-
cative musical activity within music. Expert accompanists point out that
they have an important role in making music that is better than that cre-
ated by the soloist him/herself. It would seem that professional accompa-
nists should have an ability to perform with an overall structure of music
with confidence from the very start of rehearsal. Furthermore, it is likely
that particular techniques for accompaniment, including practical com-
munication skills (social, physical, and musical), with flexibility and ar-
tistic excellence, have considerable influence on their work.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Procedure
The questionnaire was uploaded onto a website, and the answers from the
participants were analyzed.
RESULTS
Personality
Interests
The biggest gaps, that is, differences and difficulties between their co-per-
former(s) and themselves, which the accompanists experience are: tempo
(35.8%), ritardando (30.5%), articulation (30.5%), and boundaries involving
places (27.4%) and treatment (27.4%). They also feel gaps of different expres-
sion caused by a different instrument (22.1%), accelerando (18.9%), dynamics
(18.9%), mood (17.9%), and tone quality (16.8%).
Pianists who have accompanied for more than 30 years, especially those
aged 41-50 (p<0.05), feel more gaps of dynamics than less experienced play-
ers (p<0.05). Furthermore, pianists who accompany instrumentalists feel
more gaps of ritardando than those who accompany singers (p<0.001).
Aims of rehearsal
Table1. Aims of rehearsal with level of importance (1-10, with 10=very important).
Ability
ability to have an immediate grasp of the overall structure of the music are
important. Furthermore, accompanists report that communication skills have
a significant influence on their work; these involve social, physical (keys of
synchronization/leading the co-performer), and musical (give and take)
skills.
Secondly, personality affects their work considerably. Most reported that
sensitivity and tactfulness are important for their effective work and flexibil-
ity is a necessity. Without accepting and leading the others expression and
opinions, collaborative expression will not work satisfactorily.
Finally, musical excellence (artistry) was mentioned by a number of par-
ticipants. Although some accompanists insist that they should follow the solo-
ists expression, the majority of accompanists would agree with expressions
like these:
Make your partner comfortable, while not losing your own musical integ-
rity (participant 91).
DISCUSSION
Accompaniment is the most rewarding and fun thing one can do as a mu-
sician! (participant 48).
Acknowledgments
References
Adler K. (1965). The Art of Accompanying and Coaching. New York: Da Capo Press.
Davidson J. W. & King E. C. (2004). Strategies for ensemble practice. In A. Williamon
(ed.), Musical Excellence (pp.105-122). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kemp A. E. (1981). Personality differences between players of string, woodwind, brass
and keyboard instruments, and singers. Council for Research in Music Education
Bulletin, 66/67, pp. 33-38.
Kemp A. E. (1996). The Musical Temperament. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kubota Y. (2008). A pianists expression in the role of co-performer: Changes in timing
and dynamics through communicative interaction with a violinist. In K. Miyazaki et
al. (eds.), Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference on Music Perception
and Cognition (pp. 140-149), Sapporo, Japan: ICMPC.
Moore G. (1943). The Unashamed Accompanist. New York: Macmillan.
Shaffer L. H. (1984). Timing in solo and duet piano performances. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 36, pp. 577-595.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
A cellist memorized the Prelude from J. S. Bachs Suite No. 6 for solo
cello and identified performance cues (PCs) that she attended to in per-
formance. During the next three years, she recalled the piece twice,
playing and writing out the score from memory on both occasions, in
counterbalanced order. Played recall was better than written recall.
Written recall was better at expressive and structural PCs, suggesting
that these cues provided content accessible access to declarative memory.
Written recall was worse at PCs for basic technique but less so when
written recall followed played recall. In written recall, serial cuing was
impaired by the absence of sensorimotor cues, particularly at basic PCs.
These directed the cellists attention to her actions. Reinstating sensori-
motor memory by playing through the piece reduced the impairment
caused by the absence of these actions during written recall.
musician to recall any passage in the piece by simply thinking of it (e.g. the G
section). Content addressable access is provided by performance cues (PCs)
representing landmarks in the music that the performer is able to think about
consciously during performance. PCs provide a mental map of the music that
allows the performer to monitor the performance as it unfolds and to recover
from mistakes and memory lapses.
Written recall of the score has proved an important source of evidence
that PCs provide content addressable access to memory (Chaffin and Logan
2006, Chaffin et al. 2009b). Recall is better at PCs representing musical ex-
pression and structure and declines in the bars that follow (an effect of serial
position). This pattern suggests that musicians have content addressable ac-
cess to memory at these points and then retrieve the following bars by serial
cuing. In contrast, recall is poorer at PCs representing decisions about basic
technique. One possible explanation is that musicians attend more to details
of technique at these points and so pay less attention to the notes. We tested
this explanation by comparing written and played recall of a well-prepared
piece. We have reported elsewhere that the written recall was better at
structural and expressive PCs and poorer at basic PCs (Chaffin et al. in press).
Here we ask whether the same effects occurred when the musician played the
piece at around the same time.
METHOD
Participants
Tnia Lisboa, the cellist and first author was trained in classical cello and
piano in Brazil, England, and France and currently lives in London perform-
ing as a cello soloist.
Materials
The Prelude from J. S. Bachs Suite No. 6 for solo cello explores both the
mellow quality and virtuoso aspects of the instrument. The cellist had never
learned the Suite No. 6 for performance before, although she was very famil-
iar with it and had played other works by Bach throughout her career. Writ-
ten for an instrument with five strings, Suite No. 6 presents contemporary
cellists with substantial technical challenges, as fingerings and left-hand po-
sitions must be adapted to play the notes written for the fifth string on the
four strings of a modern cello. Musically, however, the Prelude is comparable
to the other five Bach cello suites. Notated in 104 bars in 12/8 time, the piece
takes about five minutes to perform.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 317
Percent Recall
1
0.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
Written Played Played Written
Recall 1 Recall 2
Figure 1. Percentage correct on two tests of written and played recall, shown in tempo-
ral order from left to right.
Table 1. Regression coefficients for the effects of serial position of half-bars from PCs on
probability of correct recall for first/second written recalls.
Procedure
The cellist learned the Prelude for a series of eight public performances. She
then provided reports about the musical structure and PCs she attended to
during performance (expressive, interpretive, intonation, and basic tech-
nique) as part of another study (Chaffin et al. in press). Written and played
recall were each tested twice in counterbalanced order. The first test began
ten months after the last public performance with the cellist writing out the
score from memory. She then played it from memory seven weeks later, re-
cording her playing. Twenty months later, she began the second test by play-
ing the piece from memory again, and then wrote it out for a second time four
weeks later. She did not otherwise play or study the piece during this time.
Written and played recall were scored for accuracy.
318 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
100 100
75 75
% Correct Recall
% Correct Recall
50 50
25 25
0 0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Half-Bars From Section Bounds Half-Bars From Section Bounds
100
100
75
75
% Correct Recall
% Correct Recall
50
50
25 25
0 0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Half-Bars From Expressive Cues Half-Bars From Expressive Cues
100
100
75
75
% Correct Recall
% Correct Recall
50 50
25 25
0 0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Half-Bars From Basic Cues Half-Bars From Basic Cues
Figure 2. Mean probability of correct recall [written (black) and played (grey)] as a
function of serial position of half-bars numbered sequentially from beginnings of sub-
sections (structural PCs), expressive PCs, and basic PCs for first/second recall tests.
RESULTS
Recall was almost perfect for the two played tests and substantially lower for
the two written tests (Figure 1). The difference demonstrates the large role
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 319
played by motor and auditory memory. Written recall was better (71%) in the
second round of testing than in the first (56%).
Table 1 summarizes the results of a mixed hierarchical regression analysis
testing the effects of serial order and their interaction with the first and sec-
ond set of tests. For beginnings of sub-sections and expressive PCs, recall
declined as distance increased (see Figure 2, top and middle panels, respec-
tively). For basic PCs, the effect was in the opposite directionprobability of
recall was lowest at basic PCs and increased with distanceand was larger on
the second test (see Figure 2, bottom panels).
DISCUSSION
The cellists ability to play almost without error after more than two years of
not playing or thinking about the music is notable. The cellist described the
experience in an email to the second author shortly afterward:
It is awful to play without having practiced the piece for so long because,
besides memory, the hands feel soggy and I have no technical control of
anything even when I remembered it. Iwas hesitating all the way
through but managed to get to the end. At some placesmy fingers
seemed to go by themselves. Mostly, it was thinking of bowing and fin-
gering (basic PCs) thatgot me through.
At the end of this account, she points to the importance of the sensorimotor
cues created by her playing, which provided effective serial cuing of her ac-
tions, even in the absence of any declarative memory for what came next.
Sensorimotor cues were important to both played and written recall. The
reduction in sensorimotor cues in written recall explains why (1) written was
worse than played recall, (2) written recall was worse on first test than on the
second, and (3) written recall was worse at basic PCs. First, written recall was
worse than played recall because it provided fewer sensorimotor cues for
what came next. Second, written recall was better on the second test than the
first, despite the passage of two years, because the cellist had recently re-
freshed her sensorimotor memory by playing the piece in the played recall
test four weeks earlier. Third, written recall was worse at basic PCs because
they directed the cellists attention toward her actions and away from the
music when learning the piece, so that in recalling it, she relied more heavily
on cuing by the sensorimotor context. In written recall, the absence of the
sensorimotor context provided by her playing had a bigger impact at basic
PCs because she relied more heavily on these contextual cues at these points.
320 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Thus, basic PCs operated as part of the serial chain of associations that re-
minded the musician of what came next (Chaffin et al. in press).
Structural and expressive PCs, in contrast, provided content addressable
access to the cellists declarative memory, allowing her to recall a passage
simply by thinking of it. Direct access to these landmarks in memory pro-
duced better recall by allowing the cellist to recover and to begin writing
again after gaps in her memory where she was unable to recall anything. Once
begun, the memory of each passage cued recall of what followed until, at
some point, a link failed and the chain was broken, resulting in a poorer recall
as distance from the landmark increased (Roediger and Crowder 1976).
The results for the second written recall replicated the previously reported
finding that her recall was better at expressive PCs than at the beginnings of
subsections (Chaffin et al. in press). The difference supports the idea that ex-
pressive PCs marked the highest level in the cellists hierarchical organization
of the music into harmonic sections (marked by expressive PCs) and melodic
subsections. By contrasting played and written recall, this study has increased
our understanding of how basic PCs differ from other kinds of PCs.
Tnia Lisboa, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort
Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: tlisboa@rcm.ac.uk
References
Chaffin R., Ginsborg J., and Dixon J. (2009b). Serial position effects in a singers long
term recall. Paper presented at Society for Music Perception and Cognition Confer-
ence (SMPC), Indianapolis, Indiana, USA.
Chaffin R, Imreh G., and Crawford M. (2002). Practicing Perfection. Mahwah, New
Jersey, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chaffin R., Lisboa T., Logan T., and Begosh K.T. (in press). Preparing for memorized
cello performance: The role of performance cues. Psychology of Music.
Chaffin R. and Logan T. (2006). Practicing perfection: How concert soloists prepare for
performance. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2, pp. 113-130.
Chaffin R., Logan T. R., and Begosh K. T. (2009a). Performing from memory. In S.
Hallam, I. Cross, and M. Thaut (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (pp.
352-363). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lehmann A. C. and Ericsson K.A. (1998). Preparation of a public piano performance:
The relation between practice and performance. Musicae Scientiae, 2, pp. 69-94.
Roediger H. L. and Crowder R. C. (1976). A serial position effect in recall of United
States presidents. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8, pp. 275-278.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Table 1. Assumptions of the different theories about learning and teaching (adapted
from Bautista et al. in press).
investigating the role of the conceptions that students have about teaching
and learning music, as an essential component of their learning practices.
Research on the conceptions that students have on how to learn is still a
nascent area, but remarkable importance has been obtained in the last two
decades thanks to the research on metacognition, theory of mind, strategies
and styles of learning, and conceptual change from the implicit theories in
various domains (Prez Echeverra et al. 2006). The studies carried out at the
Autonomous University of Madrid concerning these conceptions have shown
that achieving a constructivist approach requires a real conceptual change, a
new mentality in teachers and students. This means that the implicit direct
theory of learning (see Table 1), close to behaviorism, or interpretive, which
assumes a cognitive activity by the student but subordinate to the achieve-
ment of results or learning product previously defined, should be changed to
promote constructivist ideas, in which the students mental activity is not
only the engine but the goal of learning (Pozo et al., 2006).
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 323
METHOD
Participants
This study was carried out with 12 cello students aged 8-12 years (5 male, 7
female) who were in four different courses of basic level (BL) in Spanish con-
servatories. While we were carrying out this study, all participants developed
their activity in various official conservatories from the regions of Madrid and
Valencia.
Materials
Procedure
The four teachers of the 12 students were first asked about the students
availability, and the parents consented to record the interviews. After this, the
children were interviewed in their conservatory or at their homes, and the
interviews were recording with a video camera.
In order to describe qualitatively these students conceptions about cello
teachers, tasks of choice and rank provided data that enabled descriptive sta-
tistical analysis (taking as independent variables the developmental and edu-
cational level). The relationship between their responses and the theory
maintained was analyzed to check the consistency of these implicit theories.
RESULTS
While we should take the results with caution, because of the small sample
size, they indicate that the children in the 1st basic level (BL) chose the con-
structive teacher as the best, while children in the 2nd and 3rd BL preferred the
324 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Table 2. Selection of the best and the worst videos of ways of teaching for students in
each basic level (BL).
Asking the students to play it, to play it, and, and, and, if they make mis-
takes, ordering them to repeat it until things work out more or
lessdemanding from them, because the mistakes should be mentioned,
and if they make mistakes again, they should be asked to repeat again,
and sometimes, if necessary, he may play with students.
When talking about why they felt the constructive theory was best, they
said that a teacher using this method should involve the student in his own
learning process in an active and reflective way.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 325
Because the teacher let the girl think, which was a good idea, and, and
the girl will do so at homebecause she helped the girl think of what
happened and, and analyze what she can do to correct herself. In fact,
you learn a lot in this wayin order that she can realize her errors, and it
was the method the teacher used.
Finally we asked about the worst theory, most students immediately cited
the direct method. They felt this method could obtain results but that the
student would not necessarily understand the process to achieve them.
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful for the assistance from the project SEJ2006-15639-C02-01,
whose principal researcher is M del Puy Prez. We also express our gratitude to Jos A.
Torrado, Amalia Casas, and Cristina Marn who have collaborated in the development of
this research, giving very helpful suggestions and insightful comments.
References
In the last decades there has been an increasing research interest in the de-
velopment of expertise in musicians through the ways they learn and practice
music. The musical score, a main element in most of the Western music
learning processes taking place in conservatories, constitutes a semiotic sys-
tem such as texts, graphs, maps, and numbers. These systems are powerful
tools through which we develop and accumulate knowledge. Drawing on evi-
dence from different fields, researchers have described different levels of
comprehension relating to some representational systems (see Bautista and
Prez Echeverra 2008, Friel et al. 2001, Kintsch and van Dijk 1978). Studies
of the development of interpretation suggest that some musicians plan inter-
pretation at the outset, primarily letting the expressive ideas guide the techni-
cal work (Chaffin et al. 2003), while others develop a performance plan after
mastering most of the technical challenges (Nielsen 2001). Although practic-
ing notes and difficult passages are important aims for students at all levels,
the experts are also interested in expressing a musical idea of the piece from
the beginning of learning it, as professional musicians state they do (Chaffin
328 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
and Imreh 2001). Novice students are mainly focused on reading and per-
forming the explicit symbols presented on the score (Gruson 1988, Hallam
1994).
The aim of this research was to explore how Spanish flute students at two
different levels of expertise used the musical score during their normal
learning practices outside the lessons and the nature of the activities they
undertook.
METHOD
Participants
Sixteen flute students from five different conservatories in Madrid (four in-
termediate and one tertiary) took part voluntarily in the study. They consti-
tuted two groups of different levels, each composed by eight students (five
females and three males). The Intermediate (I) group (mean age=21.1
years), was in its tenth year in the Spanish specialized musical education sys-
tem, which is the last year before starting the music performance degree. The
Tertiary (T) group (mean age=24.4 years), was composed of flutists in the
last two years of the music performance degree.
Materials
An individual interview was carried out with each participant. They were
asked to describe the usual study process that they follow when learning a
new piece of music. The interview was designed to explore the activities that
the students do when practicing a new piece, as well as the elements of the
score in which they focus their attention throughout their practice. The proc-
ess was divided into three stages, corresponding to the beginning, the middle,
and the end of the process. We suggested to the participants that they imag-
ined they were preparing the piece to perform in a concert. In order to give
them a context to explain the process, we used the piece An Evening in the
Village by Bla Bartk (Sz. 39), in an arrangement for flute and piano.
Procedure
Category Definition
System A: Practicing
Reading Mentioning any kind of entire reading of the score.
Passages Mentioning any group of bars that seem difficult for the students, to
practice them separately.
Recording Mentioning any kind of activity using a recording of the piece, but
not when referring to record himself/herself.
Pianist Mentioning any activity to be done with the piano accompanist.
Speed Mentioning any activity that involves modifying speed of the score.
Memory Mentioning any process of memorizing the piece.
RESULTS
In the beginning stages (see Figure 1), the I group reported being mainly
focused on reading activities, listening to recordings, and working on specific
passages. References to musical elements included focus on the notes,
rhythms, and indications about tempi. Students from the T group referred
to reading processes and to selecting and working on difficult passages. In
contrast to the I group, they referred to activities related to syntax such as
330 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Reading
6
Passages
Recording
5
Pianist
Speed
4 Memory
Notational Level
Dynamics
3
Syntactic Level
Expressivity
2 Style/Composer
Skills
1
0
1 2
Reading
6
Passages
Recording
5 Pianist
Speed
4 Memory
Notational Level
Dynamics
3
Syntactic Level
Expressivity
2
Style/Composer
Skills
1
0
1 2
Reading
6
Passages
Recording
5 Pianist
Speed
4 Memory
Notational Level
Dynamics
3
Syntactic Level
Expressivity
2
Style/Composer
Skills
1
0
1 2
identifying the structure of the piece, main themes, etc. These differences
were statistically significant (X2=6.35, p=0.012). In the middle stages, most
students in the I group (88%) mentioned expressivity, and there was an
increasing focus on dynamics. The T group demonstrated a less marked
change from the beginning stage than the I group. Reading activities de-
creased with a greater emphasis on working at different speeds, developing
skills, and focusing on specific passages and dynamics (see Figure 2). In the
end stage, the I students continued to focus on expressivity and dynamics.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 331
The T group referred to more activities (15 statements) than musical ele-
ments at this stage (5 statements). They seemed to return to a consideration
of the piece as a whole with a reduction in the focus on specific elements from
the score (see Figure 3).
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the project SEJ2006-15639-C02-01, directed by Puy Prez
Echeverra. We thank the participants of this study and their teachers.
References
Rosemary Martin
This research is driven by the key question: within the context of professional
ballet, what are the attitudes surrounding female ballet dancers dancing in
pain? The research focuses on the attitudes toward pain from the perspectives
of dancers, ex-dancers, teachers, choreographers, and artistic directors, with
my own personal experience also informing the research.
The term pain is complex, with multiple interpretations and meanings.
Pain is defined in this research as physical pain caused by noxious stimulus
334 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
or bodily harm (Melzack and Wall 1965, Woolf 1995, Loeser and Melzack
1999). This definition focuses on the physiological pain experience, rather
than the psychological pain experience. Defining pain in this way is not to
ignore how pain is also intrinsically linked and influenced by various psycho-
logical, environmental, and cultural factors. This research aims to draw on
both a biomedical model of pain, and what could be described as a contextu-
alist approach (Loland 2006) of how pain is interpreted and shaped within
the social and cultural contexts in which people live.
The pain/ballet relationship can be seen emerging in academic scholar-
ship, scientific research, autobiographical accounts, and performances, with a
growing body of literature and research that investigates wider issues related
to dancers bodies, injuries, and health. Some notable quantitative research
has investigated dancers pain (e.g. Anderson and Hanrahan 2007, Encarna-
cion et al. 1999, Krasnow et al. 1994, Tajet-Foxell and Rose 1995). This re-
search, although predominantly through quantitative investigation, is
intended to provide an understanding and awareness that dancers are experi-
encing pain.
Significant qualitative readings of subjective scholarship has surrounded
the dancers body and aesthetics (e.g. Abra 1987-88, Foster 1996, 1997, Tho-
mas 2003, Wainwright and Turner 2003a, 2003b, 2004, 2006, Wainwright
et al. 2005). This qualitative research has illuminated attitudes surrounding
dancers dancing with pain, though none of the above authors focus specifi-
cally on dancers and their attitudes toward the pain experience, which pro-
vides this research with a justification to address such a potential gap in the
literature.
Krasnow et al. (1994) conducted research which demonstrated that most
dancers do not seek medical treatment for injuries, with fewer than 50% of all
dancers injuries being treated by medical practitioners. There are various
possible explanations for why dancers are hesitant to seek professional help
for injuries, ranging from financial constraints, pressures from teachers, ar-
tistic staff or directors, as well as themselves, to continue dancing, and feeling
that there is a lack of understanding from medical practitioners about their
situation (Krasnow et al. 1994). Consequently, injured dancers may often rely
on the advice from teachers and other dancers regarding how to treat and
deal with pain and injury (Krasnow et al. 1994). Research has also queried
whether dancers really can tell the difference between different types of pain
(Anderson and Hanrahan 2007). Anderson and Hanrahan have found that
dancers will endure performance pain or injury pain, and the type of pain
experienced does not influence the cognitive appraisal made or the pain cop-
ing strategies used to manage the pain.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 335
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
a well proportioned body, arched feet, and being slim yet strong, made the
technique of ballet somewhat easier to physically perform and more aestheti-
cally pleasing to the eye. Lisa, a dance teacher, explains this further: of
course if helps to have the right body, it is a visual art form, and I think an
audience wants to see a certain aesthetic. [Ballet] can be so much easier
when things like turn-out are naturally there, the right balance of strength
and flexibility. Foucaults (1977a) notion of normalization of the body,
through social practices and institutional structures and regimes, could be
applied to the experience dancers have in trying to normalize their body into
the ballet aesthetic.
From speaking to the interviewees it can been seen that pain is something
that is part of their daily lives, so much so that many consider pain to be nor-
mal; yet most dancers felt uncomfortable to disclose or discuss pain openly.
The lack of disclosure around the subject of pain was expressed by Kerry who
said, we dont discuss it, talking about it ruins the illusion that we work so
hard for; talking about it puts us at risk of jeopardizing our opportunities,
or as Amy explains, it goes without saying that ballet hurts in some way at
sometime, talking about it isnt going to change that; it will just make us look
weak.
The dancers, ex-dancers, and teachers and choreographers all spoke of
the pressures from the ballet culture that contributed to dancers postponing
the decision to seek treatment for pain or have time off dancing to let the in-
jury causing the pain to heal. These included practical constraints of time,
scheduling, and the need to produce a performance. Pressures felt by the
dancers came from the company management, teachers, repertoire, and
peers. This led to the feeling that their position in the company was not se-
cure and that they were easily replaceable; moreover, time off from dancing
was often seen as essentially wasting time, jeopardizing their career pro-
gression. Foucault (1977b) describes culture as a hierarchical organization of
values, accessible to everybody, but at the same time the occasion of a
mechanism of selection and exclusion (p. 173). The ballet culture and hierar-
chy of the ballet company, with its own system of beliefs and structures, can
shape how and when dancers decide to stop dancing with pain through the
surveillance and disciplinary power it holds as an institution (Foucault
1977a).
IMPLICATIONS
From the analysis of the interviews and the narratives collected, it is apparent
that dancers are dancing with pain. They are dancing for prolonged periods of
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 337
time with pain. They often do not know when to stop dancing with pain and
are fearful of the repercussions stopping dancing may have on their career
and identity. In a dance style that is evolving, extending and creating new
physical boundaries, while also trying to maintain and adhere to the structure
and form of the classical aesthetic, do these attitudes surrounding female
dancers dancing in pain need to change? It is one thing to acknowledge that
there are particular attitudes towards pain in the ballet environment; it is
another to demand that these attitudes must change. From this research it
can be concluded that if we would like dancers to have longer and healthier
careers with less physical pain, then yes, these attitudes will have to shift. The
question of how this shift might occur within the balletic culture, which holds
power over the dancers bodies and minds, imposing constraints, prohibitions
and obligations, is something that will take more than a quick fix solution
and will require much research and investigation.
This research has not attempted to uncover one particular truth or prove
a certain hypothesis; rather it has aimed to further open up a dialogue about
ballet and pain, and through the collected ethnographical interviews and
analysis, present multiple accounts and experiences. Ballet is not always easy;
achieving the physical form it requires, and the mindset that allows this form
to take place, may not always be pain free. Yet by beginning to understand
how and why dancers feel the need to excessively conceal and silence pain, to
dance through it, ignore it, even crave it, rather than listening to their bodies,
there is potential to offer alternatives and directions to allow them to experi-
ence less pain while dancing.
Rosemary Martin, Dance Studies, National Institute of Creative Arts and Industries,
University of Auckland, Building 421, Level 5, 26 Symonds Street, Auckland 1010, New
Zealand; Email: rmar161@aucklanduni.ac.nz
References
Abra J. (1987-88). The dancer as masochist. Dance Research Journal, 19, pp. 33-39.
Anderson R. and Hanrahan S. (2007). Dancing in pain: Pain appraisal and coping in
dancers. In R. Solomon and J. Solomon (eds.), Proceedings of the Seventeenth An-
nual Meeting of the International Association for Dance Medicine and Science (pp.
245-248). Canberra, Australia: International Association for Dance Medicine and
Science.
Castells M. (2004). The Power of Identity. Oxford: Blackwell.
338 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Encarnacion M., Meyers M., Ryan M., and Pease D. (1999). Pain coping styles of ballet
performers. Medicine, Science in Sport and Exercise, 31, pp. 217.
Foster S. (1996). The ballerinas phallic pointe. In S. Foster (ed.), Corporealities (pp. 1-
24). London: Routledge.
Foster S. (1997). Dancing bodies. In J.Desmond (ed.), Meaning in Motion (pp. 235-
259). Durham, North Carolina, USA: Duke University Press.
Foucault M. (1977a). Discipline and Punish. New York: Vintage.
Foucault M. (1977b). Nietzsche, genealogy and history. In D. Bouchard (ed.), Language,
Counter-memory, Practice (pp.139-164). Ithaca, New York, USA: Cornell Univer-
sity Press.
Grosz E. (1994). Volatile Bodies. Bloomington, Indiana, USA: Indiana University Press.
Krasnow D., Kerr G., and Mainwaring L. (1994). Psychology of dealing with the injured
dancer. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 9, pp. 7-9.
Loeser J. D. and Melzack R. (1999). Pain: An overview. Lancet, 353, pp. 1607-1609.
Loland S. (2006). Three approaches to the study of pain in sport. In S. Loland, B.
Skirstand, and I. Waddington (eds.), Pain and Injury in Sport (pp.49-62). New
York: Routledge.
Melzack R. and Wall P. D. (1965). Pain mechanisms: A new theory. Science, 150, pp.
971-979.
Parfit D. (1971). Personal identity. The Philosophical Review, 80, pp. 3-27.
Tajet-Foxell B. and Rose F. D. (1995). Pain and pain tolerance in professional ballet
dancers. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 29, pp. 31-34.
Thomas H. (2003). The Body, Dance and Cultural Theory. New York: Palgrave Mac-
Millan.
Wainwright S. and Turner B. (2003a). Reflections on embodiment and vulnerability.
Journal of Medical Ethics: Medical Humanities, 29, pp. 4-7.
Wainwright S. and Turner B. (2003b). Corps de ballet: The case of the injured ballet
dancer. Sociology of Health and Illness, 25, pp. 269-288.
Wainwright S. and Turner B. (2004). Epiphanies of embodiment: Injury, identity and
the balletic body. Qualitative Research, 4, pp. 311-337.
Wainwright S. and Turner B. (2006). Just crumbling to bits? An exploration of the
body, ageing, injury and career in classical ballet dancers. Sociology, 40, pp. 237-
255.
Wainwright S., Williams C., and Turner B. (2005). Fractured identities: Injury and the
balletic body. Health, 9, pp. 49-66.
Woolf C. J. (1995). Somatic pain: Pathogenesis and prevention. British Journal of An-
aesthesia, 75, pp. 169-176.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Music has played a central role in popular and elite culture throughout Euro-
pean history, spanning sacred and secular spaces and representing private
and communal experiences. Yet there was, until now, no museum of art and
design that connected music and its performance with other art forms and
objects in interactive and historically meaningful ways.
The Listening Gallery is a project in which new and existing recordings of
music have been integrated into two major exhibitions at the V&A:
342 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
The expertise in music, history, and art that the project has brought together
has given rise to new knowledge on performance. The examples of new re-
cordings provided below are presented under separate headings, either Ba-
roque 1620-1800 or Medieval and Renaissance Europe. The rooms in which
the music is heard, as well as the objects with which the music is associated,
are briefly described in order to place the music in context.
Baroque 1620-1800
The magnificence and splendour of Baroque, one of the most opulent styles of
the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries, was the subject of the V&As 2009
spring exhibition.
A recording of Davide Perez (1711-1778), Domine Deus, from the Messa con 5
strumenti, c.1750
The Sacred Spaces section of the exhibition was dedicated to religious devo-
tion and the ritual of Mass. Displays included Portuguese artefacts made in
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 343
Rome, for consumption in Portugal, during the reign of John V. The Model of
the Chapel of St John the Baptist in the Church of So Roque, Lisbon (Rome,
1742-44), by Giuseppe Palmes, after drawings by Luigi Vanvitelli (1700-73), is
a testimony to the characteristic of artefacts built in Portugal during this pe-
riod: the influence of Italian style and artists. This became the focal point
around which the music was chosen.
In 1743-44, the Portuguese king John V ordered a team of artists based in
Rome to build, decorate, and fit out a royal chapel for the Jesuit church of So
Roque in Lisbon. In 1751, John V called the Neapolitan composer Davide
Perez to Lisbon. Under his influence, Portuguese musicboth sacred and
secularbecame entirely dominated by the Italian operatic style, as is imme-
diately apparent upon hearing this recording.
The music associated with this splendid altarpiece highlights the stylistic
influences between Rome and Lisbon, felt as strongly in music as in art and
architecture, and the influence of Italian opera in Portuguese music post-
1750, including sacred music.
The manuscript of Perezs Mass is held in the RCM Library, MS 977. The
Domine Deus was recorded in the RCM Studios in February 2009, with the
orchestra and five soloists directed by Ashley Solomon. It is the only existing
recording of this piece.
As well as the music for great religious occasions and occasions of state, the
baroque period enjoyed performance of more intimate music for the
delectation of a smaller audience; lesser households, too, could command
music as integral to that richness of lifestyle.
The Secular Spaces section of the exhibition, in which the recording of
music for spinet and other instruments could be heard, focused on the most
intimate part of a household: the bedroom and closet. Beyond the public
rooms of a house, in which guests were welcomed, the bedchamber was open
only to the select few. Into the inner sanctum of the dressing room and closet
was packed a level of furnishing and luxury unseen outside. The exclusivity of
these rooms and their relative lack of ceremonial function also made them the
setting for influential innovations in furniture and interior decoration. Among
the objects that would have been found in these most intimate rooms is the
spinet, a small keyboard instrument of the harpsichord family.
The main instrument used in this recording was built in London in the
1680s by Stephen Keene, preserved in the RCM Museum of Instruments
344 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
(RCM 179). The instruments case is made of walnut wood, and there is a spe-
cial decorative laburnum wood veneer on the panel above the keyboard.
The music in this recording is taken from a single, late-seventeenth cen-
tury English keyboard manuscript belonging to the RCM Library, MS 2093.
Although its ownership is uncertain, the manuscript is typical of the virginal
books used to tutor players, especially women, at the time. It is remarkable
for two reasons. Firstly, its contents are divided into two collections, one of
preludes and the other of fugues. Secondly, it preserves pre-civil war music by
Byrd, Bull, and Weelkes, alongside later Baroque composers such as Locke
and Blow.
The recording of RCM MS 2093 was made in the RCM Museum in Febru-
ary 2009, with RCM students of harpsichord, violin, and recorder performing
under the direction of Terence Charlston. This is the first recording of the
manuscript, an important exemplar of virginal music of the period, as well as
a testimony of the intimate type of music that would have been heard in a
seventeenth century English home.
A missal is a book that contains the texts and music needed by a priest and
choir to celebrate Mass. This highly decorated missal was probably made
around 1350 in Paris for use at one of the altars at the royal abbey of St Denis.
Research into the service books and the liturgical life of the abbey (Robertson
1991) led to the identification, performance, and recording of the Dionisian
sequence Salve Pater Dyonisi, which would have been performed on the
Feast Day of St Denis.
Many recordings of Gregorian chant are available commercially, yet this is
the first recording to be made of Salve Pater Dyonisi, or indeed any part of
the St Denis Missal. There are seven verses that praise St Denis and his two
co-martyrs, St Rusticus and St Eleutherius. The words are sung in Latin to a
tune that was borrowed and adapted from pre-existing pieces to create a fresh
and new work. The recording was made in June 2009 at the Parish Church of
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 345
Our Lady of Mount Carmel and Simon Stock, London. Five male voices were
used, under the direction of Jennifer Smith.
Notation knives
IMPLICATIONS
The Listening Gallery has far reaching implications. The most obvious and
immediate benefit has been for visitors to the V&A. The music performed on
the four recordings discussed above originates from precisely the same years,
from the same cities, and is chosen from works commissioned by the same
patrons who commissioned artefacts on display in the exhibitions. The
recordings of Perez and RCM MS 2093 are contemporary with their
corresponding objects and have the same provenance; in the case of the
missal and the notation knives, the music is actually written on the objects.
Much of the music associated with the objects in the Galleries was re-
corded for the first time for this project. Much of the music can now be heard
via the V&As website, with annual online traffic of approximately 15 million
people. For further information on the pieces discussed above, see www.vam.
ac.uk and www.listeninggallery.rcm.ac.uk.
The project has demonstrated that music and musical performance were
central to the domestic and public life of those periods and how the
experience of music and art reunited reveals much about Europes geographi-
346 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
cal and cultural diversity. The objects on display, together with the worlds
they represent, have been experienced today as they would have been in the
past.
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank the following collaborators: Paul Banks, Clifford Bartlett, Terence
Charlston, Glyn Davies, Flora Dennis, Sebastian Durkin, Stuart Frost, Jakob Lindberg,
William Lyons, Peta Motture, Jenny Nex, Joanna Norman, Jennifer Smith, and Lance
Whitehead. The Listening Gallery is funded by the Arts and Humanities Research Coun-
cil (AH/F016840/1).
Giulia Nuti, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort
Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: gnuti@rcm.ac.uk
References
The aim of this study was to investigate the difference in the nature of
shaking (the longitudinal and lateral components) and press (the vertical
component) forces during the production of a vibrato tone by 10 novice
players as compared with 10 expert players of the violin. A violin in-
stalled with a 3-D force transducer was used for the measurement of the
force while performing a successive A (open) and D (vibrato and force
measurement) tone production task for 30 s at 4.5 Hz vibrato rate at mf
(75-77 dB). The index, middle, ring, and little fingers were used for the
measurement. The average, amplitude, and peak-to-peak time of shaking
and press forces were evaluated for each trial data. The results indicated
that the intra-individual variability of the amplitude of shaking force and
its peak-to-peak time for all fingers was significantly larger for the nov-
ices than the experts. The novices had smaller mean values of shaking
and press forces than the experts, and this group difference was larger for
the index and little fingers than for the other fingers. Novices have been
believed to use a grip too firm to shake the hand. The present data sug-
gest that it is the opposite.
It has been suggested that music may be more than just an art form, and that
it may indeed be able to produce benefits in other domains, such as cognitive
abilities or general intelligence. According to Schellenberg (2005, 2006a),
different lines of research have shown that both passive listening to music
and formal musical training reveal such a connection. However, the differ-
350 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
ences between these two activities make it unlikely that both could have
similar effects upon the non-musical aspects of human behavior (Schellen-
berg 2006a). This study focuses upon the hypothesis that music lessons could
provide benefits in non-musical areas of cognition.
Of the musical activities that appear to improve cognitive performance,
the most significant appear to be the playing of a musical instrument (par-
ticularly keyboard instruments), vocal training, musical learning in general,
and rhythmic training. The underlying transfer effect (Barnett and Ceci 2002)
may prove to be unique in children that have received music lessons for pro-
longed periods; as such, lessons involve activities such as:
task (abstract, numerical, verbal, mechanical, and spatial) was compared with
that of students who had not had music lessons. The general academic per-
formance of the students was also taken into account.
METHOD
Participants
The sample population (n=134) consisted of students from the seventh grade
of the basic music course (specialized musical education program), and oth-
ers undergoing conventional education. The average age was 13 years. The
students were divided into three groups, two experimental and one control:
(1) students receiving specialized musical education, who had had between 5-
7 years of musical training (n=63, SME 5-7), (2) students from specialized
musical education with 3-4 years of musical training (n=21, SME 3-4). The
control group (n=50) consisted of students undergoing conventional school-
ing (CS), which did not include music lessons. All students were from a
similar socioeconomic and cultural background.
Materials
The data used in this study was obtained using a Battery of Reasoning Tests,
BPR/7-9 (Almeida and Lemos 2006), designed to assess general capacity of
reasoning (g factor). These tests, which had been specially adapted to the
Portuguese population, included five instruments to evaluate performance in
different tasks of reasoning: abstract, numerical, verbal, mechanical, spatial.
Procedure
The participants were recruited from ten schools around Portugal by means
of an invitation addressed to their parents or guardians. The pupils were
tested individually using the BPR/7-9 at the beginning of the school year.
Statistical tests were then performed to gauge differences between groups.
RESULTS
Comparing the average g factor scores, it was found that the SME group with
5-7 years of musical education had the highest average, followed by the SME
group with 3-4 years of musical education (see Figure 1). Sheffs post hoc
tests showed that only the differences between the groups CS and SME 5-
7 were significant (mean difference=-2.12, SE=0.493, p=0.000). In short,
the students that had musical education displayed a higher general capacity
352 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
16
12
Average
8
4
0
CS without SME 3-4 yrs SME 5-7 yrs
Figure 1. Mean general capacity of reasoning for the CS, SME 3-4, and SME 5-7 groups.
of reasoning than the students from the control group, a difference which
became more pronounced with the duration of musical training.
Using MANOVA, the performances of the three groups was compared
with relation to each task. Scheff tests were used to assess the differences
between the various tests in the three groups. Although the students from the
SME 5-7 and SME 3-4 groups scored better than those from the CS group in
abstract and verbal reasoning and those from the SME 5-7 group scored bet-
ter than the CS group for mechanical reasoning, the differences were not sig-
nificant. However, in the spatial reasoning tasks, the difference between the
averages obtained by the CS group and the SME 5-7 and SME 3-4 groups was
significant (respectively, mean difference=-2.97, SE=0.710, p=0.000, and
mean difference=-2.44, SE=0.974, p=0.046). In the numerical reasoning
task, there was a significant difference between the CS group and the SME 5-7
group (mean difference=-3.15, SE=0.666, p=0.000).
DISCUSSION
The results of this study corroborate the hypothesis that there is a significant
relationship between musical education and cognitive growth. This associa-
tion between the general capacity of reasoning and the number of years in-
volvement with musical education is confirmed by literature dealing with the
relationship between musical education and cognitive ability (Schellenberg
2005). As in some of the studies cited above, this research showed that asso-
ciations were limited to specific tasks. However, when we considered educa-
tional performance as a whole, it was found that music lessons affected
general intellectual abilities. Students that had a musical education generally
obtained higher grades in academic achievement than their counterparts who
had not, and this difference seemed to increase with the number of years of
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 353
music lessons. We might infer that music lessons play an important role in
the results achieved, rather than extracurricular activities as this was tested
elsewhere (Santos-Luiz et al. 2009).
The music students in our sample had been tested for musical aptitude,
among other things, before embarking on their musical studies. Researchers
examining whether musical aptitude is associated to other cognitive capaci-
ties have noted that performance in this type of task tends to be positively
correlated with general intelligence (Lynn et al. 1989). Therefore, it is possi-
ble that increased musical aptitude through musical training is accompanied
by improved general cognition (Schellenberg 2006a).
The association of music with intelligence and academic performance may
result from a simple characteristic or from a cluster of abilities that are fos-
tered by musical education or musical stimulation. Musical structures are
abstractions that involve a range of transformations, and mental representa-
tions of music must be sufficiently abstract to allow recognition of similarities
between varying patterns. Formal musical training may promote the ability to
recognize regular patterns and think in a flexible way (Schellenberg 2006b).
These abilities are central to the g factor. This study therefore appears to cor-
roborate the assumption that there are associations between music lessons
and intellectual ability, although further research is required before this can
be asserted with confidence.
Acknowledgments
This work has been supported by the Fundao para a Cincia e Tecnologia (FCT) and
Programa Operacional Cincia e Inovao 2010 (POCI 2010), co-funded by the Portu-
guese Government and European Union by the FEDER Program.
References
Barbro B. J. (2006). Music and brain plasticity. European Review, 14. pp. 49-64.
Barnett S. M. and Ceci S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A
taxonomy for transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, pp. 612-637.
Bilhartz T. D., Bruhn R. A., and Olson J. E. (2000). The effect of early music training on
child cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20,
pp. 615-636.
354 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Butzlaff R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of Aesthetic Education,
34, pp. 167-178.
Ho Y.-C., Cheung M.-C., and Chan A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not
visual memory: Cross sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuro-
psychology, 17, pp. 439-450.
Huttenlocher P. R. (2002). Neural Plasticity. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Harvard
University Press.
Lamb S. J. and Gregory A. H. (1993). The relationship between music and reading in
beginning readers. Educational Psychology, 13, pp. 19-27.
La Mont K. A. (2008). Exploring the Foundations of the Relationship between Music
Training and Mathematical Cognition. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard Uni-
versity.
Lynn R., Wilson R. G., and Gault A. (1989). Simple musical tests as measures of Spear-
mans g. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, pp. 25-28.
Maureen H. (2008). The effects of music instruction on learning in the Montessori
classroom. Montessori Life, 20, pp. 24-31.
Orsmond G. I. and Miller L. K. (1999). Cognitive, musical and environmental correlates
of early music instruction. Psychology of Music, 27, pp. 18-37.
Rauscher F. H., LeMieux M., and Hinton S. C. (2006). Quality piano instruction affects
at-risk elementary school childrens cognitive abilities and self-esteem. Paper pre-
sented at the Ninth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition,
Bologna, Italy.
Santos-Luiz C., Coimbra D., and Silva C. F. (2009). Impacto do ensino especializado da
msica no desempenho acadmico. Paper presented at the IV Encuentro de Prima-
vera, University of Granada, Spain.
Schellenberg E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, pp.
511-514.
Schellenberg E. G. (2005). Music and cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psycho-
logical Science, 14, pp. 322-325.
Schellenberg E. G. (2006a). Exposure to music: The truth about the consequences. In G.
E. McPherson (ed.), The Child as Musician (pp. 111-134). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Schellenberg E. G. (2006b). Long-term positive associations between music lessons and
IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, pp. 457-468.
Vaughn K. (2000). Music and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed rela-
tionship. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, pp. 149-166.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Sofia Serra-Dawa1,2
Instrumental and singing teaching has previously been studied with par-
ticular focus on pedagogical, cognitive, technical, and developmental as-
pects. However, the relationship and interactions that take place between
the teachers and students in that setting deserve more extensive explora-
tion. This study approaches the singing teacher-student relationship with
a particular focus on the observation of personality traits and the levels of
attachment identified between teacher and student. Eleven singing
teachers with 54 students were observed in one-to-one singing lessons
through video observations made during one academic year. Addition-
ally, teachers and students completed the questionnaires NEO FFI-R and
Adult Attachment Scale to evaluate personality and attachment. The in-
teraction between teacher and student include a wide list of variables:
personality combination, the individual background, kind of attachment
felt toward each other, and many other singing aspects, suggesting that
the relationship with the student could have major impact on other as-
pects of singing.
Instrumental and singing teaching has previously been studied with particu-
lar focus on pedagogical, cognitive, technical, and developmental aspects.
However, the relationship and interactions that take place between the teach-
ers and students in that setting deserves extensive exploration.
Teacher-student interactions have powerful potential to improve and fa-
cilitate learning (Siebenaler 1997), with relationships playing a role in stu-
dents development. Burland and Davidson (2002) suggest that the teacher in
356 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
The participants in the current study were 11 singing teachers and 54 under-
graduate and postgraduate students from six colleges and universities in the
UK and Portugal. All students were studying singing as their main instru-
ment. From the same sample, 35 participants completed personality and
adult attachment scale measures.
Materials
The study had three stages where participants were video recorded during
one academic year. These recordings were complemented with existing psy-
chological questionnaires in order to access the personality types and the
levels of attachment in each dyad. For the personality identification, the NEO
FFI-R by Costa and McCrae (1985) was used, and adult attachment was
measured using the Adult Attachment Scale by Collins and Read (1990).
Procedure
Six higher education institutions (universities and colleges) from the UK and
Portugal were contacted through their heads of vocal studies. These two
countries were chosen to allow a wider range of comparison and a diverse
cultural environment. The head of vocal studies were asked to choose several
singing teachers who were seen as being different or contrasting in relational
terms, and these staff were approached for a longitudinal video collection of
their singing lessons. The teachers who agreed to participate were then asked
to choose at least two contrasting students that would follow these differ-
ences: (1) age, (2) academic year, (3) levels of development, (4) that the
teachers felt they related to in different ways, and/or (5) students they felt
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 357
RESULTS
Ongoing analysis illustrates that the interaction between teacher and student
includes a wide list of variances: personality combination, the individual
background, kind of attachment felt toward each other, and many other as-
pects suggesting that the relationship with the student could have major im-
pact on other aspects of singing. The longitudinal observation indicated that
the relationships develop into becoming more personal and often include
elements from the personal lives of both teacher and student. These elements
of students lives are often used toward interpretative development and be-
come interesting strategic tools for the singing teachers. The relational inter-
actions between teacher and student are in all cases conditioned by the
academic years schedule and there is a noticeable peak of relationship ap-
proximation during the middle stage of the year in relation to the beginning
of year (where participants are getting to know each other) and the last stage
(where the pressure of examination normally drives the lesson).
The observational part of the study presented fluctuations of behavior and
adaptability between teacher and student during the course of the one aca-
demic year. Particularly interesting to note is the teachers adaptability to
each student, making the relationship unique. The personality and attach-
ment types of each participant seem to play an important role in the individu-
ality of each relationship.
DISCUSSION
Sofia Serra-Dawa, School of the Arts, Portuguese Catholic University, Rua Diogo Bo-
telho 1327, Porto 4169-005, Portugal; Email: sofiaserra99@yahoo.com
358 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
References
Burland K. and Davidson J. W. (2002). Training the talented. Music Education Re-
search. 4, pp. 121-140.
Collins N. and Read S. (1990). Adult attachment relationships, working models and
relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 58, pp. 644-683.
Costa P. T. and MacCrae R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory Manual. Odessa,
Florida, USA: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Siebenaler D. (1997). Analysis of teacher-student interactions in the piano lessons of
adults and children. Journal of Research in Music Education, 45, pp. 6-20.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
nature and time course of vocal planning can be inferred by examining subtle
movements in these muscle groups that occur prior to sound production.
Such movements reflect a dynamic process in which action and decision-
making are tightly coupled (McKinstry et al. 2008).
Muscular activity associated with emotional communication overlaps with
muscular activity used for effective singing, but it is not identical. While a
performer is singing it may be difficult to disentangle movements that sup-
port accurate vocal production from those that support emotional communi-
cation. Similarly, movements that occur immediately prior to vocal
production combine plans for accurate vocal production with plans for emo-
tional communication.
Once vocal production has terminated, however, the bodily movements
and facial expressions that persist cannot be interpreted as a reflection of
vocal production mechanisms. Emotional facial expressions linger beyond the
time it takes to deactivate muscular activity, often lasting several seconds or
more. Such activity can only be interpreted as a constituent of emotional
communication and is referred to as emotional lingering.
Emotional lingering can be investigated in both production and percep-
tion studies. First, facial expressions and other movements that occur fol-
lowing the cessation of vocal production can be captured and analyzed using
technologies such as motion capture and electromyography. These strategies
can be used to confirm the existence of emotional lingering and determine its
properties and time course. Second, perceptual studies can be used to deter-
mine how perceivers are affected by emotional lingering phenomena. Do fa-
cial expressions that persist beyond the termination of sound provide reliable
emotional information, and do they influence emotional interpretations of the
music?
Livingstone et al. (2009) confirmed that singers maintain emotional fa-
cial expressions beyond the cessation of vocal production. Participants were
recorded with motion capture or electromyography (EMG) as they watched
and imitated phrases of emotional singing. All were shown audiovisual re-
cordings of sung phrases performed with happy, sad, or neutral emotional
expressions. Their task was to imitate the target stimulus, emphasizing the
emotion expressed. Facial expressions for happy and sad sung phrases were
significantly different from facial expressions for neutral sung phrases. How-
ever, these differences were observed not only during the imitation, but also
before and after vocal production. The authors concluded that facial expres-
sions not only support music performance during vocal production, but also
reflect processes of emotional planning and lingering.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 361
METHOD
Participants
Seventeen first year psychology students took part in the experiment in re-
turn for course credit. All had normal or corrected to normal vision.
Materials
Procedure
Figure 1. Timeline of a trial involving point-light display. Each black dot represents a
marker used to track facial motion. Markers were placed on the forehead, temples,
nose-bridge, nose-tip, eyebrows (2 each), and lips (upper, lower, and corners).
domized. Participants completed six practice trials for each display type be-
fore completing each block. Illustrated in Figure 1, each trial began with 1 s of
black screen, followed by the 2.4 s stimulus and then another 1 s of black
screen. Participants then rated the emotion perceived on a scale from -3 (very
sad), to 0 (neutral), to +3 (very happy). A computer mouse was used to select
ratings displayed on the monitor. Once participants rated the stimulus they
could progress to the next trial when ready.
RESULTS
Figure 2. Mean emotion ratings for full video (left panel) and point-light display (right
panel), and standard error bars.
DISCUSSION
Results confirm that facial expressions that linger beyond the cessation of
vocal production contain reliable emotional cues. Indeed, when full video was
available, discrimination of emotional facial expressions was observed prior,
during, and after vocalization. Such facial expressions of emotion may func-
tion to shape a listeners interpretation of the music (Thompson et al. 2008).
When only point-light display was available, discrimination was observed
prior and during vocalization, but participants could not discriminate happy
expressions from other emotional intentions in the post-production epoch,
indicating that marker movement alone carried insufficient information for
decoding happy expressions in that epoch. It is possible that isolated happy
emotional expressions are ambiguous in the lingering epoch because they
begin with raised lip corners and end with a neutral expression, implying
364 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by an ARC Discovery Grant DP0987182 awarded to the
first and last authors. Alex Chilvers, Catherine Greentree, and George Narroway pro-
vided research assistance.
References
Juslin P. N. and Sloboda J. A. (2001). Music and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Livingstone S. R., Thompson W. F., and Russo F. A. (2009). Facial expressions and
emotional singing: A study of perception and production with motion capture and
electromyography. Music Perception, 26, pp. 475-488.
McKinstry C., Dale R., and Spivey M. (2008). Action dynamics reveal parallel competi-
tion in decision making. Psychological Science, 19, pp. 22-24.
Thompson W. F., Russo F. A., and Quinto L. (2008). Audio-visual integration of emo-
tional cues in song. Cognition & Emotion. 22, pp. 1457-1470.
Thompson W .F., Graham P., and Russo F. A. (2005). Seeing music performance: Visual
influences on perception and experience. Semiotica, 156, pp. 203-227.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
Fifteen female and three male pianists, aged between 17-57 years, partici-
pated. They all studied or worked at the National Music Conservatory in Ath-
ens and their treatmentsurgical or conservativewas also provided in
Athens. The diagnosis had been made on the basis of clinical presentation,
and on six patients, nerve conduction and electromyographic studies were
also performed. Twelve patients had undergone surgical decompression and
six had had non-surgical treatment with a combination of advice on modify-
ing their practice, physiotherapy, splints, local anti-inflammatory injections,
and oral anti-inflammatories.
Procedure
RESULTS
In the pre-treatment period, our results showed that the early diagnosis was
followed with relief of the symptoms without the need for surgery. Most de-
layed diagnoses were either due to fear of diagnosis or due to fear of potential
surgical treatment (see Table 1). We found a correlation between symptoma-
tology and technical difficulties in practice or occasions where low morale and
social or career pressures were prominent (see Table 2).
In the post-treatment period, we defined our results according to the out-
lined criteria (poor: no change/possible deterioration, good: improvement/
possible relapse, excellent: improvement/no relapse) for both conservatively
and surgically treated pianists (see Table 3) and found that although surgical
treatment had been of good initial relief in 50% of the patients, a further re-
education of practicing was essential in order to maintain the outcome.
In the evaluation of this result, it is difficult accurately to detect retro-
spectively the surgeon/physicians familiarity/awareness with the particular
technical issues that affect the pianists. In our questionnaire, we asked
whether the patient felt that the medical practitioner was able to discuss and
understand the pianistic demands and peculiarities of their technique, and
we based our results on their replies (see Table 4). It could be said that, the
less aware the surgeon, the more his/her inclination toward surgical treat-
ment.
DISCUSSION
will not recover with conservative means and surgery will become unavoid-
able.
We also found a correlation between symptomatology and technical diffi-
culties in practice or occasions where low morale and social or career pres-
sures were prominent. It has been discussed that misuse of the tendons
and/or excessive wrist flexion are contributing factors to the development of
carpal tunnel syndrome. It may also be the case that psychological matters
that give rise to tension in the upper limbs play some role in this or other
conditions that affect piano players. Further studies should be conducted to
determine the statistical significance of the psychological effects.
We found that although surgical treatment had been of good initial relief
in 50% of the patients, a further re-education of practicing was essential in
order to maintain the outcome. This is either because the pianist had not
modified their practice during the earlier stages of the condition or because
the relief from the acute symptoms following surgery gave the freedom and
reassurance to the pianist to practice without deliberating their technique. It
is therefore essential for any re-education to happen soon after the operation
and to be maintained during the rehabilitation period and beyond in order to
prevent reoccurrence.
It could be said that, the less aware the surgeon, the more his/her incli-
nation toward surgical treatment. This we cannot prove with this study, as
due to its retrospective nature it cannot reflect accurate assumptions. How-
ever, we do take the opportunity to highlight this issue and emphasize that as
the pianist needs to be aware of the early warnings of a possible carpal tunnel
syndrome, the surgeon should be also aware of the possible technical aspects
that can contribute to this condition and be in a position to discuss them with
his patient before applying treatments.
We conclude that it is of major importance to maintain the clinicians
awareness of the particular demands that the pianists have from their hands
all the way through the diagnostic and treatment process. The debate of sur-
gical versus conservative management still remains open, as there are cases
that do equally well with either type of care. Carpal tunnel syndrome should
be broadly made known to piano players, and the early clinical symptoms
should be highlighted and possibly taught to the pianists early in their studies
and careers. Whether the symptoms are due to wrong technical habits or
whether they are due to unavoidable pathologies, it is crucial to understand
that early diagnosis and management can relieve the pianist from the anxiety
that carpal tunnel syndrome may cause.
370 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
References
Gohl A. P., Clayton S. Z., Strickland K. et al. (2006). Median and ulnar neuropathies in
university pianists. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 21, pp.17-25.
Lederman R. J. (2004). Peripheral nerve disorders in instrumentalists. Annals of Neu-
rology, 26, pp. 640-646.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
After approval of the study by the local ethics committee and obtaining in-
formed consent, we included 28 professional musicians diagnosed with focal
dystonia at the outpatient clinic of the Hanover Institute of Music Physiology
and Musicians Medicine (index patients). Based on history and clinical fea-
tures, all were classified as having likely primary dystonia. Fourteen of these
index patients had a reported positive family history (FH+) of FTSD and were
matched to 14 patients with no known family history (FH-) for age, sex, in-
strument group, and type of dystonia (limb versus embouchure). In a first
telephone contact, all index patients were asked to report known cases of
dystonia in their families (family history interview). All available first- and
second-degree relatives with no known dystonia underwent a standardized
telephone screening interview using a modified version of the Beth Israel
Dystonia Screen containing additional questions screening for MD (BIDS,
adapted from Saunders-Pullman et al. 2005). Videotaped neurological ex-
aminations were performed at a home visit by the same examiner (AS) in all
28 index patients, in all 15 relatives with a known or reported form of FTSD,
and in all 11 relatives who screened positive for dystonia in the BIDS.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 377
Molecular analysis
Peripheral blood samples were collected from all probands, and DNA was
extracted and screened for the known three-nucleotide (GAG) deletion in the
DYT1 gene. To test for a possible involvement of the DYT7 locus, suitable
families were investigated for linkage using the following six DNA microsa-
tellite markers: D18S481, D18S54, D18S976, D18S452, D18S843, and
D18S1153.
RESULTS
Consensus diagnoses
The study procedure and main results are illustrated in Figure 1. MD was
established by onsite examination and videotape review in all 14 FH+ (defi-
nite: n=13, probable: n=1) and in all 14 FH- (definite: n=14) index patients. In
total, 97 (56 FH+, 41 FH-) first- and second-degree relatives of these index
patients were examined. Seventeen of the 56 FH+ relatives had a previously
known FTSD (MD: n=7, writers cramp (WC): n=10), two of these relatives
were deceased. Family history interview revealed another two deceased rela-
tives with MD, one FH+ and one FH-. Using the BIDS in 78 putatively unaf-
378 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study displaying study procedure and results. FH+=reported
positive family history of FTSD, FH=no known family history, pts.=patients,
MD=musicians dystonia, FTSD=focal task-specific dystonia, BIDS=Beth Israel Dysto-
nia Screen, def=definite, prob=probable, poss=possible, (+)=additional dystonias or
additional other movement disorders in some individuals definitely, probably, or possi-
bly present. (See full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
Molecular findings
The DYT1 GAG deletion was not present in any of the tested patients. Due to
the small number of affected family members, linkage to the DYT7 locus
could not be excluded in 10 of the 11 informative families. In one family, af-
fected offspring did not share a common DYT7 haplotype with their affected
father. Therefore, linkage to the DYT7 locus could be excluded in this family.
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the patients and family members for their participation in the study.
This work was supported by a grant from the Dystonia Medical Research Foundation,
the Bachmann Strauss Foundation, and the Volkswagen Foundation. CK is a recipient of
a Schilling Award from the Hermann and Lilly Schilling Foundation.
References
(for a review, see Lim et al. 2001). The latter has been reported, for instance,
in data of the Bereitschaftspotential (BP) (Deuschl et al. 1995) and the con-
tingent negative variation (CNV) (Lim et al. 2001). Furthermore, in a recent
study (Hummel et al. 2002), deficient inhibition of simple motor patterns
was demonstrated in six patients with FTSD using transcranial magnetic
stimulation (TMS) and EEG-alpha oscillatory activity. However, it remains
unknown whether FTSD also affects the inhibition (INH) of long-term over-
learned motor programs. Consequently, the aim of the present study was to
investigate, using a Go/NoGo paradigm, the neural correlates associated with
the activation (ACT) and inhibition (INH) of motor memory traces in pianists
with MD during a pianistic motor task under constraint timing conditions
with multichannel EEG. Our main hypothesis was that the functional cou-
pling during INH is impaired for pianists with MD compared with healthy
pianists.
METHOD
Participants
Nine healthy pianists (8 males, age range=26-47 years, mean=36.5 years) and
nine pianists with MD (8 males, age range=27-50 years, mean=35.3 years)
participated in this study. In all patients, the right hand was affected. All par-
ticipants were professional pianists (with accumulated practice time over
10,000 hours). Eight of the nine participants in each group were right-
handed, according to the Edinburgh Inventory (Oldfield 1971).
Procedure
Participants were seated at a digital piano (Wersi Digital Piano CT2). The
keyboard and the right hand of the participant were covered with a board to
prevent participants from visually tracking hand and finger movements. In a
modified Go/NoGo study, the task was to play upward C-major scales over
two octaves. Scales were played as sixteenth notes, and the tempo was stan-
dardized at 80 beats/min for a quarter note (one key stroke every 187.5 ms)
and paced by metronome-like auditory cues. Scales were played using the
conventional C-major fingering: 1,2,3,1,2,3,4,1,2,3,1,2,3,4,5 (the fingers 1-5
refer to thumb, index, middle, ring, and little finger, respectively). The speci-
fications of the Go/NoGo study were as follows: a first visual cue (S1) indi-
cated that participants should be prepared to start playing. The metronome
was started 2,750 ms after S1. Participants were instructed to play the first
note of any scale coinciding with the third metronome beat. A second visual
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 383
cue (S2) was presented 250 ms before the third metronome beat, indicating
that the participant should either execute (Go, green ellipse) or not execute
(NoGo, red ellipse) the motor sequence. Continuous EEG was recorded from
22 electrodes placed over the scalp according to the extended 10-20 system
referenced to linked mastoids. Data were sampled at 500 Hz; the upper cutoff
was 100 Hz, and the time constant was set to DC (DC amplifiers and software
by NeuroScan, Herndon, Virginia, USA). One bipolar EMG channel was re-
corded from surface electrodes positioned over the right flexor pollicis longus
muscle, located 6 cm apart. The bandpass filters for EMG were set to 5 Hz
(highpass) and 100 Hz (lowpass).
We performed the following analyses of the EEG signals: (1) standard
time averaging technique to analyze the slow shift of movement-related corti-
cal potentials (MRCPs), (2) wavelet based time-frequency representations
(TFR) to analyze (3) the spatiotemporal dynamics of the inter-electrode phase
coupling. All statistical tests were performed by means of nonparametric uni-
variate permutation tests and multivariate synchronized permutations (Good
2005).
RESULTS
EMG analysis
The amplitude mean value in the analysis interval and the EMG peak were
selected as EMG activity parameters. In NoGo trials, the selected time win-
dow was 0-350 ms to detect whether pianists initiated a movement around
250 ms in spite of the NoGo signal. Burst of EMG activity showed that par-
ticipants initiated movements of the thumb in some NoGo. Interestingly,
however, the first key of the MIDI piano was not actually pressed. This result
confirmed that inhibition in our paradigm demanded active suppression of
the motor program. The EMG peak was found significantly higher in pianists
with MD (mean=14 V, range=5.2-50 V) than in healthy ones (mean=5.5
V, range=3.1-16 V).
NoGo trials were associated with a robust increase in the degree of global
synchronization across the sensorimotor cortex in the theta (4-7 Hz) and
lower alpha band (7-8 Hz) with a time span of 200-400 ms, hence coinciding
with the latency when the participants were required to begin playing (Figure
2). This increase was significantly more enhanced between 230-330 ms for
healthy pianists than for pianists with MD (p=0.004, permutation test, uni-
variate test). This effect was due to a weaker phase synchronization in pian-
ists with MD between electrode Cz, representing the supplementary motor
areas (SMA), and left premotor and sensorimotor electrodes (FC3, C3, CP3).
This result confirmed our main hypothesis, that the functional coupling dur-
ing INH is impaired for pianists with MD compared with healthy pianists.
DISCUSSION
Figure 2. Phase synchronization analysis during INH. The time-frequency plots of the
inter-electrode phase synchronization index, averaged across electrodes over sensori-
motor and prefrontal areas, are presented for pianists with MD (A), for healthy pianists
(B), and for the between-group difference (C, A-B). A pointwise paired permutation test
between groups yielded significant differences (p=0.004) between 230 and 330 ms and
at 7-8 Hz, due to higher global synchronization in healthy pianists than in pianists with
MD in this time-frequency window. This region is indicated by the green contour. (See
full color version at www.performancescience.org.)
(Hummel et al. 2002). Here, we aimed at studying a task that is close to natu-
ralistic piano performance. Accordingly, we imposed high temporal con-
straints on the task, we used a large sample of patients suffering from MD,
and we had healthy musicians as controls.
In this setting, (1) the role of the inter-electrode functional coupling in the
sensorimotor integration of inhibitory processes turned out to be the most
relevant physiological marker. Our study further showed that in pianists with
MD, (2) the non-retrieval of the motor program was associated with a weaker
positive shift after-S2 over cortical sensorimotor areas. Finally, (3) the EMG
peak in NoGo trials was found to be significantly higher in pianists with MD
than in healthy pianists. Our findings, thus, offer evidence that patients with
MD, as compared with healthy pianists, have a significantly higher innerva-
tion input of the flexor pollicis longus during NoGo trials. This outcome sup-
ports the main hypothesis of deficient inhibition in pianists with MD.
The SMA is thought to play an important role in the functional control of
movement in that it has direct projections to the primary motor cortex and
the spinal chord (Matsuzaka et al. 1992). Recent data has proven the suppres-
386 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
sive influence of SMA on the primary motor cortex (M1) in motor imagery,
thus reflecting the inhibitory function of the forward connection between the
SMA and M1 (Kasess et al. 2008). Hence, our results could be interpreted as
a deficient higher order motor functioning in pianists with MD: the phase
coupling between the SMA and the left premotor and sensorimotor cortex,
which is required for the non-retrieval of the motor program, is weaker in
pianists with MD. Consequently, these data can be regarded as an electro-
physiological correlate of the impaired inhibition in pianists with MD.
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by the Center of Systems Neuroscience, Hanover, and a Marie
Curie Early Stage Training Contract (MEST-CT-2005-021014) to Mara Herrojo Ruiz.
References
Focal hand dystonia has been associated with morphometric changes and
distorted somatotopic representations in the putamen. Our objective was
(1) to test for morphometric alterations of the putamen in pianists with
musicians dystonia (MD) relative to healthy pianists without dystonia
and (2) to identify structural changes in the basal ganglia that correlate
with performance during piano playing. Eleven pianists with MD and 12
healthy pianists without dystonia underwent high-resolution T1-
weighted MRI of the whole brain. Additionally, motor performance was
investigated in a music-related task according to a protocol previously
described as a valid and reliable method to assess motor control in pian-
ists. When playing major scales on the piano, the timing of key strokes
was more variable in patients with MD than in pianists without dystonia.
Healthy musicians had a smaller grey matter volume in the right middle
putamen compared with MD patients. In dystonic and non-dystonic pi-
anists, the middle part of the left and right putamen was smaller in indi-
viduals with higher temporal accuracy during piano playing. A smaller
associative territory of the motor putamen is a structural marker for
manual skillfulness in highly trained pianists. Since this structure-func-
tion relationship is preserved in MD, we argue that the relative increase
in grey matter volume in this region reflects impaired performance in
388 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
(Nitsche et al. 2003, Antal et al. 2004). It may be hypothesized that tDCS
facilitates retraining effects in musicians with dystonia. In this paper, we pre-
sent outcome data in 33 pianists with focal dystonia after an average follow-
up period of 24 months. Pianists were treated using BT, Trhx, or PR, either as
monotherapies or in combination. Treatment monitoring was made using an
established protocol that allows assessment of motor control in a relevant
musical task. Additionally, short-term development after tDCS-supported
retraining in a single patient is reported.
METHOD
Participants
Procedure
RESULTS
Figure 1. Study 1: each line indicates the mSD-IOI values of one patient before (left
endpoint) and after follow-up (right endpoint). A high score for mSD-IOI denotes a high
level of temporal unevenness in the scales (impaired motor control), while a lower score
for mSD-IOI denotes a lower level of unevenness (less or non-impaired motor control).
Solid black lines indicate significant improvement of motor control, solid grey lines
indicate no significant change in motor control, dashed grey lines indicate significant
deterioration in motor control. Gray window: range of motor control of healthy pianists.
DISCUSSION
References
Focal dystonia is usually painless and most commonly affects only one hand,
often involving involuntary flexion of just two or three specific fingers. The
incidence may be as high as 1% of all professional musicians (Jabusch and
Altenmuller 2006). In focal dystonia, the areas in the brain responsible for
396 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
the movement of adjacent fingers have become enlarged, due to overuse, and
can overlap (Elbert 1998). Treatment options have included administration
of Trihexyphenidyl or Botulinum Toxin-A, splinting, and limb immobiliza-
tion, but only exceptionally do musicians with focal dystonia return to normal
motor control (Altenmuller and Jabusch 2007). Different methods of re-
training alone without the above medical interventions have reported anec-
dotal success, but few have applied scientific method. We have previously
reported successful treatment of FD with intensive one-to-one pianistic re-
training without the use of Botulinum Toxin. The aim of the present research
was to see whether it is possible to improve the condition of a pianist suffer-
ing from FD through pianistic retraining using the medium of video confer-
encing.
METHOD
Participants
A 65 year old male professional pianist with a five year history of FD affecting
digits 3-5 (D3-5) of his right hand participated in a specific retraining pro-
gram based on a biomechanically sound way of playing with minimal tension
through the medium of video conferencing. Subject characteristics are shown
in Table 1.
Materials
The following equipment was used: video conferencing facilities, two fixed
and two mobile cameras, and two pianos.
Procedure
Characteristic Data
Age 65 years
Sex Male
Level of performance Professional soloist
Years of playing 60
Duration of dystonia Six years
Hand dominance Right
Hand affected Right
Digits affected D3-D5
whether the hand playing was dystonic or not. The above evaluation scales
have been previously described by the author (de Lisle et al. 2006). Statistical
analysis was carried out using Generalised Linear Models in SAS v9 for Win-
dows. Estimates are the change in SQE from the initial assessment to the end
of the three week 10 session end point or the end of one year endpoint. Seven
months following commencement of retraining the author had two face-to-
face retraining sessions with the subject in London, but otherwise all training
sessions were carried out via video conferencing.
The subject had been used to playing with a very curved hand position
and had trained using finger independence exercises involving antagonistic
muscle movements simultaneously (e.g. Pischner exercises). The excessively
rounded hand position had caused the subject to play at the edge of the white
keys with the thumb over the key surface, and D2-5 rarely played between the
black keys. An ascending scale passage revealed radial deviation and any up-
ward movement of the wrist caused D3-5 to curl excessively. Descending scale
passages involved shoulder abduction when crossing over the thumb, causing
the elbow to elevate away from the body and the hand and wrist to pronate
while pivoting on the thumb. Therefore, the initial aim was to lower the wrist
and to use a flatter hand position and play with fingers less flexed. In scale
passages, the subject was instructed to avoid lifting the wrist when passing
over the thumb and instead to ride forward on D3, moving the arm toward
the fall of the piano.
Particularly challenging was any tremolo movement using rotation, where
D5 became stuck to the keys and the wrist would flex excessively. The subject
had a lack of freedom in forearm rotation and tended to direct the movement
from the wrist. This was corrected by reducing wrist palmar flexion and in-
398 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
0.8 Dystomic
.
0.7 Non-dystonic
0.6
Change in SQE
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
Initial sessions 1 year
Time of sessions
creasing the rotation of the forearm (pronation and supination). Lifting the
fingers simultaneously from the metacarpophalangeal (MCP) joints was
problematic when pivoting on D2 because D5 tended to lag behind the other
fingers and this was given as a specific exercise. This lagging was also appar-
ent in descending scales but could be prevented if the movement of crossing
over the thumb was directed from D5 without flexing the wrist. In ascending
broken chord playing, passing from D5 to D1 caused excessive curling of D5,
but this was preventable at a slow tempo by actively extending D5 away from
the cramping movement. Passing from D2-D3 caused cramping of D5, but
this could be prevented by a downward convex movement of the wrist to align
the fingers with the keys. Chord playing using D1,3,5 was difficult, but D1,4,5
was accomplished with ease.
RESULTS
The analysis showed a significant improvement of the SQE from the pre-as-
sessment to the point in time of the three week, 10 session intensive retrain-
ing of 0.46 points [95% CI=(0.11,0.81), p=0.01], after controlling for
dystonic/ non-dystonic hand, this effect remained statistically significant. An
analysis of the effect by hand showed a small non-significant improvement in
the non-dystonic hand of 0.20 points [95% CI=(-0.13,053), p=0.23], while
the dystonic hand showed a statistically significant improvement of 0.68
points [95% CI=(0.20,1.17), p<0.01]. When the blind assessor was assessing
the playing, they correctly identified the non-dystonic hand 85% of the time
with the pre-assessment and 95% of the time at the end of the 10 sessions;
this was not statistically different (p=0.29). Whereas with the dystonic hand
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 399
the assessor was able to detect the dystonic hand 95% of the time at the pre-
assessment, this decreased significantly to 44% after training (p=0.0006).
When we compared the data from the pre-assessment to the end of year
assessment (see Figure 1), we again found an overall improvement, in this
case of 0.35 points [95% CI=(0.11,0.59), p=0.0044]. After controlling for the
effect of hand, this again remained significant. Similarly, analysis by hand
found a small but non-significant improvement in the non-dystonic hand of
0.15 points [95% CI=(-0.07,0.36), p=0.17] but a significant improvement in
the SQE for the dystonic hand of 0.53 points [95% CI=(0.24,0.82),
p=0.0004]. Again, assessment of the non-dystonic hand was successfully
assessed 81% of the time at the pre-assessment, and this improved slightly to
91% and the end of training (p=0.26). As before, the dystonic hand was accu-
rately detected 91% of the time at the pre-assessment, and this decreased
significantly to 69% at the end of training (p=0.03).
DISCUSSION
establishing new movement patterns the results are probably slower than
those possible in the same location. However, it could be an effective tool to
supplement and monitor progress after an initial period of onsite retraining.
Acknowledgments
The technical expertise of the IT departments at the University of Auckland and Royal
College of Music, London, is gratefully acknowledged.
Rae de Lisle, School of Music, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland,
New Zealand; Email: r.delisle@auckland.ac.nz
References
Altenmuller E. (2000). From Laetoli to Carnegie: Evolution of brain and hands as pre-
requisites of music performance in the light of music physiology and neurobiology.
Paper presented at the Tenth Symposium of the International Study Group of the
Archaeology of Music, Kloster Michaelstein, Germany.
Altenmller E. and Jabusch H. C. (2007). Focal dystonia in musicians: Recent results
and new developments. Paper presented at Medical Problems of Musicians and
Dancers, Aspen, Colorado, USA.
Altenmuller E. and Jabusch H. C. (2006). Focal dystonia in musicians: From phenome-
nology to therapy. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, pp. 207-220.
de Lisle R., Speedy D. B., Thompson J., and Maurice D. G. (2006). Effects of pianism
retraining on three pianists with focal dystonia. Medical Problems of Performing
Artists, 21, pp. 105-111.
Elbert T. (1998). Alteration of digital representations in somatosensory cortex in focal
hand dystonia. NeuroReport, 9, pp. 3571-3575.
Jabusch H. C., Zschucke D., Schmidt A. et al. (2005). Focal dystonia in musicians:
Treatment strategies and long-term outcome in 144 patients. Movement Disorders,
20, pp. 1623-1626.
Jabusch H. C. and Altenmller E. (2006). Focal dystonia in musicians: From phenome-
nology to therapy. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, pp. 207-220.
Schuele S., Jabusch H. C., Lederman R. J., and Altenmller E. (2005). Botulinum toxin
injections in the treatment of musicians dystonia. Neurology, 64, pp. 341-343.
Thematic session:
Understanding performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Daniel Bangert
School of English, Media, and Performing Arts, University of New South Wales,
Australia
This paper takes a process view of intuition based on dual process theo-
ries of cognition that distinguish between processes that are unconscious,
rapid, and automatic (System 1), and those that are conscious, slow, and de-
liberative (System 2) (see Hammond et al. 1987, Epstein 1991, 1994, 2003,
Hogarth 2001, Evans 2008). Many of the characteristics of System 1 can be
found in definitions of intuition:
METHOD
Procedure
Analytical framework
quired to make sense of the participants life world through a process of in-
terpretative activity (Smith et al. 1999, pp. 218-219).
RESULTS
DISCUSSION
learn, your instincts towards something can change. But for me, I use in-
stinct as the opposite of thoughtfulness, consciousness, so that you do
something without being conscious of why youre doing it in the moment.
Acknowledgments
I thank Alice Evans for participating in this study and am grateful for the assistance of
my joint PhD supervisors at UNSW, Dorottya Fabian and Emery Schubert.
Daniel Bangert, School of English, Media and Performing Arts, University of New South
Wales, Sydney, New South Wales 2052, Australia; Email: danielbangert@gmail.com
408 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
References
Betsch T. (2008). The nature of intuition and its neglect in research on judgement and
decision making. In H. Plessner, C. Betsch, and T. Betsch (eds.), Intuition in
Judgement and Decision Making (pp. 3-22). New York: Taylor and Francis.
Carlson N. R. (2004). Physiology of Behaviour (8e). New York: Pearson Education.
Chaffin R. and Crawford M. (2007). Unresolved dissonance? Subjectivity in music re-
search. In A. Williamon and D. Coimbra (eds.), Proceedings of ISPS 2007 (pp. 155-
160). Utrecht, The Netherlands: European Association of Conservatoires (AEC).
Dunsby J. (2002). Performers on performance. In J. Rink (ed.), Musical Performance
(pp. 225-236). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Epstein S. (1991). Cognitive-experiential self-theory: An integrative theory of personal-
ity. In R. Curtis (ed.), The Relational Self (pp. 111-137). New York: Guildford Press.
Epstein S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious.
American Psychologist, 49, pp. 709-724.
Epstein S. (2003). Cognitive-experiential self-theory of personality. In T. Millon and M.
J. Lerner (eds.), Handbook of Psychology (pp. 159-184). Hoboken, New Jersey,
USA: John Wiley and Sons.
Evans J. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cogni-
tion. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, pp. 255-278.
Hammond K. R., Hamm R. M., Grassia, J., and Pearson, T. (1987). Direct comparison of
the efficacy of intuitive and analytical cognition in expert judgment. IEEE Transac-
tions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 17, pp. 753-770.
Hodginkson G. P., Langan-Fox J., and Sadler-Smith E. (2008). Intuition: A fundamen-
tal bridging construct in the behavioural sciences. British Journal of Psychology,
99, pp. 1-27.
Hogarth R. (2001). Educating Intuition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Landridge D. (2007). Phenomenological Psychology. Harlow, UK: Pearson Education.
Simon H. A. (1987). Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion.
Academy of Management Executive, 1, pp. 57-63.
Smith J. A., Jarman M., and Osborn M. (1999). Doing interpretative phenomenological
analysis. In M. Murray and K. Chamberlain (eds.), Qualitative Health Psychology
(pp. 218-240). Thousand Oaks, California, USA: Sage Publications.
Sprenkle J. M. (2005). Defining Intuition: A Framework for Understanding Intuition
in Psychology. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Alliant International University.
Weber E. U. and Lindemann P. G. (2008). From intuition to analysis. In H. Plessner, C.
Betsch, and T. Betsch (eds.), Intuition in Judgement and Decision Making (pp. 191-
208). New York: Taylor and Francis.
Willig C. (2008). Introducing Qualitative Research in Psychology (2e). Maidenhead,
UK: Open University Press.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
This paper reflects principally on the perception of musical form and its
dependence on the temporality in front of the conceptual system, related
to the nominalizations and numerical groupings of the theoretical repre-
sentation of the structure. Through the assumption of practice as the only
way to experience music, this paper calls for sensations and emotional
intelligence with regard to musical appreciation, including form as one of
the most important elements of musical comprehension. The capacity of
the performers for evoking musical form, through its knowledge syn-
chronically to its temporal experience, is defended as a potential tool for
consolidating memory due to the security it provides in performance.
Huron (2006) explains, with a sweet anticipation that makes possible the
evocation/recovery (the last period of memory capacity after appropriation
and storage) of that which is to be played. It is absolutely feasible to play,
even by heart, ignoring the formal dimension of a piece, although it is not
possible to perform (Stravinsky 1942) a musical work without a solid as-
sumption of its form. Tovey reminds us that players should understand what
they play (Lester 1995), and formal analysis is a relevant tool for connecting
the multiple musical elements (values, frequencies, rhythm, melody, nuances,
tempo, agogic, harmony, motives, cadences, etc.) for this purpose.
Performing from memory can trigger fears or insecurity about final re-
sults in live performances. But at the same time, playing by heart awakens in
players a sensation of freedom with regard to the musical appropriation
which provides a very convenient state of introspection and individuality
(Delannoy 2008) with the support of the emotional intelligence (Goleman
1995). While in theory the formal analysis rests on the number (number of
sections, phrases, motives, measures, notes, etc.), the execution embraces
time as the perception of form. Music is a temporal artistic representation,
and this is the crucial point through which a performer may transcend what
he or she appreciates at a cognitive level to the experience of practice (Elliot
1995). Rousseau (1760) explained that only through our own movement we
acquire the idea of extension (p. 82). In a parallel way, it is possible to affirm
that only through the experience of time can we acquire the final idea of mu-
sical form. Intellect and perception (Renaud 1999) plus imagination are the
keys for consolidating musical memory (Snyder 2000, Barbacci 1987) in or-
der to attempt a rhetoric expression of structure.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
To know the limits of the musical form does not mean to understand it. The
border that divides the knowledge and the comprehension of the musical
structure can be as wide or as narrow as the capacity of the performer for
letting himself or herself be attracted by the temporal experience. For a theo-
retical analysis of the structure of a piece it is indispensable to develop logi-
cal-mathematical and spatial intelligences (Gardner 1993). Logical-
mathematical intelligence is required for synthesizing, through the number, a
pieces parts and limits, while spatial intelligence is required as a visual help
for speeding up the logical-mathematical intelligence in the preliminary stage
of a pieces progress. Perhaps both intelligences were considered by Gardner
as constituent parts of another of his defined intelligences: the musical intel-
ligence. However, for transcending the results of the theoretical recognition
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 411
The form is what defines, configures, or holds up an object due to the limits
that it imposes on the matter of which they are made. In music, once it is
transcended from the score/object to the sonorous world thanks to practice,
the only way to appreciate its form is hearing it attentively and letting the
time occur, trying to perceive when the sound and the multiple associations
that the composer establishes with it start and end. This is the perception of
the sound duration. Kivy (2001) affirms:
That musical form is spatial I can only take to be a claim based on the
conflation of musical form with its mode of representation. It is an un-
fortunate choice of words to have started calling sonata, variation, rondo,
and so forth formsat all, for the obvious reason that form is a word
more closely connected in our common usage with two-dimensional or
spatial images. But musical forms are not, except metaphorically (al-
though the metaphor is ubiquitous) musical shapes. They are temporal
patterns (p. 211).
standing the obtained data from the score. This capacity allows us to
corroborate with certainty the perceptive and formative unities, their
functions, hierarchies and the different levels of tension-rest (p. 311).
What is important in Tarchinis words for this study is the acceptance she
assumes of a temporal awareness in performance and its connection with the
musical form.
The number: The organizer concept of the musical form for theory
Although for the performers and the audience the musical form is essentially
perceived in time, in theory the form is conceptually measured in the num-
ber of figures and silences, measures, cells, semi-phrases, phrases, themes,
motives, sections, or movements registered through a graphic code in a par-
ticular space: the score. The resultant quantities of all the elements of the
musical form are, in theory, as static and tangible as the papers fragments
that contain them. Musical theory has named some of the most common mu-
sical forms throughout history. Sonata, lied, or fugue are some of these stan-
dardized forms. But, obviously, they do not represent the fingerprint of any
particular piece. When theory wants to recognize and describe a specific
piece, it uses the number to elaborate the exhaustive and individual structure
of its content. As an example, the formal fingerprint at the beginning of
Chopins Scherzo Op.20 is represented in Table 1. Evidently, the names motif,
exposition, section, introduction, or transition that appear in the analysis-
table are valid for many other masterpieces, usually in the same order, but
what is almost impossibleor would be the result of chanceis that the se-
quences that conform to the different named parts are the same in other
pieces regardless of the number and the association with the measures they
have.
Section Measures
Introduction 1-8
Section A 9-68
Exposition Theme A-1
Motif A-1 9-16
Presentation motif A-1 9-11
Transport motif A-1 11-13
Conclusion motif A-1 13-16
Motif A-1 transported 17-24 etc.
two important resources for avoiding the so feared lapse. Even when a slip
emerges, the musical mind tries to reconstruct the material with reference
to the specific details of the general elements. Form is essentially a general
element so, if performers remember it, the last opportunity to reconstruct
the musical material is in some way guaranteed. Memory survives partially
thanks to the growth of security; when a commanded formal evocation be-
gins, the consciousness of the musical content is more effective and the per-
formance evolves synchronically from a passive verification of audition
synchronically to a simple and physical execution to a controlled mental
complex. This is thanks to the re-creative intention, which in symbiosis with
the bodily experience provides a sensation of pleasure and security in prac-
tice.
IMPLICATIONS
Acknowledgments
I wish to acknowledge Juan Pineda (Spain) and Pamela Huebner (Armstrong School,
Hoffman Estates, Illinois, USA).
Elena Esteban Muoz, Royal Conservatoire of Music, C/Doctor Mata 2, Madrid 28012,
Spain; Email: elenaesteban@edu.ucm.es
References
The bulk of past studies examining music performance within the discipline
of psychology have utilized an experimental research paradigm centered on
the music listener. These studies have particularly focused on the relationship
between music and the listeners emotion experience (Gabrielsson and Juslin
1996, Huron 2006, Minsky 1982, Sloboda 1991, Waterman 1996). In com-
parison to the music listener, the perspective of the music performer has been
relatively underrepresented in this literature. The majority of studies have
used an experimental research paradigm to examine the importance of per-
416 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
before beginning the research, evolves during the process (Strauss and Cor-
bin 1998, p. 202).
The data on which this article is based are drawn from a semi-structured
interview carried out with Jeremy Kelshaw (JK), an experienced bassist cur-
rently playing in Cloud Control, a moderately successful self-described indie,
alt-folk, pop band. It was initially thought that JKs interview would serve as a
pilot study. However, the data emerging from this interview were rich and
substantial enough to allow the interview to be included as the first in a pro-
gram of research involving interviews with professional musicians. The un-
predictability of theoretical sampling and the incomplete nature of this
research make it important to stress the preliminary nature of findings re-
ported in this article.
The interview
In describing his ideal performance experience, JK not only spells out the
specific type of performance experience that he aims to create but also reveals
his understanding of excellence:
If youre not nervous and [the] crowd is already into itand if its a song
that you know backwardsyou just go into a little bit of a zone. In that
blissful moment its the same feeling you have when you really enjoy
anything I think. Your body knows what to do, and you just go into this
trance. You just enjoy yourself, and you dont have to think. Its that
feeling of success and a feeling of accomplishment and privilege at the
same time. Its just bliss.
Vibe
Although the word was originally introduced into the interview by the re-
searcher (AG), JK demonstrates a predilection toward using vibe in his de-
418 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
It is the four of us, first and foremost, trying to create a connection with
each other and then that being broadcast to the audience. In the same
way that you cant force a vibe with an audience, you cant force a vibe
with a song.
A fleeting uniqueness
A live performance is unique in that it happens once and then its gone.
[It] is unique to you and unique to the people that youre playing with.
Indefinable connection
Although some variables may be out of a performers control, for JK, the
amount of enjoyment derived from a performance and the amount of effort
put into a performance are not. Even if other factors are less than ideal, in-
tention seems conducive to establishing vibe.
IMPLICATIONS
appreciation for the amount of work and talent the intricacies of successful
music performance demand. This work also holds potential implications for
the ways in which music is learned, taught, and performed.
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank the following people, without whom this paper would not have been
possible: Jeremy Kelshaw, John Sutton, Wayne Christensen, and Anthony Arcuri.
References
Berger H. M. (1999). Metal, Rock and Jazz. London: University Press of New England.
Berliner P. F. (1994). Thinking in Jazz. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cskszentmihlyi M. (1990). Flow. New York: Harper & Row.
Dunsby J. (2002). Performers on performance. In J. Rink (ed.) Musical Performance
(pp. 225-236). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gabrielsson A. and Juslin P. N. (1996). Emotional expression in music performance:
Between the performers intention and the listeners experience. Psychology of Mu-
sic, 24, pp. 68-91.
Glaser B. G. and Strauss A. L. (1967). The Discovery of Grounded Theory. Hawthorne,
New York, USA: Aldine Publishing.
Huron D. (2006). Sweet Anticipation. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.
Minsky M. (1982). Music, mind, and meaning. In M. Clynes (ed.) Music, Mind, and
Brain (pp. 1-20). New York: Plenum Press.
Monson I. (1996) Saying Something. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Palmer C. (2005) Sequencing memory in music performance. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 14, pp. 247-250.
Sloboda J. A. (1991). Music structure and emotional response: Some empirical findings.
Psychology of Music, 19, pp. 110-120.
Strauss A. L. and Corbin J. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research. London: Sage.
Sudnow D. (1978). Ways of the Hand. London: Routledge.
Waterman M. (1996) Emotional responses to music: Implicit and explicit effects in
listeners and performers. Psychology of Music, 24, pp. 53-67.
Williamon A. (2002) Memorising music. In J. Rink (ed.). Musical Performance (pp.
113-126). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thematic session:
The perception of technique
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
During Pablo Casalss 97 years (1876-1973), the cello emerged from reclusion
to take its place alongside other popular solo instruments. Major additions to
the repertoire presented cellists with new challenges, and advances in re-
cording possibilities ensured that technical proficiency became the norm.
Although much has been written about string technique, there is no detailed
study of the expressive characteristics of shifting. Pleeth (1982) suggests that
the feel for distance in movement and the variety of tension in the left-hand
are essential in creating good shifts. He also links portamenti to singing and
states that it is the notes that give birth to the slide, not the other way
around (p. 37). However the study of such a technical, yet expressive, tool
can be problematic. Mantel (1975) suggests that technique cannot be sepa-
rated from the experience of making music. For practical purposes [however]
we can separate technique from interpretationa point of departure for the
analyses of this technique (p. XV). Although such separation may be possible
in order to refine technique, there seems to be little point in practicing tech-
424 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Five recordings by distinguished cellists (see Table 1) of the Finale from Don
Quixote by R. Strauss were selected. This excerpt was chosen for its extensive
potential for expressive portamenti.
Procedure
All recordings were analyzed and compared focusing on portamenti. For the
purposes of homogeneity, recordings were normalized and noise reduction
filters were applied. The participants were asked to evaluate an extract of the
Finale (see Figure 1), with the five recordings played in a random order, using
a scale of 1 to 7 and to provide the reasons for their ratings. Subsequently, the
participants were required to listen to the same extract a second time, rating
the use of portamenti for the following categories: (1) quality (QP), (2) appro-
priateness for the interpretation (HA), and (3) overall importance (OI). Semi-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 425
Figure 1. Cello part of the extract played from the Finale of Don Quixote by R. Strauss.
Table 1. Cellists, recording labels, and dates of the five recordings, as well as the esti-
mated onsets, offsets, and durations of the selected portamenti.
structured interviews were carried out with the cellists, probing their ap-
proaches to shifting, including practice strategies, decisions about choices of
portamenti, relationship with stage fright, and training received.
RESULTS
Figure 2. Listeners mean ratings (cellists and non-cellists) for PE (top left), HA (top
right), QP (bottom left), and OI (bottom right).
Figure 3. Graph of interactions (mean ratings for cellists and non-cellists) for PE.
DISCUSSION
This exploratory study shows that (1) approaches to portamenti are highly
individual and (2) this plays an important role in listeners preferences for
certain interpretations. The results of the evaluations highlight the prefer-
ences for contemporary styles with fewer expressive shifts as well as the
higher ratings by cellists when asked to focus upon specific technical and
musical aspects of shifting.
This preliminary study will be further developed by focusing on teaching.
Observations, interviews, and intervention studies will provide insights into
contemporary and artistic approaches involved in shifting that may have an
impact on how cellists prepare for performance. In these ways, the continua-
tion of the research will have implications for performing, teaching, and
learning, stimulating performers toward more creative and artistic ap-
proaches to the role of the left-hand in cello playing.
Acknowledgments
We wish to acknowledge the help and assistance of Sam Thompson, Aaron Williamon,
and Alexander Demos.
Tnia Lisboa, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort
Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: tlisboa@rcm.ac.uk
References
Drost U., Rieger M., Brass M. et al. (2005). Action-effect coupling in pianists. Psycho-
logical Research, 69, pp. 233-241.
Kay B. A. (2003). An early Oscillator model: Studies on the biodynamics of the piano
strike. Motor Control, 7, pp. 1-45.
Mantel G. (1975). Cello Technique. Bloomington, Indiana, USA: Indiana University
Press.
Pleeth W. (1982). Yehudi Menuhin Music Guides: Cello. New York: Schirmer Books.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The tremolo is one of the popular ways of playing the mandolin (Fujita 2005).
Though there is almost no agreement concerning the physical characteristics
of tremolo played by experts, the feeling of fluctuation on tremolo is thought
to be concerned with subjective evaluation for performance proficiency. The
fluctuation strength (FS) has been suggested as an index for evaluating
hearing sensation in fluctuating sounds at low frequencies, and past studies
have investigated that of modulated pure tones and broadband noise (Ter-
432 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
hardt 1968, Fastl 1982, Fastl and Zwicker 1990). However, few have reported
on the FS of fluctuating musical sounds produced by musical instruments and
proficiency. In addition, there are no suggestions for an index corresponding
to their proficiency.
Therefore, tremolo fluctuations are described by FS, and the relation be-
tween FS and tremolo proficiency was investigated through evaluation ex-
periments. The plucking rate, onset deviation, and amplitude deviation are
set on the basis of our assumption that they affect the hearing sensation and
subjective evaluation for tremolo more strongly than the other factors. Here,
both onset and amplitude deviation are called irregularity on tremolo. Aes-
thetic performances of tremolo are said to be smooth or not fluctuating by
trained players, so the amount of FS for tremolo is originally calculated by
extracting a fluctuation component of about 4-8 Hz from acoustic data. The
procedure for objectively calculating the amount of FS is designed based on
facts reported by past studies (Terhardt 1968, Fastl 1982, Fastl and Zwicker
1990) and an assumption that the characteristics of the tremolo are some-
what similar to the AM SIN at slow speeds (Roads 2001).
Therefore, we hypothesized that the fluctuation components extracted
originally from a tremolo influences the feeling of fluctuation (or an aesthetic
evaluation). Here, the calculated FS is called physical FS, while the FS pro-
posed by Fastl et al. is called psychological FS. We conducted four studies
using performance sounds and two kinds of synthesized sounds. In Study 1,
the region of plucking rates used in actual tremolo performance was investi-
gated. In Study 2, in order to describe tremolo fluctuation by FS, the relation
between feeling of fluctuation, and physical FS was investigated using synthe-
sized sounds that have just a feeling of fluctuation without tremolo profi-
ciency, played at several plucking rates (2-16 Hz). In Study 3, the relation
between physical FS and tremolo proficiency was investigated using perform-
ance sounds played at three plucking rates (6, 8, and 9 Hz). However, factors
that affect the feeling of fluctuation and subjective evaluation for tremolo
proficiency are not only plucking rate but also onset deviation and amplitude
deviation. The unified irregularity on tremolo is rarely found by simply ob-
serving actual performance. So, in Study 4, the relation between physical FS
and tremolo proficiency was investigated using other synthesized sounds by
controlling three parameters that have both tremolo proficiency and feeling of
fluctuation. Here, synthesized sound used in Study 2 is called imitated
sound, while the sound used in Study 4 is called simulated sound.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 433
METHOD
Participants
In Study 1, four mandolin players with over two years of experience were used
as listeners. In Study 2, two of the listeners were mandolin players, and three
of the listeners had no experience of playing the mandolin. In Study 3, seven
mandolin players (P1-P7) with over three years experience were used as man-
dolin players. Six other mandolin players with over a half year of experience
were used as listeners. In Study 4, five other mandolin players with over a
year of experience were used as listeners.
Materials
Procedure
RESULTS
The result of Study 1 is shown in Figure 1; it was found that simulated sounds
synthesized at plucking rate of 6-13 Hz were evaluated as tremolo sounds.
The result that compared FS with amplitude-modulated pure tone (Terhardt
1968, Fastl 1982, Fastl and Zwicker 1990), as well as comparing to physical
FS, is shown Figure 2. The magnitude of fluctuation for imitated sounds in
434 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
100
90
ess
caf 80
o ool 70
re m
b etr 60
m
un sa
50
of de
tioa ivce 40
r re 30
gea p
erv 20
A
10
0
1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19
Plucking rate [Hz]
0
2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16
Modulation frequency [Hz]
Figure 3. Relation between our results and psychological FS. (See full color version at
www.performancescience.org.)
DISCUSSION
As seen from Study 4, highly rated tremolos had a relatively low physical FS.
Furthermore, the physical FS has a strong negative correlation with a high
subjective evaluation (r=-0.60, n=27). Therefore, this shows that physical FS
can be used as an evaluation index of the tremolo performance proficiency
played on the mandolin.
The relation between results obtained in this study and psychological FS
is shown in Figure.3. The realizable region is obtained from the results of
Study 1. As can be seen in Figure 3, two strong relations were confirmed
among the calculated physical FS: the subjective evaluation results and the
psychological FS.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Prof. Hugo Fastl for discussing with us the application of fluctuation
strength to actual musical contents, and Profs. Yanagida and Yamada for their help in
436 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
giving suggestions and ideas for this study. We also express our sincere thanks to
OB/OG, as well as the players belonging to the Ryukoku University Mandolin Orchestra
and the listeners for their help. This study is partly supported by High-Tech Research
Center project for private universities: matching fund subsidy from the Japanese Min-
istry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT), 2006-11.
Nozomiko Yasui, Graduate School of Science and Technology, Ryukoku University, 1-5
Yokotani, Oe-cho, Seta, Otsu, Shiga 520-2194, Japan; Email: n-y@mail.ryukoku.ac.jp
References
Bksy G. (1935). ber akustische Rauhigkeit. Zeitschrift fr technische Physik, 16, pp.
276-282.
Fastl H. (1982). Fluctuation strength and temporal masking patterns of amplitude-
modulated broadband noise. Hearing Research, 8, pp. 56-69.
Fastl H. and Zwicker E. (1990). Psychoacoustics Facts and Models. Berlin: Springer-
Verlag.
Fujisawa N., Yamada M., and Nakayama I. (2000). Fluctuation in frequency of Japa-
nese pressed voice, Dami-Goe. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan, 21, pp.
365-367.
Fujita O. (2005). The Correct Way to Play Mandolin: Illustrated for Beginners to In-
structors with Concert Repertories. Tokyo: Zen-On Music Company Ltd.
Kurakata K., Kuwano S., and Namba S. (1993). Factors determining the impression of
the equality of intensity in piano performance. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
Japan, 14, pp. 441-447.
Rauber A., Pampalk E., and Merkl D. (2002). Using psycho-acoustic models and self-
organizing maps to create a hierarchical structuring of music by sound similarity.
Paper presented at the Third International Conference onMusic Information Re-
trieval, Paris.
Roads C. (2001). The Computer Music Tutorial. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT
Press.
Terhardt E. (1968). ber akustische rauhigkeit und schwankungsstrke. Acustica, 20,
pp. 215-224.
Thematic session:
Emotion in performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The focus of this study is to examine the effects of social interaction and
musical syntax in a music performance with emphasis on emotion per-
ceptions. The syntactical elements that were analyzed during the course
of these experiments included pitch, tuning, and timing. Two experi-
ments were conducted to examine the relationship of syntax and per-
formance. In the first experiment, subjects were asked to state preference
between altered stimuli with higher levels of expressivity and unaltered
stimuli with lower levels of performer expressivity. In the second experi-
ment, subjects were asked to sing familiar or traditional songs in social
and individual settings. Results confirm that participants demonstrated a
definitive preference toward social interaction when actively engaged in
embodied musical performance. However, the outcome was less defini-
tive in passive listening environment of the first experiment.
tion and group attuning (Leman 2008). De Bruyn et al. (2009) found that
social interaction between music listeners affected music perception and mu-
sical meaning formation processes. Participants in a social setting were better
able to synchronize with music and responded with increased intensity of
corporeal articulations in social conditions.
The purpose of the present study was to explore the settings in which the
importance of syntax is undermined by the context in which the music is
performed. The first experiment measured the effect of changes in syntax of
the music on listeners preferences and perceptions. The second experiment
measured the effect of changes in syntax of the music on subjects preferences
in musical performance environments. In both experimental settings, we
examined methods to measure the elements involved in embodied music
cognition when syntactical elements are altered.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
joyment using the slider as part of the experimental task, allowing for the
collection of data that reflected changing musical preference during active
participation in the performance. Data were analyzed according to a timeline
corresponding with video data.
Procedure
RESULTS
Experiment 1
average throughout the musical excerpts. These data demonstrated that a less
expressive performance is preferred when syntax is altered in a passive lis-
tening situation.
Experiment 2
In the group setting, subjects responded that it was easier to keep the right
tone, there was more fun of singing together and the combination of the
voices was more aesthetically pleasing. The altered syntax was not mentioned
as a factor in responses for the group singing evaluations. Seventy-five per-
cent felt uncomfortable in their individual performance. In addition, sharing
difficulties when performing music increased the enjoyment of some 19% of
participants, who stated that sharing of the experience made them happy
and comfortable.
Slider data confirmed the overall preference for the group setting when
syntax was altered in a musical performance setting. Fifty-nine percent of
participants had a greater number of increases in enjoyment along the scale
when performing stimuli in the group setting. Twenty-two percent had more
increases of enjoyment in the individual setting. Instances where the degree
with which increases in enjoyment were more substantial than decreases in
enjoyment occurred for 46% of participants in the group setting and 15% of
participants in the individual setting. Instances where the degree with which
decreases in enjoyment were more substantial than increases in enjoyment
occurred for 12% of participants in the group setting and overall 38% of par-
ticipants in the individual setting. Fifty percent of participants spent more
time at higher levels of enjoyment in the group, as opposed to 9% who spent
more time at higher levels of enjoyment in the individual setting.
DISCUSSION
Analysis of data facilitated the observation of the cultural and social dynamics
influencing music performance. In Experiment I, participants preferred the
more expressive performance overall when syntax was unaltered. However,
fewer participants with musical experience preferred more expressive per-
formances than those without musical experience. In situations where the
syntax was altered, the majority of participants selected the less expressive
performance. The introduction of an additional level of expressivity may in-
fluence experimental results. In Experiment 2, participants preferred social
interaction in the group setting, as it allowed them to form a common under-
standing regarding music perception. In future research, it may be possible to
integrate syntax alteration through a more ecological approach. Ideally syntax
change would be introduced through the performance of participants or per-
formers themselves. A more nuanced distinction between corporeal and cere-
bral understanding of music and an implementation of a taxonomy of
structural cues to analyze movement data in relation to syntactical change
444 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Ivan Schepers and Pieter Coussement for assistance with technological devel-
opment and design and to Michiel Demey for his assistance with MaxMSP. The authors
also wish to acknowledge Frank Desmet for his assistance with the statistical analysis.
Special thanks also to Liesbeth Bodyn and Anne Claessens for the practical assistance in
experimental administration.
Katty Kochman, Institute for Psychoacoustics and Electronic Music, Ghent University,
Blandijnberg 2, Ghent 9000, Belgium; Email: kjkochman@yahoo.com
References
Baldwin D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C.
Moore and P. J. Dunham (eds), Joint Attention (pp. 131-158) Hillsdale, New Jersey,
USA: Erlbaum.
De Bruyn L., Leman M., Moelants D., and Demey M (2009) Does social interaction
activate music listeners? In S. Ystad and Kronland-Marinet R. (eds), Computer Mu-
sic Modeling and Retrieval (pp. 93-106). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Verlag.
Koelsch S. E., Kasper D., Sammler K. et al. (2004). Music, language and meaning: Brain
signatures of semantic processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, pp. 302-307.
Leman, M. (2008) Embodied Music Cognition and Mediation Technology. Cambridge,
Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.
Overy K and Molnar-Szakacs I. (2009). Being together in time: Musical experience and
the mirror neuron system. Music Perception, 26, pp. 489-504.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Figure 1. Phrase structure of sections 1 and 2 of the Pizzicato Polka, with listeners mean
arousal, velocity, and acceleration values. Theoretical phrase boundaries indicated as
dots. Phase-plane plot for sub-phrase a1 in section 1 (inset) shows velocity and accel-
eration values and begins at the filled square.
RESULTS
Listeners mean emotional arousal values across the entire section 1 were
highly similar to those for section 2, which occurred later in the musical con-
text (mean arousal: r=0.84, p<0.01, mean velocity: r=0.70, p<0.01, mean
acceleration: r=.69, p<0.01). Next we measured response similarity among
the 2D phase-plane plots, shown in Figure 2, at the level of individual sub-
phrases. The procrustes similarity values for repetition pairs (Table 1, first
column) indicated that listeners emotional responses to individual sub-
448 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 2. Phase plane plots for sub-phrases sections 1 (top row) and 2 (bottom) of the
Pizzicato Polka. Sub-phrases begin at the filled squares and possess a cyclical structure.
Figure 3. Automatic phrase detection outcomes aligned with listeners mean arousal
responses (velocity) for section 1. Filled circles indicate theoretical phrase boundaries.
Bars indicate points in time at which a phrase boundary was algorithmically detected
(taller bars=smaller p-values).
DISCUSSION
cessfully. The algorithm operates under the premise that listeners responses
to identical musical sections are most similar at phrase boundaries. Phrase
boundaries are points of perceptual salience, and listeners emotional re-
sponses may accelerate at these locations. Use of the first and second deriva-
tives of emotional arousal helped to provide a more precise temporal marker
of change in listeners responses. This research demonstrates novel analytic
techniques for measuring listeners emotional responses and their correspon-
dence with the underlying phrase structure of music.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Canada Research Chairs program and NSERC Grant
298173 to the fourth author. We thank Erik Koopmans and Frances Spidle.
References
Cambouropoulos E. (2001). The local boundary detection model (LBDM) and its appli-
cation in the study of expressive timing. Proceedings of the International Computer
Music Conference (ICMC01) (pp. 232-235). Havana, Cuba.
Gabrielsson A. and Lindstrom E. (2001). The influence of musical structure on emo-
tional expression. In P.N. Juslin and J. S. Sloboda (eds.), Music and Emotion. Ox-
ford: Oxford University Press.
Kendall R. A. and Carterette. E. C (1990). The communication of musical expression.
Music Perception, 8, pp. 129-164.
Krumhansl C. L. (2002). Music: A link between cognition and emotion. Current Direc-
tions in Psychological Science, 11, pp. 45-50.
Livingstone S. R. and Thompson W. F. (2009). The emergence of music from the theory
of mind. Musicae Scientiae, 10, pp. 83-115.
Ramsay J. O. and Silverman B. W. (2005). Functional Data Analysis. New York:
Springer.
Schubert E. (1999). Measuring emotion continuously: Validity and reliability of the two-
dimensional emotion-space. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, pp. 154-165.
Vines B.W., Nuzzo R. L., and Levitin D. J. (2005). Analyzing temporal dynamics in
music: Differential calculus, physics and functional data analysis techniques. Music
Perception, 23, pp. 137-152.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Procedure
The students preparation of the music was monitored in three phases. The
first phase comprised nine weeks of practice without guidance. The perform-
ances and interviews were recorded in weeks 2, 5, and 9 of the students work.
Complementarily, the students at the laboratory (1) watched videos of profes-
sional pianists playing Brahms, Liszt, Schumann, and Chopin works (week 3),
(2) discussed the relationship between bodily expression and performance
(e.g. Maria Joo Piress masterclass) (week 6), (3) received tips from a profes-
sional pianist concerning practical ways to achieve sound effects as indicated
on the score (week 7), and (4) discussed the structural characteristics of the
score among themselves (week 8).
In the second phase, a sample from the third recording session was evalu-
ated by two referees who focused on contour, articulation, tempo, timing,
dynamics, movements/gesture, and global coherence. A stimulated recall
interview was conducted after each student watched and graded his or her
individual recorded sample. Before entering the third phase, the German
terms present in the score (feurigst and rascher) were again discussed with
the student without reference to actual playing.
During phase three, students practiced for a further two weeks, at the end
of which there was another recording session. Students were then requested
to reveal the emotion they had intended in their performance, making use of
terms defined in Russells circumplex model.
RESULTS
Grades were assigned by two independent piano teachers, who acted as refe-
rees (R1 and R2), and by each student after analyzing the third recorded per-
formance on video in terms of phrase contour, articulation, tempo, timing,
dynamics, movements/gestures, and global coherence on a scale from 1-10,
with 10=highest.
The accuracy for the mean grade and standard deviation (SD) attributed
by the two referees, as well as those given by the student himself or herself,
was calculated for each parameter. Results (see Figure 1) express the degree
attributed by the student himself/herself with the corresponding SD. The
454 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 1. Mean score assigned in each category by each student and respective SD in
comparison to the mean grades assigned by the two referees. Students A, B, and C are
freshmen, D and E are juniors, and F is a graduate student From top left, the graphs
indicate phrase contour, articulation, tempo, timing, dynamics, movements/gestures,
and global coherence.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 455
DISCUSSION
Juslin and Madison (1999) used a combination of analysis and synthesis af-
forded by the ability to resynthesize human performance by means of com-
puter technology. This approach was employed in the investigation of the
relative importance of timing patterns in the communication of emotions
through musical performance. The authors gradually removed different ex-
pressive cues (tempo, dynamics, timing, articulation) from piano perform-
ance played with various intended expressions (anger, sadness, happiness,
fear) to see how such manipulations would affect a listeners ability to decode
the intended emotional expression. The results indicated that (1) removing
the timing pattern yielded a decrease in the listeners decoding accuracy, (2)
timing patterns alone were capable of communicating at least some emotions
with high accuracy, and (3) timing patterns were less effective in communi-
cating emotions than were tempo and dynamics.
According to these authors, listeners can use timing patterns to decode
the emotional expression of a performance. In our present study, the low
degree of correlation between intended and perceived emotion may be asso-
ciated with a high SD in scores of timing, dynamics, and movement/gesture.
The results suggest that in their practice our participants neglected some if
not many of the most fundamental means for achieving global coherence in
terms of the demands of the score and its expression in their musical per-
formances.
Our results suggest that the students have to be made aware of the im-
portance of choosing a tempo (speed, pacing) in the planning and enunciation
of all other parameters evaluated in this study in order to convey their in-
tended expressive qualities in their performances. Furthermore, timing and
movement/gesture seem to develop along and to increase with expertise and
maturity (see Figure 1). For this particular study group, contour was shown to
be well assimilated and less dependent on students level of expertise. We
conclude that expertise in the establishment and manipulations of tempo has
been an aspect largely overlooked in a musicians instruction, and this will be
456 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
the focus of our future research. It seems that the appropriate preparation of
a score depends fundamentally on the articulation of all parameters. It is our
hypothesis that the deliberate choice of tempo sets a precedent in which other
parameters such as contour, articulation, timing, and dynamics will be pro-
portioned, thus fostering global coherence and the expression of the intended
expressive qualities during performance.
Cristina C. Gerling, Graduate Music Program, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul,
Rua Prof. Annes Dias 112, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul 90020-090, Brazil; Email:
cgerling@ufrgs.br
References
Aaron Williamon, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Con-
sort Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: awilliamon@rcm.ac.uk
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Colin Lawson, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort Road, London SW7 2BS, UK;
Email: clawson@rcm.ac.uk
Friday
18 December 2009
Symposium:
Student musicians motivation,
learning, and performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Large-scale studies in the 1990s (Ericsson et al. 1993, Sloboda et al. 1996)
demonstrated the crucial role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of ex-
pertise in musical performance, and more recently, researchers (e.g. Austin et
al. 2006) have discussed the need for powerful motivational resources to
sustain such work, which by definition is not inherently enjoyable. Intrinsic
470 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
motivation is one construct that has been invoked to account for the devel-
opmental shift toward sustained and more self-controlled engagement in the
learning of musical performance skills (Renwick and McPherson 2009,
Sloboda and Davidson 1996). According to self-determination theory, intrin-
sic motivation is the inherent tendency to seek out novelty and challenges, to
extend and exercise ones capacities, to explore, and to learn (Ryan and Deci
2000, p. 70), whereas people are extrinsically motivated when they engage in
an activity such as musical practice in order to obtain a separate outcome,
such as a reward, or to avoid punishment or a sense of shame.
The present study builds on our earlier work examining young musicians
motivational beliefs in light of self-determination theory (Renwick 2008,
Renwick et al. 2002, 2009) by investigating the possible existence of motiva-
tional profiles consisting of different combinations of intrinsic and extrinsic
motives. Such multiple goal profiles have been documented in terms of
achievement goal theory in academic learning contexts (Meece and Holt 1993,
Seifert 1995, Valle et al. 2003), and also in terms of self-determination theory
in physical education (Boich et al. 2008) and athletics (Gillet et al. 2009).
Such findings show that high levels of intrinsic motivation (or mastery goals)
can coexist with high levels of extrinsic motivation (or ego goals) to predict
high levels of achievement. To our knowledge, this possibility has not been
investigated in the domain of music performance training; hence, this paper
aims to extend our prior variable-centered approach to a person-centered
one, by using cluster analysis to identify groups of individuals sharing similar
motivation beliefs.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Figure 1. Mean standardized factor scores for each cluster on each motivational factor.
Procedure
RESULTS
the more internal forms (the dashed line); 174 respondents reported above-
average levels of all five forms of motivation (double line), while the remain-
ing 94 showed below-average levels of all five motives (unbroken line).
A one-way ANOVA was used to test for differences between the cluster
groups in terms of their results in the AMEB examination. Achievement dif-
fered significantly between groups (F3,668=5.13, p<0.01, 2=0.02). Post-hoc
comparisons using the Bonferroni adjustment revealed that the all-high
group (mean=4.87, SD=1.25) achieved significantly (p<0.05) higher results
than both the all-low group (mean=4.37, SD=1.44) and the high-exter-
nal/low-internal group (mean=4.43, SD=1.46). In addition, the high-inter-
nal/low-external cluster (mean=4.79, SD=1.38) achieved significantly
(p<0.05) higher results than the high-external/low-external group. Other
comparisons were non-significant.
DISCUSSION
The results of the cluster analysis showed a remarkably clear pattern (al-
though given the exploratory nature of cluster analysis, this finding should be
treated with some caution). The four groups were strongly differentiated into
(1) a large group reporting mostly internal motivation, (2) a group reporting
mostly external motivation, (3) a group reporting high levels of all five types
of motivation, and (4) a group reporting low levels of all five types. This find-
ing is consistent with prior research in self-determination theory (Boich et
al. 2008, Gillet et al. 2009): because researchers have typically found that
internal motivation and external motivation are only weakly related (r=-0.20
in the present study), a pattern of motivation whereby students can sustain
high levels of a range of motives along the continuum is to be anticipated.
Thus, even though research has shown that the provision of extrinsic rewards
can undermine intrinsic motivation (Deci et al. 1999), it would appear that as
part of long-term motivational profiles, extrinsic and intrinsic motives can
coexist in a complex interplay.
The study found that high levels of internalized motives (sometimes
called mastery goals) can work together with high levels of external motives
(sometimes conceptualized as ego goals) to produce excellent performance,
but external motives alone are insufficient.
Acknowledgments
This study was made possible by a Large Australian Research Council grant (number
A00102974) to Gary McPherson and John McCormick in 2001. We thank the partici-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 473
pants and acknowledge the invaluable assistance of the AMEB (NSW) with data collec-
tion.
References
Gary E. McPherson
spective is built around the central premise that all individuals have natural,
innate and constructive tendencies to develop an ever more elaborated and
unified sense of self (Deci and Ryan, p. 5). SDT is concerned with the choices
people make on their own and how they choose to behave in a manner that
reflects their autonomy and their behavior, free of the need to receive an ex-
ternal reward. In this way, SDT is used to examine the degree to which an
individuals behavior is self-endorsed and self-determined. A key component
of SDT is the three innate and universal psychological needs which Deci and
Ryan state are essential to an individuals psychological health and wellbeing.
The need to feel competent underpins our desire to believe that we are capa-
ble and competent and in control of our environment. When we feel compe-
tent, we are better positioned to seek further challenges and to engage in an
activity at a more concentrated level. According to Deci and Ryan, when
autonomous, individuals experience their behavior as an expression of the
self, such that, even when actions are influenced by outside sources, the ac-
tors concur with those influences, feeling both initiative and value with regard
to them (p. 8). Finally, people also have a need for relatedness, to be con-
nected with and cared for by others. The sense of belongingness that can
come from participation and involvement in a musical ensemble, for example,
is one way this psychological need can be met, especially given that this di-
mension concerns the psychological sense of being with others in secure
communion or unity (p. 7). In summary, SDT proposes that individuals are
active in their pursuit to satisfy these three psychological needs and is being
applied in various disciplines to explain why people become sufficiently in-
trinsically motivated that they will engage in an activity for its own sake and
at a level that becomes fully internalized and autonomous.
The purpose of this study was to clarify the degree to which 34 highly in-
volved young school-aged musicians believed that their personal learning
agenda was being fulfilled, and the ways in which the teaching and learning
process to which they were exposed satisfied their psychological needs as
defined by Deci and Ryans (2002) SDT.
METHOD
Participants
The study involved individual interviews with 34 musicians during their last
two years of high school (years 11 and 12). All were identified by their teachers
as among the most dedicated and highly involved in a large Midwest state in
the United States. The musicians ranged from those who were section leaders
in one of the most prominent youth symphony orchestras in the country, to
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 477
students in school music programs that have received national and interna-
tional recognition for the quality of their program, through to others who
were performing and composing for highly regarded folk ensembles and rock
groups.
Materials
Procedure
As shown in Figure 1, key issues covered in the interviews focused on how the
three psychological needs were being met, the quality and quantity of in-
volvement and engagement with music, the types of networks (such as peers,
social groupings, parental support) which facilitated the musicians interest in
music, their sense of purpose as a musician, and the degree to which their
involvement in music both socially and personally was a valued part of their
lives. Additional issues concerned what these students hoped to accomplish
in music both now and in the future, and the extent to which their personal
musical learning agenda was being met through their participation in formal
and informal learning processes, plus whether any of these might comple-
ment or conflict with other aspects of their learning. To clarify this last issue,
questions were asked about the musicians short and long-term goals in mu-
sic, and the influences that were impacting on their decision whether to
choose music as a career beyond high school.
RESULTS
As confirmed by their teachers, all of the musicians had the potential to be-
come professional musicians. Most felt comfortable with their school or
community music experiences, where like-minded peers, supportive teachers,
and an environment that was conducive to their needs challenged them to
achieve at a constantly higher level. Others had ceased participating in certain
musical activities that they felt were not at the level of their own musical
skills, while others were agonizing with personal and external influences
478 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
As the only musician in her family, and recognizing also the commitment
her parents had made to supporting her musically, this young musician wor-
ried that she takes the limelight away from her other two sisters. As one of
the best flutists in the country for her age, there can be no doubt about the
intensity and level of her current musical engagement. Digging more deeply
to compare her current and future self however, revealed many niggling wor-
ries and self-doubts about whether she wanted a career in music, whether she
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 479
would still feel the same way about music in the years ahead, and whether a
career in music was worth the effort, given the possibility that she may be
separated from her family and the city she loved. Many of these worries and
self-doubts were perceptions about music and musicians that she had picked
up from others, with many of the more important coming from her mother:
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by a NAMM Sounds of Learning research grant (RFP 2.7).
Gary McPherson, School of Music, University of Melbourne, 234 St. Kilda Road,
Southbank, Victoria 3006, Australia; Email: g.mcpherson@unimelb.edu.au
References
Jane Ginsborg
Strategies for ensemble rehearsal have been explored via studies of duos (e.g.
Williamon and Davidson 2002, Ginsborg and King 2007), string quartets
(e.g. Davidson and Good 2002), and wind quintets (Ford and Davidson 2003,
Burt-Perkins and Mills 2008), suggesting the importance of clarity as to en-
semble members roles in order to reduce potential conflicts and effective
verbal and non-verbal communication in relation to social as well as musical
issues. Ericsson et al. (1993) and Lehmann (2002) found that positive corre-
lations exist between concentration and effort, and concentration and enjoy-
ment, but negative correlations between effort and enjoyment. The present
study investigated the extent to which successful ensemble rehearsal (defined
482 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
Three student ensembles were recruited: one newly-formed (SQ1) and one
established string quartet (SQ3) and a newly-formed first-year wind quintet
(WQ1). SQ1 comprised two male and two female first year students matched
on the basis of their performance in college entrance auditions. SQ3 was
formed by its membersthree male third year students and one female stu-
denttwo years before the present study began. WQ1 comprised a mixed
group of four first year students and one second year female student, selected
on the basis of the instruments they played.
Printed diary templates were devised for ensemble rehearsals and practice
sessions in which music for ensemble rehearsals was prepared. The ensemble
rehearsal diaries requested information including date, place, time, overall
duration, and content/duration of each activity within the rehearsal; with
reference to up to five activities, what participants thought their ensembles
goal was, how they went about achieving it, to what extent their strategy was
effective, using a rating scale (1=useless to 10=achieved goal 100%) and
what their goal would be next time they engaged in this activity; ratings of
their own and their peers focus (1=very distracted to 10=completely fo-
cused), effort (1=just went through the motions to 10=worked really
hard), and enjoyment (1=not at all to 10=had a great time). Finally, par-
ticipants were asked to describe any performance they had given that day and
nominate one thing [they] were really happy about and one thing [they]
would like to do better in [their] next performance. The practice diaries used
similar wording, adapted for individuals, and rating scales but did not ask
about performance. During the first two terms of the academic year partici-
pants were asked to complete diaries whenever they had undertaken individ-
ual practice on material for ensemble rehearsals and, as individuals, after
each rehearsal.
RESULTS
At the end of the academic year, WQ1 won a major prize. At the time of writ-
ing, its members are still performing together: it can be considered a suc-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 483
Table 4. Ratings of own focus, effort and Table 5. Ratings of others focus, effort,
enjoyment in ensemble rehearsals. and enjoyment in ensemble rehearsals.
DISCUSSION
just two rather unsuccessful coaching sessions early in the first term. Fur-
thermore, WQ1 reported the highest levels of focus, effort, and enjoyment in
rehearsal both for themselves and their colleagues. It may thus be possible for
student musicians to do less individual practice if they have plenty of, and
enjoy, ensemble rehearsal, although this is, of course, dependent on their role
within the group. A final contrast is between SQ1, who rated focus, effort, and
enjoyment for individual practice higher than WQ1 but provided the lowest
ratings on all three dimensions for ensemble rehearsalit was perhaps in-
evitable that they would fail as a group.
Further analyses of the participants goals, strategies, and plans remain to
be carried out; nevertheless, the findings of the present study have implica-
tions for theories of motivation in relation to practice versus rehearsal, as well
as practical applications for those engaged in the teaching and coaching of
chamber musicians.
Jane Ginsborg, Centre for Music Performance Research, Royal Northern College of
Music, 124 Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9RD, UK; Email: jane.ginsborg@rncm.ac.uk
References
Burt-Perkins R. and Mills J. (2008). The role of chamber music in learning to perform:
A case study. Music Performance Research, 2, pp. 26-35.
Davidson J. W. and Good J. J. (2002). Social and musical communication between
members of a string quartet: An exploratory study. Psychology of Music, 30, pp.
186-201.
Ericsson K. A., Krampe R. T., and Tesch-Rmer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate prac-
tice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, pp. 363-
406.
Ford L. and Davidson J. W. (2003). An investigation of members roles in wind quintets.
Psychology of Music, 31, pp. 53-74.
Ginsborg J. and King E. C. (2007). The roles of expertise and partnership in duo per-
formance. In A. Williamon and D. Coimbra (eds.), Proceedings of ISPS 2007 (pp.
61-66). Utrecht, The Netherlands: European Association of Conservatoires (AEC).
Lehmann A. (2002). Effort and enjoyment in deliberate practice: A research note. In I.
M. Hanken, S. G. Nielsen, and M. Nerland (eds.), Research In and For Music Edu-
cation (pp. 153-166). Oslo: Norwegian Academy of Music.
Williamon A. and Davidson J. (2002). Exploring co-performer communication. Music
Scienti, 6, pp. 53-72.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Higher scores indicate greater anxiety and psychological distress. This scale
demonstrated excellent internal reliability (=0.94, Kenny et al. 2004).
Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE, Watson and Friend 1969) assesses
expectations of being evaluated negatively and consists of 30 items that are
answered either true or false. The FNE has high internal consistency
(=0.94-0.96), strong one-month test-retest reliability (r=0.78-0.94), and
criterion validity (Watson and Friend 1969).
Surface electromyography (EMG) is a safe and non-invasive method for
measuring the nerve activation that occurs in a muscle as a result of voluntary
or reflex activation (Turker 1993). Stress levels and occupational loads are
two factors that influence surface EMG output readings, with particularly
responsive sites for these measures in the upper trapezius and frontalis mus-
cles (Westgaard 2000, Attebrant et al. 1996). Ten-second window measure-
ments were taken from the EMG recording of the upper left trapezius muscle
of the flute players in this study. A ratio of the maximal contraction from the
EMG (in Hz) to a reference voluntary contraction was recorded.
A silver chloride electrode was used to measure heart rate (ECG).
Maxima from the heart rate trace were averaged over blocks of time immedi-
ately before performance to achieve a resting heart rate and during perform-
ance of the musical item.
Till Eulenspiegels Lustige Streiche (R. Strauss, Op. 28), was chosen as the
set work because it is a well-known excerpt from the orchestral flute reper-
toire. A scoring protocol designed specifically for the study was developed by
a flute player from a premier national orchestra. Players were scored on a
criterion-referenced scale from 1 to 10 (each mark required the standard to be
met for all rankings below the ranking given).
Procedure
Ten days before the scheduled participation, participants were sent the FNE
and STAI-T questionnaires, the orchestral extracts, an instruction letter, a
map of how to get to the venue, and clothing instructions. These practices are
employed in most orchestral auditions, thus giving participants exactly the
same time-frame in which to prepare the music. Participants had an electro-
myographic trace of the left trapezius muscle taken and their HR recorded
during their performance in the acoustic laboratory at the Australian Centre
for Applied Research in Music Performance (ACARMP), Sydney Conservato-
rium of Music, University of Sydney. An expert flute player with extensive
adjudicator experience blind rated all the (recorded) performances in terms
of musical and technical proficiency from the industry standard recording.
490 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
RESULTS
The sample comprised 20 flute players (3 males, 17 females) who were all
students of flute at the Sydney Conservatorium of Music studying in the
Bachelor of Music (n=17) or Master of Music (Performance) (n=3) degrees.
Participants were aged between 19 and 32 years of age. They had been study-
ing flute for a minimum of 10 years and a maximum of 22 years (Table 1).
Fifteen of the participants (75%) wanted to become professional musicians,
three were undecided, and two did not wish to become professional musi-
cians.
A number of univariate analyses were undertaken to assess the relation-
ship of demographic and other factors with performance quality. None were
significantly associated (see Discussion). Hierarchical regression analyses
tested the hypothesis that the specified variableslevel of accomplishment on
the flute (ranking given for the highest level of achievement as a soloist),
performance preparation (number of minutes the test pieces were practiced),
psychological measures (K-MPAI, FNE, STAI), and physiological measures
(HR, EMG)would account for variation in expert ratings of the Strauss ex-
cerpt. Because the variables in the predictive model were on different scales,
they were converted to z-scores with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of
1 before being entered into the regression analysis. ANOVA indicated a sig-
nificant F-statistic (F7,12=4.59, p=0.01), showing that using the model in-
creased the probability of correctly predicting expert ratings over chance. The
overall R2 for the model was 0.73 (i.e. the model accounted for 73% of the
variance in expert ratings in the Strauss excerpt, see Table 2).
DISCUSSION
In the univariate tests, the amount of time participants had been playing their
instrument was not associated with how much MPA, fear of negative evalua-
tion, or state or trait anxiety they experienced. Length of time one has been
playing ones instrument is a poor proxy for both the quality of practice that
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 491
Table 1. Means and standard deviations (SD) for player characteristics and expert rat-
ings of musical performance.
Characteristics Mean SD
Age 23.05 3.76
Years playing flute 13.98 3.15
Practice episodes per week 6.90 3.09
Average daily practice in minutes 131.25 58.71
Highest level of achievement as a soloist 4.50 2.12
Highest level of achievement in ensemble 5.05 2.26
Minutes practiced test piece 41.25 30.30
Expert rating Strauss 5.60 1.96
Physiological measures
EMG percent Strauss excerpt 24.42 9.22
Heart rate playing Strauss excerpt 99.15 13.31
Table 2. Beta coefficients, t-tests, and significance for the regression model predicting
expert ratings for the Strauss excerpt.
Unstand. Stand.
Strauss coeff. coeff. t Sig.
Z scores of predictors B SE Beta
(Constant) 1.78 2.75 0.65 0.53
Highest achievement as soloist 0.85 0.16 0.91 5.34 0.001
Minutes practised test piece 0.03 0.01 0.39 2.19 0.05
MPA total 0.04 0.02 0.38 1.83 0.09
FNE total -0.03 0.03 -0.15 -0.80 0.44
EMG percent Strauss excerpt 0.04 0.04 0.20 1.07 0.31
Heart rate Strauss excerpt -0.04 0.03 -0.26 -1.42 0.18
the performer undertakes and the amount of musical ability the performer
has. A better measure to assess these elements is the highest level of achieve-
ment attained in solo and/or ensemble performance, which was associated
with significantly lower reported MPA. Those with the most ensemble experi-
ence and achievement also had significantly less fear of negative evaluation. A
very robust yet simple model was developed for the prediction of expert rat-
ing of skilled performance. Three factorshighest level of achievement on
flute, minutes practiced the test piece, and MPAaccounted for 73% of the
492 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Dianna Kenny, University of Sydney, PO Box 170, Lidcombe, New South Wales 1825,
Australia; Email: d.kenny@usyd.edu.au
References
Attebrant M., Mathiassen S. E., and Winkel J. (1996). Normalizing upper trapezius
amplitude: Comparison of ramp and constant force production. Journal of Elec-
tromyography and Kinesiology, 5, pp. 245-250.
Barlow H. (2000). Unravelling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the per-
spective of emotion theory. American Psychologist, November, pp. 1247-1263.
Kenny D. T. (2004). Music performance anxiety: Is it the music, the performance, or the
anxiety? Music Forum, 10, pp. 38-43.
Kenny D. T., Davis P., and Oates J. (2004). Music performance anxiety and occupa-
tional stress amongst opera chorus artists and their relationship with state and trait
anxiety and perfectionism. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, pp. 757-777.
Spielberger C. (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, Califor-
nia, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Turker K. S. (1993). Electromyography: Some methodological problems and issues.
Physical Therapy, 3, pp. 698-710.
Watson D. and Friend R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of
Consultative Clinical Psychology, 33, pp. 448-457.
Westgaard R. H. (2000). Work-related musculoskeletal complaints: Some ergonomics
challenges upon the start of a new century. Applied Ergonomics, 31, pp. 569-580.
Thematic session:
Performers health
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Anke Steinmetz
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
Musicians with neck pain show a delay of deep cervical flexor activation
during upper limb movement (e.g. playing their instrument).
In musicians with neck pain, muscle function of scalene and sternocleido-
mastoid muscles are impaired.
Pain in the arm and the shoulder alters muscle activation and recruitment
patterns of trapezius muscle.
Musicians with low back pain show signs of delayed deep abdominal mus-
cle activation.
of scapular stabilization system was found within the examination and in 71%
the lumbopelvic stabilization system (deep abdominal and back muscles).
Preliminary results of ultrasound investigations of the activation of the trans-
versus abdominus muscle in musicians with musculoskeletal dysfunctions
support these findings. This research data suggest an association of the im-
pairment of the lumbopelvic stabilization system with PRMD. Muscle dys-
functions and postural stabilization impairments can influence performance
excellence in various ways. An example demonstrating this phenomenon is
the case of a cellist with impaired interossei muscles, contributing to a lack of
stabilization abilities of the fourth finger of the left hand. A second example is
a violinist with neck and shoulder pain experiencing an alteration of shoulder
muscle activation patterns, resulting in a changed up-bow pattern showing
increased trapezius activation.
IMPLICATIONS
References
Cholewicki J., Silfies S. P., Shah R. A. et al. (2005). Delayed trunk muscle reflex re-
sponses increase the risk of low back injuries. Spine, 30, pp. 2614-2620.
Cools A. M., Witvrouw E. E., Declercq G. A. et al. (2003). Scapular muscle recruitment
patterns: Trapezius muscle latency with and without impingement syndrome.
American Journal of Sports Medicine, 31, pp. 542-549.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 499
Ervilha U. F., Farina D., Arendt-Nielsen L., and Graven-Nielsen T. (2005). Experimen-
tal muscle pain changes motor control strategies during dynamic fatiguing contrac-
tions. Experimental Brain Research, 164, pp. 215-224.
Ervilha U. F., Arendt-Nielsen L., Duarte M., and Graven-Nielsen T. (2004). The effect of
muscle pain on elbow flexion and coactivation tasks. Experimental Brain Research,
156, pp. 174-182.
Falla D., Rainoldi A., Merletti R., and Jull G. (2003). Myoelectric manifestations of
sternocleidomastoid and anterior scalene muscle fatigue in chronic neck pain pa-
tients. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, pp. 488-495.
Falla D., Bilenkij G., and Jull G. (2004a). Patients with chronic neck pain demonstrate
altered patterns of muscle activation during performance of a functional upper limb
task. Spine, 29, pp. 1436-1440.
Falla D., Jull G., and Hodges P. W. (2004b). Feedforward activity of the cervical flexor
muscles during voluntary arm movements is delayed in chronic neck pain. Experi-
mental Brain Research, 157, pp. 43-48.
Falla D., Jull G., Edwards S. et al. (2004c). Neuromuscular efficiency of the sternoclei-
domastoid and anterior scalene muscles in patients with neck pain. Disability and
Rehabilitation, 26, pp. 712-717.
Falla D., Farina D., and Graven-Nielsen T. (2007a). Experimental muscle pain results in
reorganization of coordination among trapezius muscle subdivisions during repeti-
tive shoulder flexion. Experimental Brain Research, 178, pp. 385-393.
Falla D., Farina D., Kanstrup Dahl M., and Graven-Nielsen T. (2007b). Muscle pain
induces task-dependent changes in cervical agonist/antagonist activity. Journal of
Applied Physiology, 102, pp. 601-609.
Fry H. J., Hallett M., Mastroianni T. et al. (1998). Incoordination in pianists with over-
use syndrome. Neurology, 51, pp. 512-519.
Hodges P. W. and Richardson C. A. (1996). Inefficient muscular stabilization of the
lumbar spine associated with low back pain. A motor control evaluation of transver-
sus abdominis. Spine, 22, pp. 2640-2650.
Hodges P. W. and Richardson C. A. (1999) Altered trunk muscles recruitment in people
with low back pain with upper limb movement at different speeds. Archives of
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 80, pp. 1005-1012.
Hodges P. W. and Moseley G. L. (2003). Pain and motor control of the lumbopelvic
region: Effect and possible mechanisms. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesi-
ology, 13, pp. 361-370.
Hodges P. W. (2001). Changes in motor planning of feedforward postural responses of
the trunk muscles in low back pain. Experimental Brain Research, 141, pp. 261-
266.
Jabusch H.-C., Vauth H., and Altenmller E. (2003). Quantification of focal dystonia in
pianists using scale analysis. Movement Disorders, 19, pp. 171-180.
500 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Leinonen V., Kankaanpaa M., Luukkonen M. et al. (2001). Disc-herniated back pain
impairs feed-forward control of paraspinal muscles. Spine, 26, pp. E367-372.
Lin j.-J., Wu Y.-T., Wang S.-F., and Chen S.-Y. (2005). Trapezius muscle imbalance in
individuals suffering from frozen shoulder syndrome. Clinical Rheumatology, 24,
pp. 569-575.
Ludewig P. M. and Cook T. M. (2000). Alterations in shoulder kinematics and associ-
ated muscle activity in people with symptoms of shoulder impingement. Physical
Therapy, 80, pp. 276-291.
Schulte E., Ciubotariu A., Arendt-Nielsen L. et al. (2004). Experimental muscle pain
increases trapezius muscle activity during sustained isometric contractions of arm
muscles. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, pp. 1767-1778.
Steinmetz A. (2007). Differences in musculoskeletal dysfunctions between music and
nonmusic students. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 22, pp. 169-178.
Steinmetz A. and Seidel W. (submitted). Impairment of postural stabilization systems in
musicians with playing-related musculoskeltal disorders.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
The aim of this study was to find out whether the masticatory and pos-
tural muscles are used by pianists during their music performances. The
study also aimed to ascertain whether the complex neuromuscular activ-
ity involved in the act of playing the piano also encourages hyperactivity
in terms of the masticatory muscles. The bio-electric potentials of the
masticatory and postural muscles of 20 pianists were recorded by surface
electromyography (EMG). The EMG recordings obtained regarding the
temporal and masticatory muscles are much higher than those recorded
when in the resting position. These recordings, which are not the same as
those obtained, for example, when the individuals are chewing hard food
such as a carrot, are nonetheless indicative of daily parafunctional activ-
ity in musicians who often study for as much as seven hours per day.
2006) or from an inadequate dental treatment and increase with stress. In all
cases, it may be detrimental to the musicians careers.
Pianists are among the highest in number of music college students in
Portugal, both in classical and jazz degree courses. Research on piano related
injuries is mainly focused on the upper limbs, namely the hand adaptation to
the keyboard, hand pain, focal hand dystonia, finger joints or tendon, and
arm overuse related problems (Shields and Dockrell 2000, Sakai 2008). Yet,
little has been written about their orofacial activity. Given the duration and
intensity of the daily practice these pianists might undertake (as much as
seven hours per day), they may well develop a parafunctional activity, espe-
cially in the masticatory and postural muscles. It is extremely important to
monitor such activity so that muscular hyperactivity does not interfere in
maintaining a functional equilibrium of the cranio-cervic0-mandibular com-
plex. Hence, the aim of the present study was to determine whether the mas-
ticatory muscles, whose main functions are chewing, swallowing, and speak-
ing, are used by pianists during their music performances and so to ascertain
whether the complex neuromuscular activity involved in the act of playing the
piano also encourages hyperactivity in terms of the masticatory and postural
muscles. Since we had both classical and jazz pianists in our sample, an addi-
tional aim was to ascertain whether different styles have a different effect on
the masticatory and postural muscles.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
The electromyographic (EMG) activity was recorded using the Bio EMG 2
electromyograph with eight channels (Bioresearch Associates Inc., Milwau-
kee, Wisconsin, USA).
Procedure
An alcohol pad was wiped in the area where the sensors were to be placed and
a conductive gel was placed on a 9 mm diameter disposable silver/silver-
chloride bipolar surface electrodes (Duo-Trode, Myotronics Inc., Seattle,
Washington, USA), before their attachment to the skin. These bipolar surface
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 503
RESULTS
The head posture adopted by piano players during their performance gave
different patterns of EMG activity in the masticatory and postural muscles.
The visual system used in this study played an important role in the percep-
tion that there are variations in seated positions between classical piano and
jazz players, with direct implications on the cranio-cervico-mandibular com-
plex.
Likewise, it is possible to verify an adoption of an anterior head position
on jazz players, with a tendency of most of these subjects to maintain a jaw
position by using the anterior temporal more than the masseter muscles. In
classical players, there were higher values on the EMG activity of the masse-
ter muscles compared with the jazz players. A contributing factor can be that
jazz players are used to improvising during their performance while classical
players have a prolonged and strained stress placed on the masticatory mus-
cles, specifically the masseter muscle. For the particular case of classical pi-
anists, we were able to compare the activity of a mandible elevator muscle
504 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
(masseter) during one of its main functions: eating. When eating a cake, the
bioelectrical potential of the masseter muscles reached 15-18 V, while play-
ing for example the C minor Rachmaninoff concerto, the masseter muscle can
reach values of 49.6 V.
The findings of this study illustrate that pianists may feel persistent neck
and orofacial pain due to the large number of hours of piano exposure, high-
lighting the physical demands as well as the psychosocial factors involved in
such a demanding profession. Understanding what kind of muscles are being
used in the cranio-cervico-mandibular complex during the performance of a
pianist is of vital importance as it can help in the correct diagnosis of a con-
crete problem originated by repetitive movements.
The aim of the study was to understand how the orofacial muscles behave
during voluntary exercise such as piano playing, where the activity of some
muscleslike masseter, digastrics, or the anterior temporalshould have
minimum activity given that their key activity should occur mainly during
mastication, speech, and swallowing. In this particular case it was not a ques-
tion of being able to evaluate if the fatigue of these muscles could restrict the
pianist to the point of no longer being able to performing the task: piano
playing. An overuse of these muscles may have a direct impact on the pianists
quality of life when performing their maximal voluntary performance of these
orofacial muscles during normal tasks like eating.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 505
In time, these kinds of restrictions can induce pain that can be related to
certain movements or occur at any time of the day. This special attention
toward performers health issues and specific needs will provide a working
diagnosis, allowing health care professionals to focus their examination
rather than conducting a series of tests that are usually time consuming.
Having piano players, teachers, and performing arts medicine profession-
als conscious of what is actually happening to the orofacial region while
playing piano will encourage the daily supervision of any parafunctional habit
like clenching their teeth during performance.
DISCUSSION
Do pianists really play with their teeth? The essential point is yes, some
pianists do in fact play with their teeth, since they have an activity of their
masseter and anterior temporal muscles that acts as an elevator of the man-
dible, forcing the mandible teeth to contact the maxillary teeth. So here piano
players, during their daily performance, have parafunctional habits inducing
hyperactivity of some of the orofacial muscles that very often is associated
when an individual is concentrated on a particular task or when anxiety levels
rise.
506 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
This work has been supported by the Fundao para a Cincia e Tecnologia (FCT) and
Programa Operacional Cincia e Inovao 2010 (POCI 2010), co-funded by the Portu-
guese government and European Union by the FEDER program.
Sofia Loureno, Research Centre for Science and Technology of the Arts (CITAR),
School of the Arts, Portuguese Catholic University, Rua Diogo Botelho 1327, 4169-005
Porto, Portugal; Email: slourenco@porto.ucp.pt
References
Altenmller E. (2000). From Laetoli to Carnegie: Evolution of brain and hand as pre-
requisites of music performance in the light of music physiology and neurobiology.
Paper presented at the Tenth Symposium of the International Study Group of the
Archeology of Music, Kloster Michaelstein, Germany.
Ericsson K., Krampe R., and Tesch Romer C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in
the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, pp. 363-406.
Hallam S. (1997). Approaches to instrumental practices of experts and novices: Impli-
cations for education. In H. Jorgensen and A. Lehmann (eds.), Does Practice Make
Perfect? (pp. 89-107). Oslo: Norwegian Academy of Music.
Morse T., Ro J., Cherniack S., and Pelletier S. (2000). A pilot population study of mus-
culoskeletal disorders in musicians. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 15,
pp. 81-87.
Raney N. (2006). The effects of orthodontic appliances on wind-instrument players.
Journal of Clinical Orthodontics, 40, pp. 384-387.
Sakai N. (2008). Keyboard span in old musical instruments: Concerning hand span and
overuse problems in pianists. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 23, pp. 169-
171.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
We have looked at data from three clinics active in London in the period
1990-94: ISSTIP, BAPAM, and APC. For the period 1995-2005, we only ex-
amined the data from the ISSTIP clinics. The ISSTIP clinic had a break in
operation in 2005 but is due to restart in 2009.
Procedure
We analyzed the collected data from those attending the clinics in two peri-
ods: 1990-94 and 1995-2005. During the first period of our study, we exam-
ined comparative data from ISSTIP, BAPAM, and APC clinics. In the second
period of the study we looked exclusively at the ISSTIP clinic, and we com-
pared these results with those of the earlier period of the same clinic.
RESULTS
ISSTIP and BAPAM offered three areas of help: medical, postural, and psy-
chological. APC offered just psychological help and referred cases needing
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 509
Table 1. Overall profile of the three clinics studied for the period 1990-94.
medical or postural treatment to the other two bodies. As such its number of
cases is proportionately less. The preponderance of females in the first two
clinics is interesting since numbers were equal in those seeking psychological
help (see Table 1). The breakdown by profession (see Table 2) shows an
ISSTIP sample composed almost entirely of musicians. The choice of a music
college setting with its attendant students would bias the numbers toward
musicians in the case of ISSTIP, though it is clear that the clientele most in-
terested in using such clinics was musicians in general. Musicians seem more
willing to seek direct help from performing arts organizations, although
dancers and actors do also have their own health networks. Table 3 shows
very considerable bias to classical musicians which is not mirrored in the
general pool of active musicians. With a music college setting we might expect
this bias in the ISSTIP clinics, but it also occurs in the APC clinics.
ISSTIP clinics saw a proportionately larger number of students and
teachers than BAPAM or APC (see Table 4), and the assumption is that again
this is because clinics took place in a music college where teachers also sat in
and took an interest. Even so, the fact that 36% of ISSTIP clients are profes-
sional musicians shows that the clinic was known and used by a much wider
clientele. Also interesting is that music students, who would typically have a
generalist student counselor available to them, preferred to seek specialized
help with their performance problems. One quarter of APC clients were from
music schools that had their own student counselors, indicating that special-
ist knowledge of dealing with performance problems responds to a need in
music students. When Andy Evans was student counselor at the Royal Acad-
emy of Music, three quarters of the cases he saw were related to performance
issues, rather than general counseling matters (Evans 2003).
ISSTIP clinics had an unexpectedly high percentage of keyboard players;
other instruments being broadly comparable (see Table 3). This may be a bias
due to the fact that Carola Grindea was a well-known piano pedagogue.
Clearly, the profile of the organization affects the profile of those using it.
Given the already established weighting toward classical musicians we would
also expect larger numbers of string players to reflect their distribution in
orchestras. Among the wind players, flute and clarinet predominated. Gui-
tarists tended to go more to BAPAM where they would get medical help, while
singers tended to make more use of psychologists. The nature of each organi-
zation determined the primary service offered to those who used it, and the
primary focus in the ISSTIP clinics was on posture control, breathing, and
music ergonomics (player-instrument interaction), with a secondary empha-
sis on medical interventions and a tertiary emphasis on psychological meth-
ods (see Table 5). This was the other way around in APC consultants who
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 511
DISCUSSION
Clinics all have a distinct profile depending on the founders and their influ-
ence, the name of the organization and its implications, and the location of
the clinics. This profile acts as a triage system for not only those who attend
but the focus of the treatment they are thought to need and the treatment
they actually receive. Clinics have been largely built around musiciansnot
only as clients but also music as the secondary skill of the practitioner. The
early 1990s were a particularly active time for clinics as performers aired their
problems in many cases for the first time. Ideally a performer who attends a
clinic will get an impartial assessment entirely congruent with his or her
needs. This sample of data shows that this was not happening because of in-
herent bias within the organizations. This may be an insoluble problem, and
it is left ultimately with the client to make their own choices. In an ideal world
referrals would take place routinely to the appropriate specialist, and it is to
be hoped that teamwork will always be important in the field of performance
health. Another issue is that medical practitioners are trained to pass clients
to specialists, but this is less the case with therapists and posture teachers
who believe they deal with the whole person, not the problem. The field of
performance health however relies on specialists, and it is important that
specialization is respected by all who practice in this field, passing clients on
where appropriate for the client and the problem.
From these conclusions, we can ask wider questions as to whether such
clinics will always represent a symbiosis between the individual nature of
512 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
1990-94 1995-2005
Overall number seen 363 376
Male/female (%) 37.0/63.0 48.0/52.0
Musicians/dancers, actors, etc. (%) 96.4/3.6 98.0/2.0
Classical/pop, jazz (%) 98.0/2.0 94.0/6.0
Piano, keyboard (%) 36.0 64.0
Professional (%) 36.0 25.0
Students (%) 51.2 57.0
Teachers (%) 8.4 11.0
Medical treatment (%) 42.7 6.5
Psychology (%) 5.8 0.0
Posture (%) 51.5 93.5
their setup and how they function, or whether we should seek to move toward
a more all-embracing provision of care that could be replicated in a more
generic way for future clinics. Some such questions include (1) whether there
are a set of protocols that would be helpful, (2) how staff should be chosen
and trained, and (3) what should be the relationship between clinics, support
organizations, educational establishments, and the wider world of the arts
with a goal of providing students, teachers, and professional performers with
a better and more effective service. When ISSTIP clinics resume later in
2009, these questions will be in the forefront of discussions.
Hara Trouli, ISSTIP Head Office, 6 Jonathans, Dene Road, Northwood, Middlesex HA6
2AD, UK; Email: isstip@gmail.com
References
There is little research into how young students experience the educational
dance activities designed by adults (Bamford 2006). Phenomenological re-
search suggests that dancers share common experiences across age, culture,
and gender (Alter 1997, Bond and Stinson 2000, Bracey 2004, Lindqvist
2001, Stinson 1997, Stinson et al. 1990). It has also been suggested that envi-
ronmental factors may influence this experience (Fensham and Gardner
2005, Green 1999, Hefferon and Ollis 2006, Oliver 2000, Thomas 1993). In
516 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
The participants comprised 48 dancers that had accepted a place in the pro-
gram. This group contained equal numbers of female and male dancers
(mean age=16.25 years, SD=2.41). The second group of participants con-
tained 9 former dancers, aged between 17 and 23 years, who participated in
focus groups designed to generate material for the Q-sort.
Materials
Basic demographics were taken for each participant including sex, age, and
how many years they had been involved in (1) dance and (2) the program. The
content of the focus group discussions served to generate Q-sort materials: 19
experience statements, which represented overarching descriptions of
grouped quotes, and 8 categories, which grouped similar experience state-
ments. The future in dance category contained only a future in dance experi-
ence statement. The program elements category contained performing,
production week, rehearsals, and dancing experience statements. The cho-
reographic process category contained the idea/concept, team-
work/groupwork, and making up movement experience statements. The
peers category held only one experience statement: other dancers. The role
models category included choreographers and the artistic director. External
people contained outside friends and parents. The personal attributes cate-
gory included growth/maturity, self-disciple, and confidence. Finally, the
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 517
Procedure
The focus groups were conducted leading up to and beginning the program.
The Q-sort was conducted between 2-3 weeks after the conclusion of the
performances. The instrumental basis of the Q-methodology is the Q-sort
technique (McKeown 1988). This required the participants to rank-order
systematically the 19 experience statements in a forced choice procedure ac-
cording to their own personal perspective of the program. The Self Descrip-
tion Questionnaire (SDQ-III) was administered as a pre and post measure.
RESULTS
DISCUSSION
The program provided different types of dancers with different ways of con-
necting into dance and demonstrated how dance programs can promote ef-
fective differentiated learning and opportunities for both male and female
dancers. The dance as performance experience produced educational out-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 519
Shona Erskine, Dance Department, Edith Cowan University, PO Box 432, Bassendean,
Western Australia 6054, Australia; Email: shonaerskine@gmail.com
References
Alter J. B. (1997). Why dance students pursue dance: Studies of dance students from
1953 to 1993. Dance Research Journal, 29, pp. 70-89.
Bamford A. (2006). The Wow Factor. Berlin: Waxmann Mnster.
Bond K. E. and Stinson S. W. (2000). I feel like Im going to take off! Young peoples
experiences of the superordinary in dance. Dance Research Journal, 32, pp. 52-87.
Bracey L. (2004). Voicing connections: An interpretive study of university dancers
experiences. Research in Dance Education, 5, pp. 7-24.
520 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Barbara Snook
Barbara Snook, Dance Studies, National Institute of Creative Arts and Industries, Uni-
versity of Auckland, Building 421, Level 5, 26 Symonds Street, Auckland 1010, New
Zealand; Email: b.snook@auckland.ac.nz
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Since the early 1990s, work within contemporary dance in Australia has been
undertaken almost entirely by independent dance artists. Existing company-
based employment usually takes the form of contract-based employment
interspersed with other work. As we have argued previously (Pollitt 2001,
Bennett 2009), what was once a political choice in response to previous tra-
ditions or loyalties is now the mainstream in contemporary dance. As a result,
the term independent artist today encapsulates the majority of contempo-
rary dance artists, who typically complete tertiary dance training before en-
tering a competitive market where they either secure one of few contract
positions or, most commonly, join the industry as an independent artist. As
such, independent status often occurs immediately upon graduation and is a
default position: less a choice than a necessity.
524 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD
Participants
The study reported here focused on mid-career dance artists, all with between
10 and 16 years experience in the sector, and asked the questions: what are
the impact and direct affects of ongoing instability on the professional and
personal identity status of mid-career dance artists? What are the available
support mechanisms and personal strategies of artists in navigating this cru-
cial career phase? What does a real-life mid-career in dance look like? All of
the survey and interview participants were Australian-based dance artists
with an active dance practice. The first phase of research involved a survey of
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 525
71 dance artists from which the responses of nine mid-career dance artists (7
F, 2 M) were extracted. This was followed by in-depth case studies of three
dance artists in their mid-thirties: Maya and Steven, who are both identified
with pseudonyms, and Jo, the third participant and principal investigator. It
is the results of these case studies that form the basis of this paper. Jos inclu-
sion positioned her as both investigator and participant, giving her the op-
portunity to contribute in terms of both her lived experience as a dance
artist and from an academic standpoint. This methodology proved to be
highly effective in eliciting deep responses.
Procedure
RESULTS
Results of the study can be summarized in three key areas. First, the study
highlighted the need for professional and economic support to be continued
through mid-career. This is contrary to the traditional model of supporting
emerging artists on the basis that initial support will lead to a level of stabil-
ity. Second, the three case studies brought to the fore the complexities of
family responsibilities and between male and female parenting and artist
roles. Finally, the study shed some light on the quest for long-stability and the
impact of this on creative dance practice.
DISCUSSION
Dance is inherently unstable, and the instability comes with the dance career
rather than being chosen: its not necessarily that I would like to be in a high
526 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
risk business, it just happens to be [that] what I have chosen is most high
riskIm an artist (Maya). All three case studies are dance artists who juggle
a multitude of roles, often back-to-back or simultaneously. All accepted that
the protean nature of working as a dance artist is a reality from the outset.
The impacts of instability were seen as both positive and negative. Nega-
tive impacts relate to the difficult economic and ecological landscape of
working in dance. Card (2006, p. 13) offers that dance artists in the small to
medium sector are akin to a bunch of responsive scavengers who function in
a state of perpetual crisis though a life of irregular support and erratic em-
ployment histories. However, positive impacts of instability include the de-
velopment of a strong sense of self preservation and adaptability, alongside
the freedom to be creative, unorthodox, or innovative.
The instability of dance is a given, but in many ways it is manageable. The
difficulty seems to lie more in the disconnect between the instability of dance
and the stability of everyday life; most participants reported to being person-
ally and emotionally stable, in contrast to the unknown components under-
pinning the instability of their professional lives: working in arts in Australia
youre often able to work if other people allow you to work, rather than just
continuing with your work. So thats unstable, because its not up to you
(Maya). Multiple participants identified a need to justify their dance practice
to friends and family who have difficulty understanding, accepting, and
valuing irregular, often erratic work patterns. Such irregular project-based
work was noted to be problematic in terms of accessing community resources
such as childcare, which demands commitment to a regular schedule well in
advance.
Acceptance of and rebellion against instability appears to be a gradual
shift in attitude and priorities rather than a sudden decision point or age. For
two of the case studies, parenthood was also a factor in the reprioritizing. Yet
dance is not something that can be put down and picked up, either physically
or emotionally. Jo reported that project-based practice results in a deep sense
of loss during down times when she is financially unable to justify continu-
ing work without funds. The reality that physicality needs to be available on
demand, regardless of other priorities and activities, was for Jo becoming
both difficult and impractical. For both Jo and Steven the pursuit of stability
was coupled with an overt grief at the potential loss of the physical dance
practice. For Steven, this occurred when he made the decision to focus on
business interests to work smarter, not harder. Stevens failed attempt to
create a stable income in support of an integrated work/art/life philosophy,
when theory and practice didnt add up, resulted in a loss of physicality that
denied him opportunities to pursue his physical dance practice. His desire to
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 527
teach also diminished as it was his artistic practice that gave him the juice
to teach: a duel loss of both practice and other work potential related to prac-
tice. For Jo, the grief was a recurring state of pre-heartbreak during every
project, knowing an end is imminent.
Reflecting on his life in dance, Steven considered himself to have ca-
reered rather than had a career, implying a lack of control in his careering
through both dance and jobs outside of the arts. In identifying his motivation
for working, he was clear that I work for my family now, not for myself.
Because of time constraints and family responsibilities, Jo stressed the need
to generate income directly from artistic projects. For Maya, too, it was finan-
cially necessary to maintain a reliable income source. She separated her paid
work from her artistic practice, though she still expected to generate her
future income from her arts practice. This is in line with our previous re-
search, which found that the income source of artists may not align with their
professional identity.
In summary, this paper has provided insights in three key areas. First, the
real life snapshot of a dance artist in mid-career is complicated and unique to
each individual: Maya remained hopeful and reconciled, the same as al-
ways; Jo was building stable frames around her to house the continuation of
an ongoing practice in increasingly unstable circumstances, yet she did not
envisage a departure from dance; and Steven continued to research job op-
tions and was prepared to let go of dance in order to be the provider,
though he was still deeply engaged in an ongoing inquiry of his artistic prac-
tice. All but one of the participants expressed a deeply felt need to stay con-
nected to their own physical/emotional constitution, and grief when this need
was not met. Secondly, the case studies, with their different stages and rela-
tionships with dance, all acted as reflexive balancers of the unstable. Possibly
in direct response to the inherent instability in the dance environment, they
actively sought to cultivate sustainability in their work, family, and lives, and
all desired a home base in which to locate and situate the chaos (Jo). The
role of parenthood here arose as a major factor in slowing the artistic no-
madism. Finally, the results reiterate the vital need for support once dance
artists have moved beyond the emerging phase. Ironically, though mid-ca-
reer is contemporaneous with the period at which many dance artists begin to
enjoy an established practice and senior artist status, the factors of mid-
career deeply challenge the equilibrium. The need for support is evidenced by
the 107% increase in applications for the Scope for Artists program, with over
half of participants currently in their thirties. Further funding schemes, such
as the new mid-career dance fellowship in Western Australia will allow artists
to benefit from the available career development assistance and continue to
528 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
We would like to acknowledge the time so generously given to this project by the dance
artists who shared their experiences and expertise.
References
Bennett D. (2008). Understanding the Classical Music Profession. Aldershot, UK: Ash-
gate.
Bennett D. (2009). Dance careers: Beyond performance to the real world of work. Jour-
nal of Dance Education, 9, pp. 27-34.
Card. A. (2006). Body for Hire? The State of Dance in Australia (Platform papers No.
8). New South Wales, Australia: Currency House Inc.
Flick E. (2002). An Introduction to Qualitative Research. London: Sage Publications.
Levine M. (2005). Beyond Performance. The Hague, The Netherlands: International
Organization for the Transition of Professional Dancers (available from www.iotpd.
org/beyond_performance.pdf).
Patton M. Q. (1990). Qualitative Evaluation and Research Methods. Newbury Park,
California, USA: Sage Publications.
Pollitt J. (2001). Accumulated Experience in Live Improvised Dance Performance.
Unpublished masters thesis, Edith Cowan University.
Thematic session:
Physicality of performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
paring for performance (Chaffin and Logan 2006). An important early facet
of an experts approach to preparing a musical work for performance is to
form an artistic image of the piece (Chaffin et al. 2003). This includes imag-
ined musical character and emotions to be realized as sound through move-
ment. Recent empirical evidence suggests notional audiationimagining or
hearing sounds purely from reading musical notation aloneto be a process
involving kinesthetic vocal and motor processes in expert musicians (Brodsky
et al. 2008). Hence, musical imagery and movement appear to be inextricably
linked in expert music practice. While cognitive strategies and musical im-
agery would be expected features in professional marimba players practice,
movement imagery is also vital in the Western classical art form.
Movement imagery is the mental rehearsal of visual and kinesthetic
properties of movements to enhance learning and performance of motor
skills (Gregg et al. 2007, p. 1). According to Guillot et al. (2004), visual im-
agery is self-visualization of movement whereas kinesthetic imagery requires
one to feel the movement, i.e. to perceive muscle contractions mentally (p.
190). Arvinen-Barrow et al. (2007) found elite athletes cognitive-specific
(sports skills) imagery ability was significantly higher than novices. Solo ma-
rimba performance involves overt and spatial movement where the performer
is not in tactile, or often direct visual, contact with their instrument; move-
ment imagery is anecdotally reported to be important in developing expertise.
An entire pedagogical approach to marimba playing, Ideo Kinetics (Stout
2001), aims to develop accurate movement patterns through kinesthetic
awareness and imagery. Therefore, professional marimba players might be
expected to possess greater movement imagery than student marimba play-
ers.
The aims of this study are twofold. Firstly, we offer preliminary docu-
mentation of professional and student solo marimba players practice strate-
gies, both physical and imagined. Qualitative data are expected to
demonstrate that where professional marimba players focus on cognitive
strategies and imagery, undergraduate student marimba players practice
focuses on physical or technical aspects. Secondly, since marimba playing is a
complex motor skill that develops over time, we investigate kinesthetic and
visual imagery ability of participants. It is predicted that professional ma-
rimba players self-report greater kinesthetic and visual imagery than under-
graduate student marimba players.
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 533
METHOD
Participants
RESULTS
whole piece through, I keep a log of the most challenging sections and focus
on those as separate exercises (N).
The student musicians reported regular practice habits (mean hours per
day=3.25, SD=0.96; mean days per week=6.00, SD=0.96). As expected, their
practice focused on physical and technical elements: Warm-ups, technique,
repertoire, mess around (D), Learn the notes, work with a metronome,
practice fast changes (J). Strategies for learning a new piece involved break-
ing it into manageable chunks: Separate the piece into chunks, and learn
each chunk until I finish the piece (S), Divide the number of measures by
the number of days (e.g. until performance); work the sections each day
until the piece is done (D). Attention was also directed toward locating and
working on the most difficult sections: I read through the entire piece at first
to get a feel for itfind the difficult spots and try to work on them first (S),
Practice black sections slowly (JS). Musical interpretation was briefly
mentioned: After I have learned all the notes and listened to a recording(s)
of the piece, I put character to it (S).
Professional musicians self-reported imagery as part of their usual prac-
tice: Kinesthetic (100%), e.g. You have to remember the movementsto play
smoothly and accurately (JG), Slow, accurate practicehelps to develop
kinesthetic memory of the notes (G); Visual (100%), e.g. You cant hit what
you dont see (JG), Develop a visual image of the printed page (G); Audi-
tory (100%), e.g. You cant shape musically what you cant hear (JG); Emo-
tional/character (100%), e.g. Projectmy relationship with the piece (JG).
The student musicians self-reported imagery less often: Kinesthetic
(50%), e.g. To train muscles for different phrasing or difficult pas-
sagesdifferent intervals using kinesthetic imagery (J); Visual (75%), e.g.
The instrument, the audience, the performance settingvisual imagery of
the keyboard (J); Auditory (50%), e.g. sometimes I try to sound like a singer
or violin; recently, I started to just sit somewhere and play my tune in my
head (J), Decide on the auditory response I want the instrument to have
(JS); Emotional/character (75%), e.g. It helps to make a story or a scene out
of the music; depending on what emotions I have while I practice, I use
them and turn them into emotions that will hopefully bring out the musicality
of the piece (S), Imagery of particular experiences Ive had which inform my
approach (J).
Mann-Whitney U tests were conducted on the kinesthetic and visual im-
agery scales separately, with musical expertise as the grouping variable. As
predicted, the professional marimba players kinesthetic imagery (Md=26.6,
n=4) was significantly greater than the undergraduate student (Md=20.5,
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 535
30
25
20
Imagery score
15 Pr ofession a l
Stu den t
10
0
Kin a est h et ic im a g er y V isu a l im a g er y
Figure 1. Median imagery scores for the professional and student marimba players on
the kinesthetic and visual imagery subscales of the MIQ-R.
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
A University of Western Sydney postgraduate award to the first author supported this
research. Thanks are extended to the faculty and students of the Zeltsman Marimba
Festival 2004 who participated in this study and, in particular, to Nancy Zeltsman for
facilitating the research.
References
Arvinen-Barrow M., Weigand D. A., Thomas S. et al. (2007). Elite and novice athletes'
imagery use in open and closed sports. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19,
pp. 93-104.
Barry N. H. and Hallam S. (2002). Practice. In R. Parncutt and G. E. McPherson (eds.),
The Science and Psychology of Music Performance. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Brodsky W., Kessler Y., Rubinstein B. et al. (2008). The mental representation of music
notation: Notational audiation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Per-
ception and Performance, 34, pp. 427-445.
Chaffin R., Imreh G., Lemieux A. F., and Chen C. (2003). Seeing the big picture: Piano
practice as expert problem solving. Music Perception, 20, pp. 465-490.
Chaffin R. and Logan T. (2006). Practicing perfection: How concert soloists prepare for
performance. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, pp. 113-130.
Gregg M., Hall C., and Butler A. (2007). The MIQ-RS: A suitable option for examining
movement imagery ability. Evidence-based Complementary and Alternative Medi-
cine, 10.1093/ecam/nem170, pp. 1-9.
Guillot A., Collet C., and Dittmar A. (2004). Relationship between visual and kinesthetic
imagery, field dependence-independence, and complex motor skills. Journal of
Psychophysiology, 18, pp. 190-198.
Hall C. R. and Martin K. A. (1997). Measuring movement imagery abilities: A revision of
the Movement Imagery Questionnaire. Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, pp. 143-154.
Hallam S. (2001). The development of expertise in young musicians: Strategy use,
knowledge acquisition and individual diversity. Music Education Research, 3, pp.
7-23.
Stout G. (2001). Ideo Kinetics. Ithaca, New York, USA: G and C Music.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
There are many factors that contribute to the occurrence of injuries: fa-
tigue, excessive exercise/overtraining, inappropriate warm-up and cool-
down, continuing to work when injured, deficient physical condition, hypo- or
hypermobility, postural problems, alignment deficits, inadequate en-dehors,
poor nutrition, disordered eating, menstrual irregularity, osteoporosis, psy-
chological factors (sleeping problems, anxiety, depression), and factors re-
lated to equipment use (point shoes, floor type, wardrobe).
It seems to be consensual that injury happens when some of these factors
occur together, with it being impossible to consider the injury occurrence the
result of just one factor. However the training factors have a great contribu-
tion: 90% of the injuries happen when the dancer is fatigued (Liederbach
1997). Used to training over and over, it is common for dancers to hide the
injuries and to keep working. Hamilton (1998) was responsible for an inquiry
that collected the following results: 66% of dancers had continued to work
with a chronic injury, 52% with tendinitis, and 27% with stress fracture and
with arthritis.
One of the central problems of training consists of the specific choice of
the balance between work and rest. To find the adequate amount of load and
the adequate duration of recovery is important to improve performance and
to prevent injuries. An insufficient load does not improve performance. On
the other hand, loads too high or with a lack of time to recover are negative
for health and performance maintains or decreases.
The aim of the study is to quantify the energy expenditure of high level
performance in dance in order to characterize the dance task requirements
and energetic costs in different time periods, and also to understand the bal-
ance between load and recovery.
METHOD
Participants
A 31 year old principal ballet male dancer was studied for eight days. For this
study, the subject wore a multiple physiological sensor (SenseWear Pro Arm-
band) during a week of performing Copplia.
Materials
The arm band provided the following measures: total energy expenditure and
active energy expenditure (at 3.0 METS or higher), physical activity duration,
intensity (sedentary, moderate, vigorous, and very vigorous), and sleep dura-
tion. All the stage performances, individual and company rehearsals, and
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 539
Procedure
The subject wore the SenseWear Pro Armband on the right arm, and energy
expenditure was estimated using proprietary equations developed by the
manufacturer. The measurement periods were continuous for several days
(until 14 days). Time ranges were selected according to the desired analyses
work periods: week and day, including class, rehearsal, and performance. The
collected data were analyzed in research software. The classes, rehearsals,
and performances were filmed and recorded in digital base for posterior
analysis with the observational system.
Estimated energy expenditure from the SenseWear Pro Armband is
broadly validated in the literature, namely with standard indirect calorimetry.
The sensewear data should be accompanied with information obtained
through the completion of a simple diary. The different data were integrated
over time. The equipment proved to be reliable and easy to use for all levels of
dancers, and even during performance. A brief instruction period to the sub-
jects is enough to achieve reliable results.
For the observation of the motor actions, the system of observation of
motor behavior in dance created and developed by Rodrigues (1992, 1999)
was utilized in this study. Through the systematic observation of motor be-
havior in dance we have identified nine motor actions: steps, jumps, turns,
gestures, falls, displacements, postures, balances, and contacts. The list of
motor actions proposed is limited and their definition based on two criteria:
the support (link to the ground) and the group of specific features of each
action. The observational system was validated in 1999.
RESULTS
Energy expenditure
On average the total energy expenditure was of 2933 Kcal, oscillating between
2300 on days-off and 3500 on performance days. During the eight days ob-
served, the dancer had three performances of 2 hours and 20 mins (including
intervals) and lost on average 600 Kcal in each one. For 50 mins he was in
sedentary activity and about 90 mins in physical activity. The physical activity
was distributed in 74 mins of moderate (3.0-6.0 METS), 14 mins of vigorous
540 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
(6.0-9.0 METS), and 3 mins in very vigorous (9.0 METS and higher). The
sleep period was on average 8 hours.
The analysis of movement allows us to identify the relation between en-
ergy expenditure and density of movement: there is an increase of energy
expenditure when the number of actions per min. also increases and mainly
when those actions are moveable (jumps, displacements, and steps). How-
ever, some differences in the content are found between the classes (individ-
ual or collective) and the specificities of performance because classes are
more like a routine without a direct relationship with the different require-
ments of choreographies.
Another characteristic that is important is the difference between what
the subject does in his individual trainings before the performance and what
he does during the company class. The expenditure (about 400 Kcal) is higher
during his individual trainings of 1 hour and 15 mins (on average 4.0 METS)
than during group classes (270 Kcal, 3.0 METS) with the same time. This last
activity presents a sedentary and moderate activity, while the first situation
has periods of vigorous activity like in the performance.
Work-to-rest ratio
One of the most interesting observations in this study concerns the compari-
son of the collective training with individual training. Typically, on perform-
ance day the company (with about 70 dancers) usually does a class similar to
the other days. But on performance days our subject (principal dancer) holds
his individual preparation/training just immediately before performance,
instead of the collective class that occurs two hours before performance. What
are the differences between these two classes?
In terms of content the differences are not much (at the end of the indi-
vidual training there is some specific work/preparation with passages of the
choreography, but only in the last 5 mins), but differences arise at the level of
work-to-rest ratio: for a similar duration of 73 and 71 mins, in the collective
class there was a total of 28-45 mins from work to rest, while in the individual
training 32-39 mins of work to rest. The average time of the barre exercises is,
respectively, 90 and 70 s, with similar values (33 s, 32 s) of rest.
In the centre the differences increase, with values of work of 28 s and 30
s, but with periods of rest significantly different from 180 s and 45 s, respec-
tively. The organization of the exercises in small groups can explain this dif-
ference; in a class where there are on average 40 dancers, the rest time of
each dancer will at least triple. The problem is that the physiological effects
are quite differente.g. if one waits 3 mins to work about 30 s, or if one just
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 541
DISCUSSION
The results shows that physical activity is mainly moderate (91%): on average,
the eight days observed presented 3 daily hours of moderate physical activity,
about 15 mins of vigorous, and 2 mins of very vigorous activity. The complex-
ity of the tasks, which demands high coordination, may justify these results.
Another aspect that we must enhance is the importance of individual
training for success. In the individual training periods, we found a work-to-
rest-ratio more similar to performance than in the group class company.
The underlying question that arises in this study (in our line of research)
is how to assist the performers to reach the peak level of performance at the
right moment? Planning and monitoring are key words to achieve these ob-
jectives. If we continue to train only based on tradition, with a routine daily
lesson for all the elements of the company, probably the same high rate of
injuries will continue and the individuals potential is not developed: at the
highest professional level, a lesson for all is a lesson for anyone. We need to
individualize the training, planning well to find a balance between work peri-
ods and rest periods and to avoid overuse injuries.
References
Matthew Wyon, School of Sport, Performing Arts, and Leisure, University of Wolver-
hampton, Gorway Road, Walsall, West Midlands WS1 3BD, UK; Email: m.wyon@
wlv.ac.uk
Thematic session:
Defining performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
But these processes take place by the performers as well, who, consciously
or not, more or less trained and more or less (historically) informed, end up
projecting a personal interpretative style; and, we suspect, they take place
also by composers, who, again, consciously or not, more or less trained and
with more or less erudition, end up producing a score that is inevitably a pale
reflection of their creationthat is, of the performance that, intimately, they
were imagining when composing. We believe that personal meanings play a
central role in the creative process, unifying all the dimensions of the musical
work and determining decisively the actions and events of a musical perform-
ance, be it imagined or actually performed. These meanings guide the per-
formers intentionality in the unifying process of the musical creation and
embodiment.
However, it is the embodied nature of music that is perhaps most relevant
to musical understanding as it will be explored in this article. Based on the
theory of enactive cognition (cf. Gibbs 2006, Thompson 2001), Martinez
(2008) argued that structural metaphors might, to a certain extent, model
music experiences and demonstrated how listeners use the metaphorical
component of music cognition to experience the underlying structure of tonal
music. For professional performers, music is inseparable from a communica-
tive dramatic attitude, their account of the music they are about to play is
always (or, at least, hopefully) an aesthetically oriented narration of dra-
matic action (Maus 1997, p. 129). After having collected the available infor-
mationbe it anecdotal, historical, or analyticperformers create a kind of
synthesis, a personal amalgam that they apply to the music material. This
synthesis is, thus, much more an open, personal, subjective, symbolic amal-
gam, emotionally motivated to a large extent, than a reductive, closed, rigor-
ous, conceptual definition, deductively constructed. This synthesis has indeed
to be concentrated, like an embryo that develops adapting itself to the envi-
ronment, in order to pragmatically fulfill its function of (e)motionally ex-
ploring the musical material. Putting aside the issues concerning the specific
instrumental techniques of each musical genre or style, which are in fact just
means of expression, performers hermeneutic approach to scores, in other
words, their subjective, symbolically charged, embryonic amalgam has a
clear purpose: to create a communicative, convincing narrative of musical
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 549
gestures: performers tell a story and the concert begins with Once upon a
time (interview with Patrick Gallois, in Correia 2003). When applying
their subjective, symbolically charged, embryonic amalgams to explore
(e)motionally the musical pieces, performers are exploring figures, which are
constellations of embodied symbolic meanings, and invoking inevitably
analogies, metaphors, or, simply, memories. This implies that it is the realm
of tacit knowledge that is aimed in this (e)motional exploration, as if the fig-
ures would invoke the actual presence of the (e)motional memories and the
performers would re-enact them, recuperating their unconscious dimensions
to a large extent and making in this way their interpretations more authentic
and convincing. But, if composition is as a mode of performing, composers,
when composing, should be applying a subjective, symbolically charged,
embryonic amalgam as well, actively imagining a performance which would
guide their compositional decisions.
To find out more about this, and having in mind that one may express
through different sense modalities (aurally or visually) the same gestural dy-
namics and shaping, we designed the experiment we report here. It is based,
thus, on the application of the concept of intermodality (cf. Johnson 1987,
Hatten 1999), which will enable us to compare the performers and the com-
posers personal (embodied) meanings for the same piece. The simple verbal
explanation of these meanings would utterly miss the point: their embodi-
ment. Thus, we had to create the conditions under which these personal
meanings could be manifested while keeping their embodiment. In order to
do that, we asked both performer and composer to express in visual terms
(directing videos for two excerpts of the piece) the same subjective, symboli-
cally charged, embryonic amalgam they had created for the musical work.
They were asked to translate their personal (embodied) meaningswhat-
ever they intimately and introspectively have imagined for each musical ex-
cerptinto visible scenes, actions, and physical gestures; our main objective
being to understand if the role that embodied meanings play in composition
is similar to the role they play in performance. If they play a similar role, then
composition should be considered a mode of performing.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Two excerpts with about 50 seconds duration taken from the work Solos III
for flute by Sara Carvalho (CD Nova Msica para Flauta 1999, ed. Numrica,
No. 1095) were chosen as the musical material upon which the video clips
were created. The first excerpt (M1) corresponds to the score measures from 1
to 16; the second excerpt (M2) from measure 123 to 137. For the video re-
cordings, an amateur digital camera was used (Samsung S760-7.2 mega pix-
els).
Procedure
Sara Carvalho wrote the script and directed and edited two of the video clips
expressing the view of the composer (Cv1, Cv2). She was asked to express in
this way the meanings that each of the excerpts had for her when composing
the piece Solos III. The performer was asked to do the same for the same two
excerpts, reporting himself on the time when he was preparing to perform
this piece (Pv1, Pv2). In all videos, the actors were asked to do exactly what
the directors required, neutralizing as much as possible any kind of interfer-
ence. The four videos are available on the internet: www.youtube.com, search
term jorgesalgadocorreia.
RESULTS
The videos of the composer (Cv1, Cv2) tell the story of a man who sees and
meets two women in two different scenarios; in Cv1, the woman is lying in the
sun on a beach, and in Cv2, the meeting happens in a coffee shop. In her
script the composer explained that, in her narrative for this piece, each sec-
tion corresponds to a meeting of the same man with different women. The
video of the performer for the first (Pv1) excerpt portrays the anxiety of a
woman looking for something but trying not to be seen by the boy who hap-
pened to be in the same room. In the second video of the performer (Pv2), the
camera catches a domestic discussion between a man and a woman.
DISCUSSION
Beginning with what the two pairs of videos have in common, the main thing
to highlight seems to be the profound sense of narrative that all of the videos
clearly display. Both composer and performer thought and worked in terms of
constructing a narrative, of telling a story. The main structural points of the
musicbeginning with the presentation of a conflict or an opposition (in M1
the opposition between the pizzicatos and the normal notes, in M2 the oppo-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 551
sition or dialog between low and high registers); growing tension through
repetition (in M1 repetition and some degree of stretto, in M2 repetition and
ascendant melodic movement); and conclusion (in M1 resolution of the oppo-
sition, confluence, in M2 definitive separation of the opposites, divergence)
are clearly reproduced in the videos of both composer and performer. Of
course, they tell different stories, but it is remarkable how the important
structural features are present in both versions. For example, Cv1 and Pv1
both express in the end a sense of success, while Cv2 and Pv2 both express
the same feeling of irreparable separation; also the points of highest tension
in the videos are the same, corresponding to measure 8 in M1 and measure
135 in M2. It seems, thus, that embodied imagination assisted composer and
performer in mapping the same expressive process between different experi-
ential domains, and more importantly, this intermodal or transmodal opera-
tion was only possible because the structural features emerged from a system
of spatial-temporal organization elaborated upon patterns of image schemata
(cf. Johnson 1987).
It is this fundamental connection to a bodily ground that makes personal
meanings so decisive in musical meaning production and communication.
But there was a remarkable difference as well. The two videos of the com-
poser have both the characteristic of suggesting much more psychological
action than physical: the viewer is invited to imagine what the characters are
feeling and the musical gestures seem to portray much more these emotional
experiences than the physical actions of the actors. There are a few exceptions
where there is obvious coordination between the musical events and the visi-
ble actions in the videos of the composer: at the beginning and the end of Cv1,
measure 8 in Cv1 where some agitation in the music is expressed through the
movements of the actress, in Cv2 measure 134, and at the end where again
and respectively the actor and the actress seem to physically express, say,
more literally, the correspondent musical gesture. The physical actions in the
two videos of the performer are much more connected with the musical
events: the characters move with the music, so to speak, starting and stopping
in synchrony with the musical phrases, expressing with movement and physi-
cal gestures the relations of tension and relaxation of the music. Only in Pv2
there are two occasions (curiously, both have sustained multiphonics) where
the viewer has no physical action to interpret and is, thus, implicitly, invited
to imagine what emotions the characters are experiencing; the first is in
measure 128, where the man sits impotent and leans with his elbow on the
arm of the sofa, and the second is at the end, measure 137, where the man
walks devastated away from the woman and toward the window. To our un-
derstanding, these two different approaches result directly from the specific
552 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Acknowledgments
References
Seeking excellence in
danced postgraduate degrees
Maggi Phillips
METHOD
Participants
RESULTS
Responses indicated that the primary motivation for doctoral degrees is the
validation of dance knowledge, whether performative or choreographic,
pedagogical or therapeutic. Given the context, this emphasis on intellectuali-
zation was unsurprising and, yet, arguably slightly at odds with an aesthetic
form often propelled by the rebellious forces of its contemporary manifesta-
tions. Ironically, when faced with delineating attributes that characterize
doctorateness, the range of participants expressed a slightly divergent, though
not incompatible, view emphasizing the role of engagement or the wow
factor which ostensibly appears more consistent with traditional perceptions
of dancing than a reliance on its intellectualism.
Indeed, engagement emerges as a key attribute of evaluation when deal-
ing with ideas of excellence where the modewhether dance, writing, or
556 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Generating dynamism
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
I wish to acknowledge my colleagues, Cheryl Stock and Kim Vincs, the Australian
Learning and Teaching Council, Ausdance, and the participants, who willingly contrib-
uted their ideas about the difficult issues of examination and excellence.
Maggi Phillips, Western Australian Academy of Performing Arts, Edith Cowan Univer-
sity, 2 Bradford Street, Mt Lawley, Western Australia 6050, Australia; Email: maggi.
phillips@ecu.edu.au
References
Wimsatt and Beardsleys essay The Intentional Fallacy (1946), which criti-
cized the relevance of authorial intention as a criterion for critical evaluation,
defended the text, and not the author, as the focal point for interpretation.
This approach led to the announcement of the death of the author by
Barthes (1994), a position that was subsequently reformulated by Foucault
(1992).
In music, the debate has mostly been focused on the relevance of com-
posers intentions for interpretation. Intention, as discussed by Dipert or
Butt, is presented as a multi-level/multi-sided concept, following a similar
approach by some literary-studies theorists (namely Hancher 1972). The fo-
cus is mostly placed on the composer or the musical text/score, and its sig-
nificance for the performers own range of interpretative choices has been
neglected.
This neglect derives from post-romantic views of the work concept, views
that emphasize the primacy of author and text over the contingencies and
variability normally assigned to performance. Burke points out that the cru-
cial historical change in conceptions of authorship did not occur in the theo-
retical upheaval of the last thirty years but with the romantic revolution and
the eighteenth-century philosophical and aesthetic discourses upon which it
grew (Burke 1995, p. xix). This statement could also be applied to changes in
the notion of musical work during the same period, which led to differenti-
ated views of the relative relevance of composing/performing/listening as
creative actions. The intentional fallacy criticized by Wimsatt and Beardsley
located the merits of a literary work in the correct interpretation of the au-
thors intentions. The tension generated by the pressure of playing under the
ideal of Werktreue (implying fidelity to the works essence), mentioned by
Goehr (1992), is a parallel situation, placing constraints and restrictions upon
performers on the basis of authorial intention.
This article intends to demonstrate: (1) that the concept of authors inten-
tion, which is traditionally viewed by performers as a mandatory interpreta-
tive framework, can also be addressed from the performers perspective and
(2) that a multi-level appraisal of intention can be applied to collaborative
procedures in order to promote a successful performance.
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
This research was developed in the context of a case study documenting the
collaboration between a composer and a performer, which involved the pre-
mire of a new composition for the pianoforte. The concepts of authorship
and intention provided a theoretical framework for the analysis of the col-
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 561
Composer/performer/work
The rise of the concept of the autonomous musical work and the associated
concept of Werktreue led to the redefinition of the relation composer/per-
former and the gradual dissociation of these roles, most noticeably from the
beginning of the nineteenth century. Goehr mentions that Liszt spent a con-
siderable part of his musical life developing two distinct forms of perform-
ance, first, performances committed to faithful renditions of his works,
second, virtuoso performance devoted to the art of extemporization and the
show of impressive performance technique (Goehr 2002, pp. 232-233).
Liszts option for two different types of performance is representative of the
establishment of a viewpoint that distinguished composers and performers
activity. The text became paramount in this new outlook.
The relevance of intention has led to an ongoing debate in the realm of liter-
ary studies, linked to the notion of authorship and the establishment of criti-
cal parameters. This research extends this theoretical stance to music,
drawing a parallel between the dichotomies author/reader and com-
poser/performer. This parallel is not fortuitous: as pointed out by Kivy, there
is an almost universal tendency since at least the eighteenth century to try to
understand absolute music on a linguistic model of one kind or another
(Kivy 1995, p. 284).
The debate surrounding the notion of authorship in the late 1960s chal-
lenged an author-centered appraisal of literary texts. Barthes announced the
death of the author in his much-debated essay, highlighting the multiplicity
of voices implied in a text: there is one place where this multiplicity reas-
sembles, and this place is not the author, as hitherto said, its the reader
(Barthes 1994, p. 595). Foucault, on the other hand, does not dismiss the in-
tellectual implications of the author figure: a discourse associated to an au-
thors nameis not a fleeting and momentary discourse, immediately
consumed, but rather a discourse that is perceived in a certain manner and
given, within a given culture, a certain status (Foucault 1992, p. 45). Fou-
cault, however, dissociates author and writer, characterizing the author as a
function that can acknowledge the existence of a multiple I.
562 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Can be analyzed into three kinds: his intention to make a literary work of
a certain sort (programmatic intention); his intention to be (under-
stood as) acting in a certain way (active intention); and his intention to
cause something or other to happen beyond the mere understanding of
his meaning (final intention).
The hierarchical/categorical approach has also been the preferred model re-
garding theories on musical intention.
The concept of intention in musical interpretation has been criticized for
its prescriptive stance, often based on textual and analytical constraints that
limit the performers freedom of expression. As Taruskin ironically contends,
composers do not usually have intentions such as we would like to ascertain,
andthe need obliquely to gain the composers approval for what we do be-
speaks a failure of nerve (Taruskin 1995, p. 98). Historically-informed per-
formance, nevertheless, is undeniably connected to the maintenance of
intention as a working concept, and represents an innovative trend in musical
performance that cannot be dismissed solely on the grounds of poor herme-
neutical grounding.
Part of the problem regarding the adaptation of intentional theory to mu-
sic may lie on the exclusive focus on the composers perspective, which con-
trasts with the above-mentioned trends in literary theory that question the
conventional notion of authorship and emphasize the importance of multiple
perspectives. Diperts description of intentional levels, and its re-evaluation
by Butt (2002), are, as acknowledged by Butt, unsatisfactory as theoretical
tools, as the distinction between levels of intention, despite the fact that they
are established with the composer in mind, is in fact partly or mainly depend-
ent on performers choices. Dipert, while mentioning the limited relevance of
composers intentions for performance, proposes a 3-levelled hierarchy. Butt
rejects the hierarchical outlook, and proposes a division between active in-
tentiona composers specific decisions concerning such matters as instru-
mentation, tempo, dynamic, and passive intentionthose factors over
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 563
work sessions, both composer and performer worked on their own vision
of the piece for further discussion.
Final intention was shared by composer and performer, who centered
their attention in details that differed in some instances: the composer
mostly focused on structural coherence and impact, whereas the per-
former was more focused on aspects such as effective dynamic contrasts,
expressivity, and even articulation.
IMPLICATIONS
References
Barthes R. (1994). La mort de lauteur. In R. Barthes (ed.), uvres Compltes (pp. 491-
495). Paris: Seuil. (Originally published in 1968.)
Burke S. (1992). The Death and Return of the Author. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh Uni-
versity Press.
Burke S. (1995). Authorship: From Plato to the postmodern. Edinburgh, UK: Edin-
burgh University Press.
Butt J. (2002). Playing with History. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dipert R. R. (1980). The composers intentions: An examination of their relevance for
performance. The Musical Quarterly, 66, pp. 205-218.
Foucault M. (1992). O Que um Autor? Lisbon: Vega. (Originally published in 1969.)
Goehr L. (2002). The Imaginary Museum of Musical Works. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Hancher M. (1972). Three kinds of intention. Modern Language Notes, 87, pp. 827-851.
Kivy P. (1995). Authenticities. Ithaca, New York, USA: Cornell University Press.
Taruskin R. (1995). Text and Act. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Thematic session:
Psychology of performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Laura A. Stambaugh
Musicians and music educators are always seeking to better understand fac-
tors contributing to efficient and effective practice. Motor learning research,
which studies how people acquire and refine movements over time, offers a
potentially valuable framework for exploring the challenges of instrumental
performance. To the extent that playing a musical instrument is a motor task,
motor learning theories and methods may be relevant to the study of music
practice.
568 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
METHOD: EXPERIMENT I
Participants
Materials
The stimuli (e.g. see Figure 1) were composed by the researcher to meet the
following constraints: limited performance range, task brevity, transposable
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 569
to two other keys to control for any aural confounds, inclusion of a novel pitch
to prevent ceiling effects, and majority tonality. There were two screening
tasks, three practice/retention tasks, and two transfer tasks.
Procedure
Students began by playing the screening tasks, and this accuracy score was
used as a covariate. The practice sessions occurred in group settings led by
the researcher. Microphones were attached to students clarinets during all
playing. Cubase multi-track software enabled individual recordings to be
made of each students playing while in the group setting.
Students within schools were randomly assigned to either the blocked or
random practice condition, attending three practice days and one delayed
testing day. Students were instructed to play as accurately and quickly as
possible. In the blocked group, students played 18 trials of one task on each
day. Random group students played 6 trials of each of the 3 tasks in a random
order. At retention testing, students within groups were further divided for
testing in either a blocked or random order (blocked practice-blocked reten-
tion, blocked practice-random retention). After performing 3 retention tri-
als of each task and 3 trials of the transfer tasks, students completed a brief
questionnaire about their attitude toward their practice condition.
Accuracy and speed scores were determined for each of the 3,554 per-
formance trials. Accuracy was scored through repeated listening by the re-
searcher using a point-deduction system, and speed was measured in Cubase
as the time lapsed from the onset of the first pitch to the onset of the last
pitch in each trial. Temporal evenness was scored for the final three practice
trials (acquisition), retention, and transfer trials (1,286) using an equation
determining the average interonset interval in relation to trial duration:
|IOIx-IOIm|=IOI, IOI/IOI.
RESULTS: EXPERIMENT I
Figure 2 presents means and standard deviations of speed and accuracy aver-
aged for every 3 practice trials. After 18 practice trials, there were no signifi-
cant differences between groups for accuracy, speed, or temporal evenness
(p>0.02). A repeated measures ANCOVA for speed found a main effect for
trial (F5,34=5.05, p=0.001, 2=0.11), indicating both groups did improve speed
570 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 2. Accuracy and speed mean scores averaged across every three trials.
during the practice trials. At 24-hour delayed retention, the random group
performed significantly faster than the blocked group (F1,38=24.95, p<0.001,
2=0.92), and pairwise comparisons indicated the blocked group played sig-
nificantly slower at retention than at acquisition (p<0.001). There were no
significant differences for accuracy or evenness at retention, and no signifi-
cant differences between groups on transfer tasks. When students within
practice groups were tested in blocked or random orders at retention, there
were no significant within-practice group differences for accuracy, speed, or
evenness. Regarding attitude toward practice, both groups exhibited a similar
(t40=-1.10, p>0.28) positive response.
METHOD: EXPERIMENT II
Participants
Materials
The researcher composed the stimuli (e.g. see Figure 3) to be a major tonality,
short task, and difficult enough to prevent a ceiling effect.
Procedure
blocked practice order was 9 trials of each task on both days in a blocked or-
der, to control for consolidation effects.
RESULTS: EXPERIMENT II
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
Experiment I is based on the authors doctoral thesis, Effects of Blocked and Random
Practice Schedules on Performance by Beginning Wind Players, University of Wash-
ington, adviser Steven M. Demorest.
572 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
References
criticized for lacking objectivity. In addition, research would suggest that im-
agery ability is comprised of three distinct features: vividness, controllability,
and accuracy of reference (Denis 1991).
One potential alternative to self-report questionnaires employs temporal
comparisons between live and mental performances, also termed mental
chronometry, as employed by Repp (1999a, 1999b) and Wllner and Willia-
mon (2007). These researchers proposed that by removing sensory feedback
during performance, insight could be gained into the strength and content of
musicians mental images. The use of chronometric comparisons between live
and mental performances is also based upon work from human movement
sciences which suggests that imagery processes are mediated by neuronal
mechanisms similar to those used in perception (Mellet et al. 1998).
While previous research would indicate that musicians are capable of
performing works with similar timing profiles across repeated performances,
results derived from studies employing mental chronometry caution that
there is variability among musicians ability to perform such a task, particu-
larly in the absence of kinaesthetic feedback (c.f. Repp 1999a, 1999b, Wllner
and Williamon 2007). Due to these varying results, musicians ability to per-
form this type of chronometric task successfully is still somewhat unclear, as
is the range of factors that may help or hinder their ability to do so.
Given the current state of understanding surrounding musicians imagery
use and abilities, this study sought to investigate the efficacy of a context-
specific and objective method for assessing musicians imagery abilities. In
particular, timing profiles of live and mental performances were compared to
determine if such an approach could be a valid indicator of imagery ability
and vividness. Of interest was how performance on this type of task may re-
late to scores obtained on standard imagery use and vividness measures.
METHOD
Participants
Materials
Procedure
ity between the IOIs of the two performances within each of the two condi-
tions was assessed using one-tailed Pearson correlations. One-tailed Pearson
correlations were next used to assess the similarity of the IOIs between the
two conditions. All correlations were normalized by conversion to Z-scores
for use in further analyses with the imagery use and vividness measures.
RESULTS
When asked to identify the types of mental strategies or activities that the
participants employed as part of their regular practice activities, imagery was
identified most often (n=20 of 32). Of those who cited regularly employing
imagery, they reported using it for an average of 25.95 minutes per day
(SD=28.54), that they had been using it for 6.48 years (SD=5.87), and rated
their effectiveness with imagery at 4.80 out of 7 (SD=1.28). Within this, the
participants reported singing or hearing their music in their minds, memo-
rizing music away from their instruments, and playing on a surface other than
their instrument (or finger practice).
For the Betts QMI, the participants obtained a total mean score of 89.66
(SD=26.09). The potential range for this questionnaire is 35-245, with a
lower score indicating greater imagery vividness. The participants ability to
imagine sounds (auditory sub-component) and movements (kinaesthetic sub-
component) were the senses achieving the greatest vividness on this measure,
while the participants ability to imagine smells (olfactory sub-component)
achieved the least amount of vividness.
When considering the participants performance on the mental chrono-
metry task, all 32 participants achieved a significant correlation between the
IOIs of the two live performances. When examining the mental performance
condition, however, only 17 participants achieved a significant correlation.
Comparing the IOIs between the two conditions, 22 of the 32 participants
yielded a significant correlation between the two conditions.
Comparing the results from the different assessment measures (see Table
1), a significant positive correlation emerged between the amount of time per
day that the participants reported employing imagery and their ability to
perform consistently within the mental performance condition. A significant
positive correlation also emerged between the participants self-rated ability
to engage in imagery and their ability to give multiple temporally similar
performances within the live performance condition. A link between imagery
vividness and ability within the live performance condition was also found
with the Betts QMI. The normalised Z-scores from the live performance con-
dition achieved significant negative correlations with total scores from the
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 577
Table 1. Significant Pearson correlations between the Z-scores derived from the mental
chronometry task with the imagery use and vividness measures.
DISCUSSION
For 63% of the participants, imagery formed a significant part of the practice
activities. Within this, auditory and kinesthetic imagery were the senses for
which the participants reported the greatest amount of vividness. Within the
mental chronometry task, while all participants were able to give two per-
formances of comparable timing profiles within the live performance condi-
tion, some musicians were less able to do so with the removal of sensory
feedback in the mental performance condition, supporting results from pre-
vious investigations of this type. Based upon examination of the results from
the three assessment measures, the three scores derived from the mental
chronometry task appear to provide an indication of either imagery vividness,
controllability, or accuracy of representation, supporting the proposal that
imagery ability is comprised of three distinct features (Denis 1991).
Possessing high levels of vividness and control over their imagery should
be of great importance for musicians, and while previous research would sug-
gest that greater imagery ability facilitates enhanced performance quality
(McIntyre et al. 2002), this link within music needs to be explored more fully.
Having reliable methods for accurate assessment of imagery ability is central
within this. Lastly, these findings support recommendations for the use of a
mixed-methods design when assessing musicians imagery use and abilities,
578 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
with a mental chronometry task such as that employed here adding an ele-
ment of objectivity to these investigations.
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank David Burnand and Andrew Earis for their advice on methods for
generating timing profiles arising from the mental chronometry task.
Terry Clark, Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, Prince Consort
Road, London SW7 2BS, UK; Email: tclark@rcm.ac.uk
References
Emmanuel Bigand, Institut Universitaire de France , LEAD UMR 5022 CNRS, Ple
AAFEEsplanade Erasme, University of Burgundy, BP 26513 21065, Dijon Cedex,
France; Email: bigand@u-bourgogne.fr
Thematic session:
Perceiving performance
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Nicholas J. Bailey, Centre for Music Technology, Department of Electronics and Electri-
cal Engineering, University of Glasgow, Rankine Building, Oakfield Avenue, Glasgow
G12 8LT, UK; Email: n.j.bailey@elec.gla.ac.uk
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
METHOD
Participants
Nine highly trained pianists from the Royal Scottish Academy of Music and
Drama and the Universities of Glasgow and Edinburgh were recorded playing
the selected preludes. They were paid a one off sum of 25. Fifteen musically
knowledgeable audience judges were recruited by email from the University
of Glasgow and local orchestras. They were paid 6 per hour for participation.
Materials
The two test pieces performed were Chopin Preludes Op.28 No.7 in A major
and No.6 in B minor. Prelude 7 was chosen as a control piece across perform-
ers as it comprises rhythmically repeating 2-bar phrases. Prelude 6 is slightly
more complicated, as it contains expanded phrases and changes in rhythm;
the opening 2-bar phrase of each prelude is shown in Figure 1. Eighteen au-
dience-perspective recordings were used to generate 56 perceptual stimuli
comprising two practice performances by a tenth performer and presenta-
tions of each prelude by the nine performers in three modalities: visual only
(VO), audio only (AO), and audiovisual (AV). Presentation stimuli were ~40 s
each in duration for Prelude 7 and ~90 s for Prelude 6.
Procedure
Nine highly trained pianists performed the two preludes on a Roland RD-150
88-key weighted keyboard and were recorded through audio, MIDI, and mo-
tion capture. Passive markers were attached to the upper body and head via a
cap and jacket worn by all performers. Performers were required to perform
the preludes as they would in a normal concert setting. Performances were
also filmed from an audience perspective. The 56 audience perspective
performance stimuli were presented to 15 audience participants in AO, VO,
and AV modes across two sessions. Participants were initially presented with
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 587
Figure 1. Opening 2-bar phrase of Chopins Prelude in A major Op.28 No.7. (left) and
Chopins Prelude in B minor Op.28 No.6 (right).
the VO stimuli and asked to shape the phrasing structure of each performance
using a slider bar moving from left-right-left as the phrase was shaped. They
were then presented with the stimuli in all three modalities, and the task was
repeated. Stimuli were presented in randomized order to avoid order effects.
RESULTS
Performance Analysis
Estimations of tempo, dynamics, and motion were taken from the MIDI, au-
dio and motion streams, respectively. Performed notes were matched to the
score notes using performance markup language and matcher (described in
McGilvray 2009). Queries on note information were returned by the Pullinger
database (Pullinger 2009). The inter-onset intervals (IOIs), normalized to a
quarter note beat, were divided by 1/60 to give an estimation of beats per
minute. Dynamics were estimated by the RMS amplitude of the signal. 2D
motion curves were extracted by performing principal component analysis
(PCA) of all markers and adding the weighted components. Figure 2 shows
audio, MIDI, and video data plotted against phrase boundaries as they occur
in the audio. To allow direct comparison across performers, all data was time-
warped with respect to the occurrence of phrase boundaries in the audio.
Distances to the nearest troughs in dynamics and tempo curves were ex-
tracted for each phrase boundary. Distances from either troughs or peaks
were extracted for the motion curves depending on each performers move-
ment patterns. Two-way Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) showed significant
effects of both performer and phrase number on motion norm (F=3.56,
p<0.001 and F=6.63, p<0.01) and a significant effect of performer on dy-
namics (F=5.78, p<0.001) and of phrase on tempo (F=37.24, p<0.001). Other
effects were not significant. This suggests performers have a distinct motion
style varying between phrase boundaries and that they differ in their use of
diminuendo. While performers may not vary how they use ritardandi, this is
varied between phrases. Two-way ANOVAs for the first three phrases of Prel-
ude 6 also demonstrate a significant effect of performer on motion (F=12.85,
588 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
Figure 2. RMS amplitude (top), tempo (middle), and motion norm (bottom). Variance
weighted average of principal component scores of motion markers against phrase
boundaries (blue vertical arrows) for performances of Prelude 7 by performers 2 and 7.
Figure 3. Comparison of responses by all participants for all three presentation modes.
Troughs indicate perceived phrase boundaries, vertical lines indicate phrase transitions.
(See full color versions at www.performancescience.org.)
Perceptual analysis
Table 1. Three-way ANOVA for factors presentation mode, phrase, and performer.
Source SS df MS F p>F
Presentation mode 0.3374 2 0.1687 239.48 p<0.01
Phrase no. 10.6658 7 1.52369 2162.94 p<0.01
Performer 0.0426 8 0.00533 7.56 p<0.01
Mode x phrase 0.0464 14 0.00332 4.71 p<0.05
Mode x performer 0.0760 16 0.00475 6.74 p<0.01
Phrase x performer 0.0279 56 0.0005 0.71 0.9238
Error 0.0789 112 0.0007
Total 11.2751 215
DISCUSSION
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank Frank Pollick and Lukasz Piwek of the University of Glasgow Psychol-
ogy Department, and Stuart Pullinger and Douglas McGilvray at the Centre for Music
Technology for their support and access to equipment. This research is EPSRC funded.
References
Buck B., MacRitchie J., and Bailey N. (2009) Perceptual recognition of embodied musi-
cal structure. Paper presented at The Musical Body, University of London.
Davidson J. (1993) Visual perception of performance manner in movements of solo
musicians. Psychology of Music, 21, pp. 103-113.
MacRitchie J., Buck B., and Bailey N. (2009) Visualising musical structure through
performance gesture. Paper presented at the Tenth Conference of the International
Society of Music Information Retrieval, Kobe, Japan.
McGilvray D. (2009) On the Analysis of Music by Computer. Unpublished doctoral
thesis, University of Glasgow.
Pullinger S. (2009) A System for the Analysis of Musical Data. Unpublished doctoral
thesis (forthcoming), University of Glasgow.
Vines B., Krumhansl, C. L., Wanderley, M. M., and Levitin, D. J. (2006) Cross modal
interactions in the perception of musical performance. Cognition, 101, pp. 80-113.
Wanderley M. M (2001) Quantitative analysis of non-obvious performer gestures. In I.
Wachsmuth and T. Sowa (eds.), Gesture and Sign Language in Human Computer
Interaction (pp. 241-153). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
George Dodd
METHOD
Procedure
Participants
Materials
RESULTS
of the objective sound and, hence, cannot reflect on the performance of the
electro-acoustical components. We thus conclude that consciousness of the
substitution of electro-acoustical elements, in what originally would have
been a totally mechanical chain of sound production, transmission, and re-
ception, is a determining factor for audiences. This could explain the opposi-
tion that is encountered toward the concept of active auditoria.
A comparison of the result from question 9 (which showed a large 90% pref-
erence for live performance) with the data that respondents gave on their
listening patterns indicates that there is a large discrepancy between how
people would prefer to listen to their choice of music and how they actually
do. However, the answers to question 11where the option of listening at
home was deliberately included in the experimentconfirm that there is an
unsatisfied demand for live performance.
DISCUSSION
George Dodd, Acoustics Research Centre, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019,
Auckland, New Zealand; Email: g.dodd@auckland.ac.nz
References
Dodd G. (2001). Listener habits and choicesand their implications for public perform-
ance venues. Journal of Sound and Vibration, 239, pp. 589-606
Parkin P. H., Allen W. A., and Bean T. E. (1964). A special report on the experimental
Assisted Resonance system in the Royal Festival Hall. Journal of Sound and Vibra-
tion, 1, pp. 335-342.
Poletti M. (1994). The performance of a new assisted reverberation system. Acta Acus-
tica, 2, pp.511-524.
Keynote paper
International Symposium on Performance Science The Author 2009, Published by the AEC
ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 All rights reserved
Sylvie Fortin
how dancers conceptualize their health and the impact on their performing
practices and vice versa. Although my research pertains to dance, I believe
that many ideas can be transferred to other performing art forms.
Adopting the epistemological claim that individuals construct reality,
my research is part of a growing body of literature on how diverse cultural
groups conceptualize their health differently. More specifically, I look at how
artists construct themselves and are constructed in many ways by various,
and sometimes competing, discourses. According to Foucault (1963), dis-
courses are composed of ideas, attitudes, beliefs, courses of action, and prac-
tices that enable, as much as they constrain, what can be said or done in
particular times and places. Discourses construct current truths and what
power relations they carry with them.
For example, the dominant Western health discourse posits individuals as
being primarily responsible for their own health. This discourse implies con-
stant surveillance and auto-surveillance, encouraging individuals to avoid risk
and to adopt safe behaviors such as not smoking or eating junk food. Critics
of this discourse say that personal body maintenance is promoted to reduce
the economic burden on public services, and ultimately, it undermines social
responsibility to improve health and wellbeing. Assuming that individuals
will act in their own self-interest is also problematic since individual health is
often subject to external forces (Crawford 2006).
MAIN CONTRIBUTION
This idea is corroborated by dance research that shows how dancers privilege
the integrity of the art form over their own safety. Whereas ballet and con-
temporary dance can differ in their underlying assumption about the body,
the contemporary dancers in our sampling tended to align themselves with
the dominant discourse of Western theatrical dance that promotes an ideal
body where aesthetic criteria of beauty, slimness, virtuosity, devotion, and
asceticism prevail, and where fatigue, pain, and injury are accepted silently
(Laws 2005, Sorignet 2004, Wainwright et al. 2005).
When questioned about the relationship between health and dance, per-
forming artists in our research relegated the former to a backstage role,
valuing pushing the limits of the art form and expanding individual skills
sometimes beyond reasonable limits. The artists we interviewed regarded
dance as a risky endeavor, since dance-making involves seeking innova-
tion, and this invariably damages the body. Thus, we concluded that danc-
ers appropriated certain elements of both the contemporary dance discourse,
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 601
which assumes a certain risk associated with innovation, and the dominant
health discourse, which preaches physical self-discipline and the avoidance of
risks.
Without exception, all the artists considered themselves as healthy but dam-
aged. They did not feel that their current and past injuries conflicted with
their sense of being healthy, since being healthy means being functional
that is, being able to respond to the choreographic demands. To this end,
dancers employed various strategies to prepare, protect, and repair their
body. They attributed failure to themselves and consequently felt guilty, tell-
ing themselves they could have had better concentration, warmed-up or
trained more, stopped dancing earlier, eaten better, slept more, etc.
Confusion and uncertainty regarding the effectiveness of different options
sometimes engendered what seems to be an endless search for ways to heal
injuries and to relieve pain and psychological distress.
Individual guilt has been related to the dominant health discourse, which
is driven by a focus on the individuals responsibility and which minimizes
socio-economic and political factors (Lupton 1995). Although some dancers
spoke out against low income, unsecure employment, and difficult work con-
ditions (e.g. additional rehearsals before shows, freelancers fatigue from
holding down two jobs, inadequate studios, tight schedules aimed at mini-
mizing touring costs, etc.), they rarely acted to counter these issues, which is
not surprising in a competitive and small work environment. In this context,
the likely result of complying with the dominant health discourse is that col-
lective mobilization of dancers around health issues is restricted.
In Qubec, most dancers are freelancers. Bringing them into a worker union
was initiated ten years ago. Some individuals regarded union contracts as a
way to improve working conditions and better health for dancers. For others,
the legislative framework constituted a threat because, for them, it had a sti-
fling effect on the creative processes which need to come out of disorder,
disequilibrium, and instability. These artists revealed a profound attachment
to this dominant artistic discourse that they considered to be incompatible
with their perception of regular disciplined and ordered workplace environ-
ments. As such, they were reluctant to take on the employer-employee eti-
quette, and they did not like the term cultural industry because their
motivation to dance is aesthetic rather than economic. Choreographers did
602 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
not like to think of themselves as employers, although all of them are leading
their own dance companies. Many mentioned preferring to be called creators
rather than choreographers.
The process of unionizing dancers indirectly summons the dance com-
munity to take a critical attitude to the concept of individual responsibility for
health, and calls into question such firmly-grounded dance milieu values as
the normalizing of pain and the acceptance of precarious working conditions
as being commonplace. Dance artists narratives reflected a low propensity to
push collectively for change, which might encourage governments and agen-
cies to keep investing so little in the arts, and this eventually has conse-
quences on artists health. In our opinion, to improve the state of health of
dance artists there must be a critical analysis of the cultural, social, and eco-
nomic contexts in which dance is situated.
In our interviews, many addressed the issue of the financial health of the
dance milieu, which in Canada (as in many other countries probably) suffers
from chronic under-financing. But can it really be said that increasing fund-
ing to dance companies would directly improve dancers health? On one
hand, yes, since the equation money=health is verifiably true. For example,
all the artists raised the negative impact on their health of substandard re-
hearsal studios and theatres. On the other hand, however, no, because inju-
ries and suffering are not solely due to financial conditions. Aesthetic choices
often take precedence over health considerations. From our analysis, we con-
cluded that having more funds does not necessarily mean that more is spent
on health.
Worthy of note is the case of a choreographer whose assumption of re-
sponsibility for his dancers health was shown in the partial financial support
he provided for their training and therapeutic treatments. The same person,
however, disavowed responsibility by pushing his dancers to extreme limits,
leaving the protection of dancers health up to the rehearsal director and the
dancers themselves. He confided to us: the choreographers intention is to
present something striking on stage. I require dancers who can impel this
idea, and there are no limits to this. As the choreographer I have all the
rights. This is an attitude that even this choreographer considers extreme
and problematic. To us, dance development cannot rest on the acceptance of
excess, unless specific occupational health and safety conditions are imple-
mented. Improved financing is one of the means for preserving both the
dancers health and the choreographers vision; but what is also needed is a
INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 603
professional dance career, since expressing pain may have negative conse-
quences. The following comments of an experienced dancer speak directly to
this issue:
That dancers give life to the choreography is an idea often expressed in the
dance literature; less so is the notion that choreographers participate in
molding dancers bodies (Dantas 2008). During our interviews one choreog-
rapher remarked: I constantly put my dancers in an uncomfortable situa-
tion; so where is the boundary between making an effort and going too far?
Between injuring ones body and being violent to oneself? The question of
exceeding the limits perhaps takes on lesser significance in other art forms
because in dance it is the dancers body that is at stake. As a dancer put it, a
painter will look at his canvas and wonder if the good colors are correct, but
the canvas will not ache the way we dancers do.
Interestingly, a denunciation of human injustice forms the subtext of
many choreographies, but in our observations of different creative processes,
we sometimes noted a lack of consideration for the health and wellbeing of
the performing artists who give tangible form to the work. Jackson (2008)
explains that there is a tendency in the dance world to correlate good prod-
ucts (choreography) with good processes and good people (choreographers,
dancers, rehearsal directors), as if no dichotomy could exist between the
moral dimensions of a choreography and the people that engendered it. If
someone appears to have been mistreated, or if an artist is shown to have
acted immorally, this is viewed as an exception or as necessary for the sake of
art. Health poses a huge challenge for choreographers who have the pressure
to create original work that meets the assessment criteria of grant agencies.
Can health advocates expect that one day grant agencies will require dance
companies to deal comprehensively with aspects of occupational health and
606 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG
safety, or that soon dance schools and dance organizations will seriously en-
gage in health-promotion? I hope so because, if the dance profession does not
rise to the challenge, it may come to be regarded as irrelevant by education-
ists, decision makers, and the public alike.
At one point, I was skeptical. I felt like saying to the dance schools give
more technique classes to your dancers. I was worried that I would have
work with dancers who are too preoccupied by their anatomy, who know
that it is a particular muscle which is sore, who know they shouldnt force
another one [laughing]. But now I see that the young dancers work so
well. I would have loved to have had such knowledge of my body.
IMPLICATIONS
art form is often presented as an explanation for why artists jeopardize their
health and accept difficult working conditions, we conclude that it is the
dominant performing arts discourse, which values the supremacy of the ar-
tistic work and the surpassing of ones limits, that heads a long sequence of
decisions negatively impacting artists health. Unless all participants in the
dance milieu, individually and collectively, critically address the different
discourses and their embodied truths, changes in dancers health and well-
being will remain limited. Games of truth are unavoidable, but they can be
played out with an ethically-based concern for oneself and for others.
Acknowledgments
References