Doce PDF
Doce PDF
Doce PDF
S. Rachman
PII: S0005-7967(14)00176-4
DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.10.006
Reference: BRT 2787
Please cite this article as: Rachman, S., The Evolution of Behaviour Therapy and Cognitive Behaviour
Therapy, Behaviour Research and Therapy (2014), doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.10.006.
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to
our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all
legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
THE EVOLUTION OF BEHAVIOUR THERAPY AND COGNITIVE
RI
BEHAVIOUR THERAPY
SC
S. Rachman
U
Psychology Dept., University of British Columbia, Vancouver
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
Email: rachman@mail.ubc.ca
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ABSTRACT
The historical background of the development of behaviour therapy is described. It was based on
the prevailing behaviourist psychology and constituted a fundamentally different approach to the
PT
causes and treatment of psychological disorders. It had a cold reception and the idea of treating
the behaviour of neurotic and other patients was regarded as absurd. The opposition of the
RI
medical profession and psychoanalysts is explained. Parallel but different forms of behaviour
SC
therapy developed in the US and UK. The infusion of cognitive concepts and procedures
generated a merger of behaviour therapy and cognitive therapy, cognitive behaviour therapy (C
U
BT). The strengths and limitations of the early and current approaches are evaluated.
AN
KEYWORDS: Eysenck’s house, behaviourism, conditioning, operant conditioning,
M
The decision to start a journal devoted to publishing articles on the radical new
developments in psychological therapy was taken after dinner on a rainy night in Professor
The new approach, behaviour therapy (BT), was a major shift away from the prevailing
PT
psychiatric treatment for psychological disorders (mainly medications and physical treatments),
RI
and fundamentally different from the psychoanalytic method.
In these circumstances it was difficult to publish clinical accounts of the new methods and
SC
associated research. The editors of medical journals were opposed to non-medical psychologists
carrying out treatment and the analytic journals were closed because behaviour therapy was
U
regarded as completely wrong and potentially harmful. The rejection letters from various
AN
journals were brief, occasionally rude and sometimes fruity.
association with Dr. Joseph Wolpe, a brilliant, diffident, quietly cheerful clinical scientist,
D
Eysenck sent proposals for a new journal to several publishers of scientific/academic journals
TE
but they were slow to respond and not interested. However, Pergamon Press had published
Eysenck’s important edited book on the new approach (Behaviour Therapy and the Neuroses,
EP
1960), and it was felt that the owner of this rapidly expanding scientific publishing firm, Mr R.
Accordingly, the full proposal was sent to Maxwell and a meeting was arranged. During a
AC
pleasant and lively dinner at Eysenck’s house, politics, literature, and London were discussed. At
the end of the evening as Mr.Maxwell prepared to leave, he casually said that the proposal was
acceptable and he would publish the journal. “Call my assistant director Mr. RIchards and he
will supply you with whatever you need to start the journal.”
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
The first issue of Behaviour Research and Therapy, with its chic acronym –BRAT—
appeared in April 1963. Fifty years and 51 volumes later, the journal is widely read and has a
What were these radical new developments, and what were the ideas driving them ? The
PT
basic idea was that psychologists should apply the methods of behaviourism to clinical problems.
RI
Introspectionist psychology which focused on minute analyses of a person’s reactions to physical
stimuli, mainly auditory and visual, had nothing to offer, and the enterprise was embarrassed by
SC
failures of replication. Behaviourists rejected the discredited introspection and insisted on the
need to study observable behaviour (Watson,1919; Skinner,1953; 1959). It was a move away
U
from the stagnation of introspectionist psychology towards the optimism of behaviorism. As the
AN
distinguished historian E.G.Boring put it, “For a while in the 1920’s it seemed as if all America
abnormal behaviour. The founding Editor of Behaviour Research and Therapy, Hans-Jurgen
TE
Eysenck, stated the rationale succinctly: “Neurotic symptoms are learned patterns of behaviour
which for some reason or another are unadaptive,” (Eysenck 1959, p. 62, original
EP
emphasis).Treatment should promote the extinction of the unadaptive behaviour and enhance
dissemination of the new ideas and methods (1960,1985,1990).He was a formidable critic and
AC
his1952 article disputing the evidence that had been advanced in support of the claim that
psychotherapy is effective, set off a storm. He asserted the need to introduce rigorous standards
Certain types of abnormal behaviour were suitable targets for the behavioural clinicians to
take on. In the fifties and sixties there were many patients who suffered from agoraphobia (
Mathews, Gelder and Johnston,1981). Their unadaptive behaviour was restricted mobility which
PT
prevented them from leaving their homes, and intense fear when they attempted to do so. The
RI
clinical problem was construed as a problem of observable behaviour, and therefore respectable
SC
A behavioural treatment was developed. Patients were encouraged and assisted to engage in
increasingly lengthy and prolonged therapeutic walks, and the method was reasonably effective.
U
(The term ‘agoraphobia’ was not entirely appropriate because many of the affected people also
AN
experienced intense fear at home. Years later the emphasis shifted from restricted mobility to the
The introduction of behavioural treatment sounds simple and it was, but at the time there
D
was strong medical opposition to psychologists carrying out treatment. Moreover, a treatment for
TE
modify the person’s behaviour. Prominent psychoanalysts were convinced that the removal of a
neurotic symptom by behaviour therapy would be followed by symptom substitution (it was not)
C
and they were seriously worried that behaviour therapy would be harmful. However absurd the
AC
new ideas seemed to be, it became more and more difficult to ignore the accumulation of
successfully treated cases; however, numerous editors and professors did manage to overcome
the difficulty.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
behaviour therapy psychologists working in clinics/hospitals carried out tests of intelligence and
aptitude. Diagnosis and treatment were not part of the curriculum and their clinical experience
was narrow and minimal. Hence their attempts to provide therapy were opposed. They were
PT
obliged to teach themselves and they did. The gradual, discreet and sneaky expansion of clinical
RI
psychology from testing to treatment took place when the medical profession was hierarchical
and dominating. Psychologists were limited to treating only those to patients referred by a
SC
physician. When case-reports of patients treated by psychologists were published they included
an acknowledgement thanking the responsible psychiatrist for permission to see their patient.
U
Nevertheless, steady progress was made and fresh methods were developed for treating a
AN
variety of psychological disorders, particularly ‘neuroses’, roughly equivalent to ‘anxiety
disorders’. Large numbers of patients were helped and in the process a deeper understanding of
M
fear and anxiety was achieved. Behaviour Research and Therapy played a major part in
D
The early attempts at behaviour therapy were followed by the development of cognitive
therapy, and latterly by cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT). Behaviour therapy emerged in
EP
independent but parallel paths in the United States and the United Kingdom during the period
from 1950 to 1970.The second stage, the development of cognitive therapy, took place in the
C
U.S. from the mid-1960s onwards. The third stage, the merging of behaviour therapy and
AC
cognitive therapy into CBT gathered momentum in the late 1980s and is now well established in
Britain, North America, Australia and parts of Europe. It is the most broadly and confidently
endorsed form of psychological therapy and is the mainstay of the momentous expansion of
psychotherapy services in the U.K. (Rachman and Wilson, 2008).For a detailed description of
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
the historical development from behaviour therapy to cognitive behaviour therapy see Rachman
(2009).
The Evolution of Behaviour Therapy. The origin of BT can be traced to Pavlov’s fundamental
PT
work on the process of conditioning (Asratyan, 1953; Pavlov, 1955 Edition). His discovery of
RI
conditioned salivary reflexes led to many new findings and in time he established an
SC
Pavlov proved that abnormal and lasting disruptions of behaviour can be produced by exposing
animals to insoluble perceptual discriminations or to intense stress, and he mapped out the
U
effects of these induced disturbances (Pavlov,1955 ed., pp. 234–244). It is a curiosity that Pavlov
AN
never took the logical next step, from causation to cure. His recommendations for treating the
induced neuroses and clinical conditions were conventional, not de-conditioning but drugs
M
(especially bromides),sleep, rest, and removal to protected shelter (Asratyan, 1953, pp. 128–
D
130).
TE
Later researchers reasoned that if neuroses in animals can be developed through conditioning,
it should be possible to de-condition them. Prominent among the pioneers of this exciting
EP
possibility were Gantt (1944), Liddell (1944), Masserman (1943), and Wolpe (1952). Pavlov’s
experimental model laid the basis for the scientific study of how abnormal behaviour, and fear in
C
particular, is acquired (Mineka 1985, 1987) and his work was used as the basis for a conditioning
AC
In 1920, Watson& Rayner published their famous case of little Albert to demonstrate how
emotional responses can become conditioned in humans. A distinct fear was established in a
stable 11-month-old boy by presenting him with a rat and then making a sudden loud noise
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
behind him. After repeating this sequence a number of times the child began to display signs of
fear when the rat was introduced. This reactivity persisted and then generalized to other stimuli.
The significance of this demonstration of inducing a fear was over-interpreted, but the idea that
human fears can be conditioned inspired the valuable research of Mary Cover Jones (1924) on
PT
the unlearning of children’s fears. Her enterprise made therapeutic fear-reduction seem viable
RI
and directly influenced the forms of BT that were developed for children and adults some 30
years later. After testing a number of possible methods, Jones concluded that two were reliably
SC
effective in reducing the fears: direct conditioning, in which the feared object is repeatedly
shown to the child at gradually increasing proximity and the child’s negative reactions are
U
dampened by associating them with pleasurable eating, and by the imitation of ‘fearless’
AN
children. Remarkably, these two tactics still have merit and are used in many circumstances. The
graded and gradual exposures to fear stimuli were an implicit element of her conditioning
M
method and are a central feature of the contemporary method of exposure and response
D
prevention (ERP). The full value of her work emerged after a dormancy of three decades and is
TE
an historical example that provides a spark of hope for clinical researchers who yearn for the
experimental neuroses in animals provided a basis for his method of reducing human fears
gradual and graded imaginal exposures to the conditioned stimuli that evoked the fear response.
Each evocation of anxiety was damped down by a relaxation response; exposure followed by
inhibition (EXP+I).
PT
evidence of the fear-reducing effects of Wolpe’s method of repeated imaginal presentations of
RI
the fear stimuli (Lang,1968; Lang and Lazovik,1963). Meanwhile, a shift was taking place in
clinical practice. Therapists began using exposures to real situations and objects, described oddly
SC
as “in vivo exposures.” The reasons for this change were not made explicit but probably were a
U
evidence that in vivo exposures were superior. The combination of repeated exposures and
AN
response prevention was effective and widely adopted. Wolpe worked in isolation in his private
practice and it was impractical to arrange in vivo exposures. Hospital clinicians had superior
M
resources and most of the refinements and improvements in the methods of treating anxiety took
D
Few of Wolpe’s (1958) other methods, whether borrowed or invented (thought- stopping,
assertive behaviour, aversion relief), were adopted. His stimulating theory of reciprocal
EP
inhibition had explanatory value and enabled clinicians to think constructively about therapeutic
Using the experience he had gained in treating agoraphobia at the Maudsley Hospital Dr.
AC
Victor Meyer, a dedicated high-spirited therapist not averse to a fine cigar, made a breakthrough
in the treatment of OCD at the Middlesex Hospital in London in 1965. He applied the
behavioural technique of repeated exposures in the treatment of two severely afflicted in-patients
suffering from OCD. At the time there was little to offer patients with OCD and numbers of
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
severe cases underwent major psychosurgery, as had one of the two patients who was ultimately
treated by Meyer. Both of the patients were given 24-hour care during which prolonged sessions
of exposure to their fear-inducing stimuli/ situations were undertaken. During and after the
sessions they were prevented from undoing the effects of the anxiety evoked, by washing their
PT
hands compulsively or by other compulsive behaviour –the response prevention component of
RI
ERP. They made some significant improvements, and Meyer’s (1966) case-reports in Behaviour
Research and Therapy inspired other behaviour therapists to tackle this complex intractable
SC
disorder. Within a few years the method was refined and made available for out–patients. In
1979 a randomized control trial of behaviour therapy for OCD was reasonably successful
U
(Rachman et alia, 1979). Steady progress was also made in treating social anxiety and
AN
generalized anxiety. Bandura (1969,1977a), a ’social learning’ theorist who attached importance
to the social-cognitive factors in behavioural treatment, was the first experimenter to develop a
M
proved its therapeutic effectiveness and introduced the concept of perceived self-efficacy
TE
(Bandura, 1977b), which was put forward as an explanation for the effects of behaviour therapy,
especially those involving the reduction of fear. Therapeutic modelling can be dramatically
EP
(1989). The method has been so refined that one-session treatments are now used routinely by
C
How did the ideas which promoted behaviour therapy fare in all this progress? Eysenck’s
(1959,1960) general theory of ‘neurotic behaviour’, stated that neurotic symptoms are learned
patterns of behaviour that “for some reason or another are unadaptive”. Influenced by Pavlov’s
thinking, it was postulated that the ‘some reason or another’ might be conditioning. The
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
conditioning theory of fear acquisition (Eysenck, 1960;Wolpe,1958; Wolpe & Rachman, 1960)
retains some explanatory value but did not provide a comprehensive account of the genesis of
fears. At least two other pathways are involved: vicarious and informational
(Rachman,1977,1991,2013).
PT
It was reasonable to expect that neurotic behaviour would fade out precisely because it is not
RI
adaptive, but it did not. Mowrer’s (1960) explanation of the “neurotic paradox” proved to be
extremely useful for tackling the problem. He set out a two-stage model of fear and avoidance
SC
and argued that avoidance behaviour persists because it is successful in reducing or preventing
fear. Any avoidant or other behaviour that reduces anxiety ---for examples, agoraphobic
U
avoidance of public places, or compulsively washing one’s hands to reduce a fear of serious
AN
contamination(e.g. Rachman and Hodgson,1980)--will be strengthened. Mowrer’s explanation
was timely and fitting and behaviour therapists exploited its explanatory value. However, his
M
explanation of the persistence of avoidance behaviour dealt with avoidance driven by fear, but
D
people also engage in avoidance behaviour for a variety of reasons that include discomfort,
TE
disgust, impatience, or simple convenience. It is also a common and important means of gaining
and maintaining a sense of safety (Rachman,1990; Rachman, Radomsky and Shafran,2008). The
EP
cognitive explanation for persistence postulates that the abnormal behaviour is caused by
maladaptive beliefs and misappraisals, and it continues until they are corrected.
C
AC
problems that did not lend itself to behaviour therapy, not least because BT was absolutely
behavioural, and cognitions were not on the agenda at that time. Wolpe (1958) had limited
success in treating OCD; the application of desensitization to the tentacles of severe OCD was
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
lengthy and laborious. As mentioned above, Victor Meyer’s (1965) exploratory treatment paved
the way for the use of ERP in dealing with some manifestations of OCD. It is moderately
effective in treating patients suffering from a fear of contact contamination, but of minimal value
in treating obsessions (e.g. by looped tapes etc) or mental contamination. Progress in treating
PT
these manifestations of OCD was made when mental contamination was recognised, and by the
RI
introduction of cognitive construals and techniques (Rachman, Coughtrey, Shafran and
Radomsky, 2014).
SC
Reinforcement procedures. While these events were taking place in the
U
United Kingdom, psychologists in the United States were applying Skinner’s(1953;1957;1959)
AN
ideas and operant conditioning techniques to clinical problems. His forceful writings and
inspired attempts to use operant conditioning in order to help psychiatric patients (e.g.
D
Lindsley,1956).The idea was that the abnormal behaviour of psychiatric patients is the result of
TE
an inappropriate conditioning history, and it can be reshaped into normal forms by providing the
behaviour was followed by rewarding consequences and that inappropriate behaviour was
extinguished by withholding rewards. This seemingly simple approach was often used with
C
The entire medical model, with its concepts of “psychiatric disorder,” “abnormal behavior,”
and “mental illness,” was thrown overboard and a new vocabulary was introduced. The patient’s
disorder was redefined as a behaviour problem, pure and simple, and the solution lay in
For institutional and theoretical reasons, the U.S. and U.K. psychologists turned their
attention to different samples and different problems. The British psychologists were employed
in National Health Service (NHS) clinics and hospitals, and worked primarily on ‘neuroses’, in
adult out-patient facilities. The American psychologists tried to improve the life of people with
PT
severe disorders, such as schizophrenia, manic-depression, childhood autism, developmental
RI
disorders, and self-injurious behaviour. Some of these intrepid early researchers, such as Ayllon
(1963) and Ayllon & Azrin (1968) chose to work with the most severe cases: patients who lived
SC
in the back wards of large dismal psychiatric hospitals and whose condition was believed to be
U
They introduced what became known as token economy systems, systematic programmes of
AN
reinforcement for appropriate behaviour and the omission of reinforcements for inappropriate
behaviour. Tokens, originally plastic discs, were used as readily dispensable markers of
M
reinforcement which the patients could exchange for tangible rewards, such as sweets, cigarettes
D
and magazines. The earliest token-economy systems were established in psychiatric institutions
TE
and later introduced into homes for delinquent youths, hostels, facilities for intellectually
of faulty learning, concentrated their efforts exclusively on behaviour, and worked mainly with
AC
people who had severe, intractable problems. They espoused an unqualified environmentalism
This approach had limitations, and the British psychologists were influenced by
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
“learning theory” and unreceptive to Skinner’s ideas, which they regarded as narrow and
unhelpful. Methodologically, the virtual exclusion of evidence that was not based on observable
behaviour proved to be unsustainable. For example, the attempt to get around this self-imposed
prohibition led to the description of people’s thoughts and feelings as “verbal behavior”
PT
(Skinner,1957), or worse, “private events.” Using the latter term when interviewing patients
RI
suffering from intense anxiety (“tell me about your private events”) risked some
misunderstandings.
SC
Cognitions were disdained and Skinner’s attempt to explain language and thinking as
behaviour, verbal behaviour, generated and maintained by operant conditioning, fell flat. The
U
development and emergence of complex language early in childhood could not possibly be the
AN
result of direct conditioning, nor could a person’s ability fluently to switch from one language to
another. Original speech and original thinking are not the product of operant conditioning.
M
The Brits had a quiet reverence for the ideas and findings of Pavlov, and the two leading
D
contributors, Eysenck and Wolpe, both favoured the Hullian learning theory, a hypothetico-
TE
deductive theory that offered precision and scientific formality and was widely subscribed to at
the time (Hull, 1943). Today very few clinicians know or care about the learning theories but
EP
problems as the result of faulty unadaptive learning had merit but left much unsaid. The many
AC
and varied manifestations of neuroses (mainly anxiety disorders) were put down to faulty
conditioning, and few attempts were made to explain why one patient developed agoraphobia
and another obsessive-compulsive disorder, or social phobia, and so on. This weakness had two
consequences. The approach was too general to allow precise testing of the main propositions of
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
the overall theory, and it did not promote the development of specific treatments for specific
For a period, the well-established exposure and response prevention method was used across
PT
the board, for virtually all clinical problems. There is abundant evidence of its effectiveness
RI
(e.g.Barlow 1988,2002; Marks,1987; Steketee, 2012) and the method has the advantage of being
easily learned and easily conducted. It can be comfortably combined with other methods, notably
SC
cognitive therapy. ERP is particularly successful in treating circumscribed and other phobias,
some forms of OCD, and other anxiety problems, but is limited by flaws in the rationale (e.g. it is
U
not essential to evoke the fear in order to reduce it, Rachman,1990;2009; 2013), and the absence
AN
of cognitive analyses is a weakness.
situations/stimuli that evoke fear, and the therapeutic aim is to promote extinction of fear and
D
anxiety. The exposure treatment was widely adopted, almost to the exclusion of other techniques.
TE
For a period, it was believed that exposure was not merely sufficient but also a necessary
condition for the reduction of fear/anxiety. However, fear/anxiety can be reduced without the use
EP
incidental exposures to fear-evoking situations was tempting, but confused matters and the
C
therapy for health anxiety (McManus et alia. (2010) and other problems(e.g. Arch and
Ayers,2013;Hofmann,2010)
(2) Applied relaxation (Ost and Westling,1995; for panic disorder; and for GAD (Ost and
PT
alia,1994;Norton,2012;Ost,2000)
RI
(3)Bandura’s (1969,1977a,b) informational means of changing fear
(4) the reduction of fear/anxiety by cognitive therapy (e.g. Arntz and van den Hout,1996;Barlow,
SC
2002; Booth & Rachman, 1992; Butler et alia, 2006; D.A.Clark and Beck, 2011; D.M.Clark,
2006; Cottreaux et al.,2001; Norton and Price,2007;Ost,Thulin and Ramnero, 2004; Rachman,
U
Coughtrey, Radomsky and Shafran, 2014; Salkovskis, Clark and Hackmann,1991; van Oppen et
AN
alia,1995; Visser and Bouman, 2001;Whittal, Thordarson and McLean,2005; Whittal et alia,
(5) the frequently observed (untreated) reduction of fear/anxiety after successful treatment of
D
depression,
TE
The emergence of Cognitive Therapy Given the absence of progress in treating depression and
the waning prohibition against using cognitive concepts, behaviour therapists followed Beck’s
C
work (1976, 1993) with keen interest, reassured in part by the inclusion of behavioural
AC
assignments in his therapy protocols. They were also impressed by his insistence on accurate and
repeated recording of events and by the self-correcting nature of the programme. Setting aside
their reservations about the acceptability of cognitive concepts, behaviour therapists began
treating patients with cognitive techniques. Therapeutic successes removed their remaining
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
inhibitions about cognitive therapy, at least when used alongside BT and with an emphasis on the
who absorbed Beck’s cognitive concepts and became a major contributor to the growth of CBT,
acknowledged the debt to Beck: “There can be no doubt that Beck’s cognitive approach to the
PT
understanding and treatment of emotional problems represented a paradigm shift, and that
RI
paradigm has truly shifted” (Salkovskis 1996,p. xiii).
Beck (1967, 1976, 1993; D.A. Clark and Beck,2011; Hofmann, Asmundson and
SC
Beck,2013) postulated that depression and other psychological disorders arise from faulty
cognitions and/or faulty cognitive processing, and that the remedy is to correct the mis-appraisals
U
and promote corrective actions and appraisals. In the mid- 1960s, Beck “became familiar with
AN
BT and incorporated many principles from this approach” (Beck 1993, p.13). He used
corrective information and allow the patient to test the validity of their dysfunctional beliefs and
D
confused with exposure therapy. Unlike ERP which is used in order to generate extinction of
the purpose of the behavioural experiments is to collect specific, vivid, direct information. There
is seldom any reason to repeat a behaviour test, and response prevention tactics do not feature in
C
the tests. Behavioural experiments are particularly useful in treating panic disorder, social
AC
Beck’s rationale was that “an individual’s affect and behavior are largely determined by the
The thrust of his early work was on understanding and treating depression, a clinical
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
problem that remained unsolved by behaviour therapists, and they sensibly turned to Beck’s
work for guidance. Unpredictably, CBT made an even greater contribution to the treatment of
anxiety disorders than to depression. The development of behavioural activation for the
treatment of depression was a late but welcome turn of events (Jacobson et al.,1996).
PT
Cognitive therapy was developed in the wake of the growth of cognitive psychology, but
RI
the early claims of crucial connections between CBT and cognitive psychology were exaggerated
(Teasdale, 1993, p. 341; Seligman, 1988). At opposite ends of the spectrum there was a sharp
SC
contrast between cognitive psychology and behaviourism. Radical behaviourism, especially as
U
contrast, Philadelphia was cognitive rich (Beck, Seligman).
AN
The merging of behaviour therapy and cognitive therapy. The adoption of cognitive concepts
M
into therapy was inspired by the development of cognitive psychology and by a desire among
D
clinicians to pay more attention to the humanistic concerns of their patients. Behaviorism left
TE
little place for the content of the patient’s anxieties, and clinical conversations and analyses were
regarded as distractions from the need for patients to wend their fearful way up the anxiety
EP
hierarchies. Clinicians also felt under pressure to produce a method for treating patients who
were depressed.
C
The time was ripe for a cognitive-behavioural form of therapy, and the two streams,
AC
cognitive and behavioural, were brought together by Clark’s (1986) cognitive theory of panic
disorder. Barlow’s (1988,1999, 2002) comparable model was formulated independently and
made an invaluable contribution to the development of CBT. For ease of exposition, this account
of events and their significance focuses on the work of David M. Clark (1986) and Paul
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Prior to the introduction of Clark’s of the nature and causes of episodes of panic, the
PT
Panics were not treated directly but were expected to fade out once the agoraphobia was treated.
RI
Klein’s (1987) original biological analysis of panics elevated them to a problem in their own
right, and Clark’s (1986) theory was an attempt to provide a psychological explanation. “Panic
SC
attacks result from the catastrophic misinterpretation of certain bodily sensations”, such as a
shortness of breath, pounding heart, and dizziness (Clark, Behaviour Research and Therapy
U
1986, p. 462). These sensations are interpreted as being signs of imminent danger, most
AN
commonly of a serious threat to one’s health—”I am having a heart attack” or “I am going into a
coma.” The immediate cause of the panic is a threatening cognition. It follows that
M
for the cognitive analysis of various manifestations of anxiety, and the effectiveness of the
therapy deduced from the theory, is impressive. The evidence pertaining to the theory is mainly
EP
confirmatory, but some problems were encountered (Barlow, 2002; Clark,1988; Craske,1999;
Rachman,1990).
C
behaviour therapy. He showed the value of taking into account of the person’s interpretations of
their thoughts and feelings about their compulsions and obsessions (Salkovskis, Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1985). Additionally, he singled out the crucial role that feelings of
inflated responsibility play in many cases of OCD. By focusing attention on the patient’s
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
explanation of their disturbing thoughts, pressing urges, and compulsive behaviour, Salkovskis
filled an empty stage. Previously, the nature and significance of the specific content of the
obsessions and compulsions went unexamined, but then clinicians became acutely attuned to the
patients’ explanations, understanding, appraisals, wishes, and fears. Prior to the infusion of
PT
cognitive ideas obsessions were regarded, indeed defined, as unwanted, intrusive thoughts (plus
RI
images and impulses) but the content of the unwanted thoughts was of marginal interest.
Nowadays the precise content of intrusive thoughts is of central importance in dealing with
SC
obsessions, (Rachman, 2003, Whittal et alia, 2010).The psychological assessment of anxiety
disorders has been greatly improved by the development of cognitive scales and standardized
U
interview schedules. The recent surge of interest in images (Hackmann,Bennett-Levy and
AN
Holmes,2011) has therapeutic implications. Intrusive, highly emotional, fully formed, effortless,
extraordinarily stable visual ‘pictures in the mind’, often are of great personal significance, and
M
many assessment protocols now include an evaluation of this important cognition. This
D
a fresh and powerful new technique. Cognitive analyses and tactics, including rescripting, are
EP
and Shafran,2104).
C
The work of Clark and Salkovskis started the search for increasingly specific explanations
AC
of the various anxiety disorders and correspondingly specific treatments. The expansions include
theories of post-traumatic stress disorder (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), social phobia (D.M. Clark &
(Frost&Hartl,2003; Steketee &Frost, 2003; health anxiety (Salkovskis & Clark, 1993;Salkovskis
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
&Warwick 1986,Warwick & Salkovskis, 1990; Rachman,2010). Similar to Clark’s panic theory,
the outdated concept of hypochondriasis is now construed as “health anxiety disorder,” in which
sensations and cognitions. However, unlike panic, in which the feared event is imminent (“I am
PT
having a heart attack and will die”), in cases of health anxiety, the dreaded misfortune
RI
is distressing but not an imminent danger. Moreover, the provoking bodily sensations in health
anxiety tend to be persistent, even chronic, and give rise to extensive and intensive safety
SC
behaviour, and repeated requests for reassurance.
U
empiricism was absorbed into cognitive therapy. The behavioural style of conducting empirical
AN
outcome research was adopted, with its demands for rigorous controls, statistical designs,
treatment integrity and credibility, and so forth. In turn, cognitive concepts were absorbed into
M
advances that it has promoted, the CBT approach is not without problems. The term ‘cognitive
EP
behaviour therapy’ is used too loosely. Sometimes the B in CBT refers primarily to exposure
therapy, but at other times CBT refers primarily to cognitive re-appraisaIs. In everyday practice
C
the distinction matters little, but in research (and of course in meta-analyses) it is well to clarify
AC
It has proven difficult consistently to demonstrate the therapeutic superiority of CBT over
BT, and in several instances, the effects of ERP equalled those of CBT. For example, in Margraf
& Schneider’s (1991, Margraf,1995) study of panic disorder, the patients who received pure
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
enduring as the patients receiving pure cognitive therapy in which exposures were excluded.
Negative cognitions can decline after a directly cognitive treatment or after an indirect
PT
treatment such as exposure. One possibility is that with each exposure, the patient acquires fresh
RI
disconfirmatory evidence (e.g. no heart attack, did not lose control). The accumulation of this
SC
puzzling that the direct modification of the negative cognitions is not consistently more effective
than the indirect effects of exposure. The superiority of CBT over exposure has been
U
demonstrated in various projects (e.g.Clark et al., 2006), and these are important. The remaining
AN
question is why do cognitive improvements take place after treatments that do not explicitly
tackle the negative cognitions? One obstacle to answering questions of this type is the timing of
M
Reductions in fear and avoidance are easy to observe and measure, but they can
TE
occur slowly, over weeks rather than minutes. In cases of panic, the measures typically range
over days or weeks (e.g. the number of panics are recorded per week or even per month). If a
EP
patient records a decrease in panics, from four per week to one per week, when exactly did this
decline take place? Cognitions are evanescent and the time of the changes in mis-appraisals and
C
cognitive shifts are slow to develop, changing over weeks rather than minutes. Changes of
interpretation can initiate a process of behavioural change that only becomes evident some time
later.The cognitive processes set in motion during a CBT session often have their effects at some
point between sessions. For example, a patient receiving cognitive therapy may experience a
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
reduction in fear after an interval in which he/she had no contact with the phobic situation and
made no deliberate attempt to facilitate the fear reduction between sessions. An illustration of
this type of delay in the effects of CBT was reported during the experimental reduction of
PT
There are also problems of causality. In the view of Seligman(1988) the results of CBT are
RI
open to more than a single interpretation. Seligman incisively questioned why people adhere to
their irrational and unadaptive appraisals with such tenacity. Why do patients with anxiety
SC
disorders, such as panic, continue to believe in their irrational catastrophic interpretations of their
U
panic victim still believe he or she will have a heart attack in spite of 1000 extinction trials?”
AN
(Seligman,1988, p. 328).
The probable explanation for this apparent anomaly is that in these cases the fear of a heart
M
panic, and that cognition is intermittently confirmed. Nonetheless, the tenacity of the irrational,
TE
unadaptive cognitions about fear and danger that are postulated to generate and sustain anxiety is
a critical problem that remains to be explained. On similar lines, the catastrophically inflated
EP
feelings of responsibility encountered among patients with anxiety disorders, notably OCD, can
be frustratingly tenacious.
C
The development of CBT spans nearly a century from conditioned reflexes to catastrophic
AC
cognitions, and there was an inevitable lag between the scientific advances
and the dissemination of the effective methods (Clark, 2004). In keeping with the growing
insistence for evidence-based treatments, in 2007 the U.K. Minister for Health announced a plan
for massively expanding o the provision of psychological therapy for the treatment of anxiety
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
and depression (Rachman & Wilson,2008). He stated, “Psychological therapies have proved to
these common mental health problems and are often more effective in the long run”
(www.gnn.gov.uk; Oct. 10, 2007). The government introduced a six-year plan to improve the
PT
service by allocating more than 300 million pounds (roughly $600 million) in the first three years
RI
in order to train an additional 3600 psychological therapists in “giving evidence-based
treatment,” specifically CBT. The number of specially trained therapists is approaching 8000.
SC
Summary and conclusions.The development of psychological treatment for anxiety disorders and
U
depression has improved health care for many thousands of people, and the massive expansion of
AN
psychotherapeutic services for people in the United Kingdom will doubtless inspire comparable
improvements elsewhere. Clinical psychologists and clinical researchers provided the means for
M
this unparalleled improvement in mental health care. In the post-war period the medical
D
profession strongly opposed the provision of therapy by psychologists. The massive expansion of
TE
psychological services in the United Kingdom, hints at the outcome of that tendentious
professional dispute.
EP
Developing the methods of treatment greatly enhanced our understanding of the nature of
fear—the methods were, after all, methods for reducing fear. Peter Lang (1968) introduced a
C
fascinating conception of fear: fear is not a lump, but rather is a loosely connected set of three
AC
stimulating construal of prepared fears was introduced by Seligman(1971), and Clark (1986)
introduced the first cognitive explanation of episodes of intense fear. Naturally many questions
remain to be tackled, but insufficient progress has been made in answering one of Seligman’s
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Consideration of tenacious irrational beliefs are not confined to psychologists. In his book “The
Heel of Achilles” the philosophically-minded novelist Arthur Koestler pondered whether “the
prevalence of passionately held irrational beliefs” will ever become comprehensible (Koestler,
PT
1974, p.16).
RI
Acknowlegements.
SC
Thanks for comments and advice to M. Berger, Clare Philips, D.N.Q. Radomsky, R. Shafran,
W.Yule.
U
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
References
Arch, J. & Ayers, C. (2013). What treatment worked better for whom? Group CBT versus
adapted mindfulness stress reduction for anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and
PT
Therapy, 51, 434-442.
RI
Arntz, A. & van den Hout, M. (1996). Psychological treatment of panic disorder without
SC
34, 113-121.
U
Asratyan E. (1953). Pavlov: His Life and Works. Moscow: Foreign Lang. Publ.
AN
Ayllon T. (1963). Intensive treatment of psychotic behaviour by stimulus satiation and food
Barlow, D. (2002). Anxiety and Its Disorders. New York: Guilford. 2nd ed.
Beck, A. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: Int. Univ. Press.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Cultural Press.
Beck, J. et al. (1994). Comparison of cognitive therapy to relaxation training for panic disorder.
PT
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 818-826.
Bennett-Levy, J., Butler,J., Fennell,M. & Hackmann, A. (2004). Oxford Guide to Behavioural
RI
Experiments in Cognitive Therapy. Oxford, Oxford Univ. Press.
SC
Booth, R. & Rachman, S. (1992). The reduction of claustrophobia. Behaviour Research and
U
Boring, E. (1950). A History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Appleton
AN
Butler, A. et al. (2006). The empirical status of cognitive therapy. Clinical Psychology Review,
M
26, 17-31.
D
Clark, D. A. (2004). Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Obsessive Compulsive Disorders. New
TE
York: Guilford.
Clark, D. A. & Beck, A. T. (2011). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders. New York, Guilford
EP
Press.
C
Clark, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 461–
AC
470.
Clark, D. M. (1988). A cognitive model of panic attacks. In S. Rachman & J. D. Maser (Eds.),
Clark,D.M. et alia (2006) Cognitive therapy versus exposure and applied relaxation in social
Psychology,74,566-578.
PT
Cottraux,J. et alia (2001). A randomized control trial of cognitive therapy vs intensive behavior
RI
Clark D.M, Ehlers A, Hackmann A, McManus, F. (2006). Cognitive therapy vs. exposure and
SC
applied relaxation in social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. J. Consult. Clin.
Psychol. 74:567–78
U
Clark, D.M. and Fairburn, C. eds. 1997. The Science and Practice of Cognitive Behaviour
AN
Therapy. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
M
Clark DM, Wells A. 1995. A cognitive model of social phobia. In Social Phobia, eds. R
Heimberg, M. Liebowitz, D. Hope and F. Scheier, pp. 69–93. New York: Guilford
EP
Ehlers, A and Clark, D.M.(2000). A cognitive model of PTSD. Behav. Res. Ther. 38:319–45
AC
Eysenck, H.J. (1952). The effects of psychotherapy: An evaluation. J.Consult. Psychology, 16,
319-326.
Eysenck, H.J. (1959). Learning theory and behaviour therapy. J. Ment. Sci. 105:61–75
Eysenck, H.J. ed. (1960). Behaviour Therapy and the Neuroses. Oxford, UK: Pergamon
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Eysenck, H.J. and Rachman, S. (1965). The Causes and Cures of Neuroses. London: Routledge
Frost, R. and Hartl, M. (2003). Compulsive hoarding. In Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, eds.
PT
R. Menzies and P. de Silva, pp. 163–80. Chichester, UK: Wiley
RI
Gantt, WH. (1944). Experimental basis for neurotic behaviour. Psychosom. Med. 3:82–98
SC
Therapy Oxford , Oxford Univ.Press
U
Heimberg, R., Liebowitz, M., Hope, D. and Schneier. F. eds. (1995). Social Phobia. New York:
AN
Guilford Press
Hofmann,S., Admunson,G.and Beck, A.T. (2013). The science of cognitive therapy. Behavior
TE
Therapy,44, 199-212.
Klein, D. (1987). Anxiety reconceptualized. In Anxiety, ed. D Klein, pp. 235–64. Basel,
AC
Switzerland: Karger
Lang, P. (1968). Appraisal of systematic desensitization techniques with children and adults.
Process and mechanisms of change, theoretical analysis and implications for treatment
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
and clinical research. In Assessment and Status of the Behavior Therapies and
PT
Psychol. 66:519–25
Liddell, H. (1944). Conditioned reflex method and experimental neurosis. In Personality and the
RI
Behavior Disorders, ed. J McV Hunt, pp. 63–85. New York: Ronald Press
SC
Lindsley, O. (1956). Operant conditioning methods applied to research in chronic schizophrenia.
U
Margraf, J. (1995). Cognitive behavioural treatment of panic disorder: three-year follow-up.
AN
World Congress Cogn. Behav. Ther., July. Copenhagen
M
treatments for panic disorder. Am. Assn. Behav. Ther. Conf., November. New York
D
TE
Marks, I. (1987). Fears, Phobias and Rituals. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press
Masserman, J.H. (1943). Behavior and Neuroses. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
EP
Mathews, A., Gelder, M., and Johnston, D. (1981). Agoraphobia: Nature and Treatment. New
C
Mineka S. (1985). Animal models of anxiety-based disorders. In Anxiety and the Anxiety
Disorders, ed. A. Tuma and J. Maser, pp. 199–234. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
PT
Therapy, ed. H. Eysenck, and I. Martin, pp. 81–111. New York: Plenum
Muris,P. and Field,A. (2010). The role of verbal threat information in the development of
RI
childhood fear. Clinical Child Family Psychological Review, 13, 129-150.
SC
Norton,P. ( 2012). A randomized control trial of transdiagnostic cognitive behaviour therapy
Therapy,43,506-517.
U
AN
Norton,P. and Price,E. (2007). A meta-analytic review of adult CBT across the anxiety disorders.
M
Ost, L.G. (1989). One-session treatment for specific phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 27:1–8
D
TE
Ost, L.G., Svensson, L., Hellstrom,K. and Lindwall, R. (2001). One-session treatment of
Pavlov, I.P. (1955). Selected Works, 1955 Edition. Moscow: Foreign Lang. Publ.
Rachman, S. (1978, 1990). Fear and Courage, Second Edition. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
2nd ed.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Rachman, S. (1991). Neo-conditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisition. Clin. Psychol.
Rev. 11:155–73
Rachman, S. (2003). The Treatment of Obsessions. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
PT
Rachman,S. (2009). Psychological Treatment of Anxiety: The Evolution of Behavior Therapy
and Cognitive Therapy. Ann. Review of Clinical Psychology. doi. 032408 153635
RI
Rachman, S. (2012). Health anxiety: A cognitive construal. Behaviour Research and Therapy,50,
SC
502-512.
U
AN
Rachman,S., Coughtrey, A., Radomsky,A. and Shafran,R. (2014,in press) The Treatment of
Rachman, S., Cobb, J., Grey, S., McDonald, B., Sartory, G. and Stern, R. (1979). The
Rachman, S. and Hodgson, R. (1980). Obsessions and Compulsions. New York: Prentice
AC
Rachman, S. and Wilson, G.T. (2008). Expansion in the provision of psychological treatment in
Salkovskis, P. and Clark,D.M. (1993). Panic disorder and hypochondriasis. Adv. Behav. Res.
Ther. 15:23–48
Salkovskis,P., Clark,D. and Hackmann,A. (1991). Treatment of panic attacks using cognitive
PT
therapy without exposure or breathing retraining. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29,
161-166
RI
Salkovskis, P. and Warwick, H. (1986). Morbid preoccupations, health anxiety and reassurance:
SC
a cognitive- behavioural approach to hypochondriasis. Behav. Res. Ther. 24:597–602
U
Seligman, M. (1988). Competing theories of panic. In Panic: Psychological Perspectives, eds. S
AN
Rachman and JD Maser, pp. 321–30. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
M
Steketee, G. and Frost,R. (2003). Compulsive hoarding. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23:905–27
EP
Steketee, G.,Ed. (2012) Oxford Handbook of Obsessive Compulsive and Spectrum Disorders.
C
Teasdale, J. (1993). Emotion and two kinds of meaning. Behav. Res. Ther. 31:339–54
van Oppen,P. et alia (1995). Cognitive therapy versus Exposure in Vivo in the treatment of
PT
Watson, J.B. and Rayner, P. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 3:1–14
RI
Whittal,M.,Thordarson,D. and McLean, P. (2005). Treatment of OCD: Cognitive behaviour
therapy vs Exposure and response prevention. Behaviour Research and Therapy ,43,
SC
1559-1576.
U
obsessions: A randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 295-
AN
303.
M
Wilhelm,S. et alia (2005). Effectiveness of cognitive therapy for OCD: An open trial. J.Cognitive
Psychotherapy,19,173-179.
D
TE
Wilhelm,S. et alia (2009). Modular Cognitive Therapy for OCD: A wait-list controlled trial.
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. Press
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
HIGHLIGHTS:
PT
• The two original streams, classical conditioning and operant conditioning, gradually
merged
• Greatest success was made in explaining and treating anxiety disorders
RI
• Behaviour therapy was enormously enhanced by the infusion of cognitive analyses and
techniques in the eighties (Cognitive Behaviour Therapy)
•
SC
Currently CBT is the most widely used psychological therapy
• The strengths and shortcomings of CBT are under continuing scrutiny
U
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC