Isaac Asimov Asks, "How Do People Get New Ideas?"
Isaac Asimov Asks, "How Do People Get New Ideas?"
Isaac Asimov Asks, "How Do People Get New Ideas?"
Presumably, the process of creativity, whatever it is, is essentially the same in all
its branches and varieties, so that the evolution of a new art form, a new gadget, a
new scientific principle, all involve common factors. We are most interested in
the “creation” of a new scientific principle or a new application of an old one, but
we can be general here.
One way of investigating the problem is to consider the great ideas of the past
and see just how they were generated. Unfortunately, the method of generation is
never clear even to the “generators” themselves.
But what if the same earth-shaking idea occurred to two men, simultaneously and
independently? Perhaps, the common factors involved would be illuminating.
Consider the theory of evolution by natural selection, independently created by
Charles Darwin and Alfred Wallace.
There is a great deal in common there. Both traveled to far places, observing
strange species of plants and animals and the manner in which they varied from
place to place. Both were keenly interested in finding an explanation for this, and
both failed until each happened to read Malthus’s “Essay on Population.”
Both then saw how the notion of overpopulation and weeding out (which
Malthus had applied to human beings) would fit into the doctrine of evolution by
natural selection (if applied to species generally).
Obviously, then, what is needed is not only people with a good background in a
particular field, but also people capable of making a connection between item 1
and item 2 which might not ordinarily seem connected.
Undoubtedly in the first half of the 19th century, a great many naturalists had
studied the manner in which species were differentiated among themselves. A
great many people had read Malthus. Perhaps some both studied species and read
Malthus. But what you needed was someone who studied species, read Malthus,
and had the ability to make a cross-connection.
That is the crucial point that is the rare characteristic that must be found. Once
the cross-connection is made, it becomes obvious. Thomas H. Huxley is
supposed to have exclaimed after reading On the Origin of Species, “How stupid
of me not to have thought of this.”
But why didn’t he think of it? The history of human thought would make it seem
that there is difficulty in thinking of an idea even when all the facts are on the
table. Making the cross-connection requires a certain daring. It must, for any
cross-connection that does not require daring is performed at once by many and
develops not as a “new idea,” but as a mere “corollary of an old idea.”
It is only afterward that a new idea seems reasonable. To begin with, it usually
seems unreasonable. It seems the height of unreason to suppose the earth was
round instead of flat, or that it moved instead of the sun, or that objects required a
force to stop them when in motion, instead of a force to keep them moving, and
so on.
A person willing to fly in the face of reason, authority, and common sense must
be a person of considerable self-assurance. Since he occurs only rarely, he must
seem eccentric (in at least that respect) to the rest of us. A person eccentric in one
respect is often eccentric in others.
Consequently, the person who is most likely to get new ideas is a person of good
background in the field of interest and one who is unconventional in his habits.
(To be a crackpot is not, however, enough in itself.)
Once you have the people you want, the next question is: Do you want to bring
them together so that they may discuss the problem mutually, or should you
inform each of the problem and allow them to work in isolation?
The presence of others can only inhibit this process, since creation is
embarrassing. For every new good idea you have, there are a hundred, ten
thousand foolish ones, which you naturally do not care to display.
Nevertheless, a meeting of such people may be desirable for reasons other than
the act of creation itself.
No two people exactly duplicate each other’s mental stores of items. One person
may know A and not B, another may know B and not A, and either knowing A
and B, both may get the idea—though not necessarily at once or even soon.
Furthermore, the information may not only be of individual items A and B, but
even of combinations such as A-B, which in themselves are not significant.
However, if one person mentions the unusual combination of A-B and another
the unusual combination A-C, it may well be that the combination A-B-C, which
neither has thought of separately, may yield an answer.
It seems to me then that the purpose of cerebration sessions is not to think up new
ideas but to educate the participants in facts and fact-combinations, in theories
and vagrant thoughts.
But how to persuade creative people to do so? First and foremost, there must be
ease, relaxation, and a general sense of permissiveness. The world in general
disapproves of creativity, and to be creative in public is particularly bad. Even to
speculate in public is rather worrisome. The individuals must, therefore, have the
feeling that the others won’t object.
If a single individual present has a much greater reputation than the others, or is
more articulate, or has a distinctly more commanding personality, he may well
take over the conference and reduce the rest to little more than passive obedience.
The individual may himself be extremely useful, but he might as well be put to
work solo, for he is neutralizing the rest.
The optimum number of the group would probably not be very high. I should
guess that no more than five would be wanted. A larger group might have a
larger total supply of information, but there would be the tension of waiting to
speak, which can be very frustrating. It would probably be better to have a
number of sessions at which the people attending would vary, rather than one
session including them all. (This would involve a certain repetition, but even
repetition is not in itself undesirable. It is not what people say at these
conferences, but what they inspire in each other later on.)
For best purposes, there should be a feeling of informality. Joviality, the use of
first names, joking, relaxed kidding are, I think, of the essence—not in
themselves, but because they encourage a willingness to be involved in the folly
of creativeness. For this purpose I think a meeting in someone’s home or over a
dinner table at some restaurant is perhaps more useful than one in a conference
room.
To feel guilty because one has not earned one’s salary because one has not had a
great idea is the surest way, it seems to me, of making it certain that no great idea
will come in the next time either.
I do not think that cerebration sessions can be left unguided. There must be
someone in charge who plays a role equivalent to that of a psychoanalyst. A
psychoanalyst, as I understand it, by asking the right questions (and except for
that interfering as little as possible), gets the patient himself to discuss his past
life in such a way as to elicit new understanding of it in his own eyes.
In the same way, a session-arbiter will have to sit there, stirring up the animals,
asking the shrewd question, making the necessary comment, bringing them
gently back to the point. Since the arbiter will not know which question is
shrewd, which comment necessary, and what the point is, his will not be an easy
job.
As for “gadgets” designed to elicit creativity, I think these should arise out of the
bull sessions themselves. If thoroughly relaxed, free of responsibility, discussing
something of interest, and being by nature unconventional, the participants
themselves will create devices to stimulate discussion.