Research On Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting
Research On Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting
Research On Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting
Accounting
How Does Moral Intensity Impact the Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions
of Professional Accountants?
Tara J. Shawver Lynn H. Clements John T. Sennetti
Article information:
To cite this document: Tara J. Shawver Lynn H. Clements John T. Sennetti . "How
Does Moral Intensity Impact the Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions of
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
INTENTIONS OF PROFESSIONAL
ACCOUNTANTS?
ABSTRACT
erature. First, there are few studies in this area and Jones (1991) identi-
fies that moral intensity is issue contingent; therefore, replication studies
using different scenarios are needed. Second, Bailey, Scott, and Thoma
(2010) have suggested that accounting ethics research has focused too
narrowly on Component II of Rest’s Four-Component Model. None of
the previous studies looked at all three steps in Rest’s Model; therefore,
our manuscript provides an important contribution over the other pre-
vious studies. Third, our sample uses professionals and not students as
surrogates for professionals.
Keywords: Accounting decisions; earnings management; ethical
evaluation; financial statement fraud; harm and pressure; moral
intensity
Accounting numbers are expected to account for, and in the moral sense to
be responsible for, the information they provide. In this way, accounting
numbers are expected to have integrity. Professional accountants, like the
numbers they represent, are expected to have the same integrity (AICPA,
Rule 102, 2013). Public accountants hold themselves out to the public to be
considered exemplary, because they “serve the public interest” and “honor
the public trust” (AICPA, ET Section 53, Article II, 2013). Because of this,
they are expected to have a higher moral sensitivity on matters relating
to the public, such as publicly released financial statements. However,
accountants often experience a variety of pressures to manage or to permit
the management of these statements in meeting analysts’ expectations, in
improving results preceding initial public offerings, in managing stock-
financed acquisitions, and in many other cases. Not all forms of earnings
management are unacceptable. For example, it is natural to delay expenses
during lower revenue periods. But it is easy to manage accounting choices
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 29
sentations of Oracle’s bribes (Jones & Rubin, 2012), all question the sensi-
tivities of accountants or their auditors in making ethical evaluations on
fraud. Recently, the U.S. Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC)
announced that “a broad shuffling of resources in the agency’s enforcement
division will include an increased focus on accounting fraud” and on the
ethical decision making of management and professional accountants
(Eaglesham, 2013). Furthermore, Bailey, Scott, and Thoma (2010) have sug-
gested that accounting ethics research has focused too narrowly on
Component II of Rest’s Four-Component Model. They suggest that if the
ultimate goal of ethics research in accounting is to improve the ethical per-
formance of accountants, then research must consider all four components
(Bailey et al., 2010, p. 18). Therefore, we attempt to understand the moral
judgments and whistleblowing intentions of accounting professionals for
two types of financial statement fraud and address gaps in the literature by
connecting links among moral intensity and three of the four components in
Rest’s model (ethical evaluations, moral judgments, and the intentions to
whistleblow).
There are several studies that attempt to explore determinants of whistle-
blowing intentions; however, none of the following studies examine the
effect of perceived moral intensity. Bernardi, Banzhoff, Martino, and
Savasta (2011) find that students who have whistleblown in the past indi-
cate a higher intention to whistleblow for cheating behaviors even after
controlling for social responsibility response bias. Bernardi, Larkin,
LaBontee, Lapierre, and Morse (2012) find that as the number of reasons a
student provides to not whistleblow increases, the probability of whistle-
blowing decreases. Furthermore, knowing a student who regularly cheats
reduces the probability of whistleblowing. Shawver and Clements (2007)
examine philosophical constructs and find that accounting students use jus-
tice as reasons to whistleblow for situations involving product safety, unfair
loans, early shipments, and reducing bad debts to increase reported income,
while relativism was significant for issues of product safety and unfair loans
30 TARA J. SHAWVER ET AL.
scenarios and different moral problems are needed. Second, Bailey et al.
(2010) have suggested that accounting ethics research has focused too nar-
rowly on Component II of Rest’s Four-Component Model. Prior research
has not examined the first three steps in Rest’s Model within the same
study to predict whistleblowing intentions; therefore, our manuscript pro-
vides an important contribution over previous studies. Third, our sample
uses professional practicing accountants rather than surrogates (students)
as professional accountants, and we test these relationships using both uni-
variate and multivariate models.
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
Consistent with the moral intensity literature, we find that the compo-
nents of moral intensity can be factored into forces of perceived overall
harm and societal pressure and these in turn correlate with ethical evalua-
tions, which vary by level of materiality. Second, we find as in prior
research, the degree of moral intensity may be dependent upon the context.
Third, the ethical evaluations may become affected by perceived overall
harm and whistleblowing intentions may respond more to perceived socie-
tal pressure. Finally, in both cases examined, the professional’s judgments
are most affected by moral intensity, consistent with expectations, since
whistleblowing intentions involve many other mitigating variables, such as
audit reporting or non-audit reporting limited by codes of conduct.
1. Magnitude of consequences,
2. Societal consensus,
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 33
3. Probability of effect,
4. Temporal immediacy,
5. Concentration of effect, and
6. Proximity.
Many empirical studies suggest that moral intensity directly affects the
steps in Rest’s model of ethical decision making. Fleischman, Valentine,
and Finn (2010, p. 29) find “… perceived moral intensity is … associated
with increased ethical issue recognition, ethical judgment, and relief judg-
ment across two scenarios … that require extensive moral evaluations.”
Butterfield, Trevino, and Weaver (2000) suggest that perceptions of social
consensus should enhance recognition of an ethical issue (moral sensitivity).
Prior research has explored the effects of the six components of moral
intensity on moral sensitivity with varying results (Chia & Mee, 2000; Frey,
2000; Kelley & Elm, 2003; Leitsch, 2004; May & Pauli, 2002; Singhapakdi
et al., 1996). Singhapakdi et al. (1996) found that all six dimensions of
moral intensity affect ethical perceptions of marketing professionals. Chia
and Mee (2000) found that social consensus and magnitude of conse-
quences influence the moral sensitivity for business professionals, but found
limited support for the effects of temporal immediacy, proximity, and prob-
ability of effect, and found no support that concentration of effect influ-
enced recognition of moral issues. May and Pauli (2002) did not find a
relationship between these moral intensity characteristics in a situation
involving product safety; however, they found that magnitude of conse-
quences is significant when evaluating environmental issues. Frey (2000)
found that moral sensitivity is related to societal consensus and magnitude
of consequences for New Zealand managers. Kelley and Elm (2003) found
that the nature of client relationships influences the moral intensity of the
issue. Leitsch (2004) found that students’ perceptions of the components of
moral intensity were influenced by the type and intensity of the moral issue.
Valentine and Hollingworth (2012) explored only four of the six items of
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 35
moral intensity and found that magnitude of consequences is the most sig-
nificant factor in a scenario related to poor efficiency and staff reduction,
while temporal immediacy is the most significant factor in a scenario
related to routine maintenance and a replacement of a part that contains
hazardous chemicals.
computer viruses.
Cohen and Bennie (2006) found that magnitude of consequences is con-
sidered the most important factor in three audit-related situations exam-
ined in the study, followed by societal consensus and probability of effect.
Coram, Glavovic, Ng, and Woodliff (2008) found auditors interpret vary-
ing levels of moral intensity for seven situations involving audit quality.
Johnson, Fleischman, Valentine, and Walker (2012) examined the factors
of societal consensus and magnitude of consequences and found that man-
agers may rationalize unethical behaviors when the organization experi-
ences favorable consequences that offset the questionable behavior of
earnings management. The findings may suggest that minor ethical lapses
gradually undermine the ethical climate of the organization. Although not
specifically examining the items suggested by Jones (1991), McDevitt and
Van Hise (2002) found varying effects of moral intensity items on possible
embezzlement and expensing personal items (software and a laptop) as
business expenses.
Prior research suggests that the six moral intensity items represent two
dimensions of “perceived overall harm” and “perceived societal pressure”
that one may face when evaluating ethical decisions. The first dimension,
perceived overall harm, is composed of the four measures: magnitude of
consequences, probability of effect, temporal immediacy, and concentration
of effect. The second dimension, perceived societal pressure, is composed of
the other two measures in the Jones (1991) model: proximity and societal
consensus. Prior research has found that the perceived overall harm and
perceived societal pressure of an action do affect moral judgment
(Clements & Shawver, 2011; Shawver, 2011; Shawver & Shawver, 2013;
Singhapakdi et al., 1996; Sweeney & Costello, 2009; Yang & Wu, 2009).
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 37
There are few studies that examine the impact of moral intensity on moral
judgment in an accounting context, even fewer examining the impact of
moral intensity on moral intentions, and none that examine moral intensity
and actual behavior.
This study explores the two dimensions of “perceived potential harm”
and “perceived societal pressure” that one may face when evaluating ethical
decisions. Therefore, based on prior literature, we present the following
hypotheses:
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
through ethical codes of conduct. Alpern (1982) suggests that engineers are
ethically entitled to encourage ethical behavior. Pletta (1986) suggests that
legislation acts too slowly to protect the public and that engineers should
hold their peers responsible for professional and ethical codes rather than
waiting for legislation to right the wrongs. A study by Ahern and
McDonald (2002) attempts to identify beliefs that could be motivational
factors for whistleblowers in an attempt to explain why some people report
misconduct while others do not. Their study of registered nurses indicates
that whistleblowers believe that nurses are primarily responsible to the
patient and should protect the patient, while non-whistleblowers believe
that nurses have an obligation to follow a physician’s order at all times and
are equally responsible to the patient, the physician, and the employer.
A third area of prior research on whistleblowing is focused on the conse-
quences of not blowing the whistle. Engineering and nursing professions
demand the reporting of wrongdoing because the wrongdoing often
involves life and death issues. Although accounting fraud usually does not
involve life or death, the economic losses can be astounding and may have
disastrous effects on the financial and emotional health of the general pub-
lic. Public outcry has demanded an increased level of integrity, and a
responsibility to report fraudulent accounting practices. Certified Public
Accountants (CPAs) and Certified Management Accountants (CMAs) are
expected to adhere to codes of conduct. According to Section 52 of the
AICPA Code of Professional Conduct, AICPA members “have a continu-
ing responsibility to cooperate with each other to improve the art of
accounting, maintain the public’s confidence, and carry out the profession’s
special responsibilities for self-governance” (AICPA, 1997). Members of
IMA are expected to behave ethically and to commit to ethical professional
practice. If one is aware of unethical situations, the IMA Statement of
Ethical Professional Practice states that an individual should “discuss the
issue with your immediate supervisor except when it appears that the super-
visor is involved. In that case, present the issue to the next level. If you
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 39
cannot achieve a satisfactory resolution, submit the issue to the next man-
agement level. If your immediate superior is the chief executive officer or
equivalent, the acceptable reviewing authority may be a group such as the
audit committee, executive committee, board of directors, board of trustees,
or owners” (IMA Statement of Ethical Professional Practice, 2005).
Public accountants are somewhat limited from whistleblowing by Rule
301 of The AICPA Code of Professional Conduct, which limits revealing
client information. However, this “shall not restrict (a CPA’s) exchange of
information in connection with the investigative or disciplinary
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
proceedings.”
A fourth area of whistleblowing research focuses on the consequences of
whistleblowing. Xu and Ziegenfuss (2003) investigated the issue of whistle-
blowing in the preparation of financial information. Their experiment
examined whether reward systems impact auditors’ whistleblowing beha-
vior. Based on their results, internal auditors are more likely to blow the
whistle for a reward (i.e., cash or a continuing employment contract).
Kaplan, Pany, Samuels, and Zhang (2009) suggest that females’ reporting
intentions are higher than males’ reporting intentions for an anonymous
reporting channel. Kaplan and Schultz (2007) identify that the existence of
an anonymous reporting channel reduces the likelihood of reporting to
non-anonymous channels and that internal audit department quality does
not affect reporting to non-anonymous reporting channels in an experiment
using MBA students. Kaplan, Richmond Pope, and Samuels (2011) suggest
that reporting intentions to an internal auditor are stronger than reporting
intentions to an external auditor in an experiment using MBA students.
Our study involved few internal auditors and mostly public accountants
whose intentions may not be as strong,
Once an action has been evaluated as unethical, an individual is faced
with evaluating whether to report the questionable behavior or do nothing.
Given that perceived overall harm and societal pressure do have an effect
on the evaluation to report actions, we may then expect this to extend to
whistleblowing moral judgments. Therefore, we hypothesize the following:
RESEARCH METHOD
Stice and Stice (2006) provide a discussion of different types of earnings
management and fraud. The two types of accounting manipulations used
in this study are classified by Stice and Stice (2006) as fraudulent reporting
(non-GAAP reporting by improperly capitalizing routine maintenance
expenses) and fraud (ignoring customer returns). The scenarios used in this
study were created uniquely here; however, many of the survey questions
measuring the individual items in the moral intensity scale were adapted
from prior research (Clements & Shawver, 2011; Shawver, 2011; Shawver &
Shawver, 2013; Singhapakdi et al., 1996). A pretest of the instrument was
completed using accounting students prior to collecting the data reported
in this study using accounting professionals. Minor modifications were
made to the instructions and the instrument as a result of the pretest.
Accounting professionals attending (CPA-related) state society-sponsored
continuing education classes were invited to participate in this controlled
experiment and thereby enter a lottery to win a small financial prize valued
at $25. Of the 1,127 attendees, 220 agreed to participate, providing a 20%
response rate. Table 1 provides additional selected demographic informa-
tion about the participants of this study. Of the 220 participants, most
(82%) identified themselves as accountants and not “management” or
“other” positions, most (71%) identified themselves as male, and most
(74%) as older than 50.
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 41
Table 1. Demographics.
Panel A: Gender of Participants
Female 62 28.18
Male 157 71.36
Prefer not to answer 1 0.06
Total 220
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
20 29 4 1.82
30 39 12 5.45
40 49 41 18.64
50 59 89 40.45
60 69 74 33.64
Total 220
Panel C: Occupation
this action will cause any harm” (reverse coded). Temporal immediacy is
measured by responding to, “This action will not cause any harm in the
immediate future” (reverse coded). Proximity is measured by responding
to, “If the controller is a personal friend, the action is wrong.”
Concentration of effect is measured by responding to, “The action will
harm very few people, if any” (reverse coded). The magnitude of conse-
quences, probability of effect, temporal immediacy, and concentration of
effect variables were reverse coded so that all moral intensity items are eval-
uated on a 7-point scale with the high end of the scale indicating more
moral intensity and the opposite indicating low moral intensity.
Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations for the variables in
this study. Appendix B presents the two scenarios. Each participant
received one of two possible sets, and each set, I or II, contained two of the
Variables are measured on a 7-point Likert scale, 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree.
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 43
four scenarios used in this study. For set I, participants answered questions
about capitalization of expenses. For set II, participants answered ques-
tions about ignoring customer returns. Appendix C presents the questions
used in this study and identifies how the study variables relate to Rest’s
and Jones’ models.
In Table 4, we report the correlation matrix for the three dependent vari-
ables (ethical evaluation, whistleblowing moral judgment, and whistleblow-
ing intention) and two independent variables (perceived overall harm and
perceived societal pressure) used in this study. An increase in the perceived
overall harm variable correlates to a decrease in the ethical evaluation vari-
able (closer to unethical), and hence negative correlations of perceived
overall harm and ethical evaluation, whereas an increase in perceived socie-
tal pressure corresponds to an increase in whistleblowing, as shown in the
positive correlations.
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
H1 through H4
To examine the steps in Rest’s model, Table 5 shows the results of sev-
eral univariate analyses that are consistent with Rest’s Four-Component
Model of ethical decision making. For both situations, moral judgments
(Rest’s model, step 2) are dependent upon ethical evaluations (Rest’s
model, step 1).
In Table 6, the hierarchical regression (Rest’s model and steps 1, 2,
and 3) confirms that whistleblowing intentions are dependent upon whis-
tleblowing moral judgment. The dependent variable in each model is
whistleblowing intention. In the hierarchical regression, the variable iden-
tifying ethical evaluation is entered first, followed by whistleblowing
moral judgment. For both situations, whistleblowing intention (Rest’s
model, step 3) is dependent upon moral judgment (Rest’s model, step 2).
Table 4. Simple Linear Correlation Coefficients.
46
Variable Ethical Whistleblowing Whistleblowing Perceived Perceived Gender Age
Evaluation Moral Judgment Intention Overall Harm Societal Pressure
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
Panel A: Correlation coefficients for perceived overall harm and perceived societal pressure on evaluation in the vignette capitalizing expenses
Panel B: Correlation coefficients for perceived overall harm and perceived societal pressure on evaluation in the vignette ignoring customer
returns
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed); *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed).
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 47
Variable t Significance
Variable t Significance
Variable t Significance
Variable t Significance
H3 and H4
The evidence from Tables 4 and 7 supports H3 and H4 that the two dimen-
sions of moral intensity (specifically, perceived overall harm and societal pres-
sure, respectively) affect the moral judgment to whistleblow (i.e., the staff
accountant should blow the whistle). Similar to the ethical evaluations in
Table 7, whistleblowing judgment is conditionally affected by both dimen-
sions of moral intensity and whistleblowing intention is more affected by per-
ceived societal pressure than perceived overall harm. Perceived societal
pressure is significant in both situations, supporting H4. Similar to findings in
H2, the results may suggest that this sample of accountants may seek gui-
dance from colleagues or agree with the sentiments of a majority of the pro-
fession when considering evaluating whether someone should whistleblow.
H5 and H6
The evidence from Tables 4 and 7 does not support H5, that the greater the
perceived harm, the greater the likelihood of the intention to whistleblow,
50 TARA J. SHAWVER ET AL.
but Table 7 does conditionally support H6, that the greater the perceived
social pressure, the greater this intention. We asked the participants in this
study to evaluate whether most staff accountants would report the requests
made by the controller (the dependent variable) in each scenario, with the
factors of perceived overall harm and perceived societal pressure as the
independent variables, respectively, in the analyses.
Table 8 finishes Tables 4 7 and provides a unique simultaneous equa-
tions approach to Rest’s model and steps 1, 2, and 3. All three of these
multivariate equations are intercorrelated, as shown in Table 8. In much
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
the same way that multicollinear variables become less significant in the
presence of more dominant ones in univariate models, so do the moral
intensity variables become less statistically significant for steps 1 and 3, in
the presence of the more dominant step 2 (Dependent variable: moral judg-
ment) which shows the strongest impact from moral intensity in both panels.
Only in Panel B, Table 8, the case of clear fraud, do the moral intensity
forces combine for an interaction effect. So, even though whistleblowing
intention can be modeled in the univariate form by perceived social pressure
and not perceived overall harm in Table 7, Panel D, that Table 7 model is
due to the degree of moral judgments made, as shown by Table 8.
We know from prior research that other factors may become more
important when considering whistleblowing intentions after judgments are
made (and we see this in the changing R2 values in Panel A, from 0.509 to
0.159 and Panel B, from 0.604 to 0.025, Table 8). These factors may include
the fear of retaliation (Keenan, 1990), the reward systems (Xu &
Ziegenfuss, 2003), the role prescription (Miceli & Near, 1991), the personal
costs (Ayers & Kaplan, 2005), the reporting channels (Kaplan & Schultz,
2007), and the encouragement from ethical leaders (Bhal & Dadhich, 2011).
Professional accountants responding to this study are mostly public (but
non-auditor) accountants following their state’s version of the AICPA
Code of Professional Conduct Rule 301, Client Confidentiality, and Rule
501, Acts Discreditable (a rule often enhanced by each state). These rules
suggest personal costs to CPAs who may know of wrongdoings but do not
report them, such as in cases A and B studied here, or actions not permitted
for CPAs (e.g., convictions of drunk driving in Texas). The Texas State
Board Report lists individuals who submit their licenses back to the state
(and who may be reinstated later), in order to limit their liability exposure.
CONCLUSIONS
This study explores the effects of moral intensity (Jones, 1991) on ethical sen-
sitivity using a sample of professional accountants to examine two situations
Moral Judgments and Whistleblowing Intentions 51
(2010), and Johnson et al. (2012), Shawver (2011), Clements and Shawver
(2011), and Shawver and Shawver (2013). Furthermore, this study addresses
gaps in the literature by examining the important issues facing accounting
professionals and the connecting links among ethical evaluations, moral judg-
ments, and the intentions to whistleblow.
We asked accounting professionals to evaluate several situations to inves-
tigate the factors most important when evaluating accounting situations
involving earnings manipulations, and to determine whether an accountant
should and would report questionable actions. This study provides evidence
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
inquiries on fraud. The cases examined here were not explicitly stated as
audit clients and the respondents included only eight external and seven
internal auditors, so we must presume that the moral intensity on whistle-
blowing fraud from the rest of the respondents was not as impacted as was
their judgments. When fraudulent financial reporting is not reported but
later discovered, investor confidence decreases and the efficiencies of invest-
ment markets are impaired. This becomes particularly relevant now that
the SEC has increased its software capabilities to identify financial state-
ment fraud (Eaglesham, 2013).
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors wish to extend their sincere appreciation to Cynthia Jeffrey and
two anonymous reviewers, whose feedback greatly improved this manuscript.
54 TARA J. SHAWVER ET AL.
REFERENCES
Ahern, K., & McDonald, S. (2002). The beliefs of nurses who were involved in a whistleblow-
ing event. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 38(3), 303 309.
Alpern, K. D. (1982). Engineers as moral heroes. In V. Weil (Ed.), Beyond whistleblowing
(pp. 400 451). Chicago: Center for the Study of Ethics in the Professions, Illinois
Institute of Technology.
American Institute of Certified Public Accountants. (1997). Code of Professional Conduct as
Adopted January 12, 1988, amended January 14, 1992 and October 28, 1997. ET
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
Coram, P., Glavovic, A., Ng, J., & Woodliff, D. (2008). The moral intensity of reduced audit
quality acts. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory, 27(1), 127 149.
Eaglesham, J. (2013). Accounting fraud targeted. The Wall Street Journal, 261(124), C1 C6.
Retrieved from http://online.wsj.com/article/SB10001424127887324125504578509241
215284044.html. Accessed on May 27, 2013.
Fleischman, G., Valentine, S., & Finn, D. (2010). Moral intensity, ethical reasoning, and
equitable relief judgments. Research on Professional Responsibility and Ethics in
Accounting, 14, 79 112.
Frey, B. F. (2000). The impact of moral intensity on decision making in a business context.
Journal of Business Ethics, 26, 181 195.
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
May, D. R., & Pauli, K. P. (2002). The role of moral intensity in ethical decision making.
Business and Society, 41(1), 84 117.
McDevitt, R., & Van Hise, J. (2002). Influences in ethical dilemmas of increasing intensity.
Journal of Business Ethics, 40(3), 261 274.
Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. P. (1984). The relationships among beliefs, organizational position,
and whistleblowing status: A discriminant analysis. Academy of Management Journal,
27, 687 705.
Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. P. (1991). Whistleblowing as an organizational process. Research in
the Sociology of Organizations, 9, 139 200.
Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. P. (1992). Blowing the whistle: The legal implications for companies
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
Wright, G., Cullinan, C., & Bline, D. (1997). Recognizing ethical issues: The joint influence of
ethical sensitivity and moral intensity. Research on Accounting Ethics, 4, 29 52.
Xu, Y., & Ziegenfuss, D. E. (2003). Reward systems, moral reasoning, and internal auditors’
reporting wrongdoing. Journal of Business and Psychology, 22(4), 323 331.
Yang, H., & Wu, W. (2009). The effect of moral intensity on ethical decision making in
accounting. Journal of Moral Education, 38(3), 335 351.
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)
58 TARA J. SHAWVER ET AL.
Vignette
Please indicate how strongly you agree or disagree with the following state-
ments by circling one answer for each of the following statements using the
following scale:
APPENDIX B: VIGNETTES
Capitalization of Expenses
expensed. The adjustment would increase net income by 4% for this pub-
licly traded company. The accountant agreed to make the adjustment.
Question Variable
The adjustment made by the staff accountant is ethical. Ethical evaluation, Rest’s model
step 1
The staff accountant in the scenario should report this Whistleblowing moral judgment,
Downloaded by Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan At 02:10 14 February 2016 (PT)