Segrin Nabi 2002 TV Viewing Marriage PDF
Segrin Nabi 2002 TV Viewing Marriage PDF
Segrin Nabi 2002 TV Viewing Marriage PDF
One explanation for the high divorce rate in our society focuses on the idealistic
expectations with which many people enter into marriage. The media have been
cited as the source of or major contributor to these expectations; however, no em-
pirical evidence exists to support that claim. Based on a survey of 285 never-mar-
ried university students, this study sought to examine the relationship between tele-
vision viewing, holding idealistic expectations about marriage, and intentions to
marry. Results from regression and path analyses indicate that, although overall
television viewing has a negative association with idealistic marriage expectations,
viewing of romantic genre programming (e.g., romantic comedies, soap operas)
was positively associated with idealistic expectations about marriage. Further, a
strong and positive association between these expectations and marital intentions
was evidenced. These findings are discussed in terms of both cultivation theory
and the uses and gratifications perspective of media influence.
The fact that the majority of marriages in the United States end in divorce is widely
known. In 1997, over 1.16 million divorces were granted in the United States—a
rate of 4.3 per 1000 (National Center for Health Statistics, 1998). From the 1960s to
the 1980s, the divorce rate increased by well over 200%. Although this increase
leveled off in the 1990s, the failure of marriages in U.S. society continues to be a
pervasive social problem (e.g., Hoffman & Duncan, 1988). This phenomenon has
sparked what is sure to be a lengthy quest to discover the causes of divorce and
the factors that contribute to its prevalence (e.g., Gottman, 1994).
Like any pervasive problem, no single cause of divorce can be isolated. How-
ever, one of the more powerful contributing factors appears to be the unrealistic
expectations that accompany, if not propel, people into marriage (Larson, 1988).
Recently, Demo and Ganong (1994) argued that “one of the most insidious factors
undermining marital satisfaction and longevity is that individuals enter into mar-
riage with unrealistic, idealistic, and romanticized notions about marriage”
Chris Segrin (PhD, 1990, University of Wisconsin) is an associate professor in the Department of
Communication at the University of Arizona. Robin L. Nabi (PhD, 1998, University of Pennsylvania) is
an assistant professor in the same department.
247
Journal of Communication, June 2002
248
Television and Marital Expectations
1982; Morgan, 1986), alienation (e.g., Morgan, 1986), and gender stereotypes (Gross
& Jeffries-Fox, 1978). However, the most studied issue in the extant cultivation
literature is the prevalence of violence on television and its effects on perceptions
of real-world incidence of crime and victimization (see review in Potter, 1993).
Numerous content analyses of network television programs have demonstrated
that the number of violent acts on U.S. television greatly exceeds the amount of
real-world violence (Gerbner & Gross, 1976; Gerbner et al., 1977). In turn, re-
search by Gerbner and his colleagues has shown that heavy television viewers: (a)
overestimate the incidence of serious crime in our society, and (b) are more likely
to believe that the world is a mean place where people cannot be trusted (i.e., the
“mean world” syndrome; e.g., Gerbner et al., 1994).
In the context of marital expectations, cultivation theory suggests that in por-
traying idealized images of marriage, the media may be cultivating unrealistic
beliefs about what marriage should be. By “idealized images of marriage” we
mean portrayals that include, for example, a great deal of romance, physical inti-
macy, passion, celebration, happiness, “love at first sight,” physical beauty, empa-
thy, and open communication. At the same time, media portrayals that exclude or
minimize conflict and mundane marital behaviors and interaction could also con-
tribute to idealized views of marriage. Although there are no current content
analyses of portrayals of marriage on American television, a review of content
analyses of British television revealed that “family roles in general are portrayed as
largely conflict-free relationships, with an emphasis on affection and altruism . . .
and a minimum of negative or rejecting interactions” (Livingstone, 1987 p. 253).
Ultimately, Livingstone (1987) concluded that “television provides a highly dis-
torted representation of personal relationships” (p. 253).
Interestingly, the one published study in the area of cultivation and marriage
seems to suggest the media are not cultivating idealized views of marriage. In her
sample of high school seniors, Signorielli (1991) found that TV viewing main-
tained small, though positive and significant, associations with negative beliefs
about marriage (i.e., questioning marriage as a way of life, preferring to live to-
gether before marriage, and believing that monogamous relationships are too
restrictive). Yet, Signorielli also found a small, positive, and significant relationship
between TV viewing and future marital-related behavior (i.e., getting and staying
married). Collectively these findings suggest that, at the very least, television is
sending a mixed message about marriage; that is, marriage should be a part of
one’s life, even though it may pose difficulties. Although these findings are rel-
evant to the idea that television viewing may relate positively to desire to marry,
the measurement of negative beliefs, but not idealistic beliefs, about marriage
makes the study’s use in examining the potential link between idealistic views of
marriage and media exposure tenuous.
To explore the potential existence of such a link, understanding the composi-
tion of idealistic marital beliefs is of central concern. The conceptualization of
idealistic marital expectations has been informed by the literature on unrealistic or
dysfunctional beliefs and schemas that contribute to marital distress (e.g., Eidelson
& Epstein, 1982; Epstein & Eidelson, 1981). Two dominant themes emerged from
this research: (a) the expectation that partners who really care about each other
249
Journal of Communication, June 2002
should have complete understanding of the partner’s needs and preferences, and
(b) the expectation that sexual relations will be “perfect.” Related to this latter
theme, Jones and Nelson (1996) classified respondents as pessimistic, realistic,
and idealistic, based on descriptions of their marital expectations. They discov-
ered a linear association between respondents’ sense of idealism and their ten-
dency to endorse an “eros” love style that entails a very romanticized view of love
and emphasizes perfection and idyllic physical compatibility. In conceptualizing
idealistic marital expectations, we also drew on the concept of “romantic imagina-
tion” from Bachen and Illouz’s (1996) work on culture and media. They stated that
“romantic imagination incorporates meanings that are socially produced and cul-
turally patterned” (p. 280). They further noted that “the pervasive nature of ro-
mance in popular media and its association with central cultural themes and prac-
tices like fulfillment and marriage have made it a potent cultural symbol” (p. 285).
In accord with the pattern of findings from these past studies, we employed
multiple indicators of idealized marital expectations in the present study. These
included the eros love style with its focus on unusually passionate and compatible
physical intimacy, a measure of idealistic intimacy expectations that stresses com-
plete and in-depth understanding and trust between partners, and a measure of
fantasy rumination developed for this investigation. This latter instrument was
designed to reflect the concept of romantic imagination and assesses blissful con-
templation of a wedding and imagination of a happy marriage.
In this study, we sought to address two research questions about media con-
sumption, idealistic beliefs about marriage, and marital intentions. Derived from
work on cultivation theory, the first research question focused on the relationship
between television viewing and holding idealistic expectations about marriage:
A second research question was driven largely by ideas about the relationship
between attitudes and behaviors as captured in the theory of reasoned action
(TRA; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). According to the TRA, the most proximal predictor
of behavior is one’s behavioral intention, which is largely the product of one’s
relevant attitude and its underlying beliefs. Applied to the issue of marital expec-
tations, one might predict that those who hold idealized views of marriage might
have more immediate intentions to marry. These intentions are, then, likely to
predict actual marriage behavior, thus generating a population of marriages from
which future divorces will come. If a link between television exposure, idealized
attitudes toward marriage, and intentions to marry can be established, we will
have empirical evidence that is consistent with the as yet unsupported claims that
the media are contributing to the high rate of divorce in this country. Thus, the
second research question focused on the association between holding idealistic
expectations about marriage and the desire to enter into a marriage:
250
Television and Marital Expectations
Method
Participants
Participants in this study were 285 undergraduate students enrolled in communi-
cation courses at a large university in the southwestern U.S.; the students partici-
pated in exchange for extra credit toward their course grade. Participants’ mean
age was 20.87 years (SD = 2.50, range: 18–46) with 40% males and 60% females.
The racial composition of the sample was 1% American Indian/Alaskan Native,
5% Asian/Pacific Islander, 4% Black, 11% Hispanic, 78% White, and 1% Other/
Unknown. To be eligible, participants were required to have never been married
so as to eliminate potential respondents with attitudes and beliefs based on direct
experience with marriage. Because there are no clear boundaries to indirect expe-
rience, we did not attempt to control for it by measuring, for example, exposure to
the marriage or divorce of parents, other relatives, or friends. Approximately 45%
of the sample indicated that they were not currently dating, 27% indicated that
they have been dating a boy/girlfriend for less than 1 year, and 27% indicated that
they have been dating a boy/girlfriend for over 1 year.
Procedure
Participants completed a questionnaire with measures described below. The vast
majority of these questionnaires were completed in class, though in some cases,
participants completed them in a smaller laboratory setting.
Measures
Participants’ generalized marital expectations were assessed with open-ended es-
says describing their hypothetical marriages. Participants’ idealized expectations
about marriage were then assessed with three scales that each capture important
aspects of such expectations: fantasy rumination about marriage; idealized expec-
tations for intimacy within the marriage; and a passionate, romanticized love style
(i.e., eros). In addition, the survey included measures of behavioral intentions,
television viewing, genre-specific television viewing, and perceived televi-
sion reality.
Generalized marital expectations. To assess participants’ marital expectations
in terms of issues that were most important to them, they were asked to respond
to the following open-ended instruction: “Imagine that you were married and
were describing your marriage to a close friend. Describe what you think your
marriage would be like, using any concepts and terms that are important to you.”
The questionnaire provided participants with a full page on which to write an
answer. This question was positioned first on the questionnaire to avoid any
potential influence of subsequent questions.
Based on an examination of a random sample of approximately 100 cases, a
coding scheme was developed by the first author to classify responses to this
question. This examination revealed 13 different “fantasy themes” that were evi-
dent in the responses. Two independent coders were then trained to identify
these fantasy themes in the responses and coded all 285 cases, coding 25% of the
sample in common as a basis for assessing intercoder reliability. Because the
251
Journal of Communication, June 2002
categories in the coding scheme were not mutually exclusive, coders made a
dichotomous judgment about the presence or absence of each of the 13 themes in
each participant’s response. These dichotomous judgments were totaled over the
13 themes, resulting in a scale range of 0–13, where high scores indicate a greater
number of different fantasy themes in the response. Participants had, on average,
4.11 (SD = 1.71) out of a possible 13 fantasy themes in their responses to this
question. To evaluate whether high scores could be the result of just writing long
answers, word counts were tabulated for each response and correlated with the
total fantasy theme score. Total word count and total number of fantasy themes in
the responses were significantly correlated, r(280) = .39, p < .001; however, the
response length, as indexed by word count, explains only approximately 15% of
the variance in total fantasy theme scores. Fantasy theme categories, reliabilities,
and sample responses appear in Table 1.
Fantasy rumination. A 4-item measure was created to assess the extent to
which participants think or fantasize about marriage. Items included, “I think my
wedding day will be the happiest day of my life,” “I often catch myself thinking
about how nice it would be to be married,” “I have put a lot of thought into what
kind of wedding I would like to have,” and “I often find myself talking about
romantic relationships.” Questions were answered on 5-point Likert scales. The
four items formed a single factor with an internal consistency of α = .79.
Idealistic expectations about marriage. Tornstam’s (1992) Expectations for Inti-
macy scale is a 5-item index that measures idealized expectations for intimacy in
a close relationship. Sample items include “you should know each other’s inner-
most feelings” and “you should be able to talk openly about everything.” Items
were measured with 5-point Likert scales and had an internal consistency of
α = .82.
Eros love style. The eros subscale from Hendrick and Hendrick’s (1986) love
attitudes scale was administered as an additional index of idealized relational
expectations. This 7-item scale measures a romantic, passionate love style. Items
from the original scale were edited slightly so that they were stated as expecta-
tions. Sample items from this scale include “my partner would fit my ideal stan-
dards of physical beauty/handsomeness” and “my partner and I would be at-
tracted to each other immediately after we first met.” Questions were answered on
5-point Likert scales and had an internal consistency of α = .73.
Marital intentions. To measure respondents’ behavioral intentions to get mar-
ried and stay married, we constructed a 7-item instrument using Fishbein and
Ajzen’s (1975) notion that attitude and behavior measures correspond in terms of
action, target, context, and time as a conceptual guide. Items expected to be
related to an idealized marriage attitude focused on expectations to marry, condi-
tions under which the marriage would occur, and expectations for preserving the
marriage. Items included, “I expect to be engaged or married within the next five
years,” “I would not get married unless I was in love,” “When I get married, I
intend to stay married until I or my spouse dies,” and “When married, I expect to
spend a lot of time with my spouse.” Questions were answered on 5-point Likert
scales, and the seven items formed a univariate factor with an internal consistency
of α = .71. This measure was significantly correlated with related constructs as-
252
Television and Marital Expectations
Table 1. Frequency, Coder Reliability, and Examples of Fantasy Themes From Marriage Essays
a
Low kappa reliability was due to infrequent occurrence, but the variables were retained
as coders exhibited over 90% agreement.
sessed in this study (r = .29 with the eros love style, r = .40 with idealistic expec-
tations about marriage, and r = .53 with marital fantasy rumination).
TV viewing. Following Shrum, Wyer, and O’Guinn’s (1998) procedures, respon-
dents were asked to indicate how many hours of television they watched during
each of four time periods during the average weekday (6am–noon, noon–6pm,
6pm–midnight, midnight–6am), the average Saturday, and the average Sunday.
These data were combined (weighting the “average week day” questions by a
factor of five compared to the “Saturday” or “Sunday” items) and averaged to
create an “average TV viewing hours/day” variable.
Genre-specific TV viewing. A “genre-specific” measure was created to deter-
mine the extent to which respondents regularly watched each of four types of
television programming on 5-point scales: “romantic comedies,” “soap operas,”
“daytime talk shows,” and “reality-based shows about relationships (e.g., A Wed-
ding Story).” Reponses to these four items were combined to form a one-factor,
composite index with reliability of α = .68.
253
Journal of Communication, June 2002
Perceived TV reality. To assess the extent to which participants felt that televi-
sion presented truthful portrayals, we administered Rubin’s (1981) perceived real-
ism of television content scale. This 5-item scale included items such as “television
shows life as it really is” and “television lets me see how other people live” and
had a reliability of α = .82.
Results
254
Television and Marital Expectations
Table 2. Demographic and Media Viewing Predictors of Idealistic Expectations and Marital
Intentions
β R2 β R2 β R2 β R2
Each of these regression analyses has power > .99 to detect a medium-sized effect
of R2 = .13 (Cohen, 1988). Results of these analyses appear in Table 2.
Results for the score on the eros scale indicate no significant effect for sex and
age. However, there was a significant, positive association between watching the
relationship-genre and eros, β = .15, p < .05, suggesting those who watch roman-
tically themed programming are more likely to endorse the eros love style. There
was also a significant, negative effect for perceived TV reality, β = -.15, p < .05,
indicating that the more people feel that TV contains realistic portrayals, the less
they endorse the eros style of love.
Results pertinent to expectations for intimacy indicate that there was no effect
for sex, but there was a significantly negative association with age and expecta-
tions for intimacy, β = -.21, p < .001. Both of the TV viewing variables were
significantly associated with expectations for intimacy. However, whereas TV view-
ing hours per day was negatively associated with expectations for intimacy, β =
-.13, p < .05, watching the relationship genre was positively associated with such
expectations, β = .23, p < .01. Perceived TV reality also proved to be a significant
predictor of expectations for intimacy, β = -.14, p < .05. Here again, the more
people feel that TV contains realistic portrayals, the less they endorse the expec-
tations for intimacy.
The final indicator of idealistic expectations, fantasy rumination, was not sig-
nificantly associated with either sex or age individually; however, these two vari-
255
Journal of Communication, June 2002
256
Television and Marital Expectations
Table 3. Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Among Variables in the Path
Model
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
TV viewing (hrs/day) —
Marital intentionsa -.11 .27*** .24*** -.09 -.12* .29*** .40*** .53*** —
a
The scale range for this variable is 1–5.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p <.001.
β = -.13, p < .05, with those who felt that television contains realistic portrayals
indicating lesser marital intentions. Collectively, these results indicate that media
exposure is associated with marital intentions, provided that one considers the
effect of genre-specific TV viewing. Even controlling for sex and age, those who
watched more of the relationship-specific genre expressed more immediate and
idealistic marital intentions.
As a further examination of the two research questions about TV viewing and
idealistic expectations, and TV viewing and marital intentions, a path analysis was
conducted to model the associations among these variables. For this analysis,
television viewing, perceptions of television realism, and the demographic vari-
ables of sex and age were treated as exogenous variables. The demographic vari-
ables were included because sex exhibited a significant association with watching
relationship genre-specific programming (females watching more) and age being
negatively associated with idealistic expectations about marriage. Similarly, we
included perceptions of television realism because of its negative association with
idealistic expectations and marital intentions. Idealistic expectations about mar-
riage was conceptualized as a latent variable marked by three indicators measured
in this study: the eros love scale, the expectations for intimacy scale, and the
fantasy rumination scale. Finally, marital intentions was conceptualized as the
final variable in the path. Correlations among the variables appear in Table 3 and
257
Journal of Communication, June 2002
TV Realism
Age
-.17
-.17
.24
.22
Sex
Idealistic .70 Personal Marriage
Expectations Expectations/
Intentions
.49
.47
.69
.44 .60
Genre
Eros Idealism Rumination
-.24
.12 -.15
Avg. TV
Hrs/Day
Figure 1. Path analysis of relationships among amount of television viewing, genre, idealistic
expectations of marriage, and personal marriage expectations/intentions.
the relations tested in the final path model are depicted in Figure 1. We arrived at
this final model by initially including relationships among variables based on
significant correlations and systematically removing any paths that did not evi-
dence significant coefficients at p < .05.
The goodness of fit of the path model was assessed by considering the chi-
square value, the Bentler-Bonett normed fit index (NFI), Bollen’s (1986) relative fit
index (RFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The
goodness of a path model’s fit to the sample data can be judged using four criteria:
(a) a χ2/df ratio of 5 or less (Marsh & Hocevar, 1985), (b) an NFI of .90 or greater
(Bentler & Bonett, 1980), (c) an RFI close to 1 (Bollen, 1986), and (d) an RMSEA
less than or equal to .08 (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).
This model had a χ2 = 78.95, df = 23, p < .001. The χ2/df ratio for this model was
3.43, indicating an acceptable fit to the sample data. Other fit indexes were simi-
larly suggestive with the NFI = .99 and the RFI = .98. The RMSEA = .09 suggests a
marginally acceptable fit, though there is some room for improvement in the
specification of this model. Individual path coefficients suggest that idealistic ex-
pectations about marriage are influenced to some extent by age, with young
people expressing more idealistic expectations about marriage than their older
counterparts (β = -.17). Although females watch less television overall, enjoy greater
consumption of the relationship-specific genre, and perceive television program-
258
Television and Marital Expectations
Discussion
This study sought to explore the association among television viewing and hold-
ing idealistic expectations about marriage, as well as holding marital intentions
that were immediate (i.e., “I plan to get married soon”) and idealized (i.e., “my
marriage will last forever”). The results of several different analyses converge to
suggest that, whereas overall television viewing is not a good predictor of either
idealistic expectations of marriage or marital intentions, particular television genre
viewing is. That is, viewing television programming that focuses on marriage and
close relationships (e.g., romantic comedies and soap operas) is associated with
each of these constructs. Results of the path model highlight the seemingly pow-
erful role of idealistic expectations about marriage in shaping intentions to marry.
Demo and Ganong (1994) argued that many people enter into marriage with
idealized and romanticized expectations. Data from this study are consistent with
that argument. For example, on the three measures of idealized expectations, the
means, expressed on a 5-point scale, were 4.50 for expectations for intimacy, 3.90
for the eros love attitude, and 3.53 for fantasy rumination. Further, despite com-
mon knowledge of the high divorce rate in U.S. society, most participants re-
mained confident that they would never be divorced. Two relevant items on the
marital intentions scale, “When I get married, I intend to stay married until I die or
my spouse dies” and “I will have one and only one marriage in my lifetime,” had
means or 4.29 and 4.13, respectively, on a 5-point scale. Collectively, these data
suggest at best a strong sense of optimism on the part of our participants and at
worst a failure to recognize a pressing reality. Our data indicate that, consistent
with conjectures in the literature, maintenance of idealized expectations is clearly
associated with watching a genre of television that focuses heavily on romantic
and marital relationships.
Interestingly, these data also support a recurrent criticism of cultivation theory
regarding the effects of genre-specific programming. Gerbner and his associates
have argued that because conventions of storytelling cut across all genres, the
beliefs and attitudes cultivated by the media should be the same, regardless of the
type of programming viewed. Yet, critics have suggested that exposure to differ-
ent genre types (i.e., content-specific programming; Hawkins & Pingree, 1982)
should have differential cultivation effects, and evidence supporting this notion
259
Journal of Communication, June 2002
260
Television and Marital Expectations
expectations. For example, the contribution of movies, video rentals, novels, and
music were not assessed in our study. Yet, as with television, there is good reason to
suspect that these other forms of media may contribute to a person’s expectations
about romance and marriage and his or her intentions to marry (e.g., Radway, 1983).
One particularly interesting pattern of findings to emerge from this study was
the negative associations between age and holding idealistic expectations about
marriage. Why might older participants hold less idealistic expectations about
marriage than their younger counterparts? One possibility is greater exposure to
long-term personal relationships. A post hoc analysis showed that people who
indicated that they were currently in a dating relationship of over 1 year were on
average 21.8 years of age. On the other hand, those who were currently in a
dating relationship of less than 1 year, or not dating at all were significantly younger
(as determined by an LSD post hoc test) at 20.7 and 20.4 years, respectively.
Unfortunately, this “dating status” variable is an imperfect indicator of relational
experience, as someone who recently broke up from a 5-year relationship could
still be categorized as “single and not dating” despite their extensive experience.
Furthermore, this dating status variable, on its own, did not explain significant
variation in marital attitudes and expectations. These issues notwithstanding, older
participants may be more likely to be in or have been in cohabitating or other very
well-developed relationships, again giving them more relational experience. Aside
from direct experience with relationships, age may be accompanied by greater
indirect experience with relationships. Older participants may have had more
opportunities to learn about and be aware of marital and relational problems from
friends, other family members, and coworkers. This may also explain the negative
associations between age and idealistic attitudes about marriage.
Finally, we note that, consistent with the TRA, positive attitudes about marriage
in the form of idealized expectations were related to marital intentions (i.e., inten-
tions to marry once and soon, until death parts). Although our sample may not
have the opportunity to act on their intentions for months or, more likely, years,
and thus the intention measure may ultimately prove to have a limited association
with actual behavior, the fact that those with more idealized views are more likely
to intend to enter into marriage soon (i.e., at an earlier age) suggests that they are
not only more at risk for being disillusioned and ultimately unhappy in their
marriages but also more at risk to have their marriages end in divorce.
To conclude, we offer evidence that there is a relationship among genre-spe-
cific television viewing, idealized expectations of marriage, and intentions to marry.
Although we cannot assume causality, we are confident that the media do play a
role in developing and reinforcing beliefs about marriage.
References
Bachen, C. M., & Illouz, E. (1996). Imagining romance: Young people’s cultural models of romance
and love. Critical Studies in Mass Communication, 13, 279–307.
Baucom, D. H., Epstein, N., Daiuto, A. D., Carels, R. A., Rankin, L. A., & Burnett, C. K. (1996).
Cognitions in marriage: The relationship between standards and attributions. Journal of Family Psy-
chology, 10, 209–222.
261
Journal of Communication, June 2002
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covari-
ance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606.
Bollen, K. A. (1986). Sample size and Bentler and Bonett’s nonnormed fit index. Psychometrika, 51,
375–377.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S.
Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Demo, D. H., & Ganong, L. H. (1994). Divorce. In P. C. McKenry & S. J. Price (Eds.), Families and
change: Coping with stressful events (pp. 197–218). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Eidelson, R. J., & Epstein, N. (1982). Cognition and relationship maladjustment: Development of a
measure of dysfunctional relationship beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50,
715–720.
Epstein, N., & Eidelson, R. J. (1981). Unrealistic beliefs of clinical couples: Their relationship to expec-
tations, goals, and satisfaction. American Journal of Family Therapy, 9, 13–22.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and
research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Gerbner, G. (1969). Toward “cultural indicators”: The analysis of mass mediated message systems. AV
Communication Review, 17, 137–148.
Gerbner, G., & Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The violence profile. Journal of Communica-
tion, 26(2), 173–199.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Eleey, M. F., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., & Signorielli, N. (1977). TV
violence profile no. 8: The highlights. Journal of Communication, 27(2), 171–180.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1982). Charting the mainstream: Television’s
contributions to political orientations. Journal of Communication, 32(2), 100–127.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1994). Growing up with television: The cultivation
perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp.
17–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotion-
ally. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gross, L., & Jeffries-Fox, S. (1978). What do you want to be when you grow up, little girl? In G.
Tuchman, A. K. Daniles, & J. Benet (Eds.), Hearth and home: Images of women in the mass media
(pp. 240–265). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hawkins, R. P., & Pingree, S. (1982). Television’s influence on social reality. In D. Pearl, L. Bouthilet, &
J. Lazar (Eds.), Television and behavior: Ten years of scientific progress and implications for the
eighties (DHHS Publications No. ADM 82-1196, Vol. 2, pp. 224–247). Washington, DC: U.S. Govern-
ment Printing Office.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. (1986). A theory and method of love. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 50, 392–402.
Hoffman, S. D., & Duncan, G. J. (1988). What are the economic costs of divorce? Demography, 25,
641–645.
Illouz, E. (1998). The lost innocence of love: Romance as a postmodern condition. Theory, Culture,
and Society, 15, 161–186.
262
Television and Marital Expectations
Jones, G. D., & Nelson, E. S. (1996). Expectations of marriage among college students from intact and
non-intact homes. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 26, 171–189.
Katz, E., Blumler, J., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Utilization of mass communication by the individual. In J.
Blumler & E. Katz (Eds.), The uses of mass communication: Current perspectives on gratifications
research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Larson, J. H. (1988). The marriage quiz: College students’ beliefs in selected myths about marriage.
Family Relations, 37, 3–11.
Marsh, H. W., & Hocevar, D. (1985). Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of self-
concept: First- and higher-order factor models and their invariance across groups. Psychological
Bulletin, 97, 562–582.
Morgan, M. (1986). Television and the erosion of regional diversity. Journal of Broadcasting & Elec-
tronic Media, 30, 123–139.
National Center for Health Statistics. (1998). Births, marriages, divorces, and deaths for 1997 (Monthly
Vital Statistics Report, vol. 46, no. 12). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Potter, W. J. (1993). Cultivation theory and research: A conceptual critique. Human Communication
Research, 19, 564–601.
Radway, J. A. (1983). Women read the romance: The interaction of text and context. Feminist Studies,
9, 53–78.
Rubin, A. M. (1981). An examination of television viewing motivations. Communication Research, 8,
141–165.
Shrum, L. J., Wyer, R. S., & O’Guinn, T. C. (1998). The effects of television consumption on social
perceptions: The use of priming procedures to investigate psychological processes. Journal of Con-
sumer Research, 24, 447–458.
Signorielli, N. (1991). Adolescents and ambivalence toward marriage: A cultivation analysis. Youth and
Society, 23, 121–149.
Tornstam, L. (1992). Loneliness in marriage. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 9, 197–217.
Weaver, J., & Wakshlag, J. (1986). Perceived vulnerability to crime, criminal victimization experience,
and television viewing. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 30, 141–158.
263