Investigation of Sequence Processing A C PDF
Investigation of Sequence Processing A C PDF
Investigation of Sequence Processing A C PDF
and computational modelling to enable holistic investiga- proposed that maturation of the prefrontal cortex might
tion of a cognitive phenomenon. make possible the age-related developmental progression
of human infants on working memory tasks. The working
memory studies described above give clues to the adaptive
Issues related to serial order
origin of some behavioural sequences.
Certain types of adaptive behaviour such as imitation
In the following sections, a detailed discussion of several
also involve serial order. It has also been proposed that
issues related to serial order is presented. The issues discussed
propensity to engage in complex sequential activities
are the origin of sequential behaviour in humans, possible
such as imitation is a precursor to high-level behaviours
representation schemes available, the organization of se-
such as empathy and mental simulation5. This kind of
quences, the order and timing of individual elements of a
imitation-based learning is a useful adaptive behaviour
sequence, learning modes, stages and strategies applica-
and is being increasingly used for training robots. Schaal6
ble for sequence learning and the issues in sequence percep-
demonstrated that imitation learning offers a promising
tion. It is to be noted that no single paradigm covers all
route to gain new insights into mechanisms of perceptual
the issues in a unified fashion. Therefore, we discuss each
motor control that could ultimately lead to the creation of
of these issues with the help of a representative example.
autonomous humanoid robots.
Thus, some behavioural sequences have pre-wired origin,
Origins of sequential behaviour while many more are learned.
sequence of turn–pull–push. But such activity was not In human behaviour, hierarchical structuring has been
observed while the monkey was waiting to perform a dif- argued to be essential for many acquired skills, such as
ferent sequence of turn–push–pull for which a different language, problem-solving and everyday planning10–13.
set of cells was activated. Further, studies show that representation at the higher level
In summary, while studies by Georgopoulos et al.7, and supports grouping of low-level units to form what are popu-
Lu and Ashe8 indicated a distributed representation for larly known as chunks14–19. Chunking also enables over-
individual arm movements, studies by Tanji and Shima9 coming the limitations imposed by limited-capacity working
suggested a possible local representation for sequences of memory, whose limit is proposed to be 7 ± 2 constituents20.
movements. In summary, strength of the evidence from experimental
and theoretical studies so far points to a hierarchical rep-
resentation.
Organization of sequences
a1234567 a1 a2 a3 a4 a5 a6 a7
a1 a2 a3 a4 a5 a6 a7
Figure 1. Flat versus hierarchical representation. Schematic diagram depicts a sequence of seven elements represented in flat
(linear) and hierarchical (nonlinear) arrangement. In hierarchical organization, nodes a123 and a4567 represent chunks of elements.
Si, Si+1 . . . , Sk → Yes or No, where 1 ≤ i ≤ k ≤ ∞. ning from left to right, serves as a standard landmark
called the central sulcus. The cortex anterior to the central
(ii) Recall/generation: Given a sequence Si, Si+1 . . . , Sk, sulcus is called the anterior (frontal) lobe and that which is
recall or generation problem involves generating or re- posterior is called the posterior lobe. Anterior lobe com-
calling the next item Sk+1. prises the prefrontal cortex, supplementary motor area,
pre-motor area and the primary motor area. Posterior lobe
Si, Si+1 . . . , Sk → Sk+1, where 1 ≤ i ≤ k ≤ ∞. comprises the sensory cortex, parietal cortex, occipital
(visual) cortex and the temporal areas. Some of the prominent
In both recognition and recall, an internal representation nuclei that lie below the cerebrum, i.e. in the sub-cortical
of sequence may be needed for making perceptual judgments region are the basal ganglia, the hippocampus and the amyg-
and for prediction (or generation) of subsequent elements, dala.
respectively. Future experimental investigations would need The cerebellum is located below the cerebral cortex.
to tease out differences in the cognitive processes involved The spinal cord connects the brain at the base near the
and the concomitant differences in brain areas sub-serving medulla oblongata. In the remainder of the section, we
recognition and recall/generation of sequences. describe results from our own and other experimental efforts
investigating the brain areas associated with various aspects
of serial order.
Brain areas sub-serving aspects of serial order
The current working assumption in neuroscience is that Brain areas involved in serial order
various functions are organized in a modular fashion in
the brain. In this section, we shall first present a brief over-
In this subsection we summarize some representative experi-
view of various brain areas and their function.
mental findings corresponding to the issues related to se-
The central nervous system is composed of the brain
rial order elucidated earlier.
and the spinal cord. Stimuli from sensory/peripheral organs
are carried via the spinal cord and processed in the brain.
Origins: The area specifically proposed for procedural
The corresponding motor signals (outputs) get transmitted
memory (such as skills and habits) is the striatum32. Berridge
to the appropriate peripheral system via the spinal cord. The
and Whishaw3 showed that lesions of the precentral corti-
brain is composed of three major components, namely the
cal areas or of other neo-cortical areas did not affect the
cerebrum, the cerebellum, and the sub-cortical areas. The
performance of ritualistic behavioural sequences in rats.
brain is divided into two hemispheres called the left hemi-
On the other hand, lesions of the neostriatum, which receives
sphere (left lobe; Figure 3) and the right hemisphere (right
inputs from the cerebral cortex, impaired the performance
lobe). These hemispheres have rich interconnections
thereby implicating a role for the striatum in pre-wired
through the corpus callosum. A prominent groove on the
behavioural sequences. Curran24 and Clegg et al.33 also
surface of the cerebrum when viewed from the top and run-
summarized evidence for striatal involvement in sequence
learning based on experiments involving implicit learning
tasks such as the SRT.
and motor tasks. They pointed out that these areas might Theoretical framework and computational modelling
be playing an important role in perception–action cycle.
So far, we had highlighted the cognitive neuroscience
perspective of serial order in cognition. In this section a
Learning: We pointed out earlier, the various aspects of
discussion is presented on how computational neuroscience
sequence learning, namely learning modes, learning stages
efforts can be directed towards theoretical investigation of
and learning strategies. Curran24 described distinct brain
serial order. In the literature, several researchers described
areas involved in the implicit and explicit modes of se-
computational models for serial learning. These models
quence learning. He suggested involvement of striatum in
can be broadly classified into three categories – biologically
the implicit learning mode and the medial, temporal and
inspired models, connectionist models, and hybrid models.
diencephalic brain regions during the explicit learning
In biologically inspired computational models, some aspects
mode. Squire and Zola32 also reported similar findings
of anatomical organization and function are mimicked44,45.
during implicit and explicit memory-related tasks.
In connectionist models, the aim is to mimic the overall
Studies by Jupetner et al.26,27 have shown that as learning
behaviour of the biological system rather than replicating
progressed from controlled (new learning) stage to automatic
the internal organizational details46. In hybrid models,
stage in a finger movement learning task, brain activation
engineering principles enable the construction of models
shifted from the anterior to the posterior parts both in the
that illuminate biological function. These models usually do
neocortex and the sub-cortical structures. Sakai et al.28
not attempt an explicit replication of anatomical organiza-
demonstrated transition of brain activity from the frontal
tion47,48.
regions (pre-SMA and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) to the
In order to illustrate the typical activity taken up under
parietal regions (precuneus and lateral parietal cortex) as
the enterprise of computational modelling, we present here
learning advanced from early to non-early (intermediate
a theoretical framework for modelling sequence processing
and late) stages respectively. Our behavioural37 and functional
focusing on the organizational aspects. The framework
magnetic resonance imaging experiments investigated
we propose here falls under the third category, i.e. hybrid
different aspects of procedural memory such as represen-
computational model. We propose a two-level model where
tation38, complexity39 and learning mode40. We proposed
Markov models and reinforcement learning are combined to
possible cortical localization of various modules and
specifically address how biological systems learn to organize
mappings that subjects use while practising a set of finger
sequential information in a hierarchical fashion.
movements in response to visual stimuli. In early stages
Curran24 summarized results from the implicit learning
of learning this task, subjects may follow a long route in
studies and concluded that in sequence learning tasks, basal
which the response is mediated by a visuo-spatial mapping
ganglia systems may enable extraction of first-order sequen-
followed by a spatial-motor mapping. In the late stages of
tial dependencies and that cortical–basal ganglia loops
learning this task, subjects may follow a shorter route,
may be responsible for learning higher order structures.
where they utilize a direct visuo-motor mapping. Further,
Based on this empirical evidence we propose a two-level
we hypothesized that there are two sequence representations,
model where at the lower level, first-order sequential depend-
effector-independent in visual/spatial coordinates and ef-
encies are extracted and at the higher level, hierarchical
fector-dependent representation in motor coordinates.
structure corresponding to the entire sequence is captured
Possible neural bases for the effector-independent se-
using reinforcement learning.
quence representation may be in the parietal–prefrontal
network and effector-dependent representation may be in
the SMA–primary motor cortical network. Basal ganglia First-order Markov model
structures may also be differentially involved in supporting
different representations and the premotor cortex may Markov model is a well-formulated mathematical framework
mediate various mappings37,38,41. for capturing first and higher order sequential dependen-
Doya29,42 suggested that the cerebellum, basal ganglia, and cies among random variables describing the behaviour of
cerebral cortex are specialized for different strategies of a system. The main assumption (also called Markov
learning, namely supervised learning, reinforcement assumption) is that prediction of the next state depends
learning and unsupervised learning respectively. only on a portion of the previous history of state transi-
tions. In the case of first-order Markov models, the prob-
ability that qt (state at time t) is equal to i is completely
Perception: In a recent investigation, Pasupathy and
predictable by knowing qt–1 and ignoring the rest of the
Miller43 demonstrated that the learning-related activity in
previous state history (a second-order Markov model
prefrontal cortex and striatum showed different time courses
would require qt–1 and qt–2 to predict qt). A formal defini-
during associative learning. Their results on monkeys sug-
tion of the first-order Markov model is given below.
gested that the striatum generates quick predictions about
the behavioural choice and the prefrontal cortex reveals
the slower accumulation of the correct answer. P(qi = i|qt–1 = j, qt–2 = k, . . .) = P(qt = i|qt–1 = j).
Markov Dependencies
Understanding
serial order/sequencing
ACTION
problem solving
Value
Function TD Error
Behavioural and Functional
REWARD neural correlates understanding
Theoretical framework/
ENVIRONMENT Behavioural and computational
imaging experiments modelling
effort enables understanding of the behavioural and neural 19. Pammi, V. S. C., Miyapuram, K. P., Bapi, R. S. and Doya, K.,
correlates of sequencing. The latter effort engenders func- Chunking phenomenon in complex sequential skill learning in
humans. In LNCS, Proceedings of International Conference on
tional understanding. It is difficult, if not impossible, to Neural Information Processing (eds Pal, N. R. et al.), Springer-
gain the complete functional understanding of a cognitive Verlag, Heidelberg, 2004, vol. 3316, pp. 294–299.
phenomenon by a pure empirical approach. Similarly, a 20. Miller, G. A., The magical number seven, plus or minus two:
pure theoretical or modelling approach will run the risk Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol.
of lacking the biological realism and relevance. Hence, the Rev., 1956, 63, 81–97.
21. Grossberg, S., Some networks that can learn, remember, and repro-
empirical and modelling efforts are of utmost importance duce any number of complicated space time patterns I. J. Math.
and they reinforce each other. The other point emphasized Mech., 1969, 19, 53–91.
here is that these efforts must go hand-in-hand. The mod- 22. Janata, P. and Grafton, S. T., Swinging in the brain: Shared neural
elling exercise may also spawn further predictions to be substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music. Nature
experimentally verified in future. Although the current ar- Neurosci., 2003, 6, 682–687.
23. Anderson, J. R., Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychol. Rev., 1982,
ticle has focused on sequence learning, the proposed inte- 89, 369–406.
gration would serve as an indispensable model for organizing 24. Curran, T., On the neural mechanisms of sequence learning.
scientific endeavours toward understanding any cognitive PSYCHE, 1995, 2.
phenomenon. 25. Fitts, P. M., Perceptual motor skill learning. In Categories of Human
Learning (ed. Melton, A. W.), Academic Press, New York, 1964,
pp. 243–285.
1. Lashley, K. S., The problem of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral 26. Jueptner, M., Stephan, K. M., Frith, C. D., Brooks, D. J., Fracko-
Mechanisms in Behavior (ed. Jeffress, L. A.), Wiley, New York, wiak, R. S. J. and Passingham, R. E., Anatomy of motor learning.
1951, pp. 112–136. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. J. Neurophysiol., 1997,
2. Yarbus, A. L., Eye movements during perception of complex ob- 77, 1313–1324.
jects. In Eye Movements and Vision (ed. Riggs, L. A.), Plenum 27. Jueptner, M., Frith, C. D., Brooks, D. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J. and
Press, New York, 1967, pp. 171–196. Passingham, R. E., Anatomy of motor learning. II. Subcortical
3. Berridge, K. C. and Whishaw, I. Q., Cortex, striatum, and cerebel- structures and learning by trial and error. J. Neurophysiol., 1997,
lum: control of syntactic grooming sequences. Exp. Brain Res., 77, 1325–1337.
1992, 90, 275–290. 28. Sakai, K., Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., Takino, R., Sasaki, Y. and
4. Diamond, A., Evidence for the importance of dopamine for prefrontal Pütz, B., Transition of brain activation from frontal to parietal areas
cortex functions early in life. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London, Ser. in visuomotor sequence learning. J. Neurosci., 1998, 18, 1827–
B, 1996, 351, 1483–1494. 1840.
5. Arbib, M. and Rizzolatti, G., Neural expectations: a possible evolu- 29. Doya, K., What are the computations in the cerebellum, the basal
tionary path from manual skills to language. Commun. Cognit., ganglia, and the cerebral cortex. Neural Networks, 1999, 12, 961–
1996, 29, 393–424. 974.
6. Schaal, S., Is imitation learning the route to humanoid robots? 30. Sun, R., Introduction to sequence learning. In Sequence Learning:
Trends Cognit. Sci., 1999, 3, 233–242. Paradigms, Applications and Algorithms (eds Sun, R. and Giles,
7. Georgopoulos, A. P., Lurito, J. T., Petrides, M. and Schwartz, A. C. L.), Springer-Verlag, LNAI, 2000, vol. 1828, pp. 1–10.
B., Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science, 31. Sun, R. and Giles, L., Sequence learning: from prediction and rec-
1989, 243, 234–236. ognition to sequential decision making. IEEE Intelligent Syst.,
8. Lu, X. and Ashe, J., Anticipatory activity in primary motor cortex 2001, 16, 67–70.
codes memorized movement sequences. Neuron, 2005, 45, 967–973. 32. Squire, L. R. and Zola, S. M., Structure and function of declara-
9. Tanji, J. and Shima, K., Role for supplementary motor area cells tive and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
in planning several movements ahead. Nature, 1994, 371, 413–416. USA, 1996, 93, 13515–13522.
10. Chomsky, N., In Syntactic Structures, Mouton & Co, The Hague, 33. Clegg, B. A., DiGirolamo, G. J. and Keele, S. W., Sequence learn-
1957. ing. Trends Cognit. Sci., 1998, 2, 275–281.
11. Newell, A., Shaw, J. C. and Simon, H. A., Elements of a theory of 34. Shin, J. C. and Ivry, R. B., Spatial and temporal sequence learning
human problem-solving. Psychol. Rev., 1958, 65, 151–166. in patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions. J. Cognit.
12. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K. H., In Plans and the Neurosci., 2003, 15, 1232–1243.
Structure of Behaviour, Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, 1960. 35. Sakai, K., Hikosaka, O., Takino, R., Miyauchi, S., Nielsen, M. and
13. Newell, A. and Simon, H., In Human Problem Solving, Prentice Tamada, T., What and when: Parallel and convergent processing
Hall, NJ, 1972. in motor control. J. Neurosci., 2000, 20, 2691–2700.
14. Wickelgren, W. A., Context sensitive coding, associative memory, 36. Kennereley, S. W., Sakai, K. and Rushworth, M. F. S., Organiza-
and serial order in (speech) behavior. Psychol. Rev., 1969, 76, 1– tion of action sequences and role of the Pre-SMA. J. Neuro-
15. physiol., 2004, 91, 978–993.
15. Wickelgren, W. A., Webs, cell assemblies, and chunking in neural 37. Bapi, R. S., Doya, K. and Harner, A. M., Evidence for effector in-
nets: Introduction. Can. J. Exp. Psychol., 1999, 53, 118–131. dependent and dependent representations and their differential
16. MacKay, D. G., The problem of flexibility, fluency, and speed- time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Exp.
accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychol. Rev., 1982, 89, Brain Res., 2000, 132, 149–162.
483–506. 38. Bapi, R. S., Graydon, F. X. and Doya, K., Time course of learning
17. Rosenbaum, D. A., Kenny, S. B. and Derr, M. A., Hierarchical of visual and motor sequence representations. Society for Neuro-
control of rapid movement sequences. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Per- science Annual Meeting, USA, 2000.
cept. Perform., 1983, 9, 86–102. 39. Pammi, V. S. C., Miyapuram, K. P., Bapi, R. S., Samejima, K.
18. Sakai, K., Kitaguchi, K. and Hikosaka, O., Chunking during human and Doya, K., Acquisition of complex sequential skills: Behav-
visuomotor sequence learning. Exp. Brain Res., 2003, 152, 229– ioral and fMRI Investigation. Building the Brain, Manesar, India,
242. 2003.