Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Michelle E. Seat
EPSY 400
1
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Introduction:
Motivation is one of the most puzzling, yet most critical, elements in learning considered
by teachers. Practically every person has dealt with a problem involving it; and yet, there is still
strategies with the hopes of discovering what makes students motivated; yet, it often depends on
the individual student. Interestingly research has begun to implicate issues of motivation
stemming from unconscious, implicit beliefs held by the student in question. These beliefs about
one’s intelligence may be different based on subject or domain. Overall I consider beliefs of
hold for our schools, class assessment policies, and in particular standardized testing policies.
Standardized testing has been an understood fact of life for all public school students in
the United States. Most standardized tests begin in middle school and really become important as
their years in school progress. Not coincidentally, as students progress through school their self-
efficacy lowers especially once they reach middle school (Usher, 2009). This might imply that
approaches in test prep contribute to students’ beliefs of their own intelligence and how those
beliefs can have an impact on their testing performance and overall understanding of tested
concepts. The theory for beliefs of intelligence documented by Dweck and Leggett (1988) will
Although the Common Core State Standards (2010) have been created, the United States
is without a standardized curriculum across all public schools. The implementation of the CCSS
has been controversial with mixed results. Without a standardized curriculum, it is impossible to
2
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
know from only a high school degree that a student is qualified for entrance to a certain college
or program. Therefore standardized tests must be implemented to evaluate and label a students’
intelligence because all students are assessed on the same content through the same format,
standardized tests given are often failing to encourage growth in students’ intelligence. And the
test preparation materials fail to encourage a progression of knowledge and understanding, rather
motivation, prior achievement, parental impact, practice testing and pre/post testing. How those
measures promote an incremental or entity belief of intelligence (Dweck and Leggett, 1988)
While motivation as a construct may be difficult to assess, Xie’s (2013) study finds the
participants’ strategies used to assess their learning or performance based goals. When
motivation fails to show its relevance in participants’ success, researchers look to participants’
prior achievement (Buchman et al., 2010; Hong et al., 2006). Several studies also consider the
parental figures of the participants involved including their highest education level and their
expectations for their children to attend college (Park & Becks, 2015; Devine-Eller, 2012).
Finally the last approach considered, and potentially the most productive, is practice testing
(McDaniel et al., 2013; Lane et al., 2009) and pre/post testing along with other test preparation
These various measures are analyzed to see which test preparation strategy promotes an
incremental belief of intelligence. Then suggestions for standardized test delivery and
preparation can be and will reflect the importance of growing your understanding of concepts.
3
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Theory Review:
Although you cannot pinpoint the specific moment a student’s belief about their ability in
math begins you can be sure it forms differently for every student. Imagine you are a sixth grader
and every time you ask your parents or guardians for help on your math homework they say,
“Oh, I was never good in math. Ask your teacher after school.” This student was struggling and
instead of getting help from a valuable influence in their life they were convinced only an expert
can help. Now instead image your parent said, “Hm, I haven’t seen multiplying fractions in a
while but let’s see what we can do.” Not only is that student now gaining the assistance they
were brave enough to seek out but the parent is showing them how to persevere.
intelligence. In the first response, you see the parent enforcing an entity belief; that their
intelligence in math is fixed. In the other response, you see the parent promoting an incremental
belief; that even though they do not know something now it can be learned and their overall
intelligence in mathematics can change and improve with effort and perseverance.
Implicit beliefs students hold are based on unconscious, personal beliefs about the world
and how they think they fit into it. These beliefs evolve over time and contribute to different
actions students make. Depending on the beliefs students hold, they pursue their work with
different goals in mind. When students hold incremental beliefs they tend to pursue learning
goals which emphasize mastery in understanding a topic. They often seek new challenges and
persist through struggles and failures. In contrast, when students hold entity beliefs they tend to
pursue performance goals which will allow them to prove their knowledge (Bruning et al.,
2011). They often avoid challenges and quit when struggles or failures come their way. When
students avoid challenges they demonstrate a “learned helplessness,” meaning they perceive a
4
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
relationship between their behavior and the event of a past failure; and they have decided they
had no control over the situation. This relationship is powerful because if students do not think
they had control in their success or failure in the first instance they are unlikely to attempt a new
challenge they do not believe they can control either (Dweck, 1975). These students with entity
beliefs, demonstrating learned helplessness, often become verbally defensive when they face
failure or are challenged. The do not think they have control over their success and they make
that known to
Experiences
those around with:
them, adamantly
Prior Parent Practice Pre/Post
Motivation
and often loudly. Achievement Interaction Testing Testing
—Impacts a person’s—
In Figure 1, there
Beliefs About
is a visual Positive Experiences Intelligence Negative Experiences
tend to foster: tend to foster:
representation of Incremental Beliefs Entity Beliefs
the components —tend to support—
Learning Goals Performance Goals
involved in
—tend to hold—
beliefs about Avoids
Seeks Challenges
Challenges
intelligence based
Persists Gives Up
on Bruning et
Attributes Attributes
al.’s (2011) Success to Effort Success to Abilty
descriptions for Demonstrates
Self-Regulated
Learned
beliefs of Helplessness
Figure 1. Characteristics in Beliefs about Intelligence
intelligence. It
5
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
demonstrates the difference a student’s belief can have on what type of goals they work towards.
It is important to consider that this social-cognitive model can apply to all beliefs of intelligence.
Although someone may have an incremental belief in one area they may have an entity belief in
another. For example someone may believe they can never improve their harmonizing skills.
They do not know how to harmonize and may have even embarrassed themselves once trying to
do so. In contrast, that same person may believe they can improve their soccer skills through
practice and observation of other skilled player. They may have even witnessed their own
Teachers in various subjects, especially math, are constantly combating students with entity
beliefs as they demonstrate their learned helplessness. Mathematics is a subject which the
majority of people have an opinion about. They hate it; they like it; they think it’s boring; but
most of all, they think they are either good or bad at it and nothing can change that ability or lack
thereof. Bruning et al. (2011) describe five “guidelines for fostering adaptive goals” which
describe promoting incremental ideas while deemphasizing other ideas (p. 144) (See Table 1 for
a summarized representation). Teachers can help students forming their beliefs by consistently
classroom. Although students are receiving information about their intellectual ability elsewhere,
they can begin to see how important it is to face their education with perseverance and
determination. One of the critical features to emphasize while considering assessments along
with is theory is “individual evaluative feedback.” With individual evaluative feedback the
teacher can not only boost their student’s inner confidence but they can also boost the student’s
Table 1
Emphasize: Deemphasize:
Mistakes are Normal and Healthy Mistakes are a Result from Negative Ability
Cohen et al. (1999) define assigning competence as “a public statement that specifically
recognizes the intellectual contribution a student has made to the group task…[in order] to
change not only the student's expectations for competence but also to raise the group's
expectations for that student” (p 81). Obviously it won’t be very helpful to assign competence to
a student for doing something unrelated to the learning task like their ability to sit quietly or
doodle in their notebook. Those competences may be important in other scenarios; however,
important to stay relevant to the task. For example, if a quiet student suddenly says “oh, this is
kind of like what we did yesterday,” a teacher could come by and make a point to emphasize the
remark and who made it. Then they could discuss the importance of reflecting on conceptual
connections and drawing on those connections to further their progress on the task.
Assigning competence to students publically is not easy for most teachers. It takes an
extensive knowledge of students as well as a very observant moment. It can be difficult to find
7
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
the right words to say as well, especially knowing that a lot of lower status students don’t like to
talk in a full classroom and be the center of attention. Thus assigning competence can be helpful
ability can grow, there are required tests which can skew a student’s belief in their ability based
on their overall score versus their learning progress. These tests for achievement can have a
lasting impact on students’ mentality if delivered with the emphasis that their final score means
more than their development in the material. Not only are tests required by most organizations as
proof of a person’s knowledge but test can be enforced culturally as a means to label someone’s
level of intelligence. Even with so much negativity around testing there can there be positives
In order for test preparation to be positive for the students’ incremental belief of their
intelligence the test preparation must emphasize an understanding of the concepts covered in the
exam and include individualized feedback versus skill and drill procedures with little to no
feedback. The preparation should also emphasize the process of learning by including pre/post
The theory for people’s beliefs of intelligence applies to any and all contexts and may
change based on the subject the person is being tested on. For example, a student may believe
they can consistently improve their writing skills through practice and perseverance but they may
also think that their intelligence in mathematics is limit and no amount of effort will change that.
In order for people to participate in test preparation well, the must believe they can improve in
the subject through better understanding rather bettering their score. This belief needs to be
8
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
encouraged by their instructor but also their peers. Hopefully through a productive test
preparation experience the person can learn the value of their effort in the learning of a concept.
1st Approach—Motivation:
On factor studied by researchers is motivation. For example, Xie (2013) designed a study
addressing preparations strategies used to improve College English Test Band 4 (CET4) test
scores. This English proficiency test is considered a graduation requirement for most universities
and contribute to job applications. There were different sample sizes taken at different parts of
the study. It included 847 people taking the pretest, 833 taking the posttest, and 873 taking the
questionnaire.
preparing for the actual test. Overall, Xie measured what test takers do to prepare, if there any
patterns in those preparations, and how those test preparations contributed to test performance.
With the self-reported answers to the questionnaire, Xie found a pattern of “the most
frequently used test preparations included practicing test-taking skills, test-taking preparation
management, drilling, memorizing, and socioaffective strategies” (Xie, 2013, p. 210), in that
order. The author began to distinguish between these strategies when they considered how the
test preparation strategies contributed to test-takers performance. Xie coded some strategies as
“use strategies” which enhanced test performance and “learning strategies” which enhanced
language learning and attainment. Although the different types of strategies can attest to a small
effect in test scores, it was more effective dependent on the type of preparation. Overall they
distinguished types of test-preparation and determined the most productive strategies for learning
versus performance.
9
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Because the students involved where volunteers, Xie considers the type of motivation
they had throughout the program. Ideally you would think volunteers would be motivated but the
author saw a lack of motivation at times, especially during the posttest. This contradicts the
participants having an incremental belief in their intelligence because they would have continued
to persist.
Hong, Sas, and Sas, (2006) study the motivational concerns behind test-preparation and
test-taking strategies. They focused on high academic achievers in mathematics versus low
achievers. Although there were many participants available the researchers chose 15 students to
represent the high-achievers and 11 to represent the low-achievers based on their interest in
mathematics, level of class taken, and scores on the Activities and Accomplishment Inventory.
Along with the AAI, the researchers measured students’ mathematical courses taken,
strategies. To do so participating teachers helped the researchers by collected data in two phases,
first administering the AAI then researchers analyzed that data followed up with individual
high-achieving students used cognitive strategies, including reviewing, solving, and note-taking,
“accommodated their surroundings, arranged time, and sought assistance from teachers or
Overall higher-achieving students were more concerned with the structural organization
of solving problems and checked their answers more frequently. However motivational issues
were not found to have a significant impact on students. Hong and colleagues found no
10
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
difference in the effort made by participants; however, found that low-achieving students were
The attempt to not just look at what students do while studying or preparing for tests and
consider instead what strategies are more important to them is valuable in consideration to their
belief of their intelligence. Belief in one’s work can make a large impact on their preparation and
overall ability. But also you may believe a certain strategy is important and incredibly useful but
not actually find the time to do it for a multitude of reasons. The authors fail to consider how
participating in the study and verbally reflecting on their beliefs in testing issues may have
Although an important assessment for all high school students is the SAT, Buchmann,
Condron, and Roscigno (2010) question how SAT preparation may be unfairly available based
on status inequalities leading to issues in college enrollment. Buchmann et al. use data collected
by NELS to evaluate students’ use of SAT preparation considering their parents’ educational
background. While their parents’ educational background can be tied into their family income,
their parents’ highest degree obtained speaks to the ideals of education pushed in their home
which can greatly affect a student’s desire to strive and persevere in higher education.
Whereas NELS collected data from around 25,000 8th graders through parents’ surveys,
Buchman et al. only consider 8,150 students who reported already taking the SAT/ACT or
planning to take it when they became seniors. When the authors considered students highest-
level of test preparation by parental education, they found the highest parental education led to
the highest ODDS ratio at 1.451 for private tutoring. They also found that having a parent with a
11
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
college degree or higher had a significant impact to students going to any type of college;
however, having a parent with only some college only had a significant effect on a student
students’ enrollment in college but not necessarily their use of SAT preparation.
Compared to Buchmann et al.’s (2010) work with the data from NELS, Devine-Eller’s
(2012) analyzes the National Household Education Survey (NHES) data as it was more recently
collected and asks whether students prepped in the current school year, thus providing more
information on the timing of test preparation activities. There are 3,373 participants included in
this analysis of whether students participated in test prep activities. Data was collected through
The NHES reports on many measures including family income, parents’ highest level of
education, parents’ expectations for their children’s attainment of professional degrees, race,
students’ grade level, and course grades. These measures support the main dependent variable of
Overall Devine-Eller finds that timing played a significant role in whether a student
participated in test prep. For example the author reported that for all race groups, the probability
of participating in test prep increased as they progressed through 9th-12th grade with 42% of 12th
graders prepping and only 8% of 9th graders prepping. Test prep is relevant to college admission
and interestingly only 47% of responding parents have a college degree or more. However, 73%
of parents expect at least college degree attainment from their children. Finally in consideration
to race, income, grade level, and course grades, only black non-Hispanics and students with
Ultimately more data is necessary to establish the most equitable test prep availability
and in what forms. The author speaks to the validity of the test prep activities done outside of
school, which the NELS data has specific information on. A more productive impact in
preparation is likely to stem from a student’s individual desire to do well on the test for their own
needs. A student believing they can gain intelligence from their preparation is crucial to their
Instead of considering data from NELS or NHES, Parks and Becks (2015) considered
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) data which included 8,652 students in the
Education Longitudinal Study of 2002 (ELS-2002). They studied the influence of race, amongst
other variables, on SAT/ACT preparation and overall performance. This surveyed students, their
parents, and administrator in 2002, while students were sophomores, and in 2004, as seniors.
Overall Park et al.’s (2015) study investigates the “highest level of test preparation”
(Buchmann et al., 2010) and highest composite ACT/SAT score. The survey gave the researchers
information including race, gender, family income, ect. However, another important variable was
parents’ and student’s expectation of attending college and discussions about college and
ACT/SAT preparation.
Their findings show female students and black students were more likely to participate
in all forms of test prep (i.e., books and videos, high school course, private course, and private
tutoring), while Asian American students were more likely to only use a private course or
tutoring. Participating in a private course or tutoring was also positively related to the parents’
educational aspirations for their child. However, not all discussions with parents proved helpful
for students’ actual test scores. For example, while more discussions between parents and
13
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
children about SAT/ACT prep were associated with higher scores, more discussions about going
Overall findings were limited to the data set. And although the authors discussed the
cultural capital reflected in their study and how parents with advanced degrees are likely to see
the importance of SAT scores for college admissions, their importance should not be solely
based on their score improving but instead understanding the concepts within the assessment.
McDaniel, Thomas, Agarwal, McDermott, and Roediger (2013) study in their first
understanding of concepts and terms when followed up by a more summative exam in the future.
Participants in this study came from a public middle school located in a suburban middle-class,
Midwest community. Although 142 students participated in the study, only 61 students managed
All participants took a set of initial quizzes including a quiz after they completed their
assigned textbook reading, after the teacher taught the lesson, then another quiz the day before an
exam. The exam was then given at the end of unit, around 11 days after the first introduction to
the material.
Within these assessments, students were aware of their quiz score while the teacher was
only knowledgeable of their general quiz scores as a class and their individual exam scores.
Overall their results showed a significant improvement in exam performance when students’
underwent initial quizzing. The strength in results could stem from the memorization of material
by the time of the exam but they may also come from a fuller understanding of the concepts due
In this study we are unaware of the sort of atmosphere the teacher has created to
encourage an incremental belief, although the results show that students are increasing their
understanding of the science concepts and terms no matter the type of questions assessed.
Although she was unaware of the specific scores of her students her continual interaction with
them can have an effect on their thinking when they see their own scores displayed on the board.
It is possible they held the belief that their score can be improved before their exam or the belief
that their score during the initial quizzing would probably be their score on their exam.
While McDaniel et al. (2013) studied the affect of anonymous quizzing; Lane, Kalberg,
Mofield, Wehby, and Parks (2009) studied the outcomes associated with students participating in
a specific program including quizzes, “Preparing for the ACT” program. They studied 126
eleventh graders in a school in Middle Tennessee who willfully participating in the program for
two academic years. The researchers took many measures into consideration including the
students’ grade point average (GPA), office disciplinary referrals (ODF), the actual ACT test, an
The program was put in place through the students’ homeroom sessions one a week for
26 weeks. The teachers administered the practice tests, ongoing probes to practice for the actual
test. There were six curriculum-based probes in total throughout the lessons. The lessons were
not necessarily in-depth but concentrated on developing students’ familiarity with the elements
of the subjects and tests covered. Students were encouraged to monitor their progress and seek
further assistance.
Overall the researchers found that for all subject areas, students’ probe scores after the
intervention were significant in predicting actual ACT scores. When they considered the impact
of the other measures, English was the only subject area to find relevance to student GPA and
15
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
ODRs and their actual English ACT score. With all of their results it’s important to see that the
program made a positive difference in the performance of the students who participated.
Although we can’t be certain what specific elements of the program were most useful to
students.
Farnsworth (2013) studied the effect of targeted test preparation given to students taking
English as a second language (ESL) tests. These tests include a scripted oral interview based test,
the Basis Skills Test Plus (BEST Plus) and a standardized, computer based test called the
Versant English Test (VET). The differences in delivery and measures of the tests led
Farnsworth to question how score improvements from coaching, or targeted test preparation, are
Seventy-one students in an ESL adult program in NYC took full part in the study, with
the majority of participants reporting Spanish as their first language (n=59). Since the goal is to
measure the effect of coaching on the overall student’s score, each student was given both
assessments as a pretest and a posttest. After students were given the pretests they were
randomly given a “treatment” which consisted of 12 hours of “intense strategy and test format
coaching” (Farnsworth, 2013, p. 149). Thus over 6 weeks the students were only receiving
coaching in one of the testing formats however all students received both posttests. Although the
sample at posttest dwindled, the results showed scores increased significantly on both tests no
matter which treatment was given. While this study revealed large score gains, it is difficult to
discern the effect the coaching had on student test scores specifically.
Farnsworth explains the participants in this study were primarily long-tern immigrants
who have not had many opportunities to practice their English or learn from English
16
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
professionals in any sense. Although this seemed to make most of the students appreciate the
extra support and be motivated to learn, the fact that less than 45 students showed up for their
Not only do pre and post tests contribute to Farnsworth’s work, it is also a focus in
Fakcharoenphol and Stelzer’s (2014) work on the effect of practice exams and other learning
resources on students’ physics exam scores. Their goal was to measure the change in exam
scores based on the difference in specific learning resources given to students. The 44 college
participants whom completed the study were split into four groups. One set of students was the
control group and received not additional resources, one was given only practice exams, another
was given practice exams and three homework problems based on their practice exam answers,
and the last group was given those resources plus one-on-one tutor sessions.
The researchers found students for the study based on students’ first midterm scores.
Once enrolled in the study, non-control group students came in for three sessions before their
next midterm. Those given extra homework problems were given a maximum of three problems
and final answers related to their practice test and instructed to return them before their next
session. Students receiving extra homework problem and tutoring took the practice test than
immediately had an hour to work with the tutor. Overall they found a significant improvement in
exam scores between the treatment participants than the control group. In fact, on average, scores
from the first test to the second test decreased for the students in the control yet the other groups
of students had an increasing average score. However the tutoring group did not do significantly
better than the other groups. Ultimately, practice tests were a positive study strategy to improve
Although they consider the effect that participants dropping out of the study had an effect
on the tutoring treatment they do not consider the procedure of the tutoring. The tutoring being
done immediately after their practice exam give the students no time to self-motivate and learn
where their mistakes were. I have to wonder what sort of affect the timing of the tutoring effects
students’ idea of improvement. They were given related homework based on the tutor’s
assessment of their needs which allows them to go back, try more problems and reflect. However
if that process was done before their tutoring session, students may have showed more
motivation for their own independent growth or are they basing their growth on the success of
Conclusion:
Not only do the results from these nine articles have implications for test preparation,
they also have implications for teachers to improve students’ beliefs of intelligence. The theories,
including entity beliefs and incremental beliefs, suggest teachers use several methods to promote
adaptive goals which in turn foster learning. Techniques to promote learning include
emphasizing the process of learning and that mistakes including failure are part of a normal and
healthy learning process. An emphasis of effort and improvement should be a basis for how
intelligence is grown; because if you foster adaptive goals, you believe that intellectual
development is controllable.
Limitations:
In the consideration of the studies summarized in this paper, Xie’s (2013) work is the
most specific in considering participants learning goals versus performance goals. She considers
a model that describes “use strategies” to enhance test performance versus “learning strategies”
to enhance language learning and attainment. Based on her data however she finds “that test
18
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
takers used learning strategies scarcely relative to their uses of use strategies during test
preparation period” (Xie, 2013, p. 221). This along with the fact that she found overall
motivation decrease towards the end of the test cycle indicates the participants’ pursuit in
Motivation was also considered in Hong et al.’s (2006) work. However they found that
motivational issues were not found to have a significant impact on participant’s test preparation.
Instead they found that past low achievement led to more passive behaviors in consideration to
test-taking strategies. Buchman et al. (2010) also addresses participants’ prior achievement.
However, in contrast to Hong et al’s (2006) findings, Buchman et al. (2010) found that
participants with past low achievement participated in more test preparation. These contrasting
studies show there is more factors to test preparation than prior achievements. In consideration of
beliefs of intelligence, some may argue that in Buchman et al.’s (2010) study the participants
deciding to take part in test preparation after prior failures may have an incremental belief of
their intelligence. However their data is not definitive per that consideration.
Another factor considered in some studies was the participants’ parents’ highest level of
education. Many studies found that the higher the parents’ education the more the participants
took part in higher level or higher quantity of test preparation (Buchman et al., 2010; Devine-
Eller, 2012). Parents’ expectations for their children and conversations with them can also have
in impact on the students test preparation strategies (Park & Becks, 2015). Still more parents
expect their children to receive college degrees than have a college degree themselves. This
demonstrates an importance of the surrounding educated people; however, the conversations had
with parents are influential to the students’ belief of their own intelligence and what dedication it
Practice testing is considered in these studies along with various strategies along with the
formal testing. Lane et al. (2009) incorporated specific ACT practice tests along with a full
intervention created by the ACT creators. Overall the program led to a positive difference in the
performance of the students who participated although the action of completing a practice test
was not compared to the rest of the intervention. And McDaniel et al. (2013) incorporated graded
quizzes in their study to assess students’ improvement throughout practice. Their study found a
great improvement in scores through quizzing along with textbook readings and teacher taught
lessons. With this consistency in quizzing it is possible students were pursuing performance
goals throughout the study; however, some may also argue that the repetition of individualized
Farnsworth (2013), Fakcharoenphol et al. (2014) incorporate pre/post testing along with
another learning strategy. Farnsworth (2013) studied the effect of coaching attributes while
Fakcharoenphol et al. (2014) studies the effect of immediate tutoring. Although coaching and
tutoring can be productive strategies in encouraging adaptive goals the implementation is critical.
The tutoring sessions were given immediately after an assessment and rather encourage the
student to consider their mistakes first and how they can grow from them, they were given help
with how to answer them along with a new set of homework problems to work through.
Although coaching was beneficial to students’ overall score, the coaching involved intense help
in test format and test-taking strategies. Those coaching activities build a better understanding of
the test itself, which is important, but does not necessarily build a better understanding of the
content.
Overall test preparation can be helpful to students’ test performance. Whether students
have equitable access to test preparation materials is an issue that I believe needs further
20
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
analysis. Some of the included studies attempts to address that by considering social economic
status, race, ethnicity, parents income, parents highest education, etc.; however, no study
considers public school or free test preparation and their benefits or pit falls.
Suggestions:
A study which takes into consideration the availability of test preparation materials and
the beliefs students hold about their own intelligence in the area of assessment is needed. The
study would examine how and what the participants do to prepare for major assessments; but
more specifically, why they prepare in that way with those materials. Like some of the included
studies, this study would also consider the culture around standardized test and test preparation
and address those concerns when considering the test preparation materials and strategies. If test
preparation only encourages a better score, then the culture around standardized test will never
change.
21
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
References:
Bruning, R. H., Schraw, G. J., & Norby, M. M. (2011). Cognitive Psychology and Instruction
Buchmann, C., Condron, D. J., & Roscigno, V. J. (2010). Shadow education, American style:
Test preparation, the SAT, and college enrollment. Social Forces, 89(2), 435-462.
http://www.library.illinois.edu/proxy/go.php?url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sof.2010.0105
Cohen, E. G., Lotan, R. A., Scarloss, B. A., & Arellano, A. R. (1999). Complex instruction:
https://doi.org/10.1080/00405849909543836
Devine‐Eller, A. (2012). Timing matters: Test preparation, race, and grade level. Sociological
Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned
Fakcharoenphol, W., & Stelzer, T. (2014). Physics exam preparation: A comparison of three
methods. Physical Review Special Topics – Physics Education Research, 10, 010108.
http://www.library.illinois.edu/proxy/go.php?url=http://dx.doi.org//10.1103/PhysRevSTP
ER.10.010108
Farnsworth, T. (2013). Effects of targeted test preparation on scores of two tests of oral English
http://www.library.illinois.edu/proxy/go.php?url=http://10.1002/tesq.75
22
Beliefs of Intelligence and Test Preparation
Hong, E., Sas, M., & Sas, J. C. (2006). Test-taking strategies of high and low mathematics
http://www.jstor.org/stable/27548124
Lane, K. L., Kalberg, J. R., Mofield, E., Wehby, J. H., & Parks, R. J. (2009). Preparing students
http://www.library.illinois.edu/proxy/go.php?url=http://dx.doi.org//10.1177/0741932507
314022
McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R. C., Agarwal, P. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2013).
Park, J. J., & Becks, A. H. (2015). Who benefits from SAT prep?: An examination of high
school context and race/ethnicity. The Review of Higher Education, 39(1), 1-23.
http://www.library.illinois.edu/proxy/go.php?url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/rhe.2015.003
Xie, Q. (2013). Does test preparation work? Implications for score validity. Language