Rol Empathy
Rol Empathy
Rol Empathy
Development of Antisocial
Behaviors
Megan Schaffer
Stephanie Clark
Elizabeth L. Jeglic
John Jay College of Criminal Justice, New York
This study examined the relationship among parenting, empathy, and antisocial
behavior. Two hundred forty-four undergraduate students attending an urban
university completed self-report questionnaires assessing their antisocial
behavior, empathy, and mothers’ and fathers’ parenting styles. Support was
found for a model in which maternal permissive parenting contributed
directly and indirectly to antisocial behavior, through its effects on cognitive
and emotional empathy development. Findings are discussed in relation to
the current literature on empathy, parenting, and adult antisocial behavior.
Authors’ Note: Address correspondence to Megan Schaffer, MA, John Jay College of
Criminal Justice, Department of Psychology, 445 West 59th Street, New York, NY 10019;
e-mail: mschaffer@gc.cuny.edu.
586
Method
Measures
The following measures were used to capture the variables of interest in
this study.
Figure 1
The Hypothesized Models
Emotional Empathy
Antisocial Behavior
Cognitive Empathy
Antisocial Behavior
Table 1
Demographic Characteristics
Characteristic n (%)
Age
18-22 200 (82.0)
Older than 22 44 (18.0)
Gender
Female 174 (71.3)
Male 70 (28.7)
Ethnicity
Hispanic 112 (45.9)
Caucasian 50 (20.5)
African American 46 (18.9)
Asian 15 (6.1)
American Indian 1 (0.4)
Other 20 (8.2)
Religion
Catholic 124 (50.8)
Jewish/Muslim/Protestant/Buddhist 30 (12.2)
Other religion 56 (23.0)
No religion 34 (13.9)
Current annual family income
Less than $10,000 47 (19.3)
$10,000-$20,000 47 (19.3)
$20,001-$30,000 53 (21.7)
$30,001-$40,000 33 (13.5)
$40,001-$50,000 22 (9.0)
More than $50,000 42 (17.2)
Annual income of the childhood home
Less than $10,000 27 (11.1)
$10,000-$20,000 67 (27.5)
$20,001-$30,000 40 (16.4)
$30,001-$40,000 38 (15.6)
$40,001-$50,000 28 (11.5)
More than $50,000 44 (18.0)
Arrest
Yes 15 (6.1)
compassion for others more often; higher scores on the Perspective Taking
subscale indicate that an individual frequently takes another’s viewpoint.
Employing these two seven-item subscales as independent measures of
cognitive and emotional empathy follows a convention utilized by many
current empathy researchers and is supported by validity and reliability stud-
ies (Davis, 1983b; Duriez, 2004; Jolliffe & Farrington, 2004; Spinella, 2005).
In our sample, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the Perspective Taking and
Empathic Concern subscales were .75 and .74, respectively.
Results
Table 2
Mean Scores of Participants on Administered Measures
Measure M (SD)
Antisocial behavior
Self-Report Delinquency Scale 17.9 (31.9)
Empathy
Interpersonal Reactivity Index
Empathic Concern subscale 19.9 (4.9)
Perspective Taking subscale 17.3 (5.2)
Maternal authority style
Permissive subscale 25.1 (6.8)
Authoritarian subscale 31.6 (7.7)
was nonsignificant, χ2(1, N = 244) = .068, p > .05, and χ2(1, N = 244) = .068,
p > .05, respectively (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2000). The comparative fit index
(Bentler, 1980) for each model also indicates that the models fit well, Model
1 = 1.00, Model 2 = 1.00 (i.e., both over .95; Bentler, 1992; Hu & Bentler,
1999). Finally, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), which
estimates the lack of fit in a model when it is compared to a perfect model,
shows that the models were not significantly different from a statistically per-
fect model, Model 1 = 0.00, Model 2 = 0.00 (i.e., less than .05; Browne &
Cudeck, 1993).
Figure 2 shows the models and the standardized regression weights for
structural paths. In these models, the relationships between permissive par-
enting and empathy, whether cognitive or emotional, and empathy and anti-
social behavior are significant. Because the three paths between the predictor
variable (permissive maternal parenting), the potential mediating variable
(cognitive or emotional empathy), and the outcome variable (antisocial
behavior) were all significant, no formal tests for mediation were performed.
That is, the inclusion of the paths between the predictor and the third variable
and between the third variable and the outcome measure in each model did
not render the predictor to outcome path nonsignificant. This result precludes
the potential for an occurrence of a mediating relationship between the pre-
dictor and the third variable (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Holmbeck, 1997).
Discussion
Figure 2
Results of the Tests of the Hypothesized Models
Emotional Empathy
–.22** –.16*
.10
Cognitive Empathy
*
p < .05. **p < .01. All parameter estimates are standardized.
support was found for a model in which permissive, but not authoritarian,
maternal parenting contributed to low levels of empathy and antisocial
behavior in young adulthood. Our study is one of the first to propose and test
a causal pathway to adult antisocial behavior that includes social ability
(i.e., empathic abilities) and parenting variables (Knutson et al., 2004).
Of particular importance among our findings is the impact of permissive
parenting on empathy and antisocial behavior development. That is, our
results show that lax parenting influences antisocial behavior through its
direct effect on behavior and through its effect on hindering the development
of empathic abilities, which also contribute to behavioral tendencies. This
finding adds needed explanation to findings that show parenting style to be
an important predictor of antisocial behavior (Knutson et al., 2004).
Furthermore, it suggests that dysfunctional parenting and low empathy may
contribute to antisocial behavior, an additive effect that has been obscured in
some multivariate analyses that have found the effects of dysfunctional
parenting on antisocial behavior in children to be moderated by empathy
level (Frick, 2006).
Interestingly, permissive, but not authoritarian, maternal parenting style
is associated with decreased empathy and antisocial behavior development.
Authoritarian and permissive parenting styles have been independently
The limitations to this study suggest directions for future research. Self-
report measures were chosen because they provide an efficient, cost-effective
means of collecting data. Self-report measures are also recognized as the best
means currently available for obtaining accurate and complete information
about an individual’s antisocial behavior (Jolliffe & Farrington, 2004). Asking
individuals to self-report information about long-ago relations, however, may
be less than ideal, because resulting data may be inaccurate owing to recall
problems. To get the most accurate data, future studies may wish to obtain
multiple measures of each construct. This study, as one of the first to use path
analysis in this body of literature, suggests a feasible direction for the incor-
poration of such measurement into study design. Structural equation modeling
allows individuals to obtain multiple measures of one variable as a means of
refining the measurement of a construct (Byrne, 2001).
Although prospective studies may still offer the most thorough information—
and, therefore, understanding—of how antisocial behavior develops, such
studies are time-consuming and expensive. Utilizing path analysis or struc-
tural equation modeling to analyze survey data may provide a feasible,
timely, and cost-effective manner to improve current understanding of risk
factors for antisocial behavior. The employ of structural equation modeling
and path analysis is valuable in that it allows for complex interactions
among the risk factors (and protective factors), relationships that have long
been hypothesized (Moffitt, 1993) but rarely studied (Farrington, 2000;
Gibson, Piquero, & Tibbetts, 2001), to be discerned and quantified. Future
studies should subject a wider array of known risk factors for antisocial
behavior, such as biological factors and social, community, and other familial
variables (Augustino, 2006; Knutson et al., 2004; Reti et al., 2002; Spinella,
2005), to this type of multivariate analysis.
References
Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). Amos 5.0 user’s guide. Chicago: Small Waters.
Augustino, B. (2006, November). Biological predispositions to crime: A review of the state of
the literature and future recommendations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Society of Criminology, Los Angeles.
Barnow, S., Lucht, M., & Freyberger, H. J. (2005). Correlates of aggressive and delinquent
conduct problems in adolescence. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 24-39.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social
psychology research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consideration. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.
Beck, J. E., & Shaw, D. S. (2005). The influence of perinatal complications and environ-
mental adversity on boys’ antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
46(1), 35-46.
Bentler, P. M. (1980). Multivariate analysis with latent variables: Causal modeling. Annual
Review of Psychology, 31, 419-456.
Bentler, P. M. (1992). On the fit of models to covariances methodology to the Bulletin.
Psychological Bulletin, 112, 400-404.
Blair, R. J. R., Monson, J., & Frederickson, N. (2001). Moral reasoning and conduct prob-
lems in children with emotional and behavioral difficulties. Personality and Individual
Differences, 31, 799-811.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen &
J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 445-455). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Buri, J. R. (1991). Parental authority questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment,
57(1), 110-119.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications,
and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
D’Antonio, A. (1997). The effect of empathy on aggression and antisocial behavior among
low-impulsive and high-impulsive children and adolescents. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 57(12B), 7721-7818.
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS
Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
Davis, M. H. (1983a). The effects of dispositional empathy on emotional reactions and helping:
A multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality, 51(2), 167-184.
Davis, M. H. (1983b). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidi-
mensional approach. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44, 113-126.
de Kemp, R., Overbeek, G., de Wied, M., Engels, R., & Scholte, R. (2007). Early adoles-
cent empathy, parental support, and antisocial behavior. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
168(1), 5-18.
Duriez, B. (2004). Taking a closer look at the religion–empathy relationship: Are religious
people nicer people? Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 7, 249-254.
Ehrensaft, M. K., Wasserman, G. A., Verdelli, L., Greenwald, S., Miller, L. S., & Davies, M.
(2003). Maternal antisocial behavior, parenting practices and behavior problems in boys at
risk for antisocial behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 12, 27-40.
Elliot, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Ageton, S. (1985). Explaining delinquency and drug use.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Farrington, D. (2000). Explaining and preventing crime: The globalization of knowledge—
The American Society of Criminology 1999 presidential address. Criminology, 38, 1-24.
Robinson, R., Roberts, W., Strayer, J., & Koopman, R. (2007). Empathy and emotional
responsiveness in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents. Social Development, 16(3),
555-579.
Roche, K. M., Ensminger, M. E., & Cherlin, A. J. (2007). Variations in parenting and adoles-
cent outcomes among African American and Latino families living in low-income, urban
areas. Journal of Family Issues, 28(7), 882-909.
Shechtman, Z. (2003). Cognitive and affective empathy in aggressive boys: Implications for
counseling. International Journal for the Advancement of Counseling, 24, 211-222.
Smith, C. A., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Continuities in antisocial behavior and parenting
across three generations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 230-247.
Soenens, B., Duriez, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Goossens, L. (2007). The intergenerational trans-
mission of empathy-related responding in adolescence: The role of maternal support.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 299-311.
Spinella, M. (2005). Prefrontal substrates of empathy: Psychometric evidence in a community
sample. Biological Psychology, 70(3), 175-181.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2000). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn
& Bacon.
Thornberry, T., Freeman-Gallant, A., Lizotte, A., Krohn, M., & Smith, C. (2003). Linked lives:
The intergenerational transmission of antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 31(2), 171-184.
Verona, E., Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2005). Psychopathy and suicidality in female
offenders: Mediating influences of personality and abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 73(6), 1065-1073.
Verona, E., & Sachs-Ericsson, N. (2005). The intergenerational transmission of externalizing
behaviors in adult participants: The mediating role of childhood abuse. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(6), 1135-1145.
Williams, K., McAndrew, A., Learn, T., Harms, P., & Paulhus, D. L. (2001, August). The dark
triad returns: Entertainment preferences and anti-social behavior among narcissists,
Machiavellians, and psychopaths. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American
Psychological Association, San Francisco.
Megan Schaffer is a fourth-year forensic clinical psychology PhD student at John Jay College
of Criminal Justice, City University of New York. Her research interests include suicidal and
aggressive behavior and depression. Before attending CUNY, she received her BA from the
College of William and Mary. She currently serves as an adjunct professor at John Jay College.
Stephanie Clark has recently completed her master’s in forensic psychology at John Jay
College of Criminal Justice. Her research interests include factors affecting antisocial and sui-
cidal behavior, treatment evaluation for sex offenders, and procedural justice judgments in a
restorative justice context.
Elizabeth L. Jeglic is an associate professor at the John Jay College of Criminal Justice. Her
research interests include the development and evaluation of treatment programs for suicidal
and offending populations.