Unit Xi - Sensation and Perception PDF
Unit Xi - Sensation and Perception PDF
Unit Xi - Sensation and Perception PDF
1. SENSATION
What does it mean to sense something? Sensory receptors are specialized neurons that
respond to specific types of stimuli. When sensory information is detected by a sensory
receptor, sensation has occurred. For example, light that enters the eye causes chemical
changes in cells that line the back of the eye. These cells relay messages, in the form of
action potentials (as you learned when studying biopsychology), to the central nervous
system. The conversion from sensory stimulus energy to action potential is known as
transduction. You have probably known since elementary school that we have five senses:
vision, hearing (audition), smell (olfaction), taste (gustation), and touch (somato-sensation).
It turns out that this notion of five senses is over simplified. We also have sensory systems
that provide information about balance (the vestibular sense), body position and
movement (proprioception and kinesthesia), pain (nociception), and temperature (thermo-
perception).
The sensitivity of a given sensory system to the relevant stimuli can be expressed as an
absolute threshold. Absolute threshold refers to the minimum amount of stimulus energy
that must be present for the stimulus to be detected 50% of the time. Another way to think
about this is by asking how dim can a light be or how soft can a sound be and still be
detected half of the time. The sensitivity of our sensory receptors can be quite amazing. It
has been estimated that on a clear night, the most sensitive sensory cells in the back of the
eye can detect a candle flame 30 miles away (Okawa & Sampath, 2007). Under quiet
conditions, the hair cells (the receptor cells of the inner ear) can detect the tick of a clock
20 feet away (Galanter, 1962).
It is also possible for us to get messages that are presented below the threshold for
conscious awareness these are called subliminal messages. A stimulus reaches a
physiological threshold when it is strong enough to excite sensory receptors and
send nerve impulses to the brain: This is an absolute threshold. A message below
that threshold is said to be subliminal: We receive it, but we are not consciously aware of it.
Over the years there has been a great deal of speculation about the use of subliminal
messages in advertising, rock music, and self-help audio programs.
Research evidence shows that in laboratory settings, people can process and respond to
information outside of awareness. But this does not mean that we obey these messages like
zombies; in fact, hidden messages have little effect on behavior outside the laboratory
(Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Rensink, 2004; Nelson, 2008; Radel, Sarrazin, Legrain, &
Gobancé, 2009; Loersch, Durso, & Petty, 2013).
Absolute thresholds are generally measured under incredibly controlled conditions in
situations that are optimal for sensitivity. Sometimes, we are more interested in how much
difference in stimuli is required to detect a difference between them. This is known as
the just noticeable difference (JND) or difference threshold. Unlike the absolute
threshold, the difference threshold changes depending on the stimulus intensity.
As an example, imagine yourself in a very dark movie theater. If an audience member
were to receive a text message on her cell phone, which caused her screen to light up,
chances are that many people would notice the change in illumination in the theater.
However, if the same thing happened in a brightly lit arena during a basketball game, very
few people would notice. The cell phone brightness does not change, but its ability to be
detected as a change in illumination varies dramatically between the two contexts. Ernst
Weber proposed this theory of change in difference threshold in the 1830s, and it has
become known as Weber's law: The difference threshold is a constant fraction of the
original stimulus, as the example illustrates.
2. PERCEPTION
While our sensory receptors are constantly collecting information from the environment,
it is ultimately how we interpret that information that affects how we interact with the
world. Perception refers to the way sensory information is organized, interpreted,
and consciously experienced. Perception involves both bottom-up and top-down
processing.
Ø Bottom-up processing refers to the fact that perceptions are built from sensory
input.
In the early part of the 20th century, Max Wertheimer published a paper demonstrating
that individuals perceived motion in rapidly flickering static images an insight that came to
him as he used a child's toy tachistoscope. Wertheimer, and his assistants Wolfgang Kohler and
Kurt Kofka, who later became his partners, believed that perception involved more than
simply combining sensory stimuli. This belief led to a new movement within the field of
psychology known as Gestalt psychology. The word gestalt literally means form or
pattern, but its use reflects the idea that the whole is different from the sum of its parts. In
other words, the brain creates a perception that is more than simply the sum of available
sensory inputs, and it does so in predictable ways. Gestalt psychologists translated these
predictable ways into principles by which we organize sensory information. As a result,
Gestalt psychology has been extremely influential in the area of sensation and perception
(Rock & Palmer, 1990).
PRINCIPLES:
1. One Gestalt principle is the figure-ground relationship. According to this
principle, we tend to segment our visual world into figure and ground. Figure is
the object or person that is the focus of the visual f i eld, while the ground is the
background. As our perception can vary tremendously, depending on what is
perceived as f i gure and what is perceived as ground. Presumably, our ability to
interpret sensory information depends on what we label as figure and what we
label as ground in any particular case, although this assumption has been called
into question (Peterson & Gibson, 1994; Vecera & O'Reilly, 1998).
Figure: The concept of figure-ground relationship explains why this image can be
perceived either as a vase or as a pair of faces.
2. Another Gestalt principle for organizing sensory stimuli into meaningful
perception is proximity. This principle asserts that things that are close to one
another tend to be grouped together.
Figure: When looking at this array of dots, we likely perceive alternating rows of colors. We
are grouping these dots according to the principle of similarity.
Two additional Gestalt principles are the law of continuity (or good continuation) and
closure.
4. The law of continuity suggests that we are more likely to perceive continuous,
smooth owing lines rather than jagged, broken lines.
Figure: Good continuation would suggest that we are more likely to perceive this as
two overlapping lines, rather than four lines meeting in the center.
5. The principle of closure states that we organize our perceptions
into complete objects rather than as a series of parts.
Gestalt theorists have been incredibly influential in the areas of sensation and
perception. Gestalt principles such as figure-ground relationship, grouping by
proximity or similarity, the law of good continuation, and closure are all used to
help explain how we organize sensory information. Our perceptions are not
infallible, and they can be influenced by bias, prejudice, and other factors.