Mindfulness, Acceptance, and Positive Psychology (DR - Soc)
Mindfulness, Acceptance, and Positive Psychology (DR - Soc)
Mindfulness, Acceptance, and Positive Psychology (DR - Soc)
book is a stunning example of its power. Editors Kashdan and Ciarrochi have
taken two of the most influential therapies of our time—acceptance and com-
mitment therapy and positive psychology—and have initiated a dialogue that
both clarifies and deepens the discoveries that have emerged independently in
each. By establishing seven foundations of well-being that are both scientifically
based and practically oriented, they enable a coherent and fascinating look at the
role of mindfulness and an array of potent, cutting-edge therapeutic strategies.
This fresh synthesis is destined to be a landmark in the literature of emotional
healing and spiritual transformation.”
—Tara Brach, PhD, author of Radical Acceptance and True Refuge
“To every innovator working to create new tools and technologies for increased
well-being: Mindfulness, Acceptance, and Positive Psychology should immediately
go to the top of your must-read list. The new research and provocative ideas in
this collection will undoubtedly spark countless fresh ideas and help shape, for
the better, the next wave of life-changing, positive interventions. This book will
give an invaluable edge to designers and developers who want to ground their
creative work in leading-edge scientific research.”
—Jane McGonigal, PhD, author of the New York Times bestseller
Reality Is Broken
The
Mindfulness & Acceptance
Practica Series
psychology
the seven foundations
of well-being
EDITED BY
TODD B.
KASHDAN, PhD
AND
JOSEPH
CIARRO CHI, PhD
Context Press
An Imprint of New Harbinger Publications, Inc.
Publisher’s Note
This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the
subject matter covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in
rendering psychological, financial, legal, or other professional services. If expert assistance or
counseling is needed, the services of a competent professional should be sought.
5 Perspective Taking���������������������������������������������������������107
Ian Stewart, National University of Ireland, Galway; Louise
McHugh, University College Dublin
vi
Contents
vii
Alphabetical List of Contributors
x
CHAPTER 1
Joseph Ciarrochi
University of Wollongong, Australia
Todd B. Kashdan
George Mason University, USA
Russ Harris
Private Practice, Melbourne, Australia
2
The Foundations of Flourishing
3. contact with the present moment: engaging fully with your here-
and-now experience, with an attitude of openness and curiosity
3
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
and disorder. While this emphasis has led to efficacious treatments for a
variety of psychological problems, the primary reasons for living have
been ignored. Nobody lives to be merely free of distress and disorder, and
the positive is not merely the absence of distress and disorder. There are
other ingredients to a life well lived, and these ingredients have been the
focus of positive psychology research and practice.
When first introduced to the world, Seligman and Csiksentmihalyi
(2000) mapped out the terrain covered by positive psychology. The field
of positive psychology at the subjective level is about valued experiences:
well-being, contentment, and satisfaction (in the past); hope and opti-
mism (for the future); and flow and happiness (in the present). At the
personal level, it is about positive individual traits: the capacity for love
and vocation, courage, interpersonal competence, perseverance, forgive-
ness, originality, future mindedness, spirituality, and wisdom. At the
group level, it is about the civic virtues and institutions that move indi-
viduals toward better citizenship: responsibility, nurturance, altruism,
civility, tolerance, and work ethic.
The working assumption of positive psychology is that the positive,
healthy aspects of life are not simply the bipolar opposite of distress and
disorder. This theme arises again in a special issue of the Review of
General Psychology dedicated to positive psychology, where the editors
claim that psychology has been effective at learning “how to bring people
up from negative eight to zero, but not as good at understanding how
people rise from zero to positive eight” (Gable & Haidt, 2005, p. 103).
That is, the primary aim is to address and cultivate positive experiences,
strengths and virtues, and the requirements for positive relationships and
institutions.
In this description, positive psychology seems to push too far to the
other extreme, focusing only on the positive, with a caveat that of course,
pain and suffering are important as well. It is only in the last few years
that researchers have advocated for the need to move beyond the super-
ficial connection between the “positive” and “negative” dimensions of
the human psyche (Sheldon, Kashdan, & Steger, 2011b). For instance, if
you are attempting to teach children to be compassionate, you simply
cannot ignore the negative, because it is built into the fiber of empathy,
and perspective taking. Prominent positive psychologists often discour-
age a focus on weaknesses (because this is less efficient and profitable);
(Buckingham & Clifton, 2001) and reinforce the notion that when it
4
The Foundations of Flourishing
5
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
two fields actually sat down and spoke to each other. Would this not
accelerate progress? This book asks that question in every chapter, and
in every case it returns a clear answer: Yes.
If we are going to sit down and talk, the first thing we need to do is
develop a common language. Without that language, we will be con-
fused with each other and grow frustrated. Indeed, this confusion is
widespread in psychology. Every subdiscipline seems to create its own
island of words and constructs. Positive psychology talks about the pres-
ence of a nearly universal list of 24 character strengths, ACT focuses on
six core processes, personality researchers focus on the big five personal-
ity dimensions, and emotional intelligence researchers focus on the five
(+-2) components. Each new researcher that comes upon the scene
seems keen to create a new brand or at least a few new psychological
terms that can be uniquely associated with him or her. Meanwhile,
people on the front lines, such as therapists, coaches, and consultants,
are drowning in a sea of jargon.
We propose there is a way to survive the flood and even navigate it
effectively. Our solution is to identify a small set of basic factors (or foun-
dations) from which we can build a wide range of larger psychological
constructs, in much the same way that we can build complex physical
compounds from simple primary elements (e.g., we can manufacture
steel from iron and carbon). But how do we select these psychological
foundations when there are so many options to choose from?
We decided to select a set of basic ingredients that can be arranged
and rearranged into almost any strand of well-being. We selected our
foundations on the basis of two key criteria: (1) they must be guided by
the best available science, and (2) they must be of direct practical use for
facilitating cognitive and behavioral change to improve well-being. Thus,
we did not select brain regions or neuronal pathways as foundations,
because although knowledge of these things is relevant to well-being, it
does not provide a practitioner with direct ways of instigating positive
behavioral change. Similarly, we did not select elements that are associ-
ated with well-being if the implications for intervention were unclear; a
good example is the personality dimension of “extraversion.”
At this point, let’s note that although some positive psychology prac-
titioners describe happiness as being synonymous with “well-being,” the
truth is that “happiness” is only a single strand of a multidimensional
6
The Foundations of Flourishing
7
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
8
The Foundations of Flourishing
9
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Functional Beliefs
Functional beliefs are central to various forms of cognitive-behavioral
therapy (Barlow, 2002; Beck, 1983; Ciarrochi & Bailey, 2008). For
example, Beck’s therapy focuses on core and intermediate beliefs (Beck,
1995), Young’s therapy focuses on schema (Young, 1990), and Wells’s
therapy focuses on meta-beliefs about emotions and worry (Wells, 1997).
ACT does not focus on specific beliefs but rather encourages the practi-
tioner to a) identify when beliefs are dominating over other sources of
information (other thoughts, the environment) and when beliefs are
“unworkable” (i.e., acting on the belief does not work to make life rich,
full, and meaningful)(Ciarrochi & Robb, 2005). ACT undermines the
power of beliefs through the use of defusion (changing the context so that
a person can experience their belief as nothing more or less than a passing
thought, which they do not have to act on). Positive psychology seeks not
so much to undermine unhelpful beliefs but rather to promote positive,
functional beliefs such as hope, self-esteem, and a positive problem-solving
orientation (Ciarrochi, Heaven, & Davies, 2007; Sheldon, Kashdan, &
Steger, 2011a). However, plenty of models within positive psychology do
advocate the active challenging and disputation of dysfunctional beliefs.
Chapters 3 and 4 discuss beliefs related to love and self-compassion, and
chapters 7, 8, and 10 discuss the issue of how beliefs are best modified.
Mindfulness
Mindfulness, broadly defined, means conscious awareness with an
open, receptive attitude, of what is happening in the present moment
(Bishop et al., 2004; Williams, 2008). Conscious awareness involves
intentionally regulating attention toward what is happening here and
now. We can describe a person as “gently observing” what is happening,
as opposed to “judging” it. An “open, receptive attitude” reflects the
quality of one’s attention, characterized by curiosity, and a turning
toward one’s experience rather than away from it. For example, even
when our thoughts and feelings are painful or difficult, in a state of
mindfulness we are receptive to and curious about these psychological
events instead of trying to avoid or get rid of them (Hayes, Luoma, Bond,
Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). In and of itself, awareness of one’s environment
10
The Foundations of Flourishing
Perspective Taking
Many domains of psychology study perspective taking, and it goes by
such labels as “psychological mindedness,” “reflective functioning,”
“empathy,” and “theory of mind” (Eisenberg, 2003; Eisenberg, Murphy,
& Shepard, 1997). In positive psychology, researchers and practitioners
have given minimal attention to perspective taking; they have generally
classed it under character strengths as a merger of “personal intelligence”
and “perspective” (Peterson & Seligman, 2004). ACT-related interven-
tions and research focus heavily on perspective taking and empathy
(Ciarrochi, Hayes, & Bailey, 2012; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999),
and in particular on the development of an observer perspective, techni-
cally referred to as “self-as-context.” This is a perspective from which all
experience can be noticed and you are aware of your own flow of experi-
ences but without any investment in or attachment to them. Chapters 5
11
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Values
We can define values in many ways, but generally we can think of
them as verbal descriptions of what people are personally invested in,
regard highly, and seek to uphold and defend. Recognizing and endorsing
these cherished ideals is quite different from behaving in ways that are
congruent with them (e.g., see section on behavioral control). Many
researchers view values as central to a person’s sense of self; they operate
as standards that guide thought and action (Feather, 2002; Hitlin, 2003;
Kristiansen & Zanna, 1994; Rohan, 2000; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987).
Positive psychologists discuss values in the form of personal strivings,
goal setting, or personal philosophies for what is most important in life
(Emmons, 1996; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990). The distance between valued
preferences and actual behavior can be vast, which can be a pivotal point
of intervention.
ACT makes use of positive psychology literature but speaks of values
in a specific way, as qualities of purposive action that can never be
obtained as an object, but can be instantiated from moment to moment
(Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 2011). Thus, ACT sees values as desired
global qualities of ongoing action (or, in layman’s terms, “your heart’s
deepest desires for how you want to behave as a human being”). This
definition is consistent with ACT’s focus on behavior.
There is also a substantial literature on meaning and purpose in life
that seems closely linked to values. Some researchers have defined purpose
as a “central, self-organizing life aim” (McKnight & Kashdan, 2009;
Steger, 2009). Others have fleshed out this definition by unifying princi-
ples from positive psychology and ACT (Kashdan & McKnight, 2009).
12
The Foundations of Flourishing
Experiential Acceptance
Experiential acceptance means embracing “private experiences” (e.g.,
thoughts, emotions, memories—experiences an individual has that no
outside observer can directly see) and allowing these experiences to be
present without trying to avoid or get rid of them. Willingness, a close
ally of acceptance, involves allowing difficult private experiences to be
present, in the service of a valued action (Ciarrochi & Bailey, 2008).
Experiential avoidance—the ongoing attempt to avoid or get rid of
unwanted private experiences—transforms the perfectly normal experi-
ence of pain to one of suffering and ineffective action (Ciarrochi,
Kashdan, Leeson, Heaven, & Jordan, 2011; Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, &
Steger, 2006). There are two major reasons for this. First, attempts to
control or suppress feelings often result in an increase in those feelings,
as when attempting to not feel anxious makes you more anxious. Second,
emotions and values are often two sides of the same coin, and therefore
to avoid one means to avoid the other. You cannot have loving relation-
ships without risking vulnerability and all the painful thoughts and feel-
ings that inevitably go with it. Positive psychology addresses the
experiential avoidance component under the umbrella of mindfulness
(Brown & Ryan, 2003) or effective emotion regulation (John & Gross,
2004).
13
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Behavioral Control
Behavioral control refers to one’s ability to regulate behavior in a way
that is consistent with one’s values. Positive psychology might label this
component “perseverance,” “self-regulation,” or “willpower.” Research in
this area often focuses on identifying factors that promote goal success,
such as mental contrasting (considering benefits and barriers related to
goals) (Oettingen, Mayer, Sevincer, et al., 2009), implementation inten-
tions (establishing if-then plans to deal with barriers to goals) (Gollwitzer
& Schaal, 1998), and self-concordance of goals (the goals match your
inner-most needs) (Koestner, Lekes, Powers, & Chicoine, 2002; Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001).
When effort is devoted to valued aims, ACT refers to this as “com-
mitment.” Generally, there is a tight link between values, purpose, and
commitment, and these dimensions are not always easy to disentangle.
Despite the difficulty level, we think it is pragmatic to separate values
from commitment to highlight the difference between knowing what you
want (values, purpose) and acting on what you want (behavioral control).
Chapters 6, 10, and 11 tackle the issues of values, purpose, and
commitment.
Cognitive Skill
Cognitive skill refers to components of intellectual functioning such
as reasoning, problem solving, and attentional control. Both positive psy-
chology and ACT are somewhat neutral with regard to this factor, except
14
The Foundations of Flourishing
15
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
The Importance of
Intervention Purpose
To truly integrate positive psychology and ACT, we must first look a little
deeper at their purpose and philosophical assumptions. We shall see that
as long as positive psychology and ACT adopt similar philosophical
assumptions, they can work well together.
At this point, a warning to the reader: we have named the seven
foundations as if they are real entities, like animals walking about in the
world. However, in reality we view these foundations through a prag-
matic philosophy. The foundations help us to organize a rather bewilder-
ing array of constructs. We make no assumption that they are real entities
lying in the brain waiting to be discovered by some neurosurgeon.
Our pragmatic view stands in contrast to more mechanistic views.
As a contrast to our approach, consider the following recent declaration
by Seligman (2011):
16
The Foundations of Flourishing
Elemental realism gets its name because it assumes that one can know
the true nature of reality and objectively discover the elements of which
it is composed. The elemental realist views the universe as a machine
consisting of parts that interact. The goal of analysis is to model the
universe accurately.
The key question for the elemental realist is, “What elements and
forces make the model work?” Success is defined by how well a model is
able to make predictions and establish meaningful, reliable causal pat-
terns. Most forms of cognitive psychology are good examples of elemen-
tal realism, as are the information processing models found in positive
psychology. There is of course, absolutely nothing wrong, outdated, or
inferior with this philosophical stance. Acknowledging one’s philosophi-
cal worldview simply means owning up to the improvable assumptions
upon which one’s work rests. Thus one philosophical worldview can
never refute another.
The worldview adopted by ACT and some positive psychologists is
functional contextualism (a form of pragmatism). Functional contextu-
alism assumes we can never know the true nature of reality or the ele-
ments that comprise it; all we can do is observe how an aspect of the
universe functions in a given context (and part of that context will
always be the human mind itself). Functional contextualism focuses on
something called the “act-in-context.” “Context” means whatever comes
before the act that influences it (antecedents) and whatever follows the
act that reduces or increases the chance of its recurring (consequences).
The “act” is whatever happens in between the antecedents and the
consequences.
The key question in functional contextualism is, “How can we
manipulate the antecedents and consequences to best achieve our goals?”
The functional contextualist will divide an event into “elements” (e.g.,
antecedents and consequences) but does so purely for pragmatic pur-
poses (i.e., Does the division help us achieve our goals?). The functional
contextualist would make no assumption that this “division” uncovers or
reveals something of the “true nature” of reality; it is nothing more or less
than a useful strategy for achieving a specific goal.
The goal of functional analysis is to find ways to predict and influence
behavior. Prediction in itself is not enough. Typical research in this tradi-
tion focuses on manipulating antecedents and consequences and observ-
ing how behavior changes as a result. A particular activity is “successful”
17
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
18
The Foundations of Flourishing
19
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
20
The Foundations of Flourishing
21
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
22
The Foundations of Flourishing
which has emergent qualities that cannot be inferred from hydrogen and
oxygen. Can the same be said about positive and negative emotions? Is it
sometimes unhelpful to talk about “positive” and “negative” emotions?
Given that fear, sadness, and guilt are useful, life-enhancing emotions
that play a major role in building a rich, meaningful life, is it fair to call
them “negative”? Would we do better to talk about “pleasant” and
“unpleasant” emotions rather than “positive” and “negative”?
23
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Mindfulness √ √ √
and
Awareness
Perspective √ √
Taking
Values √ √
Experiential √ √ √
Acceptance
Behavioral √
Control
Cognitive √
Skill
24
The Foundations of Flourishing
√ √ √
√ √ √ √
√ √ √ √ √
√ √
√ √ √ √
√ √
25
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
References
[Baker, L., & McNulty, J. (2011). Self-compassion and relationship maintenance:
The moderating roles of conscientiousness and gender. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 100, 853-873.
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety
and panic (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In P. J.
Clayton & J. E. Barrett (Eds.), Treatment of depression: Old controversies and
new approaches. New York: Raven Press.
Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond: New York: Guilford Press.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Anderson, N., Carlson, L., Segal, Z. V., et al.
(2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 11, 230-241.
Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B., & Minhas, G. (2011). A dynamic approach to
psychological strength development and intervention. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 6, 106-118.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness
and its role in psychological well- being. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 84(4), 822-848.
Buckingham, M., & Clifton, D. O. (2001). Now, discover your strengths. New York:
The Free Press.
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training interven-
tion to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. Psychological Record, 61,
173-198.
Ciarrochi, J., & Bailey, A. (2008). A CBT-Practitioner’s guide to ACT: How to bridge
the gap between Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.
Ciarrochi, J., Hayes, L., & Bailey, A. (2012). Get out of your mind and into your life:
Teens. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Ciarrochi, J., Heaven, P. C., & Davies, F. (2007). The impact of hope, self-esteem,
and attributional style on adolescents’ school grades and emotional well-being:
A longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1161-1178.
Ciarrochi, J., Kashdan, T. B., Leeson, P., Heaven, P., & Jordan, C. (2011). On being
aware and accepting: A one-year longitudinal study into adolescent well-being.
Journal of Adolescence, 34(4), 695-703.
Ciarrochi, J., & Robb, H. (2005). Letting a little nonverbal air into the room:
Insights from acceptance and commitment therapy: Part 2: Applications.
Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 23(2), 107-130.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achieve-
ment gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307-1310.
Cormier, L. S., & Cormier, W. H. (1998). Interviewing strategies for helpers:
Fundamental skills and cognitive behavioral interventions (4th ed.). Pacific Grove,
Calif.: Brooks/Cole.
26
The Foundations of Flourishing
27
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Jaeggi, S., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. (2008). Improving fluid intelli-
gence with training on working memory. Proceedings from the National Academy
of Sciences, 105, 6829-6833.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation:
Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development.
Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1301-1333.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Experiential
avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with
coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44,
1301-1320.
Kashdan, T. B., Biswas-Diener, & King, L. A. (2008). Reconsidering happiness:
The costs of distinguishing between hedonics and eudaimonia. Journal of
Positive Psychology, 3, 219-233.
Kashdan, T. B., & McKnight, P. E. (2009). Origins of purpose in life: Refining our
understanding of a life well lived. Psychological Topics, 18, 303-316.
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental
aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 865-878.
Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers, T., & Chicoine, E. (2002). Attaining personal
goals: Self-concordance plus implementation intentions equals success. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 83, 231-244.
Kristiansen, C. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1994). The rhetorical use of values to justify
social and intergroup attitudes. Journal of Social Issues, 50, 47-65.
Linley, A. (2008). Average to A+: Realising strengths in yourself and others. Coventry,
UK: CAPP Press.
McHugh, M., McNab, J., Symth, C., Chalmers, J., Siminski, P., & Saunders, P.
(2004). The availability of foster carers: Main report. Sydney: Social Policy
Research Centre, University of New South Wales.
McKnight, P. E., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Purpose in life as a system that creates
and sustains health and well-being: An integrative, testable theory. Review of
General Psychology, 13, 241-251.
McNulty, J., & Fincham, F. (2012). Beyond positive psychology? Toward a contex-
tual view of psychological processes and well-being. American Psychologist,
67(2), 101-110.
Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino, P. J., Breen, W., Levesque,
C., et al. (2010). Being present in the face of existential threat: the role of trait
mindfulness in reducing defensive responses to mortality salience. Journal of
Personality & Social Psychology, 99, 344-365.
Norem, J. K. (2002). Defensive self-deception and social adaptation among opti-
mists. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 549-555.
Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Sevincer, T., Stephens, E. J., Pak, H.-J., & Hagenah, M.
(2009). Mental contrasting and goal commitment: The mediating role of ener-
gization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 608-622.
Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. (2004). Strengths of character and well-
being. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 603-619.
28
The Foundations of Flourishing
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook
and classification. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rohan, J. (2000). A rose by any name? The values construct. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 4, 255-277.
Rousseau, J. J. (1783/1979). Emile, or On Education (A. Bloom, Trans.). New York:
Basic Books.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of
human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 550-562.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and
structure of values: Extensions and cross- cultural replications. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 878-893.
Seligman, M. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-
being. New York: Free Press.
Seligman, M. E., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduc-
tion. American Psychologist, 55(1), 5-14.
Sheldon, K., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2011a). Designing positive psychology.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sheldon, K., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2011b). Designing positive psychology:
Taking stock and moving forward. New York: Oxford University Press.
Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance, goal attainment,
and the pursuit of happiness: Can there be an upward spiral? Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 152-165.
Silvia, P. J., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Interesting things and curious people:
Exploration and engagement as transient states and enduring strengths. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 785-797.
Steger, M. (2009). Meaning in life. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Oxford handbook of positive
psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, S., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological per-
spective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: A practice manual and concep-
tual guide. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley .
Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Mindfulness, depression and modes of mind. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 32, 721-733.
Wilson, K., & Murrell, A. (2004). Values work in acceptance and commitment
therapy: Setting a course for behavioral treatment. In S. C. Hayes, V. Follette,
& M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding the cognitive-
behavioral tradition. New York: Guilford Press.
Wong, P., & Fry, P. (1998). The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological
research and clinical application. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Young, J. E. (1990). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused
approach. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange.
29
CHAPTER 2
Eric L. Garland
Florida State University
Barbara L. Fredrickson
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
31
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
32
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
33
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Edie steps up to bat and takes a swing at the ball on the tee, she misses
with a resounding “whiff” of the bat through the air. When her class-
mates laugh, instead of being embarrassed, she laughs along with them.
Edie takes another swing, and this time, the ball stays in place while the
bat flies out of her hand and lands a few feet away. Her peers laugh even
harder, and she joins them in sharing the hilarity of the situation. She
meets their smiles with her own and feels the glow of positive emotions.
Consequently, she’s more willing to give it another try. Just before her
third swing, Edie notices that she’s been holding her right elbow too
high. She corrects her stance and smacks the ball down the field. Her
peers cheer for her, and after class, they approach her to giggle about the
crazy day at tee-ball. Sharing in a few more laughs, they become friends,
and later decide to ask their parents to enroll them in an afterschool tee-
ball team. In no time, Edie increases her athletic skills and cements
lasting friendships that promote positive emotions in the future.
Eventually, Edie goes on to play ball in high school and later decides to
major in journalism to become a sports reporter. She eventually lands a
high profile position on a major sports television network.
Conversely, Max is embarrassed by his inexperience with tee-ball.
He tries to avoid the gaze of his gym teacher so he won’t be picked to play
in the class tee-ball game. When he is called up, he swings and misses,
and focuses his attention on the laughing faces of his peers. He interprets
their expressions as evidence that they are mocking him, and he feels
ashamed and angry. He refuses to take a second swing and sits down
disengaged on the sidelines. In so doing, he robs himself of the opportu-
nity to experience positive emotions that might otherwise promote his
engagement in new behaviors and relationships. He grows up believing
“I’m no good at sports” and shuns any social interaction that centers on
athletic events, a pattern of experiential avoidance that causes him to
lead a circumscribed life well into adulthood.
Insofar as positive emotions broaden cognition and increase engage-
ment in behaviors leading to increased social bonds, they conferred an
evolutionary advantage to human ancestors, who may have been better
equipped to survive by virtue of the development of such adaptive mind-
sets, skills, and resources. Positive emotions, although fleeting, can have
a long-lasting impact on functional outcomes by expanding people’s
mindsets in ways that lead to enhanced well- being and social
connectedness.
34
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
35
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
36
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
37
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
38
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
39
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
events to one another and to alter the functions of events based on their
contexts (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). Language and
cognition encode these arbitrary relations between events. As such,
behavioral responses derive not from events themselves but rather from
the network of arbitrary relational rules established via a history of con-
ditioning and reinforcement. By virtue of such relational framing, we
make sense of events and appraise their emotional significance.
The drive to make sense or meaning of one’s experiences is central
to human existence (Frankl, 1959; Singer, 2004). Moreover, the need to
make sense of experience and to regulate emotion in desired directions
motivates changes in belief (Boden & Berenbaum, 2010). In turn,
changes in emotion arise from the specific meaning appraisals made in a
situation (Frijda, 1986). Appraisals are discrete and pertain to specific
events; in contrast, beliefs often extend over time and scope, and there-
fore have the potential to influence many discrete situational appraisals
(Boden & Berenbaum, 2010; Lazarus, 1991). For instance, for an indi-
vidual who holds the belief “I must be perfect,” receiving an A+ on a test
will result in the appraisal “I am perfect” and consequent feelings of con-
tentment, whereas receiving an A-results in the appraisal “I am a failure”
coupled with sadness. Thus, beliefs serve as the lens through which situ-
ational appraisals are made, with attendant emotions.
In turn, emotions influence the content and conviction of beliefs.
The interoceptive experience of an emotion provides a signal that tunes
attention to the stimuli that elicited the emotion (Clore & Gasper, 2000).
This emotional signal alerts the human information processing system to
the presence of objects and events that are related to a given belief.
Subsequently, stimuli that are mutually congruent with belief and the
current emotional state are preferentially selected for further processing
(Boden & Berenbaum, 2010). In addition, because emotion is directly
experienced as self-evident (Clore & Gasper, 2000), it often serves as
evidence that supports or disconfirms a given belief (Centerbar, Schnall,
Clore, & Garvin, 2008). Put another way, emotions that are consistent
with a given situational appraisal or belief tend to strengthen it, whereas
explanation- inconsistent emotions tend to weaken that appraisal or
belief. Moreover, emotional valence can influence the content of inter-
pretations of ambiguous stimuli. For example, participants experiencing
experimentally induced sadness reported greater self-blame for relation-
ship conflicts in contrast to those experiencing induced happiness
40
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
41
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
42
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
43
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Positive Reappraisal
Fortunately, the stress response is fluid and mutable; new data from the
changing environment coupled with novel information about one’s own
reactions to the threat may initiate a reappraisal process, in which the
original stress appraisal is changed as a result of the feedback. For
example, a stimulus that was originally appraised as threatening may be
reinterpreted as benign. Reappraisals modify the physiological, psycho-
logical, and social consequences of the stress reaction through dynamic
feedback-feedforward mechanisms, which alter the informational value
or meaning of the stimulus while calibrating the behavioral response to
that stimulus.
Reappraisal may be one of the keys to resilience. In the face of adver-
sity, people often believe that they have personally benefited or grown
from dealing with the stressful event. This positive emotion–focused
coping strategy, known as positive reappraisal, is the adaptive process
through which stressful events are reconstrued as benign, beneficial, or
meaningful (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Positive reappraisal, alternately
conceptualized as benefit finding (Affleck & Tennen, 1996), is
44
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
45
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
46
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
which, when engaged repeatedly over time, may accrue into trait or dis-
positional mindfulness (Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009), the propen-
sity toward exhibiting nonjudgmental awareness in everyday life.
The metacognitive state of mindfulness can moderate the impact of
potentially distressing psychological content through the mental opera-
tion of stepping back from thoughts, emotions, and sensations, known as
decentering (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002) or reperceiving (Shapiro,
Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, 2006). This set-shifting function may be a
key link between appraisal and reappraisal, involving a shift in mental
process rather than in contents (Hayes & Wilson, 2003). Shifting from
the contents of consciousness to the process of consciousness is thought
to lead to a lack of attachment to thoughts and emotions, liberating
awareness from fixed or rigid narratives about self and world (Niemic et
al., 2010; Shapiro et al., 2006). “Through reperceiving brought about by
mindfulness, the stories (e.g. about who we are, what we like or dislike,
our opinions about others, etc.) that were previously identified with so
strongly become simply ‘stories’” (Shapiro et al., 2006). The cognitive
set-shifting process of reperceiving or decentering may afford a funda-
mental cognitive flexibility, facilitating the flexible selection of cognitive
appraisals as “we become able to reflectively choose what has been previ-
ously reflexively adopted or conditioned” (Shapiro et al., 2006).
Ultimately, decentering creates distance between thoughts and thinker,
and thus, from socially conditioned narratives. This new distance or
space enables the selection of values that are more congruent with indi-
vidual goals.
Mindfulness is arguably the keystone in the arch of positive reap-
praisal. For a person to reconstrue his or her appraisal of a given event as
positive, he or she must suspend the initial stress appraisal and disengage
cognitive resources from it—in effect, “letting go” of the appraisal while
viewing it from a metacognitive vantage point that attenuates semantic
evaluations associated with the event. According to the mindful coping
model (Garland et al., 2009), when a given event is viewed as a threat
that exceeds one’s capabilities, this stress appraisal results in perturba-
tions to bodily homeostasis. In turn, feedback from the body is often
interpolated as emotion (Friedman, 2011; James, 1890). As one becomes
aware of the presence of negative emotions resulting from the stress
appraisal, one can initiate an adaptive response by “stepping back,” or
47
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
decentering from this stress appraisal and its resultant emotions into the
state of mindfulness. Operating from this mindful state results in
increased capacity to reorient attention to novel stimuli (Jha, Krompinger,
& Baime, 2007) as well as greater cognitive flexibility (Moore &
Malinowski, 2009). As a result, individuals can access new data with
which to reappraise their circumstances and reframe them as meaningful
or even beneficial. Attending to what is benign, purposeful, or affirming
in the situation may result in experiences of positive emotion, which
mediate and further promote positive reappraisal (Tugade & Fredrickson,
2004). Hence, from the metacognitive vantage point afforded by mind-
fulness, positive features of the object, event, circumstance, or context
that had been previously unattended now become accessible to con-
sciousness as the “stuff” that reappraisals are made of.
While mindfulness may temporarily suspend evaluative language,
because the human mind is embedded in narratives that reduce uncer-
tainty and produce a coherent life story (Olivares, 2010), it is inevitable
that one will reengage into a semantic-linguistic mode as one integrates
the encounter with the stressor into one’s autobiographical memory. As
one returns to this narrative mode from the state of mindfulness, reap-
praisals may arise either through a conscious process of reflection or
through more automatic processes, based on spontaneous insight. The
result of such positive reappraisal is positive emotions such as hope, com-
passion, and love, and accepting attitudes such as trust, confidence, and
equanimity, which reduce stress and in turn influence subsequent
appraisal processes.
Ultimately, the repeated, intentional engagement of the metacogni-
tive state of mindfulness may result in the development of trait mindful-
ness over time. Developing a more mindful disposition, in turn, leads to
a heightened propensity toward making positive reappraisals in the face
of distress as a cognitive coping style (see Figure 1). Recent evidence
from our prospective observational study of adult participants of a
mindfulness-based stress and pain management program supports this
assertion; we found that increases in dispositional mindfulness were
reciprocally linked with increases in positive reappraisal and that the
stress-reductive effects of increases in dispositional mindfulness were
mediated by increases in positive reappraisal (Garland et al., 2011).
Similarly, findings from a quasi-experimental study comparing college
students participating in a mindful communication course to those
48
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
Savoring Positive
Emotion
Positive Reappraisal
Attention to Positive
Contextual Features
State of Mindfulness
Decentering
Positive Reappraisal
Attention to Positive
Contextual Features
State of Mindfulness
Decentering
Stress Appraisal
49
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
50
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
51
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
52
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
53
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
54
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
Positive Emotion
Meaningful Action
Elaborated Positive
Reappraisal
Attention to Contextual Features
That Support the Reappraisal
Positive Reappraisal
Savoring
Positive Emotion
Broadened Attention to
Positive Contextual Features
State of Mindfulness
Decentering
Stress Appraisal
55
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Clinical Recommendations
Provide mindfulness training as a means of increasing psychological flex-
ibility and promoting positive reappraisal by encouraging clients to engage
in formal mindfulness practice after the initial stress appraisal and prior
to developing the reappraisal. This technique of “mindful reappraisal” is
outlined below and further detailed elsewhere (Garland, forthcoming):
2. Remind the client that even if he/she feels upset, he/she still has
a choice about how to think about and respond to the situation.
56
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
57
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
58
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
through the space afforded by decentering that the client can come into
closer contact with a wider range of experiences, some of which may be
reward or reinforce valued action. As Hayes, Strosahl, and Wilson (1999)
state, “Cognitive defusion and openness to experience have the side
effect of sensitizing the client to natural rather than verbally constructed
contingencies, which in turn may allow the client to respond more effec-
tively to environmental demands” (p. 237). Consequently, new cognitive
appraisals of self and world can be made that advance the commitment
to act in accordance with deeply held values. As such, mindfulness and
acceptance may be key mechanisms that underlie the therapeutic influ-
ence of reappraisal on well-being, a notion supported by data showing
that lower levels of experiential avoidance (i.e., greater acceptance)
mediate the relationship of cognitive reappraisal and positive psychologi-
cal outcomes (Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, & Steger, 2006). Further, the
capacity to accept experiences rather than avoid them moderates the
effect of traumatic life events on meaning in life and posttraumatic
growth (Kashdan & Kane, 2011), a construct closely linked with positive
reappraisal. Thus, we argue that mindful acceptance is the fulcrum on
which reappraisals can be leveraged in service of living a more meaning-
ful life.
The aim of this process is not to regulate thoughts and emotions
“away” by suppressing, denying, or avoiding them but, rather, to foster
committed action and imbue life with value. Ultimately, mindfulness
imparts the individual with the freedom and, therefore, the responsibility
for constructing a more purposeful and meaningful existence.
References
Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1996). Construing benefits from adversity: Adaptational
significance and dispositional underpinnings. Journal of Personality, 64(4),
899-922.
Albaugh, J. A., & Kellogg-Spadt, S. (2002). Sensate focus and its role in treating
sexual dysfunction. Urologic Nursing, 22(6), 402-403.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predic-
tors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111,
256-274.
Anderson, E., Siegel, E. H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). The visual
impact of gossip. Science, 332(6036), 1446-1448.
59
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
60
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
Castaneda, C. (1968). The teachings of Don Juan: A Yaqui way of knowledge. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Centerbar, D. B., Schnall, S., Clore, G. L., & Garvin, E. D. (2008). Affective inco-
herence: When affective concepts and embodied reactions clash. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 560-578.
Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An
integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 560-572.
Ciarrochi, J., & Bailey, A. (2008). A CBT practitioner’s guide to ACT. Oakland, CA:
New Harbinger.
Cicchetti, D., & Blender, J. A. (2006). A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on
resilience: Implications for the developing brain, neural plasticity, and preven-
tive interventions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094,
248-258.
Clore, G. L., & Gasper, K. (2000). Feeling is believing: Some affective influences on
belief. In N. H. Frijda, A. S. R. Manstead, & S. Bem (Eds.), Emotions and belief:
How feelings influence thoughts (pp. 10-44). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Cohn, M. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). In search of durable positive psychology
interventions: Predictors and consequences of long-term positive behavior
change.
Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M.
(2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by
building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361-368.
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-
driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-215.
Cruess, D. G., Antoni, M. H., McGregor, B. A., Kilbourn, K. M., Boyers, A. E.,
Alferi, S. M., et al. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral stress management reduces
serum cortisol by enhancing benefit finding among women being treated for
early stage breast cancer. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62(3), 304-308.
Davey, G. C., Bickerstaffe, S., & MacDonald, B. A. (2006). Experienced disgust
causes a negative interpretation bias: A causal role for disgust in anxious psy-
chopathology. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44(10), 1375-1384.
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual atten-
tion. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.
Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of
emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 736-748.
Ekman, P. (1971). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emo-
tions. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 207-283). Lincoln:
University of Nebraska Press.
Ekman, P. (1977). Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial move-
ment. In J. Blacking (Ed.), A.S.A. Monograph 15, the anthropology of the body
(pp. 39-84). London: Academic Press.
61
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
62
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
63
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Hejmadi, A., Waugh, C. E., Otake, K., & Fredrickson, B. L. (manuscript in prepa-
ration). Cross-cultural evidence that positive emotions broaden views of self to
include close others.
Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., & Tomich, P. L. (2006). A meta-analytic review
of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
74(5), 797-816.
Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychological
Review, 113(3), 439-460.
Huston, D., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. (2011). Mechanisms of mindfulness in
communications training. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 39(4),
406-421.
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203-253.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co.
Janig, W. (2002). The autonomic nervous system and its coordination by the brain.
In R. J. Davidson (Ed.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 135-186). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Jha, A., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsys-
tems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2),
109-119.
Kalisch, R. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of reappraisal: Time matters.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(8), 1215-1226.
Kalupahana, D. J. (1987). The principles of Buddhist psychology. Albany: State
University of New York Press.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Experiential
avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with
coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behavior Research and Therapy,
44(9), 1301-1320.
Kashdan, T. B., & Kane, J. Q. (2011). Posttraumatic distress and the presence of
posttraumatic growth and meaning in life: Experiential avoidance as a mod-
erator. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 84-89.
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental
aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 865-878.
Keeney, B. P. (1983). Aesthetics of change. New York: Guilford Press.
Keyes, C. L. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing
in life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 43(2), 207-222.
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy
mind. Science, 330(6006), 932.
Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2007). How meaning shapes seeing. Psychological
Science, 18(10), 845-849.
Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2001). Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and
allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 24(2), 97-129.
Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy
of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(11), 479-487.
64
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2011). Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological
Psychology, 84(3), 437-450.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect,
activation, and action. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Lazarus, R. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lazarus, R. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York: Springer.
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.
LeBel, J. L., & Dubé, L. (2001). The impact of sensory knowledge and attentional
focus on pleasure and on behavioral responses to hedonic stimuli, 13th Annual
American Psychological Society Convention. Toronto, Ontario.
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 81(1), 146-159.
Longmore, R. J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to challenge thoughts in cogni-
tive behavior therapy? Clinical Psychological Review, 27(2), 173-187.
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation
and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12(4), 163-169.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. Boston: Little
& Brown.
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disor-
ders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167-195.
McEwen, B. S. (2003). Mood disorders and allostatic load. Biological Psychiatry,
54(3), 200-207.
McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central
role of the brain. Physiological Reviews, 87(3), 873-904.
McEwen, B. S., & Wingfield, J. C. (2003). The concept of allostasis in biology and
biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior, 43(1), 2-15.
McGregor, B. A., Antoni, M. H., Boyers, A., Alferi, S. M., Blomberg, B. B., &
Carver, C. S. (2004). Cognitive- behavioral stress management increases
benefit finding and immune function among women with early-stage breast
cancer. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56(1), 1-8.
Monat, A., Averill, J. R., & Lazarus, R. S. (1972). Anticipatory stress and coping
reactions under various conditions of uncertainty. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 24(2), 237-253.
Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexi-
bility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 176-186.
Nelson, T. O., Stuart, R. B., Howard, C., & Crowley, M. (1999). Metacognition and
clinical psychology: A preliminary framework for research and practice.
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 73-79.
Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino, P. J., Breen, W., Levesque,
C., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Being present in the face of existential threat: The
role of trait mindfulness in reducing defensive responses to mortality salience.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 344-365.
Ohman, A., Carlsson, K., Lundqvist, D., & Ingvar, M. (2007). On the unconscious
subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology & Behavior, 92(1-2), 180-185.
65
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
66
Mindfulness Broadens Awareness
Talarico, J. M., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Emotional intensity predicts
autobiographical memory experience. Memory & Cognition, 32(7), 1118-1132.
Teasdale, J. D. (1997). The transformation of meaning: The interacting cognitive
subsystems approach. In M. Power & C. R. Brewin (Eds.), The transformation
of meaning in psychological therapies (pp. 141-156). Chichester: Wiley.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. (1995). How does cognitive therapy
prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness)
training help? Behavior Research and Therapy, 33(1), 25-39.
Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connec-
tion: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(2), 81-88.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emo-
tions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 86(2), 320-333.
Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Barrett, L. F. (2004). Psychological resilience
and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emo-
tions on coping and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1161-1190.
Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2008). Looking happy: The experimental
manipulation of a positive visual attention bias. Emotion, 8(1), 121-126.
Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2010). Fixing our focus: Training attention
to regulate emotion. Personality and Social Psychological Review, 15(1), 75-102.
Watts, A. (1957). The way of Zen. New York: Pantheon Books.
Watts, A. (1961). Psychotherapy East & West. New York: Random House.
Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Nice to know you: Positive emotions,
self-other overlap, and complex understanding in the formation of a new rela-
tionship. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(2), 93-106.
Waugh, C. E., Wager, T. D., Fredrickson, B. L., Noll, D. C., & Taylor, S. F. (2008).
The neural correlates of trait resilience when anticipating and recovering from
threat. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(4), 322-332.
Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern
perception. Science, 322, 115-117.
Witvliet, C. V., Knoll, R. W., Hinman, N. G., & DeYoung, P. A. (2010). Compassion-
focused reappraisal, benefit-focused reappraisal, and rumination after an inter-
personal offense: Emotion- regulation implications for subjective emotion,
linguistic responses, and physiology. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(3),
226-242.
67
CHAPTER 3
Robyn D. Walser
National Center for PTSD and TL Consultation
Services; California
69
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
70
Love and the Human Condition
perhaps can be captured in a few words but is often lived inside of one:
fear. Fear, on the one hand, is essential to our existence. It is the very
thing that can keep us alive. On the other hand, when we extend our
knowledge of fear and all that is related to it and begin to respond to it in
ways that have nothing to do with survival, we come to think of fear
itself as dangerous, something to be feared. We begin to relate to fear in
ways that are not actually in the present moment (see Hayes, Barnes-
Holmes, & Roche, 2001). Fearing the future, fearing the past, fearing
what is out of our control, we lose bits and pieces of our lives to get
control back, to try to not be fearful.
Our relationship with fear can rule our day-to-day activities and can
even come to define our existence. We get caught up in fear in ways that
stretch from our relationships with ourselves to our partnerships, to our
families, to cultures, and to nations. And in this, fear separates, espe-
cially when held and evaluated as a grave and terrible experience, and
when held as weak and wholly unwelcome. We shrink away from what is
unwanted. We can watch fear and separation unfold, starting with the
smallest moment of relationship to the self and growing into fear of
others. It is in this separation from self and others that love is lost.
Fear has great power and can eat to the bone of all who live inside of
it. We can see it in some of the painful stories that we meet in our lives.
A client in therapy reveals that he has sexual thoughts about his same
gender and is so thoroughly fearful, repulsed, and ashamed that he
seethes in constant anger at his own experience and seeks to eliminate
his own thoughts through years of substance abuse, isolation, and ulti-
mately suicide. A daughter standing at the bedside of her dying mother
watches in anguish as her mother slowly slips away. Somebody tells the
daughter she should “be happy” because her mother “is in a better place
now.” Fear of death and powerful emotions have instigated the comment.
However, the feeling of separation and aloneness grow as the comment
sinks into the daughter’s already broken heart; she does not feel happy.
There is the story of a partnership separated by years of silence and dispa-
rate lives, a partner afraid of being rejected and one jealous of the other.
The fear of abandonment grows, yet neither feels able to reach the other.
And on it goes … a family torn by tyranny, a community immersed
in poverty, a culture steeped in violence and war. Each of these experi-
ences possesses its own measure of sorrow. But each also contains the
possibility of love, a possibility that is born in the awareness to the
71
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
experience of pain and fear and is lived out in a choice to respond that is
open, accepting, present, and compassionate.
However, our least fear to our greatest can interfere with our ability
to be present and accepting of ourselves and others. When we work to
escape the experience of fear by trying to make sense of it so it can be
sorted and solved so that we will no longer have it, it seems we will then
be freed to love. In this process, however, we “leave” our experience, we
move away rather than stay, and we live in our minds with the idea of fear
(not the actual fear itself) along with all that it seems to say about us: that
we are weak, unlovable, broken. Our minds, in their vast capacity to con-
struct futures and visit the past, extend beyond the sensations, thoughts,
and emotions of fear, telling us that we are flawed and that we should
separate from ourselves and others until we are no longer fearful. The cost
to this separation, however, is too high. We are no longer open and com-
passionate; we give up the possibility of love, waiting to feel better first.
One goal in ameliorating human suffering then might be to assist
ourselves in turning back toward love—letting go of engaging escape and
staying with ourselves and others when it is most needed, instead. And
as I personally think about it, perhaps, in the end, turning toward love is
the only goal. This process, however, is difficult, as it may require ques-
tioning our own systems of what it means to be human. It may mean that
values and virtues trump feeling good. Engaging the value of being loving
here involves deep listening and awareness to one’s own and another’s
innermost places and making connection with them in a place that
includes, but is also beyond, our verbal understanding. Love involves
being with in places that are uncomfortable, not easily shared, tainted
with shame, humiliation, or embarrassment, and perhaps filled with pain
and fear. How do we open ourselves to these darkest of places and stay
present? We first need to recognize the ubiquity of human pain. In this,
we can cease to hide from our own anger, doubts, and fears. We begin to
see that these experiences also exist in the other. Second, we need to be
aware, to be fully conscious of experience. When we look at the outcome
of fear responded to without awareness, it looks reactionary; we pull away
rapidly like a hand accidentally touching a flame. Responding to pain
and fear with openness and awareness, however, can create the space
needed to allow the fear to come and go … and come and go again.
Allowing—being with—is the key to this process; allowing fear does not
make one broken, it makes one human.
72
Love and the Human Condition
73
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
74
Love and the Human Condition
75
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Being Loving
In the end, we are left with what we choose. When we truly see the
human condition, we come to know that we are not really alone. When
we contact experience, we understand, in a place that is beyond words,
that pain and fear are shared. When we truly know what it means to be
human, when we are fully awake and aware, we simultaneously see our
needs and fears in others and in ourselves. We recognize the human
condition. And it is inside of this recognition that it is good to have fear
and pain—when we no longer see fear and pain as an enemy. Our fear
can tell us what is good and virtuous. If we fear loss, then we seek togeth-
erness, if we fear loneliness then we seek connection, and if we fear
death, then life is here for us to boldly live. All that we might choose for
ourselves—fortitude, sufferance, appreciation, gratitude, grit, tolerance,
strength, or grace—include their own measure of sorrow … and all are
part of love.
Pointing to our humanness is not a call to pessimism. It is quite the
opposite. This is a call to put forth; it is a request to move through, with
awareness, the tribulations that seem to easily hold us back. It is here
that our ability to stay with is most needed. To be able to freely look into
the hearts of self and others and remain open and accepting in moments
of great despair is the foundation of love. Inside of awareness, love can be
felt as a clear and deep emotion, it can be experienced outside of words,
it can lead to a change in the relationship we have with ourselves and
others, purging separation. Love too, can be considered a virtue, an
extension of us in a selfless act of compassion and kindness. Indeed, it
can include an expansive reach of benevolent mattering to all humans
and beyond.
76
Love and the Human Condition
Perhaps this is where ACT and positive psychology are taking the
lead. Each is invested in turning toward a more vital life rather than the
difficult process of eliminating pain and fear. Each reaches to include
self, partnership, family, community, culture, and nation in the reduction
of suffering, while opening to fundamental human wholeness, including
both joy and suffering. Each is optimistic, looking to the values and
virtues that humans engage to support and sustain meaning and purpose.
If we were to weave a thread through all that ACT and positive psychol-
ogy are attempting to do, it would come down to love. And what meaning
and purpose is there but that?
References
Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of positive experience. United
Kingdom: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
Compton, W. C. (2005). Introduction to positive psychology. Thomson-Wadsworth:
Australia.
Chodron, P. (1991). The wisdom of no escape and the path of loving-kindness. Boston:
Shambhala.
Hanh, T. N. (1976). The miracle of mindfulness. Boston: Beacon Press.
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.). (2001). Relational Frame
Theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York:
Plenum Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy: The process and practice of mindful change. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford
Press.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go there you are: Mindfulness meditation in every-
day life. New York: Hyperion.
Kasl, C. (2001). If the Buddha married: Creating enduring relationships on a spiritual
path. New York: Penguin Books.
Merriam-Webster Collegiate Dictionary. (2000). Merriam-Webster.com. Retrieved
March 27, http://www.merriam-webster.com/cgi-bin/book.pl?c11.htm&1
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to
realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York: Free Press.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An intro-
duction. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14.
77
CHAPTER 4
Kristin Neff
University of Texas at Austin
Dennis Tirch
Weill-Cornell Medical College,
American Institute for Cognitive Therapy
What Is Self-Compassion?
Compassion involves sensitivity to the experience of suffering, coupled
with a deep desire to alleviate that suffering (Goertz, Keltner, & Simon-
Thomas, 2010). Self-compassion is simply compassion directed inward.
Drawing on the writings of various Buddhist teachers (e.g., Salzberg
1997), Neff (2003b) has operationalized self-compassion as consisting of
three main elements: kindness, a sense of common humanity, and mind-
fulness. These components combine and mutually interact to create a
self-compassionate frame of mind. Self-compassion is relevant when con-
sidering personal inadequacies, mistakes, and failures, as well as when
confronting painful life situations that are outside of our control.
Self-kindness. Western culture places great emphasis on being kind to
our friends, family, and neighbors who are struggling. Not so when it
comes to ourselves. When we make a mistake or fail in some way, we may
be more likely to beat ourselves up than put a supportive arm around our
own shoulder. And even when our problems stem from forces beyond our
control, such as an accident or traumatic event, we often focus more on
fixing the problem than calming and comforting ourselves. Western
culture often sends the message that strong individuals should be like
John Wayne—stoic and silent toward their own suffering. Unfortunately,
these attitudes rob us of one of our most powerful coping mechanisms
when dealing with the difficulties of life—the ability to comfort our-
selves when we’re hurting and in need of care.
Self-kindness refers to the tendency to be supportive and sympa-
thetic toward ourselves when noticing personal shortcomings rather than
being harshly critical. It entails relating to our mistakes and failings with
tolerance and understanding, and recognizing that perfection is unat-
tainable. Self-compassion is expressed in internal dialogues that are
benevolent and encouraging rather than cruel or disparaging. Instead of
berating ourselves for being inadequate, we offer ourselves warmth and
unconditional acceptance. Instead of getting fixated in a problem-solving
mode and ignoring our own suffering, we pause to emotionally comfort
79
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
80
Self-Compassion and ACT
81
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
new behaviors become possible. Like a clear, still pool without ripples,
mindfulness mirrors what’s occurring, without distortion. This allows us
to take a wiser and more objective perspective on ourselves and our lives.
82
Self-Compassion and ACT
One easy way to help clients soothe and comfort themselves when
they’re feeling emotional distress is to encourage them to give them-
selves a gentle hug or caress, or simply put their hand on their heart
and feel its warmth. What’s important is to make a clear gesture that
conveys feelings of love, care, and tenderness. If other people are
around, it’s possible to fold one’s arms in a nonobvious way, gently
squeezing in a comforting manner. Research indicates that soothing
touch releases oxytocin, provides a sense of security, soothes distress-
ing emotions, and calms cardiovascular stress (Goetz et al., 2010).
83
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
84
Self-Compassion and ACT
85
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
can’t meet their goals. Research shows that self-compassionate people are
less afraid of failure (Neff, Hsieh, & Dejitterat, 2005) because they know
they won’t face a barrage of self-criticism if they do fail. They are also
more likely to reengage in new goals after failure, meaning they’re better
able to pick themselves up and try again (Neely, Schallert, Mohammed,
Roberts & Chen, 2009). In a series of four experimental studies, Breines
and Chen (2012) used mood inductions to engender feelings of self-com-
passion for personal weaknesses, failures, and past moral transgressions.
Compared to various control conditions, self-compassion resulted in
more motivation to change for the better, try harder to learn, and avoid
repeating past mistakes. Self-compassion has also been linked to greater
personal growth initiative (Neff, Rude, & Kirkpatrick, 2007), which
Robitschek (1998) defines as being actively involved in making changes
needed for a more productive and fulfilling life.
Self-compassion has been found to promote health-related behaviors
such as sticking to a diet (Adams & Leary, 2007), smoking cessation
(Kelly et al., 2009), and starting a physical fitness regimen (Magnus,
Kowalski, & McHugh, 2010). In addition, self-compassionate individuals
demonstrate a greater ability to adjust and cope effectively with persis-
tent musculoskeletal pain (Wren et al., 2012). Thus, self-compassion
appears to enhance both physical and mental well-being.
86
Self-Compassion and ACT
87
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Self-Compassion and
Interpersonal Functioning
While there is evidence that self-compassion psychologically benefits the
individual, there is also evidence that self-compassion enhances inter-
personal relationships (Yarnell & Neff, 2012). In a study of heterosexual
couples (Neff & Beretvas, 2012), for example, self-compassionate indi-
viduals were described by their partners as being more emotionally con-
nected, accepting, and autonomy supporting while being less detached,
controlling, and verbally or physically aggressive in their relationship
than those lacking self-compassion. Not surprisingly, the partners of self-
compassionate individuals also reported being more satisfied with their
relationship. Because self-compassionate people meet many of their own
needs for care and support, they have more emotional resources available
to give compassion to relationship partners. Because they accept and
validate themselves, they don’t need to gain approval from others to
maintain a sense of self-worth. This suggests that teaching skills of self-
compassion when working with couples experiencing relationship diffi-
culties would be an effective way to break patterns of emotional neediness,
anger, control, and ego- defensiveness. It would also be a means to
enhance intimacy and mutual support among couples.
An interesting question concerns whether self-compassionate people
are more compassionate toward others. In one of the few studies on the
topic, Neff and Pommier (2012) examined the link between self-
compassion and empathy, personal distress, and forgiveness among
college undergraduates, an older community sample, and individuals
practicing Buddhist meditation. Among all three groups,
88
Self-Compassion and ACT
Origins of Self-Compassion
While research on this topic is new, there is some evidence to support
Gilbert and Iron’s (2005) contention that self-compassion is associated
with the care-giving system. In a study of adolescents and young adults,
Neff and McGehee (2010) found that maternal support was associated
with significantly greater self-compassion, while maternal criticism was
linked to less self-compassion. Self-compassion levels were also signifi-
cantly predicted by degree of family functioning more generally.
89
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Self-Compassion and
Clinical Interventions
There is some evidence to suggest that self-compassion may be an impor-
tant process in psychotherapeutic change. For example, increased self-
compassion has been found to be a key mechanism in the effectiveness
of mindfulness-based interventions such as Mindfulness-Based Cognitive
Therapy (MBCT) and Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR;
Baer, 2010). Jon Kabat-Zinn (1991) originally developed MBSR in the
late 1970s, and it is probably the most common mindfulness intervention
program offered worldwide. MBSR is an experiential learning program
that includes weekly group sessions, regular home practice, and a core
curriculum of formal and informal mindfulness practices. This core cur-
riculum was later incorporated into MBCT as an adaptation for prevent-
ing depressive relapse (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2001), and additional
psycho-education and exercises specific to depression were included.
Meta-analyses indicate that MBSR and MBCT are related to improved
outcomes in participants dealing with a variety of stressors and health
problems (Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004).
Shapiro, Astin, Bishop, and Cordova (2005) found that health care
professionals who took an MBSR program reported significantly
90
Self-Compassion and ACT
91
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
92
Self-Compassion and ACT
Self-Compassion from an
ACT Perspective
ACT practitioners and researchers have been exploring the role of self-
compassion in psychotherapy for some time now, though self-compassion
has yet to be integrated as a formal component of the ACT process
model (Forsyth & Eifert, 2008; Hayes, 2008; Luoma, Kohlenberg, Hayes,
& Fletcher, 2012; Tirch, 2010; Van Dam et al., 2011).
In order to approach an understanding of self-compassion from an
ACT perspective, we need to spend some time examining relational
frame theory (RFT), the underlying theory of cognition that ACT is
derived from. Among many other mental phenomena, RFT describes the
processes of mindfulness, self-development, and perspective taking. It
provides a behavioral account of language and cognition that can provide
a useful way of considering how humans may develop a sense of self and
a sense of others, and how we experience ourselves as situated in time
93
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
94
Self-Compassion and ACT
your experience, moment by moment, for a very long time. This is some-
times referred to as “the observing self,” or a “transcendent self” but is
most often referred to in ACT as “self as context” (Hayes, Strosahl, &
Wilson, 1999).
How is it that this “observer self,” distinct from an experiencing self,
arises? In order to understand this, let’s turn again to ACT’s roots in
research on human language and cognition, RFT. Part of human rela-
tional responding involves trained capacities for perspective taking.
Through these processes, our experience of being involves a sense of
ourselves as a perspective before which the entirety of our experience
unfolds, throughout our whole lives. This sense of ourselves as an
observer is referred to as self as context in ACT because this experiential
sense of self serves as the context within which our experiences are con-
tained (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). This sense of an “observing
self” is important because while this observer can notice the contents of
consciousness, it is not those contents themselves. We have a thought,
but we are more than that experience, just in the way that we have arms,
but we are more than just our arms. Emotions don’t feel themselves,
thoughts don’t observe themselves, and physical pain doesn’t experience
itself. Throughout our lives, we can notice the presence of an “observing
self,” before which all of our experiences arise, exist, and disappear in
time.
Upon considering the relationship between self-as-context and self-
compassion, we can note that returning to an awareness of self-as-context
offers us a nonattached and disidentified relationship to our experiences.
This allows the habitual stimulus functions of our painful private events
and stories to hold less influence over us. From the perspective of the
I-Here-Nowness of being, I can view my own suffering as I might view
the suffering of another and be touched by the pain in that experience,
without the dominant interference of my verbal learning history, with its
potential for shaming self-evaluations (Vilardaga, 2009; Hayes, 2008).
Steven Hayes (2008) has suggested that compassion may be the only
value that is inherent in the ACT model of psychological well-being.
According to him, we may find the roots of self-compassion and compas-
sion emerging from six core processes described in the ACT model of
psychotherapeutic change, sometimes known as “hexaflex” processes
(Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). These six processes work
together in an interactive way to:
95
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Dahl, Plumb, Stewart, & Lundgren (2009) have outlined how these
hexaflex processes directly affect compassion for self and others. According
to their model, self-compassion involves our ability to willingly experience
difficult emotions; to mindfully observe our self-evaluative, distressing,
and shaming thoughts without allowing them to dominate our behavior
or our states of mind; to engage more fully in our life’s pursuits with self-
kindness and self-validation; and to flexibly shift our perspective toward a
broader, transcendent sense of self (Hayes, 2008).
Hexaflex processes are seen as fundamental elements of the ACT
model of psychological well-being, which is known as “psychological flex-
ibility” (Hayes et al., 2006). Psychological flexibility may be defined as
“the ability to contact the present moment more fully as a conscious
human being, and based on what the situation affords, to change or
persist in behavior in order to serve valued ends” (Luoma, Hayes, &
Walser, 2007, p. 17). Like self-compassion, psychological flexibility is
strongly negatively correlated with depression, anxiety, and psychopa-
thology and is highly positively correlated with quality of life (Kashdan
& Rottenberg, 2010).
96
Self-Compassion and ACT
97
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Interplay of Mindfulness,
Self-Compassion, and
Psychological Flexibility
The elements of common humanity, kindness, and mindfulness are
involved in each of the hexaflex processes elaborated upon by ACT
theory, yet self-compassion also involves an intentional turning of these
processes decidedly toward the alleviation of human suffering.
Accordingly, self-compassion may account for more of the variance in
psychopathology than mindfulness alone (Kuyken et al., 2010). Recently,
Van Dam and colleagues (2011) found that self-compassion accounted
for as much as 10 times more unique variance in psychological health
than a measure of mindfulness did in a large community sample. When
we consider the role of self-compassion in ACT, there is a temptation to
find a way to fit self-compassion into the hexaflex model. It is important
to remember that the hexaflex concepts are meant to be clinically appli-
cable, midlevel terms, which describe the underlying principles of RFT
in relatively everyday language. The hexaflex components are useful
descriptors, but they do not need to represent all and everything that is
involved in human well-being and psychological flexibility. What distin-
guishes contextual behavioral science (CBS) is the application of funda-
mental behavioral principles to account for the prediction and influence
of human behavior. As we will describe, further CBS research may help
us to work in more effective ways with the powerful psychotherapy
98
Self-Compassion and ACT
Conclusion
ACT is consistent with Neff’s conceptualization of self-compassion in
multiple ways, and each approach to understanding psychological resil-
ience has something to offer the other. Although ACT’s client protocols
are generally presented in user-friendly language, the underlying behav-
ioral theory and clinician literature that ACT is based upon can be chal-
lenging for therapists who are not coming from the behavior analytic
tradition. Many find that RFT has a rather steep learning curve, given
the range of new terms and concepts. ACT practitioners may benefit
from the straightforward, direct, and understandable language and con-
ceptual explanations that have emerged from the self-compassion litera-
ture. Also, interventions based upon theories of self-compassion and
compassion such as the MSC program (Neff & Germer, 2012) and CFT
(Gilbert, 2010b) might provide ACT clinicians with a range of effective,
ACT-consistent procedures that involve the movement of common psy-
chotherapy change processes.
Similarly, research on self-compassion may benefit from examining
ways in which self-compassion is associated with ACT constructs such as
acceptance, perspective taking, and psychological flexibility. The ACT-
consistent goals of prediction and influence of human behavior, and pre-
cision, depth, and scope in functional analysis are scientifically healthy
complements to the growing body of research on self-compassion.
Obviously, ongoing clinically relevant research on all of the emer-
gent mindfulness- and compassion-informed psychotherapies is needed.
In particular, there is a need for further randomized controlled trials that
are designed with an awareness of the importance of mediational analy-
ses for psychotherapy process variables. This is true for ACT, CFT, and
MSC. By employing mediational analyses we might examine the degree
to which psychological flexibility and self-compassion serve as active
process variables in these therapies. Component analyses of ACT inter-
ventions, which examine the relative contribution of different hexaflex
99
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
100
Self-Compassion and ACT
References
Adams, C. E., & Leary, M. R. (2007). Promoting self-compassionate attitudes
toward eating among restrictive and guilty eaters. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 26, 1120-1144.
Baer, R. A. (2010). Self-compassion as a mechanism of change in mindfulness-and
acceptance-based treatments. In R. A. Baer (Ed.), Assessing mindfulness and
acceptance processes in clients: Illuminating the theory and practice of change (pp.
135-153). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.
Barnard, L. K., & Curry, J. F. (2012). The relationship of clergy burnout to self-
compassion and other personality dimensions. Pastoral Psychology, 61,
149–163.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayes, S. C., & Dymond, S. (2001). Self and self-directed rules.
In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory:
A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 119-139). New
York: Plenum.
Birnie, K., Speca, M., Carlson, L. E. (2010). Exploring Self-compassion and Empathy
in the Context of Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (MBSR). Stress and
Health, 26, 359-371.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., …
Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230-241.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H.
K., Waltz, T., & Zettle, R. D. (in press). Preliminary psychometric properties of
the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire—II: A revised measure of psycho-
logical flexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness
and its role in psychological well- being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 822-848.
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological
Bulletin, 130, 392-414.
101
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Dahl, J. C., Plumb, J. C., Stewart, I., & Lundgren, T. (2009). The art and science of
valuing in psychotherapy: Helping clients discover, explore, and commit to valued
action using acceptance and commitment therapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Forsyth, J. P., & Eifert, G. H. (2008). The mindfulness & acceptance workbook for
anxiety: A guide to breaking free from anxiety, phobias, and worry using Acceptance
and Commitment Therapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Gilbert, P. (2009). The compassionate mind. London: Constable.
Gilbert, P. (2010a). An introduction to compassion focused therapy in cognitive
behavior therapy. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3(2), 97-112.
Gilbert, P. (2010b). Compassion focused therapy: Distinctive features. New York:
Routledge.
Gilbert, P. & Irons, C. (2005). Focused therapies and compassionate mind training
for shame and self-attacking. In P. Gilbert, (Ed.), Compassion: Conceptualisations,
research and use in psychotherapy. London: Routledge.
Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high
shame and self-criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach.
Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13, 353-379.
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolution-
ary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351-374.
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based
stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research, 57(1), 35-43.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York:
Guilford Press.
Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K.D., & Wilson, K.G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. T. (2001). Relational frame theory: A
post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy: Model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 44(1), 1-25.
Hayes, S. C. (1984). Making sense of spirituality. Behaviorism, 12, 99-110.
Hayes, S. C. (2008a). The roots of compassion. Keynote address presented at the
fourth Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Summer Institute, Chicago, IL.
http://www.globalpres.com/mediasite/Viewer/?peid=017fe6ef4b1544279d8cf27
adbe92a51
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness and compas-
sion meditation: Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical Psychology
Review, 31, 1126-1132.
Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1987). Personal and contextual determinants of
coping strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(5), 946-955.
Hollis-Walker, L., & Colosimo, K. (2011). Mindfulness, self-compassion, and happi-
ness in non-meditators: A theoretical and empirical examination. Personality
and Individual Differences, 50(2), 222-227.
102
Self-Compassion and ACT
Horney, K. (1950). Neurosis and human growth: The struggle toward self-realization.
New York: Norton.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1991). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind
to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Dell.
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental
aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 865-878.
Kelly, A. C., Zuroff, D. C., Foa, C. L., & Gilbert, P. (2010). Who benefits from train-
ing in self-compassionate self-regulation? A study of smoking reduction. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 727-755.
Kernis, M. H., Paradise, A. W., Whitaker, D. J., Wheatman, S. R., & Goldman, B.
N. (2000). Master of one’s psychological domain? Not likely if one’s self-esteem
is unstable. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1297-1305.
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, R. S., Byford, S., …
Dalgleish, T. (2010). How does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work?
Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 1105-1112.
Lapsley, D. K., FitzGerald, D. P., Rice, K. G., & Jackson, S. (1989). Separation-
individuation and the “New Look” at the imaginary audience and personal
fable: A test of an integrative model. Journal of Adolescent Research, 4,
483-505.
Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., & Hancock, J. (2007). Self-
compassion and reactions to unpleasant self-relevant events: The implications
of treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
887- 904.
Lee, W. K. & Bang, H. L. (2010) Effects of mindfulness-based group intervention
on the mental health of middle-aged Korean women in community. Stress and
Health, 26, 341–348.
Luoma, J. B., Hayes, S. C., & Walser, R. D. (2007). Learning ACT: An Acceptance
& Commitment Therapy skills-training manual for therapists. Oakland, CA: New
Harbinger.
Luoma, J. B., Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Fletcher, L. (2012). Slow and steady
wins the race: A randomized clinical trial of acceptance and commitment
therapy targeting shame in substance use disorders. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 80, 43-53.
MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: A meta-analysis of the
association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology
Review, 32, 545-552.
Magnus, C. M. R., Kowalski, K. C., & McHugh, T.-L. F. (2010). The role of self-
compassion in women’s self- determined motives to exercise and exercise-
related outcomes. Self and Identity, 9, 363-382.
Martin, M. M., Staggers, S. M., & Anderson, C. M. (2011). The relationships
between cognitive flexibility with dogmatism, intellectual flexibility, prefer-
ence for consistency, and self-compassion. Communication Research Reports,
28, 275-280.
103
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Neely, M. E., Schallert, D. L., Mohammed, S. S., Roberts, R. M., & Chen, Y. (2009).
Self-kindness when facing stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation,
and support in college students’ well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 33(1),
88-97.
Neff, K. D. (2003a). Development and validation of a scale to measure self-
compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223-250.
Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy
attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85-102.
Neff, K. D. (2009). Self-compassion. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook
of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 561-573). New York: Guilford
Press.
Neff, K. D., & Beretvas, S. N. (2012). The role of self-compassion in romantic rela-
tionships. Self and Identity. DOI:10.1080/15298868.2011.639548
Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. K. (2012). A pilot study and randomized controlled trial
of the Mindful Self-Compassion Program. Journal of Clinical Psychology. DOI:
10.1002/jclp.21923
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y., & Dejitterat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, achievement goals,
and coping with academic failure. Self and Identity, 4, 263-287.
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and its link to
adaptive psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41,
139-154.
Neff, K. D. & McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and psychological resilience
among adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity, 9, 225-240.
Neff, K. D., & Pommier, E. (2012). The relationship between self-compassion and
other-focused concern among college undergraduates, community adults, and
practicing meditators. Self and Identity. DOI:10.1080/15298868.2011.649546
Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2007). An examination of self-
compassion in relation to positive psychological functioning and personality
traits. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 908-916.
Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self-esteem: Two dif-
ferent ways of relating to oneself. Journal of Personality, 77, 23-50.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the dura-
tion of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74, 116-143.
Powers, T., Koestner, R., & Zuroff, D. C. (2007). Self-criticism, goal motivation, and
goal progress. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25, 826-840.
Rimes, K. A., & Wingrove, J. (2011). Pilot study of Mindfulness-Based Cognitive
Therapy for trainee clinical psychologists. Behavioural and Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 39, 235-241.
Robins, C. J., Keng, S., Ekblad, A. G., & Brantley, J. G. (2012). Effects of mindful-
ness-based stress reduction on emotional experience and expression: A ran-
domized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 117-131.
Robitschek, C. (1998). Personal growth initiative: The construct and its measure.
Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 30, 183–198.
104
Self-Compassion and ACT
Rockcliff et al. (2008). A pilot exploration of heart rate variability and salivary
cortisol responses to compassion-focused imagery. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 5,
132-139.
Salzberg, S. (1997). A heart as wide as the world. Boston: Shambhala.
Sbarra, D. A., Smith, H. L., & Mehl, M. R. (2012). When leaving your ex, love
yourself: Observational ratings of self-compassion predict the course of emo-
tional recovery following marital separation. Psychological Science, 23,
261-269.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Mindfulness-based Cognitive
Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York:
Guilford Press.
Shapira, L. B., & Mongrain, M. (2010). The benefits of self-compassion and opti-
mism exercises for individuals vulnerable to depression. The Journal of Positive
Psychology, 5(5), 377-389.
Shapiro, S. L., Astin, J. A., Bishop, S. R., & Cordova, M. (2005). Mindfulness-based
stress reduction for health care professionals: Results from a randomized trial.
International Journal of Stress Management, 12, 164-176.
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., & Biegel, G. M (2007). Teaching self-care to caregiv-
ers: Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of ther-
apists in training. Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 1, 105-115.
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K., Thoresen, C., & Plante, T. G. (2011). The moderation of
mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: Results from a
randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67, 267-277.
Swann, W. B. (1996). Self-traps: The elusive quest for higher self-esteem. New York: W.
H. Freeman.
Tesser, A. (1999). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior.
In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), The self in social psychology (pp. 446-460). New York:
Psychology Press.
Tirch, D. (2010). Mindfulness as a context for the cultivation for compassion.
International Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 3, 113-123.
Törneke, N. (2010). Learning RFT: An introduction to relational frame theory and its
clinical applications. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Van Dam, N., Sheppard, S. C., Forsyth, J. C., & Earleywine, M. (2011). Self-
compassion is a better predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and
quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25,
123–130.
Vettese, L. C., Dyer, C. E., Li, W. L., & Wekerle, C. (2011). Does self-compassion
mitigate the association between childhood maltreatment and later emotional
regulation difficulties? A preliminary investigation. International Journal of
Mental Health and Addiction, 9, 480-491.
Vilardaga, R. (2009). A relational frame theory account of empathy. The
International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 5, 178-184.
Wang, S. (2005). A conceptual framework for integrating research related to the
physiology of compassion and the wisdom of Buddhist teachings. In P. Gilbert
105
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
106
CHAPTER 5
Perspective Taking
Ian Stewart
National University of Ireland, Galway
Louise McHugh
University College Dublin
Introduction
Perspective taking is seen as a crucially important social skill that starts
in early infancy and develops throughout childhood and beyond (e.g.,
Baron- Cohen, 1994). In positive psychology, perspective taking and
related phenomena are allotted key theoretical importance. For example,
perspective taking itself is one of the 24 key characteristics listed in
Peterson and Seligman (2004) and seems related to others including
open mindedness, kindness, social intelligence, forgiveness, fairness, and
self-control.
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
108
Perspective Taking
109
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
110
Perspective Taking
111
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
think to myself “That was stupid,” after making a mistake, for example,
then I, HERE and NOW am judging myself THERE and THEN (making
the error). In summary, deictic frames establish a constant division
between the speaker, who is always HERE and NOW, and the spoken
about, which is THERE and THEN.
Empirical Support
Evidence supporting the RFT account of perspective taking as
deictic relational framing has been accumulating. McHugh, Barnes-
Holmes, & Barnes-Holmes (2004) employed a protocol that focused on
the three perspective-taking frames (I-YOU, HERE-THERE, and NOW-
THEN), in conjunction with three levels of relational complexity (simple,
reversed, and double-reversed relations) to provide a developmental
profile of the deictic framing skills of individuals across different age
groups. Findings indicated a clear developmental trend. Young children
(aged 3–5) produced more errors than all of the older age groups (ranging
from 6–30). Furthermore, these differences were broadly consistent with
the mainstream cognitive-developmental literature that has reported
that performances on simple theory of mind tasks generally develop
across the ages of four and five years old, and are usually well established
by age six.
Further research has examined how deictic relational framing might
be involved in a variety of perspective-dependent skills, including false
belief understanding and deception. False belief understanding requires
the ability to attribute an incorrect belief to another person and to dis-
criminate when he or she is acting on that belief. False belief understand-
ing is seen as an important milestone in cognitive development as well as
a prerequisite for understanding deception, which requires the deliberate
creation of a false belief in another (e.g., Perner, Leekam, & Wimmer,
1987). McHugh, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, and Stewart (2006)
investigated the role of deictic framing in true and false belief under-
standing, and McHugh, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, Stewart, and
Dymond (2007) conducted a relational frame analysis of deception. Both
studies showed clear developmental trends in the performances of par-
ticipants, aged from early childhood to adulthood, that were supported
112
Perspective Taking
113
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
114
Perspective Taking
115
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
116
Perspective Taking
117
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
118
Perspective Taking
or her future self more similarly to the way in which he or she responds
to his or her present self so that a choice between less for the present “I”
and more for the future “I” might be more likely to result in a self-
controlled rather than an impulsive response. Future RFT research
might examine the developmental trajectory of temporal deictic relations
in more detail and assess the effect of training such relations, particularly
with regard to the outcome of self-control.
119
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
and Hayes (2009), by using questions such as “If you were in her place,
how would you feel?” After the empathic response is established, further
work may be needed to support particular patterns of responding beyond
the felt emotion. For example, the child might need to be taught how
best to help the other person. In addition, he or she might need to be
taught to track rules linking helping behavior with relatively delayed
consequences (e.g., “If you help your sister then she’ll be happier and the
whole family will enjoy the day more”).
The above sequence constitutes a possible guide for purposes of
research and training in applied arenas. One possible area of relevance is
bullying. Sutton, Smith, and Swettenham (1999) argue that bullying
among children or adolescents often takes place in a complex social
context involving jostling for power among members of a bullying gang in
which there is a need by the lead bully to maintain social allegiance by
members and that in such interactions, advanced perspective-taking skills
are needed at least by the leader. In addition, indirect methods of bullying
involving social exclusion and rumormongering would also seem to require
well-developed perspective taking. Despite the latter, however, bullies
show low levels of empathy. In some rare cases (i.e., individuals classified
as having “antisocial personality disorder”) there may be genetic abnor-
malities causing extremely low levels of emotional distress that allow little
or no transformation of aversive emotional functions through deictic rela-
tions. Such individuals may be incapable of sharing the distress of another
and thus difficult or impossible to treat, at least with respect to the possi-
bility of instilling empathy. However, most often there are probably other
reasons levels of empathy on the part of bullies are relatively low, such as
particular patterns of relational framing with respect to self or other by
these individuals in the bullying context or more broadly that make the
transformation of functions less likely. For example, perhaps the bully feels
emotion but deliberately suppresses it to avoid losing others’ respect.
Individuals with autistic spectrum and other developmental disor-
ders may also show extreme lack of empathy. In this case, the primary
cause is deficits in relational framing, especially deictic relational framing,
which may therefore need to be trained using procedures such as those
described earlier in this chapter. Once this repertoire is in place further
emotion-relevant training will be needed to properly establish the trans-
formation of emotional functions via deictic relations; however, such
training will be more similar to that needed for other populations.
120
Perspective Taking
SELF-AS-CONTENT
Self-as-content (conceptualized self) consists of elaborate descriptive
and evaluative relational networks that a person constructs about himself
and his history over time (e.g., “I am a husband and father. I’m a bit
standoffish. I love baseball and am a big fan of the Atlanta Braves”). Self-
evaluations are always made HERE and NOW about our behaviors that
occur THERE and THEN. However, we rarely attend to the process of
interpreting and evaluating as it happens in the present moment.
Difficulties occur when products of relational responding (e.g., thoughts,
beliefs, judgments) are treated as objectively true and inherent aspects of
the real world, a process referred to within ACT as cognitive fusion. This
can be problematic when self-evaluations come to appear as historically
121
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
rooted and unchangeable; our stories may become rigid and ossified and
no longer simply describe our past behavior but also guide future behav-
ior in directions that maintain the coherence of the story.
SELF-AS-PROCESS
Self-as-process (knowing self) is the ongoing verbal discrimination
of psychological events as they occur (e.g., “I feel sad”). The knowing self
feeds the conceptualized self (e.g., If I discriminate that I frequently feel
sad then I may describe myself as “a depressed person”) and is also neces-
sary to contact the transcendent self since a self-monitoring repertoire is
required to observe the observer.
The knowing self is extremely useful in behavioral regulation both
for the socioverbal community and for the person him- or herself. It
allows others to predict a person’s behavior without knowledge of his or
her learning history. For example, if someone says that she feels angry
then this allows a level of prediction of her behavior. Self-as-process is
also critical in the psychological development of the individual. In order
to respond effectively to one’s own responding, one must first be aware of
the response and its impact. For example, understanding and responding
to my thoughts and feelings about other people’s behavior in a fluid and
flexible manner is critical for establishing personal relationships.
Self-as-process may be underdeveloped if there is inadequate training
by the verbal community, such as when awareness and expression of emo-
tions, thoughts, and sensation are punished, ignored, denied, or contra-
dicted, as is often observed in the case of child neglect or abuse. Weak
self-knowledge may also be the result of experiential avoidance, the ten-
dency to avoid or escape aversive experiences. Chronic experiential avoid-
ance results in the difficulty observing and describing one’s thoughts,
emotions, and sensations seen in disorders such as depression for example.
SELF-AS-CONTEXT
Self-as-context (transcendent self) is the invariant in all self-
discriminations. If someone answers many different questions about
himself and his behavior, then the only aspect of his answering that will
be consistent across time is the context from which the answer is given,
that is, “I, HERE and NOW.” Since self-as-context is an abstraction from
122
Perspective Taking
123
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
person is more likely to reveal his or her feelings (e.g., “I’m a little disap-
pointed to hear you say that”). Other-as-context (the transcendent other)
is relatively uncommon, occurring when the speaker is psychologically
connected to the listener as a purely conscious person. Transcendence is
always experienced as “HERE and NOW” and thus at this level speaker
and listener are one, since “HERE and NOW” cannot be more than one
event by definition. As such, self-as-context is automatically linked with
the transcendent other, and thus the term “self-other-as-context” or
“transcendent self-other” can be used to describe this perspective.
124
Perspective Taking
Conclusion
Positive psychology aims to “achieve a scientific understanding and effec-
tive interventions to build thriving in individuals, families, and communi-
ties” (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000, p. 13). Contextual behavioral
science, which includes both RFT and ACT, is an empirically based and
practically oriented approach to psychology that has begun to provide new
insights into the behavioral origins of key categories of behavior in which
125
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
References
Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayes, S. C., & Dymond, S. (2001). Self and self-directed rules.
In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory:
A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 119-139). New
York: Plenum.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mech-
anisms in mind reading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 13, 513–552.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a
“theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
Dymond, S., Roche, B., & De Houwer, J. In press. Advances in Relational Frame
Theory: Research and Application. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Empathy and sympathy. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (677-691). New York: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-
skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.
Hayes, S. C. (1995). Knowing selves. The Behavior Therapist, 18, 94-96.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy:
The process and practice of mindful change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Heagle, A. I., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Teaching perspective-taking skills to typi-
cally developing children through derived relational responding. Journal of
Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 3(1), 1–34.
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and
justice. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Teaching children with autism to
mind-read: A practical guide. Chichester: Wiley.
Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P., & Catts, S. (2001). Visual and cognitive
perspective-taking impairments in schizophrenia: A failure of allocentric sim-
ulation? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6(4), 241-269.
Lattal, K. A. (1975). Reinforcement contingencies as discriminative stimuli. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 241-246.
126
Perspective Taking
McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Dymond, S.
(2007). Deictic relational complexity and the development of deception. The
Psychological Record, 57, 517-531.
McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004) Perspective-taking
as relational responding: A developmental profile. The Psychological Record, 54,
115-144.
McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2006). False
belief as generalised operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 56, 341-364.
Perner, J., Leekam, S., & Wimmer, H. (1987) Three year olds’ difficulty with false
belief. The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 5, 125-137.
Perner J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” Attribution
of second-order beliefs by 5-to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook
and classification. Washington, D.C.: APA Press and Oxford University Press.
Rehfeldt, R., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., & Kowalchuck, R. (2007). Assessing
relational learning deficits in perspective- taking in children with high-
functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23-47.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An intro-
duction. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14.
Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Vintage.
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., Swettenham, J. (1999). Social cognition and bullying: Social
inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
17(3), 435-450.
Valdivia-Salas, S., Luciano, C., Gutierrez-Martinez, O., & Visdomine, C. (2009).
Establishing empathy. In R, A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes. (Eds.), Derived
relational responding applications for learners with autism and other developmental
disabilities. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Vilardaga, R., Estévez, A., Levin, M. E., & Hayes, S. C. (2012). Deictic relational
responding, empathy and experiential avoidance as predictors of social anhe-
donia: Further contributions from relational frame theory. The Psychological
Record, 62, 409-432.
Vilardaga, R., & Hayes, S. C. (2009). Experiential avoidance and superstition:
Considering concepts in context. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology, 15 (3),
269-271.
Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2008).
Assessing perspective taking in schizophrenia using Relational Frame Theory.
The Psychological Record, 60, 413-424.
Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2010).
Adopting the perspective of another in belief attribution: Contribution of
Relational Frame Theory to the understanding of impairments in schizophre-
nia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 125-134.
Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. (2011). Establishing a deictic relational
repertoire in young children. The Psychological Record, 61, 371-390.
127
CHAPTER 6
Committed Action
129
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
instance. Indeed food is better if it is neither too hot nor too cold. Experience
and ideas like these are, likely, part of the impetus behind “pacing.”
Setting aside that pacing possibly “lacks consensus of definition,”
let’s take just one meaning of the term. According to the ordinary daily
use of the term, “pacing” means to set or regulate the rate of an activity,
particularly so that an activity can be continued without needing to stop.
In chronic pain treatment settings pacing is contrasted with “avoidance”
and what is variously called “overactivity cycling,” “overactivity-
underactivity cycling,” “boom and bust,” and so forth. Once again,
pacing is trying to be somewhere between doing too little, almost all of
the time, and doing too much, at least intermittently. Pacing methods
teach patients to break up activities into pieces, to use rest breaks, to slow
down, to use switching of tasks, and to do what is called the “3 Ps,” “pri-
oritize, plan, and pace.” It all sounds reasonable, but when these patterns
of activity are studied it is typically found that they are associated more
with patterns of avoidance, distress, and disability than with patterns of
healthy engagement and positive well-being (McCracken & Samuel,
2007). In some studies such patterns seem simply to bear no relation to
daily functioning (Karsdorp & Vlaeyen, 2009), which of course should
not be the case if they are meant to serve the improvement of daily func-
tioning. Here the “happy medium” in activity seems not to be the answer,
but how does that happen?
One of the problems with pacing is that it derives mainly from
common sense and not from a deep understanding of how behavior is
actually coordinated. As a method based in common sense, training in
pacing probably does not typically appreciate the complexity of the
behavior change challenge it is trying to tackle. In a sense common sense
is not enough. One problem not appreciated is the strength and durabil-
ity of influences on behavior that coordinate avoidance. For example, if
people suffering chronic pain are fearful and avoidant, instructions and
reassurance are unlikely to change that in many cases (Linton,
McCracken, & Vlaeyen, 2008). More potent and precise methods are
needed. Attempting to alter surface features of behavior, however it is
structured, such as in rate and pattern over time, may do nothing to
undermine the influence of feelings of pain, fear, and anxiety, and the
content of thoughts. And, in people who are seeking treatment for
chronic pain and pain-related disability it is virtually assured that their
thoughts harbor avoidant influences. To say it another way, attempts to
130
Committed Action
131
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
as with a high rate of activity that leads to isolation and inactivity, then
this pattern reflects psychological inflexibility by definition. So in the
context of pain, both overdoing it and avoidance can function as avoid-
ance and can reflect psychological inflexibility. Both can lack openness,
have qualities of disconnection with direct experience, and may not
serve goals and values.
132
Committed Action
133
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
134
Committed Action
135
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Exercise
First, do appropriate preparations, provide some guidance on character-
istics of useful goals (i.e., specific, measurable, attainable, relevant, and
time limited), and then have participants sit with eyes closed and focus
on your instructions. Next ask them to imagine a goal they have, to
picture what it would be like to achieve it, and then ask them to notice
what happens as they do that, pausing appropriately. Typically,
136
Committed Action
137
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
reaching are being done persistently and flexibly as ongoing actions. This
inevitably means that patients engage in these between sessions and the
therapist and patient either observe that these qualities are being
achieved or continue to address the stumbling blocks that happen until
they are.
Summary
Committed action is action that is present focused, linked to values, and
part of a pattern of larger activity that similarly serves one’s values. It is
sometimes referred to as choosing and choosing again. It is not primarily
a kind of pledge for the future. It is a way to create flexible persistence
where the behavior pattern might otherwise have the quality of choose,
experience an unwanted result, and give up. As it is values based and
action oriented it is akin to the domain of “the engaged life” in current
conceptual frameworks of positive psychology (Duckworth et al., 2005).
Committed action toward goals that we care about inevitably leads
to potential barriers in the form of feelings and thoughts, and this is
when methods in ACT that enhance acceptance and cognitive defusion
can be very useful. In an overall pattern of successful committed action,
thought and feelings, experience of failure, and so forth are not things to
be avoided; they are things to be incorporated as part and parcel of the
whole pattern of action.
An example was chosen for analysis here; this is the method of train-
ing in pacing that is done almost universally in pain management centers
that employ psychological methods. Accepting that there can be differ-
ent meanings of the word pacing and a range of methods focused on the
same, there is nonetheless a lesson to learn from the approach of pacing.
Pacing is an amazingly commonsense approach. It also has been around
for more than 30 years—and it has essentially no evidence. Pacing
attempts to create a behavior pattern that includes persistent engage-
ment, the capacity to pursue an activity without the need to stop. One of
the lessons from pacing seems to be that pacing is too weak as a behavior
change method in relation to the strength of the behavior pattern it
attempts to replace. That behavior pattern is persistent disengagement,
or avoidance. Brute force activation, a quality of “just do it,” or activity
138
Committed Action
References
Duckworth, A.L., Steen, T.A., & Seligman, M.E.P. (2005). Positive psychology in
clinical practice. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 629-651.
Gill, J. R., & Brown, C. A. (2009). A structured review of the evidence for pacing
as a chronic pain intervention. European Journal of Pain, 13, 214-216.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy: The process and practice of mindful change. New York: Guilford Press.
Karsdorp, P. A., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2009). Active avoidance but not activity
pacing is associated with disability in fibromyalgia. Pain, 147, 29-35.
Linton, S. J., McCracken, L. M., & Vlaeyen J. W. S. (2008). Reassurance: Help or
hinder in the treatment of pain. Pain, 134, 5-8.
Luoma, J. B., Hayes, S. C., Walser, R. D. (2007). Learning ACT: An acceptance &
commitment skills training manual for therapists. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
McCracken, L. M. (1998). Learning to live with the pain: Acceptance of pain pre-
dicts adjustment in persons with chronic pain. Pain, 74, 21-27.
McCracken, L. M., Gutiérrez-Martínez, O., & Smyth, C. (2012). “Decentering”
reflects psychological flexibility in people with chronic pain and correlates
with their quality of functioning. Health Psychology. Advance online publica-
tion. doi:10.1037/a0028093
McCracken, L. M., & Samuel, V. M. (2007). The role of avoidance, pacing, and
other activity patterns in chronic pain. Pain, 130, 119-125.
McCracken, L. M., & Yang, S-Y. (2006). The role of values in a contextual cognitive-
behavioral approach to chronic pain. Pain, 123, 137-145.
McNulty, J. K., & Fincham, F. D. (2011). Beyond positive psychology? Toward a
contextual view of psychological processes and well- being. American
Psychologist. doi: 10.1037/a0024572
White, P. D., Goldsmith, K. A., Johnson, A. L., Potts, L., Walwyn, R., J. C.
DeCesare, … Sharpe M. (2011). Comparison of adaptive pacing therapy, cog-
nitive behaviour therapy, graded exercise therapy, and specialist medical care
for chronic fatigue syndrome (PACE): A randomised trial. Lancet, 377,
823-836.
139
CHAPTER 7
Positive Interventions:
Past, Present, and Future
Acacia C. Parks
Hiram College
Robert Biswas-Diener
Portland State University
Positive Acorn
141
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
1 We don’t have space or license here to do justice to all of the things that are wrong
with The Secret, nor to sufficiently express how icky it makes us feel when people think
The Secret is part of positive psychology.
142
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
not suffering from a mental disorder. While this definition is more selec-
tive than the previous two, it excludes one prominent positive interven-
tion—positive psychotherapy (PPT)—which has been applied in major
depression (Seligman, Rashid & Parks, 2006), schizophrenia (Meyer et
al., in press), and nicotine dependence (Kahler et al., 2011).
While each of these definitions seems reasonable at first glance,
each is uniquely problematic when used as a stand-alone method for clas-
sifying interventions as “positive” or not. We believe this is because the
goal of creating a single definition may be impractical. Research on posi-
tive interventions was well under way before anyone attempted to infuse
it with theory, and so the research follows no common theoretical thread.
Any definition we create, then, is going to be a post hoc rationalization
of the research that has been done so far rather than a theory-driven
attempt to classify. It will not be simple because it is an attempt to bring
together a broad body of work that was not a cohesive effort. Thus, rather
than creating a single definition, we propose a set of criteria derived by
integrating and refining the above definitions:
143
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
144
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
3 Whereas Gallup’s survey is pay only, the VIA strengths inventory is available for free
online. This difference, in part, explains why the VIA model is so strongly represented
in the literature.
145
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
146
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
rely on intuition or trial and error. Haidt (2002) provides a list of ideas,
compiled by his positive psychology undergraduates, for situations in
which each of the 24 VIA strengths can be applied. This list, however, is
only the beginning of what needs to be a comprehensive resource to
practitioners and clients hoping to promote strengths development.
Gratitude—Some of the earliest positive interventions targeted grati-
tude, which Wood, Froh, and Geraghty (2010) define as a general habit
of noticing and being appreciative of whatever is good in one’s life. In
their seminal paper, Emmons and McCullough (2003) randomly assigned
participants to keep a weekly gratitude journal. In this journal, the par-
ticipants wrote down up to five things they were grateful for. Compared
to participants who kept track of either hassles or neutral events, partici-
pants in the gratitude condition scored better on a range of emotional
and physical health outcomes. Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, and Schkade
(2005) both replicated this finding and found evidence that the “dosage”
used by Emmons and McCullough (2003), once per week, may be the
ideal frequency for a gratitude journal; participants in a condition that
kept a more frequent gratitude journal (3 times per week instead of once)
did not experience the same improvements as the once-per-week group,
instead reporting that the activity felt stale and overdone (Lyubomirsky,
Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005).
Seligman, Steen, Park and Peterson (2005) proposed and tested a
related activity, titled the Three Good Things journal. They asked par-
ticipants to keep a nightly journal of positive events that took place
during the day that just ended;4 this activity resulted in increased happi-
ness and decreased depressive symptoms by 1-month follow-up, with
gains continuing to increase over 3-month and 6-month follow-ups.
While this finding may at first seem to conflict with Lyubomirsky,
Sheldon, and Schkade’s (2005) finding that gratitude can be “overdone,”
there is an important difference between the gratitude journal and Three
Good Things: they operate at completely different levels of analysis.
Whereas a gratitude journal can and often does revolve around ongoing
4 It is debatable whether Three Good Things belongs under the umbrella of “grati-
tude.” This uncertainty is an excellent example of the general lack of theory underlying
many existing positive interventions. Three Good Things was designed to make people
happier without any specific underlying theory. Only after the exercise appeared to be
effective did researchers begin to speculate as to how it works.
147
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
areas of gratitude (e.g., family, friends, a good job), Three Good Things
requires the individual to focus on events that took place during the
current day. Thus, while a gratitude journal might get repetitive if prac-
ticed too often, as the content doesn’t vary much, a Three Good Things
journal has different content every day.
Seligman, Steen, Park and Peterson (2005) report findings on a
second gratitude activity, in which participants compose a detailed
thank-you letter to someone in their life and deliver the letter in person
(“The Gratitude Visit”). In contrast with the previous exercise, The
Gratitude Visit led to large initial boosts in happiness—substantially
larger than the placebo conditions—but these changes were transient,
having faded substantially by 1-month follow-up and entirely by 3 months.
While some researchers have presented ideas about how to prolong these
effects—for example, a client might keep a daily log of the things her
spouse does that she appreciates, then use that log to create “gratitude
reports” once per month on an ongoing basis—nobody has tested an
“improved” gratitude visit design to date. However, Lyubomirsky,
Dickerhoof, Boehm, and Sheldon (2011) did find that writing a gratitude
letter, without the added step of delivering the letter to its target, did lead
to well-being improvements in their sample. Whereas delivering a
glowing letter of thanks to someone may be very powerful the first time,
one can imagine that repeated instances could become stale or awkward;
by removing the “delivery” step of writing a gratitude letter and the awk-
wardness that may come along with it, it becomes more plausible that a
person might practice this exercise repeatedly.
While the existing literature presents a relatively compelling case
that gratitude is a worthwhile practice, gratitude is also one of the few
areas in which deleterious effects have been observed.5 For example, Sin,
Della Porta, and Lyubomirksy (2011) report that writing gratitude letters
reduced immediate well- being for individuals with mild- moderate
5 To be clear, gratitude is also one of the only areas in which anyone has looked at
moderators of outcome to begin with, and so if deleterious effects do occur in other
activities (as, we imagine, they must for some subset of people), these effects are unlikely
to have been detected. We discuss these findings not to suggest that gratitude interven-
tions are bad but rather to encourage researchers to do more of this type of research on
other types of interventions. This type of nuance can only help us apply positive inter-
ventions more effectively and with better precision.
148
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
149
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
How, then, does one decide to whom one would recommend grati-
tude activities? Sin, Della Porta, and Lyubomirsky (2011) report that per-
ceived fit is an important predictor of outcome—that is, participants
who looked at the gratitude activity and thought it would be helpful for
them generally found it helpful. This highlights the importance of choos-
ing positive intervention collaboratively with clients and perhaps even
suggests that the “buffet” approach—trying all activities, then selecting
those that work best for an individual—may not be optimal for all clients.
Forgiveness—In common parlance, forgiveness is often associated
with reconciliation. However, in the forgiveness intervention literature,
forgiveness is conceptualized as a primarily internal process. An indi-
vidual who has experienced a transgression lets go of the negative feel-
ings associated with the transgression and the transgressor, and this
change may or may not result in any sort of behavioral change in rela-
tion to the transgressor. The emphasis in the literature on emotional
forgiveness is largely due to the fact that the emotional aspects of for-
giveness appear to play the largest role in the robust link between for-
giveness and physical health (Worthington, Witvliet, Pietrini, & Miller,
2007).
The majority of forgiveness interventions follow process- based
models, which allow for gradual, stage-like progress toward the decision
to forgive (Baskin & Enright, 2004). The “REACH” model is an example
of a process-based approach to forgiveness: individuals recall the trans-
gression; develop empathy for the transgressor, which is an altruistic act;
commit to forgive; then work to hold on to that forgiveness. Worthington
(2006) provides an example of a 6-session group intervention following
the REACH model. A recent meta-analysis focusing only on process-
based forgiveness interventions found an average effect size of .82 on
forgiveness outcomes, .81 on positive affect, and .54 on negative affect,
suggesting that forgiveness interventions can reliably promote forgive-
ness and improve the emotional damage that grudges can cause (Lundahl,
Taylor, Stevenson, & Roberts, 2008).
Smaller scale interventions that target forgiveness also exist.
McCullough, Root, and Cohen (2006), for example, tested a writing
intervention in which participants spent 20 minutes writing about per-
sonal benefits that arose as the result of a transgression they had experi-
enced. Compared to control groups focusing on an unpleasant aspect of
150
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
151
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
152
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
published studies to date have looked at this activity on its own. However,
informal data analyses and anecdotal observations from the first author
suggest that active-constructive responding may be subject to what Parks,
Della Porta, Pierce, Zilca, & Lyubomirsky (in press) refer to as “degrada-
tion.” In other words, active-constructive responding may be too complex
to teach via simple written instructions (as many positive interventions
are), and as a result, it may lose its potency when it is implemented in the
real world. Further research should examine the efficacy of active-
constructive responding as a sole activity and should disentangle the
relative importance of hands-on instruction (as opposed to brief written
instructions) for its efficacy.
Meaning—Prevailing theories suggest that people derive a sense of
meaning by forming a coherent narrative about their lives (Pennebaker &
Seagal, 1999). Thus, it makes sense that the majority of meaning inter-
ventions involve writing. Seminal studies on meaning making involve
personal narratives of traumatic or stressful life events, but more recently,
research has begun to examine the formation of narratives around posi-
tive life events and, in particular, events that one expects to occur in the
future. King (2001) instructed people to write about their “best possible
self”—a future version of themselves who has turned out according to
their highest hopes and aspirations—for 20 minutes a day over the course
of 4 days. Seligman, Rashid, and Parks (2006) used a similar activity,
which they call the “Life Summary,” wherein participants write a 1-2 page
essay describing their life as they hope to have lived it; as part of the activ-
ity, participants are also instructed to consider the ways that they are and
are not actively progressing toward the long-term goals described in the
essay. Subsequent work by Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2006) suggests that
the benefits of imagining one’s positive future are not limited to writing.
They asked participants to think about their best possible selves at least
twice a week and found that doing so was also beneficial.
It is worth mentioning, however, that the first author has encoun-
tered a handful of cases where writing about a positive future for oneself
has been unpleasant for participants; in particular, relatively anxious stu-
dents have sometimes reported that trying to imagine their future made
them more anxious (keep in mind, though, that these are college stu-
dents, whose futures are quite uncertain; it may be that anxious responses
are an artifact of the age group the activity has been tested in). We have
also found that more depressed students can find the activity
153
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
154
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
Having tried each component separately, they were able to fully experi-
ence the hot chocolate, tasting each individual aspect and enjoying the
interplay between them. The entire process took only a few minutes, but
as a group, we (the first author and the students) found it to be a very
potent experience—one that was amplified by the fact that we shared it
together.
Savoring can be applied to nonsensory experiences as well. One can
savor a present moment via “memory building”—taking photographs, for
example. These types of activities bring one’s attention to the transience
of a present experience and lead to better savoring of that experience
(Kurtz & Lyubomirsky, in press). Memory building also paves the way for
“reminiscence,” which is savoring one’s memory of a past experience
(Bryant, Smart, & King, 2005). Whereas memory building is a technique
for promoting enjoyment in the moment, reminiscence is a more cogni-
tive activity, characterized by the use of imagery as one remembers a
valued past experience in as much detail as possible. Several published
studies have found that deliberately reminiscing more often leads to
improvements in depressive and anxious symptoms, as well as increasing
positive affect and life satisfaction, particularly among older populations
(Bryant, Smart, & King, 2005).
Perspective Taking—Perspective taking has not received much atten-
tion from positive psychology proper, but it is, we believe, an important
construct, with several successful interventions designed to increase it.
Perspective taking is important because it drives people to embrace and
help others. By reducing the perceived “distance” between you and your
neighbor6 (“self-other overlap”), your sense of empathic concern makes
you feel like your neighbor’s problems are your problems, too (Davis,
Conklin, Smith, & Luce, 1996). This makes you more likely to help your
neighbor and also makes you more interested in helping other people you
might group together with your neighbor (Batson, Chang, Orr, &
Rowland, 2002).
While perspective taking is often a component of forgiveness inter-
ventions (see above), we focus in this section on interventions designed
to cultivate perspective taking directly. Perspective-taking interventions
have successfully been applied in a variety of contexts, ranging from
loved ones (romantic partners, parents and children) to the people one
6 Or whomever.
155
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
156
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
Future Directions
Thus far, we have provided an overview of existing positive interven-
tions. We turn, now, to some important issues that we hope to see the
field tackle in the coming years.
Alternative outcomes. One of the preliminary steps in establishing
positive psychology as an empirically based endeavor was to include
prominent scholars with scientific acumen and well-regarded reputations
(Seligman & Csikszentimihaly, 2000). Among these early “recruits” were
pioneering members of the positive psychology steering committee,
including Mihalyi Csikszentimihaly and Ed Diener. Although certainly
unintended, one consequence of aligning so heavily with well-being
157
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
158
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
our knowledge, the first proposal of its kind, but it’s an essential one, in
our view. In our experience talking with both researchers and practitio-
ners in positive psychology, we have found that the prevailing sentiment
is that positive interventions are very unlikely to cause harm, particularly
when used in normative, rather than clinical (“more high-risk”), popula-
tions. However, recent research is beginning to show that this is an unre-
alistic viewpoint. Now that the field is beginning to build a substantial
repertoire of interventions that we believe to be effective, we must turn
to the question of how these interventions can be implemented
responsibly.
Evidence already exists that certain activities work better than
others for a given individual (Schueller, 2010). Happiness seekers are not
homogeneous in terms of their initial levels of happiness and depressive
symptoms (Parks et al., in press), nor in terms of their motivation and
interest in becoming happier (Lyubomirsky et al., 2011), and so it is inap-
propriate to assume that a particular activity can be “universally” effec-
tive. Individual differences matter, not only for outcome (Sergeant &
Mongrain, 2011), but for the likelihood that an individual will use activi-
ties in the first place (Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006). In addition to the
handful of published studies discussed above, the first author partici-
pated in a recent discussion on the FRIENDS-OF-PP listserv in which
several members told stories of positive interventions “backfiring” with
certain clients. It happens—we just don’t know the full extent of when,
and for whom, it happens.
On a broader scale, we propose that caution is necessary in how we
approach efforts to increase happiness in general. Recent work by Mauss
et al. (2011) suggests that holding happiness as a goal makes it more dif-
ficult to achieve that goal—by telling oneself that one “should” be happy,
one is more easily disappointed by one’s own emotional experiences.
Work by Louis (2011), mentioned above, highlights the importance of
how the exact same activity—in this case, taking a strengths assessment
and using that assessment to modify one’s behavior—is presented to and
interpreted by clients. Something as simple as the wording of an activity’s
prompt can make all the difference between the activity being helpful or
harmful.
In short, while the evidence seems clear that individual differences
matter, we do not have a great sense of how to use this information in
practice. It is our hope that the recent wave of positive intervention
159
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
160
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
Conclusions
In this chapter, we provided a new definition of a positive intervention:
an activity that successfully increases some positive variable and that can
be reasonably and ethically applied in whatever context it is being used.
We presented evidence that positive interventions targeting a variety of
construct exist, and each has at least preliminary evidence of effective-
ness, broadly construed. We also argued that caution is warranted when
putting positive interventions into real-world practice; we know some of
these activities can “backfire” but do not yet understand when, how, and
for what activities this occurs. Finally, we argue that positive interven-
tions are distinctive from other psychological approaches in general and
from ACT in particular
References
Aknin, L., Barrington-Leigh, C., Dunn, E., Helliwell, J., Biswas-Diener, R., Kemeza,
I., Nyende, P., Ashton-James, C., Norton, M. (in press). Prosocial spending and
well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology.
Baskin, T., & Enright, R. D. (2004). Intervention studies on forgiveness: A meta-
analysis. Journal of Counseling and Development, 82, 79-90.
Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., & Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and
action: Can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help
the group? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1656-1666.
161
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is
stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370.
Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B., & Minhas, G. (2011). A dynamic approach to
psychological strength development and intervention. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 6, 106-118.
Bryant, F. B. (2003). Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs
about savoring. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 175-196.
Bryant, F. B., Smart, C. M., & King, S. P. (2005). Using the past to enhance the
present: Boosting happiness through positive reminiscence. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 6, 227-260.
Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of positive experience.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. (1996). Effect of perspective
taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and other.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 713-726.
Diener, E., & Diener, C. (2011). Monitoring psychosocial prosperity for social
change. In R. Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychology as social change (pp. 53-
72). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer Press.
Driver, J. L., & Gottman, J. M. (2004). Daily marital interactions and positive affect
during marital conflict among newlywed couples. Family Processes, 43,
301-314.
Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others
promotes happiness. Science, 319, 1687-1688.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens:
An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily
life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377-389.
Fordyce, M. W. (1977). Development of a program to increase personal happiness.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 24, 511-521.
Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase happiness: Further studies. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 30, 483-498.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dynam-
ics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60, 678-686.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do
when things go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing
positive events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 228-245.
The Gallup Organization. (1999). Clifton StrengthsFinder. Washington, DC: Author.
Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541–553.
Haidt, J. (2002). It’s more fun to work on strengths than weaknesses (but it may not
be better for you). Manuscript retrieved from http://people.virginia.edu/~jdh6n/
strengths_analysis.doc
Hodges, S. D., Clark, B., & Myers, M. W. (2011). Better living through perspective
taking. In R. Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychology as a mechanism for social
change (pp. 193-218). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer Press.
162
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
163
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
164
Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and Future
165
CHAPTER 8
Mairéad Foody
Yvonne Barnes-Holmes
Dermot Barnes-Holmes
National University of Ireland, Maynooth
167
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). The theory proposes that all aspects of
human language and cognition, including self-perceptions and perspec-
tive taking, are complex “verbal acts” that are generated by individual
histories. As we will explain shortly, this approach facilitates an under-
standing of the self, while accommodating the fact that our histories
make us unique. A greater understanding of the self has obvious and
broad applicability to the way in which human suffering is perceived and
treated, at the level of both individuals and communities.
Chapter Aims
The primary aim of the current chapter is to suggest the applicability, and
explore potential sources of overlap, between RFT’s technical account of
the self and interventions employed by positive psychologists. Self plays a
pivotal role in both traditions, and it is often a useful exercise to explore
how different approaches interpret common ground. Our key question is
whether a functional behavioral account of self can speak directly to the
understanding and pursuit of human flourishing, as articulated by posi-
tive psychology. In our attempt to build this conceptual bridge, the chapter
is divided into two parts. Part one provides a brief summary of the RFT
account of self, technical in parts, but necessary for understanding the
theory’s specific account of self. Readers are referred directly to chapter 1
for a summary of RFT’s basic processes. Part two then describes two key
exercises promoted by positive psychology that are explicitly focused on
the self and involve attempts to manipulate one’s sense of self in the
service of better psychological health. These include writing about life’s
positive events and triumphs and expressing gratitude. In the context of
each exercise, we offer an RFT interpretation of what appear to be the key
underlying psychological processes and the potential pitfalls.
168
On Making People More Positive and Rational
Put simply, these describe the three core aspects of your perspective.
First, you see the world as I, not as YOU or as someone else (hence, we
say, I versus YOU). Second, when you see the world as I, you always see it
from HERE, not from THERE, or anywhere that is not HERE (hence,
we say, HERE versus THERE). Third, when you see the world as I, you
always see it from NOW, not from THEN, or some other point in time
that is not NOW (hence, we say, NOW versus THEN). In summary, I
always see my world from HERE and NOW and from my perspective,
and I always see YOU from THERE and THEN.
I versus YOU. When we operate from the perspective of I, we dis-
tinguish, compare, and contrast ourselves to others across a myriad of
dimensions. As children, we learn to do this initially through physical
attributes (e.g., Mommy is taller than me, but Daddy is taller than her).
Harmless as these may seem, the fact that we can already compare and
contrast ourselves with others means that we can also evaluate these
comparisons. Consider, for example, Ann, who has always felt inferior to
her more attractive sister Mary. In relational terms, Ann’s perspective
holds that Mary is more physically attractive than Ann, more attractive
equals better, hence Mary is also better than Ann. Although the initial
comparison between the sisters may have been based on a single physical
attribute (which may not even have been observed directly by others),
Ann’s comparative relations have allowed her to contrast this physical
superiority, and her coordination relations have allowed her to coordi-
nate this superiority with being better generally. Of course, the latter
would also quickly have become coordinated with Ann feeling bad about
herself. So, it is easy to see how even simple relations and the fact that
they involve feelings, can lead us early in our lives toward a path of low
self-esteem.
The above example also illustrates how perspective-taking behavior
becomes increasingly arbitrary and thus, at one level, moves further and
further away from physical attributes. Indeed, as verbally sophisticated
adults, most of our comparisons with others are not based on physical
attributes. For example, I might be consumed by jealousy because, from
my perspective, my neighbor appears to be richer than I am, and yet I
have no way of knowing how much money my neighbor actually has.
In fact, who we come to know ourselves to be across time emerges on
the basis of our perspective on others, such that it seems unlikely that
there would be an I without a YOU. What makes perspective taking
169
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
distinct from other types of relational activity is the paradox that I vs.
YOU relations become a constant reference point for our perceptions,
even when many aspects of our lives are constantly changing. For
example, if I suddenly learn that my “rich” neighbor has lost his job, I
may now believe that we are equal in wealth, but we are still different
people. That is, aspects of who you are may change constantly, but your
perspective from which these changes are observed does not. In short,
you always see the world from your own perspective.
HERE versus THERE. There is a long literature on the importance
of a sense of place in human development, and, for RFT, this is captured
by the spatial relations of HERE and THERE. In conjunction with our I
vs. YOU relational development, we learn to distinguish between HERE
(i.e., not THERE) and THERE (i.e., not HERE). For example, when
Daddy comes home, a child might say, “I am watching TV (HERE), but
Mommy is in the kitchen (THERE).” Similar to I-YOU relations, spatial
relations become increasingly less based on physical locations and
become increasingly arbitrary. For instance, if I say “I am here” at this
moment in time, I am in my office, but if I say, “I am here” one hour from
now, I will be in my kitchen. In other words, the word “HERE” coordi-
nates with where I am at that point in time, hence it is constantly chang-
ing. Paradoxically, much of the language that we use refers to physical
space, but in a metaphorical way. Consider Sarah who describes her feel-
ings of depression as “bearing down on her” or as a sense of “carrying the
weight of the world on her shoulders.”
It is clear from the examples above that I-YOU relations are implicit
in HERE-THERE relations because it would be impossible to specify a
perspective from a particular location if there was no perspective from
which to operate. As a result, Sarah is talking about her feelings, which
are part of the way she sees herself at that time. As such, the spatial rela-
tions are a critical feature of one’s perspective because I is always coordi-
nated with HERE (and distinct from THERE) and YOU is always
coordinated with THERE (and distinct from HERE).
NOW versus THEN. Temporal relations are another core feature
of perspective taking, and RFT refers to these as NOW vs. THEN rela-
tions. As the name implies, temporal relations refer to time and are natu-
rally implicit in most everyday sentences. For example, “At two o’clock I
was working but by three, I was at home.” Again, we learn temporal
relations using physical features, especially when we first learn to tell
170
On Making People More Positive and Rational
time. Once that skill is acquired, these relations then become largely
arbitrary because you can make reference to time without knowing what
time it actually is and because temporal relations can be extended across,
days, weeks, months, and even years. Consider an athlete who breaks her
leg and is unable to participate in any more competitions. She may
become fixated on what she was last year and lose sight of her life in the
present (e.g., last year (THEN), I was a winner, but this year (NOW) I
am a loser). Again, I-YOU relations are implicit in temporal relations
because one’s perspective is always from NOW and distinct to THEN.
Of course, it is important to remember that even if you are referring to
how you felt in the past (THEN), the temporal relation from your current
perspective is always NOW.
171
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
172
On Making People More Positive and Rational
173
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Perspective Psychological
HERE-NOW Content Self as Process
HERE-NOW
THE OBSERVER THE DOER
174
On Making People More Positive and Rational
relationship between the observer and the doer at any point in time that is
captured by the three selves, and in self as content, they are at their most
coordinated (or psychologically “equivalent”).
Self as process. Self as process is a psychological space in which on-
going activity facilitates continuous distinctions between the observer
and the doer. In short, who you are is distinct from what you think, feel,
remember, and so on. For ACT, this psychological space does not entail
fusion, because the observer can perceive the processes of thinking,
feeling, and so on (the doings), and the evaluations that accompany
these. Naturally, this distinction has the potential to reduce the previous
pain associated with those evaluations (i.e., the observer wouldn’t feel as
overwhelmed as when in self as content).
The ongoing awareness of those content-based processes is a critical
feature of self as process. In essence, because you can perceive your
content as continuously changing, attachment to it is not facilitated.
Consider the example “I feel depressed and am having depressed
thoughts, but these are just thoughts and feelings at this moment, and
they may be different tomorrow.” This ongoing awareness is synonymous
with the concept of mindfulness, because you are being mindful of your
content as it comes and goes.
Operating in self as process readily permits values-consistent behav-
ior, rather than content-driven behavior, because you have a distinct
awareness of your content while remaining open to values. In simple
terms, self as process permits both, thereby allowing you greater flexibil-
ity with regard to your actions. Hence, you can live a rich and full values-
based life if you can see your content for what it is. Notice, however, that
self as process does not require you to somehow be “content-free” (if that
were even possible) but simply offers you a different place from which to
observe the ever-changing nature of your content.
On balance, it is also important to recognize that sticky psychological
content (with which you have struggled in the past) is likely to create
resurgence to self as content. That is, you cannot assume that all you need
to do is stay in self as process, because your history will likely dictate that
certain powerful content will slide you back into self as content, the
minute it shows up. Accordingly, for ACT, there is a very fine line between
self as content and self as process, and it is primarily ongoing awareness
that keeps you in the latter and away from the grasp of the former. Let us
return to our client Martin to illustrate how self as process works.
175
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
On a regular basis, Martin feels pain in one part of his body or maybe
several at the same time. As soon as this experience occurs, his history
of operating in self as content with this experience is almost inevitably
brought to bear, and his behavioral flexibility is greatly reduced so that
the alleviation of pain is perceived as the only option (i.e., “I must be
someone who is pain free”). What this fusion in self as content precludes
him from seeing is that even in that instant he does have other options
regarding his pain, although they will certainly be difficult. For example,
what he cannot see is the logical possibility that the pain can be endured,
in the service of values. In short, perhaps, even when pain is present,
other valued actions can be conducted. But, none of this can be experi-
enced if pain-related content is not experienced on an ongoing basis, as
one pain-related experience after another, and another, and another.
Because even before Martin can learn to accept the pain, he must learn
to operate from a different place in which the processes of his pain expe-
riences can be observed. This would be self as process.
Now let us consider how RFT can account for what is happening at
a relational level in self as process. Look at the right-hand side of Figure
1 again, and you will see that there is no relational difference between self
as content and self as process, although they are significantly different
experientially. In both selves, the doer is located HERE-NOW and, as
always, the observer is HERE-NOW also. In self as process, there is a
sense of the movement of psychological content from HERE-NOW to
THERE- THEN. This means that although psychological content is
problematic in self as content because it is HERE-NOW, self as process
is different in that your content as HERE-NOW is simply the way we
learn to talk about our thoughts, feelings, and behavior, and thus facili-
tates self-knowledge rather than fusion.
Self as context. Self as context is a psychological space in which
there is the strongest distinction between the observer and the doer. For
ACT, self as context embodies defusion because who you are is distinct
from what you think, feel, remember, and so on. Consider the following
example, “I feel depressed and am having depressed thoughts, but I know
that who I am is more than these negative thoughts and feelings.”
Naturally, this distinction has the potential to reduce the previous pain
associated with content and its evaluations (i.e., the observer wouldn’t feel
as overwhelmed as when in self as content). To illustrate the ACT
approach with regard to facilitating self as context, let us return to Martin.
176
On Making People More Positive and Rational
177
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
dangerous place to be because you can easily get sucked back into self as
content, ACT exercises also promote flexibility in switching from self as
process to self as context (i.e., to maximize defusion).
Consider again the example of Martin. In the beginning, Martin was
operating in self as content with regard to his experiences of pain and
became primarily a pain sufferer (i.e., maximum fusion). This fusion
then makes it more likely that his behavior will be largely governed by
attempts to remove the pain, with thoughts such as “I cannot be who I
was until the pain is gone.” Teaching Martin to operate with regard to
his pain from self as process would allow him to notice on an ongoing
basis the nature and frequency of pain experiences and would facilitate
some defusion in this regard. For example, his ACT professional might
encourage him to notice one pain event after another, and another, and
another, like leaves floating down a stream. This self as process exercise
would reduce the fusion with some pain events but of course not all,
given his history. Hence, at the same time, it would be important to
teach Martin how to operate from self as context in which he can come
to see that he is much more than his pain experiences (i.e., BIG observer,
small doer). In this case, both self as process and self as context can be
used to facilitate behavioral flexibility in the service of values because
Martin can learn that even when pain shows up, it can be noticed,
acknowledged, and yet alternative values-based action can still happen.
In the sections below, we explore the potential overlap between ACT
and RFT approaches to self and various exercises from the field of posi-
tive psychology. Although each hails from a very different philosophical
and psychological tradition, there is much utility in examining potential
common ground for both scientific and therapeutic perspectives. In par-
ticular, we selected two positive psychology exercises that appeared to
have a strong basis in one’s sense of self.
178
On Making People More Positive and Rational
179
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
180
On Making People More Positive and Rational
181
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
At an intuitive level, you may be more likely to become fused with your
negative content, because it is more painful and more likely to be avoided.
So, the harder you fight not to have it, the more of a threat it becomes.
This is perhaps less likely to be the case with positive content, which you
are less likely to try to avoid. Nonetheless, at a process level, you can
become attached to your positive content, and this is indeed what many
clients try to do (i.e., “I will be better if I could just have happier
thoughts”). But this move would be just as problematic as attachment to
negative content, because you are always more than your content (good
and bad). In short, neither self as content nor self as process separate out
“good” content from “bad”; all content is, by definition, problematic
when viewed from self as content and must be observed constantly in self
as process. To illustrate this critical difference, consider the example of
ACT’s metaphor of the chessboard.
182
On Making People More Positive and Rational
The aim for a client in ACT is to identify with the board as a useful
perspective on her content of white and black pieces. The board holds
the pieces and they need it, not vice versa. If you are attached to any of
the pieces, playing the game is very important and you will be focused on
“winning.” As the board, you can see all of the pieces, you can hold
them, and you can watch the war being played out. You cannot become
attached to specific pieces (even white) without abandoning board level.
Hayes (2004) captured the essence of operating at board level in the fol-
lowing quote: “ACT clients are encouraged to abandon any interest in
the literal truth of their own thoughts or evaluations” (p. 647). As such,
no content is to be believed as literally true and/or as saying something
about who you are as a person, thereby holding firm to a distinction
between the observer and the doer. If you permit attachment to any type
of content, even positive, this distinction is sacrificed.
From an RFT/ACT perspective, it seems plausible that positive
writing exercises serve one of two functions. The first is that they may
facilitate self as content, instead of self as process. This might encourage
you to become fused with your positive content if you are seeking to
reduce or remove negative content. This type of experiential avoidance
is associated with poor mental health outcomes and is not at all consis-
tent with ACT (Hayes, Pankey, & Palm, 2005). The second possibility is
that these exercises facilitate defusion and values clarification. For
example, you might write “I am a really good parent,” if you value sound
parenting. The defusion element of this allows you to have distance from
any painful experiences you may have of faulty parenting. The values
clarification element allows you to see which aspects of parenting you
might do better in the future, and this may enhance your motivation for
achieving this value. Defusion and values clarification have been critical
ACT components since the beginning and there is sound empirical
support for their role in positive clinical outcomes (see Hayes, Luoma,
Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006).
183
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
184
On Making People More Positive and Rational
185
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
on both what I value and what you value. And in turn, you must have a
perspective on what I value and what you value.
Consider the following example. As an adult, you realize that your
parents had a formative role in your education, which subsequently led to
your achieving a professional career that you now highly value. First,
consider your parents’ perspective taking regarding their role in your
career. Unbeknownst to you in your youth, your parents were enduring
financial hardship to pay your school fees. This is based on the value they
placed on your education and the fact that they believed that you would
in turn value this in the future. So, they had perspectives on their own
values (being good parents, etc.), and their actions and hardships were
consistent with these. And, they had a sense of your perspective later in
life, and their actions were also consistent with your future values. Now
consider your perspective taking. At the time, you probably had no per-
spective on your parents’ actions or perspectives, and you perhaps had
limited insight into what you valued, even then. You certainly would
have been unlikely to have a perspective on what you would value in the
future. Now you have a perspective on your own values and on your
parents’, both then and now. As a result of the perspective taking you
have on what your parents valued, how they acted on it, and the perspec-
tive they had on your future perspective, you have a strong sense of
enduring gratitude, because there was coalescence between what they
valued and what you now value highly. This complex perspective taking
may be illustrated in the following quotation by DeWall et al. (2012):
“When experiencing gratitude, a person is sensitive to the emotion,
thoughts, and actions that underlie the positive contributions of others
… which reflect a shift away from self-interests to mirroring and under-
standing another person” (p. 2).
As well as demonstrating the complex perspective taking and values-
based action involved in gratitude, the example above illustrates how
temporal and causal relations, especially IF- THENs, are important.
Consider the example again. Your parents’ perspective on your future
may be described as “If we pay for her college fees now and she then gets
a career, she will benefit from our efforts then because she will value then
what we value now.” And if you didn’t value your career now, you wouldn’t
feel gratitude for this. However, if you do value your career, your perspec-
tive will go something like this: “I am grateful to my parents now because
what I value now, they facilitated then, and if they had not done so, I
186
On Making People More Positive and Rational
would not have achieved what I value.” In a nutshell, this latter type of
perspective taking typifies gratitude.
Furthermore, interventions in both ACT and positive psychology
are most often anchored around behavior change in the service of values
(e.g., Hayes et al., 2006; Seligman et al., 2005). In ACT, for example,
values are highly personalized, and values clarification is designed to
facilitate behavior regulation rather than psychological fusion or avoid-
ance (Fletcher & Hayes, 2005). Similarly, in positive psychology, the
gratitude list is a primary example of a technique in which you must
clarify what you value before you can flourish.
For illustrative purposes, consider again our previous example of
Martin. Imagine his therapist asked him to write down three things in
his life for which he was grateful. And Martin selected his children, the
relationship with his wife, and his career. For ACT, the list serves as
values clarification and as a reliable reference against which his behavior
may be examined. For example, the therapist may ask, “Do your actions
with your children often demonstrate the extent to which you value
them?” In response to this, Martin would likely say, “Yes, when I am pain
free, we have great fun, but when I am in pain, things are more con-
strained.” “What if,” the therapist then asks “you took some time each
day to spend with your children, irrespective of how your pain is at that
time?” This would likely differ considerably from what Martin has done
to date, because pain has been opposed to playing with the children, that
is, he thinks that the value “cannot” be acted upon when pain is present.
Now, in terms of his relationship with his wife, imagine that the
therapist asks Martin to take her perspective on his suffering. He will
begin to see that one of the consequences of his suffering is that she has
also suffered. And is her suffering in any way less “legitimate” than his?
Again, this is unlikely to be a perspective from which Martin has oper-
ated to date. He perhaps sees his own suffering as above all else, includ-
ing values and the suffering of others in the sense that when pain is
present, these other things matter less and require less attention. The
clarification of these two values may then be highlighted, and Martin’s
perspective on his wife’s and children’s perspectives may be enhanced by
a gratitude exercise in which he shares with them his perspective and his
perspective on their perspective. In addition to showing gratitude, he
may make commitments to them to ensure that future actions are more
consistent with shared values. Thus, the gratitude exercise facilitates self
187
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
1. When values have been clarified and are genuine, then grati-
tude is almost inevitable, and the expression of this gratitude
likely serves an important purpose in bonding future behavior to
these values. In this sense, gratitude is a likely outcome that
follows values clarification. Now, think of this the other way
around. What if you were advised to express gratitude but values
had not been clarified? This may be problematic because you
may not experience a sense of gratitude and you would not be
sure why you were doing it in the first place. As a result, grati-
tude exercises are likely to be most effective only in a context of
shared values. Gratitude without this may be meaningless or even
counterproductive.
188
On Making People More Positive and Rational
even though you feel anger, rather than love and gratitude, at
that point in time. In short, the gratitude that you are expressing
reflects the effect that many of her previous actions have had on
you and on your relationship across time rather than reflecting
current feelings. Again, for ACT the focus is on values and not
feelings because acting on the basis of current feelings likely
reflects self as content. Acting in the service of what your expe-
rience across time tells you and what you value are more indica-
tive of self as process and context.
Concluding Comments
The current chapter set out to articulate the putative role of RFT’s three
selves in two key interventions proposed by positive psychology. The
interpretation offers a functional, process-based account of what is hap-
pening when these techniques are used and why, we believe, they have
the outcomes they do. Furthermore, the chapter described a broader
overlap between positive psychology and ACT, with a strong sharing of
emphasis on the self, defusion, and values clarification. In summary,
there is no right or wrong way to do self-enhancement or therapeutic
work, and no right or wrong interpretation of what is happening at the
189
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
References
Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in actions: The “other-
praising” emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105-127.
Bandura, A. (1999). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavior change. In
R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), The self in social psychology (pp. 240-279). New York:
Psychology Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. (1999). Relation of threatened egotism to
violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. In R. F. Baumeister
(Ed.), The self in social psychology, (pp. 240-279). New York: Psychology Press.
Bono, G., Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2004). Gratitude in practice and
the practice of gratitude. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology
in practice (pp. 464-481). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Bono, G., & McCullough, M. E. (2006). Positive responses to benefit and harm:
Bringing forgiveness and gratitude into cognitive psychotherapy. Journal of
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 20, 147-158.
Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2004). The health benefits of writing about intensely
positive experiences. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(2), 150-163.
Ciarrochi, J., Heaven, P. C., & Davies, F. (2007). The impact of hope, self-esteem,
and attributional style on adolescents’ school grades and emotional well-being:
A longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1161-1178.
Cohn, M. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). In search of durable positive psychology
interventions: Predictors and consequences of long-term positive behavior
change. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(5), 355-366.
Crocker, J., & Park, L. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological
Bulletin, 130(3), 392-414.
DeWall, C. N., Lambert, N. M., Pond, R. S., Kashdan, T. B., & Fincham, F. D.
(2012). A grateful heart is a nonviolent heart: Cross-sectional, experience sam-
pling, longitudinal, and experimental evidence. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 3(2), 232-240.
Duckworth, A. L., Steen, A., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Positive psychology in
clinical practice. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 629-651.
Emmons, R. A. (2004). Gratitude. In C. Peterson & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.),
Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification (pp. 553-568).
New York: Oxford University Press.
190
On Making People More Positive and Rational
191
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
192
On Making People More Positive and Rational
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of
narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1243-1254.
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychol-
ogy progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist,
60(5), 410-421.
Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depres-
sive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: A practice-friendly
meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(5), 467-487.
Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2004a). A closer examination of the written disclosure
paradigm. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 165-175.
Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2004b). Taking pen to hand: Evaluating theories
underlying the written disclosure paradigm. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 11, 121-137.
Smyth, J. M., Stone, A. A., Hurewitz, A., & Kaell, A. (1999). Effects of writing
about stressful experiences on symptom reduction in patients with asthma or
rheumatoid arthritis: A randomized trial. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 281, 1304-1309.
Spera, S. P., Buhrfeind, E. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1994). Expressive writing and
coping with job loss. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 722-733.
Stanton, A. L., Danoff-Burg, S., Sworowski, L. A., Collins, C. A., Branstetter, A.
D., Rodriguez-Hanley, A., Kirk, S. B., & Austenfeld, L. (2002). Randomized,
controlled trial of written emotional expression and benefit finding in breast
cancer patients. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 20(20), 4160-4168.
Tsang, J-A. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behaviour: An experimental test of
gratitude. Cognition and Emotion, 20(1), 138-148.
Wing, J. F., Schutte, N. S., & Byrne, B. (2006). The effect of positive writing on
emotional intelligence and life satisfaction. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
62(10), 1291-1302.
Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., & Geraghty, A. W. A. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A
review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 890, 905.
Wood, A. M., Joseph, S., & Maltby, J. (2008). Gratitude uniquely predicts satisfac-
tion with life: Incremental validity above the domains and facets of the five
factor model. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 49-54.
Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Gillett, R., Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2008). The role of
gratitude in the development of social support, stress, and depression: Two
longitudinal studies. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 854-871.
193
CHAPTER 9
Microculture as a Contextual
Positive Psychology Intervention
Robert Biswas-Diener
Portland State University, Positive Acorn
Nadezhda Lyubchik
Portland State University
195
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
196
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
197
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
The Importance of
Microculture to Practice
As positive psychological science improves in sophistication, it grows
more elegant in its approach to intervention. Where early positive psy-
chology research focused on narrow outcome measures—usually happi-
ness related—by which discrete intervention tools were validated, more
recent work has become necessarily nuanced. For instance, Sin, Della
Porta, and Lyubomirsky (2011) argue that attention should be paid to
person factors that might influence the effectiveness of positive interven-
tions and that interventions should be accordingly tailored for better
individual fit. Similarly, Seligman (2011) argues that the traditional posi-
tive psychology focus on individual intervention needs to be replaced by
attention to group-level intervention and well-being. This call has been
answered by Biswas-Diener and Patterson (2011), Veenhoven (2011), and
others who argue that we need to shift our collective attention away from
individual happiness as the ultimate outcome toward other desirable out-
comes such as increased social capital, greater interpersonal connection,
and collective welfare. In this way positive psychology is seen not so
much as the science of pursuing happiness but as a mechanism for social
change (Biswas-Diener, 2011).
Attention to cultural factors is an important advance for positive
psychology and for practice in general. Of the 194 citations that appear
in the PsycInfo database for the flagship publication of positive psychol-
ogy, Journal of Positive Psychology, only three are cross-indexed with the
keyword “culture”; thankfully, edited volumes have started to appear
that address well-being from a cultural perspective (e.g., Delle Fava,
Massimini, & Bassi, 2011). We argue that microculture represents a
special case of culture that should be of particular interest to interven-
tionists because it is relatively easier to intervene. We offer three com-
ments on microculture interventions that, we hope, establish its
usefulness in affecting change with clients:
198
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
199
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
200
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
An Introduction to Microculture
Microculture is simply the distinctive culture—the agreed upon ways of
temporarily behaving, thinking, or feeling—of a small group of people
within a limited geographic area or a small organization such as a school
or business. There are, for instance, separate microcultures for riding on
an airplane rather than riding on a public bus regardless of geographic
location. Riding on a plane, specifically, lends itself to a higher incidence
201
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
202
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
1. Rituals/Traditions
Whether it is a Yanomamo ritual in which men strike one another
on the chest (Chagnon, 1996) or patriots standing for the national
anthem at the onset of a sporting event, rituals and traditions provide a
shared sense of purpose and identity. It may even be that engaging in
rituals that are closely associated with a cultural identity prime that iden-
tity and make certain features of it salient. These habitual behaviors are
often engaged in without reflection.
This is particularly important in the psychotherapy context. Clinical
delivery has seen a number of innovations since its Freudian heyday of
couches and dream analysis. The advent of various technologies has
impacted clinical delivery such as the use of physiological monitoring to
provide neurofeedback (Masterpasqua & Healey, 2003) and the use of
telephonic technologies to allow therapy to be delivered across geo-
graphic distances (Jerome et al., 2000). In recent years there has also
been a groundswell of popularity in therapeutic techniques related to
mindfulness (Vujanovic et al., 2011) and well-being enhancement (e.g.,
Frisch, 2006). Despite these trends there has been surprisingly little
innovation in talk therapy itself as a delivery mechanism for change.
Despite variability in therapy duration and theoretical orientations, the
basic format of talk therapy follows the same conversational format.
Therapists and clients sit in chairs in a private office and discuss prob-
lems. One noteworthy exception to this format is the addition of experi-
ential exercises typical to the expressive therapies such as art therapy
(Kossak, 2008) and drama therapy (Fox, 1987).
It is surprising how little deviation there is in the format of typical
talk therapies until one considers the powerful but subtle influences of
cultural norms. Most, if not all, practicing therapists are socialized in
graduate school regarding the norms of therapy. This commonly includes
discussions of accepting gifts from clients, professional dress codes, and
other issues that are not, in themselves, directly therapeutic but which
still reflect widespread professional norms. It is curious that therapists
have not overtly questioned—in a systematic rather than individual
203
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
204
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
2. Role Induction
Role induction is the use of social influence to assign expectations of
specific roles. People, especially those in “power positions,” often acculter-
ate new or lower ranking members of a group to how role expectancies
will be filled through didactic instruction and/or subtle cues. A business
owner, for instance, might indicate to a job applicant where to sit in the
office, how formally or informally to behave, or when it is appropriate to
shift from the initial “chit-chat” phase of the interview to the more
serious part. Similarly, therapists often engage in pretherapy role induc-
tion processes, such as discussing the importance and limits of confiden-
tiality, setting expectations for treatment compliance, and opening a
dialogue about the nature of the therapeutic alliance. Professional
coaches who discuss the distinctions between therapy and coaching with
their clients are engaging in a form of role induction that establishes
clear cultural parameters for the relationship. In our own introductory
coaching sessions we explain to clients that—unlike therapy as it is por-
trayed in popular media—we, the coaches, will interrupt frequently. We
explain that this is done strategically in an effort to move the action
forward rather than rehashing past events. Childress and Gillis (1977)
suggest that pretherapy role induction is an important predictor of later
therapy success because, in part, it clarifies expectations and raises the
chances for client compliance.
When thinking about the creation of microcultures, bear in mind
that role induction can occur at the level of conscious awareness or as
the result of nonverbal paracommunication, such as when instructors
implicitly reward students at the front of the classroom by being more
likely to include them in discussion and call on them when their hands
are raised. Similarly, when an executive sits behind a large desk or a
workshop leader stands in front of a room full of participants or touches
someone on the shoulder, he is using haptics (relating to the sense of
touch) and kinesics (relating to the movement of the body in space) to
communicate with others (Beitman & Viamontes, 2007). Facial
205
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
expression, touch, the strategic use of space, and other nonverbal cues
can influence role induction. Seemingly small behaviors such as smiling
to reinforce desirable behavior or arranging chairs so that they are angled
both toward one another and toward a white board can be subtle but
powerful indicators of the role expectancies of both the client and the
practitioner.
In a dramatic illustration of role induction, Rose Inza Kim, a thera-
pist working in Seoul, South Korea, limits the number of sessions with
clients at the onset to motivate them to seek rapid solutions and expend
effort. By addressing limited sessions, Kim explicitly suggests that being
resourceful and committed is an essential part of the client role. This
may differ from expectations some clients bring to therapy in which they
assume therapy is a passive, enduring endeavor.
3. Contrasting
Contrasting is the use of mental comparisons to arrive at a particular
emotional, behavioral or attitudinal perspective. Contrasting is a basic
part of information processing and everyone engages in cognitive con-
trasting automatically, as in the case when we use a mental “anchor”
point and then adjust our thinking against it (e.g., a charity solicits a
donation and requests 50 dollars but allows you to write in any amount—
presumably a lesser amount— influenced by this anchor point).
Contrasting is occasionally used explicitly for therapeutic outcomes. In a
“solutions focus” approach to therapy and coaching, where there is a
focus on progress, growth, and possibilities (Jackson & Waldman, 2010),
techniques include “scaling” (using numeric or other scales to evaluate
progress) and “interviewing for exceptions” (asking clients to identify
instances when they have not experienced problems or setbacks). Both
techniques surprise clients by presenting questions and information that
is in stark contrast with client expectations. In solutions focus scaling, for
example, a score of a 3 on a 1 to 10 scale assumes that the client auto-
matically attends to the distance between his or her score—a 3—and a
presumed perfect outcome of a 10. The solutions focus practitioner trans-
forms this assumption by shifting attention to the distance between the
score—a 3—and the presumed worst score of a 1. This perspective
206
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
4. Shared Experience
Shared experience can be a powerful force in the development of a
collective identity because it allows individuals a sense of intimacy pro-
vided by the perception of increased understanding of one another.
People who have been through natural disasters together, for instance,
can find a mutual point of connection that can help them make sense of
the events. Candau (2010) suggests that shared experiences act as “socio-
transmitters” that connect people to one another much in the same
fashion that neurotransmitters act as delivery mechanisms for complex
connections in the brain. Shared experiences are—perhaps—the aspect
of microculture that is the least artificial. Shared experiences, regardless
of their specifics, include shared emotions, a common reference point (as
in the case of “inside jokes”), and increased self-other overlap (Aron,
207
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Aron, & Smollan, 1992). There is, for example, initial research suggest-
ing that people naturally “sync” with one another by mimicking body
language (Vacharkulksemsuk & Fredrickson, 2012) and that mere expo-
sure breeds liking for an object (or person) as well as approach motiva-
tion toward that object (or person) (Jones, Young, & Claypool, 2011).
Indeed, research suggests that shared experience— especially highly
positive experiences—are linked to relationship satisfaction (Reissman,
Aron, & Bergen, 1993). These studies point to the importance of shared
experience as a relationship and—we would argue here—a microculture
building phenomenon at the heart of which is a number of empathic
benefits. It should be noted, however, the majority of self-other overlap
and shared experience research has been conducted in the context of
close or romantic relationships and our understanding of cross-cultural
issues or stranger-stranger contact issues is limited.
Case Study
In this penultimate section we offer a brief case to showcase how micro-
cultures can be quickly and intentionally created as well as its effect on
people. Rather than make strong pronouncements about specific tech-
niques, we present preliminary observations drawn from real-world expe-
riences. To be clear, we do not believe enough is known about
microcultures and their creation to offer clear, strategic advice regarding
their construction and use. We leave it, instead, for readers and practitio-
ners to draw their own conclusions about the relative promise and limita-
tions of microculture in the contexts in which they work.
At the biannual conference of the International Positive Psychology
Association (IPPA), one of the authors (RBD) gave a 4-hour workshop in
which he attempted to create a microculture that would be noticeably
distinct from the macroculture in which it existed (American society)
and even from the subcultures that might otherwise define it (academia,
annual gatherings of professional societies). To gauge the speed of cre-
ation, extent of impact, and effectiveness of the microcultural interven-
tion, the author chose to establish a new group norm that would be
obviously counter to larger culture norms. In this particular, he decided
that no critical questions would be taken from the audience. Critical
remarks and clarifying questions have long been assumed to be an
208
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
209
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Future Recommendations
Microcultures, as discussed in this chapter, are temporary and artificial
cultures that carry unique norms, roles, and rituals. We argue that micro-
cultures can be used as meta-interventions to provide a context through
which more traditional interventions can be more effectively delivered.
We suggest here that this contextual approach is a necessary step forward
for the young science of positive psychology. As an applied science, prac-
titioners have often emphasized the relatively limited number of empiri-
cally validated tools that are widely accepted as a core component of the
positive psychology cannon. We have argued elsewhere (Biswas-Diener,
2011) that many of these empirically validated tools have been tested on
only a narrow range of outcomes and that little is known about the best
ways to deliver them, possible contraindications, and client factors that
that might affect their effectiveness.
This tool-based emphasis and preference for one’s own epistemology
is an unavoidable aspect of any intellectual endeavor (Kagan, 2009). As
positive psychology matures and explicitly addresses these issues
(Schueller, 2011), this nuance and sophistication will better inform other
intervention approaches such as coaching and ACT. We believe, in par-
ticular, that creating microcultures may represent a means of better
understanding the contextual factors of intervention, an approach that
converges with the ACT emphasis on context and flexible responding.
210
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
References
Akella, D. (2010). Learning together: Kolb’s experiential theory and its application.
Journal of Management and Organization, 16(1), 100-112.
Aron, A., Aron E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and
the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 596-612.
Asch, S. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one
against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70, 70.
Beitman, B. D., & Viamontes G. L. (2007). Unconscious role induction: Implications
for psychotherapy. Psychiatric Annals, 37, 259-268.
Biglan, A. (1995). Changing cultural practices: A contextual framework for interven-
tion research. Reno: Context Press.
Biswas-Diener, R. (2011). Editor’s foreword. In R. Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psy-
chology as social change (pp. v-xi). New York: Springer Science + Business
Media.
Biswas-Diener, R., & Dean, B. (2007). Positive psychology coaching. Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley & Sons.
Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T., & Minhas, G. (2011). A dynamic approach to psy-
chological strengths development and intervention. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 6, 106-118.
Biswas-Diener, R., & Patterson, L. (2011). Positive psychology and poverty. In R.
Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychology as social change (pp. 125- 140).
Dordrecht: Springer.
211
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
212
Microculture as a Contextual Positive Psychology Intervention
Jones, I. F., Young, S. G., & Claypool, H. M. (2011). Approaching the familiar: On
the ability of mere exposure to direct approach and avoidance behavior.
Motivation and Emotion, 35(4), 383-392.
Kagan, J. (2009). The three cultures: Natural sciences, social sciences and the humani-
ties in the 21st century. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kossak, M. S. (2008). Therapeutic attunement: A transpersonal view of expressive
arts therapy. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 36, 13-18.
Lazarus, R. S. (2003). Does positive psychology movement have legs? Psychological
Inquiry, 14(2), 93-109.
LeGoff, D. B. (2004). Use of LEGO© as a therapeutic medium for improving social
competence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 557-571.
Linehan, M. (2006). Dialectical behavior therapy with suicidal adolescents. New York:
Guilford.
Luoma, J. B, Hayes, S. C., & Walser, R. D. (2007). Learning ACT: An Acceptance
and Commitment Therapy skills-training manual for therapists. Oakland, CA:
New Harbinger Publications.
Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J. K., & Sheldon, K. M. (2011). Becoming
happier takes both a will and a proper way: An experimental longitudinal
intervention to boost well-being. Emotion, 11, 391-402.
Masterpasqua, F., & Healey, K. N. (2003). Neurofeedback in psychological practice.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34(6), 652-656.
McCrone, S. (2005). The development of mathematical discussions: An investiga-
tion of a fifth-grade classroom. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 7(2),
111-133.
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. The Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 67(4), 371-378.
Neulip, J. (2009). Intercultural communication: A contextual approach. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Parks, A., & Biswas-Diener, R. (In press). Positive Interventions: Past, Present, and
Future. In T. Kashdan & J. Ciarrochi, Eds., Mindfulness, acceptance, and posi-
tive psychology: The seven foundations of well-being. Oakland, CA: New
Harbinger.
Ramarajan, L., & Thomas, D. (2011). A positive approach to studying diversity in
organizations. In K.S. Cameron & G.M. Spreitzer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook
of Positive Organizational Scholarship, (pp. 552-565). Oxford, UK: Oxford
Unviersity Press.
Reissman, C., Aron, A., & Bergen, M. R. (1993). Shared activities and marital
satisfaction: Causal direction and self-expansion versus boredom. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 243-254.
Rosmarin, D. H., Pirutinsky, S., Cohen, A., Galler, Y., & Krumrei, E. J. (2011).
Grateful to God or just plain grateful? A study of religious and non-religious
gratitude. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 389-396.
Schueller, S. (2011). To each his own well-being boosting intervention: Using pref-
erences to guide selection. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 300-313.
213
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Scollon, C. N., & King, L. A. (2010). What people really want in life and why it
matters: Contributions from research on folk theories of the good life. In R.
Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive Psychology as Social Change (pp. 1-14). Springer
Press.
Searle, R. H., & Skinner, D. (2011). Trust and human resource management.
Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing, Inc.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and
well-being. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychol-
ogy progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist, 60,
410–421.
Sergeant, S., & Mongrain, M. (2011). Are positive psychology exercises helpful for
people with depressive personality styles? Journal of Positive Psychology, 6,
260-272.
Sin, N., Della Porta, M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Tailoring positive psychology
interventions to treat depressed individuals. In Donaldson, S., Csikszentmihalyi,
M. & Nakamura, J. (Eds.), Applied positive psychology (pp. 79-96). New York:
Routledge.
Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2007). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice.
(5th ed.) Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Vacharkulksemsuk, T., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2012). Strangers in sync: Achieving
embodied rapport through shared movements. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 48(1), 399-402.
Veenhoven, R. (2011). Greater happiness for a greater number: Is that possible? If
so, how? In K. M. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Designing
positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp. 396-409). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Vujanovic, A. A., Niles, B., Pietrefesa, A., Schmertz, S. K., & Potter, C. M. (2011).
Mindfulness in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder among military
veterans. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 42(1), 24-31.
Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., & Conti, R. (2008). The implications of two con-
ceptions of happiness (hedonic enjoyment and eudaimonia) for the under-
standing of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 41-79.
Wierzbicka, A. (2008). What makes a good life? A cross-linguistic and cross-
cultural perspective. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(4), 260-272.
Whiten, A., & Flynn, E. (2010). The transmission and evolution of experimental
microcultures in groups of young children. Developmental Psychology, 46(6),
1694-1709.
Whitworth, L., Kimsey-House, H., & Sandhal, P. (1998). Co-active coaching. Palo
Alto: Davies-Black.
214
CHAPTER 10
Elizabeth Malouf
Kerstin Youman
Laura Harty
Karen Schaefer
June P. Tangney
George Mason University
P ositive psychology is not the first thing people think about when
they think of criminal offenders, nor are values and mindfulness.
In our program of research, however, we have found multiple lines
of research from positive psychology useful at enhancing inmates’ reinte-
gration into the community. In this chapter, we describe three forms of
intervention that we have been implementing and evaluating in the
context of a larger program of longitudinal research on moral emotions
and moral cognitions (Tangney, Mashek, & Stuewig, 2007). We first
describe the Impact of Crime (IOC) workshop, an innovative group
intervention that draws on a “guilt-inducing, shame-reducing” restorative
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
216
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
217
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Upon re-examining the causes of their legal difficulties and revisiting the
circumstances surrounding previous offenses and their consequences,
many inmates experience new feelings of shame, guilt, or both. Most
intriguing to us, restorative justice is essentially a “guilt-inducing, shame-
reducing” approach to rehabilitation. Offenders are encouraged to take
responsibility for their behavior, acknowledge the negative consequences
to others, empathize with the distress of their victims, feel guilt for having
done the wrong thing, and act on the consequent inclination to repair
the harm done. Facilitators model empathy in group (and often one-on-
one) discussions about circumstances leading to past offenses and then
actively encourage participants to identify ways of repairing the harm
they have caused. Using affirmations and highlighting steps offenders
have already taken toward reparation shifts the focus from past negative
acts to future opportunities for change and restoration. Additionally,
group members are encouraged to empathize with and support each
other in order to foster openness, collaboration, and sharing of ideas for
restoration. Offenders, however, are actively discouraged from feeling
shame about themselves. In fact, restorative justice approaches eschew
messages aimed at condemning and humiliating offenders as “bad
people.” The emphasis is on bad behaviors that can be changed, negative
consequences that can be repaired, and offenders who can be redeemed.
Why is this guilt-inducing, shame-reducing characteristic of the IOC
workshop (and restorative justice approaches, in general) so important?
Research from our lab, and many others, has shown that shame and guilt
are distinct emotions with very different implications for subsequent
moral and interpersonal behavior (Tangney, Malouf, Stuewig, & Mashek,
2012; Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007). Feelings of shame involve a
painful focus on the self—the humiliating sense that “I am a bad person.”
Such shameful feelings are typically accompanied by a sense of shrink-
ing, of being small, by a sense of worthlessness and powerlessness, and by
a sense of being exposed. Ironically, research has shown that such painful
and debilitating feelings of shame do not motivate constructive changes
in behavior. Shamed individuals are no less likely to repeat their trans-
gressions (often more so), and they are no more likely to attempt repara-
tion (often less so) (Tangney, Stuewig, & Hafez, 2011). Instead, because
shame is so intolerable, people in the midst of a shame experience often
resort to defensive tactics, or what ACT refers to as experiential avoid-
ance. They may seek to hide or escape shameful feelings, denying
218
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
responsibility. They may seek to shift the blame outside, holding others
responsible for their dilemma. And not infrequently, they become irra-
tionally angry with others, sometimes resorting to overtly aggressive and
destructive actions. In short, shame serves to escalate the very destruc-
tive patterns of behavior we aim to curb.
Contrast this with feelings of guilt, which involve a focus on a spe-
cific behavior—the sense that “I did a bad thing” rather than “I am a bad
person.” Feelings of guilt involve a sense of tension, remorse, and regret
over the “bad thing done.” Research has shown that this sense of tension
and regret typically motivates reparative action (confessing, apologizing,
or somehow repairing the damage done) without engendering all the
defensive and retaliatory responses that are the hallmark of shame (Leith
& Baumeister, 1998; Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007; Tangney,
Youman, & Stuewig, 2009). Most important, guilt is more likely to foster
constructive changes in future behavior because what is at issue is not a
bad, defective self but a bad, defective behavior. And, as anyone knows,
it is easier to change a bad behavior (drunk driving, theft, substance
abuse) than to change a bad, defective self.
Many offenders come in to treatment with a propensity to experi-
ence shame rather than guilt. Some are so defensive that they feel little
of either emotion. The IOC workshop utilizes cognitive-behavioral tech-
niques to foster a more adaptive capacity for moral emotions by (a) using
inductive and educational strategies to foster a capacity for perspective
taking and other- oriented empathy; (b) encouraging participants to
broaden their vision through a better understanding of the impact of
crime and a greater taking of accountability, cutting through minimiza-
tion and denial of criminal actions; (c) understanding the relationships
between victims, offenders, and the community; (d) encouraging appro-
priate experiences of guilt and emphasizing associated constructive moti-
vations to repair or make amends; and (e) explicitly avoiding language
that may be construed as condemning or humiliating inmate partici-
pants as “bad people.” Instead, the strong message is that redemption is
possible.
The IOC curriculum begins with discussions of crime, its conse-
quences, and how to repair the harm caused, in the abstract. Over time,
the completion of workbook exercises, group discussions, and interac-
tions with guest speakers prepares participants to look at their own
actions honestly and to use their newfound understanding to change
219
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
220
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
221
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
how both reinforce each other and highlighting how each serves to over-
come treatment barriers for this challenging population.
222
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
223
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
224
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
225
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
226
Table 2: Pain and Suffering Conceptualization
Difficult Situation Pain Avoidance Behavior Suffering
Inmate A Death of a family Feelings of loss Excessive alcohol use Legal trouble
member Unemployment
Strained relationship
with family members
Inmate B Long history of Feeling of powerlessness, Selling drugs to make Legal trouble
incarceration and frustration, shame quick money Separation from
separation from family daughter and loss of her
Daughter asking for respect
money
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
227
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
228
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
229
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
defusion. Group leaders explain that taking a step back from thoughts
and emotions and adopting an outside perspective can reduce the auto-
matic impact on behavior. To illustrate this, group leaders present an
ACT metaphor of a chessboard (Hayes & Smith, 2005, p. 96). Inmates
are instructed to imagine their internal struggle as a chess game with
positive and negative thoughts and emotions as two opposing teams of
chess pieces. Group leaders then ask the inmates which part of the meta-
phor represents them (e.g., the pieces, the player, or the board).
Participants usually immediately respond that they are the pieces or the
player. Group leaders explain that the board represents an observer per-
spective; it holds the internal content (e.g., thoughts and emotions) but
is not a part of the struggle. Inmates then participate in a visualization-
based centering exercise referred to as the mountain meditation (Bowen
et al., 2011). In this meditation, an image of a mountain is used to repre-
sent a grounded, unmoving, nonreactive presence.
The worksheet assigned at the end of session 6 integrates distress-
monitoring material from sessions 4-6. Participants are first instructed to
identify their own past maladaptive reactions to distress. Then they are
asked to reflect on the distress that they will likely experience in the
future and describe alternative adaptive reactions. This allows partici-
pants to apply the material related to present awareness, short-term adap-
tive coping, and/or observer perspective to their own anticipated distress
(Bornovalova, 2008).
Session Structure
REVAMP is designed to have a consistent structure across sessions
to provide a challenging, yet predictable, group experience. Session 1
differs from the other sessions due to the need to orient participants to
the program by presenting its purpose and goals, enhancing curiosity and
interest in the remainder of the intervention, creating a safe environ-
ment to promote personal participation, and introducing centering exer-
cises for the first time. Sessions 2-8 each follow the same pattern:
• Centering exercise
230
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
• Centering exercise
231
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Acceptability
Outcome data are not yet available from our pilot randomized clini-
cal trial, but evidence suggests that REVAMP is well received by a
diverse group of inmates. All participants were given anonymous feed-
back forms to complete. Overall, feedback has been very positive.
Participants provided ratings on a scale of 1-4 of the intervention’s
quality (M= 3.3), usefulness (M= 3.5), and their overall satisfaction
with the program (M= 3.6). Retention throughout the program has
been strong for a jail-based program, comparing very favorably with
attendance observed in other multiweek programs and interventions
offered at this jail.
Applicability
REVAMP was designed to be generally applicable to a broad range of
inmate participants that are diverse in terms of age, ethnicity, criminal
history, values, and barriers (e.g., mental health concerns, substance abuse
problems, etc.). Accordingly, participants in REVAMP were eligible for the
study if they met basic requirements meant to ensure the ability to partici-
pate in the program (e.g., were assigned to the general population, had
remaining sentences long enough to participate in the program, would be
released directly to the community). Participation in REVAMP was
entirely voluntary. Thus far, participants in this program have ranged in
age (from 18 to 81), criminal history, and type of instant offense.
In sum, the 8-session REVAMP treatment has dual aims of reducing
distress and enhancing values-based living. REVAMP is specifically tai-
lored to the jail setting, and preliminary feedback suggests that it has
been well received by a diverse group of inmates. We are in the process
of collecting empirical data on REVAMP’s effectiveness.
232
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
233
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
234
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
235
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
236
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
237
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
References
Bornovalova, M.A. (2008). Distress tolerance treatment for inner-city drug users: A
preliminary trial. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from Digital Repository at
the University of Maryland. (http://hdl.handle.net/1903/8446)
Bowen, S., Chawla, N., & Marlatt, G. A. (2011). Mindfulness-based relapse prevention
for addictive behaviors: A clinician’s guide. New York: Guilford Press.
Burke, B. L., Arkowitz, H., & Menchola, M. (2003). The efficacy of motivational
interviewing: A meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 71, 843-861.
Conn, C., Warden, R., Stuewig, J., Kim, E. H., Harty, L., Hastings, M., & Tangney,
J. P. (2010). Borderline personality disorder among jail inmates: How common,
and how distinct? Corrections Compendium, 4, 6-13.
Drapalski, A., Youman, K., Stuewig, J., & Tangney, J. P. (2009). Gender differences
in jail inmates’ symptoms of mental illness, treatment history and treatment
seeking. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 19, 193-206.
Easton, C., Swan, S., & Sinha, R. (2000). Motivation to change substance use
among offenders of domestic violence. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment,
19, 1-5.
Ginsburg, J. L. D., Mann, R. E., Rotgers, F., & Weekes, J. R. (2002). Motivational
interviewing with criminal justice populations. In W. R. Miller & S. Rollnick
(eds.), Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change (2nd ed.) (pp. 333-
346). New York: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, relational frame
theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior
Therapy, 35, 639-665.
Hayes, S. C., & Smith, S. (2005). Get out of your mind and into your life: The new
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996).
Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional
approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 64, 1152-1168.
Hettema, J., Steele, J., & Miller, W. R. (2005). Motivational interviewing. Annual
Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 91-111.
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and narratives of
interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking.
Journal of Personality, 66, 1-37.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral treatment of borderline personality disor-
der. New York: Guilford Press.
238
Accepting Guilt and Abandoning Shame
Meyer, C. R., Tangney, J. P., & Stuewig, J. (under review). Why do some jail inmates
not engage in treatment and services?
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for
change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
O’Leary Tevyaw, T., & Monti, P. M. (2004). Motivational enhancement and other
brief interventions for adolescent substance abuse: Foundations, applications
and evaluations. Addiction, 99, 63-75.
Rubak, S., Sandbæk, A., Laurizen, T., Christensen, B. (2005). Motivational inter-
viewing: A systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of General
Practice, 55, 305-312.
Stein, L. A. R., Colby, S. M., Barnett, N. P., Monti, P. M., Golembeske, C., &
Lebeau-Craven, R. (2006). Effects of motivational interviewing for incarcer-
ated adolescents on driving under the influence after release. The American
Journal on Addictions, 15, 50-57.
Tangney, J. P., Malouf, E. T., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. (2012). Emotions and moral-
ity: You don’t have to feel really bad to be good. In M. W. Eysenck, M.
Fajkowska, & T. Maruszewski (Eds.), Warsaw lectures on personality and social
psychology. Personality, cognition and emotion (Vol. 2). New York: Eliot Werner.
Tangney, J. P., Mashek, D., & Stuewig, J. (2007). Working at the social-clinical-
community- criminology interface: The George Mason University Inmate
Study. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 1-21.
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Hafez, L. (2011). Shame, guilt and remorse: Implications
for offender populations. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 22(5),
706-723.
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral
behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345-372.
Tangney, J. P., Youman, K., & Stuewig, J. (2009). Proneness to shame and proneness
to guilt. In M.R. Leary & R.H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences
in social behavior (pp. 192-209). New York: Guilford Press.
VA Maryland Health Care System Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Team.
(2007). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Group Therapy Protocol:
Addictions Intensive Outpatient Program PTSD/Substance Use Dual Diagnosis
Program. Unpublished therapy manual.
Walters, S.T., Clark, M.D., Gingerich, R., & Meltzer, M.L. (2007). A guide for proba-
tion and parole: Motivating offenders to change. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of
Corrections.
Youman, K., Drapalski, A., Stuewig, J., Bagley, K., & Tangney, J. P. (2010). Race
differences in psychopathology and disparities in treatment seeking:
Community and jail-based treatment seeking patterns. Psychological Services, 7,
11-26.
239
CHAPTER 11
Michael F. Steger
Colorado State University
North-West University, South Africa
Kelly Sheline
Colorado State University
Leslie Merriman
Colorado State University
Todd B. Kashdan
George Mason University
Using the Science of Meaning
241
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
242
Using the Science of Meaning
dynamic texture of life as it is lived, full of the good, the bad, the ambiva-
lent, and the uncertain. A thoughtfully orchestrated merger between
ACT and meaning in life work could help achieve this growth.
Meaning in life research is about going beyond “feel-good” motives
to address a more complex landscape of human strivings. Although most
would associate a meaningful life with feeling good, people are also adept
at finding meaning under awful circumstances. Everyone’s life brings
some ambiguity, some struggle, and some unique burden. Meaning in life
is built from all of those experiences, as well as the happy moments. The
birth of a child and the death of a parent are both meaningful, though
either could be positive, negative, or a blend. As another example, creat-
ing meaning in life can often lead to scapegoating and the creation of
enemies. In ambiguous situations where something bad happens, we look
to make sense of it all (Sullivan, Landau, & Rothschild, 2010). Threats
of meaninglessness increase the likelihood of blaming other people for
the unfortunate situation, which temporarily restores a sense of meaning
(Sullivan, Landau, & Rothschild, 2010). Thus, when and how the pursuit
of meaning is healthy is not a simple question with a single answer.
Meaning in life shifts the emphasis from what feels good for the
individual to what is important for the individual, for his/her group, and
for the wider universe. Meaning in life also focuses on the journey, rather
than the hedonic outcomes of the journey (Steger, Kashdan, & Oishi,
2008), and thereby provides a language and stable framework for sustain-
ing fulfillment even in the face of pain and setbacks. With the right
interventions to guide people, the emphasis can be on the greatest psy-
chological benefits and the least costs to detecting and creating meaning
in areas that matter the most.
This suggests an immediate resonance with ACT’s emphasis on
experiential acceptance rather than avoidance. Much of the research lit-
erature on avoidance has contrasted the erosive influence of attempting
to avoid negative states, thoughts, and experiences with the constructive
influence of attempting instead to achieve, or obtain, positive states,
thoughts, and experiences (e.g., Elliott & Thrash, 2002). Although the
message that “avoiding bad is bad” has generally gotten across in positive
psychology, perhaps the message that “seeking good is good” may have
been overapplied. At times, the clamor for more happiness seems to have
created a warped psychological tourism, with metaphorical busloads of
people being driven past a sequence of great positive attributes to acquire.
243
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
244
Using the Science of Meaning
do, accomplish, and aspire to. This dimension has been nearly univer-
sally referred to as purpose (Kashdan & McKnight, 2009; McKnight &
Kashdan, 2009; Reker, 2000). Taken together, meaning in life is the
degree to which an individual makes sense of and sees significance in
their life and believes his or her life to have an overarching purpose
(Steger, 2009).
These two dimensions of meaning in life are readily connected to
key processes that ACT proposes lead to psychopathology and rigidity.
Comprehension links nicely with the dominance of people’s ideas about
their past and their future, which prevents nondefensive, open contact
with what is being experienced in the present. When people become lost
in their ideas about the past and future, it blocks self-development and
the ability to grow from wanted and unwanted experiences that occur in
each moment. Experiential avoidance renders people’s comprehension
systems inaccurate, fragile, and constantly in need of protection.
Comprehension also can be linked to people’s attachment to the self
as they conceive of it rather than as they experience it. When the self is
viewed through the prism of feelings, thoughts, and ideas about who one
is (the content of one’s mind; Luoma, Hayes, & Walser, 2007), the poten-
tial for personal growth is limited. As with the first pathological process
of experiential avoidance, becoming overly attached to particular con-
ceptualizations of ourselves can divide us from knowledge of our true
self. The comprehension dimension of meaning in life emphasizes the
important role played by our conceptualizations and understandings of
ourselves, the world around us, and our interactions with the world. By
incorporating the world as the context for personal experience and by
calling attention to the ways in which people understand the dynamic
interactions of their selves and changing contexts, the comprehension
dimension is broader than the two pathological processes identified in
ACT. Yet, by addressing people’s orientation to past and future events as
potential barriers of authentic experience, ACT is broader than meaning
in life theory. Whereas ACT often focuses on creating a better life by
helping people detach from unhelpful self-concepts (defusion) and main-
tain an accepting attitude about what is happening in the present
moment (mindfulness), meaning in life is about how to elaborate the
sense of self in a productive way. That is, by attending to meaning in life,
individuals create a better life through the expansion of the self out into
245
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
the world and into the abstract realm of people’s ultimate concerns. Both
ACT and meaning in life can be strengthened through collaboration.
246
Using the Science of Meaning
• Understanding Mandy
Mandy is a 34-year-old woman whose divorce was finalized 6
weeks ago. She shares custody of her two daughters with her
former husband. If you ask Mandy why she is divorced, she says
it’s because her husband wanted to “upgrade his wife.” In fact, he
is already living with someone. Like her ex-husband, her ex’s
new partner is a middle manager at a large electrical parts man-
ufacturing corporation. Mandy has been working for a title
company for 12 years, moving up to underwriter during the start
of the housing boom. Her income at the peak easily rivaled her
ex-husband’s and they enjoyed a high standard of living. In the
past few years, so few home sales have closed that Mandy’s
income has plummeted.
Mandy expresses little hope for the future, worries about the
bad influence of the new woman in her children’s lives, and has
recently emerged from a prolonged major depressive episode.
She now expresses constant worries about doing a poor job of
raising her kids, having something bad happen to them while
they are in her ex-husband’s custody, and losing her job and her
house. She says that her sleep and work performance have suf-
fered because of the time she spends worrying and the intensity
of her anxiety, particularly over the weekends when her children
are in her ex-husband’s care. When pressed, Mandy admits that
she deeply misses the days when her income was so high that she
never worried about money.
247
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
248
Using the Science of Meaning
they believe and never have to question, or the truths that they
hold to be self-evident. For example, clients might think about
their family life. I will never turn down a hug from my children.
Each child will be treated as a unique individual. Whenever pos-
sible, the family will eat dinner together. Or, I will never disci-
pline my children like my parents disciplined me. When clients
return with this homework, clinicians lead a discussion about
the implications and the themes of these givens, helping to illu-
minate the fundamental assumptions— the most basic
comprehensions—people have. These may need to be chal-
lenged in order to move forward, so clients should be encouraged
and empowered to be completely honest about their givens. In
the parenting examples above, despite their laudable nature,
“absolute” phrasing often reveals reactive givens—those that are
generated because of somewhat unresolved pain. This is most
clear in the given about never disciplining like one’s parents.
This might or might not be a great idea, but the absolute nature
of that given likely sets a person up for failure and may even be
used to prevent effective and reasonable responses to a child’s
behavior. Therapists and coaches are a great resource for helping
clients discover and reconsider their most basic comprehensions
of the world.
2. Values Work. The aim of both ACT and a meaning in life stand-
point is to assist and inspire people to live in accordance with
what they care most deeply about. This is tough to do without
exploring one’s values; an act done less often than presumed.
Here, we draw on Schwartz, Kurtines, & Montgomery (2005) to
give clients a concrete way to think about values (for additional
activities, see Shin and Steger, 2012).
Ask your client to identify one or two of the important life
choices or dilemmas he or she has faced in the past. Ask the
client to describe what made that situation so difficult. For
example, many clients face hard choices between dedicating
their time and effort to their careers or to their families. Often
clients will say that there were no good choices or that which-
ever decision they made they would let someone down. Make
note of the themes that arise, but do not address them yet. These
249
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
250
Using the Science of Meaning
asks you as a clinician about what you do best, what you most want to
give to your clients, and the lasting impact you would most desire to have
on clients. Their exercise can provide a language you can use when
working with clients to explore their values. This includes those moments
when there is conflict between your values and those of clients. Managing
conflicts requires tolerance for tension, self-honesty, and candid dialogue
with clients about how our values impact the way we interpret what our
clients tell us.
Finally, we must face the possibility that some people’s values are
morally objectionable. As one of the authors (MFS) likes to say “Saints
have holy purposes, and assholes have assholey purposes.” Based on
available research, most people have pretty decent purposes, some have
awe-inspiring purposes, and a small portion have horrible purposes.
Working with this latter group is difficult, and therapy may need to
proceed by showing how it is eventually in the client’s selfish interests to
figure out how to have reasonably prosocial values and purpose.
251
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
252
Using the Science of Meaning
well suited for helping clients chart out their existing values and compre-
hension systems, we believe it can also be used to revise and clarify a
newly emerging comprehension system in the wake of negative life
events. We call this intervention “The Photojournalist.” We also suggest
another way of eliciting growth narratives from clients, which can be
used to alter or rebuild comprehension systems.
253
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
• Mobilizing Mandy
When her therapist asks her about what she wants to accom-
plish in her life, Mandy is vague. She says she wants her daughter
to be happy and that she wants to be successful again, but when
pressed for details, she seems to become hopeless and says she’d
just be content if her ex-husband would pay for what he did.
Mandy says she is a good person at heart and that she still trusts
people, but that she feels vulnerable because of this experience
and “not like herself.” She perseverates about her husband’s new
life and frets over comparisons between herself and his new wife.
She is not sure what she wants to accomplish any more, but she
says that the most important thing to her right now is to give her
daughters a good role model. Part of this, for Mandy, is showing
how to be strong and recover from setbacks.
254
Using the Science of Meaning
255
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
you can ask your client how she or he shows the world that these
are important values. Essentially, how do other people know
what your client stands for without your client verbalizing. This
line of questioning may identify behaviors your client has already
used to activate values. You can continue this line of question-
ing by asking your client about what she or he is really good at
doing, what people compliment her or him for, and what she or
he does that feels most authentic. In other words, you are asking
your client about personal strengths. Finally, you can ask your
client what it would be like to use his or her strengths to act out
their values for the world to see. This line of questioning should
continue to push your client to talk about what the end result of
all of this would be—ideally, if we used the best of ourselves to
act out what we truly valued, we would do so in the service of a
natural purpose.
256
Using the Science of Meaning
her stagnant career with her therapist, she realizes that although
she enjoyed it at its peak, being a title officer was a job she backed
into rather than chose. Through her work experience, she came
to appreciate her talents and came to enjoy some of her work
activities. Currently, she cannot see how what she is doing at her
present job does anything to support her value of providing a
better life for her daughters. As she relinquishes her desire to
compete with—and hurt—her ex-husband, she explores her
values, talents, and interests. She realizes that she can provide a
better world for her daughters in a way that has nothing to do
with whether she earns as much as her ex-husband. This will
entail making sure that her daughters understand what hap-
pened to their family and that, even though she is hurt, Mandy
is still strong, raising them with positive values, encouraging
their dreams. In her work, she sees that she can use her book-
keeping skills to maintain steady employment while she attends
weekend classes in property law at a law school that caters to
midcareer professionals. In Mandy’s case, her value of a better
future for her daughters mingled with the craving for stability
and permanence that grew from the divorce to prompt a new
purpose: working to preserve the landscape for future genera-
tions through conservation and agricultural easements.
257
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
258
Using the Science of Meaning
In Mandy’s case, once she had identified a purpose for her career, it
became clear that there was a conflict with another purpose. Mandy
could not both attend a traditional law school and be there for her daugh-
ters during the week. Rather than giving up and viewing her purpose as
unobtainable (and herself as worthless), Mandy found another road. She
registered for weekend classes through a nontraditional law school to
gain competencies and qualifications to work effectively in her chosen
area of land preservation easements. In making this decision, she needed
to be creative and relinquish superficial needs for prestige (in the eyes of
others). It was a longer road to her career purpose, but she found a way to
encounter both life aims along the path she chose.
Mindfulness
Whereas ACT has brought a great deal of attention to mindfulness,
meaning in life research and practice has been mostly silent. Maintaining
an accepting attitude as each moment unfolds in the present would seem
to set the stage for meaning making. One isolated research study has
259
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
260
Using the Science of Meaning
261
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
We argue for the benefit of also altering language itself so that greater
attention is given to the dimensions of meaning: (a) comprehension and
(b) purpose in life. Rather than frantic avoidance, nervous rigidity, or, we
would say, ceaseless pursuit of the secret key to the good life, the lan-
guage and narrative of people’s lives should be founded on cherished
values, beliefs they have tested, a clear-eyed appraisal of their abilities
and shortcomings, and ultimate aspirations for their one shot at exis-
tence. If meaning is woven into the language by which people meet the
world, then mindfulness does not need to conflict with a desire to live in
accordance with one’s purpose— it is a way of filling one’s present
moments with gentle reassurances of what one comprehends in life and
navigational nudges toward one’s purpose.
A meaning in life approach offers ACT a way to conceptualize those
“larger and larger patterns.” Meaning in life research directly addresses
how behaviors and values, as a holistic unit, bring one’s life into harmony
by serving a greater purpose. In conjunction with ACT’s model, meaning
in life offers the people we work with greater cohesion in mindfully
instilling the moments of their lives with value and purpose.
262
Using the Science of Meaning
captures the sense that we are useful, functioning, important parts of the
world, that we ourselves “have a point.” Losing this sense of contribution
and usefulness was among the earliest identified causes of suicidal behav-
ior (Durkheim, 1897/1953). Developing an individual’s sense of meaning
in the context of a therapeutic relationship has promising applications
for decreasing suicidal ideations and possibly preventing suicide.
Research indicates that suicide is associated with hopelessness, a
negative perception of one’s future (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979),
and a lack of meaning in life (e.g., Rothermund & Brandtstädter, 2003).
Unfortunately, there are few empirical studies examining meaning-based
interventions in suicidal populations. Research by Edwards and Holden
(2001) suggests that life meaning acts as a buffer between coping style
and suicidal manifestations, and the authors argue for the addition of
meaning variables to improve the accuracy of the prediction of suicidal-
ity. Having reasons for living and/or leading a meaningful life is incom-
patible with suicide. This is an idea supported by research in which early
retirees who expressed suicidal ideation were enrolled in a personal goals
management program (Lapierre, Dubé, Bouffard, & Alain, 2007).
Following the program, participants reported increased self-efficacy in
reaching their goals and greater psychological well-being. Meaning in life
is all about linking goals to a grander life purpose.
Conclusions
As we have argued throughout this chapter, the core processes of ACT—
encouraging clients to think about what they really want in life, reflect-
ing on avoidance and control strategies, and focusing on becoming aware
of important personal values and making decisions based on those
values—are not merely harmonious with, but perfectly suited to take
advantage of the additional leverage provided by meaning in life. As a
growing body of literature has shown, ACT can achieve medium to large
clinical effects among people with depression (e.g., Bohlmeijer, Fledderus,
Rokx, & Pieterse, 2011), and research is ever-emerging on its benefits for
those with other psychological disorders. We suggest that by combining
the applications of ACT with the encompassing perspective of meaning
in life, more effective treatment strategies can be developed to help
clients live the lives they want. By anchoring clients’ personal values in
263
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
References
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual
and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125–143.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford Press.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interper-
sonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
117, 497-529.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depres-
sion. New York: Guilford Press.
Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B, & Minhas, G. (2011). A dynamic approach to
psychological strength development and intervention. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 6, 106-118.
Bohlmeijer, E. T., Fledderus, M., Rokx, T. A. J. J., & Pieterse, M. E. (2011). Efficacy
of an early intervention based on Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for
adults with depressive symptomatology: Evaluation in a randomized controlled
trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 62-67.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness
and its role in psychological well- being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 822-848.
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical
foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18,
211-237.
Cohen, S. R., Mount, B. M., Tomas, J. J. N., & Mount, L. F. (1996). Existential well-
being is an important determinant of quality of life. Cancer, 77, 576-586.
Davis, C. G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J. (1998). Making sense of loss and
growing from the experience: Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 75, 561-574.
DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of
137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124,
197-229.
Durkheim, E. (1897, 1953). Suicide. New York: Free Press.
Edwards, M. J., & Holden, R. R. (2001). Coping, meaning in life, and suicidal mani-
festations. Examining Gender Differences, 57, 1517-1534.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personal-
ity: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 82, 804-818.
264
Using the Science of Meaning
265
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
266
CHAPTER 12
Bryan Roche
National University of Ireland, Maynooth
Sarah Cassidy
Smithsfield Clinic, Co. Meath, Ireland
Ian Stewart
National University of Ireland, Galway
Authors’ footnote: Visit RaiseYourIQ.com for online relational frame training designed
to improve general intellectual ability.
268
Nurturing Genius
269
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
270
Nurturing Genius
271
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
(Raven, Raven, & Court, 2003). The findings suggested that cognitive
training may indeed show general effects that extend beyond the task
type used during training. The results were particularly exciting because
both fluid intelligence (Rohde & Thompson, 2007) and memory ability
(Pickering, 2006) are associated with scholastic success.
Jäeggi and colleagues (Jäeggi, Studer- Luethi, Buschkuehl, Su,
Jonides, & Perrig, 2010) later found that performance on the n-back task
is more strongly correlated with two measures of Gf than with measures
of working memory. In a second experiment reported in their 2010 paper,
two groups of students were trained for four weeks with either a single or
a dual n-back intervention. A control group received no training. The
researchers reported that improvements in Gf were more marked than
improvements in working memory following practice at either n-back test
compared to the outcome for the control group (see also Jäeggi et al.,
2011, for a similar study that also demonstrated maintenance of Gf
increases across a three-month follow-up period).
It is important to understand that fluid intelligence is only one com-
ponent of general intelligence and improvements in fluid intelligence
should not be mistaken for improvements in full-scale IQ (see Flynn,
1987). More specifically, fluid intelligence is associated strongly with
working memory and attentional skills such as speed of information pro-
cessing. Full-scale IQ, however, is measured across a broader ranger of
skills than this. Moreover, in full-scale IQ testing, scores on tasks that
measure working memory and speed of processing information (e.g., on
the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children [WISC]) can be ignored in
calculating overall IQ if the clinician or psychometrician believes the test
taker has attention deficits. Nevertheless, memory and attentional skills
are important in an educational context, and it is likely that n-back pro-
cedure training works by improving these skills, thus enhancing the indi-
vidual’s ability to attend to and remember key aspects of problems,
solutions to which the individual is already capable of providing.
Behavioral psychologists have also made occasional efforts to
improve intelligent behavior skills (and therefore IQ scores), usually with
special needs populations. The late O. Ivar Lovaas (1987) reported IQ
gains up to 30 points (roughly two standard deviations) following a three-
year applied behavior analysis (ABA) intervention for autism. Nearly
half of the autistic children in that study were not noticeably intellectu-
ally different from normally functioning children after the three-year
272
Nurturing Genius
273
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
274
Nurturing Genius
speaking the word “doll” aloud and then asking the child to repeat the
word.
Now suppose that the mother then reverses this relation across mul-
tiple other exemplars. Specifically, the mother first says the word “doll”
and asks the child to point to the doll (word-object relation). Once this
word-object relation has been reliably established the relation has become
bidirectional. Importantly, however, the relation was not derived by the
child; it was directly established in both directions by the mother. Across
a sufficient number of such directly trained bidirectional relations estab-
lished across numerous objects and words, a pattern of spontaneous bidi-
rectional relational responding will eventually emerge for new objects
and words (see Hayes, Fox, Gifford et al., 2001, pp. 26-27). For example,
the mother might teach the child a novel object-name relation by point-
ing to a novel object (e.g., a cat), producing its name and prompting the
child to repeat that name (e.g., “This is a cat. Can you say cat?”). This
trains a relation from “object” to “name.” If the mother subsequently asks
“Where is the cat?” the child might spontaneously point out the animal,
thus showing the derived or untrained “name-object” (i.e., reverse) rela-
tion. Derivation might also happen in the opposite direction so that
being given a novel name (e.g., “chocolate”) and taught to pick the
correct object (e.g., a chocolate bar) results in the untrained response
that given the object (e.g., seeing a similar chocolate bar on TV) a par-
ticular name is the correct one (e.g., the child says “chocolate” without
prompting).
Behavioral research has long suggested that the ability to derive rela-
tions between stimuli in this way is likely a very important skill for lan-
guage learning (e.g., Barnes, McCullagh, & Keenan, 1990; Devany,
Hayes, & Nelson, 1986). This is because language is replete with word-
sound associations that are entirely arbitrary (i.e., a child cannot figure
out what the sound “dog” refers to in print because it is an arbitrary
sound and its relation to the printed letters D-O-G must be taught).
Where the ability to derive stimulus relations is very fluent, we should
expect to see excellent vocabulary acquisition and rapid and improved
ability to remember word meanings, because the ability to derive (and, if
necessary, “re-derive”) relationships supports these repertoires.
Relational frame theory researchers maintain, for both theoretical
and empirical reasons, that the ability to derive relations must be learned
(i.e., it is not an innate ability). RFT explains how this emergence occurs
275
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
and attempts to identify the most important types of relations (e.g., more,
less, before, after, opposite, different), or relational frames, involved in
intellectual, behavioral, and emotional development. It may sound
radical, but RFT researchers attempt to understand the full array of cog-
nitive skills (e.g., language, deductive reasoning, problem solving) in
terms of a relatively small range of relational frames. From an RFT per-
spective, much of what is measured in standard IQ tests can be under-
stood in terms of relational frames. It is both a conceptual and empirical
matter to work out precisely which relations or combinations of relations
are involved in which types of intellectual skills. Nevertheless, the idea
that we may have already identified a basic behavioral unit (i.e., rela-
tional responding) of intelligence is exciting to say the least. The most
obvious implication of this is that by enhancing those fundamental rela-
tional framing skills, we may be able to move the entire intellectual skills
repertoire in one single intervention.
Importantly, the ability to derive relations between new words and
objects will remain under contextual control so that the relevant relation-
ship between any two items is always specified by a cue, such as the word
“Same.” That is, in speech we will always use (in one way or another) one
of the relational frame labels to specify for a child which relation we are
talking about when we refer to two objects, or a word-object relation
(e.g., “show me something bigger than your doll”).
Through ongoing exposure to and interactions with the socioverbal
community, relational framing becomes increasingly fluent, and the rela-
tions involved become more and more abstracted so that the ability to
frame events relationally becomes increasingly unhinged from any par-
ticular words and objects. It might be helpful for the reader to think of
mathematical relationships (e.g., 3 + 5 is the same as 8) as examples of
fully abstracted relations that are arbitrarily applicable to any set of items
(e.g., sheep, apples, widgets). In simple terms, the child has now learned
the very rules of derived relational responding and can even state them.
It is at this point that we can ask a child, for example, to imagine x
number of items that is more than y number of items. The relations and
the rules of derived relational responding are not violated by the use of
the algebraic terms x and y, but in this case there are no particular stimuli
specified at all.
So fundamental is derived relational responding ability to a whole
host of cognitive tasks that RFT theorists have suggested that it may
276
Nurturing Genius
277
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
278
Nurturing Genius
279
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
280
Nurturing Genius
281
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
3 The reader should note that establishing fluency from 1-10 is required for full
numerical competence because in the modern Western world we use a numerical system
to the base of 10. If we decided to switch to a number base system of 5, we would need
only to establish full relational fluency with the numbers 1-5 to allow counting to infin-
ity and the effective dealing with mathematical problems in precisely the same way as
we do presently.
282
Nurturing Genius
283
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Summary
Clearly, there are many more types of relational frames that we might
analyze in the current chapter (e.g., deictic or “perspective” relations,
analogical relations, etc.). However, the foregoing was intended only to
illustrate the types of interpretive analyses possible given the nomencla-
ture and concepts developed by RFT. They are all provisional and open
to correction by empirical analysis. The important point to take away,
however, is that by conceiving intellectual ability as measured by IQ tests
and other forms of educational assessment as relational framing ability,
at least to some extent, the possibility of improving this ability immedi-
ately presents itself, at least if relational skills can themselves be taught
and enhanced.
284
Nurturing Genius
285
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
286
Nurturing Genius
287
288
Table 1
Control Experimental
Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
Full Scale IQ Baseline 106.50 3.32 104-111 105.50 10.66 96-119
SE Training 107.25 4.79 101-111 110.25 5.74 105-118
Relational Training 104.25 3.86 99-108 132.75 4.03 128-137
Verbal IQ Baseline 108.25 4.86 101-111 109.25 8.88 101-120
SE Training 107.50 6.66 98-113 107.75 9.03 100-120
Relational Training 108.50 8.85 99-117 127.00 12.99 111-139
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Means, standard deviations, and IQ ranges for control and experimental participants at baseline, following stimulus equiva-
lence (SE) and relational frame training in Experiment 1 of Cassidy et al. (2011).
Nurturing Genius
289
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
follow-up testing took place. It may well be, therefore, that attentional
skills were deteriorating during this period in the absence of the usual
scholastic contingencies. Nevertheless, skills in those IQ domains repre-
senting pure relational ability continued to grow and develop across time
since the intervention. This is not at all surprising from a relational
frame theory perspective. We would fully expect a relational training
intervention to enhance repertoires that would make learning more sen-
sitive to instruction and experience on an ongoing basis. The effect of a
relational frame training intervention, therefore, should be accumulative
as well as immediate. The fact that the improved full-scale IQs reported
in Experiment 2 of Cassidy et al. (2011) have maintained well across time
strongly suggests that the relational training intervention successfully
targeted skills sets that were of enduring importance in the ongoing
intellectual and educational activities of the children.
290
Table 2
Full Scale IQ 82.9 8.3 70-92 95.9 10.6 76-111 102.57 12.19 91-126
Verbal Comprehension 82.3 7.3 73-93 92.4 9.2 83-110 100.86 6.91 90-112
Perceptual Reasoning 82.1 10.3 65-96 94.5 6.7 84-106 103.14 16.12 83-123
Working Memory 94.9 16.6 59-116 97.5 12.3 77-116 88.14 14.78 65-103
Processing Speed 91.0 9.8 83-109 107.0 15.6 78-121 98.86 10.33 86-114
Means, standard deviations, and ranges in full-scale IQ in all four IQ domains at baseline and postintervention as reported
in Experiment 2 of Cassidy et al. (2011). Previously unpublished four-year follow-up IQ scores in all four IQ domains are also
provided in the rightmost columns of the table for the seven of the eight children that were available.
291
Nurturing Genius
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
292
Nurturing Genius
293
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
5 Note that in this particular example, coins of a larger size (e.g., a nickel) are worth
less than coins of a smaller physical size (e.g., a dime), and so the monetary value of the
stimuli is not ascertainable from the formal size of the coins (i.e., their nonarbitrary
features). To create a potentially infinite variety of “coins” for use in these exercises,
simply make them out of pieces of colored paper cut into circles.
294
Nurturing Genius
295
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Conclusion
The current chapter examined a behavior-analytic and RFT conceptual-
ization of intelligence and illustrated how it might help us to better
understand in practical terms precisely which skill sets IQ tests measure.
In doing so, we have been able to point the way toward intervention
formats that may prove capable of helping to raise intelligence levels.
This is a feat that was previously considered impossible by psychologists.
Of course, our analysis is somewhat speculative and far from complete. It
296
Nurturing Genius
References
Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M.,
Morris, J. N., Rebok, G. W., Smith, D. M., Tennstedt, S. L., Unverzagt, F. W.,
& Willis, S. L. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older
adults: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 288, 2271-2281.
Barnes, D., McCullagh, P., & Keenan, M. (1990). Equivalence class formation in
non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children. Analysis of
Verbal Behavior, 8, 1-11.
297
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayden, E., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2008). The
Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a response-time and
event-related-potentials methodology for testing natural verbal relations. The
Psychological Record, 58, 497-516.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Exemplar training and
a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry. The
Psychological Record, 51, 287-308.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., Healy, O., Lyddy, F., Cullinan,
V., & Hayes, S. C. (2001). Psychological development. In S. C. Hayes, D.
Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian
account of human language and cognition (p. 161). New York: Plenum.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. (2001). Exemplar
training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symme-
try II. The Psychological Record, 51, 589-603.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. (2004). Establishing relational
responding in accordance with opposite as generalized operant behavior in
young children. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4,
559-586.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P., Strand, P., & Friman, P. (2004).
Establishing relational responding in accordance with more-than and less-
than as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of
Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 531-558.
Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-
local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2,
249-255.
Belleville, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., Gagnon, L., Menard, E., & Gauthier, S.
(2006). Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive
impairment and healthy older adults: Evidence from a cognitive intervention
program. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22, 486-99.
Berens, N. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations:
Experimental evidence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 40, 45-71.
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training interven-
tion to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61,
173-198.
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational frame theory and human
intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11, 37-51.
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experi-
ment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1-22.
Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its
cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental
Psychology, 27, 703-722.
Chabris, C. F. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the “Mozart effect”? Nature, 400,
827-828.
298
Nurturing Genius
299
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
300
Nurturing Genius
O’Hora, D., Pelaez, M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005). Derived relational responding
and performance on verbal sub-tests of the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record,
55, 155-175.
O’Hora, D., Pelaez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudary, T.
(2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational perfor-
mance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58,
569-584.
O’Hora, D., Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2002). Response
latencies to multiple derived stimulus relations: Testing two predictions of rela-
tional frame theory. The Psychological Record, 52, 51-76.
O’Toole, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Three chronometric indices of relational
responding as predictors of performance on a brief intelligence test: The
importance of relational flexibility. The Psychological Record, 59, 119-132.
Pickering, S. (Ed.). (2006). Working memory and education. Oxford: Elsevier.
Ramsden, S., Richardson, F. M., Josse, G., Thomas, M. S. C., Ellis, C., Shakeshaft,
C., Seghier M. L., & Price, C. J. (2011). Verbal and non-verbal intelligence
changes in the teenage brain. Nature, 479, 113-116.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1993). Music and spatial task perfor-
mance. Nature, 365, 611.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2003). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices
and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Reed, P., Osborne, L., & Corness, M. (2005). The effectiveness of early intervention
programmes for autistic spectrum disorders. A Report for the South East Regional
Special Educational Needs Partnership. Research Partners: Bexley, Brighton &
Hove, East Sussex, Kent, Midway, Surrey, West Sussex.
Rohde, T. E., & Thompson, L. A. (2007). Predicting academic achievement with
cognitive ability. Intelligence 35, 83-92.
Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Replicating Lovaas’ treatment and findings:
Preliminary results. PEACH. Putting Research into Practice Conference, London.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods
in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of
research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262-274.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An intro-
duction. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14.
Seligman, M. E. P., Ernst, R. M., Gilham, J., Reivich, K., & Linkins, M. (2009).
Positive education: Positive psychology and classroom interventions. Oxford
Review of Education, 35, 293-311.
Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 14, 5-13.
Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination versus matching to
sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22.
Smith, G. E., Housen, P., Yaffe, K., Ruff, R., Kennison, R. F., Mahncke, H. W., &
Zelinski, E. M. (2009). A cognitive training program based on principles of
301
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
302
CHAPTER 13
Steven C. Hayes
University of Nevada
They are not about being right or ending up on top; they are not about
power and politics; they are not about who thought of it first or last; they
are about seeing what can be gained through mutual understanding.
Most books that are meant to be conversations are structured in a
relatively rote way for structural reasons: books are linear, it is easier for
authors to state again what they have stated before, and the ideas of
others have to be considered by authors with different views, which is
difficult. But with the right posture of authors, and indeed of readers,
even the linear dictates of the written page can yield a genuine conversa-
tion. Instead of authors arriving ready to defend their views, and readers
arriving ready to be proven right in their existing beliefs, authors can do
their best to consider other points of view, and readers can bring an open
posture to the experience of reading the sequential monodies in the
volume, integrating them into a larger conversation.
Even before the exchange of ideas begins, a real conversation is made
more likely if these processes are embraced psychologically in the inter-
ests of the positive potential of dialogue. People who are interested in
positive psychology, or in acceptance, mindfulness, and values, seem par-
ticularly likely to have the psychological tools needed to enter such a
genuine conversation as writers and readers. Both of these areas address
issues such as values, relationship, empathy, compassion, and perspective
taking that are central to any genuine social interchange.
I was allowed the opportunity in this brief closing chapter to con-
sider the other chapters and to reflect on what this project may mean
going forward. It is my sense that the editors and authors of this book
have created a genuine serial conversation, and if the reader has
approached these pages with openness to possibilities, there is much to
be gained by the experience. Most of the chapters made serious attempts
to consider the interconnections between these different ideas and tradi-
tions, and they have done so in a transparent way that readers can build
upon. It does, however, require openness on the part of readers to make
sure that the potential of a genuine conversation is realized.
In this chapter I would like to explore a few of the areas of overlap
and connection that I saw, looking broadly across the chapters. Rather
then get into details I will try to bring these areas down to the ground by
speaking more generally, focusing on what practitioners and researchers
might do going forward. This book is meant to help guide teachers,
304
The Genuine Conversation
305
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
306
The Genuine Conversation
307
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
308
The Genuine Conversation
309
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
and subtractive. Just as psychology has been too focused on the negative,
it has been too focused on the subtractive approach to experience. Efforts
in experiential subtraction are rarely functionally positive regardless of
their putative purpose. Acceptance and mindfulness methods teach
people how to live more additively, and in that sense “positively,” in
which present events are present, regardless of form, and the key ques-
tion is what to add next. Ironically, negative emotions can be a great
arena in which to learn these “additive” psychological skills such as
mindfulness, curiosity, and noticing without judgment, but these same
skills apply equally to positive emotions. In some contexts it is helpful to
appreciate and savor positive emotions; in others it is more a matter of
tipping one’s hat to them and moving on to the next challenge or oppor-
tunity. As Jack Kornfield said in the title of his book, “After the ecstasy,
the laundry” (2001).
310
The Genuine Conversation
In its very name, ACT is committed to that area and has made good
progress in understanding values, but positive psychology has provided
many useful leads about how to build out a positive agenda of change.
Let me give some concrete examples.
The broaden-and-build approach of Fredrickson and colleagues (e.g.,
Fredrickson, 2004) described by Garland and Fredrickson in Chapter 2
in this volume, rightly guides the attention of practitioners toward oppor-
tunities for growth that can fill a kind of content vacuum in an ACT
approach.
An example of that vacuum that ACT clinicians know how to fill
will help explain what I mean by a vacuum. In ACT exposure work with
anxiety disorders (say, a person with panic disorder and agoraphobia),
the goal of exposure is not the reduction of anxiety per se but response
flexibility. During exposure (say, being in a shopping mall) the clinician
works first on present moment awareness, acceptance, and defusion, but
when there is a sense of openness the clinician inserts new response
functions (e.g., What would you most like to buy here? Who is wearing
the most interesting clothes?). What is inserted is not well specified—it
is left up to the creativity of the therapist and the spontaneous comments
of the client.
In this case it is generally not difficult to do so, but my point is to
note the content vacuum that can occur. If you expand out the issue
from this specific example, you will see how broadly applicable this point
is and how positive psychology might support and enhance the psycho-
logical flexibility model by providing useful content leads. Suppose an
ACT practitioner helps a depressed client be more skilled in coming into
the present moment and doing so with increased emotional and cogni-
tive flexibility through acceptance, defusion, and mindfulness work.
What might be inserted into the next moment of work? The usual answer
is that values will be a guide, which is an excellent idea, but inside that
functional structure it can help to consider proximal steps. Actions
focused on positive emotions such as gratitude, compassion, love, or
appreciation of beauty could be extremely helpful—not because the
emotions replace other emotions but because they mark actions of impor-
tance. The exercises and methods of positive psychology fit comfortably
into many such moments in the acceptance and mindfulness work. In
the right moment a gratitude journal, or exercises in forgiveness, or a
focus on the appreciation of beauty, or consciously practicing compassion
311
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
312
The Genuine Conversation
313
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
314
The Genuine Conversation
(SCS—Neff, 2003a) and as Neff and Tirch document, the benefits are
considerable.
From the point of view of the psychological flexibility model that
underlies ACT, self-acceptance and self-kindness overlap rather clearly.
Being touched by, open to, not avoiding, and not disconnecting from
experience as part of a kind effort to heal or be whole (the etymology of
“heal” is “make whole”) would be a fairly good definition of acceptance
in ACT. Self-kindness represents an attitude of benevolence and is char-
acterized by the absence of self-criticism and harsh self-judgment. These
latter features harken to an ACT posture of defusion and the choice to
embrace one’s own experience.
The same could be said of mindfulness. Neff’s approach to mindful-
ness (e.g., being nonjudgmental and in the moment) is shared not just
with ACT but with most of the mindfulness-based approaches. In that
context is it not surprising that Neff and Tirch report that the SCS cor-
relates .65 with the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes et al.,
2004; Bond et al., 2011), which is the most popular measure of ACT
processes—and in our laboratory we have found a similar relationship
between these two measures.
My point is not that self-compassion offers nothing new. Such a
claim would be inconsistent with a contextual approach to language.
Different ways of speaking offer new opportunities. For example, I have
found that speaking of “self-kindness” with clients is a very useful way to
communicate the kind of posture of acceptance ACT is trying to foster.
The reason I am making the connection in this context is that the
work on RFT analyses of acceptance or mindfulness (as an example)
should thus be directly relevant to the work on self-compassion. So far as
I am aware, self-compassion theory is not based on an experimental labo-
ratory science, and CBS brings something broadly useful to the table as
a result.
This can be seen most directly in the last attribute: the sense of
common humanity. Neff discusses this attribute as involving a sense of
connectedness with humanity at large and the sense that one’s experi-
ences, including those that are painful, are shared as part of the human
condition.
Here the work on deictic relational frames, as described by Stewart
and McHugh (Chapter 5 in this volume), provides a way forward. A
concept like “common humanity” began as a middle-level theoretical
315
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Genuine Engagement
In the beginning of their chapter, Parks and Biswas-Diener (Chapter 7)
tell the story of being asked in workshop what the difference is between
a positive intervention and ACT. They say “Obscured within this polite
statement are the questions they really want to ask: Is there anything new
about positive interventions?” Parks and Biswas-Diener go on to try to
show how positive interventions are distinctive and new.
I like to think that the questioners were not really asking “what is
the difference” in some “prove yourself” way. A more interesting way to
view the exchange is to suppose that the questioner is seeing or sensing
a deep relationship and asking whether these two fit together: Can they
be usefully integrated? Are they fellow travelers?
This book provides an answer to these three questions: “it depends”
and “probably so” and “we shall see.”
If positive interventions are really committed to replacing negative
emotions, and to a “more is better” philosophy applied to any content
that is topographically “positive,” then the two do not fit other than for
some useful borrowing. But if that is the agenda, then it appears to me
from the outside that the research literature within positive psychology
itself is rapidly trimming back this perspective. The pruning shears of
science can be harsh, but they are necessary. I think that pruning is
already happening, as several chapters show. That agenda, in my opinion,
does not comport well with what is known about human psychology, and
as positive psychology has matured there is a growing realization that
positive functions and positive forms (and negative functions and nega-
tive forms) are just not the same thing. That moment of realization pro-
vides an opening, and the acceptance and mindfulness traditions, and
316
The Genuine Conversation
especially ACT, RFT, and CBS, are stepping forward ready to engage
fully with “Positive Psychology 2.0.”
That leaves the questions of whether they can be usefully integrated,
gradually becoming mutually supportive fellow travelers. Is it possible for
this genuine conversation to blossom into a kind of genuine engagement:
an ongoing relationship that is based on common interests, shared per-
spectives, and mutual respect?
In my own view, I hope so. I think we have much to gain from each
other. As for what the future holds, I do not know; but let’s start with
where we are. Hope and optimism are here, now, in the present: they
have filled these pages. Readers reaching these last two sentences of the
volume will be the ones to decide whether they can be realized going
forward. We shall see, but it is worth taking the time to savor and appre-
ciate this moment: a genuine conversation is an excellent place to start.
References
Biglan, A., & Hayes, S. C. (1996). Should the behavioral sciences become more
pragmatic? The case for functional contextualism in research on human
behavior. Applied and Preventive Psychology: Current Scientific Perspectives, 5,
47-57. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(96)80026-6
Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B., & Minhas, G. (2011). A dynamic approach to
psychological strength development and intervention. Journal of Positive
Psychology, 6, 106-118.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H.
K., Waltz, T., & Zettle, R. D. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of
the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire—II: A revised measure of psycho-
logical inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42,
676–688.
Duckworth, A. L., Steen, T. A., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Positive psychology in
clinical practice. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 629-651. doi: 10.1146/
annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144154
Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 359, 1367-1378.
Gibson, B., & Sanbonmatsu, D. M. (2004). Optimism, pessimism and gambling:
The downside of optimism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 149-
160. doi: 10.1177/0146167203259929
Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J., Reese, H. W., & Sarbin, T. R. (Eds.). (1993). Varieties of
scientific contextualism. Oakland, CA: Context Press/New Harbinger.
317
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy: The process and practice of mindful change (2nd ed.). New York:
Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino,
D., Polusny, M. A., Dykstra, T. A., Batten, S. V., Bergan, J., Stewart, S. H.,
Zvolensky, M. J., Eifert, G. H., Bond, F. W., Forsyth, J. P., Karekla, M., &
McCurry, S. M. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test
of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553-578.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the
trajectory of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
72, 1075-1092. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1075
Kashdan, T. B., & Breen, W. E. (2008). Social anxiety and positive emotions: A
prospective examination of a self-regulatory model with tendencies to suppress
or express emotions as a moderating variable. Behavior Therapy, 39, 1-12.
Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Expanding the topography of social anxiety:
An experience sampling assessment of positive emotions and events, and
emotion suppression. Psychological Science, 17, 120-128.
Kornfield, J. (2001). After the ecstasy, the laundry: How the heart grows wise on the
spiritual path. New York: Bantam.
Levin, M. E., Hildebrandt, M. J., Lillis, J., & Hayes, S. C. (in press). The impact of
treatment components suggested by the psychological flexibility model: A
meta-analysis of laboratory-based component studies. Behavior Therapy.
Luoma, J. B., Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Fletcher, L. (2012). Slow and steady
wins the race: A randomized clinical trial of Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy targeting shame in substance use disorders. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 80, 43-53. doi:10.1037/a0026070
McNulty, J. K. (2010). Forgiveness increases the likelihood of subsequent partner
transgressions in marriage. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 787-790.
McNulty, J. K., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Beyond positive psychology? Toward a
contextual view of psychological processes and well- being. American
Psychologist, 67, 101-110. doi:10.1037/a0024572
McNulty, J. K., O’Mara, E. M., & Karney, B. R. (2008). Benevolent cognitions as a
strategy of relationship maintenance: “Don’t sweat the small stuff” but it’s not
all small stuff. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 631-646. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.631
Mitmansgruber, H., Beck, T. N., & Schüßler, G. (2008). “Mindful helpers”:
Experiential avoidance, meta-emotions, and emotion regulation in paramed-
ics. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1358-1363.
Neff, K. D. (2003a). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-
compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223-250.
Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy
attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85-101.
318
The Genuine Conversation
Oakley, B., Knafo, A., Madhavan, G., & Wilson, D. S. (Eds.). (2011). Pathological
altruism. New York: Oxford University Press.
Park, C. L., & Helgeson, V. S. (2006). Introduction to the special section: Growth
following highly stressful life events—Current status and future directions.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 791-796.
Phipps, S. (2007). Adaptive style in children with cancer: Implications for a positive
psychology approach. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 1055-1066. doi:10.1093
/jpepsy/jsm060
Seligman, M. E., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduc-
tion. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14.
Sheldon, K., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2011). Designing positive psychology:
Taking stock and moving forward. New York: Oxford University Press.
Toussaint, L. L., Williams, D. R., Musick, M. A., & Everson, S. A. (2001).
Forgiveness and health: Age differences in a U.S. probability sample. Journal of
Adult Development, 8, 249-257. doi:10.1023/A:1011394629736
Vilardaga, R., & Hayes, S.C. (2011). A contextual behavioral approach to patho-
logical altruism. In B. Oakley, A. Knafo, G. Madhavan, & D. S. Wilson (Eds.),
Pathological altruism (pp. 31-48). New York: Oxford University Press.
Wong, P. T. P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model
of the good life. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 52, 69-81.
Yadavaia, J. (2012). Using Acceptance and Commitment Therapy to decrease high-
prevalence psychopathology by targeting self-compassion: A randomized con-
trolled trial. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nevada, Reno.
319
Todd B. Kashdan, PhD, is an associate professor of psychology and
senior scientist at the Center for Consciousness and Transformation at
George Mason University. Kashdan has published over one hundred
peer-reviewed articles on meaning and purpose in life, happiness, mind-
fulness, how to deal with stress and anxiety, and social relationships. He
is the author of several books, including Curious? Discover the Missing
Ingredient to a Fulfilling Life, and a TEDx speaker. His work has been
featured in the New York Times and the Washington Post, and on CNN,
National Public Radio, and other media outlets. Kashdan received the
2013 American Psychological Association (APA) Distinguished
Scientific Award for Early Career Contribution to Psychology.
324
Index
325
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
326
Index
327
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
328
Index
329
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
330
Index
331
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
332
Index
333
Mindfulness & Acceptance for Positive Psychology
334
Index
XYZ
Youman, Kerstin, 215
335
mor e book s from
ne w ha r bi nger publications
n e w h a r b i n g e r p u b l i c a t i o n s, i n c .
1-800-748-6273 / newharbinger.com
Like us on Facebook (VISA, MC, AMEX / prices subject to change without notice)
Follow us on Twitter Don’t miss out on new books in the subjects that interest you.
@newharbinger.com Sign up for our Book Alerts at nhpubs.com/bookalerts