Conceptual Semantics
Conceptual Semantics
Conceptual Semantics
Ph. D. Thesis
Motion is central to human experience; it is pervasive in our daily lives and also in
our communicative needs. However, it is a well established fact that languages differ
in their linguistic expression of motion. Talmy’s (1985, 2000) work distinguishes
two different types of languages according to the way the different elements of a
motion event are mapped onto linguistic elements: satellite-framed and verb-framed
languages. English and Spanish are often taken as examples of this typological
difference. English, as a satellite-framed language, expresses the core component of
motion, i.e., the Path or trajectory of motion, in satellites (e.g., up, down) or in
prepositional phrases (e.g., into/out of the house), leaving the verb slot free to encode
Manner-of-motion. Spanish, as a verb-framed language, typically expresses Path in
the main verb while relegating the expression of Manner to adjuncts (e.g.,
entrar/salir corriendo lit. ‘enter/exit running’).
Talmy’s seminal work has engendered a great deal of research and debate in
the literature on motion event descriptions over the last two decades. Despite the vast
amount of research on the linguistic expression of motion events, the fact that motion
verb roots might encode information apart from Path and Manner is often
overlooked, as are minor lexicalisation patterns that might occur. Furthermore,
scholars, more often than not, have neglected the study of path verbs in favour of the
study of manner verbs, as the differing expression of Manner has so far been the
most interesting diverging point between satellite- and verb-framed languages.
Finally, hardly any work has been devoted to an in-depth analysis of the semantics of
motion verb lexicons in both verb- and satellite-framed languages beyond the
comparison of the motion verbs found in novels or elicited orally in the two types of
language.
The present thesis aims at bridging the gap in the existing literature by
addressing the semantics of English and Spanish motion verbs. In the first part of this
dissertation, a systematic and detailed account of the semantics of English and
Spanish motion verb lexicons from a contrastive point of view is provided. The
patterns of general conflations are explored, as well as more subtle path notions and
fine-grained manner information which can be conveyed by motion verbs in these
i
two languages. Comparison between English and Spanish leads to the identification
of some similarities: (a) conflation of Motion plus one additional semantic
component, either Manner or Path, is the most characteristic lexicalisation pattern of
both motion verb lexicons; (b) both languages have path verbs which express, at
minimum, 13 different types of Path; among these, the most frequently lexicalised
types of Path are ‘Away from G’, ‘Up/Onto G – Upwards’, ‘To/towards G’ and
‘Down from/to G – Downwards’, whereas the least frequent is ‘Out of G’; and (c)
both languages possess many more walking verbs than running and jumping verbs.
These tendencies suggest that there are important similarities in how English and
Spanish, and possibly other verb- and satellite-framed languages, lexicalise the
domain of motion in their verb roots. In addition, some crosslinguistic differences are
noted: (a) English manner-of-motion verbs outnumber those of Spanish and tend to
exploit some manner parameters much more often than Spanish manner verbs (e.g.,
manner information about the mental and physical state of the Figure and details
about the way the Figure moves its legs while walking, motion using vehicles,
motion while dancing); and (b) the Spanish path verb lexicon is larger in size than
the English one, although both languages are able to express similar types of Paths.
The second part of this thesis aims to investigate an important subdomain of
motion, that of human locomotion, using experimental methods. Three studies are
reported: a definition and feature listing task, a free verb listing task, and a verb-
categorization rating task. These studies offer interesting insights into how English
and Spanish organise their motion verb lexicons and into the complex semantics of
human locomotion verbs. In addition, the findings provide empirical support for
some of the observations from the linguistic analysis undertaken in the first part of
this thesis, and they suggest that, rather than instantiating a single motor pattern,
some motion verbs might better be considered as a combination of motor patterns.
ii
Acknowledgements
The completion of this thesis has been possible thanks to the support of many people;
people who set off upon this long journey with me and people whom I encountered
along the path.
First of all, I must thank my supervisor, Javier Valenzuela Manzanares, for
his support and advice through the years; his guidance, kindness and friendship not
only helped me to achieve the completion of the present work but also prepared me
for the future.
I am also grateful to the Cognitive Linguistics group at the University of
Murcia for their encouragement and friendship, especially to Antonio Barcelona for
counting on me as a research assistant during my last year as an undergraduate and
for having me for four years as a graduate student. I owe to him my scholarly
inspiration.
Additionally, I thank my friends and research fellows for their constant moral
support; I must express my gratitude to my friends from Pliego for not asking too
often ‘When are you finishing your PhD?’ and to my research fellows, especially to
Pilar Mompeán, for encouraging me with ‘You are on track! Almost there!’
I am also grateful to the Spanish Ministry of Education and Science for their
financial support (BES-2004-4450), especially during the three research stays which
I spent at Northwestern University (Evanston, IL, USA), at the Max Planck Institute
for Psycholinguistics (Nijmegen, The Netherlands) and at the University of Lousiana
at Lafayette (Lafayette, LA, USA). A big thank you to Dedre Gentner, Anetta
Kopecka, Asifa Majid and Michele Feist for supervising me during these very
productive research stays. I cannot show enough my gratitude for their sharing their
time, for their support and for their stimulating comments. In particular, I am grateful
to Anetta Kopecka and Asifa Majid for their guidance through all the steps of my
first experimental study; without their help and advice I would not have been able to
keep on track. Moreover, I would like to thank the Language and Cognition Lab at
the University of Louisiana at Lafayette, and especially Michele Feist, for their
feedback on my experimental studies, and for putting up with me so patiently during
iii
my stay as well as for giving me the so needed support for going through the final
stages of this dissertation. All of you are in every bit of this dissertation.
I would also like to thank the committee for reading the results of my efforts
along this journey, and Michele Feist for proofreading this volume.
A tremendous thank you to all the participants who kindly volunteered to take
part in my studies, to Georgina Oliver and Carmen Comeaux for running the English
version of my studies, and to the Department of English Philology at the University
of Murcia, particularly Raquel Criado, Lourdes Cerezo, María Dueñas, Ana Rojo,
and Cristina Soriano, for letting me run the Spanish versions in their classes.
Finally, but most importantly, I thank my family dearly for their support, love
and unshakeable trust during all this time. My husband, Raimundo Martínez Manuel,
provided me with the necessary faith, strength and courage to go through all the
stages of this journey; he was my best fellow traveller along the uphills, downhills
and twisted paths.
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT …………………………………………………………………………….i
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS……………………………………………………………... iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS…………………………………………………………………v
LIST OF FIGURES AND TABLES………………………………………………………ix
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………1
v
2.3.4. Cline of Path salience........................................................................... 46
2.4. The Linguistic-Relativity Hypothesis.............................................................. 47
2.4.1. Introduction.......................................................................................... 47
2.4.2. Sapir and Whorf’s Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis ........................... 49
2.4.3. Slobin’s Thinking for Speaking ........................................................... 53
3.1. Introduction....................................................................................................... 57
3.2. Thinking-for-speaking research ...................................................................... 58
3.2.1. Introduction.......................................................................................... 58
3.2.2. Narrative style...................................................................................... 59
3.2.3. Translation ........................................................................................... 65
3.2.4. Sign languages and co-speech gestures ............................................... 69
3.2.4.1. Sign languages .................................................................................. 70
3.2.4.2. Co-speech gestures............................................................................ 72
3.2.5. First language acquisition .................................................................... 74
3.2.6. Language processing............................................................................ 76
3.2.7. Extensions to the domain of vision...................................................... 79
3.3. Linguistic relativity research ........................................................................... 82
3.3.1. Introduction.......................................................................................... 82
3.3.2. Positive evidence ................................................................................. 83
3.3.2. Conflicting evidence ............................................................................ 86
3.3.3. The role of telicity, type of Manner and type of Figure....................... 87
3.5. Summary............................................................................................................ 89
4.1. Introduction....................................................................................................... 94
4.2. The Syntax-Semantics interface ...................................................................... 95
4.2.1. Introduction.......................................................................................... 95
4.2.2. Motion verb classes.............................................................................. 96
4.3. The Functional-Lexematic model.................................................................. 102
4.3.1. Introduction........................................................................................ 102
4.3.2. Lexical subdomains of motion........................................................... 103
4.4. Online verb databases..................................................................................... 107
4.4.1. Introduction........................................................................................ 107
vi
4.4.2. Adesse ................................................................................................ 107
4.4.3. FrameNet and Spanish FrameNet ...................................................... 108
4.4.4. WordNet and MultiWordNet ............................................................. 111
4.4.5. VerbNet.............................................................................................. 112
4.5. The semantics of motion verbs....................................................................... 113
4.5.1. Introduction........................................................................................ 113
4.5.2. Manner verbs ..................................................................................... 113
4.5.3. Path verbs........................................................................................... 122
4.6. Summary.......................................................................................................... 126
6.1. Introduction…………………………………………………………………..225
6.2. The meaning of move, walk, run and jump.................................................. 226
6.2.1. Introduction........................................................................................ 226
vii
6.2.2. Method ............................................................................................... 227
6.2.3. Results................................................................................................ 228
6.2.4. Discussion and conclusions ............................................................... 232
6.3. Walking, running and jumping Verbs .......................................................... 234
6.3.1. Introduction........................................................................................ 234
6.3.2. Method ............................................................................................... 235
6.3.3. Results................................................................................................ 236
6.3.4. Discussion and conclusions ............................................................... 248
6.4. Categorisation of human locomotion verbs.................................................. 251
6.4.1. Introduction........................................................................................ 251
6.4.2. Method ............................................................................................... 253
6.4.3. Results................................................................................................ 254
6.4.4. Discussion and conclusions ............................................................... 261
6.5. Conclusions...................................................................................................... 264
CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS………………………………………………………..266
REFERENCES……………………………………………………………………….272
APPENDIX 1………………………………………………………………………...295
APPENDIX 2………………………………………………………………………...392
APPENDIX 3………………………………………………………………………...432
APPENDIX 4………………………………………………………………………...438
APPENDIX 5………………………………………………………………………...440
RESUMEN...………………………………………………………………………...450
Introducción ........................................................................................................... 450
Objetivos, corpus y metodología........................................................................... 455
Resultados............................................................................................................... 461
Conclusiones finales ............................................................................................... 468
viii
List of Figures
List of Tables
ix
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Motion is central to human experience; it is pervasive in our daily lives and also in
our communicative needs. It seems to be a universal concept present in all the
languages of the world. However, it is a well established fact that languages differ in
their linguistic expression of motion. Talmy’s (1985, 2000a, 2000b) work
distinguishes two different types of languages, i.e., satellite-framed and verb-framed
languages, according to the way the different elements of a motion scene are mapped
onto linguistic elements. English and Spanish are often taken as examples of this
typology. English, as a satellite-framed language, expresses the core component of
motion, i.e., Path or the trajectory of motion, in satellites (e.g., up, down) or in
prepositional phrases (e.g., into/out of the house), leaving the verb slot free to encode
Manner-of-motion. Spanish, as a verb-framed language, typically expresses Path in
the main verb while relegating the expression of Manner to adjunts (e.g., entrar/salir
corriendo lit. ‘enter/exit running’). However, it should be borne in mind that
typologies reflect tendencies rather than absolute differences among languages
(Berman & Slobin, 1994); thus, both languages have path-conflating and manner-
conflating verbs in addition to other verbs which encode other semantic information
pertaining to the motion event, as noted by Talmy and others.
Talmy’s typological work has inspired a great deal of research on motion
event descriptions over the last two decades. Such research has taken for granted that
the main difference between satellite-framed and verb-framed languages resides in
their differential encoding of Manner of motion; Manner is readily encoded in verbs
in satellite-framed languages whereas it is optional in verb-framed languages. As a
consequence, much less research attention has been given to the Path component,
which, as core of the motion event, is encoded either in the verb or in satellites.
Moreover, research has frequently overlooked the other possible semantic
conflations (e.g., Ground, Figure) that might exist in the motion verb lexicons of
satellite- and verb-framed languages, thus impoverishing their crosslinguistic
analyses.
Regarding manner verbs, Slobin (1997: 459) has pointed out that languages
seem to have a ‘two-tiered’ lexicon of manner-of-motion verbs: (1) a general one, or
2
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION
superordinate level, represented by everyday verbs such as walk, run, jump, fly, etc.;
and (2) a more specific and expressive level consisting of different ways of walking,
such as stroll, wander, or shuffle; different ways of running such as sprint or jog, etc.
Research based on elicited narratives and novels has shown that English possesses a
very extensive and elaborated second level whereas Spanish’s manner verb lexicon is
not as rich and mainly consists of general manner-of-motion verbs. In relation to path
verbs, Özçalişkan (2004: 85) has claimed that path verb lexicons of both satellite-
and verb-framed languages ‘form a closed lexical category that does not provide
many options for elaboration’ and thus, both typological groups may possess an
approximately similar number of path verbs. However, her claims have not been
fully addressed by crosslinguistic research yet.
As a final point, despite the vast amount of research on the linguistic
expression of motion events in English and in Spanish, and of the claims about the
semantics of their manner verbs, hardly any research has been devoted to an in-depth
analysis of their motion verb lexicons, and a comparative study is, to our knowledge,
nonexistent. This dissertation aims at filling the gap in the existing literature by
providing a systematic and detailed account of the semantics of both English and
Spanish motion verb lexicons. A better understanding of the semantics of motion
verbs in these languages is of primarily importance for crosslinguistic research on
motion event descriptions in general, and for research investigating the effects that
differing semantic patterns may have on non-linguistic cognition.
The primary goal of this work is to provide a comparative account of the semantics
of a substantial part of the English and the Spanish motion verb lexicons, focussing
specifically on self-agentive and non-agentive motion verbs (Talmy, 2000b: 28). Our
starting point is Talmy’s general semantic components for motion: Motion, Figure,
Ground, Path, Manner and Cause. Our analysis, moreover, delves into the broad
categories of Path and Manner so as to look for more subtle differences in their
motion lexicons; for that purpose, some types of Path (e.g., Berthele, 2004; Wälchli,
3
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION
4
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION
The present thesis is further organised into six chapters. Chapter 2, Chapter 3 and
Chapter 4 offer an extensive literature review on the theoretical background of this
dissertation, on the bulk of research on the topic of motion, and on the different
analyses and classifications of motion verbs which have been put forward. Chapter 5
and Chapter 6 address the semantics of the English and Spanish motion verb
5
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION
lexicons. Finally, Chapter 7 provides a summary of the key findings and suggests
avenues for future research.
Chapter 2 provides a brief but general overview of the theoretical background
of this dissertation. Section 2.1 deals with the birth and main tenets of the Cognitive
Linguistic enterprise in general, and with the main tenets and lines of research of
Cognitive Semantics, with special attention to Conceptual Semantics. In Section 2.2,
Talmy’s theory of lexicalisation patterns for motion is presented. The main concern
of Talmy’s work is the exploration of the systematic relations between meaning and
linguistic forms in order to find out whether languages exhibit a small number of
patterns (i.e., a typology) or a single pattern (i.e., a universal). In his exploration of
the domain of motion, he proposes two typologies: the three-way and the two-way
classifications of languages, which result from the two ways of looking at the
relation between meaning and linguistic forms. By looking at which semantic
information gets typically expressed in verb roots, he proposes the typology of path-
conflating, manner-conflating and figure-conflating languages; by looking at which
linguistic forms typically encode the Path of motion, the typology of verb-framed
and satellite-framed languages is put forward. Section 2.3 presents two proposals for
reformulating Talmy’s binary typology so as to account for both a larger number of
languages and for the observations that languages within the same typological group
also show differences in the expression of Manner and of Path. Finally, in Section
2.4, Sapir and Whorf’s linguistic relativity hypothesis and a weaker version of this
hypothesis, Slobin’s thinking-for-speaking hypothesis are presented. The domain of
motion is suitable for exploring how language-specific semantic patterns influence
the way speakers of a language talk and think about motion.
Chapter 3 summarises the whole gamut of research influenced by the Talmian
theory of lexicalisation patterns for motion events. Such research might be grouped
into two blocks: Slobin’s thinking-for-speaking research (Section 3.2) and linguistic
relativity research (Section 3.3). Slobin’s hypothesis of thinking-for-speaking
encompasses several lines of research in the attempt to account for the pervasive
effects that language use has on mental processes tied to language (i.e., speaking,
writing, translating, gesturing, etc). Section 3.2 reviews research on the impact that
lexicalisation patterns have on narrative styles of verb- and satellite-framed
6
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION
languages and on the translation process. Moreover, research on sign languages and
co-speech gestures, on language acquisition, and on language processing is also
reviewed. Linguistic research on the expression of motion has documented habitual
ways of speaking and writing about motion which has served as foundations for
further investigating how children acquire these language-specific patterns, the
effects of these patterns in language processing and whether the effects go beyond
language and have an impact on non-linguistic cognition. In Section 3.3, linguistic
relativity research is assessed. The question of whether differences in the encoding
of motion result in divergent conceptualisations of motion events by speakers of
verb- and satellite-framed languages has been extensively investigated, but this
research has yielded inconclusive results. It is also suggested that the intrinsic nature
of the stimuli (i.e., the types of Path, the types of Manner and the types of Figure)
might be responsible for these divergent results.
Chapter 4 describes some classifications and analyses of motion verbs from
(a) different theoretical backgrounds, (b) online databases for English and Spanish
lexicons, and (c) research on motion events. Section 4.2 begins with an overview of
the Syntax-Semantics Interface approach to the analysis of motion verbs, which
explores how a verb’s semantics can determine the syntactic realisation of its
arguments. In Section 4.3, two classifications of English motion verbs within the
Functional-Lexematic model are presented. Next, Section 4.4 offers a brief overview
of some of the existing online databases for English and Spanish verb lexicons; in
these databases, classifications of motion verbs which range from the more
syntactically-oriented to the more semantically-oriented are outlined. Finally, in
Section 4.5, crosslinguistic research on manner and path verbs is reviewed. This
research analyses motion verbs in a more fine-grained fashion than under Talmy’s
initial formulation; scholars break the Path and the Manner component into more
granular categories which permit exploration of the semantics of path and manner
verbs across languages in a subtler way.
Chapter 5 explores the semantic nature of the English and the Spanish motion
verb lexicons and offers a contrastive account with special attention to the path and
manner dimensions of motion. Section 5.2 describes the aims, research questions,
corpus and the methodology followed. In section 5.3, the general distribution of
7
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION
English and Spanish motion verbs, in terms of the general semantic components that
their verbs encode, are presented. Section 5.4 focuses on path verbs and zooms in to
the sort of path notions which seem to be typically expressed in these languages.
Next, Section 5.5 ponders the semantic nature of manner verbs in each language and
the sort of manner information which seems to be characteristically lexicalised in
these languages. Finally, Section 5.6 answers the research questions addressed
throughout the chapter and sums up the main conclusions, pointing out both
crosslinguistic differences and similarities.
Chapter 6 reports three experimental studies on human locomotion. In Section
6.2, the first study (i.e., the Definition and Feature Listing task) is reported; English
and Spanish native speakers are asked to define the verbs move, walk, run and jump,
and to list their defining features. Section 6.3 presents the second study (i.e., the Free
Verb Listing task), in which English and Spanish speakers are given one minute to
list verbs for moving, walking, running and jumping. In Section 6.4, the third study
(i.e., the Rating task) is reported; English and Spanish native speakers are asked to
rate whether a list of motion verbs are good examples of walking, of running and of
jumping. This chapter sheds light on how English and Spanish organise their human
locomotion verb lexicons and into the complex semantics of human locomotion
verbs.
Chapter 7 summarises the main findings of this dissertation, addresses
possible directions for future research and suggests some applications of the work
presented. This thesis concludes by suggesting the need for further experimental
research on the semantics of motion verbs and for further crosslinguistic research to
test whether the tendencies documented in this work apply to other verb- and
satellite-framed languages.
8
CHAPTER 2
10
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
2.1.1. Introduction
Cognitive linguistics […] is an approach to language that is based on our experience of the
world and the way we perceive and conceptualize it.
In the eighties, a new approach to the study of language began to develop: Cognitive
Linguistics. This school of linguistics was born as a reaction against formal
approaches to language, such as Noam Chomsky’s Generative Grammar (e.g., 1957,
1965). Cognitive Linguistics rejects the main claims made by Generative Linguistics:
(1) language is an innate and autonomous cognitive faculty; (2) to know a language
is to know its grammar, which consists of a finite number of combinatory rules; (3)
syntax (form) is the main focus of linguistic analysis (and thus semantics (meaning)
is largely overlooked). The birth of Cognitive Linguistics is also rooted in the
emergence of the second generation of Cognitive Science in the 1970s, especially in
research on human categorisation (Rosch, 1973, 1975). The field of Cognitive
Linguistics is defined by two commitments: (1) the Generalisation commitment, and
(2) the Cognitive commitment. The Generalization commitment represents a
11
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
1
Evans, Bergen & Zinken (2007) distinguish two main branches of the cognitive linguistic movement:
(1) Cognitive Semantics (cf. Lakoff 1987; Langacker, 1987; Fillmore, 1985; Talmy, 2000a, 2000b)
and (2) cognitive approaches to grammar such as Langacker’s Cognitive Grammar (1987, 1991, 2005)
12
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
summarised as follows: (1) language is not a modular system in our brain; (2)
language reflects general cognitive principles; (3) language and cognition are
embodied, that is, our linguistic and conceptual categories are grounded in physical,
social and cultural experiences; (4) language is all about meaning; language is the
vehicle of meaning and also a window into conceptualisation; (5) there are common
structuring principles which hold across all language areas (phonology, morphology,
syntax, semantics and pragmatics).
Having dealt with the birth of Cognitive Linguistics and its main
assumptions, we move on to one of the main areas in cognitive linguistic research:
Cognitive Semantics.
Cognitive Semantics was born out of the dissatisfaction with formal theories of
meaning based on the objectivist world view. In formal theories such as truth-
conditional semantics, meaning was seen as the link between the world and words,
completely disengaged from human cognition (Sweetser, 1990: 4). In contrast,
cognitive semanticists consider linguistic meaning as a manifestation of conceptual
structure, which emerges from bodily experience. Accordingly, Cognitive Semantics
is concerned with the relationship between experience, cognition and language, with
the result that it explores the connections between human bodily experience, the
conceptual system and the semantic structure expressed by language.
and Construction Grammar (cf. Bergen & Chang, 2005; Goldberg, 1995; Hilferty, 2003; Kay &
Fillmore 1999). Some of the theories developed within Cognitive Semantics are The Cognitive
Theory of Metaphor and Metonymy (cf. Barcelona, 2000, 2002; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Lakoff,
1987; Turner, 1996; Soriano, 2005), and Blending Theory (cf. Fauconnier, 1994; Fauconnier &
Turner, 2002).
13
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
2
Unlike other approaches, the study of the meaning of both open-class words (nouns, verbs and
adjectives) and closed-class words (prepositions, conjunctions, adverbs, etc) fall within the purview of
cognitive semantics. In fact, Talmy mostly concentrates on closed-class words – or grammatical
systems – as ‘the closed-class system of language is its most fundamental and comprehensive
conceptual structuring system’ (Talmy, 2000a: 13).
14
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
15
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
Categorisation
Another phenomenon that has received great attention within cognitive semantics is
categorisation, that is, our ability to identify entities as members of groups or
categories. Research by Eleanor Rosch during the 1970s challenged the classical
view of categories, seen as sets of necessary and sufficient conditions with clear
boundaries, in favour of a prototype organisation of categories. The findings from
Rosch showed that the boundaries between categories are fuzzy and that membership
is graded instead of a matter of ‘all-or-nothing’. For example, for the category BIRD,
robins or sparrows are considered as better examples or prototypical members
whereas penguins and ostriches are less ‘bird-like’. George Lakoff’s notion of
Idealised Cognitive Model (1987) could be seen as an application to language of
Rosch’s observations on prototypicality. According to Lakoff (1987), we organise
our knowledge by means of structures called Idealised Cognitive Models (ICMs);
16
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
prototypical members are those who fit with the background assumptions and
expectations of the ICM, of our ideal representation of the world. Let’s illustrate the
notion of ICM with the classic example of BACHELOR: Casanova would fit perfectly
into our ICM of being a bachelor because he is unmarried but marriageable whereas
the Pope would not fit as nicely in this category. The Pope, although unmarried, is
not a prototypical bachelor because the concept of POPE is more often understood in
terms of the ICM of the ROMAN CATHOLIC CHURCH, whose clergy cannot marry.
17
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
images or mental pictures. They operate, instead, at the level of generality and
abstraction (Johnson, 1987: 23-24).
Conceptual Structure
Cognitive semanticists start from the assumption that semantic structure reflects
conceptual structure. They are concerned with the conceptual structuring
mechanisms reflected in linguistic structure and how the findings relative to the
linguistic representation of conceptual content are to be related to more general
cognitive processes. According to the cognitive semanticist Leonard Talmy,
language is ‘one system of mental functioning through which the mind could be
studied more generally’ (in Ibarretxe-Antuñano, 2006c: 254). The analysis of
conceptual structure and language is central and fundamental for the study of
cognition as it serves as a starting point to further understanding the organising
principles of other systems, whether by generalising the similarities or by contrasting
the differences.
In the following section, we will provide a detailed account of Leonard
Talmy’s theory of conceptual structure.
Leonard Talmy was one of the pioneers of the cognitive linguistic enterprise during
the 1970s. In his introduction to the two 2000 volumes under the generic title of
Toward a Cognitive Semantics, he explicitly characterises research on cognitive
semantics and the main methodology to be followed: ‘research on cognitive
semantics is research on conceptual content and its organization in language and,
hence, [research] on the nature of conceptual content and organization in general’
(Talmy, 2000a: 4). Moreover, he claims that introspection is a legitimate
methodology for accessing conceptual content. As the object of study is meaning and
‘meaning is located in conscious experience’, introspection takes the researcher to
where the subjective data are located. He argues that ‘the use of introspection must
be recognized as an appropriate and arguably necessary methodology in cognitive
science, together with the other generally accepted methodologies’ (Talmy, 2000a:
6).
18
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
19
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
or scaffolding for the conceptual material that is lexically specified’ (Talmy, 2000a:
21).
CONCEPTUAL LANGUAGE
STRUCTURE
Though Talmy’s work has examined both the lexical and the grammatical systems,
he has been mainly concerned with the semantics of the closed-class system. While
it is impossible to propose a finite inventory of open-class concepts, Talmy claims
that there is a universally available inventory of concepts which is encoded by
closed-class forms. A language expresses a selection of those concepts, but it does
not necessarily express all the available concepts. By examining the semantics of the
closed-class systems across languages, he identifies a number of schematic systems
which give structure to our conceptual system.
Schematic systems
The conceptual structuring system, according to Talmy, is based on a number of
schematic systems 3 . These schematic systems provide the basic architecture upon
which the rich content meaning (of open-class forms) hinges. In an interview with
Ibarretxe-Antuñano (2006), Talmy identifies five key schematic systems: (1) the
3
In part 1 of volume I (2000), Talmy introduces the notion of schematic systems. In the remaining
three parts of that volume, he devotes chapters to three schematic systems: the Configurational
System, the Attentional System and the Force Dynamics System. A comprehensive account of
Talmy’s schematic systems is also to be found in Evans & Green (2006: Section 6.2.2, Section 15.2).
20
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
Configurational System; (2) the Perspectival System; (3) the Attentional System; (4)
the Force Dynamics System, and (5) the Cognitive State System.
The first schematic system, the Configurational System, represents structure
for space or time and divides the scene into parts and participants. It includes much
that is within the schemas evoked by spatial prepositions, by temporal conjunctions,
and by aspect and tense markers. For example, at home represents a point-of-space,
in five minutes an extent-of-time, etc.
While the first schematic system establishes the basic delineations by which a
scene or event being referred to is structured, the second schematic system, the
Perspectival System, specifies the perspective from which one views a scene or
places one’s mental eyes. The perspective system includes at least the following
conceptual distinctions: spatial or temporal positioning within a larger frame, the
distance away from the referent entity, its change or lack of change of perspective
point in the course of time as well as the path it follows, and the viewing direction.
For example, in sentence (2) we have an internal perspective location, that is, our
mental eyes are inside the room. However, in sentence (3) the scene is viewed from
outside the room, in other words, we have an exterior perspective location.
(2) The lunchroom door slowly opened and two men walked in.
(3) Two men slowly opened the lunchroom door and walked in.
The third schematic system, the Attentional System, establishes how one is to
distribute one’s attention over the scene. For instance, in (4), the focal attention is on
the seller, while in example (5) the focal attention is mapped onto the buyer with
lesser attention on the remainder.
Furthermore, there are also patterns of distributing attention, such as the windowing
of attention, in which one or more (discontinuous) regions within a referent scene are
allocated greater attention. According to Talmy, ‘[t]he portions that are foregrounded
by inclusion are windowed, and the portions that are backgrounded by exclusion are
21
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
(6) The crate fell out of the plane through the air into the ocean = maximal
windowing
(7) The crate fell into the ocean = final windowing of attention
4
Talmy (2000a: 413) remarks ‘[t]he primary distinction that language marks here is a role difference
between the two entities exerting the forces. One force-exerting entity is singled out for focal attention
– the salient issue in the interaction is whether this entity is able to manifest its force tendency or, on
the contrary, is overcome. The second force entity, correlatively, is considered for the effect that it has
on the first, effectively overcoming it or not’. The focal force entity is called the Agonist, and the force
element that opposes it the Antagonist.
22
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
further divisions, such as expectations, which cover both the expected and the
surprising, as represented by closed-class systems such as the how / so forms in
English: How big your eyes are! Your eyes are so big! Finally, the affective division
(as expressed by diminutive and pejorative closed-class forms) is also a part of the
Cognitive State schematic system.
On the whole, these five schematic systems relate to fundamental aspects of
our embodied experience. The first three schematic systems (Configurational,
Perspectival and Attentional systems) have much to do with our visual perception
whereas the last two posited schematic systems (Force Dynamics and Cognitive
State) relate to our bodily experience of forces, pressures, etc, and to our intentions,
knowledge and expectations.
2.2.1. Introduction
23
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
English by verbs and by prepositions (e.g., out, into), that is, by two different
linguistic elements. According to Talmy, by looking at the relations between
meaning and linguistic forms, a range of universal principles and typological patterns
might emerge 5 .
On the whole, his approach to lexicalisation can be summarized as follows
(adapted from (1) in Talmy (2000b: 22)):
5
Talmy (2000b: Chapter 2, Section 3) provides an exhaustive classification of different types of
typologies and universals which summarises and organises his research findings.
6
According to Talmy, entities are ‘elements, relations, and structures: both particular cases and
categories of these’ (Talmy, 2000b: 22).
24
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
The basic Motion event consists of one object (the Figure) moving or located with respect
to another object (the reference object or Ground). It is analyzed as having four
components: besides Figure and Ground, there are Path and Motion. The Path (with
capital P) is the path followed or site occupied by the Figure object with respect to the
Ground object. The component of Motion (with capital M) refers to the presence per se of
motion or locatedness in the event […] In addition to these internal components, a Motion
7
Talmy (1991) suggests the typology for motion events extends as well to five other events: temporal
contouring, state change, action correlation and realisation. For some examples see section 2.2.4. For a
detailed account see Talmy (1991) and Talmy (2000b: Sections 2.5, 2.6, 2.7, and 2.8).
25
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
event can be associated with an external Co-event that most often bears the relation of
Manner or of Cause to it. (Talmy, 2000b: 25).
Harry is the Figure, the stairs is the Ground and down is the Path. The verb, to walk
expresses simultaneously the fact of Motion (framing event) and the Manner of
motion (Co-event).
The terms Figure and Ground 8 were taken from Gestalt psychology, but
Talmy gave them a distinct semantic interpretation: ‘[t]he Figure is a moving or
conceptually movable entity whose path or site is at issue. The Ground is a reference
frame, or a reference object stationary within a reference frame, with respect to
which the Figure’s path, or site is characterized’ (Talmy, 2000b: 26).
At this point, it is important to define the two types of motion found in
motion events: translational motion and self-contained motion. In Talmy’s (2000b:
35) words, ‘[i]n translational motion, an object’s basic location shifts from one point
to another in space. In self-contained Motion, an object keeps its same, or “average,”
location. Self-contained Motion generally consists of oscillation, rotation, dilation
(expansion and contraction), wiggle, local wander, or rest’. Let us consider examples
(9), (10) and (11) to illustrate these notions.
8
According to Talmy, these notions have several advantages over Fillmore’s cases. The notion of
Ground captures the commonality of functioning as reference object for a figural entity which is
present in Fillmore’s separate cases of ‘Location’, ‘Source’, ‘Goal’, and ‘Path’. The terms Figure and
Ground are close equivalents of Langacker’s trajector and landmark (1987: 217): trajector is ‘the
figure within a relational profile’, whereas landmark refers to ‘other salient entities in a relational
predication’.
26
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
(11) Claire slid through the hall in her socks = self-contained + translational
motion
Example (9) depicts the Figure’s change of location from the outside to the inside of
the room. In contrast, example (10) shows self-contained motion; the Figure stay
over the flower by moving its wings. Sometimes, it is difficult to tell translational
and self-contained motion apart, as we observe in (11). The manner verb to slide
includes a component of friction, or rubbing (i.e., self-contained motion) between the
Figure (Claire) and the Ground (the hall). However, such friction can only exist in
the course of the Figure’s translational motion (through the hall). Thus, in English,
the activity of self-contained motion has often come to be anchored in the framework
of translational motion (Talmy, 2000b: 228-229).
Self-contained motion is intrinsically linked to manner of motion, in other
words, to the Manner relation between the co-event and the motion event. In the
Manner relation, the co-event co-occurs with the motion event, as we have seen in
example (11), and is conceptualised as an additional activity that the Figure of the
motion event exhibits. This activity directly pertains to the motion event but is
distinct from it. However, the relation between the co-event and the motion event
need not be limited to that of Manner, rather it can bear a wide range of relations
(Talmy, 2000b: 42-47):
a. Causal relation: ‘the Co-event can precede the main Motion event in the case of
onset causation, or it can co-occur with the main Motion event in the case of
extended causation’ (Talmy, 2000b: 44-45).
(12) Our tent blew down into the gully from a gust of wind = Onset causation
b. Precursion relation: the Co-event precedes the main motion event but does not
cause or assist its occurrence. In the example below, the splintering of the glass
preceded but did not cause the motion of the glass onto the carpet.
27
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
c. Enablement relation: the Co-event directly precedes the main motion event and
enables the occurrence of an event that causes the Motion but does not itself cause
this Motion:
(16) Could you reach/grab that bottle down off the shelf?
d. Reverse enablement: the Co-event named by the verb is an event that has
previously taken place and that now gets undone. This new event, in turn, enables the
main motion event named by the verb particle ‘auf’ in the following example:
(17) Ich habe den Sack aufgebunden (Lit. I have the sack open-tied = I untied the
sack and opened it).
e. Concomitance relation: the Co-event co-occurs with the main motion event and is
an activity that the Figure of the motion event additionally exhibits. But this activity
does not in itself pertain to the concurrent Motion, that is, it could just take place by
itself:
f. Concurrent result: the Co-event results from or is caused by the main motion event,
and would not otherwise occur:
g. Subsequence relation: the Co-event takes place directly after the main motion
event, and is enabled by, caused by, or is the purpose of that motion event:
(22) I’ll stop down at your office (on my way out of the building).
28
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
Now that we have offered a definition of a motion event and examined the
semantic components which underpin the conceptualisation of this event and its
varying relations with a Co-event, we move on to deal with the characteristic
lexicalisation patterns observed for motion events. In the next two sections, the two
typologies of lexicalisation patterns proposed by Talmy are presented in detail9 . As
we briefly mentioned in the introduction, the three-way classification (Talmy, 1972,
1985) and two-way classification of languages (Talmy, 1991, 2000b) represent a
different perspective taken on the relations between the semantic level and the
morphosyntactic (linguistic) level. The former perspective kept constant a
morphosyntactic constituent, the verb root, and looked at which semantic
components were characteristically placed in it by various languages. It was found
that most languages characteristically express either the Path, the Co-event (the
Manner or the Cause), or the Figure in addition to the fact of Motion in the verb. In
the later perspective, the semantic component of Path was kept constant, focusing the
examination on which morphosyntactic constituent it was characteristically placed in
by various languages. It was observed that most languages characteristically place
the Path component either in the verb root (in verb-framed languages) or in the
satellite 10 and/or preposition (in satellite-framed languages).
9
Although some European linguists, Bally (1965), Tesnière (1959) and Wandruzska (1976) had
already pointed out differences in the lexicalisation of motion in French and German, they had not
provided a systematic or theoretical account of the phenomena involved.
10
According to Talmy (2000b: 102), a satellite is ‘the grammatical category of any constituent that is
in a sister relation to the verb root’. Within this category, English verb particles, German separable
and inseparable verb prefixes, Russian verb prefixes, etc are included.
29
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
In one group of languages, including Finno-Ugric, Chinese, Ojibwa, Warlpiri and all
branches of Indo-European languages (except for Romance languages), the verb
typically expresses at once the Motion and a Co-event, usually either the Manner or
the Cause of the Motion. English is the prototypical example of this group.
(24) English expressions of Motion with conflated Manner or Cause taken from
Talmy (2000: 28)
Move + Manner
Non-agentive
a. The rock slid/rolled/bounced down the hill
b. The gate swung/creaked shut on its rusty hinges
c. The smoke swirled/squeezed through the opening
Agentive
d. I slid/rolled/bounced the keg into the storeroom
e. I twisted/popped the cork out of the bottle
Self-agentive
f. I ran/limped/jumped/stumbled/rushed/groped my way down the stairs
g. She wore a green dress to the party
Move + Cause
Non-agentive
h. The napkin blew off the table
30
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
At this point, the terms non-agentive, agentive and self-agentive motion call
for a definition. Firstly, non-agentive motion has to do with situations in which
entities that are incapable of motion on their own perform some motion. Secondly,
agentive motion refers to a motion event whose Figure is moved by an agent; the
agent causes the motion but the verb can express either its Cause or the Manner in
which the Figure moves. Finally, self-agentive motion refers to events in which
Figures are able to move by themselves.
In the second typological pattern for the expression of motion, the verb conflates
both the fact of Motion and Path. Semitic, Polynesian, Romance, Korean, Turkish,
Tamil, Nez Perce, and Caddo belong to this pattern. Spanish motion verbs are perfect
examples of this type:
(25) Spanish expressions of Motion with conflation of Path taken from Talmy
(2000: 49-51)
Non-agentive
a. La botella entró a la cueva (flotando)
The bottle MOVED-in to the cave (floating)
‘The bottle floated into the cave’
b. La botella salió de la cueva (flotando)
The bottle MOVED-out from the cave (floating)
‘The bottle floated out of the cave’
Agentive
31
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
The reader should note that Spanish motion verbs are glossed as MOVED-in/out (for
non-agentive motion) and AMOVED-in/out (for agentive motion), to show the
conflation of Motion and Path.
As the examples (a-d) illustrate, if the Co-event (either Manner or Cause) is
expressed in Spanish, it tends to be in an independent element, usually an adverbial
or gerundive. In many languages, besides Spanish, the expression of Manner and/or
Cause by adverbials and gerundives is stylistically awkward. That is why
information about Manner or Cause is often omitted, especially when the Manner of
motion is a default or expected Manner of motion of the Figure, or it has been
previously established in the surrounding discourse.
In contrast, English verb roots readily conflate the Co-event but not Path.
This lexicalisation pattern is not characteristic, though English also has verbs that
incorporate Path, such as arrive, approach, circle, cross, descend, enter, exit, follow,
join, pass, rise, return, separate, etc., but most of them are historic borrowings from
Romance languages. Furthermore, Talmy (2000b: 62, 139) notes that this conflation
pattern, i.e., Motion + Path in verb roots, rarely extends to conflation of location with
site, such as ‘BE-on’, ‘BE-under’, etc, though in English, there are some incidental
cases of such conflation: surround (be around), top (be atop), flank (be beside),
adjoin (be next to), span (be from one side to the other), line (be in line) and fill.
In the third typological pattern, the verb root conflates Motion and Figure.
Languages in this type are Navajo and Hokan languages (such as Atsugewi). English
does have a few forms that conform to this pattern:
32
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
(26) English examples of conflation of Motion and Figure taken from Talmy
(2000b: 57)
Non-agentive
a. It rained in through the bedroom window
Agentive
b. I spat into the cuspidor
(27) Atwugewi verb roots of Motion with conflated Figure taken from Talmy
(2000b: 58)
a. -lup- ‘for a small shiny spherical object (e.g., a round candy, an eyeball, a
hailstone) to move/be located
b. -caq- ‘for a slimy lumpish object (e.g., a toad, a cow dropping) to move/be
located’
c. -staq- ‘for a runny icky material (e.g., mud, manure, rotten tomatoes, guts,
chewed gum) to move/be-located
(28) Atsugewi expressions of motion with conflated Figure (Talmy, 2000b: 59)
a. /’-w-uh-staq-ik - a /
Locative suffix: -ik ‘on the ground’
Instrumental prefix: uh- ‘from gravity (an object’s own weight) acting on it’
Inflectional affix set: ’-w - -a ‘3rd person subject (factual mood)’
33
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
Literal: ‘Runny icky material is located on the ground from its own weight
acting on it’
Instantiated: ‘Guts are lying on the ground’
b. /’-w-ca-staq-ict- a /
Directional suffix: -ict ‘into liquid’
Instrumental prefix: ca- ‘from the wind blowing on the figure’
Inflectional affix set: ’-w - -a ‘3rd person subject (factual mood)’
Literal: ‘Runny icky material moved into liquid from the wind blowing on it
Instantiated: ‘The guts blew into the creek’
c. /s-’-w-cu-staq-cis- a /
Directional suffix: -cis ‘into fire’
Instrumental prefix: cu- ‘from a linear object, moving axially, acting on the
figure’
Inflectional affix set: s-’-w- -a ‘I subject,3rd person object (factual mood)’
Literal: ‘I caused it that runny icky material move into fire by acting on it
with a linear object moving axially’
Instantiated: ‘I prodded the guts into the fire with a stick’
Languages can sometimes conflate the same kind of semantic distinctions but
in very distinctive ways. For example, Southwest Pomo conflates Motion with
Figure, not as Atsugewi does, but rather with the numerosity of the Figure: ‘for
one/two or three/several together … to move’.
While these three conflation systems for Motion verbs are apparently the main ones
found across languages, Talmy (200b: 62) notes other lexicalisation patterns which
occur as well, in addition to some which do not. He observes some minor systems of
agentive verbs conflating two semantic components:
a. Ground + Path + Motion, such as box and shelve in the following examples:
34
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
(31) She powdered her nose = cause facial powder to move onto
[T]he different conflation types seem to be ranked in their prevalence among the world’s
languages, with conflation of Path as the most extensively represented, of Manner/Cause
next, and of Figure least so. It may therefore be the case that Ground conflation is also a
possibility, but one so unlikely that it has not yet been instantiated in any language.
Nonetheless, this does not really explain why the lexicalisation of Ground is
dispreferred by languages 11 . The prevalence of the three lexicalisation patterns
among the world’s languages may be interpreted as if languages are much more
concerned with expressing the trajectory of motion, the manner in which the Figure
moves and even the entity that moves than with conveying the reference entity. It
might be argued that Ground is the least prominent element of a motion scene, thus,
it is disfavoured in lexicalisation; however, we have not come across any
psycholinguistic research supporting this idea.
11
I am grateful to Michele Feist for pointing this out.
35
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
b. Path-conflating verbs for autonomous motion of the Figure, that is, ‘(for a
Figure) to MOVE along X Path’ and for controlled agentive motion, that is,
‘(for an Agent) to MOVE (the Figure) along X Path while holding (it)’
(Talmy, 2000b: 65).
12
Slobin and Hoiting (1994) find that the boundary-crossing constraint (i.e., present in events in
which the Figure crosses a boundary, such as exit and enter) is at work in French, Japanese, Korean
and Turkish as well.
36
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
The two-way typology results from looking at the morphosyntactic constituents that
characteristically encode the Path component of a motion event. Thus, Path appears
in the verb root in verb-framed languages such as Spanish, in (34), and it is
lexicalised in the satellite in satellite-framed languages such as English in (35):
37
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
Atsugewi polysynthetic affixes around the verb root. Satellites 13 can encode not only
Path of motion (e.g., English out, into, away, etc) but also Path + Ground (e.g.,
Atsugewi –ict ‘into a liquid’), Manner (e.g., Nez Perce manner prefix wis- ‘traveling
with one’s belongings’) and Cause (e.g., Atsugewi Ca- ‘from the wind blowing on P
[Patient]’).
Slobin (2004) provides a detailed list of verb- and satellite-framed languages:
• Satellite-framed languages:
o Germanic: Danish, Dutch, English, German, Icelandic, Swedish, Yiddish
o Slavic: Czech, Polish, Russian, Serbo-Croatian, Ukrainian
o Finno-Ugric: Finnish, Hungarian
o Chinese: Mandarin
o Australian: Warlpiri
• Verb-framed languages:
o Romance: Catalan, French, Galician, Italian, Portuguese, Spanish
o Semitic: Moroccan Arabic, Hebrew
o Turkic: Turkish
o Basque
o Japanese
o Korean
o Sign Languages: American Sign Language, Sign Language of the
Netherlands
Talmy (1991, 2000b: Sections 2.5-2.8) suggests that the constituent where
Path is characteristically expressed is to a great extent where aspect, change of state,
action correlation, and realisation are characteristically encoded as well. In other
words, the binary typology for motion events extends to at least other four types of
events. Thus, verb-framed languages characteristically lexicalise the trajectory of
motion, aspect, change of state, action correlation and realisation in the main verb,
13
See (Talmy, 2000b: Chapter 1, Section 3) for an in depth presentation of the grammatical category
satellite and the sort of semantic notions it can lexicalise.
38
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
In all of these examples, changes of state as well as action correlation are expressed
in English with the particle or satellite, while the main verb encodes the Co-event
(Manner or Cause). In contrast, in Spanish the main verb expresses the transition to a
new state in (37), and coactivity in (38), while the adjunct, either a prepositional
phrase (de un soplido, con fuego) or a gerund (soplándola, quemándolo, tocando),
expresses the Co-event.
The shift of a language from one preferred lexicalisation pattern to another, or its
maintenance through the course of time is still an unexplored research area. The
factors that may have tilted one language towards maintaining its typological pattern
category and another language toward shifting to another must yet be discerned.
In a speculative fashion, Talmy (2000b: 118-119) comments that Latin,
classical Greek, and Proto-Germanic all exhibited the presumably Indo-European
pattern of using co-event-conflating verb roots together with path satellites that
formed prefixes on the verb roots. Possibly because of phonological changes that
39
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
made the path prefixes less distinct from each other and from the verb roots, all three
languages apparently became unable to maintain their inherited pattern. Both
Germanic and Greek 14 proceeded to develop a new set of path satellites that largely
supplanted the prior set. This permitted the maintenance of the inherited pattern for
representing motion events with co-event verb conflation. On the other hand, the
languages arising from Latin developed a new system of path-conflating verbs, rather
than re-establishing the path satellite system. In this process, Talmy (2000b: 119)
remarks that each of the daughter languages formed its set of path verbs in its own
way by variously coining new verbs or shifting the semantics of inherited verbs so as
to fill out the basic directional grid of the new path verb system. For example, the
Latin verb salio (‘jump’) which combined with path prefixes such as up and out
developed into Italian salire (‘ascend’) and Spanish salir (‘go out’) (cf. Wälchli,
ms.). At the same time, these languages may have undergone the complementary
change of advancing their gerundive construction for the expression of Manner and
Cause.
Drawing on diachronic data, Anetta Kopecka (2004, 2006a, in press) deals
with French’s typological shift from a satellite-framed pattern to a verb-framed
pattern. She provides diachronic evidence of this shift and concludes this shift can be
mainly attributed to the weakening of productivity of verbal prefixes 15 . Although Old
French prefixes lost their productivity, many remnants are found in modern French.
On one hand, there are verbs with quite transparent prefixes, such as s’en-voler ‘fly
away’ and sur-voler ‘fly over’ which conform to the satellite-framed pattern. On the
other hand, there are more opaque verbs, whose composite nature is no longer
discernable, such as arriver ‘arrive’ and éloigner ‘move away’, which is consistent
with the verb-framed pattern.
14
Note that modern Greek is categorised as a verb-framed language by Papafragou et al. (2002, 2006).
15
Other closely related factors contributed to this typological shift as well, namely, the loss of
prefixed verbs and the substitution of prefixed verbs by other lexical items.
40
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
2.3.1. Introduction
The typology of verb- and satellite-framed languages has engendered a good deal of
research on motion event descriptions in a vast number of languages. On the basis of
those numerous studies, this typology called for a reformulation. According to
Ameka & Essegbey (in press), Slobin (2004) and Zlatev & Yangklang (2004),
among other authors, there are some languages – as we will see in the following
section – which do not seem to fit nicely into Talmy’s two-way typology, as these
languages encode both Manner and Path in morphosyntactic constituents of equal
status. In the attempt to account for such linguistic diversity, the category of
equipollently-framed languages, i.e., languages in which both Manner and Path are
expressed by equivalent grammatical forms, has been proposed by Slobin (2004).
Moreover, Talmy’s theory of lexicalisation patterns does not seem to take
into account the fact that languages within the same typological group show a
differing degree of manner and path elaborations. For example, Spanish and Basque
are both verb-framed languages but their elaborations of Manner and Path are quite
different, as has been shown by Ibarretxe-Antuñano (2004a, 2004b, 2004c, 2006a,
2006b). Basque describes Manner much more often than Spanish thanks to its sound
symbolic expressions or movement imitatives, and thus Basque is closer to satellite-
framed languages. On the other hand, in terms of Path, Basque generally offers more
detailed paths or trajectories than Spanish. As a solution, it has been proposed that
ranking languages on clines or continuums of manner (Slobin, 2004, 2006a, 2006b)
and path salience (Ibarretxe-Antuñano, 2004a, 2004c, 2004d, 2008) would be more
useful than placing them into typological categories. Slobin (2004, 2006a) and
Ibarretxe-Antuñano (e.g., 2004a, 2008) identify a number of factors which contribute
to a language’s degree of path and manner salience respectively. Sections 2.3.3 and
2.3.4 will deal with this last proposal of clines of semantic component saliency.
41
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
(2) Bipartite verb languages: the Hokan and Penutian languages described by
Delancey (1989, 1996) are languages in which the verb consists of two morphemes
of equal status, one expressing Manner and the other Path. Talmy (2000b: 113),
drawing from Aoki (1970), provides a similar description of Nez Perce manner
prefixes, such as quqú. – láhsa ‘gallop-ascend’. Richard Rhodes (in a personal
communication to Slobin in 2003) reports that such constructions are typical of
Algonquian, Athabaskan, Hokan, and Klamath-Takelman. Huang & Tanangkingsing
(2004) report that at least one Austronesian language, Tsou, has apparently
developed bipartite manner-path verbs from serial-verb constructions.
42
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
good way to view such linguistic variation, but he strongly argues that the criteria
used for judging main verb status in those languages have been too few, and that
authors need to apply an expanded set of criteria to elucidate which constituent is
privileged with main verb status. These criteria or factors range from phonological to
morphosyntactic to semantic factors.
Besides allocating languages into three typological categories, Slobin (2004, 2006a,
2006b) argues for a cline or continuum of manner salience. Independently from the
lexicalisation pattern they belong to, languages differ significantly in the amount of
manner information that is given, the frequency of mention of manner details, and
the sort of fine grained manner distinctions that they encode. Thus, there are high-
manner-salient languages and low-manner-salient languages. In Slobin’s (2004: 250)
own words:
43
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
John danced in circles/from one place to another/towards the door/to the door
For telic events where the end location of the Figure is stated, verb-frames
languages cannot use a manner verb, as in (40), but necessarily mark this change of
location with a path verb (entrar ‘enter’, salir ‘exit’, cruzar ‘cross’, etc) or neutral
verb (ir ‘go’, venir ‘come’, etc). In order to add Manner some sort of subordinate
construction is required (gerundive forms, prepositional phrases, etc) as in (41).
44
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
45
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
languages) though other factors come into play interacting with it and influencing the
degree of manner salience in a given language.
46
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
motion. This factor is crucial for languages such as Basque, Chantyal, Japanese and
Turkish (Ibarretxe-Antuñano, 2008).
The third factor has to do with verb omissions. High-path-salient languages
seem to be more likely to allow verb omissions than low-path-salient languages. The
former readily omit the verb as the path complements present in the utterance are
explicit enough, whereas the latter cannot omit the verb as it is one that conveys the
necessary information for the description of the motion event.
The fourth factor is related to the existence of dummy verbs (i.e., verbs whose
semantic content is poor) in high-path-salient languages. These verbs are
accompanied by path complements for describing motion events. A typical example
is the verb ‘to be’, such as in Swiss German and in Basque.
The fifth factor has to do with the concept of language orality (Mündlichkeit).
Ibarretxe-Antuñano (2008) states that Koch & Oesterreicher (1985) and
Oesterreicher (2001) consider that irrespective the mode of expression (written, oral)
languages can be classified as conceptually written and conceptually oral.
Conceptually oral languages are languages characterised by redundancy, elliptic
constructions, hyperbolic expressions, self-corrections, etc. Ibarretxe-Antuñano
(2008) notes that conceptually oral languages, such as Basque and Swiss dialect
Muotathal, are more likely to be high-path-salient than conceptually written
languages.
Last but not least, according to Ibarretxe Antuñano (2008), cultural factors
have a say in Path salience. Wilkins (2004) and Bavin (2004) argue that aboriginal
communities show a great concern for detailed attention to motion paths and for
orientation in space. As a consequence, their linguistic descriptions reflect this
concern.
2.4.1. Introduction
The question ‘what is the relation between talking about reality and thinking about
it?’, in other words, ‘what is the relation between language and thought?’ has
troubled many linguists and psychologists for many decades. Two general
47
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
approaches can be identified in the literature concerning the nature of the relation
between language and conceptualisation: the Universalist/Nativist approach and the
Linguistic Determinism/Relativity approach.
Before focussing on the Linguistic Relativity approach, which is the one that
concerns us here, a brief presentation of the Universalist/Nativist approach is
required. The Universalist or Nativist approach emphasizes cross-linguistic
similarities due to our shared human nature. Noam Chomsky is the main figure of
this universalist approach to language and reality. His reason to study language is:
Some basic tenets of this approach are (a) conceptual structures are universal, (b)
syntax is also universal and innate, and (c) cross-linguistic variability does not reflect
conceptual but merely linguistic differences. According to universalists, speaking a
language consists of translating the language of thought (Fodor, 1975, 1983). As Li
& Gleitman (2002: 266) put it ‘humans invent words that label their concepts’;
linguistic categories are a straightforward mapping from universal concepts.
Universalists acknowledge that languages vary in the way they express reality, but
claim that this cross-linguistic variability does not reflect any substantive differences
in the way humans think about reality.
Conversely, cognitive linguistics/semantics adopts a different position. As we
have already noted, one of the basic tenets of cognitive linguistics/semantics is that
language is a window into conceptual structure. From there the claim follows that
cross-linguistic variability reflects differences in conceptualisation. This is exactly
the diverging point between universal approaches and the Linguistic
Determinism/Relativity approach, with which cognitive linguistics is consonant and
which we will present in the following section. However, there is also room for
some sort of universalism in cognitive linguistics/semantics. Divergences in
conceptualisation are ‘thought to emerge from common conceptualising capacity,
which derives from shared aspects of human cognition’ (Evans & Green, 2006: 56).
48
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
Every student of language or society should be familiar with the essential idea of linguistic
relativity, the idea that culture, through language, affects the way we think, especially
perhaps our classification of the experienced world.
Human beings do not live in the objective world alone […] The fact of the matter is that the
“real world” is to a large extent unconsciously built up on the language habits of the group
[…] We see and hear and otherwise experience very largely as we do because the language
habits of our community predispose certain choices of interpretation. (Sapir, 1929: 209)
We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native language. The categories and types
that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there because they stare every
observer in the face; on the contrary, the world is presented in a kaleidoscope flux of
impressions which has to be organized by our minds—and this means largely by the
linguistic systems of our minds. (Whorf, 1956: 213)
Their view is that languages have the power to create distinctive worldviews out of
the same reality. This implies, therefore, that speakers of specific languages differ in
the way they think and act. Despite much academic confusion in interpreting Sapir
and Whorf as pointed out by Gumperz & Levinson (1996: 2, 22) and Pourcel (2005:
13-14), they did not claim that thought is completely determined by language. Their
49
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
position seems to have been that language (as a structured semantic system)
influences habitual thought.
Sapir and Whorf were not the first to express the idea that language
influenced thought, i.e., our understanding and perception of reality. In the 19th
century, Wilhelm von Humboldt ([1836] 1988: 54) viewed language as the formative
organ of thought. He held that thought is externalised in speech, and that ‘[t]hought
and language are therefore one and inseparable from each other’. To his mind, every
language has a characteristic Weltanschauung or world-view; language steps in
between man and the world as language presents the world to man. Later on, the
anthropologist Franz Boas, who took a less deterministic approach to linguistic
diversity, observed that language ‘determines those aspects of each experience that
must be expressed’ (1938: 127).
On the whole, according to Gentner & Goldin-Meadow (2003: 4), what has
been coined as The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis states that (a) languages vary in how they
carve up the world; (b) the structure of one’s language influences how one perceives
and understands the world; (c) therefore, speakers of different languages will
perceive/conceptualise the world differently.
The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis 16 is generally considered as consisting of two
versions: the strong version known as linguistic determinism and the weak version
named linguistic relativity. Linguistic determinism defends the ideas that language
entirely determines thought, our conceptualisation of reality, and that thinking would
be impossible without language. To put it simply, according to this view, if a given
language X does not have a linguistic category, speakers of language X cannot think
about it. Thus, speakers of language X understand the world in a different way from
speakers of language Y. Linguistic relativity claims language influences thought but
not in such a deterministic way. According to this weak version, language plays a
16
See Pourcel (2005: Chapter 1) for a comprehensive and extensive discussion on Linguistic
Relativity. In this chapter, she criticises the misunderstanding in the academic world of Sapir and
Whorf’s views. On the one hand, she remarks that these scholars did not claim that language entirely
determines thought, though they appreciate its psychological scope. On the other hand, she observed
that the principle of linguistic relativity is no longer considered an aim of anthropological linguistics;
instead, there has been a shift from linguistic relativity as a research tradition to linguistic relativity as
a questionable hypothesis.
50
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
51
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
52
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
There is a special kind of thinking that is intimately tied to language – namely, the thinking
that is carried out, on-line, in the process of speaking.
Whorf was surely wrong when he said that one’s language determines how one
conceptualises reality in general. But he was probably correct in a much weaker sense: one’s
language does determine how one must conceptualise reality when one has to talk about it.
(Pinker, 1989: 360)
[I]n each language only a part of the complete concept that we have in mind is expressed,
and […] each language has a peculiar tendency to select this or that aspect of the mental
image which is conveyed by the expression of the thought. (Boas, [1911] 1966: 38-39)
Boas was probably wrong in supposing that all speakers have a common complete
concept in their minds. However, he was right in suggesting that language selects
what to communicate. In Slobin’s (1991: 76) own formulation, ‘the expression of
experience in linguistic terms constitutes thinking for speaking – a special form of
53
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
In the evanescent time frame of constructing utterances in discourse one fits one’s thoughts
into available linguistics frames. “Thinking for speaking” involves picking those
characteristics of objects and events that (a) fit some conceptualization of the event, and (b)
are readily encodable in the language. (Slobin, 1991: 76)
In the on-line process of thinking for speaking, speakers of a given language attend
to the specific aspects of reality which are to be verbalised in language. In other
words, language directs one’s attention to particular aspects of experience.
Slobin (2003: 160) extends the label ‘thinking for speaking’ to all forms of
linguistic production (speaking, writing, signing) and reception (listening, reading,
viewing) as well as a range of mental processes (understanding, imaging,
remembering) in the attempt to account for the far-reaching effects language use has
on mental processes tied to language. To his mind, research concerned with thinking-
for-speaking should have the following characteristics (Slobin, 2003: 161):
54
CHAPTER 2. COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND THE THEORY OF LEXICALISATION PATTERNS
written and oral discourse situations in which this domain can be examined (d). Thus,
the domain of motion is suitable for exploring how language-specific semantic
patterns influence the way speakers of a language talk about motion. The next
chapter will provide an in depth summary of thinking-for-speaking research on
motion.
55
CHAPTER 3
BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC
STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
3.1. Introduction
3.4. Summary
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
3.1. Introduction
The domain of motion has received a good deal of research attention since Talmy’s
seminal work (e.g., 1985, 1991, 2000a, 2000b). This chapter provides a review of the
whole gamut of research inspired or influenced by the Talmian theory of
lexicalisation patterns for motion events. Such research could be divided into two
parts: Slobin’s thinking-for-speaking research (section 3.2) and linguistic relativity
research (section 3.3).
Thinking-for-speaking research attempts to account for the far-reaching
effects that language use has on mental processes tied to language. Sections 3.2.2 and
3.2.3 review the vast amount of research on the bearing that lexicalisation patterns
have on the narrative styles of a large number of languages, as well as on the process
of translating between typologically distinct languages. Next, sections 3.2.4 and 3.2.5
summarise research on both sign languages and gestures accompanying motion event
descriptions. Such research also falls within the purview of thinking-for-speaking,
namely thinking-for-signing/gesturing. In section 3.2.6 first language acquisition
research is reviewed. This line of research addresses whether children are or are not
influenced very early by their language-specific lexicalisation patterns, and the
nature of the potential effect. After that, section 3.2.7 presents several experimental
studies concerned with language-specific semantic biases in adults’ language
processing. Lastly, preliminary investigations into the relations between motion and
vision are presented in section 3.2.8; it seems that habitual ways of speaking about
motion also affect how we talk about vision.
Finally, in section 3.3, research on the everlasting question on the relation
between language and cognition is assessed. Linguistic relativity research draws
heavily on the crosslinguistic differences in the encoding of motion and asks whether
these differences result in divergent conceptualisations of motion events by speakers
of verb- and satellite-framed languages.
57
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
3.2.1. Introduction
(1) References to manner of motion will occur frequently, across genres and
discourse contexts.
(2) Manner-of-motion verbs will be acquired early.
(3) The language will have continuing lexical innovation in this domain,
including extended and metaphorical uses.
(4) Speakers will have rich mental imagery of manner of motion.
58
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
(5) Manner of motion will be salient in memory for events and in verbal
accounts of events. (Slobin, 2003: 163-164)
Although these five consequences are called cognitive, it should be noted that the
first three consequences refer to linguistic cognition, whereas the last two are
cognitive hypotheses about speakers’ conceptualisations of motion (i.e., non-
linguistic cognition). Our review of thinking-for-speaking research in the following
sections will thus focus on the first three, leaving the final two for Section 3.3.
A great bulk of research on motion events across a wide range of languages comes
from elicited spoken narratives using a wordless picture book, Frog, Where are you?
(Mayer, 1969). The book is about a boy who goes in search of his runaway frog. The
book depicts several motion scenes, involving multiple types of Figures (humans and
animals), Paths, and Manners of motion.
Native speakers of a large number of languages have been asked to orally
describe what is happening in each of the pictorial scenes. In the majority of the
cases, participants have been both adults and children. The main outcomes of this
research have been two volumes under the same generic title Relating Events in
Narrative: the first volume edited by Berman & Slobin 1 (1994) and the second by
Strömqvist & Verhoeven 2 (2004).
1
Berman & Slobin’s Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study is the result
of collaborations with Ayhan A. Aksu-Koç, Michael Bamberg, Lisa Dasinger, Virginia Marchman,
Yonni Neeman, Philip Rodkin, Eugenia Sebastián, Cecile Toupin, Tom Trabasso and Christiane von
Stutterheim. Their original interest was in language development, though they also gathered adults’
elicited narratives.
2
Strömqvist & Verhoeven’s Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives
examines American Sign Language (Galvan & Taub) and 13 spoken languages: Arrernte (Davis P.
Wilkins), Greenlandic (Elisabeth Engberg-Pedersen and Frederikke Blytman Trondhjem), English,
Spanish and Hebrew (Slobin and Judy Kupersmitt), Japanese (Aylin C. Küntay and Keiko Nakamura),
Swedish and Icelandic (Hrafnhildur Ragnarsdottir and Sven Strömqvist), Thai (Jordan Zlatev and
Peerapat Yangklang), Turkish (Ayhan Aksu-Koc and Göklem Tekdemir), Tzeltal (Penelope Brown)
Basque (Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano), and Warlpiri (Edith L. Bavin).
59
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
In the first volume of the frog story studies, Berman & Slobin (1994: 198-
199) summed up the typological contrasts found between the narratives of three
verb-framed languages (Hebrew, Turkish and Spanish) and two satellite-framed
languages (English and German):
Satellite-framed languages allow for detailed description of paths within a clause, because
syntax makes it possible to accumulate path satellites to a single verb, along with
prepositional phrases that add further specifications (e.g., the deer threw them off over the
cliff into the water) […] The satellite-framed languages in our sample also tend towards
greater specification of manner, probably because the lexicon provides a large collection of
verbs that conflate manner with change of location (crawl, swoop, tumble, etc), often
conflating cause as well (dump, hurl, shove, etc.). In verb-framed languages, such
elaboration is more of a “luxury,” since path and manner are elaborated in separate
expressions, which are generally optional, and which are less compact in form. As a
consequence of these differences, it seems—at least in our data—that English and German
narrations are characterized by a great deal of dynamic path and manner description, while
Spanish, Hebrew, and Turkish narrations are less elaborated in this regard, but are often
more elaborated in description of locations of protagonists and objects and of endstates of
motion. (Berman & Slobin, 1994: 118-119)
As stated in the quotation, narrative styles in verb- and satellite-framed languages are
quite distinct in terms of dynamism of the events being depicted. Satellite-framed
languages presented more dynamic and lively descriptions of motion events both
with respect to manner of motion and paths, while verb-framed languages tended to
focus on static descriptions of the setting and the protagonist’s endpoint of location.
Applying Ikegami’s (1991) terminology of become-language (i.e., which focuses on
the change from one state into another, such as Japanese) vs. do-language (i.e.,
which focuses on the activity of an individual, such as English), verb-framed
languages might be described as become-languages since they devote more narrative
attention to static descriptions of the setting and tend to express more often the
Figure’s endpoint of location, leaving the Figure’s ways of moving through space
and the trajectories it follows to be inferred. These ways of moving an trajectories, in
contrast, are favoured by satellite-framed languages or do-languages. These
differences in narrative style were unexpected consequences of the two lexicalisation
60
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
patterns posited by Talmy. Importantly, the differing ways of expressing Manner and
Path in verb- and satellite-framed languages have an impact on narrative style which
would not have been observed unless studying language in use.
For the purposes of this dissertation, we are concerned with crosslinguistic
differences in the expression and elaboration of Path and Manner between English
and Spanish. We briefly illustrate these differences with examples of English and
Spanish taken from Berman & Slobin (1994) and Slobin (1996a, 2004). Regarding
the expression of Manner, English elicited narratives displayed a greater token and
type frequency of manner verbs than Spanish elicited narratives, which in turn
contained a higher number of path verbs:
(1) English motion verbs: buck, bump, buzz, carry, chase, climb, come, crawl,
creep, depart, drop, dump, escape, fall, float, fly, follow, get, go, head, hide,
hop, jump, knock, land, leave, limp, make-fall, move, plummet, pop, push,
race, rush, run, slip, splash, splat, sneak, swim, swoop, take, throw, tip,
tumble, walk, wander. (Berman & Slobin, 1994: 153)
61
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
(3) Se cayó
Furthermore, in Spanish narratives just one piece of information about the ground
was usually given, either the source (e.g., from X), the medium (e.g., along, through
X) or goal (e.g., to, towards X). English speakers, in contrast, mentioned more
ground elements per clause. In general, Spanish speakers tended not to express
complex paths with a single verb; when a complex path was described together with
one motion verb in Spanish, the trajectory usually involved the motion from a source
to a goal as in (5). In contrast, in English it is morphosyntactically possible to attach
several path segments to a single verb. Thus, English speakers readily compacted
several trajectories (expressed by satellites) with a single verb as in (6):
Finally, when Spanish speakers described complex paths, they typically did so by
using several verbs and breaking the event into several segments:
(7) El ciervo le llevó hasta un sitio, donde debajo había un río. Entonces el ciervo
tiró al perro y al niño al río. Y después cayeron.
Lit. The deer took him to a place, where below there was a river. Then, the
deer threw the dog and the boy to the river. And after that they fell.
The same patterns observed in oral elicited narratives are widely attested in
novels (e.g., Slobin, 1996a, 1996b, 1997; Özçalişkan & Slobin, 1999, 2003; Mora-
Gutiérrez, 2001), and newspapers across a range of languages (e.g., Slobin, 2003;
Slobin, 2006a). Writers of creative fiction and reporters, though free to make full and
imaginative use of the language, conform to the same patterns found in the frog
62
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
3
For research on the role of inferences in verb-framed languages versus satellite-framed languages,
see Papafragou et al. (2006) for Greek vs. English, Pourcel (2005) and Pourcel & Kopecka
(submitted) for French vs. English.
63
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
within the same typological group display different degrees of elaboration of manner
and path.
Another important outcome from the 2004 volume has been the proposal or
identification of a number of linguistic and non-linguistic factors which interact with
lexicalisation patterns. Slobin (2004) pointed out that the Talmian typology alone
cannot account for language use, and suggested that there are a number of additional
linguistic factors playing a part in the expression of motion. A language provides its
speakers with a range of lexical and morphological means for describing motion
events. It has also been shown than sound symbolic expressions contribute as well to
motion event descriptions in some languages, such as Basque (e.g., Ibarretxe-
Antuñano, 2004b) and Japanese (e.g., Hamano, 1998; Kita, 1997):
a. Basque: plisti-plasti ‘waddle’; taka-taka ‘walk with small and short steps’
4
See also Chapter 2 (Sections 2.3.3 and 2.3.4) for a review of the factors that play a role in a
language’s degree of elaboration of Manner and Path of movement.
64
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
great attention Arrernte and Warlpiri speakers give to path details. Australian
aboriginal culture highlights the importance of journeys as well as locations, and this
is without a doubt reflected in narrative style. Thus, in their frog stories, Arrernte and
Warlpiri speakers overall described scenes with many more path fragments than
speakers of other languages. Moreover, Bavin reported cultural preferences for
repetition of old information together with new information:
Information is repeated with some new added […] telling the frog story, a speaker might
give the information that someone fell, then someone fell to the water, then someone fell
down to the water and then specify that the child and dog fell. (Bavin, 2004: 20)
3.2.3. Translation
Slobin (2003) does not include any hypothesis on the consequences that differing
attention to manner may have in the translation process. However, he opened this
venue of research on translation of written narratives. This line of research addresses
the process of translating between languages which belong to different typological
groups, raising the question of how each language accommodates itself to the
demands of the other. To explore this issue, Slobin and his collaborators used corpus
data from 20th century novels and translations of The Hobbit by Tolkien (1937).
Slobin (1996a, 1997) initially focused on translations between English and
Spanish novels. In general, it was observed that English texts lose more in the
translation process than Spanish texts. With respect to manner information, when
translating from English into Spanish, only 51% of the original English manner
information was maintained in the Spanish target text, while in translations from
Spanish to English, 77% of the original Spanish manner information was kept.
65
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
Moreover, English translators generally added manner descriptions not present in the
original Spanish text. Consider the following examples taken from (Slobin, 1997:
460):
Lit. … after ten minutes of asphyxiation and pushes, (we) arrived at the entry-
way …’
(10) English translation: … after ten minutes of nearly being smothered or crushed
to death, we finally fought our way to the exit …
In the original Spanish text, the verb llegar ‘arrive’ (a path verb) has been translated
into English as fight our way. This expression encodes the difficulty of the motion
toward the entry which is not expressed by the main verb in the original text. This
manner information, however, could be inferred somehow by Spanish readers after
reading diez minutos de asfixia y empujones Lit.‘ten minutes of asphyxiation and
pushes’. Furthermore, it is important to reflect on the fact that English and Spanish
texts also differed in the type of manner information. As Slobin points out in the
quotation below, English possesses a richer and more elaborated manner-of-motion
verb lexicon than Spanish, whose manner verbs are quite general in meaning
compared to those of English.
Languages seem to have a “two-tiered” lexicon of manner verbs: the neutral, everyday verbs
– like walk and fly and climb, and the more expressive or exceptional verbs – like dash and
swoop and scramble. In S-languages [satellite-framed languages, e.g., English], the second
tier is extensive and elaborated, making distinctions that do not play a role in the
considerably smaller second tiers in V-languages [verb-framed languages, e.g., Spanish].
(Slobin, 1997: 459).
The greater diversity of manner verbs in English poses additional problems for
Spanish translators. When translating motion events without boundary crossing, they
may or may not decide to express the manner information conveyed in the English
original. If they opt to do so, Spanish translators tend to compensate lexical gaps by
66
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
67
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
the Spanish translation by including a path verb for each one of the trajectories
expressed by English satellites.
(13) English original: Then I, too, went down the steep twisting path through the
dark woods to the beach below
Lit. Then I, too, took the steep and twisting path that, traversing the dark
woods, descended to the beach.
On the whole, English translators seem to have an easier task than Spanish
translators as in English it is syntactically possible to pack both manner information
and elaborated trajectories (including several grounds) within a single clause. That
might explain why English translators often even include manner and path
information which is not present in the original Spanish text. For Spanish translators,
in contrast, the task is harder as they have to decide what sort of ground, path and
manner information to express in order not to slow down or break the pace of
narration.
More recently, Slobin (2005) has widened his research scope to include
translations from the English novel The Hobbit into nine languages: Dutch, German,
Russian, Serbo-Croatian, French, Portuguese, Italian, Spanish, Hebrew and Turkish.
His research further supports his previous findings regarding translations from
English to Spanish, and generalises them to a larger number of languages. Verb-
framed languages, as has been discussed, apparently are less concerned with manner
of motion than satellite-framed languages, and they break complex paths into several
segments. Overall, when translating from English into verb-framed languages, it
appears that there is a greater loss of both path and manner information than in
translations into satellite-framed languages.
Starting from Slobin’s insights into translations from English to Spanish,
Ibarretxe-Antuñano (2003) compared and analysed in detail the different strategies
that Spanish and Basque translators follow when rendering fragments of Chapter 6
from The Hobbit. She started from the translation strategies described above (Slobin,
68
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
1996a, 1996b, 1997, 2004). However, in the light of her data, she provided a richer
picture of translation strategies, as Table 3.1 illustrates:
MANNER INFORMATION
Strategy M-1 Omission of any manner information
Translation of the same type of manner information (verb or
Strategy M-2
separate expression)
Strategy M-3 Substitution of a manner of motion verb for a path verb
Strategy M-4 Substitution of a manner of motion verb for a motion verb
Strategy M-5 Substitution of a manner of motion verb for any verb
Strategy M-6 Translation of a portion of manner information
Strategy M-7 Translation of a different type of manner information
PATH INFORMATION
Strategy P-1 Omission of some path element
Strategy P-2 Insertion of a new motion verb, usually a path verb
Strategy P-3 Translation of all path information
Table 3.1. Strategies for translating path and manner information in Spanish and Basque (taken
from Ibarretxe-Antuñano, 2003: 165)
Her main contribution is the identification of more available strategies than those
discussed in previous research. It is necessary to take into account the quantity and
type of manner information translated into the target text: whether manner
information is totally or partly rendered, whether translators choose to render manner
verbs by other manner verbs which encode only part of the information (M-6) or
even different types of manner information (M-7), and if totally omitted, which verbs
are used instead (M-3, M-4, M-5).
69
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
gestures fit the speaker’s conceptualisation of the motion event for the purposes of
speaking. Moreover, like sign languages, gesture also encodes spatial information
which is not usually verbalised. We will consider co-speech gestures in Section
3.2.4.2.
Signed languages of course also have resources for describing motion events, some of which
may be unique to the sign modality.
5
See also Talmy (2003) for a comprehensive account on the representation of spatial structure in sign
languages.
70
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
6
See also Taub & Galvan (2000).
71
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
David McNeill and his colleagues (e.g., McNeill, 1992; McNeill & Duncan, 2000;
Taub, Piñar & Galvan, 2002; Özyürek & Kita, 1999; Kita & Özyürek, 2002; Özyürek
et al, 2005) explore the area of nonverbal expression of motion in studies of co-
speech gesturing, i.e., gestures accompanying speech. One of the methods they use is
to show participants a cartoon movie (Canary Row, with Tweety and Sylvester) and
ask for a subsequent narration, which is videotaped for later analysis. Their research
addresses the question of whether gestural descriptions of motion events reflect
cross-linguistic differences in lexicalisation patterns. On the whole, distinctly
different patterns of gesture in speakers of English, Spanish, Turkish and Japanese
were found.
McNeill (2000) and McNeill & Duncan (2000) reported distinctly different
behaviour for English and Spanish speakers’ co-speech gestures. Their studies
suggested that speakers of Spanish commonly conflated Manner and Path in their
gestures. Although Manner was often omitted in their verbal descriptions, Manner
was abundant in their gestures and was distributed over path phrases given in speech.
In other words, manner gestures were present throughout the utterance. Such
distribution of manner gestures over verbalised path expressions was termed manner
fog. Since Manner was not usually verbalised in speech but was frequently gestured,
they concluded that gestural descriptions by Spanish speakers served a compensatory
function.
Taub, Piñar & Galvan (2002) also focused on English and Spanish co-speech
gestures. Though their results were still preliminary, it seemed that English speakers
gave greater attention to Manner both lexically and gesturally than did Spanish
speakers. In line with their verbal descriptions, gestures by English speakers
expressed both manner and path information whereas gestures by Spanish speakers
generally downplayed manner details. These results might be interpreted as if
gestures only encode what is expressed in speech, and thus, gesture is tightly tied to
language 7 .
7
The interested reader can find a summary of competing theories of gesture production in Kita &
Özyürek (2002).
72
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
Kita & Özyürek (2002) and Özyürek et al (2005) found that gestures that
were used to express the same motion events in English, Japanese and Turkish were
influenced simultaneously by (a) how conceptual elements were expressed in each
language, and by (b) spatial information in the stimulus (i.e., in the Tweety and
Sylvester cartoon movie) which was never verbalised. On the one hand, their results
showed that linguistic differences in the expression of motion were reflected in
gesture. Therefore, a semantic feature such as Manner of motion, which is difficult to
verbalise in verb-framed language, is less likely to be gestured by Japanese and
Turkish speakers than by English speakers. Furthermore, if a language can compact
or package manner and path information as English does, then, it is more likely that a
single gesture will be used instead of two separated gestures (as was the case for
Japanese and Turkish speakers). Finally, it was also found that in cases where there
were no cross-linguistic differences in speech, the gesture patterns looked alike. On
the other hand, gestural representation was also found to be influenced by spatial
information of the stimulus. Speakers of English, Japanese and Turkish regularly
gestured certain spatial details which were not verbally expressed in the concurrent
speech (e.g., movement from right to left, which was not considered as relevant for
linguistic description). This sort of information is typically encoded in sign
languages.
In light of the existing research on co-speech gesture, it might be concluded
that data on gestures in Spanish is not yet conclusive. It might well be the case that
manner gestures in Spanish could provide a sort of compensation for gaps in the
lexicon, but quantitative data is needed before drawing such a conclusion. On the
other hand, research on English, Japanese and Turkish, however, conclude that
‘gestures do not merely encode imagistic representations of the events but those
aspects that fit conceptualization of the event for the purposes of speaking’ (Özyürek
et al., 2005: 224).
73
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
[L]anguage properties influence how children select or organize spatial information during
the course of development.
8
The reader interested in second language acquisition can see, for example, Hohenstein, J. Eisenberg,
A. & Naigles, L. (2006) which concentrates on cross-influence of L1 and L2 in Spanish-English
bilinguals in the linguistic expression of motion events.
9
For universalist accounts of spatial language, see, for example, Landau & Jackendoff (1993). For
experimental research suggesting general cognitive principles in language acquisition of spatial terms,
see for example, Munnich & Landau (2003), Helpos & Spelke’s (2004) replication of McDonough,
Choi & Mandler (2003) and Landau & Hoffman (2005).
74
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
percentage of tokens and types of manner verbs than children speaking verb-framed
languages, who tend to use path verbs instead. As Slobin (2003) hypothesised,
manner-of-motion verbs are acquired very early by children learning satellite-framed
languages. Furthermore, children speaking satellite-framed languages use motion
verbs with some path elaboration, whereas such descriptions are hardly used by
children speaking verb-framed languages. It is also noted that older children are
closer to the adult system than younger children. As age increases, they are capable
of providing more detailed descriptions of the motion event in line with the adult
system. Verb-framed-language speaking children are more concerned with setting
the scene and asserting changes of location (results) than satellite-framed-language
speaking children, who assert trajectories and attend to Manner of motion (actions).
Kyung-ju Oh (2003) elicited verbal descriptions from 3-year-old Korean-
speaking and English-speaking children using clips depicting everyday motion by
humans. In line with previous findings, she also found that children are influenced
very early by the typological properties of their native language. Korean-speaking
children produced manner verbs less frequently and path verbs more frequently than
English-speaking children. Furthermore, Korean children produced descriptions
encoding Path but not Manner more frequently than English children, who produced
verbal descriptions encoding both Manner and Path. On the other hand, some
common characteristics were noted between Korean-speaking and English-speaking
children. At an early age, both groups rarely expressed Path and Manner together in
their descriptions, though English children did so more frequently. Oh speculated
that at an early age children might be too limited cognitively to attend to various
components of a motion event at the same time, though another possibility might be
that they have not yet mastered the skill to pack both manner and path information
into a single description. In addition, it was found that children from both language
groups seemed to be more biased toward Manner than toward Path, and that they
tended to produce coarse manner of motion verbs (such as walk, or run).
Maya Hickman (2003, 2006) used animated cartoons on a computer screen to
elicit verbal descriptions. Her results showed that typological properties of English
and French as satellite-framed and verb-framed languages strongly influenced how
children (three- and five-year-olds) talked about motion. In line with previous
75
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
findings, she noted that English- and French-speaking children used more complex
descriptions of both Path and Manner together with increasing age.
In terms of spontaneous or naturalistic conversations, Selimis & Katis (2003)
showed that Greek- and English-speaking children followed the typological patterns
of their mother tongue in preferring verbs which lexicalise either Path (for Greek) or
Manner (for English). Moreover, it was found that these differences appeared even
earlier than two years of age, but then diminished with age as the manner verb
lexicon in Greek children got richer in later development.
All in all, these studies provide linguistic data suggesting that children adopt
language-specific characteristics from a very early age. However, some
characteristics seem to be mastered early, while others develop more slowly.
There is abundant research which has asked whether there are psychologically real
effects of language-specific semantic patterns on how we process language. Some
studies suggest that language-specific verb semantics (i.e., whether languages
typically code either Path or Manner in their motion verbs) seems to have a pervasive
influence on how speakers interpret the meaning of novel verbs, as well as on
speakers’ performance on other linguistic tasks (such as verb generation, etc).
Naigles & Terrazas (1998) found that adult English and Spanish speakers
differed in their expectations as to whether a novel verb encoded Manner or Path, the
former favouring Manner and the latter Path. Furthermore, in their experiment,
participants adjusted their assumptions about whether a novel verb expressed Manner
or Path based on the syntactic frame in which the verb was presented. Naigles &
Terrazas showed English and Spanish participants a video clip of a motion event—
e.g., a woman skipping towards a tree—and gave them a single sentence containing a
novel verb: e.g., She’s kradding the tree! or She’s kradding towards the tree!. They
used two kinds of frames: path frames containing a verb plus a noun phrase, and
manner frames containing a verb plus a preposition plus a noun phrase. Then,
participants were shown two further videos on different screens, one preserving the
Manner of the event but changing the Path (e.g., a woman skipping away from the
tree) and one preserving the path of the event but changing the manner (e.g., a
76
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
woman marching towards the tree). Participants’ task was to indicate which video
depicted the novel verb. On the whole, it was found that adult speakers of English
chose the video that preserved the Manner rather than the one that preserved Path,
and the reverse was true for Spanish. Furthermore, they found effects of syntactic
frame on the interpretation of the novel verbs; participants were more likely to
choose the manner video if they had heard the novel verb in a manner frame, and
more likely to choose the path video if they had heard the verb in a path frame.
Along the same lines, Cifuentes-Férez & Gentner (2006) tested whether
English and Spanish speakers would show effects of their differing semantic systems
when inferring the meanings of novel motion verbs in brief written narratives. They
presented English and Spanish monolingual speakers with eight short passages
containing either a novel noun or a novel verb. After each passage participants were
asked ‘What does X mean?’ or ‘What is an X?’. In order to prevent people from
simply translating the novel words into existing words, the passages described
unusual events – e.g., rolling a device designed to remove burrs over one’s clothes;
moving across a hall using cleaning-rags underneath one’s shoes, etc. The
descriptions of the events always included both a path and a manner, so that
participants could lexicalise either or both. As predicted, Spanish speakers produced
a significantly higher number of path verb interpretations than English speakers.
Conversely, English speakers produced more manner verb interpretations than
Spanish speakers. These results were obtained despite the use of syntactic frames that
biased them against the predicted patterns. Spanish speakers were given frames that
included a preposition, thus encouraging interpretation in terms of a manner verb.
English speakers were given bare transitive frames—which would normally invoke a
path interpretation—yet the English speakers still preferred manner verb
interpretations. All in all, English and Spanish speakers were guided by their
differing semantic systems in inferring the meanings of novel motion verbs from
context. Their specific linguistic systems influenced which features they extracted
from context as semantic components of the novel verbs.
Apart from cross-linguistic studies examining the pervasive influence of
language-specific semantic patterns on language processing, we also find research
addressing the nature of those effects within a single language. A good instance is
77
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
Billman & Krych (1998), which addressed how memory for motion events presented
in video clips might be influenced by linguistic information (an accompanying verb).
They presented English speaking participants with videotaped events accompanied
by either manner or path verbs. Then, participants returned for a visual recognition
test in which no verbs were presented. The task was to discriminate the old items
from new items with changed Manner or Path of motion. Their results showed that
the presence of either a path verb or a manner verb altered the participant’s
recognition of events. They found an effect of the type of verb initially given by the
experimenter on the type of recognition errors. More concretely, hearing a path verb
(as opposed to a manner verb) made participants more likely to correctly reject a
changed Path, and hearing a manner verb helped participants to reject changed
Manner.
In a later study, Billman, Swilley & Krych (2000) also looked for effects of
the type of verbs present at encoding (path or manner verbs) on later recognition.
However, the language manipulation was more indirect; in this experiment path and
manner verbs were not given by the experimenter but produced by participants.
Participants were asked to generate a verb to describe a set of video clips, and on the
next day, their task was to discriminate old clips from new clips. In line with their
previous research, they found that the type of verb generated by participants had an
effect on later event recognition. On the whole, these studies suggest that when a
person hears or produces a verb, distinctions in verb meaning (path and manner) bias
recognition. Using a particular type of verb channels the speaker’s attention to some
properties of the event while downplaying others.
Finally, Havasi & Snedecker (2004) argued that even though language-
specific semantic patterns are learned while acquiring one’s native language, both
adults and children could be trained to develop either a path or manner bias. They
taught English-speaking adults twelve new motion verbs. For each motion verb,
participants saw an ambiguous scene with a salient Path and Manner, and after
obtaining their initial interpretation, they saw five further instances of the new verb
which disambiguated its meaning. Half the participants saw video clips with the
same Manner but varying paths (which favoured a manner interpretation), and the
other half saw video clips with the same path but varying manner (which favoured a
78
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
path interpretation). Finally, participants were asked to extend the new verb. On the
whole, it was found that English-speaking adults have an initial manner bias, as
shown by their initial interpretations. However, in response to the input, they were
highly like to extend the new verb according to the common semantic pattern they
had experienced across the five exemplars. Moreover, there was preliminary
evidence that five-year-olds were sensitive to this manipulation as well; they
developed either a manner bias or a path bias in their novel word interpretations
based on which series of videos they had received. This suggests that the semantic
patterns of a language act as default biases, not as fixed rules; they can be overridden
by experience.
Some scholars have pointed out that motion language is used metaphorically in a
wide array of abstract domains. For example, Narayanan (1997) remarked that bodily
movement serves as a metaphor for political and economic events: prices can drift,
soar, lurch or plunge; Özçalişkan (2002, 2004) pointed out that manner-of-motion
verbs are used for expressing basic emotional states: run into frustration, his mind
plunges in an awkward thought. This section does not deal with those abstract
domains but concentrates instead on the relation between motion and vision.
In a highly suggestive paper, Slobin (in press) explored the relations between
paths of motion and paths of vision in English, Russian, Spanish and Turkish. In both
English and Russian there are complex visual paths such as look from X to Y, look
past X into Y and look out into X. In contrast, Spanish and Turkish generally express
a single visual trajectory (e.g., look into X, look through X, etc), though sometimes
the complex trajectory from X to Y is found. Even though visual path verbs are not
available in most languages 10 , Slobin concluded that the patterns for conceptualising
10
As Slobin (in press) noted, Toba is the only documented language that has a set of vision verbs
which also encode path information (Klein, 1981). Toba is a Guaykuruan language spoken in
Argentine Chaco whose vision verbs express the direction of vision in a similar fashion to motion
verbs. For example, wa ‘look inward’, la ‘look ahead’, sa:t ‘look up at something moving’. According
to Klein (1981: 234), ‘the notion of direction of the eyes appears to be equivalent in the mind of the
native speaker to the notion of direction of a person’s legs across space’.
79
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
and expressing physical paths carry over into the conceptualisation and expression of
visual paths. Similar conclusions are reached in Chu (2003), who analysed visual
path expressions in Mandarin Chinese and suggested that this serial verb language
uses many physical path expressions with verbs of seeing.
Cifuentes-Férez (2006) contributed to this preliminary research on the
relationship between motion and vision as well as to the research on narrative
translations from satellite-framed into verb-framed languages. For her research, she
chose the well-known English novel Harry Potter and the Order of the Phoenix and
its translation into Spanish, Harry Potter y la Orden del Fénix. Her main aim was to
investigate whether the crosslinguistic differences between English and Spanish
which had been reported for the domain of motion also applied to the domain of
vision. The basic assumption was that motion and vision are related domains at least
in two respects: (a) seeing is a type of fictive motion 11 (e.g., Talmy, 2000a: Chapter
2), and (b) verbs of seeing appear in the same kind of constructions as verbs of
motion (e.g., Gruber, 1967; Talmy, 2000a; Slobin, in press). Unlike Slobin (in press),
who just focused on the relations between paths of motion and paths of vision,
Cifuentes-Férez centred her research attention on both Path and Manner in both
domains; she compared and contrasted the degree of manner and path elaboration per
domain and language and she examined as well the translation strategies that the
Spanish translator resorted to in order to adapt the English source text to the Spanish
target text.
In terms of Manner of motion, her results were consistent with previous
research, and went a step further in unveiling some crosslinguistic differences.
English and Spanish differed in (a) which morphosyntactic constituents typically
encoded manner information, (b) the semantic specificity of their manner verbs, and
(c) the function that adjuncts encoding Manner had. As noted previously, the English
manner-of-motion verb lexicon is richer and presents finer manner distinctions than
does Spanish. It was found that Spanish motion verbs did not usually encode manner
information about the Figure’s physical or mental state (whether s/he is tired or
11
Fictive motion refers to those events which are linguistically expressed as if actual motion occurs.
Other labels for the term fictive motion are Langacker’s abstract motion (1987), and Matsumoto’s
subjective motion (1996).
80
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
angry, or s/he walks unsteadily, etc) and that the translator usually expressed this
information in adjuncts to compensate for lexical gaps (i.e., a compensatory
function). In the original English narrative text, in contrast, manner information
outside the verb root had an augmentative function as it further enriched the manner
information conveyed in manner verbs. With regard to manner of vision, it was
concluded that the English text used manner-of-vision verbs that were more specific
(i.e., richer semantically) than those of Spanish. As in the domain of motion, adjunct
expressions seemed to make up for lexical gaps (e.g., glare was translated by a
general vision verb plus an adverbial mirar con hostilidad ‘look with hostility’).
Furthermore, as Slobin (in press) had already pointed out, Spanish texts used
nominals (e.g., ojos ‘eyes’, mirada ‘look’, etc as in lanzar miradas desafiantes
‘throw challenging looks’) to describe visual acts much more often than the English
text. It seemed that the Spanish translator resorted to these linguistic means to
compensate for the lack of Spanish manner-of-vision verbs encoding such fine-
grained information. On the whole, it was observed that there was a substantial loss
of manner information in the Spanish translation: 36.46% of cases for manner-of-
motion verbs and 38.46% of cases for manner-of-vision verbs were lost in
translation. Despite the available linguistic means, manner-of-motion and manner-of-
vision in the original English text were not fully conveyed in the Spanish translation;
on many occasions, manner information was not as specific as in the English
original, and it was even translated by different manners of movement and of seeing.
When examining Path of motion, the crosslinguistic differences reported in
previous research were further attested. In the Spanish texts, the use of path verbs
was significantly higher, and Spanish descriptions of Path were less complex and
contained fewer references to the Ground than those in the English original. English
presented a greater flexibility in the sort of path and ground combinations than
Spanish, which generally only expressed one trajectory plus a ground (e.g., en X
‘in/into X’; de X ‘from X’; hacia X ‘towards X’). The most complex path segment
found in the Spanish translation was por X hacia Y ‘through X to Y’, whereas the
most complex trajectory in the English original was down X, past Y, and down into Z
(four satellites plus 3 grounds). With regard to visual paths, no vision verbs in
English and Spanish expressing Path or trajectory were found (Slobin, in press).
81
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
Visual trajectories were much less complex than physical ones and tended to mention
only one ground (i.e., the observed entity); yet, English visual paths were more
elaborated than Spanish visual paths. Regarding the translation strategies, it was
found that the Spanish translation kept path information in 51.61% of cases for
motion and in 50% of cases for vision, mostly by incorporating path verbs. However,
some trajectories were frequently omitted in the translation, particularly, paths
without grounds, such as deictic paths (e.g., around, back) and vertical paths (e.g.,
up, down).
Although examinations of original Spanish narrative texts would be needed
before we could draw sound conclusions on how Spanish expresses visual acts, this
preliminary exploration of the relations between motion and vision suggests that the
crosslinguistic differences reported for motion descriptions in narratives apply also to
the expression of visual acts. English, as a satellite-framed language, seems to have a
rich manner lexicon of verbs of seeing, and can readily compact several paths with a
single verb. In contrast, Spanish, as a verb-framed language, generally expresses a
single visual path: to/towards the entity being perceived (or Ground in Talmy’s
terminology), and their vision verbs are less specific semantically than those of
English. In sum, these differences reflect how language-specific ways of speaking
about motion influence how we talk about vision, and suggest that vision may be
conceptualised in terms of motion.
3.3.1. Introduction
The domain of motion has received special attention in linguistic relativity research.
According to Pourcel (2002: 127), motion events are an ideal arena for investigating
Whorfian effects for a number of reasons: first, languages differ in how they carve up
the domain of motion. Second, motion events are referred to in language with a high
level of frequency, and they are representative of actual language use. And third,
unlike the domain of colour, in this domain there are no ‘biologically-determined
concepts waiting to be labelled’ (Slobin, 2000: 122).
82
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
83
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
information about the setting and the protagonist’s inner state allowing inferences of
manner of motion. His findings showed elaborated mental images for Manner in
English reports and less vivid or poorly detailed mental images for manner in
Spanish. Interestingly, bilinguals behaved according to the text’s language: if the text
was in English, and therefore, they were asked to provide reports in English, their
mental images for manner were significantly richer than when the text and the task
were in Spanish.
Kersten et al. (2003) showed that greater attention to Manner of motion by
English speakers relative to Spanish speakers is revealed as well in learning tasks.
English and Spanish monolingual participants viewed animated cartoons in which
bug-like creatures moved along various non-nameable paths in various non-nameable
manners. They were told that those creatures belonged to four different species and
they had to guess which by pushing one of four buttons. After each choice they were
told if they had guessed correctly or not. On the whole, no differences in how long
they took to learn to distinguish the bug-like creatures in terms of Path was observed,
but English speakers were significantly faster in learning to distinguish them in terms
of Manner despite the fact none of these manners of motion could be lexicalised in
English manner verbs. Furthermore, they also tested bilinguals in Spanish and in
English, and their results were similar to those found with monolingual speakers.
They concluded that the observed differences between the two groups regarding
learning Manner of motion concepts were a result of the different languages they
spoke. In sum, people learn to attend to the sorts of event details that are encoded in
their language.
Kyung-ju Oh (2003) reported differences in recall of manner details between
English and Korean monolingual speakers. She devised an experiment in which the
initial task of the participants was to view and describe some video clips. The clips
included several manners of motion (e.g., walking, striding, jogging, strolling,
trudging, sprinting, etc) as well as several paths (e.g., out of, up, down, around,
along, etc). Also, some filler clips which did not depict motion or displacement were
included (cooking in a kitchen and working with a computer). As predicted for this
task, English speakers provided more tokens and types of manner verbs and extended
manner descriptions than Korean speakers. The two language groups differed
84
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
85
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
86
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
dissociable, but language made available in the experimental context may mediate
the speaker’s performance in other tasks.
Papafragou et al. (2002) conducted a similar study to Gennari et al., using
English and Greek (a verb-framed language). Their participants were both children
and adults. The tasks included (a) verbal descriptions and recognition memory of
static pictures adapted from the frog story, and (b) similarity judgments using triads
of static human motion pictures. They found that the two language groups differed in
terms of their linguistic descriptions, but their performance in the two non-linguistic
tasks was similar. Unlike Gennari et al. (2002), they did not find a facilitating effect
of verbal encoding in later non-linguistic tasks. Taken together, however, these two
studies are consistent with the Universalist approach to language and cognition:
cross-linguistic variability in the expression of motion does not reflect any
substantive differences in the ways humans think about motion.
Zlatev & David (2004, 2005) focussed on French and Swedish (a satellite-
framed language). They used sets of triads showing a smiling tomato man 12
performing the motion scenes, which varied in terms of Manner (rolling, spinning,
sliding and jumping) and Path (e.g., from left to right, from right to left). As in
previous studies, participants saw triads and were asked to choose which alternate is
most similar to the target. Their results showed an overall preference for Manner
across languages. Unlike the research we have just reviewed, which concluded that
there was no a preference for either motion dimension over the other, Zlatev &
David’s results showed a significant preference for the dimension of Manner.
Pourcel (2004a, 2004b) neither supports nor disproves linguistic relativity for the
domain of motion. However, her results are noteworthy as they show a correlation
between participants’ responses and the nature of the stimuli rather than between
responses and linguistic motion typologies.
12
Tomato man is a virtual elicitation tool developed at the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
(Nijmegen, NL).
87
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
88
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
3.5. Summary
Talmy’s binary typology has engendered a good deal of research and debate in the
literature on motion event descriptions over the last two decades. Slobin’s
hypotheses of thinking-for-speaking encompasses several lines of research in the
attempt to account for the pervasive effects that language use has on mental
processes tied to language.
Talmy’s typology provides valuable insights into lexicalisation patterns for
motion in language, but lexicalisation patterns alone cannot account for how
language is used in discourse. It has been shown that those lexicalisation patterns
have an impact on narrative or rhetorical style: English speakers seem to devote more
narrative attention to the dynamics of movement, in line with the availability of
manner verbs, which can be followed by satellites and locative prepositional phrases
to describe elaborated trajectories. Spanish speakers, in contrast, seem to be
constrained by their language to devote less narrative attention to actions and more
attention to static scenes instead. Moreover, in addition to these typological factors, a
number of non-linguistic factors, including cognitive and cultural factors have been
shown to play a part in the linguistic expression of motion.
Research on translation, reviewed in Section 3.2.3, has shown that differences
in lexicalisation patterns pose a great number of problems in terms of the expression
of both trajectories and manners of motion to translators.
Research on sign languages and co-speech gestures has also been reviewed.
Though sign languages provide richer conceptual information in their descriptions of
89
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
motion events than spoken languages, similarities between them exist as well. Both
modalities share some basic conceptual elements (Figure, Ground, Path, Manner,
etc), which are presented sequentially rather than simultaneously. In addition, in both
types of languages, a spatial situation can often be conceptualised in more than one
way. Moreover, like verb-framed languages, sign languages seem to be sensitive to
boundary-crossings. Relatedly, research on co-speech gestures concludes that
gestures, which are used to express the same motion events in different languages,
are influenced simultaneously by (a) how conceptual elements are expressed in each
language, and by (b) spatial information in the stimulus. In other words, gestures
reflect cross-linguistic differences but, irrespective of language, speakers also include
in their gestural descriptions other spatial properties of the motion event which are
not verbally encoded. In sum, due to the intrinsic iconic nature of signing and
gesturing some spatial details tend to be expressed which are normally ignored in the
verbal descriptions of motion events.
Taken as a whole, linguistic research inspired by the Talmian typology
documents habitual ways of speaking and writing about motion which have served as
foundations for investigating (a) how children learn language-specific patterns, (b)
the effects of those patterns on language processing, and (c) whether the effects go
beyond thinking for the purposes to use language.
Section 3.2.5 has concentrated on child language acquisition research. A
number of studies have concluded that children are influenced very early by the
typological properties of their languages. Verb-framed-language speaking children
are more concerned with setting the scene and asserting changes of location (results)
than satellite-framed-language speaking children, who assert trajectories and attend
to manners of motion (actions). Irrespective of language, it has also been shown that
older children are closer to the adult system than younger children. At an early age,
children rarely express Path and Manner together in their descriptions, though
English children do so more frequently than verb-framed-language speaking
children. With increased age, children are capable of providing more detailed
descriptions of motion events in line with the adult system. On the whole, it might be
concluded children are trained to think-for-speaking in terms of their mother tongue
acquiring some language-specific properties earlier than others.
90
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
91
CHAPTER 3. BEYOND LINGUISTIC TYPOLOGIES: CROSSLINGUISTIC STUDIES ON MOTION EVENTS
92
CHAPTER 4
4.1. Introduction
4.5.1. Introduction
4.5.2. Manner verbs
4.5.3. Path verbs
4.6. Summary
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
4.1. Introduction
The first aim of this chapter is to briefly review classifications and analyses of
motion verbs from different theoretical backgrounds. Although this dissertation is not
based on any of these theories or approaches, their insights into motion verbs are of
valuable interest to better understand the complex syntactic and semantic nature of
motion verbs.
Section 4.2 begins with an overview of the Syntax-Semantics Interface
approach to the analysis of motion verbs, which explores how a verb’s semantics can
determine the syntactic realisation of its arguments. Scholars within this approach
seek to both identify the semantic notions which are relevant for syntactic structure
(i.e., which semantic properties of verbs are syntactically reflected) and to put
forward motion verb classes in English and Spanish in terms of their semantic and
syntactic properties. In section 4.3, two classifications of English motion verbs
within the Functional-Lexematic model are presented. Though also concerned with
the syntactic properties of motion verbs, scholars working within this model adopt a
more semantically-oriented approach to the organisation of the motion verb lexicon.
Section 4.4 offers a brief overview of some of the existing online databases for
English and Spanish verb lexicons. In these databases, various classifications of
motion verbs can be found, ranging from more syntactically-oriented to more
semantically-oriented classifications.
The second aim of this chapter is to offer a list of fine-grained semantic
notions which have proven useful for exploring crosslinguistic differences and
similarities in the motion verb lexicon. As we presented in Chapter 2 (Section 2.2.3),
Talmy’s three-way typology proposed a classification of languages based on the sort
of semantic information which their verb roots characteristically encode; thus, he
identified three major types of motion verbs: Path-conflating verbs, Manner-
conflating verbs, and Figure-conflating verbs. However, motion verbs might be
further analysed in terms of more specific manner details (e.g., rate or speed of
motion, means of conveyance or vehicle, etc) and path details (e.g., horizontal axis,
vertical axis, motion away from/towards, etc). Section 4.5 presents the semantics of
motion in a more fine-grained fashion than Talmy’s general components. In addition,
crosslinguistic research on manner and path verbs is briefly reviewed; this research
94
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
4.2.1. Introduction
Many theories have been built on the assumption that the syntactic realisation of
arguments is largely predictable from the meanings of verbs. The fact that verbs with
similar meanings show characteristic argument realisation patterns suggests that
these patterns can be attributed to the semantic properties of each class. The main
goal of theories concerned with the close relation between syntax and semantics is to
identify the relevant components of meaning as well as to explicate their connection
to the range of argument realisation options (Levin & Rappaport Hovav, 2005: 3). In
the present work, these theories or approaches are grouped under the rubric/label The
Syntax-Semantics Interface.
Motion verbs are a problematic verb class for research on the relation
between syntax and semantics since they do not seem to behave syntactically as a
coherent semantic class. Across languages, it is observed that syntactic subjects of
some motion verbs share some properties with the direct objects of transitive verbs
(i.e., they act as Patients or Themes), whereas subjects of other motion verbs act as
Agents.
Perlmutter (1978) formulated The Unaccusative Hypothesis, by which two
types of intransitive verbs associated with two different syntactic configurations were
proposed: unacussative and unergative verbs. The former are intransitive verbs
whose syntactic subjects are not semantic Agents; whereas the latter are intransitive
verbs whose syntactic subjects act as Agents. Accordingly, intransitive motion verbs
can be further divided into unaccusative and unergative verbs.
Levin & Rappaport Hovav (1992: 252) suggested that motion verbs would be
better characterised in terms of which components of meaning are lexicalised in the
verb itself, rather than in terms of the thematic roles of the verb’s arguments –as the
95
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
Unaccusative Hypothesis claims. In other words, they argued for a closer look at
what components of verb meaning are relevant for the unaccusative-unergative
distinction. Overall, these authors concluded that the meaning components of
direction (Talmy’s Path), manner (Talmy’s Manner) and direct external cause
(Talmy’s Cause) have proven to be useful for the lexical semantic representation of
motion verbs, as they seem to account for the different syntactic realisations of
motion verbs.
Among the different classifications of motion verbs from the point of view of the
Unaccusative Hypothesis and/or the Syntax-Semantics Interface, we are especially
interested in the contributions of Levin & Rappaport-Hovav (1992) and Levin
(1993), for English motion verbs, and Cifuentes-Honrubia (1999), De Miguel (1999)
and Morimoto (2001) for Spanish motion verbs.
Levin & Rappaport-Hovav (1992: 252-253) proposed three main groups of
intransitive motion verbs:
a. Arrive class: arrive, come, go, depart, fall, return, etc. These verbs are
unaccusative and express inherent direction, as their meaning includes a specified
direction of motion.
b. Roll class: roll, slide, move, swing, spin, rotate, etc. These are unaccusative verbs
encoding Manner of motion as well as direct external cause, that is, the action
denoted by the verb is directly caused by some external agent or force. As pointed
out by the authors, one interesting property of manner verbs with direct external
cause is that they allow transitive (i.e., causative, agentive) and intransitive uses. For
example, Andrew rolled Bill down the hill / Bill rolled down the hill. This alternation
seems to be found in many languages (Hale & Keyser, 1987, 1992).
c. Run class: run, walk, gallop, jump, hop, skip, swim, etc. These verbs express
Manner of motion but lack of direct external cause (i.e., the action denoted happens
96
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
Beth Levin, in her 1993 book English Verb Classes and Alternations: A
preliminary investigation, dealt with transitive motion as well. Her study was also
driven by the hypothesis that a verb’s meaning influences its syntactic behaviour.
She showed how identifying verbs with similar syntactic alternations provides an
effective way for distinguishing semantically coherent verb classes. With regard to
transitive and intransitive motion verbs, Levin (1993: 263-270) proposed the
following nine verb classes:
a. Inherently directed motion: advance, arrive, ascend, come, depart, descend, enter,
escape, exit, fall, flee, go, leave, plunge, recede, return, rise, tumble. The meanings
of these verb include a specification of the direction of motion (i.e., Path), even in the
absence of a directional complement. According to Levin, none of these verbs
specifies the Manner of motion. However, the members of this class do not behave
uniformly in all respects. Verbs differ as to how they can express the goal, source, or
trajectory of motion; depending on the verb, these may be expressed in a
prepositional phrase, as a direct object, or both ways as shown in (1).
b. Leave verbs: abandon, desert, leave, etc. These verbs do not specify Manner of
motion; they just indicate that motion away from a location has taken place. The
direct object of these verbs is understood to be the location that has been left. The
location cannot be expressed in a prepositional phrase.
c. Roll verbs: bounce, drift, drop, float, glide, move, roll, slide, swing, etc, plus verbs
describing motion around an axis: coil, revolve, rotate, spin, turn, twirl, twist, whirl,
wind, etc. This verb class specifies manners of motion characteristic of inanimate
97
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
entities, that is, the Figure does not necessarily control its motion. In the absence of a
prepositional phrase, none of these verbs indicates the Path of motion. Levin noted
that many of the roll verbs that describe motion around an axis take a rather restricted
range of prepositions heading the prepositional phrase that describes the path of
motion, as in (3). Most verbs in this class show the Causative/inchoative alternation
only if the motion is externally controllable as in (4).
(3) a. The ball rolled down the hill/over the hill/into the gutter.
d. Run verbs: amble, backpack, bolt, bounce, bound, bowl, canter, cavort, charge,
climb, crawl, dart, dash, float, fly, gallop, goosestep, hike, hop, jump, march, mosey,
promenade, race, run, shuffle, sleepwalk, somersault, stroll swim, tiptoe, vault,
waddle, walk, etc. Most of these verbs describe manners in which animate entities
can move, although some of them may also be used to describe the movement of
inanimate entities. Further, no specific direction of motion is implied unless they
occur with an explicit directional phrase. Levin (1993: 267) pointed out that Run
verbs would probably need to be further subdivided as their syntactic behaviour is
not uniform. For example, as can be seen in (5), some Run verbs are found in
resultative constructions whereas others are not.
e. Verbs that are Vehicle names 1 : balloon, bicycle, bike, boat, bobsled, bus, cab,
canoe, caravan, chariot, coach, cycle, dogsled, ferry, gondola, helicopter, jeep, jet,
motorbike, parachute, raft, rocket, skate, ski, tram, yacht, etc. These verbs mean
roughly ‘go using the vehicle named by the noun’. In principle, it should be possible
1
For a detailed discussion see Clark & Clark (1979). In their paper titled When nouns surface as
verbs, they explore different groups of English nouns which have come to be used as verbs over the
course of time.
98
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
for any vehicle name to be used as a verb of this type. Finally, this verb class does
not imply direction of motion unless there is an explicit directional phrase present.
f. Verbs that are not vehicle names: cruise, drive, fly, oar, paddle, pedal, ride, row,
sail, tack, etc. This group of verbs denotes motion using a vehicle but the vehicle
name does not coincide with the verb. For instance, fly implies an aircraft, pedal a
bike; cruise, row, sail and tack a ship or boat, etc. Similarly to the previous verb
class, no specific direction of motion is implied unless an explicit directional phrase
is present.
g. Waltz verbs: boogie, bop, cancan, clog, conga, dance, foxtrot, tango, tapdance,
waltz, etc. These verbs mean roughly ‘perform the dance’. No specific direction of
motion is implied unless there is an explicit directional phrase present. Like vehicle
nouns, it appears that any dance noun can be used as motion verb.
h. Accompany verbs: accompany, conduct, escort, guide, lead, shepherd, etc. These
verbs relate to one person taking another from one place to another. According to
Levin, these verbs are differentiated semantically by the nature of the relationship
between the two participants.
i. Chase verbs: chase, follow, pursue, shadow, tail, track, trail, etc. These verbs are
typically transitive, with the chaser as subject and the person being chased as object.
Some of them allow an intransitive use, with the chaser as subject and a prepositional
phrase headed by after expressing what is being chased.
99
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
a. Caminar ‘walk’ class, which includes verbs such as correr ‘run’, gatear ‘crawl’,
nadar ‘swim’ and volar ‘fly’. This group refers to ways of moving which imply, but
do not specify, a change of location. To put it differently, the Figure (in Talmy’s
terminology) necessarily goes somewhere when performing these activities. Thus,
this group of verbs denotes translational motion 3 .
b. Tambalearse ‘totter, stagger’ class, which includes verbs such as agitarse ‘sway
onself’, balancearse ‘swing oneself’and temblar ‘shiver, tremble’. This group refers
to motion which is internal to the Figure; in other words, change of location is
absent. This group of verbs denotes self-contained motion.
2
Cifuentes-Honrubia (1999) is not interested in the division of intransitive verbs into unaccusative
and unergative verbs; he argues that all Spanish intransitive motion verbs are agentive, and thus, such
a division is irrelevant. However, in much the same way as in the research on the Syntax-Semantics
Interface, Cifuentes-Honrubia is concerned with identifying all the relevant semantic distinctions
among motion verbs which are reflected in syntax.
3
See Chapter 2, Section 2.2.2 for a definition of translational and self-contained motion.
100
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
verbs of inherent direction can be further subdivided into two groups, since these
verbs display syntactic differences 4 :
a. Inherently achieved location verbs: caer ‘fall’, bajar ‘go down’, subir ‘ascend’,
entrar ‘enter’, salir ‘exit’, llegar ‘arrive’, etc. This group of verbs denotes inherent
direction with an endpoint; the Figure reaches its final destination or goal.
b. Inherent path verbs: dirigirse ‘go towards’, acercarse, arrimarse ‘go closer to’,
irse ‘go’, moverse ‘move oneself’, regresar ‘come back’, etc. This group denotes
inherent direction without an endpoint; thus, though the goal or final destination is
usually expressed by complements (e.g., irse a casa ‘Lit. go home’), it has not been
reached yet.
b. Trajectory DE and/or A:
- without boundary crossing: venir ‘come’, llegar ‘arrive’, partir ‘leave’, etc.
- with boundary crossing: entrar ‘enter’, penetrar ‘penetrate’, salir ‘exit’, etc.
4
According to De Miguel, verbs of inherently achieved location (i.e., verbs denoting the endpoint or
conclusion of motion) are unaccusative verbs whereas verbs of inherent path are unergative. The
former can be found in resultative constructions: Una vez caído el Muro de Berlín, la izquierda se ha
acabado (Lit. Once fallen the Wall of Berlin, the left has finished) whereas the latter cannot:
?Regresado Miguel, nos fuimos al restaurante (Lit. Come back [past participle] Miguel, we went to
the restaurant).
101
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
To sum up, the heterogeneous syntactic nature of the motion verb lexicon has
posed problems to scholars concerned with the close relation between syntax and
semantics, leading them to propose different motion verb classes. Even though some
of the classifications are much more granular than others, all scholars agree that each
motion verb class is defined by both syntactic and semantic properties.
4.3.1. Introduction
102
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
lexicon’ (Faber & Mairal-Usón, 1999: viii). On the whole, Faber & Mairal-Usón and
Rodríguez-García suggest that the hierarchical organisation of the verb lexicon they
propose may well correspond to our mental lexicon.
Faber & Mairal-Usón (1999: 55) considered a given lexical domain to include a
group of lexical units that share the same meaning (conceptual structure) and also
show a similar syntactical behaviour. They divided the verb lexicon into thirteen
major lexical domains 5 : Existence, Movement, Position, Contact, Change,
Perception, Cognition, Feeling, Speech, Sound, Light, Possession, and Action. For
the purposes of the present work, we are mainly concerned with their proposed
hierarchical organisation of the motion verb lexicon. They partitioned the lexical
domain of Movement into 4 major categories which were further subdivided as
follows:
5
For a detailed list of the lexical domains and subdomains, see Appendix 1 in Faber & Mairal-Usón
(1999).
103
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
2. Liquid
a. To move as liquid in a particular way: flow, ripple
b. To move in/downward below the surface of a liquid: sink, plunge
c. To move over liquid: sail, cruise
d. To move upwards to the surface of a liquid: surface
3. Atmosphere
a. To move as air: blow
b. To move through the air: fly, flutter, float
c. To move upwards in the air: rise, soar
d. To move downwards in the air: swoop, dive
4. Land
a. To move in a particular way: skulk, creep, scamper
b. To cause somebody/something to move downwards to the ground: trip,
tumble
c. To move one’s body: gesture, exercise
104
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
3. Directional Motion
a. Moving/Taking somebody/something away from (horizontal axis):
separate, leave, flee, remove, withdraw
b. Moving somebody/something forwards and towards (horizontal axis)
b1. by advancing: advance, approach
b2. by gathering things: gather, join, glue, tie
b3. by gathering people: crowd, meet
c. Moving somebody/something upwards to a higher position (vertical axis):
raise, elevate, climb, ascend
6
This lexical subdomain includes motion verbs that can express both positional or locative motion
(i.e., no change of location) and directional motion.
105
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
106
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
4.4.1. Introduction
Nowadays, there are some online databases on the syntax and the semantics of verbs
embodying a variety of theoretical backgrounds. Most of these databases can be
navigated with their own browser and a few of them are freely available for
download. In this section, we provide a brief overview of some databases for English
and Spanish, in which different classifications of motion verbs can be found, ranging
from semantically-oriented classifications, such as those in ADESSE, FrameNet and
WordNet, to more syntactically-oriented classifications, such as those in VerbNet.
4.4.2. Adesse 7
7
http://webs.uvigo.es/adesse/index.html. See also García-Miguel & Albertuz (2005) and García-
Miguel et al (2005) for a more detailed explanation of this project.
8
http://adesse.uvigo.es/data/desc_verbo.php?sense=3383
107
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
SPACE and to the subclass of Displacement. When we navigate with the browser, we
learn that venir ‘come’ has been found in 1169 clauses with 53 different syntactic-
semantic realisations. Furthermore, a rich picture of the verb’s arguments, their
syntactic function and the semantic roles 9 that they play in each one of those 53
syntactic-semantic realisations emerges on the screen.
Within the domain SPACE, over 400 Spanish verbs were classified into five
sub-classes (Martínez-Fuentes, 2004):
More recently, a sixth sub-class has been added, namely Unión ‘Joint’, which
includes verbs such as juntar ‘join, put together, assemble’.
In Adesse, Displacement verbs (subclass a) include two groups of verbs: (1)
those expressing the manner of motion and (2) those of inherent direction (Martínez-
Fuentes, 2004: 889). However, in most analyses of motion verbs, these two groups of
verbs are usually differentiated and grouped into two separate verb classes (e.g.,
Cifuentes-Honrubia, 1999; Morimoto, 2001)
The FrameNet project is creating an online lexical resource for English, based on
Fillmore’s Frame Semantics 11 and supported by corpus evidence (British National
9
The specific set of semantic roles for labelling verb arguments are: Móvil ‘Figure’, Origen ‘Source’,
Dirección ‘Direction’, Trayecto ‘Trajectory’, Beneficiario ‘Beneficiary’, Finalidad ‘Aim’, Medio de
Transporte ‘Means of transport’, Manera ‘Manner’, and Lugar ‘Location’.
10
http://framenet.icsi.berkeley.edu; http://gemini.uab.es:9080/SFNsite/
11
For a brief overview of Frame Semantics see Chapter 2 (Section 2.1.3) and Evans & Green (2006:
Section 7.2).
108
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
Corpus, 100 million words). The aim is to document the range of semantic and
syntactic combinatory possibilities (valences) of each word in each of its senses. The
major product of this work, the FrameNet lexical database, currently contains more
than 10,000 lexical units, more than 6,100 of which are fully annotated in more than
825 semantic frames and exemplified in more than 135,000 annotated sentences.
According to Baker & Ruppenhofer (2002: 29), in FrameNet, verbs and other
lexical units are grouped according to conceptual structures or frames instead of in
terms of their syntactic behaviour. The main implication is that verbs grouped
together in FrameNet might be semantically similar but have different argument
realisations, and verbs which share the same syntactic behaviour might be
represented in two different semantic frames. The same applies to Spanish verbs in
Adesse. Thus, both FrameNet and Adesse adopted a semantically-oriented approach
to the classification of verbs. However, as García-Miguel, Costas & Martínez-
Fuentes (2005) remark, a major difference exists between the two databases;
semantic classes in Adesse are much more general than frames in FrameNet.
In FrameNet, a lexical unit is a pairing of a word with a meaning. Typically,
each sense of a polysemous word belongs to a different semantic frame, a script-like
conceptual structure that describes a particular type of situation, object, or event and
the participants and props involved in it. For instance, the verbs drift, float, fly, glide,
and go evoke the Motion frame. This frame describes the situation in which an entity
(Theme) starts out in one place (Source) and ends up in some other place (Goal),
having covered some space between the two (Path). Theme, Source, Goal and Path
are the frame elements.
FrameNet can be considered as dictionary and thesaurus. The lexical units
come with definitions (e.g., walk is defined as an excursion or act of travelling on
foot) and with a set of examples which are meant to illustrate the combinatorial
possibilities of the lexical units. As each lexical unit is linked to a semantic frame,
and thus to the other words that evoke that particular frame, this makes the FrameNet
similar to a thesaurus. By way of illustration, the verb walk belongs to the Self-
motion frame, which also contains verbs such as amble, back, bound, dash, hop,
march, meander, promenade, scurry, sneak, tiptoe, trip, wade, waltz, etc.
Furthermore, FrameNet provides a network of relations between frames (e.g.,
109
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
Inheritance relation, Subframe relation, Using relation, etc). For example, Self-
motion inherits from the Motion frame, which in turn does not inherit from a parent
frame. The Motion frame is the parent frame of the following frames:
Moreover, the Motion frame is used (i.e., some of its frame elements are used),
among many others, by the following frames:
• The Body-movement frame, e.g., bend, blink, bob, cross, shrug, wink
• The Bringing frame, e.g., bring, carry, cart, convey, drive, fly, take,
transport
• The Change-direction frame, e.g., swing, turn, veer
• The Departing frame, e.g., abandon, depart, desert, disappear, escape, exit,
leave, vanish, withdraw
• The Operated-vehicle frame, e.g., balloon, bicycle, bike, cruise, drive, fly,
motor, parachute, pedal, skate, taxi
110
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
WordNet is a large lexical database of English, developed under the direction of the
psychologist George Miller. English nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs are
grouped into sets of ‘cognitive synonyms’ called synsets, each expressing a distinct
lexicalised concept. Thus, synsets are the most important units; they are interlinked
by means of semantic and lexical relations, such as troponymy (i.e., Y is a way of X),
hyperonymy (i.e., X is a kind/type of Y), synonymy (i.e., X is similar to Y),
antonymy (i.e, X is not Y), meronymy (i.e., an X is a part of a Y), etc. Miller &
Fellbaum (1991: 216-220) state that troponymy (from Greek tropos, i.e., manner or
fashion), also called verb hyponymy, is the most common semantic relation among
verbs. Many verbs indicate more precisely the manner of doing something, for
example, march, strut, traipse, amble, and mosey are troponyms of to walk. To put it
differently, they are ways of walking, mutually inclusive or temporally co-extensive
with walking; thus, when someone marches or struts, s/he is walking.
To illustrate how WordNet works, let us consider the motion verb walk. In
our search with the browser, it was found that walk as a verb has 10 synsets or
distinct meanings, among them ‘to use one’s feet to advance’, ‘to accompany or
escort’, ‘traverse or cover by walking’, ‘make walk’, ‘walk at a pace’, ‘take a walk
or walk for pleasure’. If we consider the synset ‘to use one’s feet to advance’, a full
list of troponyms (e.g., march, strut, promenade, stride, march, amble, mosey),
hyperonyms (e.g., travel, move, go, locomote) and antonyms (e.g., ride ‘be carried or
travel on or in a vehicle’) are displayed on the screen.
Generally speaking, WordNet acts as a dictionary and a thesaurus in much the
same way as FrameNet. However, a major difference between the two lexical
databases is that FrameNet is founded on corpus attestations, and thus includes
examples taken from the BNC corpus. Also, FrameNet offers syntactic information.
12
http://wordnet.princeton.edu/; http://multiwordnet.itc.it/english/home.php
111
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
FrameNet, however, does not take into consideration semantic relations (e.g.,
synonymy, antonymy, hyponymy, etc) between words within frames in the way
WordNet does.
MultiWordNet is a lexical database for Italian which has been modelled after
English WordNet. The Italian synsets are created in correspondence with the
WordNet synsets, whenever possible, and the semantic relations are imported from
the corresponding English synsets. The information contained in the database can be
navigated through the MultiWordNet browser, which facilitates the comparison of
the lexicons of English and Italian. Furthermore, the MultiWordNet browser allows
for access to the Spanish, Hebrew and Romanian WordNets. MultiWordNet stresses
the usefulness of an alignment between WordNets of different languages, as it is able
to represent true lexical idiosyncrasies between languages, such as lexical gaps. For
instance, English amble, meaning ‘to walk leisurely’ has as Spanish counterpart
deambular or pasear, whereas Italian, Hebrew and Romanian do not seem to have a
semantically equivalent verb.
4.4.5. VerbNet 13
13
http://verbs.colorado.edu/~mpalmer/projects/verbnet.html
112
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
4.5.1. Introduction
113
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
By way of illustration, the motion event depicted in the sentence Dori climbed out of
the tree may be analysed as consisting of a human-like creature directed in a
descending path, in a grasping manner, from a source defined as the tree to an
unspecified goal.
These categories are useful for exploring the semantics of motion verbs in
addition to motion events. However, as we will see in the next two sections,
crosslinguistic research on motion events has proposed even more specific manner
and path categories in order to capture semantic differences among verbs across
languages. Section 4.5.2 concentrates on manner verbs and provides a summary list
of fine-grained manner distinctions proposed by a number of scholars. Finally, in
Section 4.5.3, we offer a review of some studies which direct their research attention
to the semantics of path verbs, which have been often ignored in crosslinguistic
research on motion events. As with the manner component, fine-grained path
categories are proposed as well.
114
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
[L]exemes are examined in their semantic function as against that of their neighbours in the
same language, and at the same time as they compare with semantically similar lexemes in
the other language. (Snell-Hornby, 1983: 17)
In her analysis of 483 German and 617 English verbs, she proposed 4 major fields:
Human Behaviour, Movement and Position, Sounds, and Facial Expression and
Light. Each field comprises a range of structured subfields. Thus, the major field of
Movement and Position was subdivided into 3 subfields: (1) Walking and Running,
(2) Movement in air and water, and (3) Static and Negative; these subfields, in turn,
were further subdivided (Snell-Hornby, 1983: 80, 133, 142). Here, we provide the
divisions of (1) and (2), with examples of English verbs, as these fall within the
purview of this dissertation.
115
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
bummeln, schlendern and spazieren focus on the leisurely attitude of the agent.
Amble focuses on easy pace and even movement; saunter emphasizes the agent’s
self-confident attitude, and stroll emphasizes unhurried, but not necessarily even
movements as it includes starts and stops. German schlendern can render all three
English verbs; bummeln focuses on slow tempo, pleasure and relaxed mood, with the
likehood of pauses, thus resembling stroll. Within this group, English wander and
German wandern are also included; these verbs indicate movement over a larger area
without a destination or route. However, German wandern also denotes walking
lengthy distances on foot as a healthy form of recreation, which is covered by
English ramble, which focuses on the pleasurable aspect, and hike, focussing on
walking as healthy but strenuous exercise.
Snell-Hornby compared manner verbs from two languages which belong to
the satellite-framed group. Her analysis dwells on subtleties of meaning which are
useful for understanding and discriminating the meaning of motion verbs within and
between the two languages. However, Snell-Hornby does not provide any list of
manner details which might serve as a basis for the comparison of motion verbs from
other languages.
As we saw in Chapter 3, Slobin (e.g., 2004, 2006) has observed that the
availability of an open verb slot in satellite-framed languages has led these languages
to develop a richer and more expressive manner verb lexicon than verb-framed
languages. In order to investigate this claim, the general tendency in crosslinguistic
research on motion events has been to use data from novels, oral elicited narratives,
and translations. Though a definitive count has not been undertaken, Slobin (2006a:
71) has recently estimated around several hundred manner verbs for languages within
the satellite-framed group and less than one hundred for verb-framed languages.
Accordingly, English is expected to have several hundred manner verbs whereas
Spanish less than one hundred.
Among the studies that compare and contrast manner verbs from both
satellite- and verb-framed languages, we offer a brief review of Slobin (2005) on
English, Serbo-Croatian, French and Turkish, Özçalişkan (2004) on English and
Turkish, and Cifuentes-Férez (2006) on English and Spanish. Slobin (2005)
compared translations of a chapter of Tolkien’s The Hobbit into some verb- and
116
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
satellite-framed languages. He found that the rich manner verb diversity of the
English text is matched by Serbo-Croatian (satellite-framed language), surpassed by
Russian (satellite-framed language), but reduced to a few manner verbs in French
and Turkish (verb-framed languages). Özçalişkan (2004: 81) compared manner verbs
taken from English and Turkish novels. She provided a summary table illustrating
the distribution of some English and Turkish verbs using the following specific
manner categories: rapid motion, forced motion, leisurely motion, smooth motion,
obstructed motion, furtive motion, manners of walking, manners of running, and
manners of jumping. She concluded that, for each manner category, English novelists
used a greater variety of manner verbs than Turkish ones. Much in the same vein,
Cifuentes-Férez (2006) compared manner verbs taken from an English novel and its
Spanish translation. She concluded that English carves up manner in a more fine-
grained fashion than Spanish; for example, she pointed out that Spanish does not
seem to have as many verbs encoding information about the Figure’s physical and
psychological state as English has, e.g., whether the Figure is tired as in traipse,
relaxed as in amble or stroll (Spanish ‘pasear’), injured as in limp (Spanish ‘cojear’)
or hobble.
Within the satellite-framed group of languages, Jovanovic & Martinovic-Zic
(2004) compared English and Serbo-Croatian 14 manner verbs elicited using the frog
stories, Kopecka (2006) examined English and Polish manner verbs, and Dimitrova-
Vulchanova & Martínez (ms.) focused on English, Bulgarian and Norwegian manner
verbs.
Jovanovic & Martinovic-Zic (2004) found that Serbo-Croatian speakers
produced a greater variety of manner verbs than English speakers. Instead of
analysing manner details (e.g., rate, state of the Figure, etc) encoded in verbs, they
centred their analysis on the role that aspect (i.e., temporal contouring), which is
incorporated via morphology to the verb root, plays in the meaning of manner verbs.
For example, a set of manner verbs whose basic meaning is that of jumping or
hopping is full of aspectual nuances as (11) illustrates. Thus, the greater variety of
manner verbs in Serbo-Croatian than in English seemed to be due to aspectual
notions expressed in manner verbs. On the whole, they concluded that aspect has
14
See Filipović (2007) for a detailed account on the lexicalization of motion in Serbo-Croatian.
117
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
(11) Serbo-Croatian set of manner verbs (taken from Jovanovic & Martinovic-Zic
(2004: 222)
a. skakati ‘jump’
15
Narasimham (2003: 135) considers contact, i.e., friction between the Figure and the Ground, as a
manner component relevant for exploring crosslinguistic differences in the manner verb lexicon.
16
Unlike other satellite-framed languages, English allows a great flexibility as to the types of verbs
which can combine with path satellites to express motion events. See Narasimham (2003) and
Narasimhan et al (2006) for a discussion of motion constructions and their productivity across two
118
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
(12) Sound emission verbs (taken from Goldberg & Jackendoff, 2004)
satellite-framed languages (English, Polish), and three verb-framed languages (Hindi, Turkish and
French).
119
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
In addition, Nikanne & van der Zee (2004) and Dimitrova-Vulchanova & Martínez
(ms.) identified a further manner parameter, namely, global or local path-shape as
expressed in the verbs curve and zig-zag. In contrast, path-shape is regarded as a path
17
She uses these manner categories in her analysis of Basque sound symbolic expressions for motion.
120
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
18
According to Fillmore (1982: 32-33) and Jackendoff (1985: 279, 2002), English climb is a cluster
concept, that is, there are two conditions relevant for a situation to count as an instance of climbling:
(1) effortful clambering movement (with use of limbs), and (2) upwards direction. However, these two
factors do not need to be simultaneous, and neither is necessary, but either is sufficient. For example,
in English a human can climb up and down, but a snake can climb up (upward direction condition is
met), but not down (because the limbs supporting condition and the upward direction conditions are
not being met).
121
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
since manner verbs with a preferred direction (e.g., soar, climb, upwards motion) can
combine with different path satellites (e.g., soar up and down, climb to, climb across,
climb down), it seems that path information, rather than residing in the verb, is
relegated to satellites. Therefore, it might be argued that these manner verbs are
devoid of the Path component. On the other, the fact that some manner verbs are
frequently associated with certain paths suggests that the meaning of those manner
verbs imply those paths; in other words, both Manner and Path are thought to be
conflated in the verb. Thus, soar could be said to denote a quick or fast rate in the
motion (Manner) plus an upwards trajectory (Path); climb as encoding the use of
one’s legs and/or hands (Manner) in order to go up or onto something (Path).
All in all, crosslinguistic research exploring manner verbs has zoomed into
the features which make up Manner in order to capture fine-grained semantic
differences lexicalised in verbs across languages. As has been shown, the Manner
component can be decomposed into a number of independent categories pertaining to
various aspects of the motion scene.
Despite of the importance of Path in the motion event, the emphasis of the analysis
of motion verbs has usually been on the Manner of motion (cf. Ibarretxe-Antuñano,
2003b; Sivonen, 2005). So, it is no exaggeration to state that both the semantic
component of Path and path verbs themselves demand an investigation.
In order to better understand the differences among path verbs, both within
and between languages, it is useful to start by pointing out the distinct components
which constitute this element. According to Talmy (2000b: 53-56), the three main
components of Path are the Vector, the Conformation, and the Deictic 19 :
1. The Vector refers to the direction of motion of the Figure with respect to the
Ground, which can be a source, a milestone or a goal; thus, Vector can denote
19
According to Talmy (2000b: 53), Path in spoken languages consists of three components (Vector,
Conformation and Deixis), whereas in sign languages two other components are added: Contour and
Direction.
122
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
motion from a source (e.g., move from), past or along a milestone (e.g., move along,
via), and to or towards a goal (e.g., move to, towards).
3. The Deictic component is clearly defined by Talmy (2000b: 56) as follows: ‘[t]he
Deictic component of Path typically has only the two member notions toward the
speaker and in direction other than toward the speaker’.
The three Path components can cooccur. If we consider example (13), the English
satellite into combines both Vector and Conformation; the Figure reached its goal or
endpoint of motion, which is a container. In (14), the Spanish verb salir denotes
motion from a point inside a container, and thus, it conflates Vector and
Conformation. The Spanish preposition de ‘from’ just encodes the Vector (i.e., from
a source)
Talmy pointed out than in Spanish path verbs, Conformation and Deixis cannot be
conflated together. Thus, there are Deictic verbs (e.g., venir ‘come’, ir ‘go’) on the
one hand, and Conformation plus Vector verbs (e.g., entrar ‘enter’, salir ‘exit’) on
the other.
Slobin (in press) proposed a fourth component, Earth-grid Displacement.
This component relates direction of motion to earth-based geometry: north-south-
east-west, up-down, and other absolute coordinates (upstream, downstream). For
example, the Spanish verbs ascender ‘ascend’ and descender ‘descend’ denote
motion along the vertical axis; Vietnamese and other languages have verbs for ‘go
upstream’ and ‘go downstream’.
123
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
Narasimhan (2003: 136) explored the English and Hindi path verb lexicon
taking as a starting point Talmy’s Path components. She identified the following Path
components in Hindi and English verbs of motion:
Narasimham’s components (a) - (f) are specific formulations of Talmy’s Vector and
Conformation. For example, complex endpoint is the same as Vector (towards a goal,
which is the endpoint of motion) and Conformation (the Ground is an enclosure or
volume). Components (g) and (h) pertain to Earth-grid Displacement and to the
Deictic component respectively.
In general, Vector, Conformation, Deixis and Earth-grid Displacement should
be seen as Path components which are useful for exploring crosslinguistic differences
in the type of path information expressed in path verbs. Besides these, scholars have
proposed other ways to further analyse path verbs, which may equally serve the same
purposes.
Wälchli (2001) offered a more fine-grained typology of path verbs; more
precisely, he proposed 6 types of paths or cardinal kinds of displacement (in his
terminology), which he labelled with the help of Latin prepositions:
124
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
Thus, Berthele proposed that paths be categorised without specifying the source or
goal in the vertical dimension (see his classification below). Moreover, unlike
Wälchli’s list which used the deictic verbs go and come, Berthele decided to use the
verb displace in order to avoid reference to an unnecessary deictic centre. Here is his
reformulation:
125
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
which have been proposed encompass Talmy’s Vector, Conformation and Deixis,
and Slobin’s Earth-grid Displacement (vertical and horizontal axis). For instance,
ascend might be categorised as a SUPER verb (i.e., the Figure displaces up) or as a
verb encoding Earth-grid Displacement (i.e., vertical axis: up); leave as an AB verb
(i.e., the Figure displaces away from the Ground) or as a verb encoding Deixis (i.e.,
motion away from the deictic center). In contrast, the domains of Path later proposed
by Wälchli, i.e., ENTER, EXIT, ASCEND, DESCEND, and PASS/CROSS, exclude the
deictic component of Path.
Finally, it has also been suggested that the path verb lexicon may be closed-
ended (i.e., limited in the sorts of notions it can convey) and that languages,
regardless of typological status, may have comparable path verb lexicons. Özçalişkan
(2004) found that English and Turkish present a similar number of path verbs, more
concretely, 20 types for English and 24 types for Turkish. She claimed that the
manner verb lexicon is open ended (i.e., open to new additions, cf. Slobin, 2003,
2006), but the path verb lexicon ‘forms a closed lexical category that does not
provide many options for elaboration’ (Özçalişkan, 2004: 85). Though motion
research data points in that direction, the claim of comparable size of path verb
lexicons across languages has not yet been addressed.
4.6. Summary
Motion verbs have been the subject of a great deal of research from different
theoretical frameworks because they present interesting semantic and syntactic
characteristics. Even though motion verbs are considered to be a coherent semantic
class, this chapter shows how motion verbs encode different types of semantic
information, and also display different syntactic behaviours.
Sections 4.2 and 4.3 reviewed a number of approaches to grammar centred on
the verb lexicon: the Syntax-Semantics Interface and the Functional-Lexematic
model. The general label ‘the Syntax-Semantic Interface’ was given to those
theories which assume that argument structure is largely predictable from the
meanings of verbs, and that verbs with similar meanings display similar syntactic
126
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
127
CHAPTER 4. CLASSIFICATIONS AND ANALYSES OF MOTION VERBS
concentrated on zooming into the features which make up Manner in order to capture
fine-grained semantic differences lexicalised in verbs. Thus, it has been found that
the Manner component can be decomposed into a number of independent parameters
which pertain to various aspects of the motion scene: forced motion, smooth motion,
rate of motion, state of the Figure, etc. In contrast, Path of motion has not received as
much research attention. Section 4.5.3 showed that Path may also be subdivided into
four basic components (Vector, Conformation, Deixis and Earth-grid Displacement)
which serve the purpose of exploring what sorts of path notions are encoded in path
verbs. Crosslinguistic research, moreover, suggests that path verbs may be
categorised in terms of basic types of paths or domains. Last but not least, it has been
suggested that unlike the manner verb lexicon, which seems to be open to new
additions, the path verb lexicon might be closed-ended, and that languages from both
typological groups might have path verb lexicons which are comparable in size.
128
CHAPTER 5
5.1. Introduction
5.6. Conclusions
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
5.1. Introduction
This chapter sets out to explore the semantic nature of the English and the Spanish
motion verb lexicons and to offer a contrastive account with special attention to the
Path and Manner dimensions of motion. Despite the bulk of research on the linguistic
expression of motion events in English and in Spanish, and the resulting claims about
the semantics of their verbs, hardly any research has been devoted to an in-depth
analysis of their motion verb lexicons, and a comparative study is, to our knowledge,
nonexistent. As has been stated in previous chapters, Slobin (1997: 459) pointed out
that languages seem to have a ‘two-tiered’ lexicon of manner-of-motion verbs 1 : (1) a
general one, or superordinate level, represented by everyday verbs such as walk, run,
jump, fly, etc.; and (2) a more specific and expressive level consisting of different
ways of walking, such as stroll, wander, or shuffle; different ways of running such as
sprint or jog, etc. English possesses a very extensive and elaborated second level. In
contrast, in Spanish the manner-of-motion verb lexicon is not as rich and mainly
consists of general manner-of-motion verbs. Though a definitive count has not been
undertaken, Slobin (2006: 71) estimated around several hundred manner-of-motion
verbs for English and less than one hundred for Spanish. Moreover, in an attempt to
shed light on the sort of specific manner details which are encoded in verbs and/or in
other lexical items, scholars have subdivided Manner into different semantic
categories. In terms of path verbs, it was pointed out in Chapter 4 that Özçalişkan
(2004: 85) claimed that path verb lexicon of both satellite- and verb-framed
languages ‘form a closed lexical category that does not provide many options for
elaboration’. Thus, she hypothesised that both satellite-framed and verb-framed
languages seem to possess approximately the same number of path verbs. However,
her claim has not been fully addressed by research yet.
The present chapter aims at bridging the gap in the existing literature by
examining a substantial set of motion verbs in English and Spanish: specifically, we
1
This does not seem to be only specific to motion verbs but also to verbs from other semantic fields,
such as verbs of seeing (with see and look at the superordinate level and stare, gaze, glance, gape, etc.
at the specific level); verbs of laughing or smiling (with guffaw, smirk, grin, etc at the second level),
verbs of saying, verbs of hearing, etc. However, research is needed to test whether languages have
these two levels for most semantic fields or just for some of them.
130
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
5.2.1. Aims
The primary goal of this chapter is to provide a systematic and detailed account of
the semantics of both the English and the Spanish motion verb lexicons, and to
131
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
compare and contrast them. Our starting point is Talmy’s general semantic
components for motion, but our analysis looks into the broad categories of Path and
Manner so as to search for more subtle differences in the motion lexicon, which
would enable us to (a) better understand how the lexicon for motion is
organised/distributed in these two languages, and (b) formulate hypotheses for
further psycholinguistic research (some of which will be addressed in Chapter 6).
The research questions that guide our semantic analysis are the following
ones:
1. Which semantic notions are conflated in English and Spanish motion verbs?
This research question aims at exploring which sorts of conflations are generally
found in English and Spanish motion verbs and which are not. Beyond the path-
conflating and the manner-conflating verb types, other lexicalisation patterns are
expected to be found: Motion + Figure in Co-Motion (e.g., acompañar and
accompany, in which several figures go together), Motion + Ground (e.g., atajar ‘to
go somewhere by taking a shortcut’, costear ‘to sail or move along the coast’),
Motion + Path + Ground (e.g., embarcar ‘to go on board’, embark ‘to go on board’,
emigrate ‘to leave one’s country’, land ‘down onto the ground’), Motion + Path +
Ground + Manner (e.g., ford ‘to cross a river by wading, walking); etc.
We intend to test Özçalişcan’s claim that path verb lexicons of satellite- and verb-
framed languages are comparable in size. Our research attention is directed to the
semantics of English and Spanish path verbs to unveil both crosslinguistic
similarities and differences. It might be the case that English path verbs of Latin
origin (e.g., enter, ascend, descend, arrive, etc) look like Spanish path verbs sharing
132
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
the same etymology (e.g., entrar, ascender, descender, arrivar, etc) and, thus,
express the same sorts of path information. It could be expected, however, that there
exist path verbs of a different semantic nature in the Spanish lexicon; for instance,
verbs whose etymological origin is that of a preposition (encoding Path) plus an
adverb: adentrarse ‘to go into’ = a + dentro (‘inside’), from Latin deintro;
acercarse ‘to bring nearer to’= a + cerca (‘near’), from Latin, circa ‘around’. In
general, by contrasting the English and the Spanish path verb lexicons both
quantitatively and qualitatively we mean to give path verbs the research attention
they deserve.
Crosslinguistic research has suggested that English manner verbs outnumber those of
Spanish and that they encode much finer manner information. Thus, our endeavour is
also to investigate which types of fine-grained manner information are lexicalised in
both lexicons. Furthermore, we intend to find out which types of manner information
are more productively expressed in verbs. Research has shown that English allows a
great flexibility as to the types of verbs which can combine with path satellites to
express motion events, such as sound emission verbs. In addition, English can use
vehicle names and dance names as motion verbs. Finally, our research seeks to find
out (a) whether the English manner verb lexicon is equally rich and varied for all
subdomains of motion with which we have dealt (i.e., human and animal locomotion,
motion of objects or inanimates, motion in/on vehicles, etc), and if not, (b) whether
the Spanish manner verb lexicon might be richer than the English for some particular
subdomains.
133
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Our corpus consists of 376 English motion verbs and 257 Spanish motion verbs. The
motion verbs under the scope of the present investigation include general motion
verbs (i.e., no specific semantic information apart from the Motion component, such
as, the Spanish verbs moverse and menearse ‘to move oneself’, and English move),
accompany verbs (e.g., escoltar ‘to escort’, guiar ‘to guide’, escort, guide, lead), and
path verbs (e.g., entrar ‘enter’, salir ‘exit’, arrive, come, depart, enter, exit, go) as
well as manner verbs. Manner verbs pertain to (a) human and animal locomotion
(e.g., andar ‘walk’, correr ‘run’, saltar ‘jump’, walk, run, fly, hop, pronk, slither),
(b) motion using a vehicle (e.g., conducir ‘to drive’, navegar ‘to navigate’, bike,
canoe, ferry, ship), (c) motion while dancing (e.g., bailotear ‘a careless way of
dancing’, waltz, foxtrot, twist), (d) motion of physical objects or inanimate entities
(e.g., rodar ‘roll’, oscilar ‘oscillate’, revolve, roll, spin, shake, swing) and (e) change
of posture (acostarse ‘to lay oneself down’, levantarse ‘to stand up’, arquearse ‘to
bend oneself’, kneel, lean, recline, twist).
Berthele (2004: 108-109) states that posture verbs are ‘manner of position’
verbs as they encode ‘configurational information about the dimensions and the
vertical and the horizontal orientation of the Figure’. However, we decided to count
posture verbs in our corpus as they express motion which is internal to the Figure
(i.e., self-contained motion). In other words, the Figure moves its body by changing
its posture or orientation though it does not move to a different location (i.e.,
translational motion). Talmy (2000b) widens the category Posture to include
‘postures or orientations that are assumed by the human body or by objects treated
as comparable to the body’ [emphasis ours]. Thus, inanimate objects can be Figures
of posture verbs as well. However, the semantic natures of posture verbs in English
and in Spanish are different. As Talmy (2000b: 80) remarks, English posture verbs
(lie, sit, stand, lean, kneel, squat, crouch, etc) refer to ‘being in a state’. These verbs
must be augmented by a satellite to denote getting into a posture (e.g., lie down,
kneel down). In the case of lie, it must be further augmented by an agentive
derivation (lay down) to refer to putting things into the lying posture. In contrast,
Spanish lexicalises posture notions in the agentive ‘putting into a state’ type (i.e.,
transitive or caused motion, such as acostar, levantar). To express the other types,
134
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
grammatical elements are used, for example, estar acostado ‘be in lying posture’ for
the stative; acostarse includes the reflexive morpheme ‘se’ to get the meaning of
getting into a posture (‘to lie down’). On the whole, the reflexive pronoun ‘se’ makes
an agentive posture motion verb a self-agentive one.
Manner of motion verbs mostly depict self-agentive motion and non-agentive
motion (Talmy, 2000b). Self-agentive verbs are mainly intransitive verbs which
typically denote that the Figure has control over its motion, that is to say, there is no
external agent causing its motion:
On the other hand, non-agentive verbs are also intransitive verbs, but their Figures
are inanimate entities, as in (2). Non-agentive motion typically refers to motion
where the Figure does not necessarily have direct control over its motion (Levin,
1993: 264-265; Morimoto, 2001: 46-47).
b. La pelota se deslizó/rodó/botó
Generally speaking, the motion verbs which fall within the scope of this
dissertation are those which can be found in the intransitive construction ‘Figure
(subject) + Motion Verb’ and in the transitive constructions ‘Figure (subject) +
Motion Verb + Ground (direct object)’, as in (3), and ‘Figure + Motion Verb +
Figures in Co-Motion’ (especially for accompany verbs), as in (4). Transitive motion
verbs denoting caused motion do not fall within the purview of the present work.
b. Abandonamos la casa
b. Acompañó/guió a María
135
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
2
I am grateful to José Luis Cifuentes Honrubia for sending me the full manuscript of his 1999 book,
Sintáxis y Semántica del Movimiento, as it is currently out of print.
136
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
5.2.3. Methodology
Motion verbs in English and Spanish were included in two separate Excel sheets (see
Appendices 1 and 2). Ten columns were created. The first listed the motion verbs in
alphabetical order, and English translations were provided for each Spanish motion
verb. The second column showed whether the verb expresses translational motion or
self-contained motion (Talmy, 2000b: 35). As has been stated in previous chapters,
translational motion (i.e., change of the Figure’s overall location) is conveyed
primarily by path verbs but also by some manner verbs. More specifically, manner
verbs such as walk, tiptoe, jog and run (e.g., Morimoto, 2001) imply the Figure’s
change of location though the path of motion remains unspecified. In contrast, self-
contained motion is, in most cases, linked to non-agentive manner verbs (i.e., motion
of inanimate entities), which generally express oscillation, rotation, wiggling,
trembling, etc, such as revolve, roll, rotate, swing, sway, and shake. In addition, self-
contained motion is linked to posture verbs, which denote motion of a Figure which
remains in its overall location.
The third column coded whether each motion verb can also denote agentive
motion (henceforth AM), that is, an agent that causes the Figure to move:
3
These definitions are drawn from Cambridge Advanced Learner’s Dictionary (henceforth, CALD).
137
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
The fourth column coded the general semantic components that were
conflated in each verb. A motion verb can encode one or more of the following
semantic elements 4 :
For example, arrive conflates Motion and Path, walk conflates Motion and Manner,
and blow conflates Motion and Cause (i.e., current of air or wind). Besides the Co-
event relations of Manner and Cause, we took into account whether the verbs under
the scope of this dissertation encoded any other co-event relation (Talmy, 2000b: 42-
47). As motion verbs inherently encode the Motion component, we deliberately
avoided its constant repetition throughout the analysis of verbs in the spreadsheet.
Next, in the fifth column, we counted the number of conflated elements in
each motion verb. For example, if a verb encodes Path besides Motion, then we
coded ‘1’; if a verb conflates Path and Manner besides Motion, then we coded ‘2’,
and so on. The sixth and seventh columns exhibit the sorts of path notions and types
of paths, if any, conveyed by motion verbs and the manner details.
In terms of the Path component of motion, the following notions were taken
into account:
4
See Chapter 2, Section 2.2.2, for a comprehensive account on the basic semantic components of a
motion event and the types of relations between the main event and the co-event, and Section 2.2.3 for
the different lexicalisation patterns which have been identified across languages.
138
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
• ‘Deixis’: direction toward or away from the speaker; e.g., ir ‘to go’, venir ‘to
come’, come, go.
• ‘Earth-based orientation’ 5 : up-down orientation, north-south-east-west, etc.
All these path notions may be expressed by the types of path which are listed next.
The reader should note that G stands for Ground:
• Types of Path 6 :
o To/towards G (arrival, endpoint): alcanzar ‘to reach’, reach, arrive
o Away from G (departure, source): abandonar ‘to abandon’, abandon,
depart
o Into G (container): entrar ‘to enter’, inmigrar ‘to immigrate’, enter,
immigrate
o Out of G (container): salir ‘to exit’, exit
o Up/onto G - Upwards: ascender, subir ‘to ascend’, embark, mount, scale
o Down /down from G - Downwards: descender ‘to descend’, caerse ‘to
fall down’, descend, sink, fall
o Pass/cross G (traversal, milestone): cruzar ‘to cross’, pasar ‘to pass’,
traverse, pass, cross
o Closer to G: acercarse, arrimarse ‘to move closer to G’
o Forwards: avanzar ‘to advance’, advance
o Back to G/ Backwards: desandar, retroceder ‘to walk/go backwards’,
back
o Change direction: desviar (se) ‘to divert’, virar ‘to tack’, swerve
o Multiple directions from a unique start: esparcir, dispersarse ‘to scatter’
5
Slobin (in press) adds this fourth distinction to Talmy’s subcomponents of Path (Vector,
Conformation and Deixis). The notion ‘Earth-based orientation’ has also proven useful for manner
verbs depicting self-contained motion with a specific orientation, such as posture verbs.
6
The first seven path types (i.e., from To/toward G to Pass/cross G) are drawn from Wälchli (ms.) and
Berthele (2004), whereas the last six are our own additions to account for all the verbs in our corpus.
The reader might note that ‘Closer to G’ could also have been considered as a subtype of the path
‘To/towards G’ instead of as a separate type of path. However, we opted for such granular types of
path in order to better account for the semantic nature of path verbs.
139
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
In terms of Manner, on the other hand, we have proposed the following fine-
grained manner categories which are relevant for our analysis:
• ‘Posture’:
o Vertical orientation (upwards or downwards): tenderse ‘to lie onself
down’, agacharse ‘to crouch’, accurrucarse ‘to crouch’, empinar(se) ‘to
(cause to) stand up’
o Horizontal orientation: acostarse, tenderse, echarse ‘to lie down’
o Unspecified orientation: extenderse, estirarse ‘to stretch oneself’, stretch
o Bend, Twist, Curve: arquear ‘to bend oneself’, slouch, twist
o Roll – Unroll: enroscarse ‘ to roll oneself’
o To one side: ladear(se) ‘to slant, to lean’, careen, dodge
• ‘Shake, swing, vibrate’ 7 : temblar, tiritar ‘to tremble, shiver’, tremble, sway
• ‘Revolve, rotate, spin’ 8 : rodar, girar ‘to roll, to turn’, roll, spin, twirl, turn
• ‘Bounce’: botar ‘to bounce’, rebotar ‘to rebound’
• ‘Upside down’: capsize, flip
• ‘Iterative motion’: apisonar ‘to tread repeatedly on something’, bob,
rebound (iterative up-down motion)
These manner categories are mainly related to manner verbs describing self-
contained motion, whereas the following categories are linked to verbs implying
translational Motion (i.e., such manners of motion appear to exist only in the course
of the Figure’s translational motion):
• ‘Path-shape’ 9 :
7
‘Short, sudden movements’ are also included under this category.
8
As pointed out by Levin & Rappaport-Hovav (1992), Levin (1993) and Morimoto (2001), verbs
within this category might also be found in translational motion.
9
As we pointed out in Chapter 4, ‘Path shape’ has been usually regarded as a manner parameter (e.g.,
Nikanne & van der Zee, 2004).
140
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
10
The manner category of ‘Violent Motion’ (e.g., abalanzarse ‘to dash over in a violent way’, charge,
plunge) was included within ‘Forced Motion’ as it seems that all verbs denoting violent motion do
encode forced motion as well. Moreover, two subcategories within ‘Forced Motion’ were created: (a)
Energy-high, in order to account for verbs denoting violent motion, and (b) Energy-low, to account for
other verbs denoting an effortful motion which is not violent (e.g., trudge, plod). We are grateful to
Michele Feist for suggesting these two possible subcategories.
11
In the light of our data, this manner category seems to pertain to both self-contained and
translational motion.
141
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
142
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
our own additions in order to account for the semantic specificity of the manner
verbs in our corpus.
When looking at the sorts of semantic components conflated in verbs, we first
and foremost based our decisions on definitions taken from the dictionaries cited
above, though my own introspection as a native speaker of Spanish and
introspections of some English informants were also taken into account. Only
dictionary definitions of physical or actual motion were of interest for our research
purposes, leaving fictive motion (e.g., Talmy, 2000a: 90; Langacker, 1987;
Matsumoto, 1996; Rojo & Valenzuela, 2003) and metaphorical senses (e.g.,
Özçalişkan, 2002, 2004) aside. When the majority of the dictionaries did not agree 12
about the semantic information that the verb denotes, such information was not
included in our semantic encoding; for example, the verb tear is defined as violent
motion (Manner) only in two out of five sources (i.e., Oxford English Dictionary and
Merriam-Webster Online), with the result that this semantic information was not
coded for this verb. Moreover, we often took into consideration a verb’s etymology
in order to better understand its semantics and, quite often, its composite nature. For
instance, the Spanish verb adentrarse ‘to go into/inside’ comes from a (‘to’) + dentro
(‘inside’), and has thus been analysed as conflating Motion + Path; despeñar(se) ‘to
(cause to) fall down from a rock’ comes from des (‘from’) + peña (‘rock’), and has
been analysed as conflating Motion + Path + Ground. With regard to the etymology
of English motion verbs, both the Oxford English Dictionary and Merriam-Webster
Online provided us with such information. In the case of Spanish motion verbs, we
resorted to Cifuentes-Honrubia (1999), who draws from Corominas & Pascual’s
(1984) Diccionario crítico-etimológico castellano e hispánico, and to the two
Spanish monolingual dictionaries, Diccionario de uso del Español and Diccionario
de la lengua española, which also offer the etymologies of words.
12
By agreement across dictionaries, we generally mean 3 of 5 sources for English (4 dictionaries:
CALD, TFD, MW, OED, and 1 thesaurus: LLA) and 2 of 2 dictionaries for Spanish (DUE, DRAE).
The reader should note that LLA is a thesaurus which was just used for human locomotion verbs; it
fails to include many other motion verbs. Moreover, some of the dictionaries do also often fail to
incorporate definitions for some of the English motion verbs. When that was the case, we based our
analysis on the available dictionary definitions.
143
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
For verbs that have multiple dissimilar meanings (i.e., they conflate different
semantic information) separate numbered entries are created. For example, stalk has
two separate meanings: stalk1 ‘to go stealthily to, towards (an animal) for the
purpose of killing or capturing it’ (OED) and stalk2 ‘to march proudly’ (OED). For
ease of analysis, each numbered verb entry counts as a verb; stalk1 is counted in the
group of furtive motion verbs, whereas stalk2 in the group of verbs encoding the
Figure’s mental state.
The eighth and ninth columns include definitions taken from monolingual
dictionaries and a thesaurus for English which were used when compiling our corpus
of motion verbs. Moreover, syntactic behaviour provided by the dictionaries was
included in those columns, namely, whether the verbs were intransitive, transitive or
pronominal verbs. Pronominal motion verbs are abundant in the Spanish motion verb
lexicon. They are intransitive verbs accompanied by the reflexive pronoun ‘se’. The
reflexive pronoun can either (a) denote that the Figure is acting on itself (e.g.,
acercarse ‘to move oneself closer to’, zambullirse ‘to dive oneself into’; posture
verbs such as acostarse ‘to lay oneself down’ and levantarse ‘to get (oneself) up’) or
(b) just add emphasis to their meanings (e.g., irse ‘to go away’, caerse ‘to fall
down’). All motion verbs (a) which can be both pronominal (i.e., self-agentive) and
transitive (i.e., agentive, caused motion), and (b) whose reflexive pronoun ‘se’ is
used for emphatic purposes were listed with the ‘se’ in brackets (e.g., levantar (se)
‘to raise, to rise’, agitar (se) ‘to shake, to shake oneself’). Those verbs that are
strictly pronominal verbs, are shown with the reflexive pronoun ‘se’ adjoined to the
verb root (e.g., adentrarse ‘to go into the deepest part of’, apresurarse ‘to hurry up’).
Finally, the tenth column in our corpus spreadsheet displays the motor
patterns with which, according to native speakers, human locomotion verbs are
associated. For example, the English verbs dash and dart seem to be related to the
motor pattern run, that is, the Figure seems to be running while dashing or darting.
The data shown in this column come from a study on categorisation. This
information, which constitutes a separate study, will be left out of the discussion of
the corpus and will be taken up in detail in Chapter 6, Section 6.4.
144
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
The research methodology faces some problems which need to be tackled before
undertaking our enterprise. As we pointed out in the previous section, dictionaries do
not always agree on their motion verb definitions; sometimes there is high variability
in the sort of fine-grained details they are ready to include (as we saw above with the
verb tear), whereas, on other occasions, some dictionaries include a verb sense which
might not be found in any other dictionary. For example, the OED includes a sense
for traipse, to walk in a trailing or untidy way, which is not found in any of our other
sources. In general, our undertaking faces the impossibility of accounting for all the
possible contexts of use that a verb might have—some of which are included in
dictionary definitions. Consequently, we decided to take into account in our analysis
those fine-grained details and those verb senses that were present in the majority of
the dictionaries in order to more closely account for the semantic contribution of the
verb itself.
Another related problem we dealt with is the difficulty telling apart (a) what a
motion verb encodes or lexicalises and (b) what it is just implied, which is connected
to our linguistic knowledge about how a given motion verb is used and to our
semantic interpretation 13 . More often than not, dictionary definitions incorporate
information about the types of Figures (animates, inanimates), Grounds (air, water,
etc), manners of motion (rate, motor pattern) and trajectories (up, down, etc) which
are usually associated with motion verbs, but this information does not necessarily
amount to what is encoded or lexicalised in the verbs. By way of illustration, dodder
and toddle are manner verbs which encode unsteady ways of walking; the former
seems to imply that the Figure is normally an elder person, whereas the latter implies
that the Figure is usually a toddler. However, these verbs may be used with Figures
which are neither elderly people nor toddlers. Another good example is the verb dive;
this verb seems to encode a headfirst downwards motion, which normally takes place
13
Talmy (1985: 59) notes that a meaning can be associated with a linguistic unit mainly by different
processes; among them, lexicalisation and interpretation. In his own words, ‘lexicalization is involved
where a particular meaning component is found to be in regular association with a particular
morpheme’, whereas by interpretation he meant the process of interpreting the meaning of a lexical
unit by drawing from ‘present context and general knowledge’.
145
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
into water, but not necessarily. To solve this problem, we have attempted to code and
capture that semantic information pertaining to motion which is kept constant
throughout senses regardless of preferred Figures, Grounds, Manners and Paths.
Thus, dodder and toddle are analysed as conflating Motion and Manner but not
figural information; in the case of dive, the Ground (water) is not taken as being
conflated in the verb.
Finally, as the reader might argue, our methodology has a main limitation:
our semantic coding does not equal what might or might not be in speakers’ mental
representations of these verbs. We do not intend, by any means, to argue otherwise.
The results from our analysis call for experimental research to test their
psycholinguistic reality. On the whole, the semantic analysis of the English and
Spanish motion verb lexicons offers a solid linguistic ground for further experimental
research; our results raise some questions regarding what might or might not be in
the speaker’s mind. Obviously, a complete psycholinguistic validation of our
findings falls outside the limits of this dissertation, which just explores the
subdomain of human locomotion by using experimental methods.
This section provides the answer to our first research question, specifically, which
semantic notions are conflated in English and Spanish motion verbs? In this section,
the different sorts of conflations found in our corpus are outlined and fully described.
As we will see, in addition to path-conflating verbs and manner-conflating verbs,
other lexicalisation patterns are found, though they seem to constitute minor patterns
in both languages.
Motion verbs are grouped by taking into account (a) the number of semantic
components lexicalised in each verb, and (b) the semantic information they encode.
In terms of the number of semantic elements, motion verbs fall mainly into 4 groups:
(1) Motion, (2) Motion + 1 semantic component, (3) Motion + 2 semantic
components, and (4) Motion + 3 semantic components. In terms of the semantic
146
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
information motion verbs encode, the following verb conflations are found in our
corpus of English and Spanish motion verbs:
a. Motion
b. Motion + Ground
c. Motion + Figure
d. Motion + Path
e. Motion + Manner
f. Motion + Cause
g. Motion + Concurrent Result
h. Motion + Path + Ground
i. Motion + Manner + Ground
j. Motion + Path + Manner
k. Motion + Figure + Manner
l. Motion + Cause + Manner
m. Motion + Cause + Path
n. Motion + Figure + Co-Motion
o. Motion + Manner + Co-Motion
p. Motion + Manner + Concurrent Result
q. Motion + Path + Ground + Manner
r. Motion + Cause + Path + Manner
Table 5.1 summarises the general conflations found in the English motion verbs in
our corpus. Verb conflations or lexicalisation patterns are grouped with regard to the
number of semantic elements. Then, within each group, they are organised by their
overall frequency, from most frequent types of lexicalisation patterns to the less
frequent or non-existent patterns.
147
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Only one verb encoding the Motion component alone is found in our corpus: move.
Move is a general motion verb that is defined in dictionaries (CALD, TFD, MW,
OED) both as involving a change of posture (i.e., self-contained motion) and as
involving a change of location (i.e., translational motion).
With regard to verbs that encode Motion plus a single additional semantic
component (pertaining either to the main Motion event or to the Co-event), it is
observed that two lexicalisation patterns are the most frequent in our corpus, namely
path-conflating verbs (11.23% of verbs in the English corpus) and manner-conflating
verbs (73.80% of verbs in the English corpus). We postpone an exhaustive analysis
of these verbs until Sections 5.4 and 5.5 respectively, where we will delve into their
148
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
14
As a reminder, Concurrent result is one of the types of relations which can hold between the main
Motion event and the Co-event. According to Talmy, ‘the Co-event results from – that is, is caused by
– the main Motion event, and would not otherwise occur’ (2000b: 49).
149
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
a. Motion + Path + Manner: charge, chase, climb, dive, lunge, plummet, plunge,
pounce, rocket2, scram, skedaddle, slink, soar, stalk1, stampede, tail, thrust,
track, trail, tumble.
Overall, this group of verbs denotes information about the trajectory or direction of
motion as well as about the ways in which the Figure moves 15 . Charge, lunge,
plunge and pounce denote sudden motion towards the Ground, usually in a forceful,
violent way and at a fast speed (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Plunge and lunge can also
exhibit agentive behaviour. Thrust denotes an effortful forward motion. Chase,
stalk1, tail, track and trail denotes that the Figure is moving after another; chase
denotes that the motion is fast, usually running (CALD, TFD, MW, OED), whereas
stalk1, tail and track mean that the Figure follows the other Figure furtively, in a
secretive fashion (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Finally, trail denotes that the Figure is
moving slowly behind or after the other Figure, usually in contact with the Ground
(CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Scram, slink, skedaddle and stampede mean that the
Figure is moving away from the Ground; scram and skedaddle denote that the Figure
does it so at high speed, whereas slink denotes that the Figure moves in a furtive
manner so as not to be noticed (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). In stampede, the Figure
goes away quickly because it is frightened (MW, OED).
With regard to the vertical axis, that is, upward and downward motion, dive
means to move down headfirst into water or on other Ground in a precipitous way
(CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Plummet and tumble denote downwards motion; the
former being performed at a high speed (CALD, TFD, MW, OED), and the latter
characterised by falling down quickly without control (CALD, MW, OED). Rocket2
and soar depict quick upwards motion (TFD, MW, OED), whereas climb denotes
that the Figure moves up, onto the Ground in a clambering fashion, i.e., use of one’s
15
This group of verbs is one of the most interesting for future experimental research. In Chapter 4, we
argued that it is not clear for many verbs whether they encode both Path and Manner or whether they
just encode Manner, with Path merely implied, as these verbs typically occur with certain trajectories.
Naming and typicality judgment tasks using visual dynamic stimuli with changed paths and changed
manners might help to unveil the semantic nature of this set of verbs. For example, would an event be
labelled by dive or judged as typical of diving if (a) the Figure does not move in a rapid, high
energetic way and (b) if the Figure does not follow a downwards path?.
150
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
hands and legs (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). As Jackendoff (1985: 279) noted, climb
encodes both Path (upward motion, to the top of, onto) and Manner (clambering), but
‘any of them may be missing in any particular case’. Thus, either of the two semantic
features is sufficient to label an event as climbing.
16
Talmy does not include this sort of Co-event relation in his theory. In light of our data, we decided
to create the category Co-Motion to refer to those Co-events in which there is one or more Figure
moving.
151
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
152
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Our corpus of English motion verbs does not include the following patterns: Motion
+ Path + Ground, Motion + Cause + Manner, Motion + Manner + Concurrent Result.
These three patterns, as we will see in the following subsection, are realised in some
Spanish verbs.
Finally, four verbs are found in two distinct types of conflations encoding
three semantic components besides the fact of Motion:
Table 5.2 summarises the general verb conflations found in our corpus of Spanish
motion verbs. Verb conflations are grouped with regard to the number of semantic
elements (i.e., one, two, three or four), and within each group, they are ranked by
frequency.
153
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Four verbs encoding the Motion component alone were found in our corpus:
mover(se) ‘to move, to move oneself’, menear(se) ‘to (cause to) move’, mudarse ‘to
go from one place to another’, trasladar(se) ‘to (cause to) move from one place to
another’. Mover(se), much like English move, is a general motion verb that is defined
in dictionaries both as involving a change of posture (i.e., self-contained motion) and
as involving a change of location (i.e., translational motion). Furthermore, mover is
the agentive version of the verb, whereas moverse the self-agentive and non-agentive
version; these types of motion are both lexicalised in English move. In contrast to
154
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
17
As we remarked for English circuit and circulate, Spanish circular also comes from a Latin verb,
circulāris, which denotes motion in a circle. Consequently, it could have been analysed as conflating a
circular path shape instead of Ground.
155
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
the Figures would determine the manner of motion, and the verb was thus not coded
as a manner verb.
b. Motion + Figure: aletear ‘to flap, to flutter, to wriggle’, cabecear ‘to move or
to shake one’s head’, cocear ‘(of a horse, donkey) to kick’, codear ‘to move
your elbow, to nudge’, colear and rabear ‘(of an animal) to move its tail, to
wag).
The motion verbs in this group are also denominal verbs which transparently
incorporate the body part [Figure] engaged in motion. For example, ala ‘wing’ or
aleta ‘fin’ in aletear; cabeza ‘head’ in cabecear; cola and rabo ‘tail of an animal’ in
colear and rabear, etc.
c. Motion + Concurrent result: chocar and estrellar(se) ‘to (cause to) crash’,
and colisionar ‘to crash’.
These verbs of motion conflate the result of the Motion event, that is, the collision
between the Figure and the Ground takes place as result of the Figure’s motion.
Chocar and estrellar(se) can depict self-agentive, non-agentive and also agentive
motion, whereas colisionar does not seem to be used agentively.
Unlike the English corpus of motion verbs, our Spanish corpus does not have any
verb conflating Motion + Cause.
Regarding the conflation of fact of Motion and two other semantic
components, the following patterns are observed:
a. Motion + Path + Manner: abalanzarse ‘to dash to’, acechar ‘to stalk’,
desandar ‘to walk back to a previous path’, echar(se)1 ‘to (cause to) move
towards’, escabullirse ‘to slip away’, escapar(se) ‘to escape’, fugarse ‘to flee,
to run away’, gatear2 ‘to climb like a cat’, huir ‘to flee’, invadir ‘to invade’,
posarse ‘to alight’, precipitar(se)1 ‘to (cause to) fall down from a high
place’, precipitarse2 ‘to run, to hurry to’, rastrear2 ‘to track’, trepar ‘to
climb’.
156
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
b. Motion + Path + Ground: aterrizar ‘to land’, desembarcar ‘to (cause to)
disembark’, despeñar(se) ‘to (cause to) fall down from a rock’, embarcar(se)
‘to (cause to) go on board’, emigrar ‘to emigrate’, exiliar(se) and expatriar
‘to (cause to) leave one’s country; to exile’, inmigrar ‘to immigrate’,
naufragar ‘(of a ship, people in a ship) to sink’, repatriar ‘to repatriate’,
sumergir(se) ‘to (cause to) go down into water’, vadear ‘to wade, to ford a
river’.
Most of these verbs express the Ground in the verb root in an obvious way. As was
the case for some English motion verbs encoding Path + Ground, Spanish ground-
conflating verbs also come from Latin and are made up of Latin path prefixes plus
nouns (Grounds): aterrizar – towards the ground (Spanish tierra); desembarcar – out
of or down from a ship (Spanish barco, barca); despeñar – down from a rock
(Spanish peña); expatriar – away from one´s country (Spanish patria), repatriar –
back to one’s country. In contrast, we find other verbs 18 which encode Path and
Ground as well, but less transparently: emigrar, inmigrar, meaning that the Figure
moves away from and towards one’s country respectively; naufragar and sumergirse
which denotes downwards motion into water, and vadear, which means to cross a
river.
18
These verbs are also from Latin origin: emigrāre, immigrāre, naufragāre, submergēre.
157
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
c. Motion + Manner + Ground: callejear ‘to walk around the streets’, ladear ‘to
move on the hillside’, planear ‘(of a plane, a bird) to glide’, bucear ‘to dive,
swim down under water’, nadar ‘to swim’, chapotear ‘to move noisily in
water/mud’, rastrear1 ‘to fly along at ground level’, revolotear ‘to (cause to)
fly around, to flutter’.
Planear, rastrear, revolotear denotes that the Figure is moving through the air; the
first verb describes smooth motion, the second motion along and close to the ground,
and the third encodes motion around, in circles. Bucear, nadar and chapotear
denotes motion in and on water; nadar is the least specific semantically and
expresses that the Figure moves in water in a manner which is dependent on the
Figure’s species. Bucear denotes swimming under water (i.e., the Figure’s body is
submerged), and chapotear refers to the Figure’s noisy motion which is taking place
either in water or mud. Callejear means to walk around the streets (Spanish calle)
with no special aim in the motion, and ladear to walk on the hillside (Spanish
ladera). These two verbs are denominal verbs which denote that the Figure walks
[Manner] around, over, or on the Ground.
d. Motion + Figure + Manner: patalear and patear1 meaning ‘to stamp one’s
feet showing one is angry’, pisar ‘to tread’, pisotear ‘to tread repeatedly and
violently over something’.
This group of verbs involves one’s foot or feet as Figure which moves in distinct
manners. Pisar seems to be the most general in meaning, just denoting the motion of
each foot when the Figure is walking. Patalear and patear1 encode the angry attitude
of the Figure as shown by the stamping of its feet, and pisotear depicts iterative and
effortful motion of one’s feet when treading over something.
e. Motion + Cause + Manner: espantar(se) ‘to (cause to) run away as a result of
being frightened’, merodear ‘to walk around, to prowl’, patrullar ‘to patrol’,
rondar ‘to be on patrol, to prowl about, around’.
Espantarse denotes the Figure’s runaway after being frightened. Thus, this verb
encodes the onset causation of motion, that is to say, it precedes the motion event and
158
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
influences the Figure’s state of mind and its manner of motion. Merodear, patrullar
and rondar denote motion with a specific purpose which has been considered as a
non-physical cause; specifically, to look for something, to nose or to spy in merodear
and to watch over a person or a place in patrullar and rondar. Moreover, merodear
and rondar denote that the Figure is walking, whereas patrullar denotes motion in a
vehicle (patrol car).
Unlike the English verb outrun, which conflates Motion + Manner + Co- Motion, no
Spanish verb is found in our corpus which expresses this semantic information.
Finally, two verbs which encode three semantic components beside the fact of
Motion are found in our corpus: capuzar(se) ‘to (cause to) dive in’, and zambullir(se)
‘to (cause to) go down into water in a violent way’. These verbs are agentive verbs
which are made self-agentive by incorporating the reflexive pronoun ‘se’. They
conflate Motion + Path + Ground + Manner. In both uses, the verbs denote that the
Figure goes down [Path] into water [Ground] in a sudden, high-energetic way
[Manner]. No further information about the Manner of motion is given; thus, the
Figure could go down into water headfirst (as it seems to be implied) or in another
way (e.g., in a standing up position).
159
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
In terms of the number of conflated elements, the greater part of the motion verbs in
our corpus (87.50% of English motion verbs and 79.38% of Spanish motion verbs)
encodes one additional semantic component besides the fact of Motion, whereas (a)
verbs expressing just Motion (0.27% for English and 1.56% for Spanish), (b) verbs
conflating two semantic elements (11.17% for English and 18.29% for Spanish), and
(c) verbs conflating three semantic elements (1.06% for English and 0.78% for
Spanish) are much less prevalent. For that reason, it might be concluded that the
conflation of Motion plus one semantic component is the most characteristic or
typical of both motion verb lexicons. In contrast, verbs expressing just Motion and
verbs conflating either two or three semantic elements might be said to be less
representative. This supports Talmy’s (1985: 76) observation that a ‘[c]onflation
systems of […] multi-component sort apparently never form a language’s major
system for expressing Motion’.
In terms of the semantic information English and Spanish motion verbs
encode, with a few exceptions, similar lexicalisation patterns have been found across
the two languages. English and Spanish motion verbs seem to be able to encode a
wide range of semantic information pertaining both to the main Motion event and to
the Co-event (i.e., Manner, Cause, Co-Motion, Concurrent Result, etc). The majority
of the lexicalisation patterns attested in our corpus of motion verbs seems to involve
minor patterns in these languages, that is to say, they are not characteristic or typical
for either English or Spanish. In other words, Motion + Figure (cf. Atsugewi as
documented by Talmy, 2000), Motion + Ground, Motion + Cause and more complex
conflations of Figure, Ground and Co-events relations do exist in English and
Spanish, but they do not appear to form the core system for expressing Motion in
these languages. However, as shown in Tables 5.1 and 5.2, both path-conflating
verbs and manner-conflating verbs are the most frequent in our corpus and represent
a high portion of the motion verb lexicon (320 English verbs, i.e., 85.33% of the
English motion verbs, and 186 Spanish verbs, i.e., 72.37% of the Spanish motion
verbs). As a result, these two lexicalisation patterns might be said to be characteristic
of both English and Spanish. In both languages, manner verbs outnumber path verbs.
Within verb types, English manner verbs surpass Spanish manner verbs (276 -
160
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
73.80% vs. 123 verbs - 47.86%), but Spanish path verbs outnumber English path
verbs (63 verbs - 24.51% vs. 44 verbs - 11.70%, respectively).
Other crosslinguistic differences are to be found in the lack of verbs encoding
some minor patterns in one language which are found in the other. In particular, our
English corpus does not include the two following lexicalisation patterns: (a) Motion
+ Cause + Manner, as in Spanish espantar(se) ‘to (cause to) run away as result of
being frightened’, and (b) Motion + Manner + Concurrent Result, as in Spanish
atropellar ‘to run over something or somebody while driving a vehicle’. On the other
hand, no Spanish verb is found which expresses (a) Motion + Cause, as in English
drift and blow; (b) Motion + Manner + Co-Motion, as in English outrun; (c) Motion
+ Cause + Path, as in English topple, and (d) Motion + Cause + Path + Manner, as in
English flee.
The most important crosslinguistic difference resides in the greater tendency
of the Spanish language (compared with English) to express the Ground of motion in
verbs, either conflated with Motion alone or with additional semantic elements.
Talmy (1985: 75-76; 2000b: 60-61) noted that the Ground does not by itself conflate
with the Motion verb to form any language’s core system for expressing Motion and
that conflations of this type ‘might not even form any minor system’. He speculated
that the explanation might be sought in a concept of hierarchy, that is, the different
lexicalisation patterns seem ‘to be ranked in their prevalence among the world’s
languages, with conflation of Path as the most extensively represented, of
Manner/Cause next, and of Figure least so’. Apparently, Ground conflation is also a
possibility among languages, but he argues that ‘[it is] so unlikely that it has not yet
been instantiated in any language’ (Talmy, 2000b: 61). Our data, however, seem to
challenge Talmy’s remarks; English and Spanish both have some motion verbs
conflating Ground. Even though it seems to be a minor pattern in those languages
(12.06% in the Spanish corpus of motion verbs, and 4.55% in the English), their
presence should not be overlooked.
Table 5.3 includes English and Spanish ground-conflating verbs (grouped
into four different patterns) as well as the number of verbs per pattern and their total
percentage per language in our corpus.
161
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Though the Spanish motion verbs in our corpus incorporating Ground outnumber
those of English, no significant difference is found in the distribution of Ground
conflation types across languages (Chi-square (3) = 1.7652, p > 0.05). That is to say,
the two languages do not differ in how they distribute their verbs into the four
162
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Ground conflation types. As has been noted earlier, Spanish ground-conflating verbs
are, to a certain extent, transparent in their composite nature. Most of these verbs
come from Latin verbs which were made up of nouns, preceded by prefixes encoding
Path (e.g., aterrizar, desembarcar, embarcar(se), expatriar, repatriar) (cf. Cifuentes-
Honrubia, 1999). In contrast, our corpus of English verbs contains less verbs
encoding Ground, and in most cases these verbs come from Anglo-French and
Middle French, which were from Latin origin as well, either coming from nouns
without path prefixes (e.g., circuit, circulate) or from nouns with path prefixes (e.g.,
disembark, embark). On other occasions, both English and Spanish ground-
conflating verbs appear to be derived from nouns, such as Spanish atajar, bordear,
costear, fondear, llanear, callejear and ladear, and English land, moonwalk and
skydive.
In this section, we address the following general research question: Do English and
Spanish have comparable path verb lexicons? This question unfolds into two further
questions: Are the two path lexicons comparable in size? And Are the two path
lexicons comparable in their semantic nature? The former calls for quantitative data,
and has been answered covertly in the previous section; specifically, as shown in the
general distribution of English and Spanish motion verbs in our corpus, a total of 63
Spanish path verbs versus 44 English path verbs have been identified. Consequently,
it seems that the Spanish path verb lexicon is larger and thus, it might be richer or
more varied than the English path verb lexicon. However, qualitative data are needed
before we can draw such a conclusion. It might be the case that both languages
encode similar sorts of path notions in their verbs, but Spanish might have a greater
number of verbs expressing these notions. The other possibility is that Spanish verbs
encode types of path which are not found in English verbs, thus, suggesting that the
Spanish path verb lexicon is both larger in size and more varied in the path notions
its verbs encode. This lead us to the latter research question, Are the two path
lexicons comparable in their semantic nature?, which calls for a qualitative analysis
163
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
so as to unveil both (a) the sorts of path notions which are typically lexicalised in
English and Spanish respectively, and (b) crosslinguistic similarities and differences
in the semantics of path verbs. Therefore, this section mainly focuses on the semantic
nature of path verbs in English and Spanish. Moreover, special attention is given to
their etymology 19 as many path verbs in both languages come from Latin and, as a
consequence, they might be expected to express the same sorts of path information.
Path verbs across languages have been clustered in terms of the types of path
they express. Overall, 13 types of Path have been identified in our corpus:
In the following two sections, we begin by exploring the types of paths which are
most frequently lexicalised in the English and the Spanish path verbs in our corpus,
we then, move on to deal with those which are less frequent.
19
To remind the reader, our sources for the etymology of verbs are Merriam-Webster Online and the
Oxford English Dictionary for English, and Diccionario del uso del español actual, Diccionario de la
Real Academia de la Lengua Española, and Cifuentes-Honrubia (1999), which draws on Corominas
& Pascual (1984), for Spanish.
164
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Table 5.4 summarises the distribution of English path verbs in terms of the
aforementioned types of path, ranked by their frequency relative to the path lexicon.
ENGLISH
TYPES OF PATH
# %
Away from G 11 25.00%
Up/Onto G - Upwards 5 11.36%
After G 4 9.09%
Change direction 4 9.09%
Down from/to G - Downwards 4 9.09%
To/towards G 4 9.09%
Back to G/ Backwards 3 6.82%
Pass/cross G 3 6.82%
Into G 2 4.55%
Closer to G 1 2.27%
Forwards 1 2.27%
Multiple directions 1 2.27%
Out of G 1 2.27%
TOTAL 44 100.00%
As shown in the table, some types of path are much more frequent than others in our
corpus of English path verbs; for instance, the ‘Away from G’ type is the most
frequent and the ‘Out of G’, ‘Closer to G’, ‘Forwards’ and ‘Multiple directions from
a unique start’ are the least frequent. In order to examine the path verbs which
express these types of paths, we begin with the most frequent types of path and end
with the least frequent types:
a. Away from G (departure, source) (11 verbs, 25% of English path verbs): go,
abandon, depart, desert, dodge2, escape, leave, recede, retire, retreat, stray.
This group of verbs generally denotes motion away from the Ground. This
translational motion can imply that (a) the Figures leaves the Ground (as direct
object) behind, sometimes on its own and in a bad situation (e.g., abandon, desert);
165
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
(b) the Figure is fleeing from the Ground in order to be free or to avoid danger (e.g.,
escape, retreat); (c) the Figure avoids the Ground by moving quickly to one side
(dodge2) and (d) the Figure goes away from the intended path (stray). Within this
group, only retire and retreat seem to be also used as agentive verbs.
Go, depart and leave seem to be the most general in meaning, as they seem to
refer to the Figure’s movement away, with no further implications (CALD, TFD,
MW, OED). According to the Oxford English Dictionary, go is an intransitive verb
of motion which has 3 basis senses: (a) the Figure is moving away from the speaker,
or from the point at which he mentally places himself; (b) the Figure is moving
towards a place which is not occupied by the speaker, and (c) the Figure is moving
without regard to its point of departure or destination. Thus, go in (a) and (b) is a
deictic path verb since it encodes motion away from the relative position of the
speaker and seems to imply motion ‘to/towards another place’. In contrast, go in (c)
appears to act as a general motion verb without any specific path.
In terms of their etymology, abandon comes from Anglo-French abanduner
(from (mettre) a bandun, which means ‘to hand over, put in someone’s control’)
(MW). Desert comes from Anglo-French, from Late Latin desertum (neuter of
desertus, past participle of deserere ‘to desert’, which is formed by the prefix de-
‘from, down, away’ + serere ‘to join together’). According to Merriam-Webster
Online, the Latin prefix de- meant ‘from, down, away’. It seems that by adjoining the
prefix to the Latin verb root, it altered its meaning and evolved to ‘move away from’.
Depart also comes from Anglo-French departir, which in turn comes from de- +
partir ‘to divide’ (from the Latin verb partire, from part-, pars ‘part’). Escape comes
from Anglo-French escaper, eschaper, from Vulgar Latin excappare = Latin ex- +
Late Latin cappa ‘head covering, cloak’. Leave comes from Old English læfan and
retire from Middle French retirer (re- + tirer ‘to draw’). Recede comes from Latin
recedere ‘to go back’, which is made up of re- ‘back, again’ + cedere ‘to go’. Retreat
comes from Anglo-French retrait, past participle of retraire ‘to withdraw’, which
originates from Latin retrahere = re- ‘back, again’ + trahere ‘to draw’. Last, stray
also comes from Anglo-French estraier, from Vulgar Latin extravagare = extra-
‘outside’ + vagari ‘to wander’.
166
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
20
Another possible analysis would have been to code the path verbs within this category as conflating
Co-Motion as well since there are at least two Figures involved in the motion event.
167
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
OED), but not necessarily; and shadow appear to imply secretive motion (TFD,
MW). Within this group of verbs, only pursue appears to be of Latin origin (from
Anglo-French pursure, pursiure, from Latin prosequi = pro- ‘forward’ + sequi ‘to
follow’).
168
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
which denotes a ‘Forward’ type of path. On the other hand, cross and traverse denote
motion across, through the Ground, which seems to be generally a two or three
dimensional entity instead of a point in space.
169
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
‘before’. This verb can display agentive behaviour as well (CALD, TFD, MW,
OED).
m. Multiple directions from a unique start (1 verb, 2.27% of English path verbs):
scatter.
This is a self-agentive, non-agentive and agentive verb whose Figure is usually a
group of entities, and denotes motion far apart in different directions (CALD, TFD,
MW, OED).
In Table 5.5 we present the distribution of Spanish path verbs in our corpus in terms
of the types of path that these verbs encode.
SPANISH
TYPES OF PATH
# %
Away from G 14 22.22%
To/towards G 8 12.70%
Up/Onto G - Upwards 8 12.70%
Down from/to G - Downwards 6 9.52%
Into G 6 9.52%
Back to G/ Backwards 4 6.35%
Change direction 4 6.35%
Closer to G 3 4.76%
Pass/cross G 3 4.76%
After G 2 3.17%
Forwards 2 3.17%
Multiple directions 2 3.17%
Out of G 1 1.59%
TOTAL 63 100.00%
Some types of path are much more frequent than others in our corpus of Spanish path
verbs; for instance, the ‘Away from G’ type is the most frequent, whereas the ‘After
170
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
G’, ‘Forwards’, ‘Multiple directions’, and ‘Out of G’ are the least frequent types. In
our examination of Spanish path verbs, we begin with the most frequent path types as
ranked in the table:
a. Away from G (departure, source) 21 (14 verbs, 22.22% of Spanish path verbs):
abandonar ‘to abandon’, alejar(se) ‘to (cause to) move far away from’,
apartar(se) ‘to (cause to) move away from’, desertar ‘to desert’,
distanciar(se) ‘to (cause to) move away from’, esquivar ‘to avoid, to dodge’,
ladear(se) ‘to (cause to) move away from’, largarse ‘to leave’, marcharse ‘to
leave’, partir ‘to leave’, pirarse ‘to go away (informal)’, retirar(se) ‘to
remove, to take away, to retreat’, zarpar ‘(of a ship) to set off’, ir(se) ‘to go,
to go away’.
This large group of verbs denotes motion away from the Ground. Some of these
verbs can imply that (a) the Figure leaves the Ground behind, sometimes on its own
and in a bad situation (e.g., abandonar ‘to abandon’, desertar ‘to desert’); (b) the
Figure avoids the Ground by moving quickly to one side (esquivar ‘to dodge’,
ladear(se) ‘to (cause to) move away from) and (c) the Figure initiates its motion
away from the Ground (e.g., largarse, marcharse, partir, pirarse ‘to leave’ and
zarpar ‘(of a ship) to set off’).
The verb ir(se) deserves special attention. As DUE and DRAE suggest in
their definitions, ir(se) can denote that (a) the Figure is moving away from the
speaker in a given direction (especially to a place), and (b) the Figure is moving (in
any manner, such as, on foot, vehicle, etc) without regard to its point of departure or
destination, from one place to another. Thus, in (a), ir(se) is a deictic path verb since
it encodes motion away from the relative position of the speaker, but it also can
imply the path ‘to/towards a place’. In contrast, ir(se) in (b) appears to act as a
general motion verb without any specific path.
21
The Spanish motion verbs mudarse ‘to go from one place to another’ and trasladar(se) ‘to (cause
to) move from one place to another’ were coded as general motion verbs (Section 5.4.1). As has
already been pointed out, these two verbs might also be considered as path verbs denoting motion
away from the Ground, as the Figure seems to leave a first location to get to another.
171
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Regarding the etymology of this group of verbs, some verbs are from Latin
origin, such as apartar, partir (from Latin pars, parties ‘part’), and ir(se) (from Latin
ire ‘to go’), whereas others come from other sources. Abandonar comes from the
French verb abandoner, which in turn comes from Germanic banna ‘punishment
order’; marcharse 22 seems to come from the French manner verb marcher; pirarse,
an informal verb which means ‘to go away’, comes from Caló (i.e., dialect spoken by
Spanish Gypsies); zarpar from the old Italian verb sapare. Moreover, alejar(se)
appears to be derived from the adverb lejos ‘far’, thus, alejar(se) ‘to (cause to) move
far, away from’. Last, distanciar(se) is a denominal verb according to Cifuentes-
Honrubia (1999); it comes from the noun distancia ‘distance’ (from the Latin noun
distantia).
22
In contrast to the Spanish manner verb marchar ‘to march, to walk with regular steps’, which seems
to be influenced by French marcher, marcharse as pronominal verb seems to be devoid of any manner
information.
172
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
plicāre, whose meaning was ‘to fold’. To finish, venir (from Latin venire) is a deictic
path verb which denotes that the Figure goes towards the place where the speaker is
situated. According to DUE, when used as a pronominal verb, venirse denotes that
the Figure leaves its prior location to go to where the speaker is.
173
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
These verbs denote either downwards motion relative to the earth or downwards
motion from one place to another. All these verbs come from Latin (DUE, DRAE,
Cifuentes-Honrubia, 1999): bajar comes from the Latin adjective bassus ‘short,
low’; caerse from cadere; derrumbar(se) from derrupare, which derived from rupus
‘rock’; descender from Latin descendere (de- ‘from, down, away’ + scandere ‘to
climb’); desplomar(se) from des- + plomo, which derived from Latin plumbum; and
hundir(se) from Latin fundere ‘destroy, ruin’.
e. Into G (container) (6 verbs, 9.52%): acceder ‘to gain access into’, adentrarse
‘to go into the interior part of’, encerrar(se) ‘to put something, someone or
oneself into an enclosed place’, entrar ‘to enter’, penetrar ‘to enter’,
profundizar ‘to get into’
All verbs lexicalising this type of path are from Latin origin (e.g., verbs, prefixes +
verbs, prefixes + adverbs, adjectives). Acceder comes from Latin accedere ‘to move
closer to’, but its meaning seems to have evolved from ‘to move close to’ to ‘to
enter’. Adentrarse comes from the adverb adentro whose etymological origin is that
of ad- ‘to’ + deintro ‘interior, inside’. Encerrar(se) comes from en + cerrar, which
derive from the Latin prefix in- and the Vulgar Latin verb serrāre ‘to close’. Like the
English path verbs enter and penetrate, Spanish entrar comes from Latin intrāre,
which in turn comes from intra ‘within’, and penetrar from the Latin penetratus, past
participle of the verb penetrāre (from penitus ‘deep within, far’). Finally,
profundizar comes from the Latin adjective profundus (Cifuentes-Honrubia, 1999).
174
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
175
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
dimensional entity respectively). In contrast, atravesar and cruzar only denote that
the Ground can be traversed.
m. Out of G (container) (1 verb, 1.59% of Spanish path verbs): salir ‘to exit’.
Like in our corpus of English path verbs, just one Spanish verb encoding motion out
of a container is found. Spanish salir comes from Latin salire, that is, a manner verb
which meant ‘to jump’. Wälchli (ms) points out that the frequent association of salire
with satellites such as out or up might explain how salire acquired the meaning of
motion out from a container in Spanish, and the meaning of upwards motion in
Italian.
176
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Table 5.6 summarises both qualitative data (i.e., types of path) and quantitative data
(i.e., number of path verbs per type of path and their frequency within the path
lexicon) from our analysis of path verbs in English and Spanish. The table is
arranged from the more frequent to the less frequent types of paths which are
lexicalised in Spanish. By doing so, it is possible to point out both similarities
(whether both languages follow the same tendency in lexicalising some types of path
over others) and differences between the two languages.
SPANISH ENGLISH
TYPES OF PATH
# % # %
Away from G 14 22.22% 11 25.00%
Up/Onto G - Upwards 8 12.70% 5 11.36%
To/towards G 8 12.70% 4 9.09%
Down from/to G - Downwards 6 9.52% 4 9.09%
Into G 6 9.52% 2 4.55%
Change direction 4 6.35% 4 9.09%
Back to G/ Backwards 4 6.35% 3 6.82%
Pass/cross G 3 4.76% 3 6.82%
Closer to G 3 4.76% 1 2.27%
After G 2 3.17% 4 9.09%
Forwards 2 3.17% 1 2.27%
Multiple directions 2 3.17% 1 2.27%
Out of G 1 1.59% 1 2.27%
TOTAL 63 100% 44 100%
We carried out the non-parametric test chi-square to see whether the distributions of
path verbs into 13 types of path in English and in Spanish were significantly
different. Although the two languages differ in the number of path verbs, no
significant difference is found in the distribution of path types across languages (Chi-
square (12) = 4.105, p > 0.05). This suggests that the two languages do not differ in
the organisation of their path verb lexicon.
177
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Bearing this mind, we can begin to answer our research questions regarding
path verbs. One of the two main research questions asked whether the English and
the Spanish path verb lexicons were comparable in size. If we look at Table 5.6, we
could answer: No, they are not; Spanish path verbs outnumber English path verbs. In
the Spanish corpus, 63 path verbs have been found (which amount to the 24.51% of
the Spanish motion verb lexicon), whereas in the English corpus, 44 path verbs have
been identified (which represent 11.70% of the English motion verb lexicon). The
second research question asked whether the two path lexicons were comparable in
their semantic nature. After a close examination of the types of path which are
encoded by these languages, we might conclude that although the two path lexicons
differ in size, they are able to encode the same sorts of path information; in
particular, both languages have path verbs expressing the 13 types of path used in our
analysis and their distributions do not differ significantly.
Our data, moreover, show that Spanish path verbs outnumber English path
verbs in 9 out of the 13 types of path, namely, ‘Away from G’, ‘To/towards G’,
‘Up/onto G – Upwards’, ‘Down from/to G – Downwards’, ‘Into G’, ‘Back to
G/Backwards’, ‘Forwards’, ‘Closer to’, and ‘Multiple directions’. Spanish possesses
more path verbs for some types of path than English. According to dictionary
definitions, the various path verbs within the same type of path might be used for
specific contexts (informal contexts), for certain Figures or even for certain Grounds.
In terms of the ‘Away from G’ type of path, Spanish zarpar seems to be only used
when the Figure is a ship, and pirarse and largarse are informal verbs. Within the
‘To/towards G’, where the Ground is the endpoint of motion, atracar and arribar
appear to be restricted to ships. Regarding the ‘Up/Onto G- Upwards’ path verbs, for
instance, encaramarse may be used for going onto a tree, and encumbrar for
reaching the top of a mountain; the verb subir could be used for these contexts as
well. Finally, in terms of ‘Into G’, acceder seems to be used in contexts where there
is some impediment for the Figure entering the Ground, and encerrar(se) denotes
that the Ground as container will restrain the Figure from going out. Thus, these two
path verbs might be said to be rich in implying and, possibly, in lexicalising force
dynamics (Talmy, 2000a: Chapter 7).
178
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
On the other hand, ‘After G’ path verbs in English (follow, hound, pursue and
shadow) surpass in number their Spanish counterparts (perseguir, seguir). As stated
earlier, hound, pursue and shadow seem to imply some kind of manner of motion
information; hound seems to emphasise the Figure’s relentless pursuit of the second
Figure (TFD); pursue seems to often imply that the chaser intends to capture or kill
the second Figure (CALD, TFD, MW, OED), but not necessarily; and shadow
appears to imply secretive motion (TFD, MW). Have these three verbs been
considered as manner verbs in our analysis, Spanish would have surpassed English
for this type of path as well.
As shown in the table above, both languages present the same number of path
verbs for the following types: ‘Change direction’ (Spanish desviar(se) ‘to divert, to
be diverted’, girar1 ‘to turn’, torcer(se)1 ‘to turn, to change direction’ and virar ‘(of
a vehicle, especially a ship) to swerve’; English divert, swerve, tack, turn2),
‘Pass/cross G’ (Spanish pasar ‘to go through, over, along’, atravesar ‘to cross, to go
through’, cruzar ‘to cross’; English cross, pass, traverse), and ‘Out of G’ (salir ‘to
exit’, exit).
Moreover, our data show that the most frequent types of path in both
languages are ‘Away from G’, ‘Up/Onto G – Upwards’, ‘To/towards G’ and ‘Down
from/to G – Downwards’. This suggests that these types of path (motion away
from/towards G and motion along the vertical axis) are basic spatial distinctions
which are frequently lexicalised in path verbs in both English and Spanish, and by
extension, we might predict that they might also be the most frequent path types for
other satellite- and verb-framed languages. Further research, however, would be
needed to test whether languages have a preference for lexicalising these types of
paths over others in their path verb lexicon. On the other hand, the least frequent type
of path found for English and Spanish is ‘Out of G’, which is encoded by just one
verb per language (exit, salir ‘to exit’). Interestingly enough, ‘Into G’ (motion into a
container), the opposite type of path of ‘Out of G’, is more frequent in both
languages, especially in Spanish, where 6 path verbs denoting motion into a Ground
were found (acceder ‘to gain access into’, adentrarse ‘to go into the interior part of’,
encerrar(se) ‘to put something, someone or oneself into an enclosed place’, entrar
179
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
‘to enter’, penetrar ‘to enter’, profundizar ‘to get into’). There is no apparent reason
why ‘Out of’ seems to be so disfavoured in lexicalisation.
With regard to the etymology of path verbs 23 , Spanish path verbs come
mostly from Latin, but the origin of other path verbs is traced back to French
(abandonar ‘to abandon’, abordar ‘to approach’, marcharse‘to leave’), Arabic
(atracar, encaramar(se)) and Caló (pirarse). English path verbs, on the other hand,
come mostly from Anglo-French and Middle French, which in turn come from Latin.
Some path verbs are from Anglo-French without any apparent influence of Latin
(such as recoil and return, which denote backwards motion), and some other verbs
are from Germanic origin (e.g., arise, leave, mount, rise, scatter). Therefore, some
English and Spanish verbs share their etymology (cf. Talmy, 1985: 72; 2000b), and
hence, they express the same types of path:
On other occasions, English and Spanish path verbs differ in their etymology. In
other words, we have found path verbs of Latin origin in each language which do not
seem to correspond to a path verb in the other. By way of illustration, English exit
(from Latin exire ‘to go out’ = ex- ‘out’ + ire ‘to go’); English stray, which denotes
23
Many of the path verbs from Latin origin were the result of the path prefixes becoming
phonologically fused to verb roots (usually encoding either Motion or Manner of motion), ultimately
arriving at monomorphemic path verbs (cf. Slobin, 2004; Talmy, 2000b).
24
As observed in dictionary definitions, the English verb arrive seems to be used for all types of
Figures (humans, animals, inanimates such as planes, ships, etc) whereas the use of the Spanish verb
arribar seems to be restricted to ships reaching port, and by extension, to people on ships reaching
port.
180
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
motion away from a given path (from Anglo-French estraier, from Vulgar Latin
extravagare = ex ‘outside’ + vagare ‘to wander’); English divert (from Middle
French and Latin; Middle French divertir, from Latin divertere ‘to turn in opposite
directions’); English pursue (from Anglo-French pursure, pursiure, from Latin
prosequi = pro- ‘forward’ + sequi ‘to follow’); Spanish elevar(se) 25 ‘to (cause to)
move upwards’ (from the Latin verb elevare and/or from the Latin adjective levis
‘light’); Spanish partir ‘to leave’ (from Latin partire); Spanish subir ‘to go up’ (from
the Latin verb subire, sub ‘under’ + ire ‘to go’); Spanish salir ‘to go out’ (from the
Latin manner verb salire ‘saltar’), etc. Finally, another interesting crosslinguistic
difference in the etymology of path verbs is the existence of a great number of
Spanish verbs which are made up of adverbs or nouns with or without prefixes
encoding direction. For example, acercar(se) from cerca ‘near, close’; adentrar(se)
from dentro ‘inside’; adelantar(se) from delante ‘in front of’; alejar(se) from lejos
‘far’; aproximar(se) from próximo ‘close’; distanciar(se) from distancia ‘distance’;
encumbrar from en + cumbre ‘peak, top of a mountain’. These verbs appear to
contribute as well to the larger size of the Spanish path verb lexicon, compared to the
English, in which no counterparts have been found.
On the whole, our research on the English and the Spanish path verb lexicons
has started bridging the gap in the existing literature. Our results suggest that though
the Spanish path verb lexicon is larger than the English path verb lexicon—
especially for some types of path—both languages are able to encode the same sort
of path information, as shown in the distribution of types of path across the two
languages. Differences across languages have been noticed in terms of the specific
contexts in which some Spanish path verbs might be used. Also, it has been pointed
out that, in both languages, some types of path seem to be more frequently
lexicalised in verbs whereas others may be dispreferred. Overall, our research
provides support for Özçalişkan’s (2004) claim that the path verb lexicons of both
satellite- and verb-framed languages form a closed lexical category that does not
present much elaboration, but it does not support her claim of comparable size.
25
In English, the verb elevate does exist, but it is an agentive verb of motion. That is why this verb
was not included in our corpus.
181
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
As stated throughout the present work, a great deal of research (e.g., Berman &
Slobin, 1994; Mora-Gutierrez, 2001; Slobin, 1996a, 1996b, 1997, 2004, 2006) has
shown that the English manner-of-motion verb lexicon is highly rich whereas the
Spanish is less so. However, research has quite often failed to look at the type of
fine-grained manner information encoded in English and Spanish verbs from a
comparative point of view. In this section, we are interested in finding out whether
English and Spanish manner-of-motion verbs encode comparable manner details
despite the well attested fact that English manner verbs outnumber those of Spanish
(e.g., Slobin, 2004; Slobin, 2006). Moreover, we intend to discover the sorts of
manner details which are most productively expressed in manner verbs in each
language. Finally, our research also seeks to find out (a) whether the English manner
verb lexicon is equally rich and varied for all subdomains of motion (e.g., human
locomotion or motion using vehicles), and if not, (b) whether the Spanish manner
verb lexicon might be richer than the English for some particular subdomains.
With the purpose of providing a systematic analysis which permits
crosslinguistic comparisons, we opted for grouping manner-of-motion verbs in terms
of the two types of motion the verbs can generally denote: Self-agentive Motion and
Translational Motion. In addition, the manner categories which were listed in the
methodology section were included within the type of motion they pertain to. By
way of illustration, ‘Posture’ is a manner category which is considered as describing
self-contained motion, whereas ‘Motor-pattern’ (e.g., walk, run) typically describes
translational motion. The reader should bear in mind, as has been pointed out earlier,
that manner categories are mutually exclusive, that is to say, manner verbs generally
denote more than one fine-grained manner feature. For example, jog can be analysed
as motor pattern-run (‘Motor-pattern’), slow rate of motion (‘Rate’) and regular steps
(‘Characteristic use of lower body’); stroll as motor pattern-walk and state of the
Figure (relaxed), and so on. This raises some problems when grouping manner verbs
into the proposed manner categories. If we consider stroll for example, the question
that arises is: in which manner category should it be included? In ‘Motor pattern’?, in
‘State of the Figure? Or in a combination of the two? This last alternative would
require an extensive list of all the combinations of manner categories found in our
182
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
corpus, which, in our opinion, would make the comparison between the English and
the Spanish manner verb lexicons unmanageable and less feasible. The other
alternative would be to include the verb into one manner category by taking into
account (a) whether dictionaries agree on one manner detail above all whereas they
were less clear about other manner features; (b) whether verbs denote a specific
motor pattern (walk, run, jump) and further manner details (e.g., ‘Rate’, ‘State of the
Figure’, ‘Characteristic use of lower body’) which just appear to provide further
specificity within each motor pattern, and (c) whether verbs denote contact between
the Figure and the Ground and other manner details which give semantic granularity
to verbs within this category. The latter is the option taken up for our analysis 26 .
Thus, by way of illustration, jog would be categorised in motor pattern-run, and
stroll in motor pattern-walk.
In the following subsections, we begin with the semantics of English manner
verbs, and then go on with the semantics of Spanish manner verbs. Finally, we
compare and contrast both manner verb lexicons in order to uncover crosslinguistic
differences and similarities.
As shown in Section 5.3.1, in our English corpus of motion verbs, 276 manner verbs
(Motion + Manner) were found; these verbs amount to 85.33% of the English motion
verbs in our corpus. For ease of explanation, we first deal with manner verbs
typically denoting self-contained motion (22.83% of the English manner verbs), and
then with manner verbs which generally imply translational motion (77.17% of the
English manner verbs).
Table 5.7 shows the distribution of English manner verbs which express self-
contained motion across seven manner categories (i.e., the number of verbs within
26
Future empirical research would be needed to test the validity of our analysis, in particular to shed
some light on the issue of (a) whether some manner categories (e.g., ‘Motor pattern’, ‘Contact’) might
be more important than others manner features (e.g., ‘Rate’, ‘State of the Figure’) when categorising
certain verbs (i.e., some manner details might be seen as defining features for certain verbs, but not
for others), or (b) whether all manner categories are on equal footing in the semantics of manner-of-
motion verbs.
183
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
each manner category and their proportion relative to the whole corpus of English
manner verbs).
ENGLISH
SELF-CONTAINED MOTION
# %
SHAKE, SWING, VIBRATE 18 6.52%
POSTURE 17 6.16%
Bend, Twist, Curve 6 2.17%
Vertical Orientation 6 2.17%
Downwwards 4 1.45%
Upwards 2 0.72%
Horizontal Orientation 2 0.72%
To one side 2 0.72%
Unspecified 1 0.36%
Roll - Unroll
REVOLVE, ROTATE, SPIN 14 5.07%
UNSTEADY MOTION 8 2.90%
UPSIDE DOWN 4 1.45%
BOUNCE 1 0.36%
ITERATIVE MOTION 1 0.36%
TOTAL 63 22.83%
TOTAL NUMBER OF MANNER VERBS 276
a. Shake, Swing, Vibrate (18 verbs, 6.52% of English manner verbs): flap, flick,
flutter, jig2, quake, quiver, rock, shake, shiver, shudder, sway, swing,
tremble, vibrate, wag, wave, wiggle, wobble.
This manner category includes swinging and oscillatory movements, in addition to
short, sudden and usually iterative movements of the Figure. Some verbs appear to
denote that the Figure shakes because of strong feelings or cold (e.g., quake (CALD,
TFD, MW); quiver, shudder, tremble (CALD, TFD, MW, OED)).
184
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Posture is a rich manner category as it denotes the different body positions assumed
by the human/animal body or by objects treated as comparable to the body. Postural
notions pertain to the vertical and horizontal dimension, that is, whether the Figure
ends up in a vertical (upwards or downwards) or in a horizontal orientation; but they
also relate to whether the Figure bends, twist its body, coils, uncoils, moves its body
to one side, etc. Therefore, this manner category might be subdivided into the
following groups:
b.1. Bend, Twist and Curve: bend, bow, slouch, twist, wind, wriggle.
b.2. Vertical orientation – upwards: arise, rise.
b.3. Vertical orientation – downwards: crouch, kneel, squat, stoop.
b.4. Horizontal orientation: lean, recline.
b.5. To one side: careen1, dodge1.
b.6. Unspecified orientation: stretch.
Among this group of posture verbs, bend, bow, careen1, lean, recline, twist and wind
seem to be also used agentively, that is, when an external agent causes the Figure to
assume a certain body position or orientation.
c. Revolve, Rotate, Spin (14 verbs, 5.07% of English manner verbs): bowl, coil,
pirouette, reel2, revolve, roll, rotate, spin, swirl, trundle, turn1, whirl, twine.
As Levin & Rappaport-Hovav (1992) and Levin (1993) point out for Roll verbs (i.e.,
verbs denoting motion around an axis), verbs within this category can denote both
agentive motion and non-agentive motion, and when accompanied by a prepositional
phrase they can depict translational motion as well. Within this group of verbs,
pirouette does not seem to be used agentively.
185
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
when used with animate Figures, these verbs seem to be able to describe both types
of motion (self-contained and translational); in other words, the Figure can move
unsteadily without changing its location, or it can move in the same fashion while
changing its location.
e. Upside down (4 verbs, 1.45% of English manner verbs): capsize, flip, keel,
overturn.
These four verbs can also be used agentively, that is, an agent can make the Figure
turn upside down or turn over.
186
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
ENGLISH
TRANSLATIONAL MOTION
# %
MOTOR PATTERN 65 23.55%
Walk 47 17.03%
Jump 11 3.99%
Run 7 2.54%
VEHICLE/INSTRUMENT 52 18.84%
RATE 35 12.68%
Fast 29 10.51%
Increase 3 1.09%
Slow 2 0.72%
Decrease 1 0.36%
DANCE 19 6.88%
CONTACT BETWEEN F & G 11 3.99%
Body close to the ground 4 1.45%
Smooth motion 4 1.45%
Slight contact 2 0.72%
Uncontrolled motion 1 0.36%
FURTIVE MOTION 6 2.17%
PLAYFUL MOTION 6 2.17%
PATH SHAPE 3 1.09%
OTHERS 3 1.09%
Gradual stages 2 0.72%
Travel with one's belongings 1 0.36%
Trailing clothes
STATE OF FIGURE 3 1.09%
FORCED MOTION 2 0.72%
Energy-high 1 0.36%
Energy-low 1 0.36%
OBSTRUCTED MOTION 2 0.72%
LEISURELY MOTION 2 0.72%
SMOOTH MOTION 1 0.36%
MOTION IN FILE 1 0.36%
MOTION IN SPORTS 1 0.36%
NOISY MOTION 1 0.36%
UNCONTROLLED MOTION
AWKWARD MOTION
NO AIM IN MOTION
CHAR. USE OF LOWER BODY
USE OF ONE’S HANDS/LEGS
NO AIM IN MOTION
TOTAL 213 77.17%
TOTAL NUMBER OF MANNER VERBS 276
187
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
a.1. Walk (47 verbs, 17.03% of English manner verbs): amble, clump, gimp,
goosestep, hike, hobble, limp, march1, march2, meander2, mince, mosey, pace, pad,
paddle3, parade, perambulate, plod, promenade, prowl, ramble, roam, rove, sashay,
saunter, shamble, shuffle, sleepwalk, slog, somnambulate, stalk2, stomp, stride,
stroll, strut, stump, swagger, tiptoe, toddle, traipse, tramp, trek, troop, trudge,
waddle, walk, wander.
This heterogeneous group of verbs, which is defined by dictionaries as ways of
walking, appears to denote specific information about the following manner
categories:
188
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
a.1.a. ‘State of the Figure’ (19 verbs): in amble the Figure is relaxed and
walks around at a slow rate (CALD, LLA, TFD, MW, OED); in gimp, limp and
hobble, the Figure is injured or physically impaired, and as a consequence, it may
move unsteadily and in an effortful way (CALD, LLA, TFD, MW, OED); march2
denotes that the Figure walks in a determined or angry way; pace encodes that the
Figure is anxious or worried (CALD, LLA, OED); parade is defined as to walk
proudly around as part of a public celebration or display (CALD, LLA, TFD, MW,
OED); hike, promenade, saunter and stroll denote that the Figure takes a walk for
relaxation and pleasure (CALD, TFD, MW, OED); sashay, strut and swagger denote
that the Figure walks in a proud and confident way (CALD, TFD, MW, OED); in
somnambulate and sleepwalk, the Figure walks while asleep (CALD, TFD, MW,
OED); stalk2 encodes the proud or angry attitude of the Figure (CALD, TFD, MW,
OED); stomp and stump denote that the Figure walks with heavy steps so as to show
its anger (CALD, LLA, TFD, MW).
a.1.b. ‘No aim in motion’ (7 verbs): meander2, mosey, ramble, roam, rove,
traipse and wander generally denote that the Figure walks from one place to another
aimlessly (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Moreover, mosey is also defined as a leisurely
motion (TFD, MW, OED). Regarding traipse, Talmy (1985: 137) noted that traipse
encodes a negative attitudinal content; in contrast, dictionaries do not provide any
information about the speaker’s attitude towards the event designated by this verb.
a.1.c. ‘Forced Motion’ (4 verbs): plod, trudge and slog denote that the Figure
moves with difficulty and with great effort, at a slow pace, usually with heavy steps
(CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Clump seems to encode this information together with
information about the noisy motion (CALD, LLA, TFD, MW, OED).
a.1.d. ‘Characteristic use of lower body’ (7 verbs): goosestep denotes that the
Figure moves with regular steps and with the legs lifted high and straight (CALD,
MW, OED). March1 denotes that the Figure moves with regular steps, usually in a
military way (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Mince means to walk with small delicate
steps as in an affected manner (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Tramp seems to denote
that the Figure walks with heavy steps, but unlike trudge, plod and slog, this verb
does not seem to encode ‘Forced Motion’ (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Shuffle denotes
that the Figure moves slowly without lifting the feet from the ground (CALD, LLA,
189
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
TFD, MW, OED) and it might encode some information about the sound it makes
(LLA, OED). Tiptoe denotes that the Figure is walking on tiptoes (CALD, LLA,
TFD, MW, OED), and it might also denote that the Figure does not want to be
noticed (LLA, MW). Finally, stride denotes that the Figure walks with long steps.
a.1.e. ‘Unsteady Motion’ (2 verbs): paddle3 and toddle encode that the Figure
walks unsteadily (MW, OED). On most occasions, unsteady motion is expressed
together with further semantic information in walking verbs (e.g., limp, hobble).
a.1.f. ‘Awkward Motion’ (1 verb): shamble encodes the awkward and slow
walking of the Figure (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Moreover, this verb might denote
unsteady motion (TFD, OED).
a.1.g. ‘Furtive Motion’ (1 verb): prowl denotes that the Figure walks around a
place stealthily, so as not to be noticed (CALD, TFD, MW, OED).
a.1.h. ‘Swinging motion’ (1 verb): waddle is defined as a way of walking
characterised by the Figure’s swinging (CALD, TFD, MW, OED).
To finish, unclear definitions are provided for pad, perambulate, trek and
troop. Pad is defined by LLA and OED as a quiet way of walking, usually without
shoes, but it also seem to denote walking with difficulty (e.g., through mud) (TFD)
and just to journey on foot (OED). Perambulate seems to be an old fashioned verb
which denotes ‘Leisurely Motion’, similar to stroll (CALD, TFD, MW); however, it
is also defined as ‘to go on foot’ (TFD, MW, OED). Lastly, trek is defined as a way
of walking, but dictionaries do not agree on whether it is an arduous journey on foot
(TFD, MW), a long walk over hills, mountains (CALD, LLA) or whether to journey
by ox-wagon (OED).
a.2. Jump (11 verbs, 3.99%): bound, capriole, curvet, hop, jump, leap, leap-
frog, pronk, skip, somersault, vault.
Within this group of verbs, capriole and curvet usually imply that the Figure is a
horse, and pronk seems to be used for antelopes, springboks, etc and denotes to jump
straight up (TFD, OED). Bound and leap might denote that the Figure takes high and
large jumps (CALD, LLA). The verb hop, when the Figure is a human, means to
jump on one leg (CALD, LLA, OED), whereas, when the Figure is an animal
(rabbits, grasshoppers, etc), it means to jump on two or more legs (CALD, OED).
190
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Leap-frog is a verb coming from a children’s game in which some children bend
down and another child jumps over them (CALD, TFD, OED). Skip seems to denote
that the Figures jumps after each step (CALD, LLA), hops on alternating feet (TFD,
MW) or uses a rope while jumping (OED). Somersault refers to an acrobatic jump by
which the Figure turns over (CALD, TFD, OED). Lastly, vault denotes that the
Figure uses its hands or an instrument in its jumping movement (CALD, LLA, MW,
OED).
On the whole, this category seems to be much connected with self-contained
motion in the sense that when the Figure jumps, hops, pronks, etc it does not
necessarily changes its location. Therefore, this category might be said to be defined
by the rising from the ground using the legs.
a.3. Run (7 verbs, 2.54%): gallop1, jog, lope, run, scamper, sprint, trot.
Gallop1 and trot usually refer to the running of a horse or a four legged animal. In
addition, these verbs denote high speed in motion for gallop and a slow rate of
motion for trot. Jog denotes a slow run at a regular speed (CALD, TFD, MW, OED).
According to CALD and OED, lope denotes that the Figure takes long steps in
running. Scamper might denote that the Figure takes small steps while running
(CALD, LLA). Sprint means to move at top speed for a brief period of time (CALD,
TFD, MW, OED).
In light of these data, apparently ‘Jump’ includes more verbs than ‘Run’.
However, as we will point out below, the fact that dictionaries do not agree on
whether some manner verbs (e.g., bolt, canter1, dash, dart, hurry, hurtle, nip, rush,
scoot, scurry, scuttle, spring, streak, whisk, whiz(z), zoom) are or are not running
verbs led us to place these verbs into the category ‘Rate’. This might be the
explanation for the inferior number of verbs for ‘Run’ as compared to ‘Jump’.
191
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
This wide category groups verbs denoting motion on animals, in/on vehicles and
in/on instruments. Canter and gallop denote that the Figure is moving on a horse
which is cantering or galloping. Within the group of motion on/in vehicles, we
observe three main types of verbs: (a) verbs which lexicalise a specific vehicle or
instrument (denominal verbs); (b) verbs which imply the use of some vehicles and (c)
verbs which lexicalise parts of some vehicles, and, as a consequence, they refer to
motion using these vehicles. In terms of the vehicles which are implied by verbs,
cruise implies a (luxury) ship, but the rest of the verbs can imply a wider range of
vehicles (e.g., a car, truck, motorbike, bike, etc). The latter group of verbs (oar,
paddle2, pedal, sail, row, wheel) might be considered as metonymic verbs as a
PART of a vehicle stands for the WHOLE vehicle (cf. Cifuentes-Honrubia, 1999,
2000a, 2000b; Lakoff & Johnson, 1982: 38, Lakoff & Johnson, 1999;).
When analysing this group of verbs, it was observed that some vehicle verbs
could display agentive motion, that is, the vehicle can be used to move entities from
one place to another, whereas other vehicle verbs did not seem to be capable of
denoting caused motion—at least according to dictionary definitions. Moreover, we
became aware of the inconsistencies across dictionaries in this regard. To remind the
reader, in our analysis we coded that semantic information which was present in the
majority of the dictionaries employed. Thus, in the analysis of vehicle verbs, when
three out of the four dictionaries (CALD, TFD, MW, OED) agreed on that a given
vehicle verb displayed agentive motion, we coded ‘AM’ in the column for Agentive
Motion; similarly we coded ‘NO’ when they agree on that they do not express
192
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
agentive motion. Moreover, as can be seen in Appendix 1, ‘AM?’ indicates that two
or just one out of four dictionaries stated that a given vehicle verb could be used for
agentive or caused motion.
After a closer look at vehicle verbs, we found that all dictionaries agree that
(a) bus, ferry, parachute, ship, shuttle and truck can display agentive behaviour or
caused motion, and that (b) bobsleigh, bobsled, cab, dogsled, gondola/e, jeep, kayak,
moped, motorcycle/bike, rickshaw, rollerblade, skate, skateboard, ski and yacht can
only denote that the Figure uses these vehicles or instruments as means of
locomotion. This led us to hypothesise that large vehicles might display both
agentive and self-agentive motion, as they can transport people, goods, etc, whereas
small vehicles might just be used for Figures as means of their own locomotion.
c. Rate of Motion (35 verbs, 12.68% of English manner verbs): accelerate, bolt,
canter1, careen2, career, clip, dart, dash, dawdle, decelerate, flit, hare, hasten, hurl,
hurry, hurtle, lumber, nip, pop, race, roar, rush, scoot, scramble, scud, scurry,
scuttle, shoot, speed, spring, streak, tear, whisk, whiz, zoom.
This category includes a wide variety of verbs. All of them encode information about
the speed of motion, and some of them encode additional manner details. Within this
category, 29 verbs denote a fast rate of motion (e.g., bolt, canter1, careen2, career,
clip, dart, dash, hurry, etc), 3 verbs express an increase in the rate (namely,
accelerate, hasten, hurry), 2 verbs denote a slow rate of motion (dawdle, lumber),
and one verb denotes a decrease in the rate of motion (decelerate).
It should be pointed out that many of these verbs might have been grouped in
other categories. Hurl and hurtle could have been included into ‘Forced Motion’ as
the motion depicted by these verbs requires an effort to be performed. Scurry and
scuttle might also have been categorised into ‘Motor pattern-run’ (i.e., some of the
dictionaries defined them as running verbs); these verbs, moreover, seem to denote
that the Figure takes quick short steps (CALD, TFD, MW, OED). Scramble might
have also been categorised into ‘Forced Motion’, as it denotes that the Figure moves
or climbs quickly with difficulty using its hands and legs (CALD, TFD, MW, OED).
Whiz(z) and zoom might also have been included in ‘Noisy Motion’ as they encode
such semantic information (TFD, MW, OED) besides encoding ‘Rate-fast’(CALD,
193
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
TFD, MW, OED). On other occasions, we opted for including verbs in the ‘Rate’
category when dictionaries disagreed with respect to further details of manner of
motion. By way of illustration, careen2 and career denote fast speed, but dictionaries
do not agree on whether they also express ‘Uncontrolled Motion’ or ‘Unsteady
Motion’ (that is why in our coding we added a question mark ‘?’ to some of the
manner details). The same applies to other verbs such as hare and scud; by looking at
dictionary definitions it is not clear whether hare denotes ‘Motor pattern-run’ and
‘Uncontrolled Motion’ and whether scud denotes ‘Smooth Motion’ besides encoding
‘Rate’.
d. Dance (19 verbs, 6.88% of English manner verbs): boogie, bop, cancan,
conga, dance, foxtrot, jig1, jitterbug, jive, moonwalk2, polka, quickstep, rumba,
samba, squaredance, tango, tapdance, twist2 and waltz.
This group of manner verbs means ‘perform the dance named by the verb’. It should
be noted that not all dictionaries offered definitions for these denominal verbs.
Generally speaking, these verbs might be thought to denote self-contained motion as
the Figure does not necessarily change its overall location. However, as has been
noted in the literature (e.g., Narasimhan, 2003; Narasimhan et al, 2006), dance verbs
can combine with prepositional phrases expressing direction, and thus, they can
easily convey the Figure’s translational motion.
e. Contact between the Figure and the Ground: crawl, creep2, glide, grovel,
slide, slip1, slither, skid, skim, skitter and sweep.
This group of verbs expresses that (a) the Figure’s body is close to the Ground
(crawl, creep2, grovel and sweep); (b) the Figure moves smoothly over/along the
Ground (glide, slide, slip, slither); (c) the Figure loses control over its motion
over/along the Ground (skid); or (d) the Figure is just in slight contact with the
Ground (skim, skitter). Moreover, some of these verbs encode further semantic
information. For instance, slither also encodes that the shape of the path that the
Figure follows is similar to those of reptiles (twisted, curved shape of path); skim and
skitter denote fast speed as well; and grovel denotes that the Figure has a specific
state of mind (humiliated).
194
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
j. State of the Figure (3 verbs, 1.09% of English manner verbs): storm, prance,
and tittup.
27
As we will see in Chapter 6, a study on categorisation of human locomotion verbs provides
psycholinguistic validity to our semantic analysis for these verbs; namely, they are not rated as good
examples of any motor pattern.
195
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
These three motion verbs have been analysed as expressing the psychological state of
the Figure; in storm, the Figure is annoyed and seems to show its anger by moving
violently (CALD, TFD, MW, OED); in prance and tittup, the Figure moves in a
showing-off manner. The dictionary definitions are not clear about the motor pattern
implied in these motion verbs. It is not clear whether the Figure is walking in storm
and tittup, or whether it is walking, running or jumping in prance.
196
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
motion verb lexicon has more verbs expressing this sort of manner information, but
they usually encode other manner details as well. For example, the verbs roar, whiz
and zoom have been grouped in the category ‘Rate of Motion’. An alternative
analysis would have been to include these three verbs here.
In our Spanish corpus of motion verbs, we found 123 manner-of-motion verbs which
amount to the 47.86% of the Spanish corpus. 50 manner verbs (i.e., 40.65% of the
Spanish manner lexicon) have been grouped in terms of categories which typically
express self-contained motion, whereas 73 manner verbs (i.e., 59.35% of the Spanish
manner verb lexicon) encode manner details which are generally associated with
translational motion.
Table 5.9 shows the distribution of Spanish manner verbs which express self-
contained motion across seven manner categories.
SPANISH
SELF-CONTAINED MOTION
# %
POSTURE 28 22.76%
Vertical Orientation 11 8.94%
Upwards 5 4.07%
Downwwards 6 4.88%
Horizontal Orientation 5 4.07%
Bend, Twist, Curve 5 4.07%
Unspecified Orientation 3 2.44%
Roll - Unroll 3 2.44%
To one side 1 0.81%
SHAKE, SWING, VIBRATE 11 8.94%
REVOLVE, ROTATE, SPIN 5 4.07%
BOUNCE 2 1.63%
ITERATIVE MOTION 2 1.63%
UNSTEADY MOTION 2 1.63%
UPSIDE DOWN
TOTAL 50 40.65%
TOTAL NUMBER OF MANNER VERBS 123
197
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
198
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
used in contexts that imply that the Figure moves its body to one side in order to
avoid something.
In general, as Talmy pointed out, Spanish posture verbs denote agentive
motion, but when they incorporate the reflexive pronoun ‘se’, they come to denote
self-contained motion (animates) and non-agentive motion (inanimates). In the
category of posture verbs, four verbs which can only be pronominal (and thus, self-
agentive) when expressing postural notions are also found: acurrucarse ‘to curl up’,
agazaparse ‘to crouch, to hide oneself’, echarse ‘to lie down’, encabritarse ‘to rear
up’.
The rest of the verbs which express self-contained motion belong to six other
manner categories. Unlike ‘Posture’, the following manner categories do not seem to
be frequently lexicalised in the Spanish manner verb lexicon:
b. Shake, Swing, Vibrate (11 verbs, 8.94% of Spanish manner verbs): agitar(se)
‘to (cause to) shake, to move about’, balancear(se) ‘to (cause to) swing’,
campanear ‘to swing’, estremecer(se) ‘to (cause to) tremble, shiver’,
mecer(se) ‘to (cause to) swing, rock’, ondear ‘to undulate, to sway’, oscillar
‘to oscillate, to swing’, sacudir(se) ‘to shake, to shake oneself’, temblar ‘to
shiver, to tremble’, tiritar ‘to shiver, to tremble’, vibrar ‘to vibrate’.
This manner category includes (a) short, sudden movements of the Figure which
might be caused by fear or cold (e.g., estremecer(se), temblar, tiritar), and (b)
swinging and oscillatory movements (e.g., balancear(se), campanear, oscilar).
199
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
d. Bounce (2 verbs, 1.63% of Spanish manner verbs): botar ‘to (cause to)
bounce, rebound’, rebotar ‘to bounce’.
Botar can be an agentive verb whereas rebotar does not seem to allow this usage
(DUE, DRAE). The Figures of these verbs are typically balls.
e. Iterative Motion (2 verbs, 1.63% of Spanish manner verbs): apisonar ‘to roll
flat by moving repeteadly over something, put one's feet down on the ground
repeatedly’, traquetear 28 ‘to (cause to) move repeatedly, to rattle’.
Unlike in English, no verb denoting ‘Upside Down Motion’ was found in our corpus
of Spanish motion verbs.
28
Traquetear also seems to encode some noise as result of the Figure’s movement.
200
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
SPANISH
TRANSLATIONAL MOTION
# %
MOTOR PATTERN 31 25.20%
Walk 24 19.51%
Run 4 3.25%
Jump 3 2.44%
VEHICLE/INSTRUMENT 14 11.38%
RATE 4 3.25%
Increase 3 2.44%
Decrease 1 0.81%
Fast
Slow
PATH SHAPE 4 3.25%
FORCED MOTION 3 2.44%
Energy-high 3 2.44%
Energy-low
DANCE 3 2.44%
CONTACT BETWEEN F & G 3 2.44%
Smooth motion 2 1.63%
Body close to the ground 1 0.81%
Uncontrolled motion
Slight contact
OBSTRUCTED MOTION 2 1.63%
NOISY MOTION 2 1.63%
SMOOTH MOTION 1 0.81%
LEISURELY MOTION 1 0.81%
STATE OF FIGURE 1 0.81%
MOTION IN FILE 1 0.81%
MOTION IN SPORTS 1 0.81%
PLAYFUL MOTION 1 0.81%
OTHERS 1 0.81%
Trailing clothes 1 0.81%
Travel with one's belongings
Gradual stages
AWKWARD MOTION
FURTIVE MOTION
NO AIM IN MOTION
UNCONTROLLED MOTION
CHAR. USE OF LOWER BODY
USE OF ONE’S HANDS/LEGS
TOTAL 73 59.35%
TOTAL NUMBER OF MANNER VERBS 123
201
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
As shown in the table, the majority of manner verbs in our Spanish corpus encode
‘Motor pattern’ (walk, run and jump) and ‘Vehicle/Instrument’, whereas other
manner categories seem to be less favoured for lexicalisation by themselves (though
they might combine with other manner details). We begin with the manner categories
which seem to be most frequently lexicalised in the Spanish manner verb lexicon:
a.1. Walk (24 verbs, 19.51%): andar ‘to walk’, ambular ‘to wander about’,
anadear ‘to walk like a duck, to duck walk’, caminar ‘to walk’, chancletear ‘to walk
noisily using flip-flops’, cojear ‘to limp’, contonearse ‘to walk swaying one’s hips in
a proud way’, deambular ‘to walk around’, divagar ‘to wander’, errar ‘to wander
about’, gatear1 ‘to crawl’, haldear ‘to walk quickly moving one’s skirt/dress’,
marchar ‘to march’, noctambular ‘to walk around at night’, pasear ‘to walk for
pleasure’, patear2 ‘to go on foot around a place’, renquear ‘to limp’, reptar ‘to crawl
or to move like a reptile’, trotar1 ‘of a person to trot’, vagabundear ‘to wander’,
vagar ‘to wander’, zancajear ‘to walk repeatedly from one place to another’,
zanquear1 ‘to walk with crooked legs’, zanquear2 ‘to stride’.
a.2. Run (4 verbs, 3.25%): correr ‘to run’, corretear ‘to run about in a lively
or playful way’, esprintar ‘to sprint’, galopar1 ‘to gallop’
a.3. Jump (3 verbs, 2.44%): saltar ‘to jump’, cabriolar ‘to caper about, to
capriole’, brincar ‘to jump’.
Within this manner category, it can be observed that the vast majority of verbs refer
to ways of walking over ways of running and jumping. Unlike in English, which
carves up these three motor patterns in a more fine-grained or more granular way,
Spanish manner verbs do not encode as much semantic information regarding the
way the Figure moves or changes its location. Regarding the sort of manner details
which are encoded in walking verbs in our Spanish corpus, we find the following
features:
202
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
29
Unlike English crawl, which is defined as the Figure moving close to the ground, the Spanish verb
gatear is defined as a way of walking (DUE, DRAE).
203
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
In terms of running verbs, apart from the hyperonym correr, three verbs are
found in our Spanish corpus: one of them encodes ‘Playful Motion’ (corretear),
another expresses the Figure’s fast rate for a brief period of time (esprintar), and the
third, galopar1 ‘to gallop’, expresses running when the Figure is a horse. Finally,
with respect to jumping verbs, two verbs besides the hyperonym saltar are found.
One of them seems to be used only for horses (cabriolar ‘to capriole’), and the other,
brincar, denotes the same motion as saltar ‘to jump, to push oneself into the air’
(DUE, DRAE). According to the dictionary definitions, jumping verbs seem to refer
to self-contained motion; a Figure can jump without changing its location. However,
if these verbs are followed by prepositional phrases encoding location or direction,
they can also express translational motion.
This wide category groups verbs denoting motion on animals, on/in vehicles and
on/in instruments. Within the group of motion on/in vehicles, we observe (a) verbs
which denote a specific vehicle or instrument; (b) verbs which imply the use of
vehicles; and (c) verbs encoding a part of a given vehicle to refer to the vehicle as a
whole. Like in English, we find some metonymic verbs (i.e., PART of a vehicle for
the WHOLE vehicle): pedalear ‘to pedal’, which refers to motion using a bike or any
204
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
vehicle with pedals, and remar ‘to row, to paddle’, which refers to motion on a boat,
etc.
c. Rate (4 verbs, 3.25% of Spanish manner verbs): acelerar ‘to speed up, to
accelerate’, aligerar ‘to quicken, to hurry up’, apresurarse ‘to hurry up’, desacelerar
‘to slow down’.
The manner verbs in this group, which encode information about the rate of motion,
express notions such as an increase (acelerar, aligerar, and apresurarse) or a
decrease in speed (desacelerar).
d. Path shape (4 verbs, 3.25% of Spanish manner verbs): culebrear ‘to zigzag’,
serpentear ‘to slither, to meander’, rodear ‘to surround with; to go round’,
zigzaguear ‘to zigzag’.
Within this group of verbs, we find two verbs, namely, culebrear (from the noun
culebra ‘snake’) and serpentear (from the noun serpiente ‘snake’) which denote that
the Figure moves along a curved or zigzagged path such as one which is inherent to
the motion of snakes. In sum, by containing the name of a given animal, these verbs
typically denote the path-shape that the animal follows in its motion.
f. Dance (3 verbs, 2.44% of Spanish manner verbs): bailar ‘to dance’, bailotear
‘to dance with little attention or in a careless way’, danzar ‘to dance’.
Unlike in English, Spanish has a verb expressing that the Figure dances in a careless
way (bailotear). It should be noted here that the verbs zapatear and taconear, which
have been grouped in ‘Noisy Motion’, could have been included within this category,
as they are also defined as the tapping of shoes in some dances.
205
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
g. Contact between the Figure and the Ground (3 verbs, 2.44% of Spanish
manner verbs): arrastar(se) ‘to drag something, to drag oneself, to crawl’,
deslizar(se) ‘to (cause to) slide’, resbalarse ‘to slide’.
The first two verbs can be both agentive and self-agentive verbs, whereas the last is a
self-agentive motion verb. In terms of the semantic information they additionally
express, in arrastrar(se) the Figure’s body is close to the Ground; deslizar(se)
denotes smooth contact with the Ground. Resbalarse expresses that the Figure moves
smoothly over the Ground and, according to DRAE, such motion might be
uncontrolled, as when sliding on ice or on a wet floor.
i. Noisy Motion (2 verbs, 1.63% of Spanish manner verbs): zapatear ‘to tap
shoes (when walking or dancing)’, taconear ‘to tap shoes with heels (when walking
or dancing)’.
These two manner-of-motion verbs are both defined as ways of walking (thus,
translational motion) and as ways of dancing (thus, self-contained motion) (DUE,
DRAE). This is the reason why they have not been included in either ‘Walk’ (where
chancletear ‘to walk noisily while using flip-flops’ is grouped) or ‘Dance’.
j. Smooth Motion (1 verb, 0.81% of Spanish manner verbs): flotar ‘to float or
to move smoothly (on liquid, on air)’.
Smooth Motion is a manner category connected to translational motion. However,
this verb does not seem to imply that the Figure is moving along a path, but rather
that it keeps its overall location. Thus, this verb denotes self-contained motion.
206
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
As with English travel, this motion verb does not seem to imply the use of a vehicle,
just the fact of the Figure making a journey, which implies leisurely motion.
The third group of research questions posited at the beginning of the chapter and also
at the beginning of Section 5.5. asked (a) whether the English and the Spanish
manner-of-motion verb lexicons lexicalise similar manner information; (b) which
manner categories are typically or more frequently lexicalised in each language, and
(c) whether English exhibits a higher degree of manner granularity in all subdomains
of motion (e.g., human and animal locomotion, motion of objects or inanimates,
motion in/on vehicles, motion while dancing, posture, etc), and, if this is not the case,
in which subdomain(s) Spanish presents a richer variety of manner verbs.
We can begin to answer these questions by looking at Tables 5.11 and 5.12,
which summarise the distribution of English and Spanish manner verbs in our corpus
in terms of (a) the types of motion they pertain to (i.e., self-contained and
translational Motion), and (b) the fine-grained manner categories that these verb
encode. Additionally, the number of verbs and their percentage relative to the whole
207
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
manner lexicon of which they form a part are included. Table 5.12 is presented on
the next page.
ENGLISH SPANISH
SELF-CONTAINED MOTION
# % # %
POSTURE 17 6.16% 28 22.76%
Vertical Orientation 6 2.17% 11 8.94%
Upwards 2 0.72% 5 4.07%
Downwwards 4 1.45% 6 4.88%
Horizontal Orientation 2 0.72% 5 4.07%
Unspecified 1 0.36% 3 2.44%
Bend, Twist, Curve 6 2.17% 5 4.07%
Roll - Unroll 3 2.44%
To one side 2 0.72% 1 0.81%
SHAKE, SWING,VIBRATE 18 6.52% 11 8.94%
REVOLVE, ROTATE, SPIN 14 5.07% 5 4.07%
BOUNCE 1 0.36% 2 1.63%
UPSIDE DOWN 4 1.45%
ITERATIVE MOTION 1 0.36% 2 1.63%
UNSTEADY MOTION 8 2.90% 2 1.63%
TOTAL 63 22.83% 50 40.65%
TOTAL MANNER VERBS 276 100.00% 123 100.00%
Table 5.11. Distribution of English and Spanish manner verbs (Self-contained Motion)
208
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
ENGLISH SPANISH
TRANSLATIONAL MOTION
# % # %
PATH SHAPE 3 1.09% 4 3.25%
MOTOR PATTERN 65 23.55% 31 25.20%
Walk 47 17.03% 24 19.51%
Run 7 2.54% 4 3.25%
Jump 11 3.99% 3 2.44%
RATE 35 12.68% 4 3.25%
Fast 29 10.51%
Slow 2 0.72%
Increase 3 1.09% 3 2.44%
Decrease 1 0.36% 1 0.81%
FORCED MOTION 2 0.72% 3 2.44%
Energy-high 1 0.36% 3 2.44%
Energy-low 1 0.36%
FURTIVE MOTION 6 2.17%
OBSTRUCTED MOTION 2 0.72% 2 1.63%
SMOOTH MOTION 1 0.36% 1 0.81%
LEISURELY MOTION 2 0.72% 1 0.81%
NO AIM IN MOTION
STATE OF FIGURE 3 1.09% 1 0.81%
DANCE 19 6.88% 3 2.44%
VEHICLE/INSTRUMENT 52 18.84% 14 11.38%
CONTACT BETWEEN F & G 11 3.99% 3 2.44%
Body close to the ground 4 1.45% 1 0.81%
Smooth motion 4 1.45% 2 1.63%
Uncontrolled motion 1 0.36%
Slight contact 2 0.72%
CHAR. USE OF LOWER BODY
USE OF ONE'S HANDS/LEGS
MOTION IN FILE 1 0.36% 1 0.81%
MOTION IN SPORTS 1 0.36% 1 0.81%
PLAYFUL MOTION 6 2.17% 1 0.81%
UNCONTROLLED MOTION
AWKWARD MOTION
NOISY MOTION 1 0.36% 2 1.63%
OTHERS 3 1.09% 1 0.81%
Travel with one's belongings 1 0.36%
Trailing clothes 1 0.81%
Gradual stages 2 0.72%
TOTAL 213 77.17% 73 59.35%
TOTAL MANNER VERBS 276 100.00% 123 100.00%
Table 5.12. Distribution of English and Spanish manner verbs (Translational Motion)
209
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
210
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
211
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
that English and Spanish do not have verbs lexicalising these notions, but rather that
manner verbs encoding any of these manner details together with additional manner
information have been grouped according to the additional information. As pointed
out at the beginning of Section 5.5, manner verbs typically encode more than one
manner detail. This has posed some problems when grouping manner verbs into the
proposed manner categories since it is not clear by looking at dictionary definitions
whether some manner categories (e.g., ‘Motor pattern’, ‘Contact between Figure and
Ground’) are more important than others (e.g., ‘Rate’, ‘Furtive Motion’, ‘Smooth
Motion’) for defining a motion verb, or whether all manner categories are on equal
footing. Therefore, future empirical research would be needed (a) to test the validity
of our analysis, that is, whether manner verbs truly encode the semantic information
we have extracted from dictionaries; and (b) to shed some light on the issues of
whether some types of manner information might be more important than others
when categorising a motion verb (i.e., some manner details might be seen as defining
features for certain verbs, but not for others), or whether all manner categories are on
equal footing in the semantics of manner-of-motion verbs.
Regarding the manner categories which are most frequently lexicalised in
each language, ‘Motor Pattern’ (23.55%), ‘Vehicle/Instrument’ (18.84%), ‘Rate’
(12.68%), and ‘Dance’ (6.88%) are the most frequent categories for English; on the
other hand, ‘Motor Pattern’ (25.20%) and ‘Vehicle/Instrument’ (11.38%) are the
most frequent categories for Spanish. Though English manner verbs generally
outnumber Spanish manner verbs across manner categories implying translational
motion, it can be noted that both languages devote a great part of their verb lexicon
to reference to distinct motor patterns (i.e., basic locomotive abilities by humans and
animals) and to motion using vehicles.
Within ‘Motor pattern’, we observed that walking verbs surpass running and
jumping verbs both in English (47 verbs for ‘Walk’, 7 for ‘Run’ and 11 for ‘Jump’)
and Spanish (24 for ‘Walk’, 4 for ‘Run’ and 3 for ‘Jump’). This suggests that both
languages organise this portion of their verb lexicon in much the same way despite
quantitative differences; English and Spanish seem to be more concerned with
lexicalising fine-grained information about the way the Figure walks than about how
it runs or jumps. Walking is a more basic daily activity; people might run or jump
212
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
sometimes during the day, but most of the time they walk from one place to another.
Thus, it might be hypothesised that most of the world’s languages would be more
likely to possess a more extensive manner verb lexicon for walking activities than for
running or jumping activities.
In terms of the semantic information which is typically expressed in walking
verbs, Table 5.13 summarises the sorts of fine-grained manner information conveyed
by English and Spanish walking verbs.
ENGLISH SPANISH
WALKING VERBS
# % # %
State of the Figure 19 40.43% 2 8.33%
No aim in motion 8 17.02% 7 29.17%
Char. use of lower body 7 14.89% 3 12.50%
Forced Motion 4 8.51% 2 8.33%
Unsteady Motion 2 4.26%
Awkward Motion 1 2.13%
Furtive Motion 1 2.13%
Swinging Motion 1 2.13% 1 4.17%
Iterative Motion 1 4.17%
Noisy Motion 1 4.17%
Contact between F & G 1 4.17%
Use of one's hands/legs 1 4.17%
Rate 2 8.33%
UNCLEAR 3 6.38% 1 4.17%
HYPERONYM(S) 1 2.13% 2 8.33%
TOTAL 47 100.00% 24 100.00%
Table 5.13. Fine-grained manner categories for Walking verbs in English and Spanish
It should be noted that the semantic nature of some walking verbs in both languages
remains unclear. By relying on dictionary definitions alone, it is impossible to fit
these unclear cases into any of the fine-grained categories shown above.
On the whole, both languages seem to lexicalise in their walking verbs
information (a) about the physical or psychological state of the Figure (though
English exploits this semantic information much more often), (b) about the way the
213
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Figure moves its legs (whether the Figure takes long, orshort steps, etc), and (c)
whether the Figure moves about aimlessly, unsteadily, awkwardly, in an effortful
way, etc. In addtion, both languages have a verb expressing the swinging of the
Figure while walking (waddle, anadear ‘to duckwalk’). Apart from these similarities
between English and Spanish, our data reveals some crosslinguistic differences. A
chi-square test was carried out to find out whether the two languages differed in how
they distribute their walking verbs in terms of the 13 fine-grained manner categories
displayed in the table (unclear cases and hyperonyms were not counted). Our results
showed that English and Spanish do differ significantly in their distributions (Chi-
square (12) = 24, p < 0.05). On the one hand, in Spanish, there are some verbs
expressing some sort of semantic information which does not seem to be lexicalised
in the English walking verbs in our corpus: first, within ‘Rate’, haldear ‘to walk
quickly moving one’s skirt/dress’ and trotar1, defined by dictionaries as a fast walk
for both humans and animals; second, within ‘Noisy Motion’, the verb chancletear
‘to walk noisily using flip-flops’; third, within ‘Use of one’s hands/legs’, gatear1 ‘to
crawl 30 , to walk using one’s hands and/or legs’); fourth, within ‘Contact between the
Figure and the Ground’, reptar ‘to crawl like a reptile’, that is, to walk dragging
one’s body; and finally, within ‘Iterative motion’, the verb zancajear ‘to walk
repeatedly from one place to another’. On the other hand, in English walking verbs,
there are some verbs expressing kinds of ‘Unsteady Motion’, ‘Awkward Motion’ and
‘Furtive Motion, which do not appear to be lexicalised in Spanish walking verbs:
first, within ‘Unsteady Motion’, paddle3 and toddle; second, within the ‘Awkward
Motion’ category, shamble encodes the awkward and slow walking of the Figure,
and third, within ‘Furtive Motion’, prowl denotes that the Figure walks around a
place stealthily, so as not to be noticed. To conclude our discussion of walking verbs,
we should stress that although both languages can lexicalise similar fine-grained
semantic information, English exploits some fine-grained manner categories much
more as shown by the larger number of verbs lexicalising mental states of the Figure
and details about the way the Figure moves its legs while walking.
30
Unlike English crawl, which is defined as the Figure moving close to the ground, the Spanish verb
gatear is defined as a way of walking (DUE, DRAE).
214
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
Regarding ‘Run’ and ‘Jump verbs’, both languages have fewer verbs for
these two motor patterns than for ‘Walk’. In our Spanish corpus, only four ‘Run’
verbs (3.25%) and three ‘Jump’ verbs (2.44% of Spanish manner verbs) are
identified. On the other hand, in the case of English, ‘Jump’ verbs (11 verbs, 3.99%
of English manner verbs) outnumber ‘Run’ verbs (7 verbs, 2.54% of English manner
verbs). As pointed out earlier, the lack of agreement among English dictionary
definitions with respect to whether some verbs encoding ‘Rate-fast’ are or are not
running verbs (e.g., bolt, canter, dash, dart, hurry, rush, scurry, scuttle, spring,
zoom, etc), has led us to include all these possible candidates for ‘Run verbs’ in the
category ‘Rate-fast’. The empirical research reported in Chapter 6 (Section 6.4) will
address whether these verbs might be categorised as running verbs.
In terms of the semantic information ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ verbs lexicalise in
English, it has been observed that running verbs include information mostly about (a)
‘Characteristic use of the lower body’: length of steps (long steps in lope, and small
steps in scamper) and (b) ‘Rate’ (fast as in gallop and sprint, or slow as in trot and
jog). Jumping verbs seem to encode information about (a) the length or height of
jumps (bound, leap), (b) the use of one or more legs (hop), (c) the use of one’s hands
or of a pole (vault). In contrast, Spanish ‘Run’ verbs express ‘Playful Motion’
(corretear ‘to run about in a playful way’) and the Figure’s fast rate for a brief period
of time (esprintar ‘to sprint’). Finally, with respect to Spanish ‘Jump’ verbs, two
verbs besides the hyperonym saltar have been found in our corpus: cabriolar ‘to
capriole’, which seems to be used only for horses, and brincar which denotes the
same motion as saltar ‘to jump, to push oneself into the air’ (DUE, DRAE), but
seems to be used in more informal contexts.
Before we move on to deal with other manner categories which are frequently
lexicalised in manner verbs in English and Spanish, we would like to raise the issue
of the semantic nature of ‘Jump’ (Spanish ‘Saltar’). On the whole, this motor pattern
category seems to be quite connected with self-contained motion in the sense that
when the Figure jumps, hops or pronks, it does not necessarily change its overall
location. However, if jumping verbs are followed by prepositional phrases encoding
direction, they can also express translational motion. Unlike ‘Walk’ and ‘Run’,
which seem to need or imply the Figure’s change of location (unless the Figure is
215
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
31
It seems that nouns such as car, baby-carriage and stroller cannot be used as motion verbs.
216
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
activity’. In English, ‘Contact’ verbs (11 verbs, 3.99% of English manner verbs)
expresses that (a) the Figure’s body is close to the Ground (crawl, creep2, grovel and
sweep); (b) the Figure moves smoothly over/along the Ground (glide, slide, slip,
slither); (c) the Figure loses control over its motion over/along the Ground (skid);
and (d) the Figure is just in slight contact with the Ground (skim, skitter). Moreover,
some of these verbs encode further semantic information; for example, slither also
encodes that the shape of the path that the Figure follows is similar to those of
reptiles (a twisted, curved shape of path); skim and skitter denote fast speed as well;
and grovel denotes that the Figure has a specific state of mind (humiliated). On the
other hand, Spanish have 3 verbs encoding ‘Contact’ (2.44% of Spanish manner
verbs): arrastar(se) ‘to drag something, to drag oneself, to crawl’ which denotes that
the Figure’s body is close to the Ground; deslizar(se) ‘to (cause to) slide’ which
encodes smooth contact with the Ground; and resbalarse ‘to slide’, which encodes
smooth motion over the Ground, which can also be uncontrolled. In sum, Spanish
verbs encode some of the fine-grained manner details expressed by English ‘Contact’
verbs, but not all. For example, no verb that expresses slight contact with the ground
or mental state of the Figure is found in our corpus. Regarding ‘Noisy Motion’,
Spanish has 2 verbs (1.63% of Spanish manner verbs): zapatear ‘to tap shoes (when
walking or dancing)’ and taconear ‘to tap shoes with heels (when walking or
dancing)’. These two verbs together with the walking verb chancletear ‘to walk
noisily using flip-flops’, encode cultural manners of motion, which do not seem to be
lexicalised into a single verb in English.
In general, our analysis of the English and the Spanish manner verb lexicons
has shed light on both crosslinguistic similarities and differences between the two
languages. Although the English manner verb lexicon is much larger than the
Spanish one, and thus, it exploits some manner categories much more than Spanish
does, we have found that both languages are able to lexicalise similar types of
manner information. Our analysis has also shown that the two languages distribute
their manner lexicons in different ways. In terms of the two types of motion, we have
pointed out that manner verbs implying translational motion outnumber those
denoting self-contained motion, but this difference was less pronounced for Spanish.
This may be due, in part, to the high proportion of posture verbs in Spanish (22.76%
217
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
of the Spanish manner verb lexicon). On the other hand, in terms of translational
motion, both languages devote the greatest part of their verb lexicon to reference to
distinct motor patterns (i.e., basic locomotive abilities by humans and animals), but
again they subdivide their lexicons differently both within the ‘Motor Pattern’
category and between the other manner categories. In the case of English,
‘Vehicle/Instrument’, ‘Rate’, and ‘Dance’ are the most frequent manner categories
apart from ‘Motor Pattern’, whereas for Spanish, apart from ‘Motor pattern’,
‘Vehicle/Instrument’ is, to a certain extent, frequent as well but ‘Rate’ and ‘Dance’
are not. On the whole, we can conclude that the English verb lexicon seems to have
greater manner granularity than the Spanish verb lexicon particularly in the
subdomains of (a) human and animal locomotion (walking, running and jumping
verbs), (b) motion using a vehicle, and (c) motion while dancing.
5.6. Conclusions
The present chapter has addressed three main research questions relative to the
general semantic distribution of the English and Spanish motion verb lexicons,
relative to their path verbs, and relative to their manner verbs.
The first and most general question asked about which semantic notions are
generally conflated in the motion verbs in our corpus. Our findings suggest that the
conflation of Motion plus one additional semantic component (either Manner or
Path) is the most characteristic of both motion verb lexicons. In contrast, verbs
expressing just Motion, and verbs conflating either two or three semantic elements
along with Motion might be said to be less representative. In general, English and
Spanish motion verbs seem to be to encode a wide range of semantic information
pertaining both to the main Motion event and to the Co-event. However, the
majority of the lexicalisation patterns attested in our corpus of motion verbs seem to
be minor patterns in these languages, that is to say, they are not typical for either
English or Spanish. These include, Motion + Figure, Motion + Ground, Motion +
Cause, and more complex conflations of Figure, Ground and Co-events relations.
Regarding crosslinguistic differences, it has been noted that some minor
lexicalisation patterns are instantiated in one language but not in the other; however,
218
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
a larger sample of motion verbs might yield no differences across the two languages
in this regard. In addition, Spanish tends to express the Ground of motion in their
verbs, either conflated with Motion alone or with additional semantic elements, much
more often than English does (i.e., 31 Spanish verbs, 12.06% of the Spanish corpus,
versus 17 English verbs, 4.55% of the English corpus). On the whole, these findings
contradict Talmy’s speculations on the lack of Ground conflations in motion verb
lexicons. It is true that ground-conflating verbs do not form the core system for
expressing Motion, but it seems to be a minor pattern in both Spanish and English.
The second set of research questions enquired about the size and the semantic
nature of the English and the Spanish path verb lexicons. The first question asked
whether the English and the Spanish path verb lexicons were comparable in size. Our
findings indicate that they are not; Spanish path verbs outnumber English path verbs.
In the Spanish corpus, 63 path verbs were found, whereas in the English corpus, 44
path verbs were identified. In light of our data, the larger number of path verbs in
Spanish might be attributed to path verbs which are used for specific contexts (e.g.,
informal contexts such as pirarse and largarse ‘to go away’), for certain Figures
(e.g., zarpar ‘of a ship, to depart’, arrivar ‘of a ship to arrive’), for certain Grounds
(e.g., encumbrar ‘to reach the top of a mountain’) and path verbs which encode force
dynamics (e.g., encerrar(se) ‘to put something or oneself into an enclosed place’, in
which the Ground restrains the Figure from going out). In addition, we may attribute
the greater number of path verbs in Spanish to a group of path verbs whose
etymological origin is different from English path verbs. We refer in particular to an
interesting group of Spanish path verbs that originated from adverbs and nouns (e.g.,
acercar(se) from cerca ‘near, close’; adentrar(se) from dentro ‘inside’;
adelantar(se) from delante ‘in front of’; alejar(se) from lejos ‘far’; aproximar(se)
from próximo ‘close’; distanciar(se) from distancia ‘distance’; encumbrar from en +
cumbre ‘peak, top of a mountain’). The second research question asked whether the
two path lexicons were comparable in their semantic nature. After a close
examination of the types of path which are encoded by English and Spanish path
verbs, we have concluded that although the two path lexicons differ in size, they are
able to encode the same sorts of path information; in particular, both languages have
path verbs expressing the 13 types of path used in our analysis and the distributions
219
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
of verbs across these path types do not differ significantly. Across the two languages,
the most frequent types of path are ‘Away from G’, ‘Up/Onto G – Upwards’,
‘To/towards G’ and ‘Down from/to G – Downwards’. These four types of path
constitute basic spatial distinctions which might be expected to be the most frequent
path types for other satellite- and verb-framed languages. Further research would be
needed to test whether languages have a preference for lexicalising these types of
paths over others. On the whole, our research on the English and the Spanish path
verb lexicons provides support for Özçalişkan’s (2004) claim that the path verb
lexicons of both satellite- and verb-framed languages form a closed lexical category,
but it does not support her claim on comparable size for path lexicons in the two
types of language. Further crosslinguistic research should address her claims to test
whether they apply beyond Turkish (the language she works on), English and
Spanish.
The third set of questions dove into manner-of-motion verbs and were aimed
at determining (a) whether English and Spanish manner verbs are capable of
expressing similar manner information, (b) what sorts of manner details are most
frequently lexicalised in English and in Spanish, and (c) whether the English manner
verb lexicon exhibits a greater degree of manner granularity in all subdomains of
motion, and if not, in which subdomain(s) Spanish exhibits a greater variety of
manner verbs. Overall, both languages seem to be able to lexicalise a wide range of
semantic information pertaining to the ways in which the Figure moves while
keeping its overall location (i.e, self-contained motion) or when going from its
original location to another one (i.e., translational motion). When analysing manner-
of-motion verbs, we made a categorical distinction between these two types of
motion. Thus, all manner-of-motion verbs were included in one of these two
categories. Our findings suggest that the boundary between self-contained and
translational motion is fuzzy. To put it differently, some motion verbs are defined by
dictionaries as denoting both types of motion; this was especially the case for
English, which seems capable of using any manner verb in a translational motion
frame.
In English, the greater proportion of the manner verbs in our corpus refers to
translational motion, whereas in Spanish, the proportions of verbs expressing self-
220
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
contained motion and translational motion are more balanced. In terms of self-
contained motion, ‘Posture’ and ‘Shake, Swing and Vibrate’ are the most frequently
lexicalised categories in both English and Spanish manner verbs. ‘Posture’, however,
seems to be much more often exploited in the Spanish manner verb lexicon, whereas
‘Revolve, Rotate, Spin’, ‘Upside down motion’ and ‘Unsteady Motion’ are more
often expressed by English manner verbs. In terms of translational motion, ‘Motor
Pattern’, ‘Vehicle/Instrument’, ‘Rate’ and ‘Dance’ are the most frequent manner
categories for English; while ‘Motor Pattern’ and ‘Vehicle/Instrument’ are the two
most frequent categories for Spanish. Although English manner verbs generally
outnumber Spanish manner verbs across manner categories implying translational
motion, we have found that both languages devote a great part of their manner verb
lexicon to basic locomotive abilities by humans and animals (i.e., motor patterns) and
to motion using vehicles. In terms of motor patterns, we have found that walking
verbs surpass running and jumping verbs both in English and in Spanish. This
finding suggests that the two languages organise their verb lexicon along the same
lines despite differences in the number of manner verbs; although English walking,
running and jumping verbs outnumber those of Spanish, both languages seem to be
more concerned with lexicalising fine-grained information about the way the Figure
walks than about the way it runs or jumps. Additionally, we have noted that English
and Spanish walking verbs encode similar manner information, but that English is
much more likely to exploit some fine-grained manner categories; in particular,
manner information about the mental and physical state of the Figure and details
about the way the Figure moves its legs while walking. Surprisingly, we came across
3 Spanish manner verbs which are deeply rooted in the Spanish culture: chancletear
‘to walk noisily using flip-flops’, taconear ‘to tap heels while walking or dancing’
and zapatear ‘to tap shoes while walking or dancing’. These semantic notions (i.e.,
‘Noisy motion’ + ‘mp-walk’/’Dance’) do not seem to be lexicalised in any English
walk or dance verb. As Papafragou et al. (2002: 193) noted, ‘languages differ in their
lexicons in ways that seem intimately related to cultural preoccupations’. In terms of
motion using a vehicle, our analysis has shown that both English and Spanish
possess motion verbs which encode motion on animals, in/on vehicles and in/on
instruments. Furthermore, we pointed out that, within the group of motion on/in
221
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
vehicles, both languages have (a) verbs which denote a specific vehicle or instrument
(e.g., bus, taxi, ship, jeep, esquiar ‘to ski’, patinar ‘to skate’), (b) verbs which imply
the use of particular vehicles (e.g., drive, motor, journey, conducir ‘to drive’) and (c)
verbs which encode a part of a vehicle to refer to the vehicle as a whole (e.g., oar,
row, wheel, pedalear ‘to pedal’, remar ‘to row’). Again, unlike Spanish, English
seems to be highly flexible in the use of almost any vehicle name as a motion verb;
this flexibility extends to the use of dance nouns as motion verbs as well. Therefore,
we can conclude that the English motion verb lexicon has a greater variety of manner
verbs than the Spanish motion verb lexicon in at least three subdomains of motion:
human and animal locomotion, motion using a vehicle, and motion while dancing.
The semantic analysis of the English and the Spanish motion verb lexicons
provides a solid linguistic and typological foundation for an exploration of the
semantics of motion verbs from an experimental point of view. The analysis has
raised a major question concerning the semantic nature of motion verbs: Do motion
verbs mean what dictionaries say they do? This, in turn, raises specific questions in
terms of the psycholinguistic reality of the results of our analysis. By way of
illustration, in the case of verbs which have been analysed as encoding Motion +
Path + Manner (e.g., climb, dive, plummet, plunge, slink, abalanzarse ‘to dash to’,
trepar ‘to climb’), it would be interesting to test whether, according to native
speakers, these verbs encode both Path and Manner information, as it would be
expected from our analysis, or whether these verbs simply encode Manner of motion
which typically occurs with specific paths of motion and thus, the trajectory of
motion is merely an interpretation arising from context. Naming and typicality
judgment tasks using video clips with changed paths and changed manners might
help to unveil the semantic nature of this group of verbs. For example, would an
event be labelled by dive or judged as typical of diving if (a) the Figure does not
move in a rapid, high energetic way and (b) if the Figure does not follow a
downwards path? In the case of Path verbs, it was noted that some English verbs,
namely hound, pursue and shadow, seem to encode manner information besides the
‘After G’ type of path. This might also be tested empirically. However, to address all
222
CHAPTER 5. THE SEMANTICS OF THE ENGLISH AND THE SPANISH MOTION VERB LEXICONS
these questions falls outside the limits of this dissertation and will be left for future
research.
In conclusion, our methodology has enabled us to compare and contrast
English and Spanish motion verbs and to shed some light on the ways these two
languages, often taken as prototypical examples of Talmy’s typology for Motion,
organise their motion verb lexicons. Apart from many similarities in the general
distribution of motion verbs, in their path verbs, and in their manner verbs,
crosslinguistic differences have also been pointed out. We conclude with what
Berman & Slobin (1994: 118) pointed out for their own research, ‘[a]s a general
caveat, it should be remembered that typological characterizations often reflect
tendencies rather than absolute differences between languages’.
223
CHAPTER 6
AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
6.1. Introduction
6.5. Conclusions
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
6.1. Introduction
Motion verb lexicons exhibit a highly complex semantic nature. The semantic
analysis of the English and the Spanish motion verb lexicons carried out in the
previous chapter has provided a solid linguistic and typological foundation to set off
to explore the semantics of motion verbs from an experimental point of view. A
semantic analysis based on dictionaries or on the linguist’s introspections does not
permit a researcher to talk about the speaker’s mental representations of the
meanings of motion verbs. Such fine-grained semantic analysis should be regarded
as a mere instrument for representing the products of linguistic analysis and not as a
mirror of the speaker’s mind (cf. Sandra & Rice, 1995). If these analyses are to be
taken as indicative of the meanings of the words, they must be validated
experimentally. It is necessary to resort to native speakers and carefully designed
empirical work in order to begin to know what actually goes on in their minds.
In the present chapter we are concerned with human locomotion verbs 1 . The
domain of human locomotion is universally experienced and may be structured
around three motor patterns pertaining to motion on land: English ‘Walk’-Spanish
‘Andar’, Eng. ‘Run’- Sp. ‘Correr’, and Eng. ‘Jump’- Sp. ‘Saltar’. Walking seem to
be the most important and basic human motor pattern; it is the default way of moving
for humans, whereas running and jumping seem to be more peripheral as people do
not typically run or jump in order to go somewhere. This chapter reports three
experimental studies on human locomotion whose main goals are (a) to explore the
defining features for each human motor pattern (i.e., ‘Walk’, ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ in
addition to the general category ‘Move’), (b) to provide empirical evidence for the
claim that English and Spanish organise their human locomotion verb lexicons in
similar ways (i.e., larger number of walking verbs over running or jumping verbs),
and (c) to determine the specific motor pattern (‘Walk’, ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’) which is
denoted or implied by a list of verbs from our corpus.
In the first study (the Definition and Feature Listing task) we asked English
and Spanish native speaker to define the verbs move, walk, run and jump, and to list
1
The reader should note that many manner-of-motion verbs, i.e., walk, run, jump and some of their
hyperonyms, pertain to both human and animal locomotion.
225
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
their defining features (i.e., the sorts of manner information which make a given
action be labelled by these verbs). This study aims at eliciting what speakers think
about the meaning of these verbs. In the second study (the Free Verb Listing task),
we asked English and Spanish native speakers to list verbs within one minute time
limit in order to (a) test experimentally the claim that both languages organise their
lexicons along the same lines, and (b) find out prototypical verbs for the general
category ‘Move’ as well as for the three motor pattern categories ‘Walk’, ‘Run’ and
‘Jump’. Finally, the third study (the Rating task) addresses whether a list of English
and Spanish human locomotion verbs are or are not good examples of the three
motor patterns; participants were asked to rate on separate scales whether verb X was
as a way/kind of walking, or running, and of jumping.
6.2.1. Introduction
226
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
6.2.2. Method
Subjects
A total of 51 English speakers (12 for move, 13 for walk, 14 for run and 12 for jump)
and 60 Spanish speakers (16 for moverse ‘to move’, 16 for andar ‘to walk’, 14 for
correr ‘to run’ and 14 for saltar ‘to jump’) took part in the study. English speakers
were undergraduate students at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette (US);
Spanish speakers were undergraduate students at the University of Murcia (Spain).
All participants volunteered to participate.
Procedure
We asked our participants ‘What is to move / walk / run / jump? That is, which are
the features that define the action of moving / walking /running / jumping?’ for
English, and ‘¿Qué es moverse/ andar/ correr / saltar? Es decir, ¿cuáles son las
características que definen la acción de moverse/ andar /correr/ saltar?’ for Spanish
(See Appendix 3). Each participant was asked about only one verb. They were told to
write down their answers on the sheet provided. This task had no time limit.
Design
Language (English and Spanish) and verb category (Move, Walk, Run and Jump)
were the between-subjects independent variables. The number of semantic features
contained in participants’ definitions was the dependent variable.
Coding
In order to consistently name the semantic features which participants either listed or
made explicit in their definitions, we took into account those outlined in the
methodology section in Chapter 5.
227
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
6.2.3. Results
Move / Moverse
Table 6.1 shows the semantic features contained in participants’ definitions for
English move and Spanish moverse as well as the percentage of participants per
language who included each feature.
ENGLISH SPANISH
Translational Motion 26.92% 42.86%
Agentive Motion 19.23% 0.00%
Non-human Figure 19.23% 3.57%
Human Figure 11.54% 7.14%
Self-Agentive Motion 7.69% 3.57%
Self-contained Motion 7.69% 39.29%
Any Path 3.85% 0.00%
Manner 3.85% 3.57%
TOTAL 100.00% 100.00%
As predicted, both English and Spanish participants considered that these verbs refer
to translational and self-contained motion. Moreover, both language groups included
in their definitions that these verbs denote self-agentive motion, motion of human
and non-human Figures (such as animals) and motion in a particular Manner. Unlike
Spanish participants, English participants also made reference to agentive uses of
move (e.g., ‘displacement of a physical object through time or space. Can be
228
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
accomplished through external or internal forces’) and to the possibility of the Figure
following any Path (e.g., ‘to go in many directions’).
Walk / Andar
As Table 6.2 illustrates, English walk and Spanish andar are generally defined as
particular ways of locomotion by moving one’s legs/feet in order to go somewhere;
‘Use of legs, feet’ and ‘Translational Motion’ were the most frequent defining
features across the two language groups. These results support our predictions. It
should also be noted that a higher percentage of Spanish participants talked about
‘Translational Motion’ (i.e., Spanish, 41.18% vs. English, 26.32%). In addition, these
verbs were also defined in terms of a slow rate of motion (e.g., ‘non strained pace’,
‘slow pace’), and explicit mention to human Figures was common, although Spanish
participants also included animals in their definitions: ‘acción que implica un
movimiento animal o humano a través de las piernas’, lit. ‘action that implies an
animal or human movement by using the legs’). Unlike walk, Spanish andar is
additionally described as a self-agentive motion verb (e.g., ‘desplazarse
voluntariamente’, lit. ‘to change one’s location voluntarily’) in which the Figure can
follow any Path (e.g., ‘en cualquier dirección’ lit. ‘in any direction’).
ENGLISH SPANISH
Use of legs, feet 47.37% 29.41%
Translational Motion 26.32% 41.18%
Rate 21.05% 5.88%
Human Figure 5.26% 5.88%
Any Path 0.00% 5.88%
Non-human Figure 0.00% 5.88%
Self-Agentive Motion 0.00% 5.88%
TOTAL 100.00% 100.00%
229
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
Run / Correr
Interestingly, seven English participants defined run in terms of walking and three
did in terms of jogging 2 . Five Spanish participants defined correr in terms of
walking 3 . Table 6.3 summarises the semantic features contained in participants’
definitions for these verbs and the total percentage of participants who mentioned
each feature.
ENGLISH SPANISH
Rate 30.77% 41.18%
Use of legs, feet 20.51% 17.65%
Translational Motion 15.38% 32.35%
Use of arms 12.82% 0.00%
Human Figure 7.69% 2.94%
Any Path 5.13% 2.94%
Non-human Figure 5.13% 0.00%
Push off ground into air 2.56% 2.94%
TOTAL 100.00% 100.00%
On the whole, ‘Rate’ is the most frequent defining feature across the two languages.
Furthermore, participants frequently described run and correr as motion verbs
denoting that the Figure goes from one place to another (i.e., translational motion).
Thus, as predicted, fast rate of motion and translational motion seem to be part of the
meaning of run and correr. As with walk-andar, Spanish participants made reference
to ‘Translational Motion’ more often than English participants (i.e., Spanish, 32.35%
vs. English, 15.38%). Additionally, English participants specified in their definitions
2
As examples, English participants provided these definitions: ‘it is similar to walking but faster’, ‘to
run is to use one's legs as a form of locomotion, but at a faster rate than walking’, ‘to run is to move
faster than jogging’.
3
By way of illustration, Spanish participants provided definitions such as: ‘desplazarse rápidamente,
caminar muy deprisa’, literally, ‘to change one’s location quickly, to walk very fast/quickly’; ‘modo
natural de desplazarse un ser humano, parecido a caminar’, literally, ‘natural way of human
locomotion, similar to walk’.
230
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
the way in which the Figure moves its arms, such as ‘arms move back and forth
while bent at elbow’ and ‘to run is to move quickly while bending your legs. You use
your arms and go very fast’.
Jump / Saltar
Table 6.4 summarises the semantic information contained in participants’ definitions
for jump and saltar and the percentage of participants who mentioned each feature in
their definitions.
ENGLISH SPANISH
Push off ground into air 29.63% 27.03%
Vertical Path 25.93% 21.62%
Forced motion 18.52% 10.81%
Use of legs, feet 18.52% 27.03%
Human Figure 3.70% 0.00%
Use of arms 3.70% 0.00%
Avoid an obstacle 0.00% 2.70%
Horizontal Path 0.00% 5.41%
Rate of Motion 0.00% 2.70%
Translational Motion 0.00% 2.70%
TOTAL 100.00% 100.00%
As we saw with run-correr, participants described motion verbs using other motion
verbs; one English participant defined jump using hop 4 and another using bounce 5 .
These motion verbs are manner-of-motion verbs; the former was analysed in Chapter
5 as a way of jumping (‘Motor Pattern’ category within translational motion)
4
The participant’s definition was: ‘Hop. Moving up and down on your two feet in place. Movement
of the two feet and legs at the same time’.
5
The participant’s definition was: ‘Bouncing up and down, being lifted off the ground by one’s own
force. A person going up off the ground, then coming back down’.
231
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
whereas the latter was included within the self-contained manner category ‘Bounce’.
In Spanish, one participant defined saltar by relating it to caminar 6 ‘to walk’.
On the whole, the first four features, namely, ‘Push off ground into air’,
‘Vertical Path’, ‘Forced Motion’ and ‘Use of legs, feet’ were the most frequently
mentioned features in participants’ definitions across the two languages.
Additionally, English participants made explicit mention of a human Figure which
might use his/her arms in order to jump higher; Spanish participants included in their
definitions the possibility of avoidance of an obstacle, horizontal paths (forwards)
and fast rate of motion. Unlike in definitions for move, walk and run, only one
Spanish participant mentioned that saltar denotes the Figure’s change of location
(‘Translational Motion’). Therefore, it might be concluded that translational motion
does not seem to be a defining feature for jump, at least according to our English
participants, though it might be for Spanish saltar.
In line with our predictions, move and moverse are motion verbs which denote both
translational and self-contained motion; walk and andar are typically defined as the
most basic human motor pattern in order to change one’s location; run and correr are
translational motion verbs characterised by a fast way of moving by using one’s legs;
and jump and saltar are motion verbs defined by a Figure’s pushing off the ground.
Despite the similarities in their definitions, some crosslinguistic differences
have also been pointed out. In particular, English speakers included in their
definitions information about the manner in which the Figure moves his/her arms
while walking, and occasionally while running and jumping. Explicit mention of this
sort of manner information is lacking from Spanish definitions. This suggests that
Spanish speakers might pay less attention to additional manner details of the
walking, running or jumping events. Moreover, it was noted that Spanish participants
generally mentioned ‘Translational Motion’ in their definitions more frequently than
English participants; this might suggest that the Figure’s change of location is a more
6
The participant’s definition was: ‘desplazarse dando saltos, caminar dando saltos’, literally, ‘to
change one’s location by giving jumps, walk giving jumps’.
232
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
prominent part of the semantics of these Spanish motion verbs. Finally, it was found
that, unlike in definitions for move, walk and run, none of the English participants
and only one Spanish participant mentioned that saltar denotes the Figure’s change
of location. In consequence, it might be argued that translational motion does not
seem to be a defining feature for jump but it might be so for its Spanish counterpart.
Coming back to our discussion about these verbs in the previous chapter, it should be
recalled that dictionary definitions included references to both translational motion
and self-contained motion for jump and saltar, but we argued that jumping verbs are
very much connected to self-contained motion as the Figure does not necessarily
change its location while performing these activities. All in all, it could be concluded
that jump and saltar are self-contained motion activities which can or cannot occur in
the course of the Figure’s translational motion (e.g., while walking, running, etc),
whereas walk-andar and run-correr typically are translational motion activities.
Interestingly enough, English and Spanish participants defined run-correr
and jump-saltar in terms of other motion verbs. Run and correr were defined in
terms of walking with the explicit mention of a faster speed; this might be interpreted
as if run and correr are rapid ways of walking. Jump was defined by mentioning two
specific manner verbs, whereas saltar was defined in relation to andar ‘to walk’.
This suggests that saltar might be seen as a particular way of moving which tends to
take place while walking, and thus, this might explain why Spanish saltar, unlike its
English counterpart, was defined as a translational motion verb.
On the whole, this experimental study provides us with the first insights into
speakers’ representations of some basic motion verbs. In addition, it examined how
the different types of semantic information employed for the semantic analysis in
Chapter 5 come into play when examining speakers’ definitions of these motion
verbs.
233
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
6.3.1. Introduction
The semantic analysis of English and Spanish motion verb lexicons has
shown that both languages exhibit greater manner granularity of walking verbs over
running and jumping verbs. In this section we report an experimental study in which
we intend to provide empirical evidence for these findings. The method used is
known as ‘Free Listing’ or ‘The Production Method’. Categorisation research during
the 70s (cf. Battig & Montague, 1969; Freedman & Loftus, 1971; Rosch, 1973,
1975) showed that participants list the best examples or prototypical members of a
category first. Furthermore, it has been found that (a) the items which were listed
first are more entrenched and more frequently used than the ones listed later or even
no listed at all, and that (b) the mean number of listed items is correlated with the
total number of items in the category. Therefore, participants would list more items
for wider categories (e.g., ‘Move’) than for narrower ones (e.g., ‘Jump’).
The research questions of this study are (1) whether English speakers would
list more manner-of-motion verbs for all categories than would Spanish speakers; (2)
whether, across languages, the Move category would contain the greatest variety of
manner-of-motion verbs, and (3) whether the Walk category would be richer or more
varied in terms of manner verbs than the Run or Jump categories. Additionally, we
would like to identify the prototypical verbs for each motor pattern category in
addition to the prototypical verbs for the superordinate category ‘Move’.
Our predictions are the following: first, English speakers will list far more
manner verbs for each category than will Spanish participants, as the English manner
lexicon is richer. Second, for both languages, participants will list more manner verbs
for the ‘Move’ category than for the other three categories as move is their
superordinate verb. Third, a higher number of manner verbs will be listed for the
‘Walk’ category over the ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ categories, as walking is hypothesised to
be a more basic human motion activity than running and jumping.
234
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
6.3.2. Method
Subjects
The participants in Study 1 (the Definition and Feature Listing task) also participated
in this study. Care was taken to ensure they did not get the same verb in both tasks.
64 English speakers (19 for ‘Move’, 15 each for the categories ‘Walk’, ‘Run’, and
‘Jump’) and 68 Spanish speakers (17 for each verb category) took part in the study.
English speakers were undergraduate students at the University of Louisiana at
Lafayette (US); Spanish speakers were undergraduate students at the University of
Murcia (Spain). All participants volunteered to participate.
Procedure
We asked our participants ‘In what ways can a human move / walk / run / jump?’ for
English and ‘¿De qué modos puede una persona moverse / andar / correr / saltar?’ for
Spanish. Each participant was asked about only one verb (either move or walk or run
or jump). The instructions were given in written form (see Appendix 3). Participants
were told to write down all the verbs which answer the question on the sheet
provided. A time limit of 1 minute was given.
Design
Language (English and Spanish) and verb category (Move, Walk, Run and Jump)
were between-subjects independent variables. The number of listed motion verbs was
the dependent variable.
Coding
Our participants listed verbs which do not pertain to motion (e.g., sing, talk, think,
touch, jugar ‘play’, titubear ‘to hesitate’), as well as adverbs (e.g., fast, slow, high,
far, backwards, sideways, upside down, despacio ‘slow’), noun phrases (e.g., one
foot, both feet, inside, salto de longitud ‘long jump’) and prepositional phrases (e.g.,
a saltos ‘by jumps’, de espaldas ‘by going backwards’, de puntillas ‘on tiptoes’).
Moreover, participants listed a wide range of motion verbs other than manner verbs
(e.g., path verbs, verbs encoding Figure, general motion verbs, etc) and collocations
or complex verb phrases (e.g., make your way, drag your feet, hacer footing lit. ‘do
235
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
footing’, darse prisa ‘hurry up’). These items were left out from the analyses which
were carried out to test the three predictions regarding the size of the manner-of-
motion lexicons in English and Spanish. However, their presence across subjects
should not be overlooked 7 and their occurrences in both languages have been
quantified in the next section.
6.3.3. Results
LISTED ITEMS
For clarity of exposition, we begin with what English speakers responded when
asked about each verb category and then we move on with Spanish speakers’
responses. After this, a summary table will be presented (Table 6.5).
‘Move’: 19 English participants listed 186 items, out of which 168 were all
motion verbs (90.32% of participants’ responses, 63 verb types). The other 18 items
(9.68% of participants’ responses) were: 5 non-motion verbs (babble, darf?, sing,
talk, touch, write), 3 adverbs (quick, fast (2 tokens), slow(2 tokens)), 2 noun phrases
(both feet, one foot), and 5 adverbs 8 (backwards, forwards, sideways, upside down,
rightside up). Within motion verbs, exercise, fight, kick (2 tokens), knee, nod and
punch were listed in addition to 3 path verb types (fall (3 tokens), go (2 tokens),
reach) and 53 manner types. Manner verbs amount to 155 occurrences or tokens
(83.33% of the responses): bend (2) 9 , bicycle, bike, bounce (2), climb, crawl, dance
(6), drive (3), duck-walk, flee, fly (5), glide (2), hop (6), jog (10), jump (12), leap (2),
pace, run (18), shake (2), shiver, shuffle, skate (2), ski (3), skip (9), slide (3), slither,
7
Two participants for English walk, six each for English run and jump only listed adverbs and
satellites. One participant for Spanish run and three for Spanish jump only listed adverbials, namely,
adverbs, prepositional phrases and noun phrases encoding manner information.
8
This group of adverbs denotes that the Figure is moving along an unspecified path which seems to be
defined in terms of the frame of reference of the Figure, and not relative to an external frame of
reference. It could be argued, however, that they also refer to the way the Figure is moving, for
example, in backwards the Figure moves facing in the opposite direction (cf. Feist, submitted).
9
The number in brackets refers to the number of occurrences or tokens, that is, the number of times
that the item was listed by participants. When an item is not followed by a number, it means that it
was just listed once across participants.
236
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
snowboard, spring, sprint, squat, step, stomp, stretch, stroll (3), strut, stumble, sway
(2), swim (6), swing (2), teeter, throw, tiptoe (2), totter, trip (2), trot, turn (2), twirl,
twist, waddle, walk (16), waltz (2), wave, wiggle, wobble (2).
‘Walk’: 15 English participants listed 90 items, out of which 71 were motion
verbs (78.89% of participants’ responses, 42 verb types) and 19 items (21.11%, 12
types) were adverbs expressing manner (11 types: angry, brisk (2), goofy, loud, quick
(2), soft, tiresome, slowly, hurriedly, slow (3), fast (4)) and a non-motion verb
(think). Within motion verbs, 2 complex verb phrases (drag your feet, make your
way), 2 agentive verbs (lift, carry), the general motion verb move (5 tokens), 2 Path
verbs (go, fall), the verb walk and 34 manner verb types were listed (65.56% of the
total responses, 59 occurrences). Participants listed the following manner verbs:
amble, careen, frolick, gallop (3), glide, hop (3), jog (3), jump, limp, lope, lumber,
march, moonwalk, pace (2), powerwalk, propel, run (7), saunter, sidle, skip (4),
slide, slither (2), sneak, sprint, stamp, step (2), storm, stretch, stride (2), stroll (3),
strut (4), travel, trot, waddle. Note that the verb walk was listed once.
‘Run’: 15 English participants listed 52 items, out of which 24 were motion
verbs (46.15% of participants’ responses, 12 verb types) and 28 items (53.85% , 15
types) were: 1 noun (inside), 6 satellites (away, up, to, into, from, down), 18
occurrences of adverbs (quickly, slowly, quietly, powerfully, fast, slow) and two
prepositional phrases (around the block, in shoes). In addition, within the group of
motion verbs, the verb move (1 occurrence) and 11 manner verb types were listed
with a total of 23 occurrences (44.23% of all responses): dart, gallop (2), hop, jog
(5), leap, race (2), skip (3), speed, sprint (3), trample, walk (3).
‘Jump’: 15 English participants listed 95 items, out of which 42 were motion
verbs (44.21% of participants’ responses, 13 verb types) and 53 items (57.61%, 39
types) were as follows: 1 non-motion verb (make air), 18 occurrences of adverbs and
adjectives (one-legged, aggressive, elongated, graceful, long, loud, mass up, pretty,
short, weak, fast, high, slow, low), 22 occurrences of satellites (above, across,
around, away, down, from, into, off, onto, out, over, round, to, under, up), and 12
occurrences of adverbs (back, backward, far, forward, forward and backward, near,
side-to-side, sideways, up and down). These items amount to 57.61% of English
participants’ responses. Within motion verbs, the verb jump was listed once, 2 other
237
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
motion verbs (push, release), 2 path verbs (lift (2), rise) and 9 manner verb types
were listed with a total of 36 occurrences (bounce (5), bound (2), flip, glide, hop (9),
leap (8), run, skip (7), spring (2)). Manner verbs amount to 37.89% of participants’
responses.
‘Moverse’: 17 Spanish participants listed 145 items, all of which were verbs
(45 verb types). Within these verbs, 2 non-motion verbs (soñar ‘to dream’, jugar ‘to
play’), 7 complex verb phrases (hacer footing lit.‘do footing’, hacer gestos ‘make
gestures’, dar un puñetazo ‘give a punch’, ir de puntillas (2 tokens) ‘to go on
tiptoes’, ir agachado ‘to go with a crouched posture’, ir a la pata coja ‘to go by
moving on one leg’, saltar a la pata coja ‘to jump on one leg’), 19 occurrences of
Path verbs (ir ‘to go’, bajar ‘to go down’, entrar ‘to enter’, subir ‘to go up’, salir ‘to
exit’, volver ‘to go back’), 1 motion verb encoding an unspecified ground (recorrer
‘to move around an area’), 2 general motion verbs (moverse ‘to move’, desplazarse
‘to change one’s location’) and 32 manner verb types were listed. These manner
verbs amount to 113 occurrences, that is, 77.93% of the participants’ responses:
agacharse ‘to crouch’, andar ‘to walk’ (16) , arrastrarse ‘to drag oneself’ (6), bailar
‘to dance’ (4), balancearse ‘to swing oneself’, brincar ‘to jump (informal)’ (4),
bucear ‘to dive’ (3), caminar ‘to walk’ (7), cojear ‘to limp’, conducir ‘to drive’,
correr ‘to run’ (17), deslizarse ‘to slide’ (3), esquiar ‘to ski’, gatear ‘to crawl’ (5),
girarse ‘to turn oneself’, haldar ‘to walk quickly while wearing skirts, dresses’, huir
‘to flee’, levantarse ‘to get up’, nadar ‘to swim’ (4), pasear ‘to stroll’ (6), patear ‘to
go on foot around a place for a long time’, patinar ‘to skate’, pedalear ‘to pedal’,
reptar ‘to crawl, slither’, saltar ‘to jump’ (16), sentarse ‘to sit oneself down’,
tambalearse ‘to move unsteadily’, temblar ‘to tremble, to shiver’, trepar ‘to climb, to
scale’, trotar ‘to trot’ (3), viajar ‘to travel’ (3), volar ‘to fly’.
‘Andar’: 17 Spanish participants listed 82 items, out of which 68 were
motion verbs (82.93% of participants’ responses, 26 verb types) and 14 items
(17.07% of participants’ responses, 8 types) were 1 non-motion verb (titubear ‘to
hesitate’), 6 occurrences of 2 adverbs (despacio ‘slow’, rápido ‘fast’) and 7
occurrences of prepositional phrases (a gatas ‘in a crawling posture’, a pie ‘on foot’,
de espaldas ‘backwards’ (2), de puntillas ‘on tiptoes’ (2), de rodillas ‘on one’s
238
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
knees’). Further, within the motion verbs, participants listed andar ‘to walk’ (3
tokens), mover, moverse ‘to move’, 1 motion verb encoding an unspecified ground
(recorrer ‘to move around an area’ (2 tokens)), 1 Path verb (cruzar ‘to cross’), 3
complex verb phrases (dar saltitos lit. ‘to give small jumps’, dar tumbos ‘to go from
a place to another with no purpose’, arrastrar los pies ‘to drag one’s feet’) and 18
manner verb types. These manner verbs amount to 57 ocurrences (69.51% of
participants’ responses): arrastrarse ‘to drag oneself’ (2), brincar ‘to jump
(informal)’ (2), caminar ‘to walk’ (5), cojear ‘to limp’, correr ‘to run’ (14),
corretear ‘to run about (playfully)’, deambular ‘to wander about’, deslizarse ‘to
slide’, esprintar ‘to sprint’, gatear ‘to crawl’ (3), pasear ‘to stroll’ (7), patear ‘to go
on foot around a place for a long time’ (2), pedalear ‘to pedal’, rondar ‘to be on
patrol, to prowl about’, saltar ‘to jump’ (9), trotar ‘to trot’ (4), vagabundear ‘to
wander’, vagar ‘to wander’.
‘Correr’: 17 Spanish subject listed 63 items, out of which 45 were motion
verbs (71.43% of participants’ responses, 19 verb types) and 18 items (28.57% of
participants’ responses, 15 types) were 1 non-motion verb (respirar ‘to breathe’), 11
occurrences of adverbs and adjectives (deprisa ‘fast’, despacio ‘slow’, lenta ‘slow’,
lentamente ‘slowly’, lentísima ‘really slow’, muy lenta ‘really slow’, rapidísimo
‘really fast’, rápido ‘quick, fast’, super lento ‘really slow’) and 6 occurrences of
prepositional phrases (a pasos cortos ‘with short steps’, a pasos largos ‘with long
steps’, a saltos ‘with jumps’, con ayuda ‘with help’, en sprint ‘in sprint’). Within
motion verbs, 4 complex verb phrases (hacer footing lit. ‘to do jogging’ (3), darse
prisa ‘to hurry up’), the general verb moverse ‘to move oneself’, the verb correr ‘to
run’ and 15 manner verb types were listed. These manner verbs amount to 36
occurrences (57.14% of responses): acelerar ‘to speed up’ (3), aligerar ‘to hurry up’
(2), andar ‘to walk’ (3), apresurarse ‘to hurry up’ (2), arrastrarse ‘to drag oneself’,
brincar ‘to jump (informal)’, caminar ‘to walk’ (2), corretear ‘to run about’, galopar
‘to gallop’(5), gatear ‘to crawl’, huir ‘to flee’, pasear ‘to stroll’, saltar (2), trotar ‘to
trot’ (10), zanquear ‘to stride’.
‘Saltar’: 17 Spanish subjects listed 68 items, out of which 43 were verbs
(63.24% of participants’ responses, 25 verb types) and the remaining of the 25 items
(36.76%, 25 types) were 2 noun phrases (pies juntos ‘both feet together’, salto de
239
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
longitud ‘long jump’), 13 occurrences of adverbs and adjectives (alto ‘high’, bajo
‘low’, bien ‘good, well’, cerca ‘close, near’, flojo ‘weak’, fuerte ‘strong’, largo
‘long’, lejos ‘far’, mal ‘badly’, mucho ‘a lot’, muy alto ‘very high’, muy bajo ‘very
low’, poco ‘little’), 6 prepositional phrases expressing manner (a la comba ‘using a
rope’, a la pata coja ‘to go by moving on one leg’, a pies juntillas ‘both feet
together’, a zancadas ‘with strides or long steps’, de cabeza ‘headfirst, headlong
manner’, de forma rapida ‘in a fast way’), and 4 prepositional phrases 10 (hacia
arriba ‘upwards’, hacia delante ‘forwards, to the front’, hacia detrás ‘backwards’,
hacia un lado ‘to one side’). In addition, 6 complex verb phrases (abrirse de piernas
‘to open one’s legs’, articular los huesos ‘to articulate one’s bones’, dar volteretas
lit. ‘to give turns’, dar saltos lit. ‘to give jumps’, desplegar las piernas ‘to stretch
one’s legs out’, flexionar las piernas ‘to bend one’s legs’), 3 Path verbs (subir ‘to go
up, ascend’, arrimarse ‘to go closer to’, avanzar ‘to advance’ (2)), 2 general motion
verbs (moverse ‘to move oneself’, desplazarse ‘to change one’s position’), the verb
saltar ‘to jump’ and 13 manner verb types were listed (25 occurrences, 36.76% of
their total responses). The listed manner verbs were the following ones: agacharse
‘to crouch oneself’ (2), andar ‘to walk’, bailar ‘to dance’, botar ‘to bound’ (2),
brincar ‘to jump (informal)’ (7), cojear ‘to limp’, correr ‘to run’ (2), girar ‘to turn’
(2), impulsarse ‘to propel oneself’ (3), rebotar ‘to rebound’, tambalearse ‘to move
unsteadily’, tirarse ‘to throw oneself’, trotar ‘to trot’.
Table 6.5 summarises the percentage of listed motion verbs and other listed
items per language and verb category. Moreover, the total percentage of manner verb
responses is also displayed.
10
As seen with the English adverbs forwards, backwards, sideways, etc, these prepositional phrases
denote a path of motion relative to the Figure. They might be considered deictic paths.
240
ENGLISH SPANISH
MOVE MOVERSE
Other items 9.68% Other items 1.38%
Motion verbs 90.32% Motion verbs 98.62%
Manner verbs 83.33% Manner verbs 77.93%
WALK ANDAR
Other items 21.11% Other items 17.08%
Motion verbs 78.89% Motion verbs 82.93%
Manner verbs 69.56% Manner verbs 69.51%
RUN CORRER
Other items 53.85% Other items 28.57%
Motion verbs 46.15% Motion verbs 71.43%
Manner verbs 44.74% Manner verbs 57.15%
JUMP SALTAR
Other items 55.79% Other items 36.76%
Motion verbs 44.21% Motion verbs 63.24%
Manner verbs 37.89% Manner verbs 36.76%
Table 6.5. General distribution of listed items per verb category in English and in Spanish
first. Thus, by examining the motion verbs which were listed as first items, the
prototypical verbs for these four categories can be singled out.
Table 6.6 shows all motion verbs, but also other lexical units (e.g., non-
motion verbs, adverbs) which were listed as first item per verb category and per
language. Furthermore, it displays the number of tokens or occurrences and the
frequency as first listed item across participants.
242
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
ENGLISH SPANISH
VERB CATEGORY # % VERB CATEGORY # %
MOVE 19 100.00% MOVERSE 17 100.00%
walk 7 36.84% andar ‘to walk’ 9 52.94%
jump 5 26.32% saltar ‘to jump’ 4 23.53%
run 4 21.05% correr ‘to run’ 3 17.65%
bike 1 5.26% caminar ‘to walk’ 1 5.88%
dance 1 5.26%
Other items:
1 5.26%
quick
WALK 15 100.00% ANDAR 17 100.00%
run 3 20.00% correr ‘to run’ 5 29.41%
stroll 3 20.00% andar ‘to walk’ 3 17.65%
move 2 13.33% saltar ‘to jump’ 2 11.76%
hop 1 6.67% caminar ‘to walk’ 2 11.76%
march 1 6.67% deambular ‘to wander about’ 1 5.88%
strut 1 6.67%
walk 1 6.67%
Other items:
Other items: titubear ‘to hesitate’, despacio
3 20.00% 4 23.53%
quick, fast, slow ‘slow’, rápido ‘fast’, a pie ‘on
foot’
RUN 15 100.00% CORRER 17 100.00%
jog 4 26.67% trotar ‘to trot’ 7 41.18%
walk 2 13.33% saltar ‘to jump’ 2 11.76%
gallop 1 6.67% corretear ‘to run about’ 1 5.88%
move 1 6.67% correr ‘to run’ 1 5.88%
race 1 6.67% arrastrarse ‘to drag oneself’ 1 5.88%
apresurarse ‘to hurry’ 1 5.88%
andar ‘to walk’ 1 5.88%
acelerar ‘to speed up’ 1 5.88%
Other items: Other items:
6 40.00% 2 11.76%
fast (3), quickly (3) rápido ‘fast’, a saltos ‘by jumps’
JUMP 15 100.00% SALTAR 17 100.00%
hop 3 20.00% brincar ‘to jump’ 5 29.41%
skip 2 13.33% saltar ‘to jump’ 3 17.65%
leap 2 13.33% correr ‘to run’ 2 11.76%
rise 1 6.67% impulsarse ‘to propel oneself’ 2 11.76%
tambalearse ‘to move or go
jump 1 6.67% 1 5.88%
unsteadily’
Other items:
Other items: a la pata coja ‘by jumping on
high (4), up (1), over 6 40.00% one leg’, mucho ‘a lot’, alto 4 23.53%
(1) ‘high’, salto de longitud ‘a long
jump’
Table 6.6. Motion verbs, adverbials and satellites listed as first item (per category and language)
243
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
Move / Moverse
For these categories both English and Spanish participants listed walk-andar, jump-
saltar, and run-correr as the most frequent first items. Surprisingly, participants
more frequently listed jump-saltar (English: 27.78%; Spanish: 23.53%) as first item
than run-correr (English, 22.22%; Spanish, 17.65%). In English, participants listed
the verb dance and a vehicle name (bike) as first items, which ties in nicely with our
observations on the prolific use of vehicle names and dance names as motion verbs.
Walk / Andar
Across languages, participants listed run, correr ‘to run’, hop and saltar ‘to jump’ as
first items; moreover, they even listed the same verb they were being asked for.
Above all, the most frequent verbs as first items were run and stroll for English and
correr ‘to run’ and andar ‘to walk’ for Spanish. Apart from these verbs, English
participants listed three fine-grained manner verbs, i.e., stroll (Leisurely Motion,
State of the Figure-relaxed), march (Characteristic use of lower body-regular steps)
and strut (State of the Figure-proud), and Spanish participants listed caminar ‘to
walk’ and deambular ‘to wander about’ (No aim in motion).
Run / Correr
As we have seen with the previous categories, participants listed verbs which have
not been considered ways of running; English participants listed walk and move and
Spanish participants listed saltar ‘to jump’, andar ‘to walk’ and correr ‘to run’.
Additionally, they listed more specific manner verbs: jog (Rate-slow and regular),
gallop (Rate-fast) and race (Rate-fast) for English, and trotar ‘to trot’ (Rate-fast),
corretear ‘to run about playfully’ (Playful Motion), arrastrarse ‘to drag oneself’
(Contact between Figure and Ground; Body close to the Ground), apresurarse ‘to
hurry’ (Rate-increase), acelerar ‘to speed up’ (Rate-increase).
Jump / Saltar
Once again, participants responded with the same verb about which they were asked
as first items. In addition, Spanish participants listed correr ‘to run’ as a way of
jumping, and one English participant listed the path verb rise (Earth-based
244
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
Subjects analysis 11
A 2 (Language: English, Spanish) by 4 (verb category: Move, Walk, Run, Jump)
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was performed, in which language and verb
11
The reader should note that the subjects analysis uses occurrences of manner verbs and not manner
verb types. Therefore, the fact that one language group might list a larger number of occurrences of
manner verbs does not mean that participants listed a larger variety of manner verbs. For that reason,
an items analysis looking at manner verb types was also carried out.
245
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
category are between-subjects factors. This analysis revealed two main effects: a
language effect and a verb effect, but no interaction.
This analysis showed that participants in the two language groups differed
significantly in the number of manner verbs they listed, F(1,113) = 50.365, p < 0.05;
English participants overall listed more occurrences of manner verbs (M = 4.89, SE =
0.309) than did Spanish participants (M = 3.508, SE = 0.259). Moreover, the effect of
verb category also reached significance, F(3, 113) = 35.758, p < 0.05. On average,
both language groups listed more occurrences of manner verbs for Move (English M
= 8.10, SD = 3.07; Spanish M = 6.65, SD = 2.06) than for any of the other categories.
Furthermore, English participants listed a higher number of manner verbs for Walk
(M = 4.92, SD = 3.23) than for Run (M = 2.55, SD = 1.81) and Jump (M = 4, SD =
1.12). To our surprise, on average, English speakers listed more manner verbs for
Jump than for Run. On the other hand, Spanish participants listed the highest number
of manner verbs for Walk (M = 3.35, SD = 1.41), then for Run (M = 2.25, SD = 1)
and the fewest number of manner verbs for Jump (M = 1.78, SD = 0.80). These
findings are summarized in Figure 6.1.
Six t-tests per language group were performed to further assess whether the
number of listed manner verbs differed among verb categories. These analyses
revealed that the number of verbs listed differed significantly between Move and
Walk (English t(29) = 2.740, p < 0.05; Spanish t(32) = 5.439, p < 0.05), Move and
Run (English t(26) = 4.994, p < 0.05; Spanish t(31) = 7.720, p < 0.05). For Move and
Jump, Walk and Run, and for Run and Jump there were significant differences in the
number of listed manner verbs in Spanish (Move and Jump: t(29) = 8.307, p < 0.05;
Walk and Run: t(31) = 2.754, p < 0.05; Run and Jump: t(28) = 1.389, p < 0.05), but
marginal ones in English (Move and Jump: English t(26) = 1.937, p = 0.07; Walk and
Run: English t(19) = 1.937, p = 0.0677; Run and Jump: t(16) = -2.036, p = 0.058).
Finally, for Walk and Jump, the analysis reached significance for Spanish (t(29) =
3.686, p = 0.05), but it did not for English (t(19) = 0.798, ns)
246
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
10
9
Items analysis
With respect to manner verb types, across the two languages the category ‘Move’
includes the greatest variety of manner verbs, followed by ‘Walk’, then by ‘Run’ and
finally by ‘Jump’ (Figure 6.2).
Furthermore, there was a statistically marginal difference in the number of
manner verb types listed by English and Spanish participants, Chi-square (3) = 7.08,
0.10 > p > 0.05. A higher number of manner verb types were listed in English for the
categories Move (English = 53; Spanish = 32) and Walk (English = 34; Spanish =
18), whereas more manner verb types were listed in Spannish for Run (English = 11;
Spanish = 15) and Jump (English = 9; Spanish = 13).
60
50
Number of Verb Types
40
ENGLISH
30
SPANISH
20
10
0
MOVE WALK RUN JUMP
Verb Category
Figure 6.2. Number of manner verb types per category and language
247
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
In this analysis, we included all manner verbs listed by participants (see Appendix 4
for the list of manner verbs per language and category). After a close examination of
what participants responded for ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’, we observed that they listed some
manner verbs which were not coded as running or jumping verbs in our corpus
analysis. Restricting our analysis to just those verbs which according to dictionaries
are running 12 and jumping 13 verbs, it was still found that (a) a greater variety of
running verbs than of jumping verbs were listed across languages, and that (b)
running verbs in Spanish outnumbered those of English. However, a slightly
different pattern was observed for jumping verbs; namely, English jumping verbs
surpassed those of Spanish. Finally, it should be remarked that this analysis seems to
be more an artefact of dictionary definitions than of speakers’ conceptualisations.
On the whole, it has been observed that participants often listed adverbs, satellites,
noun phrases and prepositional phrases in addition to motion verbs, especially when
responding to the ways humans walk, run and jump. A general decrease in the listing
of motion verbs and, in particular, of manner-of-motion verbs from the category
‘Move’ to the category ‘Jump’ was found. Overall, English and Spanish participants
listed the highest number of motion verbs and of manner verbs for the category
‘Move’; however, the number of motion verbs and manner verbs dropped off for
‘Walk’, then they fell further for ‘Run’ and even more for ‘Jump’. An increase in the
number of other listed items paralleled the decrease of motion verbs.
A secondary aim of this study was to find out which were the prototypical
motion verbs for each of the categories. In the case of ‘Move’, the same pattern was
found across languages: walk and andar ‘to walk’ were the most frequently listed
first items, followed by jump and saltar ‘to jump’ and by run and correr ‘to run’. For
English, bike and dance seem to be prototypical verbs for this category as well; this
12
The strict count of listed running verbs included 5 for English (gallop, jog, race, speed, sprint) and
6 for Spanish (acelerar ‘to speed up’, aligerar ‘to hurry up’, apresurarse ‘to hurry up’, corretear ‘to
run about playfully’, galopar ‘to gallop’, trotar ‘to trot’).
13
The strict count of listed jumping verbs included 5 for English (bound, hop, leap, skip, spring) and 1
for Spanish (brincar ‘to jump (informal)’).
248
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
suggests that vehicle verbs and dance verbs might be good members of the
superordinate category ‘Move’. With respect to the three motor pattern categories,
the three verbs naming the categories tended to appear in their responses; in other
words, participants listed as first item the same verb which they were being asked
about (e.g., walk when asked about ways of walking). Additionally, it was noted that
within each motor pattern category the other two motor pattern verbs were
sometimes listed; e.g., for ‘Walk’ in English and Spanish, run and correr were the
most frequent listed first items, which suggests that participants might not consider
these two actions (walking and running) as two disconnected motion events.
Regarding the motor patterns ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’, other verbs were the best exemplars
of these categories: jog and trotar ‘to trot’ for ‘Run’; hop and brincar ‘to jump
(informal)’ for ‘Jump’. Other verbs which were listed as first items, and thus, as good
exemplars of the three motor pattern categories are the following: stroll, march and
strut for ‘Walk’; caminar ‘to walk’ and deambular ‘to wander about’ for ‘Andar’ (‘to
walk’); gallop and race for ‘Run’; corretear ‘to run about playfully’, arrastrarse ‘to
drag oneself’, apresurarse ‘to hurry’, acelerar ‘to speed up’ for ‘Correr’ (‘to run’);
skip, leap and rise for ‘Jump’; and impulsarse ‘to propel oneself’, tambalearse ‘to
move or go unsteadily’ for ‘Saltar’ (‘to jump’). One drawback of this study is the
limited number of participants per verb category, which results in a restricted number
of motion verbs as first items. A much larger sample of participants might be needed
both to elicit more prototypical verbs for these categories and to find out whether the
verbs which have been found in this study are in reality prototypical verbs for these
categories within the languages.
Regarding our predictions about the quantitative differences in the
organisation of manner verb lexicons, our results partially support our first
prediction, which stated that English speakers would list more manner verbs for each
category than Spanish participants. In both subjects and items analyses, it has been
found out that English participants listed more manner verbs than Spanish
participants only for two of the four categories, specifically, for ‘Move’ and ‘Walk’.
Unpredictably, in Spanish more manner verbs were listed for ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ than
in English. A possible explanation might be sought in the fact that more than half of
the responses for English ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ were other items (i.e., no motion verbs,
249
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
with a total of 53.85% for ‘Run’ and 55.79% for ‘Jump’), whereas the percentage of
listed motion verbs in Spanish ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ surpassed those of other items. This
seems to indicate either that (a) English participants might have been more
concerned with providing any answer due to the one minute time-limit of the task,
(b) it might have been difficult for them to access their manner verb lexicon for these
two categories, (which are less central to human experience than ‘Walk’) or (c) both,
i.e., because of time constraints, it was hard to access their lexicon and they thus
resorted to other items. Overall, English participants listed other items per each
category much more frequently than Spanish participants did, which might suggest
that English participants did not as closely follow the instructions given. On the other
hand, in a second items analysis which took into account a strict count of running
and jumping verbs, it was also found that Spanish running verbs exceeded English
running verbs, but it was noted that English jumping verbs outnumbered those of
Spanish.
Furthermore, it was predicted that, across languages, participants would list
more manner verbs for ‘Move’ than for the other three categories, and that, within
the motor pattern categories, they would list a higher number of manner verbs for
‘Walk’ over ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’. These two predictions were also borne out. Move and
moverse as superordinate verbs of walk, run, jump, and andar, correr, saltar yielded
a significantly higher variety of manner verbs in both language groups and a larger
number of manner verb occurrences. Also, in line with our predictions, walk and
andar ‘to walk’ generated more manner verbs than the other two less basic motor
patterns. Although no prediction was made about differences in the number of
manner verbs for the categories ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’, our results show that, for both
language groups, the ‘Run’ category presents a greater variety of manner verbs than
the ‘Jump’ category (see Appendix 4 for a full list of these verbs in alphabetical
order). Recall that in our corpus analysis, a similar pattern was found for Spanish
(i.e., 4 verbs for ‘Run’ and 3 for ‘Jump’) but the reverse was noted for English;
English jumping verbs exceeded running verbs (i.e., 11 verbs for ‘Jump’ and 7 verbs
for ‘Run’). We attributed this to the fact that dictionary definitions were not specific
about the motor pattern of many possible candidates for running verbs.
250
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
To summarise, the present study provides empirical evidence for the claim
that English and Spanish organise their human locomotion verb lexicon in much the
same way. Irrespective of the quantitative differences in the number of manner verbs
their lexicons have, both languages have more manner verbs specifying different
ways of walking than ways of running and jumping.
6.4.1. Introduction
In this last part of the chapter, an experimental investigation on how English and
Spanish native speakers rate a list of human locomotion verbs in terms of the three
basic motor patterns ‘Walk’, ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ is reported. As has been discussed in
Chapter 5, the semantics of motion verbs is so intricate that even dictionaries often
disagree in their definitions. While compiling and analysing our corpus of motion
verbs, we came across English and Spanish motion verbs which seem to clearly
denote that the Figure is either walking, running, or jumping, whereas other verbs
seem just to imply a specific motor pattern (i.e., dictionaries failed to agree in on a
unique motor pattern or they were not specific about one). Accordingly, the former
were analysed and grouped within the ‘Motor pattern’ categories, while the latter
were grouped in other manner categories.
The main goal of this study is to determine the specific motor pattern which
is denoted or implied by a set of human locomotion verbs. The present study was
carried out before the analysis of the motion verb lexicon was completed. For that
reason, not all conflicting verbs within our corpus were included in this study.
Additionally, this study addresses the question of whether these human locomotion
verbs are better examples of the ‘Walk’ category than of ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’. Drawing
from our previous research (i.e., corpus analysis and the free verb listing study), our
major prediction is that English and Spanish human locomotion verbs will be better
examples of ‘Walk’ than of ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’ as the majority of human locomotion
verbs seem to be walking verbs. When looking at human locomotion verbs as a
whole, participants from the two language groups will give them the highest ratings
251
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
as ways of/kinds of walking; on the other hand, when looking at each verb in
particular, a larger number of verbs will be rated as good exemplars of ‘Walk’
whereas fewer verbs will be rated as good exemplars of ‘Run’ and ‘Jump’.
Moreover, it is expected that some human locomotion verbs do not fit in only one of
these three motor pattern categories as the ways of moving denoted by the verbs
might occur while the Figure is walking, running or jumping.
One issue that arose when designing the study was how the question should
be worded. Research by Cruse (1989, 2004) and Miller & Fellbaum (1991) has
provided some interesting insights into verb taxonomies, concretely, into how they
seem to be organised and what kinds of semantic relations hold among their
members. Cruse (1989) remarks that the diagnostic question for noun taxonomies Is
X a kind/type of Y? does not work for verbs as well as it does for nouns; whereas the
question Is verb X-ing a way of verb Y-ing? seems to be more appropriate for verbs.
Miller & Fellbaum (1991: 216-220), much along the same lines, state that troponymy
(from Greek tropos, i.e., manner or fashion) or verb hyponymy is the most common
semantic relation among verbs. Many verbs indicate more precisely the manner of
doing something, for example, march, strut, traipse, amble, and mosey are
troponyms of to walk, that is, they are ways of walking, mutually inclusive or
temporally co-extensive with walking. Although hyponymy is quite common among
verbs, taxonomy also occurs. In Cruse’s own words (1989: 138), ‘verbs generally
seem to show hierarchical structuring to a more limited extent than nouns; however,
just as hyponymy is quite common among verbs, a relation paralleling nominal
taxonomy occurs, too’. Although the two notions are extremely difficult to tell apart,
a troponym is not the same as a taxonym 14 . Whereas many verbs can be troponyms
of a superordinate verb, only a subset of them are taxonyms of it. For example, to
travel, to walk and to run are troponyms of to move; they all are ways of moving.
However, according to Cruse, only to walk and to run are taxonyms of to move. In
sum, both troponymic (i.e., a verb X is a way of verb Y) and taxonomic relations
(i.e., a verb X is a kind of verb Y) are to be distinguished when exploring the nature
14
According to Cruse (2004: 150), taxonymy is a subtype of a hyponymy; it seems that ‘a taxonym
must engage with the meaning of its supeordinate in a particular way, by further specifying what is
distinctive about it’.
252
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
of verb lexicons, though troponymy (or verb hyponymy) seems to be the most
frequent semantic relation. Therefore, as a secondary aim we want to test which
question type (the way-of or the kind-of question) seems to work better when
categorising motion verbs. Thus, we predict that the way of question will yield higher
ratings than the kind of question.
6.4.2. Method
Subjects
18 adult native English speakers and 18 adult native Spanish speakers volunteered or
were paid for the participation. English participants were students at the University
of Sussex (UK). Spanish speakers were students at the Universidad de Murcia
(Spain). Participants ranged from 18 to 30 years of age.
Stimuli
Our stimuli were 111 verbs for English (108 verbs plus walk, run, jump) and 57 for
Spanish (54 verbs plus andar ‘to walk’, correr ‘to run’, and saltar ‘to jump’) (See
Appendix 5).
We created separate booklets for each language group. The instructions were
given on the first page of the booklet (Appendix 3). Each booklet was divided into 3
blocks; each block consisted of questions and rating scales for each human
locomotion verb with respect to one of the superordinate categories (Walk or Run or
Jump). The questions were one of the following two throughout the booklet: ‘Is X-
ing a kind of Y-ing?’ or ‘Is X-ing a way of Y-ing?’ for the English version, and ‘¿Es
X un tipo de Y?’ or ‘¿Es X una manera de Y?’ for the Spanish version. Half the
subjects received the kind-of question and the other half the way-of question. Each
question was followed by a rating scale ranging from ‘Definitely a kind/way’ – ‘Not
sure a kind/way’ – ‘Definitely not a kind/way’. It should be noted that questions
about (a) whether walk was a way/kind of running or jumping (in the Run and in the
Jump block respectively), (b) whether run was a way/kind of walking or jumping (in
the Walk and in the Jump block), and (c) whether jump was a way/kind of walking or
running (in the Walk and in the Run block) were also included. For the English
version, each booklet contained 330 questions in total, that is, 110 verbs x 3 blocks.
253
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
The Spanish version consisted of 168 questions in total, that is, 56 verbs x 3 blocks.
Both the questions and the blocks within each booklet were randomised across
subjects to avoid order effects.
Procedure
Participants were given the booklets and told to answer all the questions by marking
an X on any point on the scale provided. They were told not to skip any question or
look ahead in the booklet. The instructions were also given in written form on the
first page of the booklet. Participants were run in a quiet room and no time limit was
given.
Design
This study used a mixed design. There were three independent variables: language
(English, Spanish), question (kind of, way of) and superordinate verb (Walk, Run,
Jump). The language and question variables were between-subjects variables, and the
superordinate verb was a within-subjects variable. The dependent variable was the
goodness of a verb as a member of a superordinate verb, defined as the score (in
millimeters) a participant gave to a particular verb with regard to a superordinate
verb (either Walk, Run or Jump) by marking an X on the rating scale.
This scale was 102 millimeters long. It ranged from ‘Definitely a kind/way
of’ on the left (score 0) to ‘Not sure a kind/way of’ to ‘Definitely not a kind/way of’
on the right (score 102). We flipped the scores for the final coding for clarity
reasons: thus, 0 means ‘Definitely not a kind/way of’ and 102 means ‘Definitely a
kind/way of’. Thus, the higher the score a verb got, the better of an example of the
superordinate category it was.
6.4.3. Results
Two ANOVAs, one treating participants as the random effect and the other treating
items as the random effect, were carried out. The participant analysis yielded no
statistically significant results because of the limited number of participants. The
254
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
item analysis is reported here instead. All reported effects were significant at the p <
0.05 level.
In addition, separate ANOVAs for each human locomotion verb were carried
out to find out the motor pattern category into which the verb is best categorised.
OVERALL ANALYSIS
A 2 (Language: English, Spanish) by 2 (Question: kind of, way of) by 3
(Superordinate verb: Walk, Run, Jump) Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), in which
all factors varied within-items revealed three significant main effects and one
interaction.
The main effect of language on acceptability ratings was significant, F(1,
164) = 4.38, p < 0.05. This means that the two language groups differed in their
ratings irrespective of question type and superordinate verb. English human
locomotion verbs got higher acceptability ratings (M = 40.70, SE = 1.12) than did
Spanish human locomotion verbs (M = 36.67, SE = 1.57).
The effect of superordinate verb on acceptability ratings was also significant,
F(2, 328) = 94.56, p < 0.05. On average, verbs were rated higher with respect to
Walk (M = 57.26, SE = 1.81) than to Run (M = 37.03, SE = 1.95) and to Jump (M =
21.76, SE = 1.56). (Figure 6.3)
The analysis also revealed that the question type had a significant effect on
the ratings, F(1, 164) = 9.38, p < 0.05. On average, responses to the way-of question
received higher ratings (M = 39.67, SE = .99) than did responses to the kind-of
question (M = 37.70, SE = 1.03).
255
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
70
60
50
RATINGS
40 ENGLISH
30 SPANISH
20
10
0
walk run jump
VERB CATEGORY
50
45
40
35
RA TINGS
30
25 English
20 Spanish
15
10
5
0
way of kind of
QUESTION TYPE
256
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
English Results
A 2 (Question: kind of, way of) by 3 (Superordinate verb: Walk, Run, Jump)
ANOVA in which the two factors varied within-items revealed two main effects
(Figure 6.5 is included below for ease of illustration).
This analysis revealed a main effect of superordinate verb, F(2, 218) = 52.75,
p < 0.05. Human locomotion verbs were rated higher with regard to Walk (M =
57.35, SE = 2.18) than to Run (M = 39.88, SE = 2.24) and to Jump (M = 24.89, SE =
1.84). Separate t-tests showed that the ratings differed significantly between the
superordinate verbs Walk and Run (t(109) = 4.87, p < 0.05 ), Walk and Jump (t(109)
= 10.74, p < 0.05), and Run and Jump (t(109) = 5.04, p < 0.05).
The results also showed that the type of question asked had a significant
effect on the acceptability ratings, F(1, 109) = 28.65, p < 0.05. The kind-of questions
were rated higher (M = 42.62, SE = 1.09) than the way-of questions (M = 38.78, SE =
1.08).
No interaction between question and superordinate verb was found.
70
60
50
RAT INGS
40
way-of
30 kind-of
20
10
0
walk run jump
VERB C ATEGORY
Figure 6.5. English: effects of the question type and of superordinate verb
257
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
Spanish results
A 2 (Question: kind of, way of) by 3 (Superordinate verb: Walk, Run, Jump)
ANOVA in which question and superordinate verb varied within-items revealed
significant effects of question and of superordinate verb, but no interaction.
There was a main effect of superordinate verb, F(2, 110) = 53.45, p < 0.05.
Human locomotion verbs were rated higher with regard to Walk (M = 57.12, SE =
2.89) than to Run (M = 34.2, SE = 3.24) and to Jump (M = 18.63, SE = 2.45). Three
t-tests showed that the ratings differed significantly between the superordinate verbs
Walk and Run (t(55) = 5.87, p < 0.05), Walk and Jump (t(55) = 11.07, p < 0.05), and
Run and Jump (t(55) = 4.02, p < 0.05). (Figure 6.6).
Furthermore, the results showed that the type of question asked had a
significant effect on the acceptability ratings, F(1, 55) = 48.40, p < 0.05. The way-of
questions were rated higher (M = 40.56, SE = 1.8) than the kind-of questions (M =
32.79, SE = 1.98)
70
60
50
RATINGS
40 way-of
kind-of
30
20
10
0
walk run jump
VERB CATEGORY
Figure 6.6. Spanish: effects of the question type and of superordinate verb
FURTHER ANALYSES
Separate one-way ANOVAs for each of the 108 English verbs as well as for the 54
Spanish verbs were performed, with supeordinate verb (Walk, Run, Jump) as the
within-items variable. For verbs that did not clearly pattern as good ways/kinds of
only one superordinate 15 category, t-tests with Bonferroni correction were also
15
When the difference between the means was equal or less than 20.
258
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
carried out to test whether the ratings differed significantly between the
superordinate verbs (See Appendix 5).
These analyses revealed that (a) most verbs are good ways/kinds of one of the
superordinate categories, (b) a few of them are not good ways/kinds of any of them,
and (c) some verbs seem to be good ways/kinds of two superordinate categories (i.e.,
no significant difference was found between their ratings in terms of the two
superordinate categories). Tables 6.6 and 6.7 summarise these results for English and
Spanish respectively. The results of these analyses are also included in the last
column of our corpus of motion verbs (in Appendices 1 and 2), where they can be
easily contrasted with the results from the semantic analysis.
NUMBER
MOTOR
VERBS OF VERBS
PATTERN
(108)
amble, clump, crawl, creep, dodder, edge, file, goosestep, hike, hobble,
inch, limp, lope, lumber, lurch, march, meander, mince, mosey, pace,
paddle, parade, perambulate, plod, promenade, prowl, ramble, roam,
WALK 54
rove, sashay, saunter, shamble, shuffle, sidle, slide, slink, sneak,
stagger, stalk, stomp, stride, strut, stump, tiptoe, toddle, totter, traipse,
tramp, trek, troop, trudge, waddle, wade, wander
bolt, canter, charge, chase, dart, dash, flee, gallop, hound, hurtle, race,
RUN 19
scurry, sprint, streak, whisk, whiz(z), zoom
JUMP bounce, hop, leap, skip, sommersault, spring, vault 7
dribble, hasten, hurry, nip, pad, reel, rush, scamper, scoot, scuttle,
WALK-RUN 14
skedaddle, skitter, sweep, trot
WALK-JUMP prance 1
RUN-JUMP 0
bound, caper, cavort, flounder, frisk, frolic, gambol, romp, scud, shoot,
NONE 13
slog, slouch, trip
259
NUMBER
MOTOR
VERBS OF VERBS
PATTERN
(54)
acechar ‘to stalk’, acompañar ‘to accompany’, ambular ‘to wander
about’, anadear ‘to duckwalk’, arrastrarse ‘to drag oneself’, callejear ‘to
walk around the streets’, caminar ‘to walk’, chancletear ‘to walk noisily
using flip-flops’, contonearse ‘to walk swaying one’s hips’,
contramarchar ‘to go backwards, to back’, deambular ‘to walk around’,
desandar ‘to walk back to a previous path’, desfilar ‘to walk in file, to
parade’, errar ‘to wander about’, gatear ‘to crawl’, merodear ‘to walk
around’, noctambular ‘to walk around at night’, patear ‘to stamp, to go
WALK 31
on foot around a place’, pavonearse ‘to strut’, recorrer ‘to move, go
around a place’, recorrer ‘to move, go around a place’, renquear ‘to
limp’, rondar ‘to be on patrol, to prowl about’, serpentear ‘to slither, to
meander’, taconear ‘to tap shoes with heels’, tambalearse ‘to move
unsteadily’, vagar ‘to wander about’, zancajear ‘to walk repeatedly
from one place to another’, zanquear ‘to walk with crooked legs, to
stride’, zaparrastrar ‘to move, walk dragging one’s clothing’,
zigzaguear ‘to zigzag’
acelerar ‘to accelerate’, aligerar ‘to quicken, to hurry up’, apresurarse ‘to
RUN hurry up’, esprintar ‘to sprint’, fugarse ‘to flee, to run away’, galopar ‘to 7
gallop’, huir ‘to flee, to run away’
JUMP brincar ‘to jump’, tropezar ‘to trip’ 2
corretear ‘to run about’, desacelerar ‘to slow down’, deslizarse ‘to
WALK-RUN slide’, esquivar ‘to avoid, to dodge’, rastrear ‘to track’, regatear ‘to 6
dribble’
WALK-JUMP zapatear ‘to tap shoes’ 1
RUN-JUMP precipitarse ‘to fall down from a high place; to run, hurry to’ 1
encorvarse ‘to bend, to curve’, pisar ‘to tread’, pisotear ‘to tread on
NONE something repeatedly’, resbalarse ‘to slide’, retozar ‘to frolic’, trotar 16 6
‘to trot’
16
The analysis performed for Spanish trotar ‘to trot’ did not reach significance, suggesting that it is
not a good way/kind of walking, running or jumping; however, by looking at the mean ratings (Walk,
M = 72.33, SD = 33.82); Run, M = 81.56, SD = 27.89; Jump, M = 80.22, SD = 32) it might be argued
that Spanish trotar is a good way/kind of all three superordinate verbs as it received high ratings for
all three categories.
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
Recall that in addition to the questions about the 108 English verbs and the 54
Spanish verbs, questions about the goodness of each superordinate verb as a
way/kind of either of the other two superordinates were included as well. For each
superordinate verb in both languages (walk, run, jump, andar ‘to walk’, correr ‘to
run’, saltar ‘to jump’) paired t-tests were used to determine whether the verb was a
better way/kind of either of the other superordinates within the language (e.g., is
walk as good of a way/kind of run as of jump?). Only one of these analyses reached
significance: the Spanish superordinate verb correr ‘to run’ was rated higher with
regard to Andar ‘Walk’ (M = 74.94, SD = 42.04) than to Saltar ‘Jump’ (M = 28.33,
SD = 39.84), t(17) = 3.540, p < 0.05. There were no other significant differences.
In the overall analysis, our results showed a main effect of language on acceptability
ratings, namely, English human locomotion verbs got higher ratings than Spanish
human locomotion verbs. In spite of this language effect, both languages showed a
similar pattern when rating verbs in terms of the three superordinate categories:
human locomotion verbs received the highest ratings with respect to Walk, then to
Run, and the lowest ones with respect to Jump. These results can be interpreted as if
the category Walk includes the greatest number of human locomotion verbs, whereas
Run comprises fewer verbs than Walk, and Jump is the category with less verbs. On
the whole, these results support our prediction and provide further support for our
previous findings: both languages exhibit a greater variety of walking verbs over
running and jumping verbs.
Further analyses on how each individual verb was rated showed that, for
English, 54 verbs of the set of motion verbs in this study are good examples of Walk,
19 are good instances of Run and 7 of Jump; for Spanish, 31 verbs of the set of
motion verbs are good examples of Walk, 7 of Run and 2 of Jump, further supporting
our previous results. In addition, these analyses revealed that (a) most verbs are good
instances of one of the three motor patterns; (b) a few of them are not good instances
of any of the three motor patterns (e.g., bound, cavort, frolic) and (c) some verbs
seem to be good instances of two motor patterns (e.g., hasten, hurry, scoot are good
261
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
instances of walking and running). By ‘a verb is a good instance of’, we mean that a
verb can either denote or imply a given motor pattern when the Figure performs the
action depicted by the verb. For example, flee, fugarse and huir ‘to flee’ are not
defined in dictionaries as related to any motor pattern, but they are good instances of
running verbs according to native speakers. As pointed out in Chapter 5 (Section
5.5.1), dictionaries are not specific about the motor pattern of a large number of
verbs which encode fast rate of motion: e.g., bolt, canter, dart, dash, hasten, hurry,
hurtle, nip, race, rush, scoot, scud, scurry, scuttle, shoot, speed, spring, streak,
whisk, whiz, and zoom. This study suggests that (a) bolt, canter, dart, dash, hurtle,
race, rush, scoot, scurry, speed, streak, whisk, whiz and zoom are good instances of
‘Run’; (b) spring is a good instance of ‘Jump’; (c) scud and shoot are not good
instances of any motor pattern categories; and (d) hasten, hurry, nip and scuttle are
good instances of both ‘Run’ and ‘Walk’. In addition, many more human locomotion
verbs which denote mixed patterns have been found. This supports our prediction
that some human locomotion verbs do not fit clearly in only one of these three
categories as they might denote that the Figure could be walking and/or running
and/or jumping when performing the action labelled by the motion verb. The most
common mixed pattern in our data is that of ‘Walk’-‘Run’ (with a total of 14 English
verbs, i.e., dribble, hasten, hurry, nip, pad, reel, rush, scamper, scoot, scuttle,
skedaddle, scurry, speed, sprint, sweep and trot, and 6 Spanish verbs, i.e., corretear
‘to run about playfully’, desacelerar ‘to slow down’, deslizarse ‘to slide’, esquivar
‘to avoid, to dodge’, rastrear ‘to track’, and regatear ‘to dribble’), whereas the
patterns ‘Walk’-‘Jump’ and ‘Run’-‘Jump’ are rarer. In English, prance is the only
verb in the list of human locomotion verbs which seems to denote a way of walking
and of jumping; Spanish zapatear ‘to tap shoes’ seems also to denote a way of
walking and of jumping, and precipitarse 17 ‘to fall down from a high place; to run,
hurry to’ a way of running and of jumping. Furthermore, it has been found that trotar
‘to trot’, which is defined in dictionaries as a fast walk by horses or people, seems to
17
Two entries for this verb were created in our corpus: precipitarse1 (Path: downwards motion +
Manner: Forced Motion (energy-high)), which is good for labelling an event in which the Figure
jumps off from a high place, and precipitarse2 (Path: to/towards G + Manner: mp-run). This might
explain why this verb has been rated high in terms of both ‘Jump’ and ‘Run’.
262
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
fit nicely in all three motor patterns as shown by its higher ratings for all three. Last,
but not least important, some verbs which are defined in dictionaries as implying or
denoting a specific motor pattern were not categorised by native speakers as good
examples of any category. In particular, (a) posture verbs such as slouch (which can
be found in translational motion as well), encorvarse ‘to bend onself’; (b) verbs
denoting obstructed motion while walking or running (e.g., trip); (c) verbs typically
defined in dictionaries as walking and running playfully (e.g., frolic, gambol, frisk,
retozar ‘to frolic’); (d) Spanish pisar ‘to tread’, pisotear ‘to tread repeatedly’,
resbalarse ‘to slide’ (which seem to occur while the Figure is walking or running);
(e) English bound (defined as a way of jumping) and slog (defined as a slow and
effortful way of walking) were not rated as good instances of either ‘Walk’, ‘Run’ or
‘Jump’.
As a secondary aim, we also posed the question of whether one question type
works better than the other when categorising verbs. Following Cruse (1989, 2004)
and Miller & Fellbaum (1991), we were inclined to predict that the way-of question
would work better (i.e., it would get higher ratings) than the kind-of question as
troponymy is the most frequent semantic relation among verbs. However, our results
confirmed our prediction only for Spanish. On average, Spanish participants gave
higher ratings for the way-of question than for the kind-of question, whereas English
participants gave higher ratings for the kind-of question. But, if we look at Figure
6.4, it can be observed that verbs from both languages got similar ratings for the
way-of question, but differed significantly in the rating when the kind-of question
was used: Spanish ratings for kind-of were much lower than those for the way-of
question, while English ratings for kind-of were somewhat higher than those for
way-of. One plausible explanation for why the two languages differed dramatically
on the kind-of question might be the wording of the kind-of question in Spanish. One
of our Spanish participants remarked that the question ¿Es X un tipo de Y? sounded
quite odd to her. Thus, the wording of the kind-of question might have been
responsible for Spanish participants favouring the way-of question instead.
To conclude, this investigation provides further empirical evidence for the
findings from our semantic analysis of English and Spanish motion verb lexicons:
despite the fact that English has many more manner-of-motion verbs, both languages
263
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
exhibit a greater variety of walking verbs over running and jumping verbs.
Furthermore, this study has shed light on the semantic nature of human locomotion
verbs in various ways: by clarifying the motor pattern that a given verb denotes or
implies, and by suggesting that motor-pattern category boundaries are not always
clear, some motion verbs have emerged as mixed motor pattern verbs.
6.5. Conclusions
This chapter has presented three experimental studies on human locomotion verbs in
English and in Spanish. As argued in the Introduction, the semantic analysis carried
out in Chapter 5 does not represent speakers’ mental representations of the motion
verbs. However, this analysis set the much-needed solid foundations to begin
experimental research into the domain of motion.
The first study (the Definition and Feature Listing Task) offers the first
insights into native speakers’ representations of some basic motion verbs, namely,
move and moverse, walk and andar, run and correr, and jump and saltar. Generally
speaking, move and moverse were defined as motion verbs which denote both
translational and self-contained motion; walk and andar as the most basic human
motor pattern to change one’s location; run and correr as translational motion verbs
characterised by a fast way of moving by using one’s legs; and, jump and saltar as
motion verbs defined by a Figure’s pushing off the ground.
The second study (the Free Verb Listing Task) provides the first empirical
evidence for some observations which resulted from our semantic analysis.
Specifically it was found that English and Spanish organise their human locomotion
verb lexicons in similar ways, that is, both languages possess a larger number of
walking verbs over running and jumping verbs, as shown by the greater number of
manner verbs which were listed for walk and andar.
In the third study (the Rating Task), further empirical evidence for these
findings is provided. In addition, this study has elucidated the specific motor pattern
which is denoted or implied by each English and Spanish human locomotion verb,
264
CHAPTER 6. AN EMPIRICAL LOOK AT HUMAN LOCOMOTION VERBS
and has suggested, for the first time in this area of research, that some motion verbs
might be considered by language users as mixed motor pattern verbs.
On the whole, our findings are explained by the fact that walking is the
default way of moving for humans; it is a more basic daily activity than jumping or
running. Nowadays, people might run to catch the bus or metro, or jump over an
obstacle, but they most often walk from one place to another. Therefore, as also
stated in Chapter 5, it could be hypothesised that the world’s languages are more
likely to posses a more extensive manner verb lexicon for walking activities than for
running or jumping activities. Moreover, this claim might be also extended beyond
manner verbs to other linguistic units which can also convey manner-of-motion
information; for example, ideophones or sound symbolic expressions such as those
examined by Ibarretxe-Antuñano (2006a).
To conclude, this chapter represents our first attempts to explore the English
and the Spanish motion verb lexicons from a psycholinguistic perspective. A
dissertation would not be enough either to uncover the semantics of all the motion
verbs included in our corpus or to answer all the questions that emerged from the
semantic analysis. The empirical research reported here, however, has satisfactorily
dealt with an important subdomain of motion, that of human locomotion. It has
provided interesting insights into how English and Spanish organise their lexicons
and into the complex semantic nature of human locomotion verbs.
265
CHAPTER 7
CONCLUSIONS
7.3. Applications
CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS
The work described in this dissertation has been primarily concerned with the
semantics of English and Spanish motion verbs from a contrastive point of view,
bearing on Talmy’s theory of lexicalisation patterns for the domain of motion
(Chapter 2). Talmy was one of the pioneers of the cognitive linguistics enterprise,
which was born as reaction against formal approaches to language.
Talmy’s seminal work has engendered a great deal of research and debate in
the literature on motion event descriptions over the last two decades (Chapter 3).
Linguistic research on the expression of motion has documented habitual ways of
speaking and writing about motion which have served as the foundation for further
investigations of how children acquire these language-specific semantic patterns, on
the effects of these patterns in language processing, and on whether the effects have
an influence on non-linguistic cognition. Despite this bulk of research, research on
motion events has frequently overlooked the fact that motion verbs might also
encode other sorts of semantic information in their verb roots apart from Path of
motion and Manner of motion, and that other minor lexicalisation patterns might
occur as well. Furthermore, more often than not, scholars have neglected the study of
path verbs in favour of the study of manner verbs, as the expression of Manner in
verbs has been so far the most interesting diverging point between satellite- and verb-
framed languages. Finally, hardly any work has carried out an in-depth analysis of
the semantics of motion verb lexicons of verb- and satellite-framed languages apart
from those motion verbs which were found in novels or were elicited orally (e.g.,
Jovanovic & Martinovic-Zic, 2005; Özçalişkan, 2004; Slobin, 2005).
Research on motion verbs from different theoretical backgrounds (Chapter 4,
Sections 4.2, 4.3. and 4.4) has provided valuable insights into the syntactic and
semantic nature of motion verbs. The general tendency has been to separate path
verbs from manner verbs in these classifications. Path verbs have been often divided
into classes in terms of the trajectories they describe or in terms of whether or not
they describe the achievement of an endpoint. Manner verbs have been grouped into
two classes, namely, translational motion verbs and self-contained motion verbs,
though much more granular classifications have been proposed as well.
267
CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS
268
CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS
state of the Figure and details about the way the Figure moves its legs while walking,
motion using vehicles and motion while dancing), and (b) Spanish path verbs exceed
in number English path verbs, although both languages are able to express similar
types of path.
269
CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS
following two groups of vehicle verbs: (a) bus, ferry, parachute, ship, shuttle and
truck, which seem to allow agentive use, and (b) bobsleigh, bobsled, cab, dogsled,
gondola/e, jeep, kayak, moped, motorcycle/bike, rickshaw, rollerblade, skate,
skateboard, ski and yacht, which only seem to denote that the Figure uses these
vehicles or instruments as his/her means of locomotion. Further research is needed to
test the hypothesis that large vehicles (i.e., the former group) might allow both
agentive and self-agentive motion, as they can transport people, goods, etc, whereas
small vehicles (i.e., the latter group) might just be used for Figures as means of their
own locomotion. Finally, it has been noted that the boundary between self-contained
and translational motion is fuzzy, especially for some manner categories such as
‘Unsteady Motion’. It might be that (a) a verb inherently encodes a specific type of
motion or that (b) that meaning derives from being immersed in a specific syntactic
frame such as the English translational motion frame (i.e., manner verb + path
satellites).
Another line of research may be needed to shed light on the semantic weight
of the manner parameters which constitute a verb’s meaning. When grouping motion
verbs into manner categories, the question of whether some manner features are more
important than others for defining a verb or whether all manner categories are on
equal footing was posed. By way of illustration, roar, whiz and zoom encode
information about the rate of motion and about the noise that results from the motion.
Thus, are events designated by these verbs better categorised as (a) noisy activities,
(b) fast speed activities, or (c) as both?
Last, it would be of value for crosslinguistic research to test whether the
tendencies described above for English and Spanish apply to other verb- and
satellite-framed languages. Specifically, do languages (a) have a preference for
lexicalising some types of paths over others and (b) have a larger number of verbs
depicting different ways in which the Figure walks than verbs denoting different
ways of running and jumping?
270
CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS
7.3. Applications
This dissertation may have three main distinct applications: (1) for research in this
field, (2) for language teaching and (3) for translation.
First, research on Motion may be benefited in a number of ways. The
approach presented in this dissertation could be extended to the study of the motion
verb lexicons of other verb- and satellite-framed languages as well as to the study of
other linguistic units which can also express path and/or manner information, such as
satellites, verb particles, and sound symbolic expressions. Furthermore, this field of
research would benefit from the better understanding of the semantics of English and
Spanish motion verbs and from the general tendencies observed across the two
languages provided by this dissertation. Many psycholinguistic studies on motion,
including those related to linguistic relativity, start from linguistic data that have not
always been validated. Studies like the present one provide a more firm foundation
for further psycholinguistic research.
Second, the semantic representations of motion verbs proposed in this
dissertation may have useful applications for teaching English as a second language,
in particular, for teaching English to speakers of verb-framed languages. Students of
English find it very difficult to learn and master the vast amount of English manner-
of-motion verbs. If manner verbs are presented to students in categories and then
explored in terms of the additional manner information they encode, it will be easier
for them to understand their semantics.
Finally, this dissertation may benefit the field of translation. The complex
semantic nature of a vast number of English and Spanish motion verbs has been
uncovered and compared. Based on this comparison, translators might either choose
the most semantically equivalent verb if the context of the utterance allows its use, or
they might render the motion verb by other linguistic means in order to be as faithful
to the original as possible while sounding natural in the target language.
271
REFERENCES
REFERENCES
ASKE, J. (1989). Path predicates in English and Spanish: a closer look. Proceedings
of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 15, 1-14.
272
REFERENCES
BILLMAN, D. & KRYCH, M. (1998). Path and manner verbs in action: Effects of
“skipping” and “exiting” on event memory. Proceedings of the Twentieth
Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsday, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
BILLMAN, D., SWILLEY, & KRYCH, M. (2000). Path and manner priming: Verb
production and event recognition. In L. R. Gleitman & A. K. Joshi (Eds.),
Proceedings of the Twenty-second Annual Conference of the Cognitive
Science. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
273
REFERENCES
CHOI, S., MCDONOUGH, L., BOWERMAN, M. & MANDLER, J.M (1999). Early
sensitivity to language to language specific spatial categories in English
and Korean. Cognitive Development, 14, 83-121.
CHOMSKY, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
274
REFERENCES
CLARK, E. V. & CLARK, H. H. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language, 55,
767-811.
275
REFERENCES
276
REFERENCES
277
REFERENCES
& J. Cuartero Otal (Eds.), Entre semántica léxica, teoría del léxico y
sintaxis. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, pp. 373-384.
HALE , K. L. & KEYSER, S. J. (1987). A View from the Middle. Lexicon Project
Working Papers, 10. Center for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA:
MIT.
278
REFERENCES
279
REFERENCES
280
REFERENCES
JOHNSON, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning,
Imagination, and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
281
REFERENCES
KOCH, P., & OESTERREICHER, W. (1985). Sprahe der Nähe-Sprache der Distanz.
Mündlichkeit und Schriftlichkeit im Spannungsfeld von Sprachtheorie.
Romanistisches Jahrbuch, 36, 15-34.
LAKOFF, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things. What Categories Reveal
about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
LAKOFF, G. & JOHNSON, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh. The Embodied Mind
and its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.
LANDAU, B. & HOFFMAN, J.E. (2005). Parallels between spatial cognition and
spatial language. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 163-185.
282
REFERENCES
LEVINSON, S.C., KITA, S., HAUN, D.B.M. & RASCH, B. J. (2002). Returning the
tables: language affects spatial reasoning. Cognition, 84: 2, 155-188.
LI, P., & GLEITMAN, L. (2002). Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning.
Cognition, 83, 265-294.
283
REFERENCES
LUCY, J.A. (1992). Language diversity and thought. Cambridge: University Press.
LUCY, J.A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual review of anthropology, 26, 291-
312.
LUCY, J.A. & GASKINGS, S. (2003). Interaction of language type and referent type
in the development of nonverbal classification preferences. In Genter, D.
& Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of
language and thought . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 465-492.
MAYER, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.
284
REFERENCES
NARASIMHAM, B. (2003). Motion events and the lexicon: a case study of Hindi.
Lingua, 113, 123-160.
NIKANNE, U. & VAN DER ZEE, E. (2004). The grain levels in the linguistic
expressions of motion. Paper given at the 21st Scandinavian Conference of
Linguistics.
285
REFERENCES
ÖZÇALIŞKAN, Ş. & SLOBIN, D. I. (1999). Learning How to Search for the Frog:
Expression of Manner of Motion in English, Spanish and Turkish. In A.
Greenhil, H. Littlefield, & C. Tano (Eds), Proceedings of the 23rd Annual
Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville,
MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 541-552.
286
REFERENCES
ÖZYÜREK, A., KITA, S., ALLEN, S., FURMAN, R. & BROWN, A. (2005). How
does linguistic framing of events influence co-speech gestures? Gesture 5:
1/2, 219-240.
ÖZYÜREK, A. & KITA, S. (1999). Expressing manner and path in English and
Turkish: Differences in speech, gesture, and conceptualization. In M.
Hahn & S. C. Stoness (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-first Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, pp. 507-512.
PAPAFRAGOU, A., MASSEY, C. & GLEITMAN, L. (2002). Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll:
the representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84:2,
189-219.
PEDERSON, E., DANZINGER, E., WILKINS, D., LEVINSON, S., KITA, S. &
SENFT, G. (1998). Semantic typology and spatial conceptualisation.
Language, 74: 3, 557-589.
287
REFERENCES
288
REFERENCES
SEMILIS, S. & KATIS, D. (2003). Literal and non-literal motion in early child-adult
interaction: cross-linguistic differences between English and Greek.
Paper given at the 8th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference.
University of La Rioja, Logroño, Spain, July.
SLOBIN, D. I. (1996b). Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish.
In M. Shibatani & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Essays in semantics. Oxford:
Oxford University Press, pp. 195-317.
289
REFERENCES
SLOBIN, D. I. (2004). The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and
the expression of motion events. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (Eds.),
Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives in
Translation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 219-257.
SLOBIN, D. I. (in press). Relations between paths of motion and paths of vision: A
crosslinguistic and developmental exploration. In V. Gathercole (Ed.),
title to be announced. [http://ihd.berkeley.edu/slobinpapers.htm].
290
REFERENCES
SUGIYAMA, Y. (2005). Not all verb-framed languages are created equal: The case
of Japanese. Paper presented to the 31st annual meeting of the Berkeley
Linguistics Society. Berkeley, California, US.
291
REFERENCES
TALMY, L. (2000b). Toward a cognitive semantics: Vol. II: Typology and process in
concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
TALMY, L.. (2003). The representation of spatial structure in spoken and signed
languages. In K. Emmorey (Ed.), Perspectives on Classifier
Constructions in Sign Language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp.
169-195.
TOLKIEN, J. R. R. (1937). The Hobbit or there and back again. London: George
Allen & Unwin.
292
REFERENCES
ZLATEV, J. & YANGKLANG, P. (2004). A third way to travel: The place of Thai
and serial verb languages in motion event typology. In S. Stromqvist & L.
Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and
contextual perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp.
159-190.
ZLATEV, J. & DAVID, C. (2004) Do Swedes & Frenchmen view motion differently?
Paper given at Language, Culture & Mind, Portsmouth, UK, July.
293
REFERENCES
ZLATE, J. & DAVID, C. (2005). Motion event typology & categorisation. Paper
given at the 9th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Seoul,
Korea, July.
DICTIONARIES
Diccionario de uso del Español. María Moliner, CD-Rom version 2.0, Gredos. 2001
294
APPENDIX 1
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
From Anglo-French abanduner, from
(mettre) a bandun to hand over, put in
someone's control. [transitive verb] to
away from [transitive verb] To forsake, leave, or
leave a place, thing or person forever.
G desert (a place, person, or cause); to
abandon Transl Path 1 TFD: To give up by leaving or ceasing
(departure, leave without one's presence, help, or
to operate or inhabit, especially as a
source) support.
result of danger or other impending
threat. MW: to withdraw from often in
the face of danger or encroachment.
[intransitive verb](MOVE FASTER)
When a vehicle or its driver
accelerates, the speed of the vehicle
increases. If a person or object
[transitive verb] To quicken, or add to
accelerate Transl Manner 1 Rate- increase accelerates, it goes faster. TFD:
the speed of (a motion or process).
[intransitive verb+ To move or act
faster. [transitive verb] To increase the
speed of. MW: to cause to move
faster. to move faster : gain speed.
[transitive verb] to go with someone.
To show someone how to get to
[transitive verb] To go in company with,
Figure + CO- somewhere. TFD: To be or go with as
accompany Transl 2 to go along with; to convoy; to escort
MOTION a companion. MW: [intransitive and
(for safety), to attend (as a retinue).
transitive verb] to perform an
accompaniment to or for.
Middle English advauncen, from
Anglo-French avancer, from Vulgar
Latin *abantiare, from Late Latin
abante in front, from Latin ab- + ante .
[intransitive and transitive verb] To
advance Transl AM Path 1 forwards [intransitive and transitive verb] to go
move forward in place.
or move something forward. TFD:To
go or move forward or onward. To
cause to move forward. MW: to bring
or move forward. to move forward.
1
The first definition provided for each verb was taken from Cambridge Advanced Learners’ Dictionary (CALD).
296
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to land on [intransitive verb] Referring chiefly to
down onto something. TFD: To get down, as the result: To land. To get down from a
alight Transl Path 1 G (arrival, from a vehicle; dismount. MW: to horse or conveyance; to dismount or
endpoint) come down from something (as a descend for the time; to finish one's ride,
vehicle). stop.
Middle English approchen, from
Anglo-French aprocher, from Late
Latin appropiare, from Latin ad- +
prope near; akin to Latin pro before .
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] To come nearer
approach Transl Path 1 closer to G come near or nearer to something or (relatively), or draw near (absolutely), in
someone in space, time, quality or space. [transitive verb] To come near to.
amount. TFD: To come near or nearer,
as in space or time. To come or go
near or nearer to. MW: to draw nearer.
to draw closer to.
[intransitive verb] to walk in a slow
and relaxed way. TFD: [intransitive [intransitive verb] Of a horse, mule, etc.:
verb] To walk slowly or leisurely; To move by lifting the two feet on one
stroll. To move along at an easy gait side together, alternately with the two
mp-walk; Rate- by using both legs on one side feet on the other; hence, to move at a
amble Transl Manner 1 slow; State of alternately with both on the other. smooth or easy pace. Of a person: To mp-walk
Figure (relaxed) Used of a horse. MW: o go at or as if ride an ambling horse, to ride at an easy
at an amble. Saunter. LLA: To walk in pace. Hence, To move in a way
a slow and relaxed way, especially suggesting the motion or pace of an
when you are going a short distance, ambling horse.
or not going anywhere in particular.
[intransitive verb] (GET UP). TFD: To
Posture (vertical- get up, as from a sitting or prone [intransitive verb] To get up from
arise1 Self-c Manner 1
upwards) position; rise. To move upward; sitting, lying, repose.
ascend.MW: To get up.
Earth-
based TFD: [intransitive verb] To move [intransitive verb] To ascend, go or
arise2 Transl Path 1
orientation: upward; ascend. MW: rise, ascend. come higher.
upwards
297
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
from Anglo-French ariver, from
[intransitive verb] To come to shore or
Vulgar Latin arripare to come to
into port; to land.To come to, land at,
to/towards shore, from Latin ad- + ripa shore.
reach (a shore, port, etc.). To come to
arrive Transl Path 1 G (arrival, [intransitive verb] to reach a place,
the end of a journey, to a destination, or
endpoint) especially at the end of a journey.
to some definite place; to come upon the
TFD:To reach a destination. MW: to
scene, make one's appearance.
reach a destination.
[intransitive verb] (occas. emphasized
Latin ascendere, from ad- + scandere
by a redundant up) To go or come up,
Earth- to climb [intransitive and transitive
originally by a gradual motion, to a
based verb] to move up or climb something.
ascend Transl Path 1 relatively higher position. Of voluntary
orientation: TFD:To go or move upward; rise.To
agents: To climb up, travel up, walk up;
upwards move upward upon or along; climb.
to soar, mount. Of inanimate things: To
MW: to move upward
rise, be raised, move to a higher level.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) move backwards. TFD: To [intransitive verb] (for refl.) To move,
cause to move backward or in a go, come back.
back Transl AM Path 1 backwards
reverse direction. To move backward. [transitive verb] To cause to move back,
MW: to move backward. to cause to put back.
go back or in reverse.
[intransitive verb] to travel or camp
while carrying your clothes and other
A pack carried on the back; spec. one
things that you need in a backpack;
consisting of a folded parachute.
Travelling with TFD: [intransitive verb] To hike while
backpack Transl Manner 1 [intransitive verb], to carry a pack on the
one's belongings carrying a backpack. MW: to carry
back: used esp. of hiking, camping, etc.
(food or equipment) on the back
Also [transitive verb]
especially in hiking. to hike with a
backpack.
TFD: [intransitive verb] To ascend or
[intransitive verb] To ascend in a
Vehicle / ride in a balloon. MW: [intransitive
balloon Transl AM? Manner 1 balloon. [transitive verb] To carry up in,
Instrument verb] to ascend or travel in or as if in a
or as in, a balloon.
balloon.
298
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) curve. TFD:To cause to
assume a curved or angular shape. To [intransitive verb] To assume or receive
assume a curved, crooked, or angular a curved form, or a shape in which one
form or direction. To incline the body; part is inclined at an angle to the other.
bend Self-c AM Manner 1 Posture (Bend)
stoop. MW: to curve out of a straight To bring into the shape or direction of a
line or position; specifically : to bent bow. [transitive verb] To put or
incline the body in token of bring into the shape of a bow; to arch.
submission. to turn or force from
straight or even to curved or angular.
[intransitive verb and transitive verb]
INFORMAL to go somewhere by
Vehicle / bicycle. To use a motorcycle to
bicycle; bike Transl AM? Manner 1 [intransitive verb] To ride on a bicycle.
Instrument deliver something to someone. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To ride or travel on
a bicycle. MW: [intransitive verb]
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move and make currents of air, or to
be moved or make something move on
a current of air. TFD: [intransitive [intransitive verb] The proper verb
verb] To be in a state of motion. Used naming the motion or action of the wind,
blow Transl AM CAUSE 1 of the air or of wind.To move along or or of an aerial current. [transitive verb]
be carried by or as if by the wind. To drive or carry (things) by means of a
[transitive verb] To cause to move by current of air
means of a current of air. MW: (of
air)to be in motion. to move or be
carried by or as if by wind.
AS NOUN: a small vehicle for
travelling on water. TFD: [intransitive [intransitive verb] To go in a boat, to
Vehicle / and transitive verb] To travel by boat / row; to conduct a freight-boat (U.S.).
boat Transl AM Manner 1
Instrument to transport by boat. MW: [transitive [transitive verb] To place in a boat; to
verb] to place in or bring into a boat. carry in a boat.
[intransitive verb] to go by boat.
299
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to move up and [intransitive verb] To move up and
down quickly and gently especially on down like a buoyant body in water, or an
the surface of water. TFD: [transitive elastic body on land; hence, to dance; to
and intransitive verb]To cause to move to and fro with a similar motion,
Iterative Motion
bob Self-c AM Manner 1 move up and down. To move up and esp. said of hanging things rebounding
(up-down)
down. MW: [transitive and intransitive from objects lightly struck by them. To
verb] to move up and down in a short move up or down with a bob or slight
quick movement. to move up and jerk. [transitive verb] To move (a thing)
down briefly or repeatedly. up or down with a bob or slight jerk.
AS NOUN: a small vehicle with long AS NOUN: orig. U.S. A sled or sleigh,
metal blades under it, built for racing made of two short sleds or sleighs
bobsleigh (UK); Vehicle / down ice-covered tracks. TFD: coupled together; used in drawing logs
Transl NO Manner 1
bobsled (US) Instrument [intransitive verb] To ride or race in or from the forest to a river or public road,
as if in a bobsled. MW: NO FOUND and for various other purposes. NO
AS MOTION VERB FOUND AS MOTION VERB
[intransitive verb] to move very fast, [intransitive verb] To spring, move
especially as a result of being suddenly, with its causal.To move or
frightened. TFD: To move or spring come as with a spring or sudden bound,
bolt Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast mp-run
suddenly. MW: to move suddenly or to dart. To dart off or away, make off
nervously. to move or proceed rapidly. with himself, take flight, escape; to rush
to dart off or away. suddenly off or away.
[intransitive verb] dance to pop music.
[intransitive verb] To dance to boogie-
boogie Self-c Manner 1 Dance TFD: To dance to rock music. MW:
woogie music.
NO FOUND AS MOTION VERB.
[intransitive verb] dance to pop music. [intransitive verb] To play bop music, or
bop Self-c Manner 1 Dance TFD: NO FOUND. MW: to dance or in the style of this. To dance to pop
shuffle along to or as if to bop music. music.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) move up or away after [intransitive verb] To move with a
hitting a surface. To move in an sudden bound. To bound like a ball; to
energetic and enthusiastic manner. throw oneself about: esp. said of an
bounce Self-c AM Manner 1 Bounce mp-jump
TFD: To rebound after having struck elastic or bounding movement by a
an object or a surface. MW: to heavy or bulky body. [transitive verb]
rebound or reflect after striking a To cause to rebound.
surface (as the ground).
300
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to move quickly
mp-jump; with large jumping movements. LLA:
[intransitive verb] To spring upwards,
Characteristic To move quickly forward with long No
leap; to advance with leaps or springs:
bound Transl Manner 1 use of lower high jumps. TFD: To leap forward or significant
said both of inanimate and animate
body (Steps- upward; spring. To progress by difference
objects.
large jumps) forward leaps or springs. To bounce;
rebound. MW: to move by leaping .
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
bend your head or body forward,
especially as a way of showing
Intransitive uses. To bend the body,
someone respect or expressing. TFD:
knee, or head, in token of reverence,
To bend or curve downward; stoop.
respect, or submission; to make
Posture (Bend, To bend (the head, knee, or body) to
bow Self-c AM Manner 1 obeisance. [transitive verb] To cause (a
Twist, Curve) express greeting, consent, courtesy,
thing) to bend; to force or bring into a
acknowledgment, submission, or
curved or angular shape; to inflect,
veneration. MW: to bend the head,
curve, crook. arch. and dial.
body, or knee in reverence,
submission, or shame. to cause to
incline.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to roll
a ball along a smooth grass or artificial
[intransitive verb] To play at bowls; to
surface during a game. TFD: roll (a
trundle or roll a bowl, etc. along the
Revolve, Rotate, ball).move along on or as if on wheels
bowl Transl AM Manner 1 ground. [transitive verb] To cause to
Spin or a wheeled vehicle. MW: to roll a
roll, to send with a rolling or revolving
ball in bowling. to roll (a ball) in
motion (a bowl, a hoop, etc.).
bowling. to travel smoothly and
rapidly (as in a wheeled vehicle).
AS NOUN: a large vehicle in which
people are driven from one place to
[intransitive verb] To travel by bus; also
another. [transitive verb] to take
in phr. to bus it. colloq. [transitive verb]
people somewhere by bus.
Vehicle / To transport (people) by bus from one
bus Transl AM Manner 1 TFD:[intransitive and transitive verb]
Instrument place to aNOther, esp. in order to
to transport in a bus; to travel in a bus.
encourage or achieve racial integration.
MW: [intransitive verb] to travel by
Hence bus(s)ed ppl. a. U.S.
bus. to work as a busboy. [transitive
verb] to transport by bus.
301
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a car with a driver whom
you pay to take you somewhere. TFD:
Vehicle / [intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] (Also to cab it). To
cab Transl NO Manner 1
Instrument ride or travel in a taxicab; to ride a travel or go in a cab. To drive a cab.
taxicab. MW: [intransitive verb] to
travel in a cab.
AS NOUN: a fast dance, originally
performed in France in the 19th AS NOUN: A kind of dance made
century, in which a row of women on popular at the public balls in Paris, with
cancan Self-c Manner 1 Dance
a stage kick their legs high and lift extravagant and indecent gestures. NO
their skirts. NO FOUND AS FOUND AS MOTION VERB
MOTION VERB
AS NOUN: a small light narrow boat,
pointed at both ends and moved using
a paddle (= a short pole with a flat
blade). [intransitive and transitive
verb] to travel in a canoe. TFD:
Vehicle / [transitive verb] To carry or send by [intransitive verb] To paddle or propel a
canoe Transl AM Manner 1
Instrument canoe. [intransitive verb] to travel in canoe; to move as in a canoe.
or propel a canoe. MW: [transitive
verb] to transport in a canoe; also : to
travel by canoe down (a river).
[intransitive verb] to go or travel in a
canoe.
Etymology: short for obsolete
canterbury, noun (canter), from [intransitive verb] Of a horse, etc.: To
Canterbury, England; from the move in a moderate gallop, raising the
supposed gait of pilgrims riding to two fore-feet nearly at the same time
Canterbury. [intransitive verb] If a with a leap or spring. Of the rider. To
canter1 Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast mp-run
horse canters, it moves at a quite fast run or move as in a canter; to move
but easy and comfortable speed. TFD: nimbly or briskly. [transitive verb] To
To go or move at a canter. MW: make (a horse) go at a canter, to ride at a
[intransitive verb] to move at or as if canter.
at a canter: lope.
Vehicle / TFD: To ride a horse at a canter. MW:
canter2 Transl Manner 1 Instrument (on a [intransitive verb] to ride a horse at a [intransitive verb] Of the rider.
horse); Rate-fast canter
302
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
Etymology: probably by shortening &
alteration from capriole. [intransitive
Low
verb] to run and jump about in an [intransitive verb] To dance or leap in a
ratings.
energetic, happy way. TFD: (as noun) frolicsome manner, to skip for
caper Transl Manner 1 Playful Motion NO
A playful leap or hop. To leap or frisk merriment; to prance as a horse. Also
significant
about; frolic. MW: [intransitive verb] with about, away.
difference
to leap or prance about in a playful
manner.
[intransitive verb] to perform a
capriole, of horses in dressage. TFD:
To perform a capriole (=An upward
leap made by a trained horse without
going forward and with a backward
[intransitive verb] To leap, skip, caper.
capriole Self-c Manner 1 mp-jump kick of the hind legs at the height of
Also said of horses (and their riders)
the leap. A playful leap or jump; a
caper.). MW: AS NOUN: a playful
leap. a vertical leap with a backward
kick of the hind legs at the height of
the leap. [intransitive verb]
Etymology: perhaps from Spanish
capuzar or Catalan cabussar to thrust
(the head) underwater. [intransitive
[transitive verb] To upset, overturn (esp.
and transitive verb] to (cause a boat or
capsize Self-c AM Manner 1 Upside Down on the water). [intransitive verb] (for
ship to) turn upside down accidentally
refl.) To be upset or overturned.
while on water. TFD: To overturn or
cause to overturn. MW: to cause to
overturn. To turn over.
AS NOUN: UK (US trailer) a wheeled
vehicle for living or travelling in,
especially for holidays, which contains
[intransitive verb] To travel or live in a
Vehicle / beds and cooking equipment and can
caravan Transl AM? Manner 1 caravan. [transitive verb] To convey by
Instrument be pulled by a car. TFD: [intransitive
caravan. rare.
verb] to travel in a caravan. MW:
[intransitive verb] to travel in a
caravan.
303
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
TFD: [transitive verb] To cause (a
ship) to lean to one side; tilt.
[intransitive verb] To lean to one side,
[intransitive verb] [transitive verb] To
as a ship sailing in the wind.To turn a
turn (a ship) over on one side for
ship on its side for cleaning, caulking,
cleaning, caulking, or repairing; to
Posture (to one or repairing. MW: [transitive verb] to
careen 1 Self-c AM Manner 1 clean, caulk, etc. (a ship so turned over).
side) put (a ship or boat) on a beach
[intransitive verb] ‘A ship is said to
especially in order to clean, caulk, or
careen when she inclines to one side, or
repair the hull. to cause to heel over.
lies over when sailing on a wind’.
[intransitive verb] to careen a boat. to
undergo this process. to heel over. to
sway from side to side.
[intransitive verb] to go forward
Rate-fast; quickly while moving from side to
[intransitive verb] [Influenced by
Uncontrolled side. TFD: To rush headlong or
careen2 Transl Manner 1 CAREER] To rush headlong, to hurtle,
Motion/Unsteady carelessly; career. MW: career. to go
esp. with an unsteady motion. Chiefly
Motion? at top speed especially in a headlong
U.S.
manner.
[intransitive verb] (especially of a
vehicle) to move fast and [intransitive verb] To gallop, run or
Rate-fast;
uncontrollably. TFD: move headlong move at full speed. [transitive verb] To
career Transl Manner 1 Uncontrolled
at high speed. MW: [intransitive verb] make (a horse) career. To move swiftly
Motion?
to go at top speed especially in a over.
headlong manner.
[intransitive verb] to jump or move
around in a playful way, sometimes Low
noisily, and often in a sexual way. orig. U.S. [intransitive verb] To curvet, ratings.
cavort Transl Manner 1 Playful Motion TFD: To bound or prance about in a prance, caper about, frisk, bound: said of NO
sprightly manner; caper. MW: a horse, or rider, and hence transf. significant
[intransitive verb] to leap or dance difference
about in a lively manner.
304
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move forward quickly and violently, [intransitive verb] To rush against or
especially towards something which upon, with all one's force, in a hostile
has caused difficulty or annoyance. way; to spur one's horse against at full
to/towards Rate-fast; LLA: To run quickly and with a lot of gallop; to bear down upon, make a
charge Transl Path + Manner 2 mp-run
G Violent Motion energy, especially when you are going violent onset on, attack or assail with
to attack someone or something. TFD: impetuosity. Esp. in military use; also
To rush forward in or as if in a violent said of a powerful animal rushing at any
attack. MW: to rush forward in or as if opponent, of players at football, etc.
in assault.
AS NOUN: a two-wheeled vehicle
that was used in ancient times for
racing and fighting and was pulled by
a horse. TFD: [intransitive and [transitive verb] To carry or convey in a
Vehicle /
chariot Transl AM Manner 1 transitive verb] To convey or ride in a chariot: [intransitive verb] To drive or
Instrument
chariot. MW: [intransitive verb] to ride in a chariot.
drive or ride in or as if in a chariot.
[transitive verb] to carry in or as if in a
chariot.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb] To pursue with a view
hurry after someone or something in
to catching. To run with speed; to hurry
order to catch them. TFD: To follow
or rush along. [transitive verb] To
chase Transl Path + Manner 2 after G Rate-fast rapidly in order to catch or overtake; mp-run
pursue for prey or sport; to hunt.
pursue. To go or follow in pursuit.
[transitive verb] To pursue or run after
MW: to follow rapidly. to chase an
in play.
animal, person, or thing.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move in a circle, often around [transitive verb] To make the circuit of,
Path-shape something. TFD: [transitive verb] To move round.
circle Transl Manner 1
(circular) move in a circle around. [intransitive [intransitive verb] To move in a circle
verb] To move in a circle. MW: to (around, about, etc.).
move in or as if in a circle
[transitive verb] To go, pass, move, or
(Motion) + [intransitive and transitive verb] TFD: travel round; to make the circuit of,
circuit Transl 1
Ground make a circuit. MW: to make a circuit. compass about. [intransitive verb] To go
or move in a circuit.
305
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move around or through something, or [intransitive verb] To move round, turn
to make something move around or round, revolve, formerly round an axis
through something. TFD: cause to (obs.), or in an orbit (arch.); Now round
(Motion) +
circulate Transl AM 1 move in a circuit or system; move in a circuit, circuitous course, system of
Ground
circles. MW: to move in a circle, pipes, or the like. [transitive verb] To go
circuit, or orbit; especially, to follow or run round; to encircle, encompass,
a course that returns to the starting surround.
point.
[intransitive verb] to climb up, across
Forced Motion or into somewhere with difficulty, [intransitive verb] To climb by catching
(Energy-low); using the hands and the feet. TFD: To hold with hands and feet; to creep or
clamber Transl Manner 1
Use of one's climb with difficulty, especially on all crawl up (or down); to climb with
hands and legs fours; scramble. MW: to climb difficulty and effort.
awkwardly (as by scrambling).
[intransitive and transitive verb] to use
your legs, or your legs and hands, to
go up or onto the top of something.
TFD: To move oneself upward,
[intransitive verb] To raise oneself by
especially by using the hands and
grasping or clinging, or by the aid of
feet.To rise slowly, steadily, or
hands and feet; ‘to mount by means of
effortfully; ascend.To move upward
some hold or footing’ (J.); to creep up;
Use of one's on or mount, especially by using the
climb Transl Path + Manner 2 up/onto G to ascend, come, or go up, a
hands and legs hands and feet or the feet alone;
perpendicular or steep place. Often with
ascend. To move in a specified
up.
direction by using the hands and feet.
to climb down: to descend by the same
MW: to go upward or raise oneself
means
especially by grasping or clutching
with the hands. to go about or down
usually by grasping or holding with
the hands.
[intransitive verb] To move the wings
[intransitive verb] TFD: to move rapidly; to fly rapidly. [intransitive verb]
clip Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast
rapidly. MW: NO FOUND (colloq.) To move or run quickly. Cf.
cut. U.S.
306
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk noisily with
mp-walk; Noisy
slow, heavy steps. LLA: To walk with [intransitive verb] To walk or tread
activity;
slow, heavy, noisy steps, for example heavily and clumsily. [This has
Characteristic
clump Transl Manner 1 because you are wearing heavy shoes. associations with CLUMP, or its Dutch mp-walk
use of lower
TFD: To walk or move so as to make sources. People clump with klumpen or
body (Steps-
a heavy dull sound. MW: to walk or wooden shoes.]
heavy, slow)?
move clumsily and noisily.
AS NOUN: a long motor vehicle with
comfortable seats, used to take groups [intransitive verb] To ride or drive in a
Vehicle / of people on journeys.TFD: coach or motor coach. [transitive
coach Transl AM? Manner 1
Instrument [intransitive and transitive verb] to verb]To convey in, seat in, provide with,
transport by or ride in a coach. MW: a coach.
[intransitive verb] to go in a coach.
TFD: [intransitive verb] to walk [intransitive verb and transitive verb] To
coggle Self-c Manner 1 Unsteady Motion unsteadily; dodder, toddle. To move shake from side to side; to be unsteady;
unsteadily; wobble. MW: NO FOUND to wabble.
[intransitive and transitive verb] TFD:
[transitive verb] To twist in or into a
to form concentric rings or spirals; To
circular, spiral, or winding shape; to
wind in concentric rings or spirals.
Revolve, Rotate, twist or wind round (something).
coil Self-c AM Manner 1 MW: [intransitive verb] to move in a
Spin [intransitive verb] (for refl.) To throw
circular or spiral course . to form or lie
oneself into a spiral or winding form, to
in a coil. [transitive verb] to wind into
twist oneself round.
rings or spirals
307
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
An elementary intransitive verb of
motion, expressing movement towards
or so as to reach the speaker, or the
person spoken to, or towards a point
where the speaker in thought or
imagination places himself, or (when he
is not himself in question) towards the
[intransitive verb] to move or travel
person who forms the subject of his
towards the speaker or with the
Deixis: narrative. It is thus often used in
speaker. TFD: To advance toward the
come Transl Path 1 towards G opposition to go, although the latter does
speaker or toward a specified place;
(speaker) not primarily involve direction, and is
approach. MW: to move toward
often used without reference thereto.
something.
Come is also used merely of the
accomplishment of the movement,
involved in reaching or becoming
present at any place or point; and
sometimes the entrance upon motion,
involved in issuing from a source, is
alone, or at least chiefly.
[transitive verb] to lead someone to a
[transitive verb] To go with, or before,
Figure + CO- particular place. TFD: To act as a
conduct Transl 2 and show the way to (any one); to lead,
MOTION conductor.To lead. MW: to show the
escort, guide.
way.
AS NOUN: a Latin American dance in AS NOUN: A Latin-American dance of
which a chain of people hold each African origin, usu. performed by
other around the waist and follow the several people in single file and
conga Self-c Manner 1 Dance
leader around using simple steps and consisting of three steps forward
kicks. [intransitive verb] TFD: to followed by a kick. Also as v.
perform this dance. MW: NO FOUND [intransitive verb], to dance the conga.
[transitive verb] to travel with a
vehicle or group of people to make
[transitive verb] To accompany, escort.
Figure + CO- certain that they arrive safely. TFD:
convoy Transl 2 To accompany as guide or conductor; to
MOTION [transitive verb] to accompany,
conduct, guide.
especially for protection; to escort.
MW: to escort for protection.
308
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to hit
[transitive verb] To break in pieces with
something, often making a loud noise
violence and noise; to dash in pieces,
or causing damage. TFD: To undergo
CONCURRENT shiver, shatter, smash. [intransitive verb]
crash Transl 1 sudden damage or destruction on
RESULT To break or fall to pieces with noise, as
impact. To move noisily or so as to
when dashed down or violently struck;
cause damage. MW: o fall, land, or hit
to smash, break up.
with destructive force.
[intransitive verb] to move slowly or
with difficulty, especially (of a person)
[intransitive verb] To move slowly in a
Contact with the body stretched out along the
prone position, by dragging the body
[between F and ground or on hands and knees. TFD:
along close to the ground, as a child
crawl Transl Manner 1 G]: Body close To move slowly on the hands and mp-walk
upon its hands and knees, any short-
to ground; Rate- knees or by dragging the body along
limbed quadruped or reptile, an insect,
slow the ground; creep. MW: To move
serpent, worm, slug.
slowly in a prone position without or
as if without the use of limbs.
[intransitive verb] to move slowly,
quietly and carefully, usually in order
to avoid being noticed. TFD: [intransitive verb] To move softly,
[intransitive verb] To move stealthily cautiously, timorously, or slowly; to
Furtive Motion;
creep1 Transl Manner 1 or cautiously. MW: to go timidly or move quietly and stealthily so as to mp-walk
mp-walk?
cautiously so as to escape notice. elude observation; to steal (into, away,
LLA: To walk quietly and carefully etc.).
because you do not want anyone to
notice you.
[intransitive verb] To move with the
body prone and close to the ground, as a
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move with short-legged reptile, an insect, a
Contact the body close to the ground, as on quadruped moving stealthily, a human
[between F and hands and knees. MW: [intransitive being on hands and feet, or in a
creep2 [crawl] Transl Manner 1
G]: Body close verb] to move along with the body crouching posture. [Formerly said of
to ground prone and close to the ground. to move snakes, worms, and other creatures
slowly on hands and knees. without limbs, for which crawl is now
more usual, though in some cases either
may be used.]
309
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
from Latin cruc-, crux [intransitive
and transitive verb] to go across from
[transitive and intransitive verb] To pass
one side of something to the other.
pass/cross over a line, boundary, river, channel,
cross Transl Path 1 TFD: To go or extend across; pass
G etc.; to pass from one side to the other of
from one side of to the other. MW: to
any space.
move, pass, or extend across
something.
[intransitive verb] To stoop or bend low
with general compression of the body, as
[intransitive verb] to bend your knees
in stooping for shelter, in fear, or in
and lower yourself so that you are
submission; to cower with the limbs
Posture (vertical- close to the ground and leaning
bent. Formerly often applied to the act
crouch Self-c Manner 1 downwards; forward slightly. TFD: To stoop,
of bowing low in reverence or
Bend) especially with the knees bent. MW:
deference. Now said also of the
to lower the body stance especially by
depressed and constrained posture
bending the legs.
assumed by a beast in fear or
submission, or in order to make a spring.
[intransitive verb] To sail to and fro over
[intransitive verb] to travel on ships some part of the sea without making for
for pleasure. TFD: To sail or travel a particular port or landing-place, on the
Vehicle /
cruise Transl Manner 1 about, as for pleasure or look out for ships, for the protection of
Instrument (ship)
reconnaissance. MW: To sail about commerce in time of war, for plunder, or
touching at a series of ports. (in modern times) for pleasure.
[transitive verb] To sail to and fro over.
[intransitive verb] TFD: to leap in a
curvet, to prance; [transitive verb] to
cause to leap in a curvet [i.e., a light [intransitive verb] Of a horse: To
leap by a horse in which both hind execute a curvet, leap in a curvet. Said
curvet Self-c AM Manner 1 mp-jump
legs leave the ground before the also of the horseman. [transitive verb]
forelegs come down. MW: To cause to curvet.
[intransitive verb] To make a curvet;
also : prance, caper.
[intransitive verb] to ride a bicycle.
Vehicle / TFD: [intransitive verb] To ride a
cycle Transl Manner 1 Instrument (bike, bicycle, motorcycle, or similar [intransitive verb]To ride a bicycle or
bicycle) vehicle. MW: [intransitive verb] to tricycle, to travel by cycle.
ride a cycle.
310
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] To leap, skip, hop, or
move the body and feet to music. glide with measured steps and
TFD:To move rhythmically usually to rhythmical movements of the body,
music, using prescribed or improvised usually to the accompaniment of music,
dance Self-c Manner 1 Dance
steps and gestures. [transitive verb] To either by oneself, or with a partner or in
engage in or perform (a dance). to a set. [transitive verb] with the name or
cause to dance. MW: to engage in or description of a dance or measure as
perform a dance. to cause to dance. cognate object.
[intransitive verb] to move quickly or
[intransitive verb] To move like a dart;
suddenly. TFD:To move suddenly and
dart Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast to spring or start with a sudden rapid mp-run
rapidly. MW: to move suddenly or
motion; to shoot.
rapidly.
311
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
from Anglo-French departir, from de-
away from + partir to divide, from Latin partire,
G from part-, pars part . [intransitive [intransitive verb] To go apart or away,
depart Transl Path 1
(departure, verb] to go away or leave, especially with its derived senses.
source) on a journey. TFD: To go away; leave.
MW: To go away; leave.
from Anglo-French descendre, from
Latin descendere, from de- + scandere
[intransitive verb] To move or pass from
to climb. [intransitive verb] to go or
Earth- a higher to a lower position in space; to
come down. [intransitive and
based come or go down, fall, sink. (The
descend Transl Path 1 transitive verb] TFD: To move from a
orientation: general word, including all kinds of
higher to a lower place; come or go
downwards downward motion, vertical or oblique;
down. MW: to pass from a higher
the opposite of ascend.)
place or level to a lower one. to pass,
move, or climb down or down along.
French déserter, from Late Latin
desertare, frequentative of Latin
deserere. [intransitive and transitive
away from [intransitive and transitive verb] To
verb] to leave someone without help
G abandon, forsake, relinquish, give up (a
desert Transl Path 1 or in a difficult situation and not come
(departure, thing); to depart from (a place or
back. TFD:leave someone who needs
source) position).
or counts on you; leave in the lurch.
MW: to withdraw from or leave
usually without intent to return.
Middle French desembarquer, from
down des- dis- + embarquer to embark.
from/away TFD: [intransitive verb] To go ashore
Path + Ground
AM 2 from G from a ship. To leave a vehicle or
(ship)
(departure, aircraft. [transitive verb] To take [intransitive verb] To go on shore from a
source) ashore from a ship. MW: to go ashore ship; to land. [transitive verb] trans. To
disembark Trans out of a ship. put ashore from a ship; to land.
312
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to jump into water,
especially with your head and arms
going in first, or to move down under [Iintransitive verb] To descend or plunge
the water. LLA: To jump into water. into or under water or other liquid.
TFD: To plunge, especially headfirst, (Usually, unless otherwise stated, to
down into Forced Motion; plunge head-foremost.). To descend or
dive Transl Path + Manner 2 into water. To go toward the bottom of
G Head first fall precipitously with increasing
a body of water; submerge. To fall
head down through the air. MW: to momentum.
plunge into water intentionally and
especially headfirst. to come or drop
down precipitously.
from Middle French & Latin; Middle
French divertir, from Latin divertere to [transitive verb] To turn aside (a thing,
turn in opposite directions. [transitive as a stream, etc.) from its (proper)
Change
divert Transl AM Path 1 verb] to cause something or someone direction or course; to deflect (the
Direction
to change direction. TFD: To turn course of something); to turn from one
aside from a course or direction. MW: destination or object to another.
to turn aside : deviate.
[intransitive verb] TFD: to shake or [intransitive verb] To tremble or shake
tremble, as from old age; totter. To from frailty. To proceed or move
Self-c / progress in a feeble, unsteady manner. unsteadily or with tottering gait; to
dodder Manner 1 Unsteady Motion mp-walk
Transl MW: to tremble or shake from totter; to potter.
weakness or age. to progress feebly
and unsteadily.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
avoid being hit by something by [intransitive verb] To avoid an encounter
moving quickly to one side. TFD: with (a person or thing) by changes of
Posture (to one
dodge1 Self-c Manner 1 [intransitive verb] To move aside or in position, shifts, or doublings; to elude (a
side)
a given direction by shifting or pursuer, etc.) by shifts or sideward
twisting suddenly. MW: an act of movements.
evading by sudden bodily movement.
313
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
avoid being hit by something by [intransitive verb] To avoid an encounter
moving quickly to one side. TFD: with (a person or thing) by changes of
dodge2 Transl Path 1 away from [intransitive verb] To move aside or in position, shifts, or doublings; to elude (a
a given direction by shifting or pursuer, etc.) by shifts or sideward
twisting suddenly. MW: an act of movements.
evading by sudden bodily movement.
AS NOUN: a sledge pulled by dogs.
Vehicle / TFD: [intransitive verb] travel with a AS NOUN: dog-sled, -sledge, a sledge
dogsled Transl NO Manner 1
Instrument dogsled, to ride (on) a dogsled. MW: drawn by dogs, as in the Arctic regions.
NO FOUND AS MOTION VERB.
[intransitive or transitive verb] (in
football or hockey) to move a ball
along the ground with repeated small
[transitive verb] In Assoc. Football, etc.
kicks or hits, or (in basketball) to
To keep (the ball) moving along the
move a ball by repeatedly hitting it
ground in front of and close to one by a mp-walk /
dribble Transl Manner 1 Motion in sports against the floor with your hand. TFD:
rapid succession of short pushes, instead mp-run
[intransitive verb] To dribble a ball or
of sending it as far as possible by a
puck. [transitive verb] To move (a ball
vigorous kick.
or puck) by repeated light bounces or
kicks, as in basketball or soccer. MW:
to dribble a ball or puck.
[intransitive verb] to move slowly,
especially as a result of outside forces,
with no control over direction. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To be carried along [intransitive verb] To move as driven or
by currents of air or water. [transitive borne along by a current; to float or
drift Transl AM CAUSE 1 verb] To cause to be carried in a move along with the stream or wind.
current. MW: to become driven or [transitive verb] To drive or carry along,
carried along (as by a current of water, as by a current of water or air.
wind, or air). to cause to be driven in a
current. to move or float smoothly and
effortlessly.
314
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move or travel on land in a motor
[intransitive and transitive verb] To
vehicle, especially as the person
guide a vehicle or the animal that draws
controlling the vehicle's movement.
it, to act as driver; also, to travel or be
TFD: [intransitive verb] To operate a
Vehicle / conveyed in a carriage under one's own
drive Transl AM Manner 1 vehicle, such as a car.To go or be
Instrument direction or at one's disposal. Also
transported in a vehicle. [transitive
[intransitive verb] (for pass.), of the
verb] To convey or transport in a
vehicle. [transitive verb] To carry or
vehicle. MW: To operate a vehicle. to
convey in a vehicle.
have oneself carried in a vehicle. to
convey in a vehicle.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to fall
[intransitive verb] To fall vertically, like
Earth- or to allow something to fall. TFD:
a single drop, under the simple influence
based To fall from a higher to a lower place
drop Transl AM Path 1 of gravity; to descend. To have an
orientation: or position. To let fall by releasing
abrupt descent in position. [transitive
downwards hold of. MW: to fall unexpectedly or
verb] To let fall
suddenly. To let fall : cause to fall.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move slowly with gradual movements
or in gradual stages, or to make
someone or something move in this
way. TFD: [intransitive verb] To move [intransitive verb] To move edgeways;
edge Transl AM Manner 1 Gradual stages gradually or hesitantly. MW: to to advance (esp. obliquely) by repeated mp-walk
advance by short moves. LLA: To almost imperceptible movements.
walk slowly and carefully, especially
sideways, along or through a small
space because you do not have enough
room to walk normally.
[intransitive verb] to go onto a ship.
TFD: [transitive verb] To cause to
board a vessel or aircraft. [intransitive [intransitive verb] To go on board ship;
Path + Ground verb] To go aboard a vessel or aircraft, to take ship. [transitive] To put on board
embark Transl AM 2 up/onto G
(ship) as at the start of a journey. MW: to go ship, make to go on board. Of the ship:
on board a vehicle for transportation. To receive on board.
to cause to go on board (as a boat or
airplane).
315
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to leave a country
[intransitive verb] To remove out of a
away from permanently and go to live in another
country for the purpose of settling in
Path + Ground G one. TFD: To leave one country or
emigrate Transl 2 another. [transitive verb] To cause or
(country) (departure, region to settle in another. MW: to
assist to emigrate; to send out to settle in
source) leave one's place of residence or
a foreign country.
country to live elsewhere.
Middle English entren, from Anglo-
French entrer, from Latin intrare, from [intransitive verb] To go or come in.To
intra within. [intransitive verb] to go or come into a place, building, room,
enter Transl Path 1 into G come or go into a particular place. etc.; to pass within the boundaries of a
TFD: [intransitive and transitive country, region, portion of space,
verb]To come or go in; make an entry. medium, etc.
MW: to go or come in.
from Anglo-French escaper, eschaper,
[intransitive verb] To gain one's liberty
from Vulgar Latin *excappare, from
by flight; to get free from detention or
Latin ex- + Late Latin cappa head
away from control, or from an oppressive or
covering, cloak. [intransitive and
G irksome condition. [transitive verb] To
escape Transl Path 1 transitive verb] to get free from
(departure, effect one's flight from (prison); to free
something, or to avoid something.
source) oneself from (a person's grasp or
TFD:To break loose from
control); to get safely out of (painful or
confinement; get free MW: to get
dangerous conditions).
away (as by flight).
[transitive verb] to go with someone or [transitive verb] To act as escort to. a.
a vehicle especially to make certain Mil. of armed men; Naut. of a convoy.
Figure + CO-
escort Transl 2 that they arrive safely or that they b. In wider sense: To accompany for the
MOTION
leave a place. TFD, MW: To purpose of protecting or conducting, or
accompany as an escort. of showing civility.
Latin, he goes out, from exire to go
out, from ex- + ire to go. [intransitive [intransitive verb] To make one's exit,
exit Transl Path 1 out of G
and transitive verb] TFD: To go out depart, disappear
of; leave. MW: To go out or away.
316
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
from Old English feallan [intransitive
[intransitive verb] To descend freely
verb] to come down onto the ground
Earth- (primarily by ‘weight’ or gravity):
or from a high position to a lower
based opposed to ‘rise’. To drop from a high or
fall Transl Path 1 position. TFD:To drop or come down
orientation: relatively high position. To drop, come
freely under the influence of gravity.
downwards or go down, in a given direction or to a
MW: to descend freely by the force of
required position.
gravity.
AS NOUN: a boat or ship for taking
passengers and often vehicles across
an area of water, especially as a [transitive verb] To carry, convey,
regular service. [transitive verb] to transport, take from one place to
transport people or goods in a vehicle, another. esp. To transport or convey
especially regularly and often. TFD: over water (now only over a stream,
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] To cross a body of canal, etc., formerly also over the sea) in
ferry Transl AM Manner
Instrument water on or as if on a ferry. [transitive a boat or ship, etc. Often to ferry (a
verb] To cross a body of water on or person, etc.) over or across. [intransitive
as if on a ferry. MW: [transitive verb] verb] To convey oneself, go; now only,
to carry by boat over a body of water. to pass over water in a boat or by a ferry.
to cross by a ferry. to convey (as by Of a boat: To pass to and fro.
aircraft or motor vehicle) from one
place to another.
[intransitive verb] to walk in a line,
Motion in file; one behind another. TFD: To march or [intransitive verb] To march or move in
file Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
mp-walk? walk in a line. MW: to march or file.
proceed in single file.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
wave something, especially wings
[transitive verb] Of a bird: To strike with
when or as if flying. TFD: [intransitive
the flat of the wing; also to drive off
verb] To move or sway while fixed at
(etc.) by flapping. [intransitive verb] Of
Shake, Swing, one edge or corner; flutter. [transitive
flap Self-c AM Manner 1 anything attached at one extremity or
Vibrate verb] To cause to move or sway with a
loosely fastened: To swing or sway
fluttering or waving motion. MW: To
about loosely; to flutter or oscillate as
beat or pulsate wings or something
when moved by the wind.
suggesting wings. To progress by
flapping. To flutter ineffectively.
317
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
318
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to stay on the
surface of a liquid and not sink.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) move easily through, or
along the surface of a liquid, or to
[intransitive verb] To rest on the surface
(cause to) move easily through air.
of any liquid; to be buoyed up; to be or
TFD: To remain suspended within or
become buoyant. To move quietly and
Self-c / on the surface of a fluid without
float AM Manner 1 Smooth Motion gently on the surface of a liquid,
Transl sinking.To be suspended in or move
participating in its motion. To move
through space as if supported by a
freely and gently in or through the air, as
liquid. To cause to remain suspended
if buoyed up or carried along by it.
without sinking or falling. MW: to rest
on the surface of or be suspended in a
fluid. to drift on or through or as if on
or through a fluid. to cause to float in
or on the surface of a fluid.
[transitive verb] TFD: To make Low
[intransitive verb] In early use, to
Unsteady clumsy attempts to move or regain ratings.
stumble. Subsequently, to struggle
flounder Self-c Manner 1 Motion; one's balance. MW: [intransitive verb] NO
violently and clumsily; to plunge, roll
Ackward Motion to struggle to move or obtain footing. significant
and tumble about in or as in mire.
CAE: to move awkwardly. difference
[intransitive verb] Of birds, etc.: To
[intransitive and transitive verb] to move or flap the wings rapidly without
make a series of quick delicate flying or with short flights; to move up
movements up and down or from side and down or to and fro in quick irregular
to side, or to cause something to do motions, or hang upon wing in the air.
Shake, Swing, this. TFD: To wave or flap rapidly in To move with a light quivering motion
flutter Self-c AM Manner 1
Vibrate an irregular manner. To vibrate or beat through the air. [intransitive verb] To
rapidly or erratically. To cause to move about or to and fro with quick
flutter. MW: to flap the wings rapidly. vibrations or undulations; to quiver.
to move with quick wavering or [transitive verb] (causatively) To cause
flapping motions. To cause to flutter. to flutter; to move (a thing) in quick
irregular motions; to agitate, ruffle.
319
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] When a bird, insect
or aircraft flies, it moves through the
air. [intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb] To move through the
travel by aircraft, or to go somewhere
air with wings. [intransitive and
or cross something in an aircraft. TFD:
transitive verb] To travel by aircraft.
fly Transl Ground (air) 1 To move through the air by means of
Also [transitive verb], to cover, traverse,
wings or winglike parts.To travel by
or perform by aircraft (also said of the
air. MW: to move in or pass through
machine).
the air with wings b: to move through
the air or before the wind or through
outer space.
[transitive verb] To go or come after (a
person or other object in motion); to
[intransitive and transitive verb] to move behind in the same direction. To
move behind someone or something go forward along (a path), to keep in (a
follow Transl Path 1 after G and go where they go. TFD: To come track) as one goes. [intransitive verb] To
or go after; proceed behind. MW: to go or come after a person or thing in
go, proceed, or come after. motion; to move behind some object;
also, to go as a person's attendant or
companion.
[transitive verb] to cross a river, where
it is not deep, on foot or in a vehicle.
[transitive verb] To cross (water) by
mp-walk; TFD: cross a river where it's shallow.
Path + Ground pass/cross means of a ford; to wade through (To
ford Transl 3 Obstructed MW: to cross (a body of water) by
(river) + Manner G walk through water or any liquid or soft
motion? wading (=to step in or through a
substance which impedes motion).
medium (as water) offering more
resistance than air).
AS NOUN: (a piece of music for) a
type of formal ballroom dance that
combines short quick steps with
foxtrot Self-c Manner 1 Dance v. [intransitive verb], to dance a fox-trot
longer ones in various patterns.
[intransitive verb] TFD: To dance the
fox trot. MW: to dance the fox-trot.
Low
[intransitive verb] TFD: To move
[intransitive verb] Of living beings: To ratings.
about briskly and playfully; frolic.
frisk Transl Manner 1 Playful Motion move briskly and sportively; to dance, NO
MW: to leap, skip, or dance in a lively
frolic, gambol, jig. significant
or playful way.
difference
320
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to behave in a Low
happy and playful way. TFD: To [intransitive verb] To make merry; in ratings.
frolic Transl Manner 1 Playful Motion behave playfully and uninhibitedly; later use, to play pranks, gambol, caper NO
romp. MW: to play and run about about. significant
happily : romp. difference
[intransitive and transitive verb](of a
horse) to run fast so that all four feet
[intransitive verb] Of a horse (occas. of
come off the ground together in each
mp-run; Rate- other quadrupeds): to go at a gallop.
gallop1 Transl AM Manner 1 act of forward movement. TFD: To mp-run
fast [transitive verb] To make (a horse, etc.)
move or progress swiftly. To cause to
go at full speed.
gallop. MW: to run fast. to cause to
gallop.
[intransitive and transitive verb] (of a
Vehicle / person) to ride a horse that is running
[intransitive verb] Of a horseman: To
gallop2 Transl Manner 1 Instrument (on a in this way. TFD: To ride a horse at a mp-run
ride at full speed.
horse); Rate-fast gallop. MW: to progress or ride at a
gallop.
Low
[intransitive verb] to run and jump in a
[intransitive verb] To leap or spring, in ratings.
happy and playful way. TFD: To leap
gambol Transl Manner 1 Playful Motion dancing or sporting; Now chiefly of NO
about playfully; frolic. MW: to skip
animals or children. significant
about in play.
difference
mp-walk; State
of the Figure
(injured) → [intransitive verb] TFD: to walk with a
gimp [limp,
Transl Manner 1 Unsteady limp. MW: [intransitive verb] limp, NO found as motion verb
hobble]
Motion; Forced hobble. MW: limp, hobble.
Motion (Energy-
low)
321
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] To pass from one
place to another by a smooth and
[intransitive verb] to move easily continuous movement, without effort or
without stopping and without effort or difficulty. Along the surface of, or
noise. TFD: To move in a smooth, through, a liquid. in general. Now often
Contact
effortless manner. To move silently applied to the progression of a person
[between F and
glide1 Transl AM Manner 1 and furtively. [transitive verb] To walking or riding, of a carriage, etc., to
G]; Smooth
cause to move or pass smoothly, express extreme smoothness of
Motion
silently, or imperceptibly. MW: to movement and the absence of
move smoothly, continuously, and perceptible motion of the limbs, wheels,
effortlessly. to cause to glide. etc. Said of the mode of progression of
reptiles. [transitive verb] to cause to
glide (in different senses).
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
silently and furtively. [transitive verb] [intransitive verb] To go unperceived,
glide2 Transl AM Manner 1 Furtive Motion To cause to move or pass smoothly, quietly, or stealthily; to insinuate
silently, or imperceptibly. MW: to go oneself, steal, ‘slip’ into, out of a place.
or pass imperceptibly.
An intransitive verb of motion, serving
as the most general expression (I) for a
[intransitive verb] to travel or move to movement viewed without regard to its
Deixis: another place. TFD: To move or point of departure or destination; (II) for
away from travel; proceed. To move away from a a movement away from the speaker, or
go Transl Path 1
G place; depart. MW: o move on a from the point at which he mentally
(speaker) course. to move out of or away from a places himself; and (III) for a movement
place expressed or implied. to or towards a place which is neither in
fact nor in thought that occupied by the
speaker.
AS NOUN: a narrow boat with a
raised point at both ends, which is [intransitive verb] To travel in a
gondola - Vehicle /
Transl NO Manner used on canals in Venice and is moved gondola. [transitive verb] to propel (a
gondole [verb] Instrument
by a man with a pole. TFD, MW: NO gondola).
FOUND AS MOTION VERB
322
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: An elementary drill in
which the recruit is taught to balance his
mp-walk;
[intransitive verb] a special way of body on either leg alternately, and swing
Characteristic
marching with the legs lifted high and the other backwards and forwards. b.
use of lower
goosestep Transl Manner 1 straight. TFD: NO FOUND AS A balance step, practised esp. by various mp-walk
body (Steps-
MOTION VERB. MW: to march in a armies in marching on ceremonial
regular; Legs-
goose step. parades, in which the legs are alternately
lifted high)
advanced without bending the knees.
[jntransitive verb] to practise this drill
[intransitive verb] to move close to or
Contact
on the ground. TFD: To lie or creep in [intransitive verb] to move with the
[between F and
a prostrate position, as in subservience body prostrate upon the ground. To
G]: Body close
grovel Transl Manner 1 or humility. MW: to creep with the grovel in the dust or dirt (fig.): to
to ground: State
face to the ground. to lie or creep with humble oneself, perform an act of
fo Figure
the body prostrate in token of humiliation.
(humiliated)
subservience or abasement.
[transitive verb] to show people round
[transitive verb] To act as guide to; to go
Figure + CO- a place. TFD: To serve as a guide for;
guide Transl 2 with or before for the purpose of leading
MOTION conduct.MW: to act as a guide to :
the way
direct in a way or course.
[intransitive verb] to run or go very
Rate-fast; mp-
quickly, usually in an uncontrolled
run; [intransitive verb] To run or move with
hare Transl Manner 1 way. TFD: To move hurriedly, as if
Uncontrolled great speed.
hunting a swift quarry. MW: to go
Motion?
swiftly .
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move or [intransitive verb] To make haste; to
act swiftly. [transitive verb] To cause come, go, or act quickly. [transitive mp-run /
hasten Transl AM Manner 1 Rate-increase
to hurry. To speed up; accelerate. verb] To cause to make haste; to urge mp-walk
MW: to move or act quickly. on; to accelerate, expedite, hurry.
323
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a type of aircraft without
wings, that has one or two sets of large
blades which go round very fast on
[intransitive and transitive verb] to fly
Vehicle / top. TFD: [intransitive and transitive
helicopter Transl AM Manner 1 with or as with a helicopter; to transport
Instrument verb] To go or transport by helicopter.
by helicopter
MW: [intransitive verb] to travel by
helicopter. [transitive verb] to
transport by helicopter.
[intransitive verb] to go for a long
walk in the countryside. LLA: To take [intransitive verb] To walk or march
mp-walk; a long walk in the countryside. TFD: vigorously or laboriously. To walk for
hike Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
Leisurely Motion To go on an extended walk for pleasure; to go for a long walk, or
pleasure or exercise. MW: to go on a walking tour, spec. in the country.
hike.
[intransitive verb] to walk in an
mp-walk; State awkward way, usually because the feet
of Figure or legs are injured. LLA: To walk with
[intransitive verb]To walk with an
(injured) → difficulty in a slow and unsteady way
unsteady rising and falling gait, as one
hobble Transl Manner 1 Unsteady because it is painful for you to walk. mp-walk
whose limbs give way under him; to
Motion; Forced TFD: o walk or move along haltingly
walk lamely and with difficulty; to limp.
Motion (Energy- or with difficulty; limp. MW: to move
low) along unsteadily or with difficulty;
especially : to limp along.
324
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] Of a person: To
spring or leap on one foot, or move
[intransitive verb] to jump on one foot onwards by a succession of such leaps.
or to move about in this way. If a To spring a short way upon the ground
small animal, bird or insect hops, it or any surface with an elastic or
moves by jumping on all or two of its bounding movement, or a succession of
mp-jump;
Self-c / feet at the same time. LLA: To jump such movements: said of persons,
Characteristic
hop Transl Manner 1 or move forward on one leg rather animals, and things. Formerly a general mp-jump
use of lower
? than two. TFD: To move with light synonym of leap; now implying a short
body
bounding skips or leaps. MW: to move or undignified leap. Of animals: To
by a quick springy leap or in a series move by leaps with both or all the feet at
of leaps; also : to move as if by once, as opposed to walking or running:
hopping. said esp. of small birds, frogs, grass-
hoppers, sand-hoppers, fleas, and the
like.
[transitive verb] to chase someone or
refuse to leave them alone, especially
because you want to get something [transitive verb] To hunt, chase, or
hound Transl Path 1 after G mp-run
from them. TFD: To pursue pursue with hounds, or as a dog does.
relentlessly and tenaciously. MW: to
pursue with or as if with hounds.
[intransitive motion] Of a winged
creature: To hang or remain suspended
[intransitive verb] to stay in one place in the air over or about a particular spot,
in the air, usually by moving the wings as by flapping the wings (to which
quickly. TFD: To remain floating, action the word is sometimes restricted
hover Self-c Ground (on air) 1
suspended, or fluttering in the air. by naturalists: cf. 4), esp. when
MW: to hang fluttering in the air or on preparing to dart or swoop in some
the wing. direction. Of a helicopter or other
aircraft: to remain stationary in the air,
relative to the ground.
325
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to move very fast,
especially in what seems a dangerous
[intransitive verb] To move, or be
way. TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
Rate-fast; Forced carried or driven with violence or
with great speed, force, or violence;
hurl Transl AM Manner 1 Motion (Energy- impetuosity; to rush impetuously; to
hurtle. [transitive verb] to throw
high) dash. [transitive verb] To drive or impel
something with a lot of force, usually
with impetuous force or violence.
in an angry or violent way. MW: rush,
hurtle. to throw down with violence.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move or do things more quickly than [intransitive verb] To move or act with
normal or to make someone do this. excited haste, or with an evident or
TFD: To move or act with speed or apparent effort at speed. [transitive mp-run /
hurry Transl AM Manner 1 Rate-increase
haste.To cause to move or act with verb] To carry, convey, or cause to go mp-walk
speed or haste. MW: to move or act with excessive haste, under the influence
with haste. To carry or cause to go of external pressure or of excitement.
with haste.
[intransitive verb] To strike together or
[intransitive verb] to move very fast,
against something, esp. with violence or
especially in what seems a dangerous
noise; to come into collision; to dash,
way. TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
Rate-fast; Forced clash, impinge; to meet in shock and
with or as if with great speed and a
hurtle Transl AM Manner 1 Motion (Energy- encounter. To dash, rush, hurry; esp. mp-run
rushing noise. [transitive verb] To
high) with noise. [transitive verb] To drive
fling with great force; hurl. MW: to
violently or swiftly; to dash, dart, shoot,
move rapidly or forcefully. [transitive
fling, cast. App. often confounded with
verb] hurl, fling.
hurl.
[intransitive verb] TFD: To enter and
settle in a country or region to which
[intransitive verb] To come to settle in a
one is not native. [transitive verb] To
country (which is not one's own); to pass
Path + Ground send or introduce as immigrants. MW:
immigrate Transl AM 2 into G into a new habitat or place of residence .
(country) To enter and settle in a country or
[transitive verb] trans. To bring in or
region to which one is not native.
introduce as settlers.
[transitive verb] to bring in or send as
immigrants
326
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move very slowly or in a lot of short
[intransitive verb] To move, advance, or
stages. TFD: To move or cause to
retreat, by inches or small degrees.
inch Transl AM Manner 1 Gradual stages move slowly or by small degrees. mp-walk
[transitive verb] To drive by inches or
MW: to move by small degrees:
small degrees.
progress slowly . [transitive verb] to
cause to move slowly.
AS NOUN: a small strong vehicle
used for travelling over rough ground,
Vehicle /
jeep Transl NO Manner 1 especially by the army. TFD: NO [intransitive verb] To travel by jeep.
Instrument
FOUND AS MOTION VERB. MW:
[intransitive verb] to travel by jeep.
AS NOUN: an aircraft with a jet
engine, which is able to fly very fast.
[intransitive verb] To travel by jet plane.
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] to travel somewhere
jet Transl AM? Manner 1 [transitive verb] to convey by jet plane
Instrument by plane. TFD: [intransitive verb] fly a
or jet engine.
jet plane. MW: [intransitive verb] to
travel by jet airplane.
AS NOUN: an energetic traditional
[transitive verb] To dance (a jig or other
dance of Great Britain and Ireland, or
lively dance). [intransitive verb] To
jig1 Self-c Manner 1 Dance the music that is played for such a
dance a jig; to dance in a rapid, jerky,
dance. TFD: To dance or play a jig.
lively fashion.
MW: to dance a jig.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move quickly up and down or from [intransitive verb] To move up and
side to side, or to make someone or down or to and fro with a rapid jerky
Shake, Swing,
jig2 Self-c AM Manner 1 something do this. TFD: To move or motion. [transitive verb] To move (any
Vibrate
bob up and down jerkily and rapidly. thing) with a light jerky motion; to jerk
MW: to move with rapid jerky to and fro or up and down.
motions.
327
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: TFD: A strenuous dance
performed to quick-tempo swing or
jazz music and consisting of various
two-step patterns embellished with [intransitive verb] To dance the
jitterbug Self-c Manner 1 Dance
twirls and sometimes acrobatic jitterbug.
maneuvers. [intransitive verb] to
perform this dance. MW: to dance the
jitterbug.
AS NOUN: a fast dance which was
very popular with young people in the
1940s and 1950s. [intransitive verb]
jive Self-c Manner 1 Dance [intransitive verb] To dance the ‘jive’
dance to jive music. TFD:To play or
dance to jive music. MW: to dance to
or play jive.
[intransitive verb] to run at a slow
[intransitive verb] To walk or ride with a
regular speed, especially as a form of
jolting pace, ‘to move with small shocks
exercise. LLA: To run fairly slowly
like those of a low trot’ (J.); to move on
mp-run; Rate- for a long distance, for exercise, and to
jog Transl Manner 1 at a heavy or laboured pace, to trudge; mp-run
slow, regular keep healthy. TFD: To run or ride at a
hence, to move on, go on, be off. More
steady slow trot. Sports To run in such
recently, to run at a gentle pace (esp. as
a way for sport or exercise. MW: to
part of a ‘keep-fit’ schedule).
run or ride at a slow trot.
[intransitive verb] to travel
somewhere. TFD, MW: [intransitive
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] To make or proceed
journey Transl Manner 1 and transitive verb] To make a
Instrument on a journey; to travel.
journey; travel. To travel over or
through.
328
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to push yourself
suddenly off the ground and into the
air using your legs. [intransitive and
transitive verb] to push yourself [intransitive verb] To make a spring
suddenly off the ground in order to go from the ground or other base by flexion
over something. LLA: To push and sudden muscular extension of the
yourself off the ground or away from legs (or, in the case of some animals, as
something, using your legs.To go over fish, of the tail, or other part); to throw
something by jumping. TFD: oneself upward, forward, backward, or
Self-c /
[intransitive verb] To spring off the downward, from the ground or point of
jump Transl Manner 1 mp-jump
ground or other base by a muscular support; to leap, spring, bound; spec. to
?
effort of the legs and feet. To move leap with the feet together, as opposed to
suddenly and in one motion. hopping on one leg. To move suddenly
[transitive verb] To leap over or with a leap, bound, or the like
across. MW: to spring into the air : movement; to ‘spring’, ‘dart’, ‘shoot’.
leap; especially: to spring free from [transitive verb] To pass clear over by a
the ground or other base by the leap; to leap or spring over; to clear.
muscular action of feet and legs. To
move suddenly or involuntarily.
[transitive verb] to leap over.
AS NOUN: a light narrow canoe with
a covering over the top. TFD:
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] To go, travel, or
kayak Transl NO Manner 1 [intransitive verb] to travel by kayak
Instrument race in a kayak. [transitive verb] To go
or travel on (a body of water) by
kayak. MW: [intransitive verb]
[intransitive verb] (a person) to fall [intransitive verb] To turn or be turned
over suddenly. If a boat keels over, it over; to be upset; to fall over or be felled
turns upside down in the water. TFD: as if by a shock. [transitive verb] To turn
keel Self-c AM Manner 1 Upside Down [intransitive and transitive verb] To up the keel of, show the bottom of. to
capsize or cause to capsize. MW: keel over, to turn over, ‘turn wrong side
[intransitive verb] to fall in or as if in a uppermost’, turn (a man or beast) upon
faint —usually used with over. his back; to upset, capsize.
329
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to hit
someone or something with the foot,
[intransitive verb] To strike out with the
or to move the feet and legs suddenly
Figure (body foot.
kick Self-c 1 and violently. TFD: To strike out with
part: feet, legs) [transitive verb] To strike (anything)
the foot or feet.To strike with the foot.
with the foot.
MW: to strike out with the foot or feet.
to strike, thrust, or hit with the foot.
[intransitive verb] to go down into, or
[intransitive verb] To fall on the knees
stay in, a position where one or both
or a knee; to assume, or remain in, a
Posture (vertical- knees are on the ground. TFD: To go
kneel Self-c Manner 1 posture in which the body is supported
downwards) down or rest on one or both knees.
on the bended knees or on one of them,
MW: to bend the knee : fall or rest on
as in supplication or homage.
the knees.
[intransitive verb] To come to land; to
go ashore from a ship or boat; to
disembark. To arrive at a place, a stage
in a journey, or the like; to come to a
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
stage in a progression; to end in
(cause to) arrive at a place after
something. To alight upon the ground,
moving down through the air. TFD:
e.g. from a vehicle, after a leap, etc. Esp.
To come to shore.To disembark. To
of an aircraft or spacecraft, or a person
descend toward and settle onto the
down onto in one: to alight upon or reach the
Path + Ground ground or another surface. [transitive
land Transl AM 2 G (arrival, ground, or some other surface, after a
(land) verb] To bring to and unload on land.
endpoint) flight. [transitive verb] To bring to land;
MW: to go ashore from a ship. to
to set on shore; to disembark. To bring
come to the end of a course or to a
into a specified place, e.g. as a stage in
stage in a journey. to alight on a
or termination of a journey; to bring into
surface. [transitive verb] to set or put
a certain position. To bring (an aircraft)
on shore from a ship.
to earth from the air; to place (an aircraft
or spacecraft, or its contents) on the
ground or some other surface after a
flight.
330
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to show the way to
a group of people, animals, vehicles,
etc. by going in front of them. TFD:
Figure + CO- To show the way to by going in [transitive verb] To conduct. To cause to
lead Transl 1
MOTION advance. MW: to guide on a way go along with oneself.
especially by going in advance. to
guide someone or something along a
way.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) slope in one direction, or to
move the top part of the body in a
particular direction. TFD: To bend or
Posture (towards slant away from the vertical.To incline [intransitive verb] To recline, lie down,
lean Self-c AM Manner 1 horizontal; the weight of the body so as to be rest. [transitive verb] To cause to bend
Bend) supported. To cause to incline. MW: or incline.
to incline, deviate, or bend from a
vertical position b: to cast one's weight
to one side for support. to cause to
lean.
[intransitive verb] to make a large
jump or sudden movement, usually [intransitive verb] To rise with both (or
from one place to another. LLA: To all four) feet suddenly from the ground
mp-jump; jump as far or high as you can. To go or other standing-place, alighting in
Self-c / Characteristic over something with a long or high some other position; to jump, spring. To
leap Transl Manner 1 use of lower jump. [intransitive and transitive verb] spring suddenly to or upon one's feet; to mp-jump
? body (Long or TFD: To spring or bound upward from rise with a bound from a sitting or
high jump) or as if from the ground; jump. To recumbent position. [transitive verb] To
jump over. MW: to spring free from or spring over; to pass from one side to the
as if from the ground. to pass over by other by leaping.
leaping.
331
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a children's game in which
a number of children bend down and
another child jumps over them one at a
time. TFD: [transitive verb] To jump
Animal-like → [intransitive verb and transitive verb] To
leap-frog Transl Manner 1 over in or as if in leapfrog.
mp-jump leap or vault as at leap-frog.
[intransitive verb] To move forward or
progress in or as if in leapfrog. MW:
to leap or progress in or as if in
leapfrog.
Middle English leven, from Old
English lǣfan; akin to Old High
German verleiben to leave, Old
away from [transitive verb] To depart from, quit,
English belīfan to be left over.
G relinquish. To go away from, quit (a
leave Transl Path 1 [intransitive and transitive verb] to go
(departure, place, person, or thing); to deviate from
away from someone or something, for
source) (a line of road, etc.).
a short time or permanently. TFD:To
go out of or away from. MW: to go
away from.
[intransitive verb] to walk unevenly
and slowly because of having an
mp-walk; State
injured or painful leg or foot. LLA: To
of Figure
walk with difficulty because you have
(injured) →
hurt one of your legs. TFD: To walk [intransitive verb] To walk lamely, to
limp Transl Manner 1 Unsteady mp-walk
lamely, especially with irregularity, as halt.
Motion; Forced
if favoring one leg. MW: to walk
Motion (Energy-
lamely; especially : to walk favoring
low)
one leg. To go unsteadily. to proceed
slowly or with difficulty.
mp-run; Rate-
[intransitive verb] (of a person or
slow;
animal) to run taking long relaxed
Characteristic [intransitive verb] To leap, jump, spring.
lope Transl Manner 1 steps. TFD: To run or ride with a mp-walk
use of lower To run with a long, bounding stride.
steady, easy gait. MW: to move or ride
body (Steps-
at a lope.
long)?
332
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to move slowly and
Rate-slow;
awkwardly. TFD: move heavily or [intransitive verb] To move in a clumsy
lumber Transl Manner 1 Awkward mp-walk
clumsily. MW: to move ponderously or blundering manner.
Motion
(=clumsily).
[intransitive verb] to move forward
suddenly and with force, especially in
order to attack someone. [intransitive
[intransitive verb] To move with a
and transitive verb] TFD: To move
to/towards Forced Motion lunge; to make a sudden forward
lunge Transl AM Path + Manner 2 with a sudden thrust.To cause
G (Energy-high) movement; to rush. [transitive verb] To
(someone) to lunge. MW: to make a
drive or thrust with or as with a lunge.
lunge : move with or as if with a
lunge. to thrust or propel (as a blow)
in a lunge.
[intransitive verb] to move in an
irregular way, especially making
sudden movements backwards or
Unsteady forwards or from side to side. LLA:
[intransitive verb] To move suddenly,
Motion; Jerky, To walk very unsteadily, moving
lurch Self-c Manner 1 unsteadily, and without purpose in any mp-walk
Sudden forward or from side to side with
direction, as, e.g. a person staggering.
movement sudden, irregular movements. TFD:
To stagger. MW: to move with a
lurch (=an abrupt jerking, swaying, or
tipping movement)
333
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
walk with regular steps keeping the
body stiff, usually in a formal group of
people who are all walking in the
same way. LLA: If soldiers march,
they all walk together with regular [intransitive verb] To walk in a military
mp-walk; steps. TFD: [intransitive verb] To manner with regular and measured tread.
Characteristic walk steadily and rhythmically Of a body of men or troops: to walk in
march1 Transl AM Manner 1 use of lower forward in step with others. To step and in time with a regular and mp-walk
body (Steps- participate in an organized walk, as for uniform movement. [transitive verb] To
regular) a public cause. [transitive verb] To cause (a person, army) to march or move
cause to move or otherwise progress in in military order.
a steady rhythmical manner. MW:
[intransitive verb] to move along
steadily usually with a rhythmic stride
and in step with others. [transitive
verb] to cause to march.
[intransitive verb] to walk somewhere
quickly and in a determined way,
often because you are angry. LLA: To
mp-walk; State [intransitive verb] To walk in a steady,
walk quickly and with firm steps,
march2 Transl Manner 1 of Figure (angry, measured, or deliberate manner; to go, mp-walk
especially because you feel angry or
determined)? proceed.
determined. TFD: To proceed directly
and purposefully. MW: to move in a
direct purposeful manner.
[intransitive verb] If a river, stream or
road meanders, it follows a route
which is not straight or direct. To walk [intransitive verb] Of a river, stream,
meander1 Transl Manner 1 Path-shape slowly without any clear direction. etc.: to flow in meanders; to follow a mp-walk
TFD: To follow a winding and turning winding course.
course. MW: to follow a winding or
intricate course.
[intransitive verb] To walk slowly
without any clear direction. TFD: To [intransitive verb] Of a person: to
mp-walk; No
meander2 Transl Manner 1 move aimlessly and idly without fixed wander aimlessly; to follow a circuitous
aim in motion
direction. MW: to wander aimlessly or course.
casually without urgent destination.
334
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk in an
mp-walk;
artificial way, with small delicate [intransitive verb] To walk with short
Characteristic
steps. TFD: To walk with very short steps and an affected preciseness or
mince Transl Manner 1 use of lower mp-walk
steps or with exaggerated primness. daintiness; to walk or move in an
body (Steps-
MW: to walk with short steps in a affected or effeminate manner.
short)
prim affected manner.
AS NOUN: a kind of dance step in
which the dancer seems to be sliding
Manner + [intransitive verb] To walk on the moon.
moonwalk1 Transl 2 mp-walk on the spot. TFD: [intransitive verb]
Ground rare.
To walk on the surface of the moon.
MW: NO FOUND
TFD: NO FOUND. MW: [intransitive [intransitive verb] To perform the
verb] to dance by gliding backwards moonwalk. [transitive verb] To make
moonwalk2 Self-c Manner 1 Dance
while appearing to make forward (one's way) as if doing the moonwalk; to
walking motions. convey (an object) by this means.
AS NOUN: a small motorcycle which AS NOUN: Originally: a motorized
also has pedals which can be used pedal cycle. Now usually: a light two-
Vehicle /
moped Transl NO Manner 1 when starting it or travelling up a hill. wheeled motor vehicle with a small
Instrument
CALD, TFD, MW: NO FOUND AS engine. NO FOUND AS MOTION
MOTION VERB. VERB.
[intransitive verb] to walk or go [intransitive verb] Originally: to go
mp-walk;
slowly, usually without a special away quickly or promptly; to make haste
Leisurely
mosey Transl Manner 1 purpose. TFD: To move in a leisurely, (now rare). Later usually: to walk in a mp-walk
Motion; No aim
relaxed way; saunter. MW: to move in leisurely or aimless manner; to amble,
in Motion
a leisurely or aimless manner. wander.
[intransitive verb] UK OLD-
FASHIONED to drive; TFD:
[intransitive verb] To drive or travel in
Vehicle / a motor vehicle. [transitive verb] To [intransitive verb] To travel or drive in a
motor Transl AM? Manner 1
Instrument carry by motor vehicle. MW: motor vehicle.
[transitive verb] to travel by
automobile. [transitive verb] to
transport by automobile.
335
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: US a small light
motorcycle. A vehicle with two [intransitive verb] To ride on a
motorbike, Vehicle /
Transl NO Manner 1 wheels and an engine. TFD: motorcycle. [intransitive verb] To ride
motorcycle Instrument
[intransitive verb] to ride a a motorcycle.
motorcycle. MW: [intransitive verb]
Senses relating to elevation or upward
movement. [intransitive verb] To fly
upwards, to soar, to ascend. Of a
missile: to rise in its flight. [intransitive
verb] To travel or proceed in an upward
direction; to ascend or climb.
[transitive verb] to go up or onto. [intransitive verb] To ascend to a higher
Earth-
TFD: to climb or to ascend. MW: level in rank, estimation, power,
based
[intransitive and transitive verb] rise, excellence, completeness. [transitive
mount Transl Path 1 orientation:
ascend. To get up on something above verb] To cause to ascend or rise; to raise,
upwards;
the level of the ground; especially: to elevate, lift, draw or drive up. Senses
up / onto G
seat oneself (as on a horse) for riding. relating to upward motion on to
something. [intransitive verb] To get up
on to the back of a horse or other animal
(occas. on a person's shoulders) for the
purpose of riding. [transitive verb] To
set on the back of a horse or other
animal.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) change position. to go to a To go from one place, position, state,
different place to live or work. TFD: etc., to another.
[intransitive verb]To change in [intransitive verb] Of a person, a part of
position from one point to another. To the body, etc.: to change position or
change posture or position. [transitive posture; to exhibit motion or physical
Self-c
verb] To change the place or position activity. [transitive verb] To change the
move + AM (Motion)
of. To cause to go from one place to place or position of (a thing; occas. a
Transl
another. MW: [intransitive verb] to go person); to cause to change from one
or pass to another place or in a certain place, position, or situation to another;
direction with a continuous motion. to to shift, remove; to dislodge or displace
change position or posture. [transitive (something fixed).
verb] to change the place or position
of.
336
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to go somewhere
quickly or be somewhere for only a [intransitive verb] colloq. (chiefly Brit.).
mp-walk /
nip Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast short time. TFD: To move quickly; To move rapidly or nimbly; to go
mp-run
dart. MW: to move briskly, nimbly, or quickly; to make a brief excursion.
quickly.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move your head down and then up,
sometimes repeatedly, especially to
show agreement, approval or greeting
[intransitive verb] To make a brief
or to show something by doing this.
inclination of the head, esp. in
TFD: To lower and raise the head
Figure (body salutation, assent, or command, or to
nod Self-c 1 quickly, as in agreement or
part: head) draw attention to something.
acknowledgment. To let the head fall
[transitive verb] To incline (the head)
forward when sleepy. MW: to make a
briefly in a nod.
quick downward motion of the head
whether deliberately (as in expressing
assent or salutation) or involuntarily
(as from drowsiness).
[intransitive and transitive verb] TFD:
To propel with or as if with oars or an
Vehicle / oar. To move forward by or as if by [intransitive verb] To row; to move or
oar Transl Manner 1
Instrument rowing. MW: to propel with or as if advance as if by means of oars.
with oars. to progress by or as if by
using oars.
[transitive verb] To outdo in running; to
[transitive verb] to move faster or run faster or further than. Also: to leave
Manner + Co-
mp-run; Rate- further than someone or something. behind by greater speed; to escape,
outrun Transl Motion / Action 2
faster than TFD: To run faster than. MW: to run elude.
correlation
faster than.
337
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
walk with regular steps in one
direction and then back again, usually
because you are anxious or worried.
LLA: To walk backwards and
[intransitive verb] To move with a
mp-walk; State forwards within a small area,
measured or regular step; to walk or
of Figure especially because you are nervous,
pace Transl Manner 1 stride along steadily. Also: to move in mp-walk
(anxious, bored, or angry. TFD: [transitive verb]
this way as an expression of anxiety,
worried) To walk or stride back and forth
frustration, etc.
across. [intransitive verb] To walk
with long deliberate steps. MW: to
walk with often slow or measured
tread.to go at a pace —used especially
of a horse.
[intransitive verb] To journey on foot,
esp. as an itinerant or a person seeking
[intransitive verb] LLA: to walk
work. [transitive verb] and [intransitive
quietly and steadily, without shoes or
verb] travel on foot; to go tramping or
with soft shoes, or on a soft surface.
mp-walk; Furtive wandering. [intransitive verb] Of a mp-walk /
pad Transl Manner 1 TFD: walk heavily and firmly, as
Motion ? person: to walk with soft, steady steps, mp-run
when weary, or through mud. MW: to
esp. without shoes, etc., putting each
go on foot : walk; especially : to walk
foot down quietly in a single motion. Of
with or as if with padded feet.
an animal: to walk softly, stealthily, or
calmly. Also with the feet as subject.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
walk with bare feet through shallow [intransitive verb] To wade, walk about,
water, often at the edge of the sea. or play in shallow water or mud; to
Manner + mp-walk; Playful LLA: British, American wade, to walk agitate water with one's feet; to dabble
paddle1 Transl 2 mp-walk
Ground (water) Motion in water that is not very deep, for one's feet or hands in water. Also
enjoyment. MW: [intransitive verb] to transitive verb: to dabble (a finger or
move the hands or feet about in toe) in water, a pond, etc.
shallow water.
TFD: [intransitive verb] To propel a
[intransitive verb] Of a person in a
watercraft with paddles or a paddle.
canoe, small boat, etc.: to move forward
Vehicle / [transitive verb] To propel (a
paddle2 Transl Manner 1 by means of a paddle or paddles. Also
Instrument watercraft) with paddles or a paddle.
with canoe, etc., as subject. To row
To convey in a watercraft propelled by
lightly or gently with oars.
paddles. MW: NO FOUND
338
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] To walk with short,
mp-walk; TFD: NO FOUND. MW: [intransitive unsteady, or uncoordinated steps like a
paddle3 Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
Unsteady Motion verb] toddle. child, often with a rocking motion; to
toddle, doddle.
[intransitive verb] to jump from an
aircraft using a parachute. [transitive
verb] to drop someone or something
from an aircraft by parachute. TFD:
[transitive verb] To drop (supplies or [transitive verb] To convey or drop
Vehicle / troops, for example) by means of a down or into a place by means of a
parachute Transl AM Manner 1
Instrument parachute. [intransitive verb] To parachute. [intransitive verb] To descend
descend by means of a parachute. by parachute; to use a parachute.
MW: [transitive verb] to convey by
means of a parachute. [intransitive
verb] to descend by means of a
parachute.
[intransitive verb] To march in
[intransitive and transitive verb](of a procession or with great display or
group) to walk or march somewhere, ostentation; to walk up and down,
usually as part of a public celebration. promenade, etc., in a public place, esp.
mp-walk; State LLA: To walk proudly around a place, in order to be seen; to show off.
parade Transl Manner 1 of the Figure in a way that shows you want people [transitive verb] To march through (a mp-walk
(proud) to notice and admire you. TFD: To place) in procession or with great
take part in a parade; march in a public display; to walk up and down,
procession. MW: to march in or as if promenade along (a street, etc.) or
in a procession. To show off. through (a place), esp. in order to be
seen.
339
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
Anglo-French passer, from Vulgar
Latin *passare, from Latin passus step.
I. To excel or surpass. [transitive verb]
[intransitive and transitive verb] to go
To go beyond (a point or place). II. To
past something or someone or move in
proceed, move forward, depart. to cause
relation to it. TFD: To move on or
to do this. [intransitive verb] Of a person
ahead; proceed.To move by.
or animal: to go by or past. To go on,
[transitive verb] To go beyond;
pass/cross move onward; to make one's way.
surpass. To go across; go through. To
G [transitive verb] To cause or enable (a
pass Transl AM Path 1 go by without stopping; leave behind.
(traversal, person or thing) to go or proceed
To cause to go by. MW: move,
milestone) somewhere; to carry, convey, or send,
proceed, go. to move in a path so as to
esp. to convey across a river or land; to
approach and continue beyond
transport. Usu. with preposition or
something. to go or make one's way
adverb of direction. intr. To go or be
through. [transitive verb] to go
transported from one place or set of
beyond. to go past (one moving in the
circumstances to (unto) or into another.
same direction). to go by : proceed or
extend beyond.
[intransitive verb] To push the pedals of
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
a bicycle or similar vehicle in a circular
push the pedals of a bicycle round
motion with one's feet, so as to propel
Vehicle / with your feet. TFD: To use or operate
pedal Transl AM Manner 1 oneself forward. [transitive verb] To
Instrument a pedal or pedals.To ride a bicycle.
propel (a bicycle, etc.) by pushing
MW: to ride a bicycle. to use or work
pedals with the feet; (also) to convey by
a pedal.
pedalling.
Latin penetratus, past participle of
penetrare, from penitus deep within,
far. [intransitive and transitive verb] to
[transitive verb] To get into or through,
move into or through something. TFD:
gain entrance or access to, esp. with
To pierce or enter into something;
penetrate Transl Path 1 into G force, effort, or difficulty; to pierce.
make a way in or through
[intransitive verb] Usu. with into,
something.To enter or force a way
through, to.
into; pierce. MW: to pass, extend,
pierce, or diffuse into or through
something. to pass into or through.
340
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk about for
pleasure. OLD USE. TFD: [transitive verb] To walk through, over,
[intransitive verb] To walk about; or about (a place or space). In early use
roam or stroll. [transitive verb] To more generally: to travel or pass
perambulate Transl Manner 1 mp-walk mp-walk
walk through.To inspect (an area) on through, to traverse. [intransitive verb]
foot. MW: [intransitive verb] to stroll. To walk, wander, or travel from place to
[transitive verb] to travel over or place; to move. Also with about, around.
through especially on foot.
[intransitive verb] a fast turn of the
body on the toes or the front part of [intransitive verb] To perform a
Revolve, Rotate, the foot, performed especially by a pirouette; to spin or walk on the points
pirouette Self-c Manner 1
Spin ballet dancer. TFD: [intransitive verb] of the toes; to move with a whirling
to perform a pirouette. MW: motion.
[intransitive verb]
mp-walk; Rate-
slow; Forced [intransitive verb] to walk taking slow
Motion (Energy- steps, as if your feet are heavy. TFD: [intransitive verb] To walk heavily or
low); To move or walk heavily or without elasticity; to move or progress
plod Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
Characteristic laboriously; trudge. MW: to walk doggedly or laboriously; to trudge
use of lower heavily or slowly. to proceed slowly (along, on, etc.).
body (Steps- or tediously.
heavy)
341
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause someone or something to)
[intransitive verb] To throw or hurl
move or fall suddenly and often a long
oneself into water or the like; to dive; to
forward/ way forward, down or into something.
fall abruptly and involuntarily, esp. from
down/ into TFD: To fall or throw oneself into a
Rate-fast; Forced a great height, into a depth; to descend.
plunge Transl AM Path + Manner 2 G; substance or place. To move forward
Motion Also: to enter suddenly into or pass
to/towards and downward violently. [transitive
rapidly through something which
G verb] To thrust or throw forcefully
surrounds one completely, as a forest, a
into a substance or place. MW:
crowd, etc. Usu. with in, into, through.
[intransitive verb] to thrust or cast
oneself into or as if into water.
AS NOUN:a fast active dance that was
popular in the 19th century, or a piece
of music that can be used for this
polka Self-c Manner 1 Dance [intransitive verb] To dance the polka.
dance. [intransitive verb] TFD: to
dance the polka. MW: [intransitive
verb]
[intransitive verb] to move quickly
and suddenly, especially from an
[intransitive verb] To move or go
Rate-fast; enclosed space. [intransitive verb] to
somewhere quickly or unexpectedly,
pop Transl Manner 1 Sudden go to a particular place. TFD: To
esp. for a short time. Usu. with in, off,
movement move quickly or unexpectedly; appear
out, up, etc.
abruptly. MW: to go, come, or appear
suddenly —often used with up.
[intransitive verb] to jump or move
quickly in order to catch or take hold
of something. TFD: To spring or
[intransitive verb] Of an animal, bird,
to/towards swoop with intent to seize someone or
pounce Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast etc.: to spring or swoop suddenly so as
G something. To attack suddenly. MW:
to catch (prey).
to swoop upon and seize something
with or as if with talons. to make a
sudden assault or approach.
342
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk in an
energetic way and with more
movement than necessary.
[intransitive verb] To dance, gambol,
[intransitive verb] When a horse
caper. To move, walk, or behave in an
prances it takes small, quick steps and
ostentatious or arrogant manner; to
raises its legs higher than usual. TFD:
swagger; to flounce. [intransitive verb]
To spring forward on the hind legs.
State of the Of a person: to ride a prancing horse; to mp-walk /
prance Transl Manner 1 Used of a horse. To spring or bound
Figure (proud) ride proudly or ostentatiously. mp-jump
forward in a manner reminiscent of a
[intransitive verb] Of an animal, esp. a
spirited horse. To ride a horse moving
horse: to rise by springing from the hind
in such a fashion. To walk or move
legs; to move by a succession of such
about spiritedly; strut. MW: to spring
springs.
from the hind legs or move by so
doing. to ride on a prancing horse. to
walk or move in a spirited manner.
[intransitive verb] to walk slowly
along a road or path for relaxation and
pleasure. TFD: [intransitive verb] To
[intransitive verb] To take a leisurely
go on a leisurely walk. [transitive
mp-walk; walk (or a ride or drive), esp. in a public
verb] To take a promenade along or
Leisurely place so as to meet or be seen by others.
promenade Transl Manner 1 through. MW: [intransitive verb] to mp-walk
Motion; State of [transitive verb] To make a promenade
take or go on a promenade. [transitive
Figure (relaxed) through; to walk about (a place) in a
verb] to walk about in or on. As noun
leisurely or ostentatious way.
= a place for strolling, a leisurely walk
or ride especially in a public place for
pleasure or display.
343
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move around quietly in a place trying
not to be seen or heard, such as when
hunting. LLA: to quietly walk around [intransitive verb] To go or move about,
an area or building, trying not to be esp. in search of or looking for
mp-walk; Furtive seen or heard. TFD: To rove furtively something; (hence) to roam or wander
prowl Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
Motion or with predatory intent. To roam about in search of plunder, prey, etc., or
through stealthily, as in search of prey with predatory intent; to move about,
or plunder. MW: to move about or around stealthily or restlessly.
wander stealthily in or as if in search
of prey. to roam over in a predatory
manner.
AS NOUN: a long narrow boat with a
flat bottom and a square area at each
[transitive verb] To propel (a punt or
end, which is moved by a person
other boat) by pushing against the bed of
standing on one of the square areas
a river, etc., with a pole; to propel in the
and pushing a long pole against the
Vehicle / manner of a punt. To convey in a punt or
punt Transl AM? Manner 1 bottom of the river. [intransitive verb]
Instrument by punting. [intransitive verb] To push
to travel in a punt. TFD: [transitive
with a punt pole; to propel a punt or
verb] To propel (a boat) with a pole.
other boat in this way; to travel in a
To carry in a punt. [intransitive verb]
punt.
To go in a punt. MW: [transitive verb]
to propel (as a punt) with a pole.
from Anglo-French pursure, pursiure,
from Latin prosequi, from pro- To follow or go in pursuit (chiefly
forward + sequi to follow. [transitive involving physical movement).
verb] to follow someone or something, [intransitive verb] To go in chase or
usually to try to catch or kill them. pursuit; to give chase. [transitive verb]
pursue Transl Path 1 after G
[intransitive and transitive verb] TFD: Originally: to follow (a person, animal,
To follow in an effort to overtake or or thing) with intent to overtake and
capture; chase. MW: to follow in order capture, harm, or kill; to hunt. Later usu.
to overtake, capture, kill, or defeat. to more generally: to chase, go after.
go in pursuit.
344
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to shake because
you are very frightened or very
amused, or to feel or show great fear.
[intransitive verb] Of a thing, esp. the
TFD: To shake or tremble, as from
Shake, Swing, earth: to shake or tremble as a result of
quake Self-c Manner 1 instability or shock.To shiver, as with
Vibrate an external or internal impulse, natural
cold or from strong emotion. MW: to
instability, etc.
shake or vibrate usually from shock or
instability. to tremble or shudder
usually from cold or fear
AS NOUN: a dance with a lot of quick [intransitive verb] To step quickly, esp.
steps, or a piece of music for this. to perform a quick step or quick march.
quickstep Self-c Manner 1 Dance
TFD: perform a quickstep. MW: NO [intransitive verb] To dance the
FOUND AS MOTION VERB quickstep.
[intransitive verb] to shake slightly, [intransitive verb] To shake, tremble, or
often because of strong emotion. vibrate, with a slight but rapid agitation.
Shake, Swing, TFD:To shake with a slight, rapid, (Said of persons, esp. under the
quiver Self-c Manner 1
Vibrate tremulous movement. MW: to shake influence of some emotion, of things,
or move with a slight trembling light, etc.) [transitive verb] To cause to
motion. vibrate or tremble.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move or go fast. TFD: [intransitive
[intransitive verb] To run a race (with),
verb] to compete in a race. To move
to compete (with) in speed. [intransitive
rapidly or at top speed. [transitive
verb] To run, ride, sail, etc. swiftly.
verb] To transport rapidly or at top
race Transl AM Manner 1 Rate-fast [transitive verb] To race with; to try to mp-run
speed; rush. MW: [intransitive verb] to
beat in running. To cause to move
compete in a race. to go, move, or
swiftly or to make rapid progress; to
function at top speed or out of control.
cause to run a race or races.
[transitive verb] to transport or propel
at maximum speed.
345
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a flat floating structure
for travelling across water, often made
of pieces of wood tied roughly
together and moved along with a
[transitive verb] To transport by water:
paddle (= pole with a flat end).
in the form of a raft, on, or by means of,
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
Vehicle / a raft. To go upon or cross (a river) by
raft Transl AM Manner 1 travel or transport something on a raft.
Instrument means of a raft. [intransitive verb] To
TFD: [transitive verb] to convey on a
use a raft for some purpose; to work on
raft. To make into a raft. [intransitive
or direct a raft.
verb]To travel by raft. MW: [transitive
verb] to transport in the form of or by
means of a raft. [intransitive verb] to
travel by raft.
[intransitive verb] to walk for [intransitive verb] Of persons: To
pleasure, especially in the countryside. wander, travel, make one's way about
mp-walk; No TFD: To move about aimlessly.To (now usually to walk) in a free
ramble Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
aim in Motion walk about casually or for pleasure. unrestrained manner and without
MW: to move aimlessly from place to definite aim or direction. [transitive
place. to wander over. verb] To wander over. rare.
[transitive verb] to arrive at a place,
to/towards especially after spending a long time [transitive verb] To come to, arrive at (a
reach Transl Path 1 G (arrival, or a lot of effort travelling. TFD: To place, object, or point in space), to get
endpoint) arrive at. To travel as far as. MW: to up to or as far as.
arrive at or come to something.
from Latin recedere to go back, from
re- + cedere to go. [intransitive verb]
away from [intransitive verb] To go back or further
to move further away into the
G off; to remove to or towards a more
recede Transl Path 1 distance, or to become less clear or
(departure, distant position. Of persons. Usually =
less bright. TFD:To move back or
source) to retreat, retire.
away from a limit, point, or mark.
MW: to move back or away.
346
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb] Of persons or parts of
lean or lie back with the upper part of
the body: To rest in a recumbent or
your body in a nearly horizontal
inclined position, lean or repose on or
position. TFD: [transitive verb] To
Posture (towards upon something. [transitive verb] To lay
cause to assume a leaning or prone
recline Self-c AM Manner 1 horizontal; Bend; down, or make to lie down (properly on
position. [intransitive verb] to lie back
backwards) the back); to cause to incline
or down. MW: [transitive verb] to
(backwards); to place in a recumbent or
cause or permit to incline backwards.
leaning position; to rest (the head, etc.)
[intransitive verb] to lean or incline
in this way.
backwards.
from Anglo-French reculer, recuiler,
from re- + cul backside [intransitive
verb] to move back because of fear or [intransitive verb] To retreat, retire, go
disgust. TFD: To spring back, as upon or draw back (or aback) before an
firing.To shrink back, as in fear or enemy or opposing force. To stagger
recoil Transl Path 1 backwards
repugnance. To fall back; return. MW: back, from the effects of a blow. To start
to fall back under pressure. to shrink or spring back in fear, horror, disgust, or
back physically or emotionally. to the like.
spring back to or as if to a starting
point .
[intransitive verb] to walk moving
[intransitive verb] Of persons (or
from side to side, looking like you are
Self-c / animals): To sway or stagger as the mp-walk /
reel1 Manner 1 Unsteady Motion going to fall. TFD: To stagger, lurch,
Transl result of a blow or encounter. Often with mp-run
or sway, as from drunkenness. MW: to
back, backward.
walk or move unsteadily.
[intransitive verb] If the place where
you are reels, what you are looking at
Self-c / seems to go round and round in front [intransitive verb] To whirl round or
Revolve, Rotate,
reel2 Transl AM Manner 1 of you. TFD: To go round and round about; to go with a whirling motion.
Spin
? in a whirling motion. [transitive verb] [transitive verb] To cause to roll, whirl.
To cause to reel. MW: to turn or move
round and round.
347
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
Middle French retirer, from re- + tirer
to draw. [intransitive verb] FORMAL
to leave a room or group of people and
go somewhere quiet or private. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To withdraw to or
To withdraw, as for rest or
away from into a place (or way of life) for the sake
seclusion.To move back or away;
G of seclusion, shelter, or security.
retire Transl AM Path 1 recede. [transitive verb] To lead
(departure, [transitive verb] To withdraw, lead back
(troops, for example) away from
source) (troops, etc.), esp. before a superior
action; withdraw. MW: to withdraw
force.
from action or danger. to withdraw
especially for privacy. to move back.
[transitive verb] to march (a military
force) away from the enemy.
From Anglo-French retrait, from past
participle of retraire to withdraw, from
Latin retrahere, from re- + trahere to
away from draw . [intransitive verb] to go away [intransitive verb] To withdraw, retire,
G from a place or person in order to draw back. Of an army or a combatant:
retreat Transl AM? Path 1
(departure, escape from fighting or danger. TFD: To retire before superior force or after a
source) To fall or draw back; withdraw or defeat. [transitive verb] To draw or lead
retire. MW: to make a retreat: back; to remove, take away.
withdraw. [transitive verb] to draw or
lead back: remove.
From Anglo-French returner, from re-
+ turner, tourner to turn [intransitive
verb] to come or go back to a previous
place. TFD: [intransitive verb]To go [intransitive verb] To come or go back
or come back, as to an earlier to a place or person. [transitive verb] To
return Transl AM Path 1 back to G
condition or place. [transitive verb] To bring or convey back to a place or
send, put, or carry back. MW: to go person
back or come back again. [transitive
verb] to bring, send, or put back to a
former or proper place.
348
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move or cause something to move
round a central point or line. TFD: To
[intransitive verb] To perform a circular
orbit a central point. To turn on an
motion; to move in a regular orbit about
axis; rotate. To cause to revolve. MW:
Revolve, Rotate, or round a fixed point. [transitive verb]
revolve Self-c AM Manner 1 Middle English, from Latin revolvere
Spin To cause (something) to travel in an
to roll back, cause to return, from re- +
orbit around a central point; to rotate
volvere to roll. to move in a curved
(something) upon an axis.
path round a center or axis b: to turn
or roll round on an axis. to cause to go
round in an orbit.
AS NOUN: jinricksha: A light two-
wheeled hooded vehicle having springs
AS NOUN: a small covered passenger and two shafts, drawn by one or more
Vehicle / vehicle with two wheels which is men. First used in Japan c1870, but now
rickshaw Transl NO Manner 1
Instrument usually pulled by one person. TFD, common in other parts of the world;
MW: AS NOUN. colloq. shortened to rickshaw.
[intransitive verb] to ride in a
jinricksha.
[intransitive verb] To sit upon, and be
carried by, a horse or other animal; to
[intransitive and transitive verb] to sit
move about, make one's way, or journey
on a horse or a bicycle and travel
upon horseback (or, in mod. use, on a
along on it controlling its movements.
Vehicle / cycle). To sit or be carried on or upon
TFD: To be carried or conveyed, as in
Instrument something after the manner of one on
ride Transl Manner 1 a vehicle or on horseback. To sit on
(horse, bike, horseback. [transitive verb] To sit or be
and move in a given direction. MW: to
vehicle) carried upon, to go or travel upon (a
sit and travel on the back of an animal
horse, or other animal of burden); to
that one directs. to travel in or on a
manage or control while seated on. To
conveyance. to travel on.
travel in or on (a train, public transport
vehicle, etc.), to be a passenger on.
[intransitive verb] To ascend, mount up.
[intransitive verb] to move upwards.
Earth- Of the heavenly bodies: To come above
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
based the horizon. Of smoke, vapour, or the
rise1 Transl Path 1 from a lower to a higher position;
orientation: like: To ascend into the air, mount up.
ascend. MW: To move upward:
upwards To move or be carried upwards; to
ascend.
ascend.
349
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to stand, especially
after sitting. TFD: To assume a [intransitive verb] To get up from
standing position after lying, sitting, or sitting, lying, or repose. To get up from
Posture (vertical-
rise2 Self-c Manner 1 kneeling. MW: to assume an upright a sitting, kneeling, or lying posture; to
upwards)
position especially from lying, assume a standing position; to get upon
kneeling, or sitting. to get up from one's feet.
sleep or from one's bed
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move about or travel, especially
without a clear idea of what you are
going to do. LLA: to walk round [intransitive verb] To wander, rove, or
mp-walk; No freely with no particular aim. TFD: To ramble; to walk about aimlessly, esp.
roam Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
aim in motion move about without purpose or plan; over a wide area. [transitive verb] To
wander. To wander over or through. wander over or through (a place).
MW: to go from place to place
without purpose or direction : wander.
to range or wander over.
[intransitive verb] If a vehicle or
aircraft roars somewhere, it moves
[intransitive verb] To travel on a vehicle
Rate-fast; Noisy there very quickly making a lot of
roar Transl Manner 1 which is making a loud noise; to motor
Activity noise. TFD: NO FOUND AS
rapidly.
MOTION VERB. MW: to proceed or
rush with great noise or commotion.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause someone or something to) [intransitive verb] To sway to and fro
move backwards and forwards or from under some impact or stress; to move or
side to side in a regular way. TFD: swing from side to side; to oscillate.
move back and forth or sideways. [transitive verb] To move (a child)
Shake, Swing, [transitive verb] to cause to move back gently to and fro in a cradle, in order to
rock Self-c AM Manner 1
Vibrate and forth. MW: To become moved soothe or send it to sleep.To move or
backward and forward under often sway (one) to and fro, esp. in a gentle or
violent impact. to move gently back soothing manner. To cause to sway to
and forth. to move back and forth in or and fro or from side to side; to move
as if in a cradle. [transitive verb] to backwards and forwards.
cause to sway back and forth.
350
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a large cylindrical object
which moves very fast by forcing out
burning gases, and which is used for
space travel or as a weapon. TFD:
Vehicle / [intransitive and transitive verb] To [transitive verb] To propel (someone) at
rocket1 Transl AM Manner 1
Instrument soar or rise rapidly. To carry by means speed, as by a rocket; to send by rocket.
of a rocket. MW: [transitive verb] to
convey or propel by means of or as if
by a rocket. [intransitive verb] to
travel rapidly in or as if in a rocket.
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
Earth-
swiftly and powerfully, as a rocket. to
based [intransitive verb] In general use: to
rocket2 Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast soar or rise rapidly. MW: [intransitive
orientation: move like a rocket, to speed.
verb] to rise up swiftly, spectacularly,
upwards
and with force.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) move somewhere by turning
over and over or from side to side. To
(cause something to) turn over onto
itself to form a cylinder or a sphere. [Intransitive verb] To move by revolving
TFD: To move forward along a or rotating on (or as on) an axis; to move
surface by revolving on an axis or by forward on a surface by turning over and
repeatedly turning over.To travel or be over. Of vehicles: To move or run on
moved on wheels or rollers. To cause wheels. [transitive verb] To move or
Self-c Revolve, Rotate, to move forward along a surface by impel forward (an object) on a surface
roll AM Manner 1
/Transl Spin revolving on an axis or by repeatedly by making it turn over and over; to shift
turning over.To move or push along about, to send down to a lower level,
on wheels or rollers MW: to move etc., in this manner. To drive or draw (a
along a surface by rotation without vehicle); to wheel (a cycle); to move by
sliding. to turn over and over. to impel means of rollers. To convey in a
forward by causing to turn over and wheeled vehicle.
over on a surface. to cause to revolve
by turning over and over on or as if on
an axis. to cause to move in a circular
manner.
351
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: one of two boots with a
single row of small wheels on the
bottom which you wear in order to
travel along quickly for enjoyment.
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] to move on a [intransitive verb] to roller-skate using
rollerblade Transl NO Manner 1
Instrument surface, using Rollerblades. TFD: skates of this type
[intransitive verb] travel on shoes with
a single line of rubber wheels attached
to their soles. MW: no found as
motion verb.
[intransitive verb] to play in a rough,
excited and noisy way. TFD: To play
Low
or frolic boisterously. To run or
[intransitive verb] To play, sport, or ratings.
advance in a rapid or easy manner.
romp Transl Manner 1 Playful Motion frolic in a very lively, merry, or No
MW: to run or play in a lively,
boisterous manner. significant
carefree, or boisterous manner. to
difference
move or proceed in a brisk, easy, or
playful manner.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) turn in a circle, especially
[intransitive verb] To move round a
around a fixed point. TFD: To turn
centre or axis; to perform one or more
Revolve, Rotate, around on an axis or center. To cause
rotate Self-c AM Manner 1 revolutions. [transitive verb] To cause (a
Spin to turn on an axis or center. MW: to
thing) to turn round or revolve on a
turn about an axis or a center :
centre or axis.
revolve. to cause to turn or move
about an axis or a center.
[intransitive verb] to move, travel or
look around especially a large area.
[intransitive verb] To wander about with
TFD: To wander about at random,
mp-walk; No no fixed destination; to move hither and
rove Transl Manner 1 especially over a wide area; roam. To mp-walk
aim in motion thither at random or in a leisurely
roam or wander around, over, or
fashion; to stray, roam, ramble.
through. MW: to move aimlessly. to
wander through or over.
352
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
cause a boat to move through water by
pushing against the water with oars.
TFD: To propel a boat with or as if
[intransitive verb] Of persons: To use
with oars. To propel (a boat) with or
Vehicle / oars, sweeps, or similar means, for the
row Transl Manner 1 as if with oars.
Instrument purpose of propelling a boat or other
To carry in or on a boat propelled by
vessel.
oars. MW: to propel a boat by means
of oars. to propel with or as if with
oars. to transport in an oar-propelled
boat.
AS NOUN: a type of dancing,
[intransitive verb] To dance the rumba.
originally from Cuba, or the music for
rumba Self-c Manner 1 Dance Also, to move as though dancing the
this. TFD: dance the rhumba. MW:
rumba.
NO FOUND AS MOTION VERB
[intransitive and transitive verb] (of
people and some animals) to move
along, faster than walking, by taking
quick steps in which each foot is lifted [intransitive verb] To move the legs
before the next foot touches the quickly (the one foot being lifted before
ground. TFD: To move swiftly on foot the other is set down) so as to go at a
run Transl Manner 1 mp-run
so that both feet leave the ground faster pace than walking; to cover the
during each stride. MW: to go faster ground, make one's way, rapidly in this
than a walk; specifically : to go manner.
steadily by springing steps so that both
feet leave the ground for an instant in
each step.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) go or do something very
quickly. TFD: To move or act swiftly; [intransitive verb] Of persons or
hurry.To cause to move or act with animals: To run, dash, or charge with
mp-
unusual haste or violence. MW: to violence or impetuous rapidity.
rush Transl AM Manner 1 Rate-fast run/mp-
move forward, progress, or act with [transitive verb] To cause to move with
walk
haste or eagerness or without great speed and force; to send or impel
preparation. to push or impel on or violently.
forward with speed, impetuosity, or
violence.
353
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] Of persons: To travel
on water in a vessel propelled by the
[intransitive verb] When a boat or a
action of the wind upon sails; now often
ship sails, it travels on the water. To
in extended sense, to travel on water in a
control a boat that has no engine and is
vessel propelled by any means other
pushed by the wind. TFD: To move
than oars; to navigate a vessel in a
across the surface of water, especially
Vehicle / specified direction. Of a ship or other
sail Transl Manner 1 by means of a sailing vessel. To
Instrument vessel: To move or travel on water by
navigate or manage (a vessel). MW: to
means of sails, or (in modern use) by
travel on water by the action of wind
means of steam or any other mechanical
upon sails or by other means. to travel
agency. [transitive verb] Of persons,
on (water) by means of motive power
also of a vessel: To sail over or upon, to
(as sail).
navigate (the sea, a river, etc.). Now
somewhat arch.
AS NOUN: an energetic dance
originally from Brazil, or music for
samba Self-c Manner 1 Dance dancing this dance. [intransitive verb] [intransitive verb] To dance the samba.
TFD: to perform this dance. MW:
[intransitive verb]
[intransitive verb] to walk confidently
while moving your hips from side to
[intransitive verb] To perform a chassé,
side in a way that attracts attention.
esp. in square dancing; freq. transf., to
TFD: To walk or proceed, especially
perform a movement similar to the
in an easy or casual manner.To strut or
chassé. To glide, walk, or travel, usu. in
mp-walk; State flounce in a showy manner. To
sashay Transl Manner 1 a casual manner. To move diagonally or mp-walk
of Figure (proud) perform the chassé in dancing. To
sideways; to travel an irregular path; to
move in a sideways manner. MW: to
wander or saunter. To move or walk
make a chassé. To strut or move about
ostentatiously, conspicuously, or
in an ostentatious or conspicuous
provocatively; to strut or parade.
manner. to proceed or move in a
diagonal or sideways manner.
354
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk in a slow
and relaxed way, often in no particular
mp-walk;
direction. LLA: To walk in a slow and [intransitive verb] To walk with a
Leisurely
lazy way, often when you should be leisurely and careless gait; to stroll.
saunter Transl Manner 1 Motion; State of mp-walk
hurrying to do something. TFD: To Also, to travel by vehicle in a slow and
the Figure
walk at a leisurely pace; stroll. MW: to leisurely manner.
(relaxed)
walk about in an idle or leisurely
manner : stroll.
from Late Latin scala ladder, staircase,
from Latin scalae, plural, stairs, rungs,
ladder; akin to Latin scandere to climb
[transitive verb] to climb up a steep [transitive verb] To climb, get over (a
scale Transl Path 1 up / onto G surface, such as a wall or the side of a wall or the like); to ascend (a mountain);
mountain, often using special to get to or reach the top of.
equipment. TFD: To climb; ascend.
To climb up or over; ascend. MW: to
climb by or as if by a ladder.
[intransitive verb] When small
children and animals scamper, they
mp-run; Playful
run with small quick steps, in a playful
Motion;
or frightened way. LLA: To run with [intransitive verb] To run or caper about
Characteristic mp-run /
scamper Transl Manner 1 short quick steps, especially when nimbly; to go or journey hastily from
use of lower mp-walk
running in a group and often in a place to place.
body (Steps-
playful way. TFD: To run or go
small)?
quickly and lightly. MW: to run
nimbly and usually playfully about.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] To separate and
(cause to) move far apart in different disperse; to go dispersedly or
Multiple
directions. TFD: To separate and go in stragglingly. [transitive verb] To
directions
different directions; disperse.To cause separate and drive in various directions
scatter Transl AM Path 1 from a
to separate and go in different (a body of men or animals, a collection
unique
directions. MW: to separate and go in of things); to disperse, dissipate (a
start
various directions : disperse. to cause quantity of matter); to dispel (clouds,
to separate widely. mists).
[intransitive verb] INFORMAL to go
[intransitive verb] To go suddenly and mp-run /
scoot Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast quickly. TFD: To go suddenly and
swiftly, to dart; to go away hurriedly. mp-walk
speedily; hurry. MW: to move swiftly.
355
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
away from [intransitive verb] INFORMAL to go
G away quickly. TFD: To leave a scene
scram Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast [intransitive verb] To depart quickly.
(departure, at once; go abruptly. MW: to go away
source) at once.
[intransitive verb] to move or climb
[intransitive verb] To raise oneself to an
quickly but with difficulty, often using
Rate-fast; Forced erect posture, to get through or into a
your hands to help you. TFD:
Motion (energy- place or position, by the struggling use
[intransitive verb] To move or climb
scramble Transl Manner 1 high); Use of of the hands and feet; hence, to make
hurriedly, especially on the hands and
one's hands and one's way by clambering, crawling,
knees. MW: to move with urgency or
legs jumping, etc. over difficult ground or
panic. To move or climb hastily on all
through obstructions.
fours.
[intransitive verb] (especially of
clouds and ships) to move quickly and Low
without stopping in a straight line. [intransitive verb] To run or move ratings.
Rate-fast;
scud Transl Manner 1 TFD: [intransitive verb] To run or briskly or hurriedly; to dart nimbly from No
Smooth Motion?
skim along swiftly and easily. MW: to place to place. significant
move or run swiftly especially as if difference
driven forward.
[intransitive verb] to move quickly,
with small short steps. LLA: To run
with short quick steps, especially
Rate-fast;
when you need to move quickly to
Characteristic
escape from danger. TFD: To go with [intransitive verb] To go rapidly, move
scurry Transl Manner 1 use of lower mp-run
light running steps; scamper. MW: to hurriedly.
body (Steps-
move in or as if in a brisk pace :
short)?
scamper. to move around in an
agitated, confused, or fluttering
manner.
[intransitive verb] to move quickly,
with small short steps, especially in
Rate-fast;
order to escape. LLA: To run with
Characteristic
short quick steps, especially to escape [intransitive verb] To run with quick, mp-run /
scuttle Transl Manner 1 use of lower
from something- use this especially hurried steps. mp-walk
body (Steps-
about small animals. TFD: To move
short)?
about or proceed hurriedly. MW:
scurry.
356
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a long board that children
play on. The board is balanced on a
central point so that when a child sits
on each end they can make the board [intransitive verb] To move up and
Vehicle /
go up and down by pushing off the down, or backwards and forwards; to
seesaw Self-c Manner 1 Instrument
ground with their fee. [intransitive undergo a see-saw motion; also to play
(seesaw)
verb] TFD: To play on a seesaw. MW: see-saw.
[intransitive verb] to move backward
and forward or up and down. to play at
seesaw
[transitive verb] to follow closely. [transitive verb] To follow (a person)
TFD: To follow, especially in secret; like a shadow; in mod. journalistic
shadow Transl Path 1 after G
trail. MW: to follow especially language said of a detective who dogs
secretly : trail. the steps of a person under surveillance.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move backwards and forwards or up
and down in quick, short movements,
or to make something or someone do
this. TFD: To move to and fro in
short, irregular, often jerky
movements. To tremble, as from cold [intransitive verb] Of things having
or in anger. To cause to move to and more or less freedom of movement: To
Shake, Swing, fro with jerky movements.To cause to move irregularly and quickly to and fro,
shake Self-c AM Manner 1
Vibrate quiver, tremble, vibrate, or rock. MW: up and down, or from side to side; to
to move irregularly to and fro. to quiver, quake, vibrate, waver. [transitive
vibrate especially as the result of a verb] To cause to vibrate, agitate.
blow or shock. to tremble as a result of
physical or emotional disturbance. to
cause to move to and fro, up and
down, or from side to side especially
in a repetitive, rhythmic, or quick
jerky manner.
357
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk slowly and
awkwardly, without lifting your feet
mp-walk; Rate- correctly. LLA: To walk slowly and
slow; Awkward rather awkwardly, bending forwards in [intransitive verb] To go with an
shamble Transl Manner 1 Motion; a tired or lazy way. TFD: To walk in awkward ungainly gait, to walk mp-walk
Unsteady an awkward, lazy, or unsteady awkwardly or unsteadily.
Motion? manner, shuffling the feet. MW: To
walk awkwardly with dragging feet :
shuffle.
[transitive verb] to send something,
usually a large object or a large
quantity of objects or people, to a
distant place. TFD: [intransitive and [intransitive verb] To go by ship to, into,
transitive verb] To travel by ship. To or from a place. [transitive verb] To put
Vehicle / cause to be transported by or as if by or take (persons or things) on board
ship Transl AM Manner 1
Instrument ship. MW: [intransitive verb] to ship; to cause (a person) to embark; to
embark on a ship. to go or travel by place (goods) in a ship for
ship. [transitive verb] to place or transportation.
receive on board a ship for
transportation by water. to cause to be
transported.
[intransitive verb] When people or
animals shiver, they shake slightly
Shake, Swing, because they feel cold, ill or [intransitive verb] To tremble, shake,
shiver Self-c Manner 1
Vibrate frightened. TFD: To shake with or as quiver; esp. to tremble with cold or fear.
if with cold; tremble. MW: to undergo
trembling : quiver.
[intransitive verb] To go swiftly and
[intransitive verb] to move in a suddenly. Of an inanimate thing (or of a Low
particular direction very quickly and living being moving involuntarily): To ratings.
shoot Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast directly. TFD: To move swiftly; dart. go or pass with a sudden swift No
MW: o go or pass rapidly and movement through space; to rush, be significant
precipitately. precipitated; to fly as an arrow from a difference
bow.
358
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to shake suddenly
with very small movements because of
[intransitive verb] To have a convulsive
a very unpleasant thought or feeling.
Shake, Swing, tremor of the body caused by fear,
shudder Self-c Manner 1 TFD: To shiver convulsively, as from
Vibrate abhorrence, or cold; hence, to tremble
fear or revulsion. To vibrate; quiver.
with horror or dread.
MW: to tremble convulsively : shiver,
quiver.
[intransitive verb] to walk by pulling
your feet slowly along the ground
rather than lifting them. LLA: To walk
mp-walk; Rate- slowly and noisily, without lifting
[intransitive verb] To move the feet
slow; your feet off the ground properly.
along the ground without lifting them, so
Characteristic TFD: To move with short sliding
as to make a scraping noise; to walk
shuffle Transl Manner 1 use of lower steps, without or barely lifting the mp-walk
with such a motion of the feet; to go
body (contact feet.To slide (the feet) along the floor
with clumsy steps or a shambling gait;
feet and ground); or ground while walking. MW: to
also said of the feet.
Noisy Motion? move or walk in a sliding dragging
manner without lifting the feet. to
move (as the feet) by sliding along or
back and forth without lifting.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
travel or take people regularly
between the same two places. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To go, move, or [intransitive verb] To go or move
travel back and forth by or as if by a backwards and forwards like a shuttle;
shuttle. [transitive verb] To transport to travel quickly to and fro. Also, to
Vehicle /
shuttle Transl AM Manner 1 by or as if by a shuttle. MW: travel in one direction using a shuttle
Instrument
[transitive verb] to cause to move or service. [transitive verb] To transport in
travel back and forth frequently. to a vehicle or craft operating a shuttle
transport in, by, or as if by a shuttle. service.
[intransitive verb] to move or travel
back and forth frequently. to move by
or as if by a shuttle.
359
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk towards or
[intransitive verb] To move or go
away from someone, trying not to be
sideways or obliquely; to edge along,
Furtive Motion; noticed. TFD: To advance in an
sidle Transl Manner 1 esp. in a furtive or unobtrusive manner, mp-walk
mp-walk? unobtrusive, furtive, or coy way. MW:
or while looking in another direction; to
to go or move with one side foremost
make advances in this manner.
especially in a furtive advance.
[intransitive verb] To become
from Old English sincan. [intransitive submerged in water; to go under or to
and transitive verb] to (cause the bottom; (of ships) to founder. To
something or someone to) go down become partly or completely submerged
below the surface or to the bottom of a in quicksand, marshy ground, snow, etc.
liquid or soft substance. TFD: To To subside or go down into, to be
descend to the bottom; submerge.To swallowed up by, the earth, etc.
sink Transl AM Path 1 downwards fall or drop to a lower level, especially [transitive verb] To cause (a vessel, etc.)
to go down slowly or in stage. To to plunge or go down beneath the water;
cause to descend beneath a surface: to submerge by rendering incapable of
sink a ship.To cause to drop or lower. floating. To submerge; to put or thrust
MW: to go to the bottom. to fall or under water. To cause (a thing) to
drop to a lower place or level. to cause descend or fall to a lower plane or level;
to sink. to force, press, or weigh down in any
way.
AS NOUN: a special boot with a thin
metal bar fixed to the bottom that you
wear to move quickly on ice, or a boot
with four small wheels fixed to the
bottom so that you can move over a
[intransitive verb] To glide over ice
Vehicle / hard surface. [intransitive and
skate Transl NO Manner 1 upon skates; to use skates as a means of
Instrument transitive verb] to move, or make a
exercise or pastime.
particular movement on a surface,
using skates. MW: [intransitive and
transitive verb] to glide along on
skates propelled by the alternate action
of the legs.
360
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a flat narrow board with
two small wheels under each end,
which a person stands on and moves
Vehicle / forward by pushing one foot on the [intransitive verb] to ride on a
skateboard Transl NO Manner 1
Instrument ground. [intransitive verb] TFD: To skateboard.
ride or perform stunts on a skateboard.
MW: [intransitive verb] to ride or
perform stunts on a skateboard.
[intransitive verb] INFORMAL to run
[intransitive verb] In general use: To go
away from away quickly. TFD: To leave hastily; mp-walk /
skedaddle Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast away, leave, or depart hurriedly; to run
G flee. MW: run away, scram; especially mp-run
away, ‘clear out’.
: to flee in a panic.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move over snow on skis. TFD:
[intransitive and transitive verb] To [intransitive verb] To travel on skis.
Vehicle /
ski Transl NO Manner 1 travel or glide on skis, especially as a [transitive verb] To travel over (a slope,
Instrument
sport. MW: [intransitive and transitive etc.) on skis; to ski at (a place).
verb] to glide on skis in travel or as a
sport. to travel or pass over on skis.
[intransitive verb] Of a wheel: To slip or
[intransitive verb] to slide along a
be dragged along without revolving, esp.
surface so that you have no control.
as the effect of having a skid or brake
Contact TFD: To slide sideways while moving
applied to it. To slip obliquely or
[between F and because of loss of traction. To slide
skid Transl Manner 1 sideways, esp. owing to the muddy, wet,
G]; Uncontrolled without revolving. MW: to slide
or dusty state of the road; to side-slip.
Motion without rotating (as a wheel held from
Usually said of cycle or motor-car
turning while a vehicle moves
wheels, but also of horse-vehicles or
onward). To fail to grip the roadway.
persons. Also, of the vehicle itself.
361
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb][intransitive verb] To
[intransitive and transitive verb] to sail, glide, float, fly, run, etc., with a
Contact move quickly just above a surface light and easy motion, on or close to
[between F and without touching it. TFD: To move or some surface, or through the air.
skim Transl Manner 1 Ground]: slight pass swiftly and lightly over or near a [transitive verb] To move, glide, fly or
contact; Rate- surface; glide. MW: to pass lightly or float, lightly and rapidly over or along
fast hastily : glide or skip along, above, or (the ground, etc.). To pass over (a
near a surface. surface) with close approach or very
slight contact.
[intransitive verb] to move lightly and
quickly, making a small jump after
mp-jump; each step. LLA: If someone, [intransitive verb] To raise oneself off
Characteristic especially a child, skips along, they the ground by a light and graceful
use of lower move along with a little jump between movement; to spring or leap lightly and
skip Transl Manner 1 body (jump after their steps. TFD: To move by hopping easily, spec. in the exercise of skipping mp-jump
each step); on one foot and then the other.To leap with a rope. To spring or leap lightly in
Instrument lightly about. MW: To move or a certain direction or to a certain point;
(rope) proceed with leaps and bounds or with to move or advance by a skip or skips.
a skip (= a gait composed of
alternating hops and steps)
[intransitive verb] (especially of a
Contact small animal, bird or insect) to move
[intransitive verb] To move or run
[between F and very quickly and lightly. TFD: To
rapidly; to hurry about; to scamper off. mp-walk /
skitter Transl Manner 1 Ground]: slight move rapidly along a surface, usually
To skip or skim along a surface, with mp-run
contact; Rate- with frequent light contacts or changes
occasional rapid contact.
fast of direction. MW: to glide or skip
lightly or quickly.
[intransitive verb] to hide or move
around as if trying not to be seen, [intransitive verb] To move in a stealthy
skulk Transl Manner 1 Furtive Motion usually with bad intention. TFD: To or sneaking fashion, so as to escape
move about stealthily. MW: to move notice.
in a stealthy or furtive manner.
362
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a sport in which a person
jumps from an aircraft and falls for as
long as possible before opening a
Earth-
parachute. TFD: To jump and fall
Ground (on air) based mp-jump; [intransitive verb] No definition is
skydive Transl 3 freely from an airplane, performing
+ Path + Manner orientation: Motion in sports provided.
various maneuvers before pulling the
downwards
ripcord of a parachute. MW:
[intransitive verb] no definition is
provided.
AS NOUN:an object used for
travelling over snow and ice which has
long narrow strips of wood or metal
under it instead of wheels, and which
is either a low frame, or (also sleigh) a
carriage-like vehicle pulled by horses [intransitive verb] To travel in a sledge.
or dogs; a type of sledge pulled by [transitive verb] To carry or convey on a
sledge (UK); sled Vehicle / animals, especially horses or dogs. sledge. [intransitive verb] To travel in a
Transl AM Manner 1
(US); sleigh Instrument [intransitive and transitive verb] to sledge. [transitive verb] To convey on a
ride or travel on snow using a sledge. sled or sleds. [intransitive verb] To
TFD: [intransitive and transitive verb] travel or ride in a sleigh.
To convey or travel on a sledge. MW:
[intransitive verb] to travel with a
sledge. British : to ride in a sleigh.
[transitive verb] to transport on a
sledge.
[intransitive verb] a person who gets
out of bed and walks around while
mp-walk; State [intransitive verb] To walk while asleep;
they are sleeping. TFD: To walk or
sleepwak Transl Manner 1 of the Figure to be in a state resembling that of a
perform other motor acts while asleep;
(slept) sleep-walker.
somnambulate. MW: to walk while or
as if while asleep.
363
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb]To pass from one
(cause to) move easily and without
place or point to another with a smooth
Contact interruption over a surface. TFD: To
and continuous movement, esp. through
[between F and move over a surface while maintaining
slide Transl AM Manner 1 the air or water or along a surface. mp-walk
G]: Smooth smooth continuous contact. to cause to
[transitive verb] To cause to move with
Motion glide or slip. MW: to move smoothly
a smooth, gliding motion; to push over a
along a surface : slip. to cause to glide
level surface.
or slip
[intransitive verb] to walk away from
away from
somewhere quietly so that you are not [intransitive verb] Of persons or
G
slink Transl Path + Manner 2 Furtive Motion noticed. TFD: To move in a quiet animals: To move, go, walk, etc. in a mp-walk
(departure,
furtive manner; sneak. MW: to go or quiet, stealthy, or sneaking manner.
source)
move stealthily or furtively.
[intransitive verb] to slide
Contact
unintentionally. TFD: [intransitive
[between F and
verb] To slide involuntarily and lose [intransitive verb] To slide or glide, esp.
G]: Smooth
slip1 [slide] Transl Manner 1 one's balance or foothold. [transitive on a smooth or slippery surface; to lose
Motion;
verb] To cause to move in a smooth, one's foothold.
Uncontrolled
easy, or sliding motion. MW: to move
Motion
with a smooth sliding motion.
[intransitive verb] To pass or go lightly
or quietly; to move quickly and softly,
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
without attracting notice; to glide or
smoothly, easily, and quietly. To move
slip2 Transl Manner 1 Furtive Motion steal. Used with various advs. and preps.
stealthily; steal. MW: to move quietly
In some cases the prominent idea is that
and cautiously.
of escape; more usually it is that of
quick, easy motion.
[intransitive verb] to move easily and
Contact quickly across a surface while twisting [intransitive verb] To slip, slide, glide,
[between F and or curving. TFD: [intransitive verb] To esp. on a loose or broken slope or with a
slither Transl AM Manner 1 G]: Smooth glide or slide like a reptile. To walk clattering noise. [transitive verb] To
Motion; Path- with a sliding or shuffling gait. To slip make or cause to slide. Of reptiles: To
shape and slide, as on a loose or uneven creep, crawl, glide.
surface.
364
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to travel or move
Low
with difficulty, for example through
mp-walk; Forced [intransitive verb] To walk heavily or ratings.
wet, sticky soil or snow, or when you
slog Transl Manner 1 Motion (Energy- doggedly [i.e., very determined to do No
are very tired. TFD: To walk or
low) something, even if it is very difficult]. significant
progress with a slow heavy pace; plod.
difference
MW: To plod heavily : tramp.
[intransitive verb] to stand, sit or walk
with the shoulders hanging forward
and the head bent slightly over so that
[intransitive verb] To move or walk with Low
you look tired and bored. TFD:
a slouch or in a loose and stooping ratings.
Self-c / [intransitive verb] To sit, stand, or
slouch Manner 1 Posture (Bend) attitude.To carry oneself with a slouch No
Transl walk with an awkward, drooping,
or stoop; to droop the head and significant
excessively relaxed posture. MW: to
shoulders. difference
walk, stand, or sit with a slouch :
assume a slouch. to go or move slowly
or reluctantly.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to go
somewhere secretly, or to take
someone or something somewhere
[intransitive verb]To move, go, walk,
secretly. LLA: To walk quietly so that
Furtive Motion; etc., in a stealthy or slinking manner; to
sneak Transl Manner 1 no-one notices you, especially because mp-walk
mp-walk? creep or steal furtively, as if ashamed or
you are doing something wrong and
afraid to be seen; to slink, skulk.
do not want to be caught. TFD: To go
or move in a quiet, stealthy way. MW:
to go stealthily or furtively : slink.
[intransitive verb] to rise very quickly
to a high level; to rise high in the air [intransitive verb] Of birds: To fly or
while flying without moving the wings mount upwards; to ascend to a towering
Earth-
or using power. TFD: To rise, fly, or height; also loosely, to sail or skim at a
based
soar Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast glide high and with little apparent great height. Occas. with up. Of
orientation:
effort. To climb swiftly or powerfully. inanimate objects: To ascend, rise up to
upwards
To ascend suddenly above the normal a height. [transitive verb] To cause to
or usual level. MW: to ascend to a soar.
higher or more exalted level.
365
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
AS NOUN: a rolling movement or
jump, either forwards or backwards, in
which you turn over completely, with
your body above your head, and finish
with your head on top again. An
mp-jump;
acrobatic stunt in which the body rolls [intransitive verb] to make or turn a
somersault Self-c Manner 1 Revolve, Rotate, mp-jump
forward or backward in a complete somersault; to turn over and over.
Spin (turn over)
revolution with the knees bent and the
feet coming over the head. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To execute a
somersault. MW: [intransitive verb]
no definition is provided.
[intransitive verb] TFD: To walk or
mp-walk; State [intransitive verb] to walk during sleep;
perform another act while asleep or in
somnambulate Transl Manner 1 of the Figure [transitive verb], to walk along (a place)
a sleeplike condition. MW: to walk
(slept) while asleep.
when asleep.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb]To go or move with
(cause to) move, go or happen fast.
speed. [transitive verb] To send with
TFD: To go, move, or proceed
speed or haste; to hurry (a person, etc.)
speed Transl AM Manner 1 Rate-fast quickly. To cause to go, move, or mp-run
out or away; also, to force to go. To give
proceed quickly; hasten. MW: to
speed to (a course, etc.); to hasten; to
move, work, or take place faster. to
cause to be rapid in movement.
cause to move quickly.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) turn around and around, [intransitive verb] To revolve or gyrate;
Revolve, Rotate, especially fast. TFD: To rotate rapidly; to whirl round.
spin Self-c AM Manner 1
Spin whirl. To cause to rotate swiftly; twirl. [transitive verb] To cause to turn or
MW: to revolve rapidly : gyrate. to revolve rapidly; to twirl or whirl.
cause to whirl: impart spin to.
[intransitive verb] to move quickly
[intransitive verb] Of persons or
and suddenly towards a particular
animals: To bound or leap. Of things: To
Rate-fast; place. TFD: To move upward or
change place or position by sudden and
spring Transl Manner 1 Sudden forward in a single quick motion or a mp-jump
rapid movement without contact; to
movement? series of such motions; leap. MW:
move with a sudden jerk or bound (in
dart, shoot. To make a leap or series of
later use esp. by resilient force)
leaps
366
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to run as fast as you
can over a short distance, either in a
race or because you are in a great
hurry to get somewhere. LLA: To run
[intransitive verb] To run, row, etc., at
mp-run; Rate- as fast as you can over a short
sprint Transl Manner 1 full speed, esp. for a short distance; to mp-run
fast distance, usually in a race. TFD: To
race in this manner.
move rapidly or at top speed for a
brief period, as in running or
swimming. MW: to run or go at top
speed especially for a short distance.
[intransitive verb] to position yourself
[reflexive verb] To seat (oneself) upon
close to the ground balancing on the
the hams or haunches; to take one's seat
front part of your feet with your legs
in a crouching attitude or posture.
bent under your body. TFD: To sit in a
[intransitive verb] Of persons: To sit
Posture (vertical- crouching position with knees bent
squat Self-c Manner 1 down with the legs closely drawn up
downwards) and the buttocks on or near the heels.
beneath the hams or in front of the body;
MW: to assume or maintain a position
esp. to sit on the ground in this way or in
in which the body is supported on the
a crouching attitude. Also jocularly, to
feet and the knees are bent so that the
sit (down).
buttocks rest on or near the heels.
AS NOUN: in the US, a traditional
dance in which four pairs of dancers
[intransitive verb] No definition is
squaredance Self-c Manner 1 Dance dance together. TFD: NO FOUND AS
provided.
MOTION VERB. MW: [intransitive
verb] no definition is provided.
[intransitive verb] to walk or move
[intransitive vern] Of a person or
with a lack of balance as if you are
animal: To sway involuntarily from side
going to fall. LLA: To walk very
to side when trying to stand or walk
unsteadily, with your body moving
erect; to totter or reel as if about to fall;
from side to side and almost falling,
Self-c / to walk with a swaying movement of the
stagger Manner 1 Unsteady Motion especially because you are injured, mp-walk
Transl body and unsteady and devious steps, as
very tired, or drunk. TFD: To move or
from weakness, giddiness, intoxication,
stand unsteadily, as if under a great
or the carrying of a heavy burden. Often
weight; totter. MW: to reel from side
with adv. or phrase indicating the
to side : totter b: to move on
direction of movement.
unsteadily.
367
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[transitive verb] to follow an animal or [intransitive verb] To go stealthily to,
person as closely as possible without towards (an animal) for the purpose of
being seen or heard, usually in order to killing or capturing it (obs.). Hence, to
catch or kill them. TFD: to follow pursue game by the method of stealthy
stalk1 Transl Path + Manner 2 after G Furtive Motion mp-walk
stealthily or recur constantly and approach, esp. by the use of a stalking-
spontaneously to. MW: to pursue horse or of some device for concealing
quarry or prey stealthily. to pursue by oneself from the view of the hunted
stalking. animal.
[intransitive verb] To walk with stiff,
high, measured steps, like a long-legged
mp-walk; State bird. Usually with disparaging notion,
[intransitive verb] to walk in an angry
of Figure implying haughtiness, sullenness,
stalk2 Transl Manner 1 or proud way. TFD: walk stiffly. MW: mp-walk
(annoyed or indifference to one's surroundings, or the
to walk stiffly or haughtily.
proud) like. [i.e., angry, proud attitude].
[transitive verb] To march proudly
through (a country, etc.).
[intransitive verb] To bring down the
[intransitive and transitive verb] to put foot heavily.To bring the sole of one's
a foot down on the ground hard and foot suddenly and forcibly down (upon
Figure (body Noisy activity; quickly, making a loud noise, often to the ground or floor, or some other
Self-c /
stamp part: foot) + 2 State of Figure show anger. TFD: To walk with object), with the object of crushing or
Transl
Manner (annoyed)? forcible, heavy steps. MW: to strike beating down something. [intransitive
or thrust the foot forcibly or noisily verb] To walk with a heavy, ‘pounding’
downward. tread; to walk noisily or laboriously,
tramp.
[transitive verb] To cause a stampede
amongst (cattle); to cause a stampede of
away from [intransitive and transitive verb] No
Rate-fast; State (a person's) cattle. [intransitive verb] Of
G definition is provided. TFD: To flee in
stampede Transl Path + Manner 2 of the Figure a herd of cattle: To become panic-
(departure, a headlong rush. MW: to flee
(frightened) stricken and take to flight. Of a company
source) headlong in panic.
of persons: To rush with common
impulse.
368
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to move by lifting
your foot and putting it down in a
[intransitive verb] To lift the foot and set
different place. [intransitive verb] to
it down again on the ground in a new
put your foot on or in something.
position; to lift and set down the feet
TFD: To shift or move slightly by
Figure (body alternately in walking; to pace, tread. To
taking a step or two. To walk a short
step Transl part: foot) + 2 mp-walk move to a new position by extending the
distance to a specified place or in a
Manner foot to a higher or lower level or across
specified direction. To move with the
an intervening object or space (e.g. in
feet in a particular manner. MW: to
entering or leaving a carriage or boat,
move by raising the foot and bringing
ascending or descending stairs)
it down elsewhere or by moving each
foot in succession. to go on foot.
[intransitive verb] to walk with
intentionally heavy steps, especially as
mp-walk; State a way of showing that you are
of Figure annoyed. LLA: To walk with heavy
[intransitive verb] = STAMP; [transitive
(annoyed); steps, making a lot of noise to show
Transl / verb] To stamp or trample on (a person,
stomp Manner 1 Characteristic that you are angry. TFD: [intransitive mp-walk
Self-c etc.). To tramp or trudge between (a
use of lower verb] To tread or trample heavily or
series of places).
body (Steps- violently. [transitive verb] To tread or
heavy) trample heavily or violently on. MW:
to walk with a loud heavy step usually
in anger.
[intransitive verb] to bend the top half
of the body forward and down. TFD:
To bend forward and down from the
waist or the middle of the back. To [intransitive verb]To bow down, to
walk or stand, especially habitually, descend. Of a person: To lower the body
Posture (vertical- with the head and upper back bent by inclining the trunk or the head and
stoop Self-c Manner 1
downwards) forward. MW: To bend the body or a shoulders forward, sometimes bending
part of the body forward and the knee at the same time. Often with
downward sometimes simultaneously down.
bending the knees. to stand or walk
with a forward inclination of the head,
body, or shoulders
369
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to enter or leave a
place in a way that shows that you are
State of Figure
angry. TFD: To move or rush [intransitive verb] To rush with the
storm Transl Manner 1 (annoyed);
tumultuously, violently, or angrily. violence of a storm.
Violent Motion
MW: to rush about or move
impetuously, violently, or angrily.
from Anglo-French estraier, from
Vulgar Latin *extravagare, from Latin
extra- outside + vagari to wander
[intransitive verb] to travel along a [intransitive verb] To escape from
away from
route that was not originally intended, confinement or control, to wander away
G
stray Transl Path 1 or to move beyond a limited area. from a place, one's companions.
(departure,
TFD: To move away from a group, [intransitive verb] To wander from the
source)
deviate from the correct course, or go direct way, deviate.
beyond established limits. MW: to
wander accidentally from a fixed or
chosen route.
[intransitive verb] to move somewhere [intransitive verb] To go or advance
extremely quickly, usually in a straight quickly; to go at full speed, to rush.
streak Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast mp-run
line. TFD: To move at high speed; [transitive verb] To cause to move fast
rush. MW: to move swiftly. or like lightning.
[transitive verb] To prostrate (oneself,
one's body); to extend (one's limbs) in a
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
reclining posture; refl. to recline at full
straighten your body or your arms or
length. To extend (the arms) laterally; to
legs so that they are as long as
expand (the wings), esp. for flight.
Posture possible, in order to exercise the joints
[reflexive verb] To straighten oneself; to
stretch Self-c Manner 1 (unspecified after you have been in the same place
rise to full height (also with up); also, to
orientation) or position for a long time. TFD: To
draw up the body, as from a stooping,
become lengthened, widened, or
cramped, or relaxed posture; to
distended. MW: to become extended
straighten the body and extend the arms,
in length or breadth or both.
as a manifestation of weariness or
langour (chiefly coupled with yawn).
370
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk somewhere
quickly with long steps. LLA: To walk
mp-walk; Rate-
quickly, taking big steps, in an angry, [intransitive verb] To walk with long or
fast;
determined or confident way. TFD: To extended steps; to stalk. Often with
Characteristic
stride Transl Manner 1 walk with long steps, especially in a implication of haste or impetuosity, of mp-walk
use of lower
hasty or vigorous way. [transitive exuberant vigour, or of haughtiness or
body (Steps-
verb] To walk with long steps on, arrogance.
long)
along, or over. MW: to move with or
as if with long steps.
[intransitive verb] to walk in a slow
mp-walk; relaxed manner, especially for [intransitive verb] To walk or ramble in
Leisurely pleasure. LLA: To walk in a slow and a careless, haphazard, or leisurely
stroll Transl Manner 1
Motion; State of relaxed way, especially for pleasure. fashion as inclination directs; often
Figure (relaxed) TFD: To go for a leisurely walk. MW: simply to take a walk.
to walk at leisure along or about.
[intransitive verb] to walk in a proud
way trying to look important. LLA: To
walk proudly with your head high,
shoulders back, and chest pushed
[intransitive verb] To walk with an
mp-walk; State forward in a way that shows you think
strut Transl Manner 1 affected air of dignity or importance, mp-walk
of Figure (proud) you are important or impressive.TFD:
stepping stiffly with head erect.
To walk with pompous bearing;
swagger. MW: to walk with a proud
gait b: to walk with a pompous and
affected air.
[intransitive verb] to step awkwardly
while walking or running and fall or
begin to fall. LLA: To walk
unsteadily, often hitting things with
your feet and almost falling, especially
[intransitive verb] To miss one's footing,
Obstructed because it is dark, the ground is
or trip over an obstacle, in walking or
stumble Transl Manner 1 Motion; mp- uneven, or because you are tired or
running, so as to fall or be in danger of
walk/mp-run drunk. TFD: To miss one's step in
falling.
walking or running; trip and almost
fall. To proceed unsteadily or
falteringly; flounder. MW: to trip in
walking or running. to walk unsteadily
or clumsily.
371
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
mp-walk; State
[intransitive verb] stomp = to walk
of Figure
with intentionally heavy steps, [intransitive verb] To walk clumsily,
(annoyed);
especially as a way of showing that heavily, or noisily, as if one had a
stump Transl Manner 1 Characteristic mp-walk
you are annoyed. TFD: To walk wooden leg. To knock on the floor in
use of lower
heavily or clumsily. MW: to walk walking.
body (Steps-
heavily or clumsily.
heavy)
372
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to move, especially
quickly and powerfully. If a road,
[transitive verb] To move swiftly and
river, range of mountains, set of steps,
evenly or with continuous force over or
etc. sweeps in a particular direction,
along the surface of. [intransitive verb]
they follow a particular curved path.
To move with a strong or swift even
[transitive verb] to quickly spread
Contact motion; to move along over a surface or
through and influence an area. To
sweep Transl Manner 1 [between F and region, usu. rapidly, or with violence or
travel across all of an area, especially
G] destructive effect; sometimes, to come
when looking for something. TFD: To
with a sudden attack, to swoop. a. of a
move swiftly with strong, steady
person, an animal, a ship (or the like).
force. To move swiftly in a lofty
To move or walk in a stately manner, as
manner, as if in a trailing robe. MW:
with trailing garments.
to touch in passing with a swift
continuous movement.
[intransitive verb] to change direction,
especially suddenly. TFD: o turn aside [intransitive verb] To turn aside, deviate
Change
swerve Transl Path 1 or be turned aside from a straight in movement from the straight or direct
direction
course. MW: to turn aside abruptly course.
from a straight line or course : deviate.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move through water by moving the
body or parts of the body. TFD: To [intransitive verb] To move along in or
Manner +
swim Transl 2 mp-swim move through water by means of the on water by movements of the limbs or
Ground (water)
limbs, fins, or tail. MW: to propel other natural means of progression.
oneself in water by natural means (as
movements of the limbs, fins, or tail).
373
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move easily and without interruption [intransitive verb] To move freely
backwards and forwards or from one backwards and forwards, as a body
side to the other, especially from a suspended from a support above; to
fixed point, or to cause something or oscillate below a point of support, as a
someone to do this. TFD: To move pendulum or the like. Of a person: To
Shake, Swing, back and forth suspended or as if move backwards and forwards through
swing Self-c AM Manner 1
Vibrate suspended from above. To cause to the air upon a suspended rope or a
move back and forth, as on a swing. swing. [transitive verb] To cause to
MW: to move freely to and fro oscillate, as a body suspended from a
especially in suspension from an support above; to move or sway
overhead support. to cause to move (something) to and fro in this or a
vigorously through a wide arc or similar manner.
circle. to cause to sway to and fro.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] Of water or of objects
(cause to) move quickly with a borne on water: To move in or upon
twisting circular movement. TFD: To eddies or little whirlpools. Of other
Revolve, Rotate, move with a twisting or whirling objects: To move rapidly in eddies or in
swirl Self-c AM Manner 1
Spin motion; eddy. To cause to move with a a whirling or circular course. [transitive
twisting or whirling motion. MW: to verb] To give a whirling or eddying
move with an eddying or whirling motion to; to bring into some position by
motion. To cause to swirl. a whirling motion; to whirl, brandish.
[intransitive verb] to move very
quickly and easily through the air,
especially down from a height in order
[intransitive verb] To make a rapid
to attack. to make a sudden attack on a
sweeping descent through the air upon
place or group of people in order to
Path + Manner + down its prey, as a bird. To come down upon
swoop Transl 3 Rate-fast surround and catch them. TFD: To
Ground towards G suddenly with a sweeping movement,
move in a sudden sweep. To make a
esp. with the intention of seizing, as a
rush or an attack with or as if with a
body of troops.
sudden sweeping movement. Often
used with down. MW: to move with a
sweep.
374
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] No definition is [intransitive verb] To shift the tacks and
provided. TFD: To change the brace the yards, and turn the ship's head
Change
tack Transl Path 1 direction or course of a vessel. MW: to to the wind, so that she shall sail at the
direction
change to an opposite tack by turning same angle to the wind on the other side;
the bow to the wind. to go about in this way.
[transitive verb] to follow and watch
someone very closely, especially in [transitive verb] To follow someone
tail Transl Path+ Manner 2 after G Furtive Motion order to get information secretly.TFD: closely; spec. to follow secretly as a
To follow. MW: to form or move in a detective or spy, etc.
straggling line.
AS NOUN: an energetic dance of
South American origin for two people,
tango Self-c Manner 1 Dance or the music for this dance. [intransitive verb]To dance the tango.
[intransitive verb] TFD: to perform
this dance. MW: to dance the tango.
AS NOUN: a type of dance in which
the rhythm is marked by the noise of
the dancer's shoes on the floor. TFD:
dance and make rhythmic clicking [intransitive verb] No definition is
tapdance Self-c Manner 1 Dance
sounds by means of metal plates provided.
nailed to the sole of the dance shoes.
MW: [intransitive verb] no definition
is provided.
AS NOUN: a car with a driver whom
you pay to take you somewhere. TFD:
[intransitive and transitive verb] To be
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] To travel in a taxi.
taxi Transl AM Manner 1 transported by taxi. To transport by or
Instrument [transitive verb] To convey in a taxi.
as if by taxi. MW: [intransitive verb]
to ride in a taxicab. [transitive verb] to
transport by or as if by taxi.
[intransitive verb] to move very
[intransitive verb] To move with
quickly. LLA: To run very quickly
violence or impetuosity; to rush or
and without really looking where you
‘burst’ impetuously or violently. colloq.
tear Transl Manner 1 Rate-fast are going, because you are in a hurry.
Sometimes with the notion of a force
TFD: To move with heedless speed;
that would tear its way through
rush headlong. MW: to move or act
obstacles.
with violence, haste, or force.
375
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] To see-saw. To move
[intransitive verb] to appear to be like a see-saw; to sway from side to
about to fall while moving or standing. side; to move unsteadily; esp. of a
Self-c / TFD: [intransitive and transitive verb] person or animal, to walk with a
teeter AM Manner 1 Unsteady Motion
Transl To walk or move unsteadily or swaying motion; to balance oneself
unsurely; totter. To cause to teeter or unsteadily on alternate feet. [transitive
seesaw. MW: to move unsteadily. verb] To move (anything) with a see-
saw motion.
[intransitive verb] to (cause to)
move/act quickly or carelessly.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [reflexive verb] To fling or cast oneself;
send something through the air with to precipitate oneself. [transitive verb]
force, especially by a sudden To project (anything) with a force of the
Forced Motion movement of the arm. TFD: To cast, nature of a jerk, from the hand or arm,
throw Transl AM Manner 1 (Energy-high); fling, or hurl something. To propel so that it passes through the air or free
Rate-fast through the air with a motion of the space; to cast, hurl, fling; spec. to cast
hand or arm. To discharge into the air by a sudden jerk or straightening of the
by any means. MW: to fling (oneself) arm, esp. at the level of or over the
precipitately. to propel through the air shoulder
by a forward motion of the hand and
arm.
[intransitive verb] To push or force one's
way, as through a crowd; to crowd in; to
make one's way or advance as against
[intransitive and transitive verb] to obstacles; to press onwards or into a
Forced Motion push suddenly and strongly. TFD: To place, etc. [transitive verb] To cause
thrust Transl AM Path + Manner 2 forwards
(Energy-high) force one's way. MW: to push (anything, esp. something grasped in the
forward. hand) to enter, pierce, or penetrate some
thing or place by or as by pushing; to
put, drive, or force into some place or
position.
[intransitive verb] To make a loud
[intransitive verb] to move, making a resounding noise like thunder; to sound
thunder Transl Manner 1 Noisy activity lot of noise. TFD, MW: NO FOUND very loudly; to roar. Sometimes
AS MOTION VERB. connoting violent movement: To rush or
fall with great noise and commotion.
376
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] on tiptoe(s) on your
toes with the heel of your foot lifted
mp-walk;
off the ground. LLA: To walk on your
Characteristic
toes because you do not want to make [intransitive verb] To raise oneself or
use of lower
tiptoe Transl Manner 1 any noise. TFD: To walk or move stand on tiptoe. To go or walk on tiptoe; mp-walk
body (on
quietly on one's toes. MW: to stand or to step or trip lightly.
tiptoes): Furtive
raise oneself on tiptoe. To walk or
Motion?
proceed quietly or cautiously on or as
if on tiptoe.
[intransitive verb] To walk or go with an
[intransitive verb] TFD: To move in a up-and-down movement; to walk in an
lively, capering manner; prance. to affected manner; to mince or prance in
State of the walk with a lofty proud gait, often in one's gait; of a horse or other animal, to
tittup Transl Manner 1
Figure (proud) an attempt to impress others; MW: to canter, gallop easily; also, to prance;
move in a lively manner often with an hence of a rider, or one driving a
exaggerated or affected action. vehicle; of a boat, to toss with abrupt
jerky movements.
AS NOUN: an object used for sliding
over snow and ice which consists of a
low frame on which a person or [intransitive verb] To ride on a toboggan
Vehicle /
toboggan Transl Manner 1 people sit. [intransitive verb] TFD: To or sleigh; esp. to ‘coast’ or slide down a
Instrument
coast, ride, or travel on a toboggan. snowy (or other) slope on a toboggan.
MW: to coast on or as if on a
toboggan
[intransitive verb] (especially of a
young child) to walk with short steps,
[intransitive verb] To walk or run with
trying to keep the body balanced.
short unsteady steps, as a child just
mp-walk; TFD: To walk with short, unsteady
toddle Transl Manner 1 beginning to walk, an aged or invalid mp-walk
Unsteady Motion steps. To walk leisurely; stroll. MW:
person; also said of a similar walk or run
to walk with short tottering steps in
of any animal.
the manner of a young child. To take a
stroll.
[intransitive verb] To fall top foremost,
Earth- [intransitive and transitive verb] to or as if top-heavy; to fall headlong,
based (cause to) lose balance and fall down. tumble or pitch over. [intransitive verb]
topple Transl AM Cause + Path 2
orientation: TFD: To totter and fall. MW: to fall To lean over unsteadily, as if on the
downwards from or as if from being top-heavy. point of falling; to overhang
threateningly.
377
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to walk in a shaky
way that looks as if you are about to [intransitive verb] To walk or move with
Unsteady
Self-c / fall. TFD: To sway as if about to fall. unsteady steps; to go shakily or feebly;
totter Manner 1 Motion; mp- mp-walk
Transl To walk unsteadily or feebly; stagger. to toddle; also, to walk with difficulty;
walk?
MW: to tremble or rock as if about to to reel, stagger.
fall. to move unsteadily.
[intransitive verb] To make a tour or
circuitous journey, in which many
places are visited, usually without
retracing one's steps; to make a
[intransitive and transitive verb] to go
prolonged excursion for recreation or
on a tour somewhere. TFD: To travel
tour Transl Manner 1 Leisurely Motion business; spec. of an actor, a theatrical
from place to place, especially for
company, or the like: to go ‘on tour’, to
pleasure. MW: to make a tour (of).
travel from town to town fulfilling
engagements. [transitive verb] To make
the tour or round of, to tour in (a country
or district).
[transitive verb] to follow a person or
animal by looking for proof that they
[transitive verb] To follow up the track
have been somewhere, or by using
or footsteps of; to trace the course or
electronic equipment. TFD: To follow
track Transl Path + Manner 2 after G Furtive Motion movements of; to pursue by or as by the
the tracks of; trail. To follow a course;
track left; with down, out, up, to follow
travel. MW: To follow the tracks or
up or trace until found or caught.
traces of : trail. to search for by
following evidence until found.
378
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(allow something to) move slowly
along the ground or through the air or
water, after someone or something.
[intransitive verb] To follow the trail or
[transitive verb] to follow the trail of
track of the game. [transitive verb] To
someone or something. TFD:
Furtive Motion?; draw behind one; to drag along upon the
[intransitive verb] To drag or be
Contact ground or other surface (esp. something
trail Transl AM Path + Manner 2 after G dragged along, brushing the ground.
[between F and hanging loosely, as a long garment);
MW: to walk or proceed draggingly,
G]? also, to drag (a person) roughly, to hale;
heavily, or wearily: plod, trudge. To
to haul. To carry or convey by drawing
follow a trail. To follow upon the
or dragging, as in a vehicle or ship.
scent or trace of : track. To follow in
the footsteps of : pursue. To follow
along behind. To lag behind (as a
competitor).
[intransitive verb] to walk from one
place to another, often feeling tired or
bored. LLA: To walk a very long way, [intransitive verb] To walk in a trailing
especially when you are looking for or untidy way; e.g. to walk or ‘trail’
mp-walk; No something or visiting different places, through the mud; to walk with the dress
traipse Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
aim in motion so that you become tired. TFD: To trailing or bedraggled; to walk about
walk or tramp about. MW: to go on aimlessly or needlessly. (Usually said of
foot. to walk or travel about without a woman or child.)
apparent plan but with or without a
purpose.
AS NOUN: an electric vehicle that
transports people, usually in cities, and
[intransitive verb] To travel by a
goes along metal tracks in the road.
Vehicle / tramway or on a tram-car. [transitive
tram Transl AM Manner 1 [transitive verb] TFD: To move or
Instrument verb] To convey (coal, ore, etc.) by a
convey in a tram. MW: [transitive
tram or trams.
verb] to haul in a tram or over a
tramway.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
walk, especially long distances or with
mp-walk; Steps- [intransitive verb] To tread or walk with
tramp Transl Manner 1 heavy steps. TFD: To walk with a mp-walk
heavy a firm, heavy, resonant step; to stamp.
firm, heavy step; trudge. MW: to
walk, tread, or step especially heavily.
379
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
step heavily on something or someone,
Figure (body
mp-walk; causing damage or injury. TFD: To [intransitive verb] To tread or walk
trample Transl part: feet) + 2
Violent Motion tread heavily or destructively. MW: heavily; to stamp.
Manner
tramp; especially : to tread heavily so
as to bruise, crush, or injure.
From Anglo-French travers (as in a
travers, de travers across), from Latin
transversum (as in in transversum set
crosswise) [transitive verb] to move or [intransitive verb] To make a journey; to
travel Transl Manner 1 Leisurely Motion
travel through an area. TFD: To travel go from one place to another; to journey.
or pass across, over, or through. MW:
to go or travel across or over. to move
or pass along or through.
[transitive verb] to move or travel
pass/cross through an area. TFD: To travel or
[transitive verb] To run across or
traverse Transl Path 1 G pass across, over, or through. MW: to
through; to cross.
(traversal) go or travel across or over. to move or
pass along or through.
[intransitive and transitive verb]
MAINLY UK to put your foot on [intransitive verb] To walk, go, pace; to
Figure (body something or to press something down set down the feet in walking; to step.
mp-walk;
tread Transl part: feet) + 2 with your foot. TFD: To go on foot; Also said of the foot. To step or walk
Violent Motion
Manner walk. To set down the foot; step. MW: upon or along; to follow, pursue (a path,
o step or walk on or over. o beat or track, or road)
press with the feet : trample.
[intransitive verb] to walk a long
distance, usually over land such as
hills, mountains or forests. LLA:
[intransitive verb] To make a journey by
Especially British, to walk a very long
ox-wagon; hence, to travel, migrate;
mp-walk; Forced way especially in the mountains. TFD:
trek Transl Manner 1 also, to go, proceed; to go away, depart mp-walk
motion? To make a slow or arduous journey.
(slang). [transitive verb] To cover
To journey on foot, especially to hike
(ground, a distance) by ‘trekking’.
through mountainous areas. MW: to
make one's way arduously; broadly :
journey.
380
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] to shake slightly,
usually because you are cold, [intransitive verb] Of persons (less
frightened, or very emotional. TFD:To commonly of animals), or of the body or
Shake, Swing,
tremble Self-c Manner 1 shake involuntarily, as from a limb: To shake involuntarily as with
Vibrate
excitement or anger; quake.To vibrate fear or other emotion, cold, or weakness;
or quiver. MW: to shake involuntarily to quake, quiver, shiver.
(as with fear or cold) : shiver.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb] To strike the foot
lose your balance after knocking your Low
against something, so as to hop, stagger,
Obstructed foot against something when you are ratings.
or fall; to stumble over an obstacle; to
trip Transl AM Manner 1 Motion; mp- walking or running, or to cause No
make a false step. [transitive verb] To
walk/mp-run; someone to do this. TFD: To stumble. significant
cause to stumble or fall by suddenly
MW: to catch the foot against difference
arresting or catching the foot.
something so as to stumble.
AS NOUN: a small vehicle with two
or four wheels that you push or pull to
transport large or heavy objects on.
US for tram. [intransitive and
Vehicle / [transitive verb] to convey by trolley;
trolley Transl AM Manner 1 transitive verb] TFD: To convey
Instrument [intransitive verb] to travel by trolley
(passengers) or travel by trolley. MW:
[transitive verb] to convey by a
trolley. [intransitive verb] to ride on a
trolley.
[intransitive verb] to walk somewhere
in a large group, usually with one
mp-walk; person behind another. TFD: To [intransitive verb] To march in rank; to
troop Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
Motion in file? assemble or move in crowds. MW: to walk or pass in order.
move or gather in crowds. to move in
large numbers.
381
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] a way in which a
four-legged animal moves which is
faster than walking, in which a front
[intransitive verb] Of a horse, and
leg and the back leg on the opposite
occasionally other quadrupeds: To go at
side move together. LLA: To run
the gait called the trot. Also said of a
mp-run; Rate- fairly slowly, taking short steps. TFD: mp-run /
trot Transl AM Manner 1 man. [intransitive verb] To go or move
slow To go or move at a trot. To proceed mp-walk
quickly; to go briskly or busily; to
rapidly; hurry. [transitive verb]To
bustle; to run. [transitive verb] To cause
cause to move at a trot. MW: to ride,
to trot; to lead or ride at the trot.
drive, or proceed at a trot. to proceed
briskly : hurry. [transitive verb] to
cause to go at a trot.
[intransitive verb] to transport
something somewhere in a truck.
[intransitive verb] To drive or take
TFD: [intransitive and transitive verb]
charge of a truck, to act as a truck-
Vehicle / To drive a truck. To transport by
truck Transl AM driver. [transitive verb] To put on or into
Instrument truck. To carry goods by truck. MW:
a truck; to convey by means of a truck or
[intransitive and transitive verb] to be
trucks.
employed in driving a truck. to
transport goods by truck.
[intransitive verb] to walk slowly with
a lot of effort, especially over a
mp-walk; Rate-
difficult surface or while carrying
slow; Forced
something heavy. LLA: To walk
Motion (Energy-
slowly and with heavy steps, [intransitive verb] To walk laboriously,
low);
trudge Transl Manner 1 especially because you are tired, it is wearily, or without spirit, but steadily mp-walk
Characteristic
difficult to walk, or you do not want and persistently
use of lower
to go somewhere. TFD: To walk in a
body (Steps-
laborious, heavy-footed way; plod.
heavy)
MW: o walk or march steadily and
usually laboriously.
[intransitive and transitive verb](to
cause something) to move slowly and [intransitive verb] To move along on a
unevenly on wheels. TFD: To move surface by revolving; to roll. [transitive
Revolve, Rotate,
trundle Transl AM Manner 1 along by or as if by rolling or verb] To cause to roll along upon a
Spin
spinning. MW: to progress by surface, as a ball, hoop, or other globular
revolving. To move on or as if on or circular object; to roll, bowl.
wheels.
382
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] To fall; esp. to fall in
a helpless way, as from stumbling or
[intransitive verb] to fall quickly and
violence; to be precipitated, fall
without control. TFD:To topple, as
Earth- headlong; also said of a stream falling in
Rate-fast; from power or a high position; fall.
based a cataract. To fall prone, fall to the
tumble Transl AM Path + Manner 2 Uncontrolled [transitive verb] To cause to fall; bring
orientation: ground; often const. down, over. Also,
Motion down. MW: to fall suddenly and
downwards to stumble by tripping over an object.
helplessly. [transitive verb] to cause to
[transitive verb] To cause to fall
tumble (as by pushing or toppling).
suddenly or violently; to throw or cast
down.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) move in a circle round a [intransitive verb] To move round on an
fixed point or line. TFD: To move axis or about a centre; to rotate, revolve,
around an axis or center; rotate or whirl, spin, as a wheel; to move partly
Revolve, Rotate, revolve. To cause to move around an round in this way, as a door or the like
turn1 Self-c AM Manner 1
Spin axis or center; cause to rotate or upon hinges, a key, a weathercock, etc.
revolve. MW: o move around on an [transitive verb] To cause to move round
axis or through an arc of a circle : on an axis or about a centre; to cause to
rotate. to cause to move around an axis rotate or revolve, as a wheel.
or a center : make rotate or revolve.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) change the direction in
[intransitive verb] To move or shift (by a
which you are facing or moving. TFD:
rotary motion, or through an angle) so as
Change To change or reverse one's way,
turn2 Transl AM Path 1 to change one's posture or position; esp.
direction course, or direction. To change the
to shift the body (as on an axis) from
direction or course of. MW: to reverse
side to side; to twist or writhe about.
a course or direction. To present by a
change in direction or position.
383
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] To wind or twist
(about, over, or round something);
[intransitive and transitive verb] to almost always of a plant: to grow in a
wrap round an object several times. twisting or spiral manner; spec. to
Revolve, Rotate, TFD: To become twisted, interlaced, become twisted or wreathed together in
Spin (wrap or interwoven. To go in a winding growing; to grow in spiral convolutions.
twine Self-c AM Manner 1
around, turn course; twist about. MW: to coil about [transitive verb] To turn (something)
around) a support. to stretch or move in a about, away, round, etc.; to twist or
sinuous manner : meander. To twist wring. To cause (one thing) to encircle
together. to form by twisting : weave or embrace another; to twist, wreathe,
clasp, or wrap (a thing) about or around
another.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] To rotate rapidly, to
(cause to) give a sudden quick turn or spin; to be whirled round or about; also
set of turns in a circle. TFD: o move or to turn round quickly so as to face or
Revolve, Rotate,
spin around rapidly, suddenly, or point the other way; also fig. of the mind
twirl Self-c AM Manner 1 Spin (turn
repeatedly. To rotate or revolve or head: to be in a whirl, be confused or
around)
briskly; swing in a circle; spin. MW: giddy. [transitive verb] To cause to
to revolve rapidly. to cause to rotate rotate or spin; to turn (an object) round
rapidly. rapidly.
[intransitive verb and reflexive] To pass
or move in a tortuous manner; to coil or
twine about or round; to penetrate into
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
something with a tortuous movement or
turn something, especially repeatedly,
action. To form into a spiral; to bend,
Posture (Bend, or to turn or wrap one thing around
twist1 Self-c AM Manner 1 curve, or coil spirally. [transitive verb]
Twist, Curve) another. TFD: To be or become
To wind or coil (a thread or the like) on
twisted. To rotate or revolve. MW: to
or round something; to attach in this
turn or change shape under torsion.
way; to encircle (an object) with or as
with a thread, etc.; to entwine in
something else.
twist2 Self-c Manner 1 Dance TFD, MW: to dance the twist.
384
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
jump over something by first putting
[intransitive verb] To spring or leap;
your hands on it or by using a pole.
spec. to leap with the assistance of the
mp-jump; Use of LLA: To jump over something, using
hand resting on the thing to be
vault Transl Manner 1 one's hands or of your hands to help you. TFD: To jump mp-jump
surmounted, or with the aid of a pole.
an instrument across or leap over (an obstacle). MW:
[transitive verb] To get over, surmount,
To leap vigorously; especially : to
by vaulting.
execute a leap using the hands or a
pole.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
shake slightly and quickly, or to cause
something to do this, in a way that is [intransitive verb] To move or swing
felt rather than seen or heard. TFD: To backwards and forwards, or upwards
move back and forth or to and fro, and downwards, with some degree of
Shake, swing especially rhythmically and rapidly. rapidity; to quiver, shake, or tremble.
vibrate Self-c AM Manner 1
vibrate To shake or move with or as if with a [transitive verb] To give a vibratory
slight quivering or trembling motion to (something); to cause to move
motion.To cause to tremble or quiver. to and fro or up and down, esp. with a
MW: To move to and fro or from side quick motion; to put in vibration.
to side : oscillate. to swing or move to
and fro.
[intransitive verb] (usually of a person
or animal with short legs and a fat
body) to walk with short steps,
swinging the body from one side to [intransitive verb] To walk with short
mp-walk; the other. TFD: To walk heavily and steps, swaying alternately from one leg
waddle Transl Manner 1 Swinging clumsily with a pronounced sway. to the other, as is done by a stout short- mp-walk
Motion MW: to walk with short steps legged person. said of animals; esp. of
swinging the forepart of the body from ducks or geese.
side to side. to move clumsily in a
manner suggesting a waddle (=an
awkward clumsy swaying gait).
385
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
walk through water with difficulty
because of the pressure of the water [intransitive verb] To walk through
against your legs. LLA: To walk water or any liquid or soft substance
mp-walk; Forced
Ground (water) through deep water. TFD: To walk in which impedes motion. Formerly often,
wade Transl 2 Motion (Energy- mp-walk
+ Manner or through water or something else to pass over a river, etc., on foot (now
low)
that similarly impedes normal rare). [transitive verb] To walk through
movement. MW: to step in or through (water, etc.).
a medium (as water) offering more
resistance than air.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
[intransitive verb] [intransitive verb] To
(cause to) move gently through the air.
pass through the air or through space; to
TFD: To float easily and gently, as on
float upon, come or go with the wind or
the air; drift. to cause to go gently and
Ground (air) + breeze. [transitive verb] Of the wind: To
waft Transl AM 2 Smooth Motion smoothly through the air or over
Manner propel (a vessel) or convey (a navigator
water. MW: to move or go lightly on
or passenger) safely. To carry
or as if on a buoyant medium. to cause
(something) through the air or through
to move or go lightly by or as if by the
space.
impulse of wind or waves.
[intransitive and transitive verb]
(especially of a tail or finger) to move
from side to side or up and down, Intransitive uses. To be in motion or
especially quickly and repeatedly, or activity; to stir, move. Now colloq.
to cause this to happen. TFD: To move (chiefly in negative context), to stir,
briskly and repeatedly from side to move one's limbs. To oscillate, shake, or
side, to and fro, or up and down.To sway alternately in opposite directions,
move (a body part) rapidly from side as something working on a pivot, fitting
Shake, Swing, to side or up and down, as in loosely in a socket, or the like. Of a boat
wag Self-c AM Manner 1
Vibrate playfulness, agreement, admonition, or or ship: To rock. Of a limb, the head or
chatter. MW: to move to and fro or up tail, etc.: To be moved briskly from side
and down especially with quick jerky to side. [transitive verb] To move (a
motions. o swing to and fro or up and limb or part of the body attached by a
down especially with quick jerky joint) to and fro, up and down, or from
motions. switch <a dog wagging its side to side: usually implying rapid and
tail>; specifically : to nod (the head) repeated movement.
or shake (a finger) at (as in assent or
mild reproof).
386
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move along by putting one foot in
front of the other, allowing each foot
to touch the ground before lifting the [intransitive verb] To journey, move
walk Transl Manner 1 mp-walk next. TFD: To move over a surface by about, esp. on foot. To go from place to
taking steps with the feet at a pace place; to journey, wander.
slower than a run. To go or travel on
foot. MW: To move along on foot :
advance by steps.
AS NOUN: a formal dance in which
two people holding each other move
around a large room, turning as they
[intransitive verb] To dance a waltz.
go and repeating a movement of three
Also, to be addicted to, or practised in,
waltz Self-c Manner 1 Dance steps, or a piece of music with three
the waltz; to dance the waltz in a
beats in a bar written for this style of
specified manner.
dancing. [intransitive verb] to dance a
waltz. TFD: To dance the waltz. MW:
to dance a waltz.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
walk around slowly in a relaxed way [intransitive verb] Of persons or
or without any clear purpose or animals: To move hither and thither
direction. LLA: To walk around with without fixed course or certain aim; to
no particular aim and in no particular be (in motion) without control or
mp-walk; No
wander Transl Manner 1 direction, especially in a place you do direction; to roam, ramble, go idly or mp-walk
aim in Motion
not know or when you are lost. restlessly about; to have no fixed abode
TFD:To move about without a definite or station. [transitive verb] To roam
destination or purpose. MW: to move over, in, through (a place); to traverse in
about without a fixed course, aim, or wandering.
goal. to go idly about : ramble.
387
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
move from side to side, or to make
something move like this while
holding it in the hand. [intransitive and
transitive verb] to raise your hand and [intransitive verb] To move to and fro or
move it from side to side as a way of up and down. [transitive verb] To move
greeting someone, telling them to do through the air with a sweeping gesture
something or adding emphasis to an (the uplifted or extended arm or hand, or
expression. TFD:To move freely back something held in the hand by one
Shake, swing,
wave Self-c AM Manner 1 and forth or up and down in the air, as extremity, e.g. a wand, a hat, or
vibrate
branches in the wind. To cause to something that flutters in the breeze, as a
move back and forth or up and down, flag, a handkerchief), often as a sign of
either once or repeatedly. MW: to greeting or farewell, or as an expression
motion with the hands or with of exultation; usually implying repeated
something held in them in signal or movements to and fro or up and down.
salute. to float, play, or shake in an air
current : move loosely to and fro. to
swing (something) back and forth or
up and down.
[transitive verb] to push an object that
has wheels so that it moves in a
particular direction. [intransitive and To move like a wheel (and connected
transitive verb] TFD: To roll or move senses). [intransitive verb] To turn or
Vehicle / on or as if on wheels or a wheel. To revolve about an axis or centre, like a
Instrument → roll, move, or transport on wheels or a wheel on its axle; to rotate; to whirl.
wheel Transl AM Manner 1
Revolve, Rotate, wheel. MW: to turn on or as if on an [transitive verb] To turn (something) on
Spin axis : revolve. to travel on or as if on or as on a wheel; to cause to revolve
wheels or in a wheeled vehicle. o about an axis; to rotate; to cause to move
cause to turn on or as if on an axis : in a circle or cycle.
rotate. to convey or move on or as if
on wheels or in a wheeled vehicle.
388
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to [intransitive verb] To move in a circle or
(cause something to) spin round. TFD: similar curve, to circle, circulate; more
To revolve rapidly about a center or an vaguely, to move about in various
axis. [transitive verb] To cause to directions, esp. with rapidity or force; to
rotate or turn rapidly. MW: to move go (wander, fly, etc.) about; to be in
Revolve, Rotate, in a circle or similar curve especially commotion. To turn, esp. swiftly,
whirl Self-c AM Manner 1
Spin with force or speed. To turn on or around an axis, like a wheel; to revolve
around an axis like a wheel. [transitive or rotate (rapidly); to spin. [transitive
verb] to drive, impel, or convey with verb] To cause to rotate or revolve, esp.
or as if with a rotary motion. To cause swiftly or forcibly; to move (something)
to turn usually rapidly on or around an around an axis, or in a circle or the like:
axis with various shades of meaning.
TFD: [intransitive verb] To move
[intransitive verb] To move with a light
lightly, nimbly, and rapidly. [transitive
rapid sweeping motion. To rush or dart
verb] To move or cause to move with
nimbly; to move about or travel swiftly
whisk Transl AM Manner 1 Rate-fast quick light sweeping motions. MW: mp-run
or briskly. [transitive motion] To move
[intransitive verb] to move nimbly and
(something) about, away, back, etc. with
quickly. [transitive verb] to move or
a light sweeping motion.
convey briskly.
[intransitive verb] to move or do
something very fast. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To make a whirring
[intransitive verb] To make a sound as
or hissing sound, as of an object
of a body rushing through the air. To
speeding through air. To move swiftly
Rate-fast; Noisy move swiftly with or as with such a
whiz (z) Transl Manner 1 with or as if with such a sound; rush. mp-run
Activity sound. [transitive verb] To cause to
MW: [intransitive verb] to hum, whir,
whizz; to hurl, shoot, or convey swiftly
or hiss like a speeding object (as an
with a whizz
arrow or ball) passing through air. To
fly or move swiftly especially with a
whiz.
389
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause to) move up and down and/or [intransitive verb] To move to and fro or
from side to side with small quick from side to side irregularly and lightly,
movements. TFD: To move back and to waggle; to walk with such a
Shake, Swing,
wiggle Self-c AM Manner 1 forth with quick irregular motions. o movement, to stagger, reel, also to
Vibrate
cause to move back and forth with waddle (now dial.); to go or move
quick irregular motions. MW: o move sinuously, to wriggle. [transitive verb]
to and fro with quick jerky or shaking To move (something) in this way;
motions. To cause to wiggle.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
turn or cause something to turn. TFD:
[intransitive verb] To move in or have
a curving or twisting course. To move
in or have a spiral or circular course. [intransitive verb] To take or have a bent
Posture (Bend,
wind Self-c AM Manner 1 [transitive verb] To wrap (something) form. [transitive verb] To turn; to cause
Twist, Curve)
around a center or another object once to move in a curve.
or repeatedly. To wrap or encircle (an
object) in a series of coils; entwine.
MW: to have a curving course or
shape.
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
(cause something to) shake or move
from side to side in a way that shows a
lack of balance. TFD: To move or
[intransitive verb] Of a person or
rotate with an uneven or rocking
animal: To move from side to side
Shake, Swing, motion or unsteadily from side to side.
wobble Self-c AM Manner 1 unsteadily or with uncertain direction.
Vibrate To tremble or quaver. To cause to
[transitive verb] To cause to move
wobble. MW: to move or proceed with
unsteadily from side to side.
an irregular rocking or staggering
motion or unsteadily and clumsily
from side to side b: tremble, quaver.
To cause to wobble.
390
APPENDIX 1. MOTION VERBS IN ENGLISH
Motor
English Motion Semantic Path Dictionary Definition (CALD 1, The pattern
Motion AM # Manner Details Dictionary Definition (OED)
verb components Details Free Dictionary, MW, LLA) Rating
Study
[intransitive and transitive verb] to
twist your body, or move part of your [intransitive verb] To twist or turn the
body, with small, quick movements. body about with short writhing
TFD: To turn or twist the body with movements; to move sinuously; to
Posture (Bend,
wriggle Self-c Manner 1 sinuous writhing motions; squirm. writhe, squirm, wiggle. [transitive verb]
Twist, Curve)
MW: to move the body or a bodily To cause to writhe, twist, or bend
part to and fro with short writhing tortuously; to move or turn writhingly or
motions like a worm to move or with quick jerks.
advance by twisting and turning.
AS NOUN: a boat with sails and
sometimes an engine, used for either
Vehicle / racing or travelling on for pleasure. [intransitive verb] To make a trip in a
yacht Transl NO Manner 1
Instrument [intransitive] TFD: To sail, cruise, or yacht.
race in a yacht. MW: [intransitive
verb] to race or cruise in a yacht.
[intransitive verb] to make a
movement or shape like a zigzag.
TFD: To move in or form a zigzag. [intransitive verb] To go or move in a
zigzag Self-c Manner 1 Path-shape [transitive verb] To cause to move in zigzag course; to have a zigzag course or
or form a zigzag. MW: to lie in, direction
proceed along, or consist of a zigzag
course.
[intransitive verb] to move very
[intransitive verb] To make a continuous
quickly. TFD: To make a continuous
low-pitched humming or buzzing sound;
Rate-fast; Noisy low-pitched buzzing or humming
zoom Transl Manner 1 to travel or move (as if) with a mp-run
Activity sound.To move while making such a
‘zooming’ sound; to move at speed, to
sound. MW: to move with a loud low
hurry. Also loosely, to go hastily.
hum or buzz. To go speedily.
391
APPENDIX 2
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
Forced («a, hacia, sobre») [pronominal verb]
to/towards
abalanzarse [to Motion Dirigirse alguien o algo violentamente [pronominal verb] Lanzarse, arrojarse en
Transl Path + Manner 2 G (arrival,
dash (over) to] (Energy- hacia un sitio. Arrojarse, echarse, dirección a alguien o algo.
endpoint)
high) lanzarse, precipitarse.
away from
del fr. «abandonner», del germ.
abandonar [to G [transitive verb] Dejar un lugar,
Transl Path 1 «bann», orden de castigo;[transitive
abandon] (departure, apartarse de él.
verb] Marcharse de cierto sitio
source)
[transitive verb] Dicho de una
(de «a-2» y «bordo») (de «borde1»,
embarcación: Llegar a otra, chocar o
del frances bord) [transitive and
tocar con ella. U. t. c. Intr. [i.e., also
abordar [to bord to/towards intransitive verb] Acercarse un barco a
used as intransitive verb]. Acercarse a
in attack; to Transl Path 1 G (arrival, otro hasta tocarlo, intencionadamente
alguien para hacerle una pregunta,
approach] endpoint) o por accidente. [transitive verb]
iniciar un diálogo o tratar algún asunto.
Dirigirse a alguien para hablarle de un
[intransitive and transitive verb] Tomar
asunto o pedirle algo.
puerto, llegar a una costa, isla, etc.
del lat. «accedere», acercarse;
acceder [to gain [intransitive verb] Entrar en un lugar o
Transl Path 1 into G [intransitive verb] («a») Tener acceso
access into] pasar a él.
a un lugar
del lat. «assectari», perseguir
Furtive [transitive verb] Observar, aguardar
acechar [to stalk] Transl Path + Manner 2 after G [transitive verb] Observar mp-walk
Motion cautelosamente con algún propósito.
cautelosamente; seguir los pasos
del lat. «accelerare» [transitive verb]
acelerar [to
Aumentar gradualmente la velocidad [transitive verb] Dar mayor velocidad,
speed up; to Transl Manner 1 Rate-increase mp-run
de un movimiento o de una acción aumentar la velocidad.
accelerate]
cualquiera
[transitive verb] («a») Poner una cosa
cerca o más cerca de quien habla o de De a-1 y cerca2. Del lat. Circa.
acercar (se) [to
algo que se expresa. Aproximar. [transitive verb and pronominal verb]
(cause to) move Transl AM Path 1 closer to G
(inf.; «a») Ir a un lugar para hacer Poner cerca o a menor distancia de lugar
closer to]
cierta cosa. (inf.; «a») Dirigirse hacia o tiempo.
una persona
393
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[transitive verb] «a-2» y «costa1»
[transitive verb] Echar o tender a alguien
acostar (se) [to Poner a alguien tumbado, para que
Posture para que duerma o descanse, y con
lay down; to lie Self-c AM Manner 1 repose; [pronominal verb] Ponerse con
(horizontal) especialidad en la cama. U. t. c. prn.
down] el cuerpo descansando en posición
[i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
horizontal. Echarse
(de «recudir», con influencia de
«acorrer») (del lat. «recutere») (del lat.
to/towards [intransitive verb] Dicho de una persona:
acudir [to go to a «accurrere», acudir) [intransitive
Transl Path 1 G (arrival, Ir al sitio adonde le conviene o es
specific place] verb] («a») Ir alguien a cierto sitio
endpoint) llamada.
donde es esperado o llamado o tiene
que hacer
de «a-2» y el lat. «corrugare»,
arrugar?) [pronominal verb] Aplicado
acurrucarse [to Posture a personas, ponerse doblado y [pronominal verb] Encogerse para
Self-c Manner 1
curl up] (Bend, Roll) encogido, ocupando el menos espacio resguardarse del frío o con otro objeto.
posible, para esconderse, para librarse
del frío.
[intransitive verb and pronominal
adelantar (se) [to verb] («a, en») Ir hacia delante; [transitive verb] Mover o llevar hacia
(cause to) move Transl AM Path 1 forwards [transitive verb] Llevar o mover una adelante. U. t. c. prn. [i.e., also used as
forwards] cosa hacia delante; Pasar delante de pronominal verb]
otro en una carrera o en otra cosa.
394
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[transitive verb and pronominal verb]
agitar(se) [to Shake,
del lat. «agitare», hacer, mover; [transitive verb and pronominal verb]
(cause to) shake, Self-c AM Manner 1 Swing,
Mover[se] algo repetidamente a un Mover con frecuencia y violentamente.
to move about] Vibrate
lado y a otro
[transitive verb] del sup. lat.
[transitive verb] Llegar a juntarse con
to/toward G «incalciare», con cambio de prefijo.
alcanzar [to alguien o algo que va delante.
Transl Path 1 (arrival, Llegar al punto en que está algo o
reach] [intransitive verb] Llegar hasta cierto
endpoint) alguien que va delante en una marcha
punto o término.
o progreso hacia cierta cosa.
away from
alejar (se) [to [transitive verb and pronominal verb]
G [pronominal verb] («de») Irse alguien
(cause to) move Transl AM Path 1 Distanciar, llevar una cosa o a una
(departure, o algo lejos del sitio que se considera.
far away from] persona lejos o más lejos.
source)
[intransitive verb] Dicho de un ave:
aletear [to flap, [intransitive verb] Mover repetidas Mover frecuentemente las alas sin echar
Figure (body part:
to flutter, to Self-c 1 veces las alas como para echarse a a volar. Dicho de un pez: Mover
wings, fins)
wriggle] volar, o las aletas. frecuentemente las aletas cuando se lo
saca del agua.
[intransitive verb] Hacer algo más
aligerar [to rápido. Acelerar, apresurar. (inf.) En [intransitive verb] Abreviar, acelerar. U.
quicken, to hurry Transl Manner 1 Rate-increase lenguaje informal se emplea también t. c. prn [i.e., also used as pronominal mp-run
up] como intransitivo: ‘Si no aligeras, verb]
llegarás tarde’.
[transitive verb] del sup. lat. «altiare»,
alzar (se) [to put Posture de «altus», alto; Poner vertical algo
[transitive verb] levantar (mover hacia
up, to lift up; to Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical- que estaba tumbado. [pronominal
arriba). [pronominal verb] Levantarse.
rise] upwards) verb] Ponerse en pie cuando se está
sentado.
mp-walk; No [intransitive verb] (del lat.
ambular [to [intransitive verb] Poco usado andar, ir
Transl Manner 1 aim in «ambulare», pasear) Ir continuamente mp-walk
wander about] de una parte a otra
Motion de un sitio a otro.
anadear [to walk mp-walk; [intransitive verb] Andar una persona
[intransitive verb] Andar una persona o
like a duck; to Transl Manner 1 Animal-like con movimientos semejantes a los del mp-walk
un animal moviendo mucho las caderas.
duck walk] → Swinwing pato.
395
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] andar1 (del sup. [intransitive verb] Dicho de un ser
rom. «amlare», del lat. «ambulare») animado: Ir de un lugar a otro dando
andar [to walk] Transl Manner 1 mp-walk Hyperonym
[intransitive verb] Moverse de un lado pasos. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
a otro dando pasos. Caminar, marchar. pronominal verb]
away from [transitive verb] Quitar a alguien o algo
apartar (se) [to [transitive and pronominal verb]
G del lugar donde estaba, para dejarlo
(cause to) move Transl AM Path 1 Alejar[se] o separar[se] una cosa de
(departure, desocupado. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used
away from] otras o de cierto sitio.
source) as pronominal verb]
396
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] Del lat. arripare, de
arribar [(of a to/towards
ripa, orilla. Mar. Llegar un barco al [intransitive verb] Del lat. arripare, de
ship) to reach Transl Path 1 G (arrival,
puerto de su destino o refugiarse en un ripa, orilla. Llegar la nave a un puerto.
port; to arrive] endpoint)
puerto. Llegar.
arrimar (se) [to [transitive verb and pronominal verb] [transitive verb] Acercar o poner algo
(cause to) move Transl AM Path 1 closer to G («a») Poner[se] una cosa cerca o más junto a otra cosa. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also
closer to] cerca. Acercar[se]. used as pronominal verb]
[transitive verb] Hacer que alguien
arrodillar (se) [to Posture [pronominal verb] Ponerse con las hinque la rodilla o ambas rodillas.
(cause to) kneel Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical- piernas dobladas, sosteniéndose sobre [intransitive verb] Ponerse de rodillas.
down] downwards) las rodillas, por ejemplo para orar. U. m. c. prn [most commonly used as
pronominal verb]
[transitive verb] Impeler con violencia
[transitive verb] Dejar caer algo
una cosa, de modo que recorra una
Forced intencionadamente desde sitio alto,
arrojar (se) [to distancia, movida del impulso que ha
Motion impulsándolo o sin impulsarlo.
throw; to throw Transl AM Manner 1 recibido. [pronominal verb] Precipitarse,
(Energy- [pronominal verb] Lanzarse,
oneself] dejarse ir con violencia. Ir violentamente
high) precipitarse, tirarse. Dirigirse brusca y
hacia alguien o algo hasta llegar a él o
violentamente sobre algo o alguien.
ello.
Earth-based
ascender [to (del lat. «ascendere») [intransitive [intransitive verb] Subir de un sitio a
Transl AM Path 1 orientation:
ascend] verb] Ir hacia arriba. Subir. otro más alto.
upwards
asentar (se) [to Posture [transitive verb] Sentar a alguien en un
[transitive and pronominal verb] De a-1
(cause to) sit Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical- sitio. [pronominal verb] Sentarse una
y sentar. Sentar en silla, banco, etc.
down] downwards) persona.
atajar [to go
(de «a-2» y «tajar» (del lat. «taliāre»,
somewhere by
Transl Ground (short cut) 1 cortar) ) [intransitive verb] Hacer más [intransitive verb] Ir o tomar por el atajo.
taking a short
corto el camino yendo por un atajo.
cut]
397
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] Dicho de un avión o
de un artefacto volador cualquiera:
[intransitive verb] Posarse en tierra un Posarse tras una maniobra de descenso,
down onto
aterrizar [to Path + Ground avión u otro aparato de navegación sobre tierra firme o sobre cualquier pista
Transl 2 G (arrival,
land] (land) aérea; puede también hacer de sujeto o superficie que sirva a tal fin.
endpoint)
«aviador, piloto», etc. Caer al suelo. [intransitive verb] Dicho de un piloto, de
un pasajero, de un paracaidista, etc.:
Llegar a tierra.
del ár. «atraqqà», ascender Del ár. at-taraqqa, la acción de anclar la
atracar [(of a to/toward G [intransitive verb] Arrimar una nave. [transitive verb] Mar. Arrimar
ship) to reach Transl Path 1 (arrival, embarcación a la costa o a otra unas embarcaciones a otras, o a tierra. U.
port] endpoint) embarcación. Arribar. Llegar a tierra t. c. intr. [i.e., also used as intransitive
una embarcación. verb] Acercar, arrimar.
atravesar [to pass/cross de «a-2» y «través» (del lat.
[transitive verb] Pasar cruzando de una
cross, to go Transl Path 1 G «transversus») [transitive verb] Pasar
parte a otra.
through] (traversal) de un lado al opuesto de una cosa.
de «a-2» y «tropel» [transitive verb] [transitive verb] Pasar precipitadamente
Manner + Alcanzar una caballería, un vehículo u por encima de alguien. Dicho de un
atropellar [to run Vehicle /
Transl CONCURRENT 2 otra cosa en movimiento a alguien o vehículo: Alcanzar violentamente a
over] Instrument
RESULT algo y pasarle por encima o arrastrarlo personas o animales, chocando con ellos
destrozándolo o causándole daño. y ocasionándoles, por lo general, daños.
[intransitive verb] Dicho especialmente
del sup. lat. «abantiare», de «ab ante» de las tropas: Ir hacia adelante. U. t. c.
avanzar [to move
Transl Path 1 forwards [intransitive verb] («a, hacia, hasta») prn [i.e., also used as pronominal verb].
forwards]
Ir hacia delante. Adelantar, progresar. [transitive verb] Adelantar, mover o
prolongar hacia adelante.
del lat. «baiulare», mecer? [transitive
[intransitive verb] Ejecutar movimientos
verb] Mover el cuerpo y los
acompasados con el cuerpo, brazos y
bailar [to dance] Self-c Manner 1 Dance miembros con ritmo, generalmente
pies. U. t. c. tr [i.e., also used as
siguiendo el compás de una música.
transitive verb] Bailar una polca
Danzar.
bailotear [to
[intransitive verb] Bailar mucho, y en
dance with little [intransitive verb] Bailar de cualquier
Self-c Manner 1 Dance especial cuando se hace sin gracia ni
attention or in a manera, con poca atención.
formalidad.
careless way]
398
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
de bajo, lat. bassus. [intransitive verb
and pronominal verb] («a, de, hacia, De bajo. Del lat. bassus. [intransitive
Earth-based
por») Ir, en sentido material o verb] Ir desde un lugar a otro que esté
orientation:
bajar [to (cause inmaterial, de un sitio o posición a más bajo. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
Transl AM Path 1 downwards;
to) go down] otros más bajos. [transitive verb] pronominal verb]. [transitive verb]
down
Poner una cosa baja o más baja de lo Poner algo en lugar inferior a aquel en
from/to G
que estaba, o llevar una cosa de un que estaba.
sitio a otro más bajo.
del antig. «balanzar», de «balanza»)
[pronominal verb and no frequently
[intransitive verb] Dicho especialmente
balancear (se) [to Shake, intransitive verb] Moverse un cuerpo
de una nave: Dar o hacer balances. U. t.
(cause to) to Self-c AM Manner 1 Swing, inclinándose de un lado a otro.
c. tr [i.e., also used as transitive verb and
swing] Vibrate [transitive verb and pronominal verb]
as pronominal verb]
Mover[se] una cosa colgante u
oscilante de un lado a otro.
bordear [to skirt, [transitive verb] Ir próximo a la orilla
Ground (edge of [transitive verb] Ir por el borde, o cerca
to go along the Transl 1 de algo sin entrar en ello. Bojar,
something) del borde u orilla de algo.
edge of] bojear, ir [o seguir] a lo largo. Rodear.
[transitive verb] Lanzar contra una
superficie dura una pelota u otro cuerpo
[transitive verb] del sup. germ. elástico para que retroceda con impulso.
«botan», golpear; Lanzar hacia el [intransitive verb] Dicho de una pelota o
botar [to (cause suelo una pelota, un balón, etc., de de un balón: Saltar al chocar contra una
to) bounce, Self-c AM Manner 1 Bounce forma que rebote. [intransitive verb] superficie dura. [intransitive verb]
rebound] Salir despedida una pelota u otra cosa Dicho de una persona: Saltar desde el
elástica al chocar en algún sitio. suelo. Dicho de un cuerpo: Chocar
Epostracismo. Arbotante, rebotar. repetidamente contra una superficie
desigual y salir despedido por la fuerza
de los choques.
[intransitive verb] Levantarse a cierta
altura en el aire mediante un esfuerzo
brincar [to jump] Self-c Manner 1 mp-jump instantáneo de los músculos, para [intransitive verb] Dar brincos o saltos. mp-jump
subirse a un sitio, o volviendo a caer
inmediatamente. Saltar.
399
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
bucear [to dive,
Manner + Ground mp-swim; [intransitive verb] Nadar [intransitive verb] Nadar con todo el
swim down under Transl 2
(underwater) underwater manteniéndose debajo del agua. cuerpo sumergido.
the water]
[intransitive verb] Subir o montar a
Vehicle / [intransitive verb] («a»: ‘a lomos, a
cabalgar [to ride caballo. U. t. c. tr [i.e., also used as
Transl Manner 1 Instrument espaldas, a mujeriegas’; «en, sobre»)
a horse] transitive verb] Andar o pasear a
(horse) Estar o caminar sobre una caballería.
caballo.
[intransitive verb] Mover la cabeza a
cabecear [to un lado y a otro o arriba y abajo; como [intransitive verb] Mover o inclinar la
Figure (body part:
move or to shake Self-c 1 se hace al dormitar, como hace a veces cabeza, ya a un lado, ya a otro, o
head)
one's head] el caballo o como hacen las flores moverla reiteradamente hacia adelante.
sobre sus tallos con el viento.
400
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «caput», cabeza, y el sup. lat.
Forced «puteare», sumergir) [transitive verb] [transitive verb] Meter a alguien de
capuzar (se) -
Path + Ground Motion Meter a alguien de cabeza o cabeza en el agua. U. t. c. [i.e., also used
chapuzar(se) [to Transl AM 3 into G
(water) + Manner (Energy- bruscamente en el agua. Chapuzar, as intransitive verb and as pronominal
(cause to) dive in]
high) zambullir. [pronominal verb] Meterse verb]
en el agua de cabeza o bruscamente.
[intransitive verb] Hacer giros o
caracolear [(of a Revolve,
caracoles el caballo. Escarcear (Vuelta [intransitive verb] Dicho de un caballo:
horse) to turn Self-c Manner 1 Rotate, Spin
que da un caballo por inquietud suya o Hacer caracoles (vueltas y tornos).
around] (turn around)
por voluntad del jinete)
mp-walk;
Vehicle /
chancletear [to [intransitive verb] Hacer el ruido
Instrument
walk using flip- Transl Manner 1 particular que se produce al andar con [intransitive verb] Andar en chancletas. mp-walk
(flipflops);
flops] chancletas. Traquetear.
Noisy
Motion
[intransitive verb] Hacer ruido en el [intransitive verb] Producir ruido al
chapotear [to
Manner + Ground Noisy agua, en el barro, etc., golpeándolos o mover las manos o los pies en el agua o
move noisily in Self-c 2
(water/mud) Motion moviéndolos desordenadamente, por el lodo, o al pisar estos. U. t. c. tr [i.e.,
water/mud]
ejemplo con los pies o las manos. also used as transitive verb]
[intransitive verb] («con, contra, en») [intransitive verb] Dicho de dos cosas:
Juntarse dos cosas con un golpe, bien Encontrarse violentamente una con otra,
chocar [to (cause CONCURRENT
Transl AM 1 moviéndose ambas, bien como una bala contra la muralla, un
to) crash] RESULT
permaneciendo quieta una de ellas. buque con otro, etc. [transitive verb]
[transitive verb] Hacer chocar. Hacer que algo choque.
(del lat. «circulāre») [intransitive verb]
circular [to move
(Motion) +Ground Moverse o dar vueltas en un circuito. [intransitive verb] Andar o moverse en
along a given Transl 1
(circuit) Por extensión, pasar, moverse dentro derredor. [intransitive verb] Ir y venir.
path, to circuit]
de un conducto o camino.
401
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
mp-walk;
[intransitive verb] Andar inclinando el
Forced [intransitive verb] Tener un andar
cuerpo más a un lado que a otro, por no
cojear [to limp] Transl Manner 1 Motion; State desigual por alguna lesión o defecto
poder sentar con regularidad e igualdad
of Figure físico.
los pies.
(injured)
colear [(of an Figure (body part: [intransitive verb] Menear la cola un [intransitive verb] Mover con frecuencia
Self-c 1
animal) to wag] tail) animal. la cola.
402
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «currere») [intransitive verb]
(«Arrancar a, Echarse a, Lanzarse a, [intransitive verb] Dicho de una persona:
Ponerse a; a, por») Ir rápidamente de Andar rápidamente y con tanto impulso
correr [to run] Transl Manner 1 mp-run Hyperonym
un sitio a otro con pasos largos que que, entre un paso y el siguiente, quedan
son saltos, pues se levanta el pie del por un momento ambos pies en el aire.
suelo antes de haber apoyado el otro.
[intransitive verb] coloq. Correr en
[intransitive verb] Ir corriendo de un
mp-run; No varias direcciones dentro de limitado
corretear [to run lado para otro, como hacen los niños. mp-run /
Transl Manner 1 aim in espacio por juego o diversión.
about] Ir sin necesidad de un lado para otro. mp-walk
Motion [intransitive verb] coloq. Andar de calle
Callejear.
en calle o de casa en casa.
costear [to sail [transitive verb] Navegar sin perder de
[transitive verb] Ir navegando sin perder
along the coast; vista la costa o la costa de cierto sitio
Transl Ground (coast) 1 de vista la costa.Ir por el costado o lado
to move along the que se expresa. Ir por el costado de
de algo, bordearlo.
edge of] una cosa.
De cruz. Del lat. crux, crucis. [transitive
pass/cross [transitive verb] Atravesar un camino,
verb] Atravesar un camino, un campo,
cruzar [to cross] Transl Path 1 G campo, calle, etc., pasando de una
una calle, etc., pasando de una parte a
(traversal) parte a otra.
otra.
Animal-like
culebrear [to [intransitive verb] Moverse o andar
→ Path- [intransitive verb] Andar formando eses
move like a snake, Transl Manner 1 haciendo eses como la culebra. mp-walk
shape y pasándose de un lado a otro.
to zigzag] Serpentear, zigzaguear.
(zigzag)
(del lat. «curvare») [transitive verb]
curvar (se) [to Posture [transitive verb] encorvar (doblar y
Dar a una cosa forma curva. Arquear,
(cause to) curve, Self-c AM Manner 1 (Bend, Twist, torcer algo poniéndolo corvo). U. t. c.
combar, doblar, enarcar, encorvar.
to bend] Curve) prn [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
[pronominal verb] Tomar forma curva.
403
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(el sup. lat. «derupare», deriv. de
«rupes», roca) [transitive verb]
Derribar o hundir un edificio o [transitive verb] Precipitar, despeñar. U.
Earth-based construcción. [transitive verb] Hacer t. c. prn [i.e., also used as pronominal
derrumbar (se) Transl AM Path 1 orientation: caer una cosa desde una roca o por una verb]. [transitive verb] Derribar, demoler
downwards pendiente escarpada. Arrojar, una construcción o parte de ella. U. t. c.
despeñar, precipitar. [pronominal prn [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
verb] Caer o hundirse una
construcción.
(del lat. «deambulare») [intransitive
mp-walk; No
deambular [to verb]Andar sin objetivo determinado; [intransitive verb] Andar, caminar sin
Transl Manner 1 aim in mp-walk
walk around] no se aplica a distancias muy grandes. dirección determinada.
Motion
Vagar. Callejear.
[transitive verb] Disminuir la velocidad.
desacelerar [to Rate- (de «des-» y «acelerar») [transitive mp-walk /
Transl Manner 1 U. t. c. [i.e., also used as intransitive
slow down] decrease verb] Reducir la aceleración. mp-run
verb]
[intransitive verb] Recorrer en
desandar [to
back to G; dirección contraria el mismo camino
walk back to a Transl Path + Manner 2 mp-walk [transitive verb] retroceder (volver atrás) mp-walk
backwards recorrido antes. Retroceder. Desandar
previous path]
lo andado.
del lat. «descendere») («de, desde,
por, hacia, hasta») [intransitive verb]
descender [to Earth-based [intransitive verb] bajar (ir desde un
Ir de un sitio a otro más bajo. Bajar.
(cause to) go Transl AM Path 1 orientation: lugar a otro más bajo). [transitive verb]
[transitive verb] Poner una cosa baja o
down] downwards bajar (poner bajo).
más baja de lo que estaba, o llevar una
cosa de un sitio a otro más bajo.
[intransitive verb] Salir de una
[transitive verb] Descargar las cosas
desembarcar [to embarcación. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used
Path + Ground que van en un barco.(«de, en»)
(cause to) Transl AM 2 out of G as pronominal verb]. [transitive verb]
(ship) [intransitive verb] Salir de una
disembark] Sacar de la nave y poner en tierra lo
embarcación.
embarcado.
desenroscar (se) [transitive verb and pronominal verb] [transitive verb] Extender lo que está
Posture
[to (cause to) Self-c AM Manner 1 Extender[se] lo que está enroscado. enroscado. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
(Unroll)
uncoil] Desenrollar. pronominal verb]
404
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
away from
(del lat. «desertare») [intransitive
desertar [to G [intransitive verb] Dicho de un soldado:
Transl Path 1 verb] («de») Abandonar un soldado
desert] (departure, Desamparar, abandonar sus banderas.
su puesto.
source)
(de «des-» y «fila») [intransitive verb]
Marchar en fila. Pasar una formación [intransitive verb] Dicho de varias
militar, por ejemplo en una personas: Marchar en fila. [intransitive
desfilar [to walk
Transl Manner 1 Motion in file solemnidad, por delante de un superior verb] Mil. Dicho de las tropas: En mp-walk
in file, to parade]
o de algún personaje. Pasar un ciertas solemnidades, marchar en
conjunto de personas, por ejemplo una formación ante alguna autoridad.
manifestación, por algún sitio.
Contact
[transitive verb] Arrastrar algo con
[between F [transitive verb and pronominal verb]
deslizar (se) [to suavidad por una superficie. U. m. c. mp-walk /
Transl AM Manner 1 and G]; Mover[se] o pasar una cosa sobre otra
(cause to) slide] [i.e., most commonly used as mp-run
Smooth suavemente.
pronominal verb]
Motion
(de «des-» y «peña») [transitive verb] [transitive verb] Precipitar y arrojar a
despeñar(se) [to
Arrojar una cosa desde lo alto de una alguien o algo desde un lugar alto y
(cause to) fall Path + Ground down from
Transl AM 2 roca o un precipicio. Precipitar. peñascoso, o desde una prominencia
down from a (rock) G
[pronominal verb] Caer por un aunque no tenga peñascos. U. t. c. prn
rock]
despeñadero. [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
[pronominal verb] Dicho especialmente
(de «des-» y «plomo».) (del lat.
de una pared o de un edificio: Caerse,
«plumbum») [pronominal verb] Caer
desplomar (se) Earth-based perder la posición vertical. [pronominal
pesadamente una cosa cualquiera por
[to (cause to) Transl AM Path 1 orientation: verb] Dicho de una persona: Caerse sin
cualquier causa. Particularmente, caer
collapse] downwards vida o sin conocimiento. [transitive
pesadamente una persona al perder el
verb] Hacer que una pared, un edificio u
sentido o quedar muerta.
otra cosa pierda la posición vertical.
[transitive verb and pronominal verb]
desviar (se) [to [transitive verb] Apartar, alejar a alguien
Change (del lat. «deviare») Apartar[se] del
divert; to be Transl AM Path 1 o algo del camino que seguía. U. t. c. prn
direction camino o dirección seguidos o de su
diverted] [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
destino, en sentido material o figurado
405
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
elevar (se) [to Earth-based [transitive verb] (del lat. «elevare») [transitive verb] levantar (mover hacia
(cause to) move Transl AM Path 1 orientation: Llevar una cosa desde un sitio a otro arriba). U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
upwards] upwards más alto. [pronominal verb] Subir. pronominal verb]
406
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[transitive verb] Introducir personas,
embarcar (se) [to [intransitive verb and pronominal mercancías, etc., en una embarcación,
Path + Ground on/onto/into
(cause to) go on Transl AM 2 verb] Meterse en un barco, tren o tren o avión. U. t. c. [i.e., also used as
(ship, plane, train) G
board] avión para viajar en ellos. intransitive verb and as pronominal
verb]
away from [intransitive verb] (del lat. [intransitive verb] Dicho de una persona,
emigrar [to Path + Ground G «emigrare») Marcharse una persona de una familia o de un pueblo: Dejar o
Transl 2
emigrate] (country) (departure, de su pueblo, región o país para abandonar su propio país con ánimo de
source) establecerse en otro. establecerse en otro extranjero.
407
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
encerrar (de «en-» y «cerrar») (del
sup. lat. vg. «serräre», del lat.
encerrar (se) [to «seräre», cerrar, con influencia de [transitive verb] Meter a una persona o a
put something, «serräre», serrar) [transitive verb] un animal en lugar del que no pueda
someone or Transl AM Path 1 into G Guardar una cosa en un sitio cerrado. salir. [pronominal verb] Retirarse del
oneself into an Meter a alguien en sitio de donde no mundo, recogerse en una clausura o
enclosed place] puede salir o escaparse. [pronominal religión.
verb] Meterse alguien en un lugar y no
querer salir de ahí.
(del lat. «incurvare») [transitive verb] Low
encorvar (se) [to Posture [transitive verb] Doblar y torcer algo
Hacer tomar forma curva a una cosa. ratings. No
(cause to) bend, Self-c AM Manner 1 (Bend, Twist, poniéndolo corvo. U. t. c. prnl. [i.e., also
Curvar. [pronominal verb] Tomar significant
to curve] Curve) used as pronominal verb]
forma curva una cosa. difference
408
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «inträre») [intransitive verb] [intransitive verb] Ir o pasar de fuera
entrar [to enter] Transl Path into G («a, en, por») Pasar al interior de una adentro. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
cosa. pronominal verb]
erguir (se) [to (del lat. «erigere») [transitive verb] [transitive verb] Levantar y poner
Posture
(cause to) Poner una cosa en posición vertical y derecho algo, especialmente el cuello o
Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical-
straighten, stand derecha. [pronominal verb] Poner la cabeza. [pronominal verb] Levantarse
upwards)
up] erguido el propio cuerpo. o ponerse derecho.
(del lat. «errare», vagabundear)
mp-walk; No
errar [to wander [intransitive verb] Andar sin destino u [intransitive verb] Andar vagando de una
Transl Manner 1 aim in mp-walk
around] objetivo fijo y sin tener residencia fija. parte a otra.
Motion
Vagar
[pronominal verb and intransitive
verb] («de, de entre, por entre») [pronominal verb] Dicho de una cosa:
away from
Escaparse algo o alguien Irse o escaparse de entre las manos.
escabullirse [to G Furtive
Transl Path + Manner 2 escurriéndose de entre las manos de [pronominal verb] Dicho de una
slip away] (departure, Motion
quien lo sujeta. Deslizarse. persona: Apartarse, sin que de momento
source)
Descabullirse, escullirse. Marcharse se note, de la compañía en que estaba.
de un sitio disimuladamente
Del lat. scalaris [transitive verb] Subir,
escalar [to scale,
Transl Path 1 up/onto G [transitive verb] Subir a algún sitio. trepar por una gran pendiente o a una
to climb]
gran altura.
away from (del lat. «ex», fuera, y «cappa», capa) [intransitive verb] Salir de un encierro o
escapar (se) [to G [intransitive verb and pronominal un peligro. [intransitive verb] Salir,
Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast
escape] (departure, verb] Librarse, huyendo, de una huir. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
source) sujeción o encierro. pronominal verb]
[transitive verb] Acompañar algo o a [transitive verb] Resguardar, conducir
escoltar [to Figure + CO-
Transl 2 alguien que va de un sitio a otro, para algo o a alguien para que llegue con
escort] MOTION
protegerlo. seguridad a su destino.
409
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «spargere») [transitive verb]
esparcir (se) [to Multiple Arrojar o enviar cosas en distintas
[transitive verb] Extender lo que está
(cause to) move in directions direcciones; [pronominal verb]
Transl AM Path 1 junto o amontonado. U. t. c. prn [i.e.,
different from a Extenderse lo que estaba junto o
also used as pronominal verb]
directions] unique start amontonado. Desparramar[se],
diseminar[se]
[intransitive verb] Realizar un sprint.
[intransitive verb] Realizar un esprint. NOUN: (Voz inglesa). Aceleración que
esprintar [to NOUN: Esfuerzo momentáneo en una realiza un corredor en un tramo
Transl Manner 1 mp-run mp-run
sprint] carrera, especialmente al final de ella, determinado de la carrera, especialmente
para lograr la victoria. en la llegada a meta para disputar la
victoria a otros corredores.
Vehicle /
[intransitive verb] Deslizarse sobre la
esquiar [to ski] Transl Manner 1 Instrument [intransitive verb] Patinar con esquís.
nieve o el agua con esquís.
(skis)
(del germ. «skiuhan», tener miedo)
away from [transitive verb] Procurar o conseguir
[transitive verb] Evitar, rehusar.
esquivar [to G con habilidad no hacer algo, no mp-run /
Transl Path [pronominal] Retraerse, retirarse,
avoid, to dodge] (departure, encontrarse con alguien o que no mp-walk
excusarse.
source) ocurra alguna cosa que a uno le
molestaría o le pondría en un aprieto.
[transitive verb and pronominal verb] [transitive verb] Alargar, dilatar algo,
estirar (se) [to Posture
Estirar y poner tensos los miembros extendiéndolo con fuerza para que dé de
(cause to) stretch Self-c AM Manner 1 (unspecified
para recobrar la agilidad después del sí. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
out] orientation)
sueño o una quietud prolongada. pronominal verb]
[transitive verb] («contra») Lanzar con
Arrojar con violencia algo contra otra
violencia una cosa contra un sitio duro
cosa, haciéndolo pedazos. U. t. c. prn
o dejarla caer contra el suelo,
estrellar (se) [to CONCURRENT [i.e., also used as pronominal verb].
Transl AM 1 haciéndola pedazos. Estampar.
(cause to) crash] RESULT [pronominal verb] Quedar malparado o
[pronominal verb] Tropezar o caer
matarse por efecto de un choque
violentamente contra algo duro,
violento contra una superficie dura.
causándose mucho daño.
410
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[pronominal verb] Quedar malparado o
(del lat. «ex» y «tremiscere», empezar
estremecer (se) Shake, matarse por efecto de un choque
a temblar) transitive verb] Hacer
[to (cause to ) Self-c AM Manner 1 Swing, violento contra una superficie dura.
temblar una cosa. [pronominal verb]
tremble, shiver] Vibrate [pronominal verb] Temblar con
Temblar alguien o algo.
movimiento agitado y repentino.
exiliar (se) [to [transitive verb] Obligar a alguien a
[transitive verb] Expulsar a alguien de
(cause to) leave exhiliarse. [pronominal verb]
Path + Ground un territorio. [pronominal verb]
one's country; to Transl AM 2 out of G Marcharse alguien de su patria
(country) Expatriarse, generalmente por motivos
exile, to go into obligado por las persecuciones
políticos.
exile] políticas u otra circunstancia.
411
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
away from
[pronominal verb] Escaparse o huir
fugarse [to flee, G
Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast alguien de un sitio en donde está [pronominal verb] Escaparse, huir. mp-run
to run away] (departure,
encerrado, sujeto o vigilado.
source)
(del fr. «galoper») [intransitive verb]
Ir a galope. galope (de «galopar») m.
galopar1 [to mp-run; Equit. Marcha, la más veloz del [intransitive verb] Dicho de una
Transl Manner 1 mp-run
gallop] Rate-fast caballo, en que éste avanza dando caballería: Ir a galope.
saltos y apoyándose sucesivamente en
las patas traseras y delanteras.
Vehicle /
galopar2 [to ride
Instrument [intransitive verb] Cabalgar en caballo
a galloping Transl Manner 1 NO FOUND IN DUE
(horse); Rate- que va a galope.
horse]
fast
mp-walk;
gatear1 [to
Transl Manner 1 Use of one's [intransitive verb] Andar a gatas. [intransitive verb] coloq. Andar a gatas. mp-walk
crawl]
hands
Animal-like
[intransitive verb] Trepar como los
→ Use of
gatear2 [to climb up/onto G; [intransitive verb] Trepar valiéndose gatos, y especialmente subir por un
Transl Path + Manner 2 one's hands
like a cat] upwards de los pies o las rodillas y las manos. tronco o astil valiéndose de los brazos y
and legs
piernas.
(clamber)
(del lat. «gyrare») [intransitive verb]
girar1 [to turn, to
Change Cambiar de dirección una calle o [intransitive verb] Desviarse o cambiar
change one's Transl Path 1
direction camino. Cambiar una persona o con respecto a la dirección inicial.
direction]
vehículo su dirección inicial.
(del lat. «gyrare») [intransitive verb] [transitive verb] Mover una figura o un
Moverse un cuerpo de modo que objeto alrededor de un punto o de un eje.
girar2 [to (cause Revolve,
Self-c AM Manner 1 describe circunferencias alrededor de [intransitive verb] Dicho de una cosa:
to) rotate, spin] Rotate, Spin
otro; [transitive verb] Mover Dar vueltas sobre un eje o en torno a un
circularmente algo. punto.
[transitive verb] Ir delante de otros o
guiar [to guide, Figure + CO- [transitive verb] Ir delante mostrando el
Transl 2 con otros, mostrándoles el camino que
to lead the way] MOTION camino.
deben seguir.
412
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
Vehicle /
hamacar (se) - Instrument
[transitive verb] Am. Mecer, columpiar,
hamaquear (se) (hammock); [transitive verb and pronominal verb]
Self-c AM Manner 1 especialmente en hamaca. U. t. c. prn
[to rock (oneself) Shake, Mecer[se] en hamaca.
[i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
on a hammock] Swing,
Vibrate.
(del lat. «fugire», por «fugore»)
away from [intransitive verb] Alejarse deprisa, por
[intransitive verb and pronominal
huir [to flee, to G miedo o por otro motivo, de personas,
Transl Path + Manner 2 Rate-fast verb] («a, de») Marcharse mp-run
run away] (departure, animales o cosas, para evitar un daño,
precipitadamente de un sitio por
source) disgusto o molestia.
temor.
Earth-based (del lat. «fundere», arruinar)
hundir (se) [to orientation: [transitive verb] Hacer caer un edificio [transitive verb] Sumir, meter en lo
(cause to) Transl AM Path 1 downwards; o construcción o parte de ellos. hondo. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
collapse, to sink] down [pronominal verb] Convertirse en pronominal verb]
from/to G ruinas.
[transitive verb] Apartar algo de su
(del lat. «inclinare») [transitive verb posición perpendicular a otra cosa o al
inclinar (se) [to Posture
and pronominal verb] Separar[se] una horizonte. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
(cause to) incline, Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical-
cosa de la posición horizontal o de la pronominal verb]. [pronominal verb]
bend, bow) downwards)
vertical. Bajar el tronco y la cabeza hacia
adelante.
[intransitive verb] Dicho del natural de
(del lat. «immigrare») [intransitive
un país: Llegar a otro para establecerse
inmigrar [to Path + Ground verb] Llegar a un territorio para
Transl 2 into G en él, especialmente con idea de formar
immigrate] (country) establecerse en él. Se aplica también a
nuevas colonias o domiciliarse en las ya
los animales. Emigrar.
formadas.
(del lat. «invadere») [transitive
verb]Entrar en un sitio por la fuerza o
invadir [to Violent [transitive verb] Irrumpir, entrar por la
Transl Path + Manner 2 into G la violencia o contra la voluntad de los
invade] Motion fuerza.
que lo ocupan; particularmente, como
acción de guerra.
413
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «ire») [intransitive verb] («a, [intransitive verb] Moverse de un lugar
hacia, para, hasta, contra») Moverse hacia otro apartado de quien usa el verbo
Deixis:
hacia un sitio que se expresa. («en, ir y de quien ejecuta el
IR(se) [to go] Transl Path 1 away from
entre, por, sobre») A veces no se movimiento.Caminar de acá para allá. U.
G (speaker)
expresa el lugar ni la dirección, sino t. c. prn [i.e., also used as pronominal
alguna circunstancia del movimiento. verb]
Vehicle /
jinetear [to ride a
Instrument [intransitive verb] Andar a caballo,
horse in a [intransitive verb] Presumir montando
Transl Manner 1 (horse); State principalmente por los sitios públicos,
showing off a caballo.
of the Figure alardeando de gala y primor.
manner]
(proud)
414
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
away from [pronominal verb] Marcharse de un
[pronominal verb] coloq. Dicho de una
largarse [to G sitio brusca o precipitadamente, por
Transl Path 1 persona: Irse o ausentarse con presteza o
leave] (departure, eludir alguna cosa o por estar a
disimulo.
source) disgusto en él.
(de «levante», part. activo de «levar»)
[transitive verb] Poner de pie o en [transitive verb] Poner derecha o en
levantar (se)1 [to Posture
posición vertical algo que estaba posición vertical la persona o cosa que
(cause to) stand Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical-
tumbado o inclinado. [pronominal esté inclinada, tendida, etc. U. t. c. prn
up] upwards)
verb] Ponerse de pie cuando se está [i.e., also used as pronominal verb].
sentado o tumbado.
(de «levante», part. activo de «levar»)
[transitive verb] Mover hacia arriba
(del lat. «leväre»). [transitive verb]
Earth-based algo. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
levantar (se)2 [to Separar una cosa del sitio donde está y
Transl AM Path 1 orientation: pronominal verb] Poner algo en lugar
lift, to raise] llevarla más arriba. [pronominal verb]
upwards más alto que el que tenía. U. t. c. prn
separarse algo del sitio donde está e
[i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
irse más arriba.
[transitive verb] Andar por un terreno
llanear [to move,
Ground (flat llano, evitando las pendientes. En [intransitive verb] Andar por lo llano,
walk or cycle on a Transl 1
terrain) ciclismo, correr con facilidad en el evitando pendientes.
flat terrain]
llano.
mp-walk;
Characteristic del fr. «marcher»[intransitive]; Mil. [intransitive verb] Mil. Dicho de la
marchar [to
Transl Manner 1 use of lower Andar los soldados en formación y tropa: Ir o caminar con cierto orden y
march]
body (regular marcando el paso. compás.
steps)
415
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] Moverse
avanzando con los pies. Andar,
away from caminar. Moverse de un lugar a otro
[intransitive verb] andar. Irse o partir de
marcharse [to go G de cualquier manera. [intransitive verb
Transl Path 1 un lugar. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
away] (departure, and pronominal verb] Dejar, por
pronominal verb].
source) decisión o movimiento propio, de estar
en un sitio, determinado o no,
expresando o no la llegada a otro
[transitive verb and pronominal verb]
Mover[se] una cosa repetidamente de [transitive verb] Mover algo
mecer (se) [to Shake, un lado al opuesto, manteniendo fijo compasadamente de un lado a otro sin
(cause to) swing, Self-c AM Manner 1 Swing, algún punto de ella o estando que mude de lugar, como la cuna de los
rock] Vibrate suspendida o sostenida sobre un pie u niños. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
objeto oscilante; como una cuna, un pronominal verb]
columpio o a un niño en su cuna.
del antig. «manear», de «mano», con
influencia del antig. «menar»,
menear (se) [to conducir; [transitive verb] Cambiar la [transitive verb] Mover algo de una parte
(Self-contained
(cause to) move] Self-c AM posición o la situación de una cosa. a otra. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
Motion)
(moverse) Mover. En particular, mover el cuerpo pronominal verb]
o parte de él. [pronominal verb]
Moverse.
416
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
del lat. «movere» [transitive verb]
[transitive verb] Hacer que un cuerpo
Cambiar la situación o posición de una
mover (se) [to deje el lugar o espacio que ocupa y pase
Self-c cosa. [pronominal verb] Cambiar de
(cause to) move, a ocupar otro. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used
+ AM (Motion) posición una cosa. [pronominal verb
change one's as pronominal verb]. [transitive verb]
Transl and intransitive verb] Iniciar la acción
location] Menear o agitar una cosa o parte de
de marcharse de un sitio levantándose
algún cuerpo.
del asiento o de otro modo.
mudarse [to go [pronominal verb and intransitive [pronominal verb] coloq. Dicho de una
from one place to Transl (Motion) verb] («de») Irse o pasar de un sitio a persona: Irse del lugar, sitio o
another] otro. concurrencia en que estaba.
[intransitive verb] Dicho de una persona
(del lat. «natare») [intransitive verb] o de un animal: Trasladarse en el agua,
Manner + Ground
nadar [to swim] Transl 2 mp-swim Moverse sobre el agua o sumergido en ayudándose de los movimientos
(water)
ella sin tocar el fondo. necesarios, y sin tocar el suelo ni otro
apoyo.
(del lat. «naufragare») [intransitive
[intransitive verb] Dicho de una
naufragar [(of a verb] Hundirse un barco en el agua
Path + Ground down into embarcación: Irse a pique o perderse.
ship, or of people Transl 2 por accidente. Dicho de una persona:
(sea) G Dicho de una persona: Sufrir el
in a ship) to sink] Sufrir el naufragio del barco en que
naufragio del barco en que viaja.
viaja.
[intransitive verb] Viajar en un buque o
en otra embarcación, generalmente por
Vehicle / (del lat. «navigare») [intransitive
navegar [to mar. U. t. c. tr [i.e., also used as
Instrument verb] Ir un vehículo, o ir en un
navigate (on the Transl Manner 1 transitive verb] Hacer viaje o ir por el
(ship, vehículo, por el agua. Conducir un
sea, on the air)] aire en globo, avión u otro vehículo
airplane) vehículo, o ir en él, por el aire. Avión.
adecuado. Dicho de un buque o de otra
embarcación: avanzar.
mp-walk; No
noctambular [to
aim in [intransitive verb] noctambular. Vagar [intransitive verb] Andar vagando de
walk around at Transl Manner 1 mp-walk
Motion; de noche. noche.
night]
Night time
[intransitive verb] Dicho de un cuerpo
[intransitive verb] Formar ondas
ondear [to Shake, flexible: Moverse formando ondas.
movibles una superficie; por ejemplo,
undulate, to Self-c Manner 1 Swing, [transitive verb]∫ Mover algo,
el agua o una bandera movida por el
sway] Vibrate especialmente una bandera o un pañuelo,
viento. Balancearse en el aire.
formando ondas.
417
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «oscillare») [intransitive
[intransitive verb] Efectuar movimientos
oscilar [to Shake, verb] Moverse a un lado y a otro una
de vaivén a la manera de un péndulo o
oscillate, to Self-c Manner 1 Swing, cosa que está suspendida, que está
de un cuerpo colgado de un resorte o
swing] Vibrate sujeta o apoyada por un solo punto o
movido por él.
que no está suficientemente sujeta.
away from
(del lat. «partïre») [intransitive verb]
G [intransitive verb] Empezar a caminar,
partir [to leave] Transl Path 1 Marcharse de un sitio para ir a otro
(departure, ponerse en camino.
distante.
source)
(del lat. «passare») [intransitive and
transitive verb] El significado básico
de este verbo es el de ir de un sitio a
otro. En segundo lugar, esta acción
puede implicar atravesar por dentro, [transitive verb] Llevar, conducir de un
por encima o por al lado de una cosa. lugar a otro. [transitive verb] Mudar,
pasar [to go pass G
En tercer lugar, la acción de pasar trasladar a otro lugar, situación o clase.
through, over, Transl Path 1 (traversal,
puede llevar consigo dejar atrás una U. t. c. [i.e., also used as intransitive
along,…] milestone)
cosa yendo más allá de ella, verb and as pronominal verb]. [transitive
rebasándola, superándola o verb] Cruzar de una parte a otra.
venciéndola. En cuarto lugar, el objeto
de interés puede ser el lugar o la cosa
por encima de la cual, por delante de
la cual, etc., se realiza el tránsito.
[intransitive verb and pronominal [intransitive verb] Ir andando por
mp-walk;
pasear [to walk verb] de «paso» Andar por placer o distracción o por ejercicio. U. t. c. tr y
Transl Manner 1 Leisurely
for pleasure] por hacer ejercicio, particularmente al
prn [i.e., also used as transitive verb and
Motion
aire libre. as pronominal verb]
[intransitive verb] Mover las piernas o
patalear [to
Iterative patas violentamente y con ligereza, para
stamp one's feet
Figure (body part: Motion; State [intransitive verb] Dar golpes con los herir con ellas o en fuerza de un
on the ground Self-c 2
feet) + Manner of Figure pies en el suelo por enfado. accidente o dolor. [intransitive verb]
showing one is
(angry) Dar patadas en el suelo violentamente y
angry]
con prisa por enfado o pesar.
418
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
patear1 [to stamp Iterative [transitive verb] coloq. Dar golpes con
one's feet on the Figure (body part: Motion; State [intransitive verb] Dar patadas en el los pies. [intransitive verb] coloq. Dar
Self-c 2
ground showing feet) + Manner of Figure suelo en señal de enfado. Patalear. patadas en señal de enojo, dolor o
one is angry] (angry) desagrado.
419
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[transitive verb] Mar. Dirigir un [transitive verb] Dirigir un buque,
pilotar [(ship) to Vehicle /
barco, especialmente a la entrada de especialmente a la entrada o salida de
steer; (car) to Instrument
Transl Manner 1 un puerto o en sitios con dificultades. puertos, barras, etc. [transitive verb]
drive; (plane) to (ship,
Conducir como piloto otra cosa Dirigir un automóvil, globo, aeroplano,
fly] airplane, car)
cualquiera. etc.
away from
pirarse [to go G (de «pira» = de origen caló; inf.)
Transl Path 1 [pronominal verb] Fugarse, irse.
away] (departure, [pronominal verb] Marcharse.
source)
piruetear [to Revolve,
Self-c Manner 1 [intransitive verb] Hacer piruetas. [intransitive verb] Hacer piruetas.
pirouette] Rotate, Spin
420
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[transitive verb] del lat.
«praecipitare»; Lanzar una cosa por
precipitar (se)1 Forced
Earth-based un precipicio. Despeñar. (más usado [transitive verb] Despeñar, arrojar o
[to (cause to) fall Motion
Transl AM Path + Manner 2 orientation: con referencia a personas; «desde, derribar de un lugar alto. U. t. c. prn
down from a high (Energy-
downwards por») Lanzar una cosa desde un sitio [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
place] high)
alto. [pronominal verb] Lanzarse
desde lo alto.
421
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
recorrer [to (del lat. «recurrere») [transitive verb] [transitive verb] Atravesar un espacio o
move, go around Transl (Motion) + Ground 1 Ir sucesivamente a distintas partes de lugar en toda su extensión o longitud. mp-walk
a place] un lugar. [transitive verb] Efectuar un trayecto.
[transitive verb] Dicho de quien está de
(de «re-» y «costa», costilla)
pie o sentado: Reclinar la parte superior
[transitive verb] («en, sobre») Apoyar
del cuerpo. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
recostar (se) [to una cosa en algún sitio, en posición
Posture pronominal verb]. [transitive verb]
(cause to) lean or Self-c AM Manner 1 inclinada; por ejemplo, la cabeza o la
(horizontal) Inclinar algo sobre otra cosa. U. t. c. prn
to lie down] parte superior del cuerpo. Reclinar.
[i.e., also used as pronominal verb].
(«en, sobre») [pronominal verb]
[pronominal verb] Acostarse durante un
Ponerse en un sillón, en la cama, etc.,
breve período de tiempo.
regatear [to Motion in [transitive verb] Hacer regates; mp-walk /
Transl Manner 1 [intransitive verb] Hacer regates.
dribble] sports particularmente, en ciertos deportes. mp-run
(de «regreso») (del lat. «regressus»)
regresar [to come [intransitive verb and pronominal [intransitive verb] Volver al lugar de
Transl Path 1 back to G
back] verb] Ir de nuevo a un sitio de donde donde se partió.
se ha salido. Retornar, volver.
Vehicle /
[intransitive verb] Mover los remos [intransitive verb] Trabajar con el remo
remar [to row] Transl Manner 1 Instrument
para impulsar una embarcación. para impeler la embarcación en el agua.
(oar, paddle)
[transitive verb] Subir una pendiente,
remontar [to go Earth-based sobrepasarla. Navegar aguas arriba en
[transitive verb] Subir una cosa. Subir
up, to swim up, to Transl AM Path 1 orientation: una corriente. Subir o volar muy alto las
por cierto sitio.
soar] upwards aves, Ascender por el aire. [pronominal
verb] Subir, ir hacia arriba.
mp-walk;
Forced [intransitive verb] Andar o moverse
renquear [to [intransitive verb] Cojear como lo
Transl Manner 1 Motion; State como renco, oscilando a un lado y a otro mp-walk
limp] hace un renco.
of Figure a trompicones.
(injured)
repatriar [to (del lat. «repatriare») [transitive verb] [transitive verb] Devolver algo o a
repatriate, (cause Path + Ground Devolver a su patria a alguien que está alguien a su patria. U. t. c. [i.e., also
Transl AM 2 back to G
to) go back to (country) expatriado. [pronominal verb] Volver used as i.e., also used as]. [intransitive
one's country a su patria el que está expatriado. verb] y m. c. [pronominal verb]
422
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
mp-walk;
Animal-like [intransitive verb] Andar con el
reptar [to crawl [intransitive verb] Andar arrastrándose
Transl Manner 1 → Contact cuerpo tocando el suelo, como los
like a reptile] como algunos reptiles.
[between F reptiles.
and G]
[intransitive verb] Desplazarse
Contact involuntariamente sobre una superficie
[between F del antig. y dial. «resvarar», de «re-» y lisa o viscosa sin dejar de rozarla,
Low
and G]; «esvarar») [intransitive verb and normalmente con alteración del
resbalarse [to ratings. No
Transl Manner 1 Smooth pronominal verb] («en, por, sobre, equilibrio. Resbalan los pies sobre el
slide] significant
Motion; entre») Moverse una cosa rozando hielo. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
difference
Uncontrolled suavemente sobre una superficie. pronominal verb]. [intransitive verb]
motion? deslizarse (arrastrarse con suavidad por
una superficie).
[transitive verb] Mover una cosa para
away from que deje de estar en contacto con otra [transitive verb] Apartar o separar a
retirar (se) [to
G o próxima a otra que se expresa. alguien o algo de otra persona o cosa o
remove, to take Transl AM Path 1
(departure, [pronominal verb] Moverse alguien de un sitio. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
away; to retreat]
source) para dejar de tocar algo o alejarse de pronominal verb]
ello.
[intransitive verb] Ir o venir de nuevo
una cosa al punto de partida o a la
retornar [to [intransitive verb] Volver al lugar o a la
situación en que antes estuvo.
return, to go situación en que se estuvo. U. t. c. prn
Transl AM Path 1 back to G Regresar, tornar, volver. [transitive
back; to give [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
verb] Volver a poner algo en el sitio
back] [transitive verb] Devolver.
donde estaba o en una situación que
antes tuvo. Devolver.
[intransitive verb] Jugar, por ejemplo Low
Playful los niños o los animales jóvenes, [intransitive verb] Saltar y brincar ratings. No
retozar [to frolic] Transl Manner 1
Motion corriendo, brincando, persiguiéndose alegremente. significant
unos a otros, etc. difference
retroceder [to go del lat. «retrocedere») [intransitive
back to G /
back, to back Transl Path 1 verb] («a, de, en») Marchar hacia [intransitive verb] Volver hacia atrás.
backwards
down] atrás.
423
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] Volar alrededor de [intransitive verb] Volar haciendo
mp-fly; algo o haciendo giros en un corto tornos o giros en poco espacio. Venir
revolotear [to
Manner+ Ground Revolve, espacio. Moverse una cosa ligera, por una cosa por el aire dando vueltas.
(cause to) fly Transl AM 2
(air) Rotate, Spin ejemplo un papel, en el aire.[transitive [transitive verb] Arrojar algo a lo alto
around, to flutter]
(turn around) verb] Lanzar una cosa al aire de modo con ímpetu, de suerte que parece que da
que vaya dando vueltas. vueltas.
(del lat. «rotāre») [intransitive verb]
[intransitive verb] Dicho de un cuerpo:
Dar vueltas una cosa sobre una
Dar vueltas alrededor de un eje, sin
rodar [to roll] Self-c / Revolve, superficie, trasladándose de lugar al
Manner 1 mudar de lugar, como la piedra de un
(rular) Transl Rotate, Spin darlas; como una pelota o las ruedas
molino, o mudando, como la bola que
de un carro. [intransitive verb] Dar
corre por el suelo.
vueltas una cosa alrededor de un eje.
[transitive verb] Estar una cosa
[intransitive verb] Andar alrededor.
rodear [to alrededor de otra. Colocarse varias
[transitive verb] Poner una o varias
surround with; to Self-c AM Manner 1 Path-shape personas, animales, etc., alrededor de
cosas alrededor de otra. U. t. c. prn [i.e.,
go round] un sitio. [intransitive verb] Poner una
also used as pronominal verb]
cosa alrededor de otra.
[transitive verb] Hacer ronda de
[transitive verb] Dar vueltas alrededor de
vigilancia o salir los mozos de ronda
algo. [intransitive verb] Andar de noche
por algún sitio. Recorrer los puestos
paseando las calles. U. t. c. tr [i.e., also
rondar [to be on de vigilancia de una plaza fuerte o un
used as transitive verb]. [intransitive
patrol; to prowl Transl Manner + Cause 2 mp-walk campamento para inspeccionarlos.
verb] Andar de noche visitando una
about, around] [transitive verb] Seguir a una persona,
población para impedir los desórdenes.
acercarse a ella o hablarle con
U. t. c. tr [i.e., also used as transitive
frecuencia para conseguir de ella
verb]
cierta cosa
Revolve, (del lat. «rotāre») [intransitive verb] [intransitive verb] rodar (dar vueltas
rotar [to rotate] Self-c Manner 1
Rotate, Spin Rodar, dar vueltas alrededor de un eje. alrededor de un eje).
[transitive verb] Mover una cosa con
brusquedad, bien haciéndola dar un
sacudir (se) [to Shake, chasquido en el aire, bien a un lado y a [transitive verb] Mover violentamente
shake; to shake Self-c AM Manner 1 Swing, otro; [pronominal verb] (del lat. algo a una y otra parte. U. t. c. prn [i.e.,
oneself] Vibrate «succutere») Quitarse una cosa que se also used as pronominal verb]
lleva encima con movimientos del
cuerpo.
424
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «salire», saltar) [intransitive
verb] («a, de») Ir fuera de un sitio.
Salir de casa. [pronominal verb]
[intransitive verb] Pasar de dentro a
Marcharse una persona de cierto sitio.
salir [to exit, to fuera. U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as
Transl Path 1 out of G Marcharse un animal del sitio en que
go out of] pronominal verb]. [intransitive verb]
está encerrado. [pronominal verb]
Partir de un lugar a otro.
Marcharse una persona de cierto sitio.
Marcharse un animal del sitio en que
está encerrado.
(Del lat. saltāre, intens. de salīre).
[intransitive verb] Levantarse una cosa
con un impulso súbito, del suelo o del [intransitive verb] Alzarse con impulso
Self-c /
sitio en que está, vertical u rápido, separándose de donde se está.
saltar [to jump] Transl Manner 1 mp-jump Hyperonym
oblicuamente, para caer en el mismo [transitive verb] Salvar de un salto un
?
sitio o en otro que esté al mismo nivel espacio o distancia.
o más alto o más bajo. [transitive verb]
Saltar o pasar por encima de algo.
(del sup. lat. «sequïre», de «sequi»;
frec. con «a», aunque el complemeno [transitive verb] Ir después o detrás de
seguir [to follow] Transl Path 1 after G directo sea de cosa) [transitive verb] alguien. U. t. c. [i.e., also used as i.e.,
En una marcha, ir después o detrás de also used as]. [intransitive verb]
cierta cosa.
Del lat. sedentare, de sedens, -entis. tr. [transitive verb] Poner o colocar a
sentar (se) [to Posture Forma causativa de «sentarse». alguien en una silla, banco, etc., de
(cause to) sit Self-c AM Manner 1 (vertical- [pronominal verb] Tomar la postura manera que quede apoyado y
down] downwards) que consiste en descansar con las descansando sobre las nalgas. U. t. c. prn
nalgas apoyadas en algún sitio. [i.e., also used as pronominal verb]
serpentear [to Animal-like [intransitive verb] Deslizarse [intransitive verb] Andar, moverse o
slither, to Transl Manner 1 → Path- formando ondas. Formar ondas un extenderse, formando vueltas y tornos mp-walk
meander] shape camino, el curso de un río, etc. como la serpiente.
[transitive verb] Recorrer yendo hacia
up/onto G;
subir [to ascend, del lat. «subïre»(«a») [intransitive arriba, remontar. [transitive verb]
Earth-based
to (cause to) go Transl AM Path 1 verb] y [pronominal verb] Ir de un Trasladar a alguien o algo a lugar más
orientation:
up] sitio a otro más alto. alto que el que ocupaba. Subir a un niño
upwards
en brazos.
425
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «submergere») [transitive
verb] Introducir una cosa en agua u
sumergir (se) [to [transitive verb] Meter algo debajo del
Ground (water) + down into otro líquido, de modo que quede
(cause to) go Transl AM 2 agua o de otro líquido. U. t. c. prn [i.e.,
Path G completamente cubierta. [pronominal
down into water] also used as pronominal verb]
verb] Hundirse debajo de la superficie
del agua o de otro líquido.
Noisy [intransitive verb] Golpetear con los [intransitive verb] Pisar con fuerza o
taconear [to tap activity; tacones en el suelo al andar deprisa, brío, produciendo ruido. [intransitive
shoes (usually Self-c / DANCE; cosa que hacen particularmente las verb] En ciertos bailes, mover
Manner 1 mp-walk
when walking or Transl Vehicle / mujeres con los tacones altos. O rítmicamente los pies haciendo ruido con
dancing)] Instrument hacerlo rítmicamente en ciertos bailes. los tacones en el suelo. U. t. c. tr [i.e.,
(heels-shoes); Zapateado. also used as transitive verb]
[intransitive verb and pronominal
tambalearse [to [intransitive verb] Moverse a uno y otro
verb] Moverse algo o alguien muy
move unsteadily Unsteady lado, como si se fuese a caer. U. m. c.
Self-c Manner 1 acusadamente a un lado y a otro mp-walk
as if it is going to Motion prn [i.e., most commonly used as
manteniendo fijo algún punto, como si
fall] pronominal verb]
se fuese a caer.
(del lat. «tremulare») [intransitive
verb] («de»: ‘de frío, de miedo’;
temblar [to Shake, [intransitive verb] Agitarse con
«con»: ‘con el susto’) Aplicado a
shiver, to Self-c Manner 1 Swing, sacudidas de poca amplitud, rápidas y
personas y cosas, moverse con
tremble] Vibrate frecuentes.
sacudidas de muy poca amplitud y
muy rápidas
426
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «torquëre») [pronominal verb]
Desviarse del camino debido o de la [transitive verb] Dicho de una persona o
torcer (se)1 [to
Change marcha favorable o conveniente; de una cosa: Desviar la dirección que
turn, to change Transl Path 1
direction [intransitive verb] Cambiar de llevaba, para tomar otra. U. t. c. [i.e.,
direction]
dirección al marchar, conducir un also used as intransitive verb]
vehículo, etc.
[transitive verb] Encorvar o doblar algo.
U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as pronominal
Posture [transitive verb and pronominal verb] verb] [transitive verb] Alterar la
torcer (se)2 [to
Self-c AM Manner 1 (Bend, Twist, Poner[se] curva o formando ángulo posición recta, perpendicular o paralela
(cause to) bend]
Curve) una cosa. que algo tiene con respecto a otra cosa.
U. t. c. prn [i.e., also used as pronominal
verb]
[intransitive verb] Ir o pasar de un punto
transitar [to go [intransitive verb] Andar por una
(Motion) + a otro por vías o parajes públicos.
along a place or Transl 1 calle, camino, etc., para ir de un sitio a
Ground [intransitive verb] Viajar o caminar
way] otro
haciendo tránsitos.
427
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
Obstructed
[transitive verb] Hacer a alguien tropezar
trompicar [to Motion;
[intransitive verb] Tropezar violenta y repetidamente. [intransitive
(cause to) trip Transl AM Manner 1 Iterative
repetidamente. verb] Dar pasos tambaleantes, tumbos o
repeatedly] Motion; mp-
vaivenes.
walk?
(del sup. lat. vg. «interpediare»)
Obstructed [intransitive verb and pronominal [intransitive verb] Dicho de una persona:
tropezar [to trip] Transl Manner 1 Motion; mp- verb] («con, contra, en») Chocar Dar con los pies en un obstáculo al ir mp-jump
walk? involuntariamente con alguien o algo andando, con lo que se puede caer.
al ir andando.
(el alto al. medio «trotten», correr)
[intransitive verb] Dicho de un caballo: High
trotar1 [(of a [intransitive verb] Andar las
mp-walk; Ir al trote. [intransitive verb] coloq. ratings. No
horse, human) to Transl Manner 1 caballerías con paso ligero levantando
Rate-fast Dicho de una persona: Andar mucho o significant
trot) a la vez el pie y la mano de distinto
con celeridad. difference
lado.
trotar2 [(of a High
Vehicle /
person) to ride a [intransitive] Cabalgar sobre un [intransitive verb] Dicho de una persona: ratings. No
Transl Manner 1 Instrument
horse which is caballo que trota. Cabalgar en caballo que va al trote. significant
(horse)
trotting] difference
[intransitive verb] Dicho de una cosa:
(del lat. «vacillare») [intransitive verb]
vacilar [to move Unsteady Moverse indeterminadamente. Dicho de
Self-c Manner 1 Moverse una cosa o una persona a un
unsteadily] Motion una cosa: Estar poco firme en su estado,
lado y a otro por falta de estabilidad.
o tener riesgo de caer o arruinarse.
[transitive verb] Pasar un río u otra
pass/cross (de «vado») [transitive verb]
vadear [to wade, Path + Ground corriente de agua profunda por el vado o
Transl 2 G Atravesar un río a pie, en caballería o
to ford a river] (river) por cualquier otro sitio donde se pueda
(traversal) en un vehículo.
hacer pie.
mp-walk; No
vagabundear [to
Transl Manner 1 aim in [intransitive verb] Andar vagabundo.
wander]
Motion
mp-walk; No vagar (del lat. «vagari») [intransitive [intransitive verb] Andar por varias
vagar [to
Transl Manner 1 aim in verb] Andar sin dirección o destino partes sin determinación a sitio o lugar, mp-walk
wander]
Motion fijos o sin especial detención en ninguno.
428
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
(del lat. «venire») [intransitive verb]
(«a, de, por») Andar o moverse hacia
el lugar donde está el que habla. [intransitive verb] Dicho de una
Deixis:
Llegar al sitio donde está el que habla persona: caminar.
venir [to come] Transl Path 1 towards G
para quedarse en él. prnl. Forma Dicho de una persona o de una cosa:
(speaker)
empleada en vez de «venir» cuando al Llegar a donde está quien habla.
significado de «llegar» se añade el de
«dejar» el lugar de donde se parte.
Leisurely
[intransitive verb] Trasladarse de un [intransitive verb] Trasladarse de un
Motion;
viajar [to travel] Transl Manner 1 lugar a otro distante. Visitar diversos lugar a otro, generalmente distante, por
Vehicle /
lugares o recorrer diversos países. cualquier medio de locomoción.
Instrument?
(del lat. «vibrare») [intransitive verb]
Shake, Moverse un cuerpo o una parte de él [intransitive verb] Mec. Dicho de un
vibrar [to
Self-c Manner 1 Swing, entre dos posiciones próximas, con cuerpo elástico: Oscilar alternativamente
vibrate]
Vibrate movimiento rápido y alternativo de la en torno a su posición de equilibrio.
una a la otra.
[transitive verb] Mar. Cambiar de rumbo
quizá del sup. celtolat. «virare», con
o de bordada, pasando de una amura a
influencia del fr. y del port.
otra, de modo que el viento que daba al
virar [(of a [intransitive verb] («a, hacia»)
Change buque por un costado le dé por el
vehicle, esp. a Transl Path 1 Volverse cambiando de dirección o de
direction opuesto. U. t. c. [i.e., also used as
ship) to swerve] orientación, particularmente un
intransitive verb]. [intransitive verb]
vehículo en marcha y más
Mudar de dirección en la marcha de un
particularmente un barco
automóvil u otro vehículo semejante.
(del lat. «volare») [intransitive verb]
[intransitive verb] Ir o moverse por el
(«Echarse a») Moverse por el aire. Se
aire, sosteniéndose con las alas.
volar [to move on aplica lo mismo a las aves u otro
Transl Ground (air) 1 [intransitive verb] Elevarse en el aire y
the air, to fly] animal como a los aviones o cualquier
moverse de un punto a otro en un
cosa más pesada que el aire que se
aparato de aviación.
sostenga y mueva en él.
[transitive verb] Sumergir una cosa
zambullir (se) [to Forced bruscamente o sin pensarlo en un
[transitive verb] Meter debajo del agua
(cause to) go Path + Ground down into Motion líquido (o en un recipiente que lo
Transl AM 3 con ímpetu o de golpe. U. t. c. prn [i.e.,
down into water (water) + Manner G (Energy- contenga). [pronominal verb]
also used as pronominal verb]
in a violent way] high) Sumergirse alguien en el agua
bruscamente.
429
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
mp-walk;
zancajear [to
Iterative [intransitive verb] Andar mucho de una
walk repeatedly [intransitive verb] Andar mucho,
Transl Manner 1 motion from parte a otra, por lo común mp-walk
from one place to yendo de un lado para otro.
one place to aceleradamente.
another]
another
mp-walk;
zanquear1 [to Characteristic
[intransitive verb] Torcer las piernas [intransitive verb] Torcer las piernas al
walk with crooked Transl Manner 1 use of lower mp-walk
al andar. andar.
legs] body (Legs-
crooked)
mp-walk;
[intransitive verb] Andar a grandes
Characteristic
zanquear2 [to pasos. Ir a grandes pasos o con prisa [intransitive verb] Andar mucho a pie y
Transl Manner 1 use of lower
stride] de un lado para otro, por ejemplo con prisa de una parte a otra.
body (Steps-
haciendo gestiones.
long)
zaparrastrar [to
[intransitive verb] coloq. Llevar
move or to walk Trailing [intransitive verb] Ir arrastrando los
Transl Manner 1 arrastrando los vestidos de modo que se mp-walk
dragging one's clothes vestidos.
ensucien. Ir zaparrastrando.
clothing]
Noisy [transitive verb] Golpear el suelo con [transitive verb] Golpear con el zapato.
zapatear [to tap activity; los pies calzados. Pisar. Hacerlo así en [transitive verb] Acompañar al tañido
shoes (usually Self-c / DANCE; el baile, lo cual, realizado con los dando palmadas y alternativamente con mp-walk /
Manner 1
when walking or Transl Vehicle / tacones y con ritmo muy vivo, las manos en los pies, siguiendo el mp-jump
dancing) Instrument constituye un paso de ciertas danzas mismo compás, especialmente en el
(shoes); españolas. baile del villano.
430
APPENDIX 2. MOTION VERBS IN SPANISH
Motor
Spanish Motion Semantic Path Manner pattern
Motion AM # Dictionary Definition (DUE) Dictionary Definition (DRAE)
Verb Components details details Rating
Study
[intransitive verb] (Del it. ant. sarpare,
este de serpe, espacio de la proa donde
away from se ponía el ancla al zarpar, y este del lat.
(del it. antig. «sarpare») [intransitive
zarpar [(of a G serpens, serpiente, por los maderos en
Transl Path 1 verb] Marcharse un barco del sitio en
ship) to set off] (departure, forma de serpentina que delimitaban ese
que está anclado.
source) espacio). Dicho de un barco o de un
conjunto de ellos: Salir del lugar en que
estaban fondeados o atracados.
431
APPENDIX 3
ENGLISH:
What is TO MOVE? In other words, which are the features that define the action of
MOVING?
What is TO WALK? In other words, which are the features that define the action of
WALKING?
What is TO RUN? In other words, which are the features that define the action of
RUNNING?
What is TO JUMP? In other words, which are the features that define the action of
JUMPING?
SPANISH:
¿Qué es MOVERSE? Es decir, ¿cuáles son las características que definen la acción
de MOVERSE?
¿Qué es ANDAR? Es decir, ¿cuáles son las características que definen la acción de
ANDAR?
¿Qué es CORRER? Es decir, ¿cuáles son las características que definen la acción de
CORRER?
¿Qué es SALTAR? Es decir, ¿cuáles son las características que definen la acción de
SALTAR?
1
Recall that participants were given only one of the four questions.
433
APPENDIX 3. INSTRUCTIONS FOR EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES
would like to know how your language expresses the ways in which people move.
You will be asked to answer a question by providing a list of verbs on the back of
this page.
You will have 1 minute to perform this task. I will let you know when to start and
when to stop.
2
Recall that participants were given only one of the following questions: In what ways can a human
move? In what ways can a human walk? In what ways can a human run? In what ways can a human
jump?
434
APPENDIX 3. INSTRUCTIONS FOR EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES
distintos. Nos gustaría saber como tu idioma expresa las maneras en las que la gente
Tendrás un minuto para hacer esta tarea. Te diremos cuando comenzar y cuando
terminar.
3
Recall that participants were given one of the following four questions: ¿De qué modos puede una
persona moverse? ¿De qué modos puede una persona andar?¿De qué modos puede una persona
correr?¿De qué modos puede una persona saltar?
435
APPENDIX 3. INSTRUCTIONS FOR EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES
This booklet contains a list of questions grouped into three blocks. Each question is
followed by a rating scale. Your task is to answer the questions by marking an X on
the scale.
For example:
X
Definitely Not sure Definitely not
a way a way a way
X
Definitely Not sure Definitely not
a way a way a way
There are no right or wrong answers, we are just interested in your judgements.
Please answer each question in turn, and do not skip any questions, or look ahead in
the booklet.
If you have any questions or concerns, please ask the experimenter now.
4
These instructions correspond to the way of question. Recall that half of the English participants got
the way of question and half the kind of question. The instructions were the same except for the use of
kind of instead of way of.
436
APPENDIX 3. INSTRUCTIONS FOR EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES
Este cuadernillo contiene una serie de preguntas divididas en tres bloques. Cada
pregunta va seguida de una escala. Tienes que responder cada pregunta escribiendo
una X en la escala.
Por ejemplo:
X
Absolutamente No es seguro Definitivamente
es una manera una manera no es una manera
X
Absolutamente No es seguro Definitivamente
es una manera una manera no es una manera
No hay respuesta correcta o errónea, simplemente nos interesa tu opinión. Por favor,
ve respondiendo cada pregunta sin saltarte ninguna y sin mirar más adelante.
5
Recall that half of the Spanish participants got the manera de question and half the tipo de question.
437
APPENDIX 4
439
APPENDIX 5
441
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
442
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
443
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
444
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
445
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
446
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
acechar F(2, 51) = 11.550 walk>run >jump 50,78 40,50 9,55 16,50 25,77 6,07 6,72 14,07 3,32
acelerar F(2, 51) = 25.448 run>walk>jump 48,11 36,21 8,53 84,44 25,15 5,93 13,44 27,04 6,37
acompañar F(2, 51) = 14.515 walk>run >jump 49,11 41,67 9,82 13,39 19,01 4,48 3,56 6,31 1,49
aligerar F(2, 51) = 21.930 run>walk>jump 70,56 37,36 8,80 82,83 27,05 6,38 18,39 27,50 6,48
ambular F(2, 51) = 28.089 walk>run >jump 78,39 31,07 7,32 36,22 34,34 8,09 8,72 17,00 4,01
anadear F(2, 51) = 11.915 walk>run >jump 55,78 34,83 8,21 27,56 29,66 6,99 10,00 18,01 4,24
apresurarse F(2, 51) = 23.588 run>walk>jump 56,33 35,09 8,27 73,22 36,92 8,70 6,39 13,07 3,08 (walk-run) t(17) = -2.350, p = 0.031
arrastrar (se) F(2, 51) = 10.321 walk>run >jump 43,72 38,57 9,09 17,72 28,67 6,76 2,06 2,84 0,67
brincar F(2, 51) = 35.810 jump>walk>run 44,44 37,09 8,74 26,39 29,08 6,85 100,17 2,98 0,70
callejear F(2, 51) = 13.232 walk>run >jump 69,50 39,20 9,24 34,67 38,80 9,14 10,22 24,08 5,68
caminar F(2, 51) = 40.613 walk>run >jump 94,89 19,72 4,65 28,83 33,82 7,97 16,06 29,12 6,86
chancletear F(2, 51) = 31.596 walk>jump>run 79,39 25,44 6,00 13,56 25,71 6,06 19,94 30,73 7,24
contonearse F(2, 51) = 22.511 walk>run=jump 65,50 33,91 7,99 13,22 20,74 4,89 13,17 24,65 5,81
contramarchar F(2, 51) = 11.554 walk>run>jump 60,11 36,31 8,56 37,39 30,17 7,11 12,00 21,87 5,16
corretear F(2, 51) = 30.608 run-walk>jump 81,22 30,43 7,17 91,83 16,67 3,93 24,72 33,05 7,79 (walk-run) t(17) = -1.195, p = 0.249
culebrear F(2, 51) = 8.390 walk>run>jump 54,50 40,40 9,52 32,72 34,19 8,06 10,33 18,37 4,33
deambular F(2, 51) = 91.800 walk>run>jump 91,61 14,14 3,33 22,44 29,20 6,88 5,72 12,93 3,05
desacelerar F(2, 51) = 5.961 walk-run>jump 37,89 33,27 7,84 24,94 32,86 7,74 5,89 12,70 2,99 (walk- run) t (17) = 1.180, p = 20.254
desandar F(2, 51) = 21.408 walk>run-jump 56,94 41,10 9,69 9,22 14,59 3,44 5,72 12,77 3,01
desfilar F(2, 51) = 116.049 walk>run-jump 92,72 15,82 3,73 18,11 23,11 5,45 8,17 14,48 3,41
deslizarse F(2, 51) = 5.074 walk-run > jump 45,56 39,30 9,26 43,17 39,18 9,24 10,67 22,71 5,35 (walk- run) t (17) = 0.203, p = 0.842
447
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
enconvarse F(2, 51) = 1.510, P = 0.231 13,17 24,32 5,73 8,28 15,55 3,66 3,28 6,44 1,52
errar F(2, 51) = 11.649 walk>run>jump 50,06 43,92 10,35 18,50 28,94 6,82 1,89 2,91 0,69
esprintar F(2, 51) = 17.164 run>walk>jump 55,28 37,24 8,78 89,00 27,09 6,39 24,28 34,30 8,08
esquivar F(2, 51) = 3.622 run-walk>jump 47,50 34,45 8,12 49,94 30,73 7,24 24,00 30,47 7,18 (walk- run) t (17) = -0.287, p = 0.778
fugarse F(2, 51) = 13.945 run>walk>jump 33,28 37,05 8,73 69,11 37,95 8,94 10,61 23,58 5,56
galopar F(2, 51) = 4.779 run >walk-jump 61,44 38,14 8,99 92,00 15,70 3,70 59,06 45,95 10,83
gatear F(2, 51) = 21.320 walk>run>jump 68,28 39,24 9,25 21,72 32,57 7,68 4,56 12,35 2,91
huir F(2, 51) = 12.287 run>walk>jump 45,17 40,36 9,51 69,11 40,04 9,44 12,06 19,38 4,57
merodear F(2, 51) = 40.798 walk>run-jump 78,33 33,80 7,97 14,83 23,27 5,49 8,56 16,99 4,00
noctambular F(2, 51) = 17.052 walk>run-jump 60,89 42,42 10,00 9,00 17,34 4,09 10,94 25,29 5,96
patear F(2, 51) = 10.436 walk>run>jump 64,28 39,80 9,38 26,83 35,26 8,31 14,22 25,90 6,11
pavonearse F(2, 51) = 7.966 walk>run>jump 45,00 43,25 10,19 14,94 21,31 5,02 8,28 16,43 3,87
pisar F(2, 51) = 1.661, p = 0.2 30,50 35,44 8,35 12,94 24,59 5,80 17,56 28,86 6,80
pisotear F(2, 51) = 1.415, p = 0.252 26,22 24,61 5,80 12,17 18,52 4,36 19,89 30,71 7,24
precipitarse F(2, 51) = 3.190, p = 0.05 run-jump>walk 22,11 28,72 6,77 51,94 34,24 8,07 39,00 42,32 9,98 (run-jump) t (17) = 0.967, p = 0.347
rastrear F(2, 51) = 8.331 walk-run>jump 50,94 38,71 9,12 33,17 35,21 8,30 8,17 15,98 3,77 (walk-run) t (17) = 1.648, p = 0.118
recorrer F(2, 51) = 20.133 walk>run>jump 71,89 34,26 8,08 49,72 32,42 7,64 10,67 18,76 4,42
regatear F(2, 51) = 2.588, p = 0.08 walk-run >jump 44,61 43,83 10,33 41,22 39,55 9,32 18,17 28,89 6,81 (walk-run) t (17) = 0.315 , p = 0.757
renquear F(2, 51) = 26.037 walk>run-jump 84,94 19,71 4,64 32,28 36,68 8,65 22,50 24,62 5,80
resbalarse F(2, 51) = 0.951, p = 0.39 15,28 22,55 5,31 29,39 38,16 8,99 21,00 29,93 7,06
retozar F(2, 51) = 0.024, p = 9.76 19,33 22,17 5,23 21,17 30,37 7,16 20,72 25,71 6,06
rondar F(2, 51) = 22.319 walk>run-jump 67,72 39,05 9,20 14,56 23,69 5,58 7,89 22,70 5,35
serpentear F(2, 51) = 21.714 walk>run>jump 73,28 28,83 6,80 28,83 37,64 8,87 10,56 18,42 4,34
448
APPENDIX 5. DATA FROM RATING STUDY
taconear F(2, 51) = 11.791 walk>jump>run 63,67 36,97 8,71 10,17 17,84 4,21 32,22 40,32 9,50
tambalearse F(2, 51) = 13.975 walk>run>jump 57,61 43,41 10,23 17,67 27,84 6,56 10,67 18,20 4,29
tropezar F(2, 51) = 3.81 jump>walk>run 18,33 23,80 5,61 7,50 20,20 4,76 33,11 38,08 8,98 (walk-jump) t(17) = -2.224, p = 0.040
trotar F(2, 51) = 0.45, p = 0.637 72,33 33,82 7,97 81,56 27,89 6,57 80,22 32,00 7,54
vagar F(2, 51) = 75.772 walk>run-jump 88,94 19,50 4,60 19,28 29,62 6,98 6,22 12,33 2,91
zancajear F(2, 51) = 7.073 walk>run-jump 79,33 30,65 7,22 45,61 32,43 7,64 41,00 36,78 8,67
zanquear F(2, 51) = 4.015 walk>run-jump 75,11 24,63 5,81 48,33 35,57 8,38 48,61 36,20 8,53
zaparrastrar F(2, 51) = 9.185 walk>run-jump 51,06 39,11 9,22 18,28 24,44 5,76 11,88 20,12 4,74
zapatear F(2, 51) = 7.570 walk-jump>run 53,50 39,27 9,26 9,94 17,19 4,05 33,22 39,38 9,28 (walk-jump) t (17) =1.588, p = 0.131
zigzaguear F(2, 51) = 17.732 walk>run>jump 74,61 22,15 5,22 51,72 36,84 8,68 19,56 24,91 5,87 (walk-run) t(17) = 2.063, p = 0.056
449
LA EXPRESIÓN DEL MOVIMIENTO EN INGLÉS Y EN ESPAÑOL:
TIPOLÓGICA Y PSICOLINGÜÍSTICA
Introducción
1
Según Talmy (2000b: 102), los satélites forman una categoría gramatical; su característica sintáctica
es su estrecha vinculación al verbo, y su función semántica es, principalmente, la de expresar el
Sendero, componente central del evento de movimiento.
451
RESUMEN
2
Ibarretxe-Antuñano (2004c) traduce el término inglés por lenguas de marco equipolente, que está
más próximo al término inglés en cuanto a forma. Sin embargo, se ha optado por traducirlo como
lenguas de marco equiparable o equivalente, ya que denota más claramente el estatus similar que los
componentes de Sendero y Manera tienen en este grupo de lenguas.
452
RESUMEN
El punto de divergencia entre las lenguas de marco verbal y satélite que mayor
atención ha recibido en los estudios de esta índole es el referente a la expresión de
Manera de movimiento. Las diferencias interlingüísticas en la lexicalización de la
Manera de movimiento no residen únicamente en dónde se suele expresar la Manera
de movimiento (en el verbo o fuera de él), sino también en el tipo de información que
se puede o no expresar. Como Slobin (1997: 459) señala, las lenguas parecen tener
un lexicón de verbos de manera con 2 niveles: (1) un nivel general o superordinado,
que está formado por verbos como walk ‘andar’, run ‘correr’, jump ‘saltar’, fly
‘volar’ etc; y (2) un nivel más especifico, que incluye verbos que denotan diferentes
modos de andar (p. ej., stroll ‘andar por placer y de un modo relajado’, wander
‘andar sin ningún objetivo o dirección’ o shuffle ‘andar arrastrando los pies’ en
inglés, o pasear, deambular), de correr (p.ej., jog ‘hacer jogging’, sprint ‘esprintar’),
etc. Un gran número de las investigaciones en el área han mostrado que el inglés
tiene un segundo nivel mucho más elaborado que el castellano, cuyo lexicón verbal
está formado por verbos de manera de movimiento más generales (cf. Berman y
Slobin, 1994; Slobin, 1996a, 1996b). Por otra parte, con el propósito de facilitar la
comparación interlingüítica en cuanto a qué tipo de información sobre la Manera de
movimiento puede expresarse en verbos u otras unidades léxicas, Ibarretxe-Antuñano
(2006a, 2006b), Özçalişkan (2004) y Slobin (2005), entre otros autores, han dividido
el componente semántico de Manera en un número de categorías o parámetros más
específicos:
• Patrón motor (las habilidades motrices básicas): andar, correr, saltar, volar
• Velocidad (lenta, rápida): apresurarse, acelerar, desacelerar
• Medio de transporte (en animal, en coche, en tren, etc): cabalgar, jinetear,
pilotar
• Movimiento con un gran esfuerzo: abalanzarse, lanzarse
• Movimiento inestable: tambalearse
• Movimiento obstruido: tropezar, trompicar
• Movimiento suave, sin obstáculos: deslizarse
• Movimiento por placer: pasear
453
RESUMEN
454
RESUMEN
455
RESUMEN
Aparte de los verbos que expresan Manera y de los que expresan Sendero, se espera
encontrar, en ambas lenguas, otros verbos que codifican otros tipos de componentes
semánticos, tales como Movimiento + Base (entidad con respecto a la que se mueve
la Figura), como en atajar (ir por un atajo) y costear (ir por la costa), Movimiento +
Sendero + Manera, como en acechar (ir detrás de alguien de un modo furtivo),
Movimiento + Sendero + Base como en embarcar (subir a bordo de un medio de
locomoción), etc.
456
RESUMEN
457
RESUMEN
458
RESUMEN
3
Para el inglés, se considera que la mayoría de diccionarios están de acuerdo cuando al menos 3 de las
5 fuentes lo están (CALD, TFD, MW, LLA, OED); en castellano, se han consultado solamente 2
diccionarios, por lo que por mayoría entendemos los dos (DUE, DRAE).
459
RESUMEN
460
RESUMEN
Resultados
Con el propósito de exponer los resultados de esta investigación del modo más claro
posible, comenzaremos con los principales resultados del análisis semántico del
461
RESUMEN
CORPUS
Distribución general
Además de verbos que amalgaman o incorporan Sendero (p.ej., entrar, salir, enter,
exit, approach) y Manera de movimiento (p.ej., bucear, correr, deambular, pasear,
volar, revolotear, correr, deambular, stroll, amble, jog), encontramos un gran
número de combinaciones de distinta información semántica que se lexicaliza en
verbos en ambos idiomas. Por ejemplo:
• Movimiento + Base (Ground): p.ej., volar, fly (la Base es aire); nadar, swim
(la Base es agua) etc. El castellano posee unos verbos donde la Base se deja
entrever en el verbo, por ejemplo, atajar ‘ir a un sitio tomando un atajo’,
costear ‘navegar o moverse a lo largo de la costa’, bordear, llanear. Este
último tipo de verbos no parece ser tan común en el léxico verbal inglés.
• Movimiento + Sendero + Base (Ground): p.ej., embarcar ‘subir a bordo’,
embark, emigrar, emigrate, aterrizar y land o alight en inglés.
• Movimiento + Sendero + Base + Manera: p.ej., ford ‘cruzar un río (a pie
normalmente)’, skydive ‘lanzarse en picado por el aire’, capuzarse.
• Movimiento + Manera + Base: p.ej., bucear, dive (nadar debajo del agua),
callejear (andar por las calles), wade (andar con dificultad por una corriente
de agua).
• Movimiento + Figura + Co-movimiento: p.ej., acompañar, accompany,
escoltar, escort, guiar, guide, lead. Estos verbos expresan que una entidad se
desplaza en relación a otra que también se desplaza a la misma vez; en otras
palabras, hay varias Figuras en co-movimiento.
462
RESUMEN
Verbos de sendero
Con respecto a verbos que lexicalizan Sendero, de un total de 257 verbos en
castellano y 376 en inglés, en nuestro corpus de verbos se han encontrado 63 para el
castellano y 44 para el inglés. Nuestros datos muestran que el castellano tiene un
gran número de verbos de etimología latina que consisten en la amalgama de la
preposición ‘a’ + ‘adverbio’, por ejemplo, acercar(se), adentrar(se), alejar(se),
aproximar(se), y a otros verbos que provienen de nombres, distanciarse (de
distancia), encumbrar (de cumbre). Este tipo de verbos no se encuentran en inglés,
aunque sí que encontramos un gran número de verbos de origen latino muy similares
a los españoles, y que por tanto, expresan los mismos tipos de Sendero:
463
RESUMEN
Nuestro análisis, además, muestra que ambas lenguas expresan hasta 13 tipos
de Sendero diferentes. Los tipos de Sendero que se expresan con mayor frecuencia
en ambos lexicones son: ‘Lejos de la Base’, ‘Hasta encima o arriba de la Base–
Hacia arriba’, ‘Hacia la Base’, ‘Hacia abajo desde la Base – Hacia abajo hasta la
Base – Hacia abajo’. Por otra parte, ‘Fuera de la Base’ es el tipo de Sendero menos
frecuente en ambos lexicones, con un verbo por lengua (salir, exit). Aparentemente,
no hay razón por la que este tipo de Sendero se vea tan desfavorecido.
Recapitulando, ambos lexicones pueden expresar los mismos tipos de
Sendero, apoyando la idea de que el léxico verbal de sendero es mucho más limitado
que el de manera, tal y como Özçalişkan argumentaba. No obstante, nuestros datos
no apoyan su propuesta sobre que las lenguas de marco verbal y satélite tienen el
mismo número de verbos de sendero; nuestros resultados muestran que el castellano
posee un mayor número de verbos de sendero. El mayor número de verbos de
sendero en castellano se puede atribuir a la presencia de un gran número verbos de
distintos orígenes etimológicos, entre los cuáles están aquellos que hemos
presentado anteriormente.
Verbos de manera
Los verbos de manera de movimiento identificados en nuestro corpus (276 para
inglés y 123 para castellano) pueden agruparse en 2 categorías generales: movimiento
interno a la Figura y movimiento translativo (cf. Talmy (2000b), self-contained
motion y translational motion). Por movimiento interno a la Figura, entendemos
aquellos modos en los que la Figura se mueve sin desplazarse a ningún otro lugar,
como por ejemplo, un péndulo que oscila, un niño que se balancea en un columpio,
etc. Por otro lado, movimiento translativo se refiere a aquellas acciones que
transcurren mientras la Figura se desplaza o cambia de lugar; por ejemplo, si alguien
camina o corre, o un pájaro vuela, se espera que haya un cambio de lugar con
464
RESUMEN
4
Nótese que los porcentajes son relativos al total de verbos de manera en cada lengua.
465
RESUMEN
• Información sobre los pasos o saltos: mince, scurry, scuttle (pasos cortos),
stride, lope (a grandes pasos o zancadas).
El inglés posee un mayor número de verbos referidos a ‘patrón motor’ y, por tanto,
presenta una mayor granularidad semántica en este subdominio. Sin embargo, ambas
lenguas muestran la misma organización del léxico verbal; tanto el inglés como el
castellano tienen una mayor número de verbos que denotan distintos modos de andar
(47 en inglés, 24 en castellano) que de correr (7 en inglés y 4 en castellano) o de
saltar (11 en inglés y 3 en castellano).
También hemos observado que en inglés, el nombre de vehículos (bike, cab,
ferry, raft, ship, truck, etc) y el nombre de distintos bailes (foxtrot, twist, waltz, etc)
pueden emplearse como verbos de movimiento. Esta versatilidad no se encuentra en
castellano; en nuestro corpus de verbos encontramos 3 verbos referidos a ‘bailes’
(bailar, bailotear (bailar de una manera descuidada) y danzar) frente a 19 en inglés,
y 14 verbos referidos a ‘vehículo’ frente a 52 en inglés. A pesar de las diferencias
cuantitativas, en ambas lenguas, encontramos tres tipos de verbos relacionados con
vehículos: (1) verbos que implican desplazamiento mediante un vehículo: conducir,
pilotar, navegar, drive, cruise, ride etc.; (2) verbos que denotan un vehículo o un
instrumento específico: bike, ferry, ship, ski, esquiar, patinar, etc, y (3) verbos
metonímicos que lexicalizan una parte de un vehículo para referirse al vehículo al
completo: remar (remo por barco), pedalear (pedal por bicicleta), oar, paddle y row
(remo por barco), pedal (pedal por bicicleta), sail (vela por barco), wheel (rueda por
coche o vehículo con ruedas).
El léxico verbal en castellano es más parco en detalles sobre maneras de
movimiento, pero encontramos tres verbos muy arraigados en la cultura española:
taconear, zapatear y chancletear, que expresan información que parece no poder
lexicalizarse en un verbo en inglés. Concretamente, estos verbos denotan un
determinado sonido/ruido que se produce al bailar y/o al andar llevando un
determinado tipo de calzado.
ESTUDIOS EXPERIMENTALES
466
RESUMEN
Uno de los problemas que surgió a la hora de codificar nuestro corpus de verbos en
inglés y en castellano fue la dificultad de decidir si un verbo denotaba una manera de
andar, de correr o de saltar, al no quedar siempre claro en las definiciones dadas por
los diccionarios. Con este estudio obtuvimos evidencia empírica sobre qué verbos de
movimiento (a) se consideran como maneras de andar, de correr o de saltar, (b)
cuáles no parecen pertenecer a ningún patrón motor específico, y (c) cuáles parecen
ser casos intermedios.
467
RESUMEN
Conclusiones finales
El análisis contrastivo de una gran porción del léxico verbal de movimiento en inglés
y en castellano ha permitido observar algunas semejanzas. En primer lugar, el mayor
número de verbos amalgama Movimiento y otro componente semántico, bien
Manera o Sendero, lo que sugiere que son los patrones de lexicalización más
frecuentes en estas lenguas. En segundo lugar, los verbos de sendero expresan, al
menos, 13 tipos de Sendero. Entre ellos, los que más frecuentemente se lexicalizan
en verbos en ambas lenguas son: ‘Lejos de la Base’, ‘Hasta encima o arriba de la
Base – Hacia arriba’, ‘Hacia la Base’, ‘Hacia abajo desde la Base – Hacia abajo hasta
la Base – Hacia abajo’. Por otro lado, ‘Fuera de la Base’ es el tipo de Sendero menos
frecuente en ambos lexicones. En tercer lugar, ambas lenguas poseen un mayor
número de verbos que expresan distintos modos de andar, que de correr o saltar.
Estas tendencias sugieren que hay semejanzas importantes entre el inglés y el
468
RESUMEN
469