Conect FX Amigdala
Conect FX Amigdala
Conect FX Amigdala
PII: S1053-8119(17)30215-X
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.007
Reference: YNIMG13882
To appear in: NeuroImage
Received date: 21 November 2016
Revised date: 2 March 2017
Accepted date: 3 March 2017
Cite this article as: Adam X. Gorka, Salvatore Torrisi, Alexander J. Shackman,
Christian Grillon and Monique Ernst, Intrinsic Functional Connectivity of the
Central Nucleus of the Amygdala and Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis,
NeuroImage, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.007
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for
publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of
the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and
review of the resulting galley proof before it is published in its final citable form.
Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which
could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Intrinsic Functional Connectivity of the Central Nucleus of the Amygdala and Bed
Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis
1
Section on the Neurobiology of Fear & Anxiety, National Institute of Mental Health,
Bethesda, MD 20892 USA,
2
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program,
University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742 USA
Abstract
The central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis
(BNST), two nuclei within the central extended amygdala, function as critical relays
within the distributed neural networks that coordinate sensory, emotional, and cognitive
responses to threat. These structures have overlapping anatomical projections to
downstream targets that initiate defensive responses. Despite these commonalities,
researchers have also proposed a functional dissociation between the CeA and BNST,
with the CeA promoting responses to discrete stimuli and the BNST promoting
responses to diffuse threat. Intrinsic functional connectivity (iFC) provides a means to
investigate the functional architecture of the brain, unbiased by task demands. Using
ultra-high field neuroimaging (7-Tesla fMRI), which provides increased spatial
resolution, this study compared the iFC networks of the CeA and BNST in 27 healthy
individuals. Both structures were coupled with areas of the medial prefrontal cortex,
hippocampus, thalamus, and periaqueductal gray matter. Compared to the BNST, the
bilateral CeA was more strongly coupled with the insula and regions that support
sensory processing, including thalamus and fusiform gyrus. In contrast, the bilateral
BNST was more strongly coupled with regions involved in cognitive and motivational
processes, including the dorsal paracingulate gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex, and
striatum. Collectively, these findings suggest that responses to sensory stimulation are
preferentially coordinated by the CeA and cognitive and motivational responses are
preferentially coordinated by the BNST.
Keywords:
Central nucleus of the amygdala; bed nucleus of the stria terminalis BNST; resting state
networks; intrinsic functional connectivity; 7-Tesla fMRI
1. Introduction
The central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) and the lateral bed nucleus of stria
terminalis (BNST) are two small, highly interconnected structures, which are the key
components of the central extended amygdala (Olmos & Heimer, 1999). Anatomical
tracing studies in animals demonstrate that the CeA and BNST are both ideally
positioned to contribute to the behavioral and physiological responses to threat (Davis &
Shi, 1999) and a large animal literature suggests that these structures function to
maintain defensive responses to threat in the environment (Davis, Walker, Miles, &
Grillon, 2010). Researchers have suggested that the central extended amygdala plays a
critical role in the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders, depression, and
substance abuse (Avery, Clauss, & Blackford, 2016; Koob & Volkow, 2010; Shin &
Liberzon, 2009) which highlights the importance of developing a deeper understanding
of the functional role played by the CeA and BNST. Previously, we reported on the
intrinsic connectivity of the BNST using ultra-high field neuroimaging (7-Tesla fMRI),
which provides increased spatial resolution and provides an opportunity to examine the
functional architecture of small neural structures (Torrisi et al., 2015). Here, we set out
to address the novel question of whether patterns of functional connections differentiate
the BNST from the CeA, within these same participants.
iFC reflects the coherence of low frequency activity among brain regions, and provides
maps of brain functional networks (Fox & Raichle, 2007) which can shed light on the
potential similarities and differences between these two nuclei. Critically, previous
research has demonstrated that patterns of iFC can characterize functional
heterogeneity within neural structures (Cohen et al., 2008; Deen, Pitskel, & Pelphrey,
2011; Shen, Papademetris, & Constable, 2010), including the amygdala (Roy et al.,
2009). Yet to date, no research has directly examined the functional heterogeneity
within the dorsal extended amygdala (i.e., the BNST and CeA) using measures of iFC.
Although resting-state functional connectivity partly reflects the strength of anatomical
projections (Deco, Jirsa, & McIntosh, 2011), it is not limited to monosynaptic
connections and can also reveal more distributed networks of regions working together
(Raichle, 2009). Importantly, neural structures that lack direct anatomical projections to
the CeA and BNST, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or posterior cingulate, are
involved with the cognitive processing of negative information (Berman et al., 2010;
Cooney, Joormann, Eugène, Dennis, & Gotlib, 2010), and may be highly relevant to
subjective feelings of fear and anxiety. Comparing the iFC networks of the CeA and
BNST can clarify whether and how these structures interact with higher-order cognitive
systems.
Previous research has demonstrated that the central/medial region of the amygdala,
including the CeA, is functionally connected to multiple nodes of the emotional
processing network, such as the hippocampus, mPFC, anterior insula, and striatum
(Roy et al., 2009). Studies that have assessed the iFC networks of the CeA specifically,
demonstrate functional connections to regions that support cued aversive Pavlovian
conditioning (i.e. the dorsal anterior cingulate and thalamus) and sensory processing
(i.e. occipital cortex, temporal area TE, and the superior temporal sulcus) (Birn et al.,
2014; Oler et al., 2012, 2016). Although previous research suggests that the BNST is
functionally connected to the mPFC, hippocampus, thalamus, and striatum (Avery et al.,
2014; Torrisi et al., 2015), these studies do not report functional connections with the
anterior insula. This raises the possibility that the CeA and BNST differ in their
functional connections throughout the brain. However, direct comparisons of the iFC
networks of the CeA and BNST cannot be based on previous literature due to variability
in the sample, data collection, and analysis methods used to determine iFC. Here, we
set out to address this gap in the literature by using ultra-high field neuroimaging (7-
Tesla fMRI) to directly contrast the relative iFC strength of the BNST, which has
previously been reported for this sample (Torrisi et al., 2015), to the relative iFC
strength of the CeA
Based on the known anatomical projections and previously reported iFC networks of
these structures, we predicted that both the CeA and BNST would exhibit strong iFC
patterns with the PAG, thalamus, and hypothalamus, all of which receive afferents from
both the CeA and BNST. Furthermore, we predicted that the CeA would exhibit
significantly stronger iFC with the neural structures that support fear-related processes
(e.g., cued aversive Pavlovian learning) and that the BNST would exhibit significantly
stronger iFC with the neural structures that support anxiety-related processes (e.g.,
contextual aversive conditioning). Accordingly, the CeA would be expected to be
coupled more strongly with the dACC, anterior insula, thalamus, and areas of sensory
cortex (Fullana et al., 2016), whereas the BNST would be more strongly coupled with
the anterior hippocampus (Avery et al., 2014; Maren, Phan, & Liberzon, 2013; Strange,
Witter, Lein, & Moser, 2014; Torrisi et al., 2015), and regions associated with cognitive
processes (e.g., worry) recruited in response to anxiety (mPFC and medial posterior
cingulate cortex (Cooney et al., 2010; Paulesu et al., 2010).Third, based on a recent
tract-tracing report (Bienkowski & Rinaman, 2012), the BNST would be relatively more
strongly coupled with the striatum, whereas the CeA would be relatively more strongly
coupled with somatosensory information processing regions.
Finally, iFC patterns of the amygdala have been shown to be functionally lateralized
(Makovac et al., 2016; Roy et al., 2009). Previous research suggests that the left
amygdala habituates more slowly than the right, which has led researchers to suggest
that the left amygdala is specialized for sustained attention to discrete stimuli, whereas
the right amygdala is specialized for the dynamic detection of emotional cues (Wright et
al., 2001). Regarding the BNST, lesions to the medial frontal lobe impact the
metabolism of the right, but not left BNST (Motzkin et al., 2015). Taken together, these
reports suggest that both the CeA and BNST may exhibit different laterality effects
across left and right hemispheres. Although we make no a priori hypotheses concerning
the location or directionality of these effects, we expect that our exploratory analyses
might reveal similar hemispheric asymmetries when comparing the CeA to the BNST,
as those reported regarding iFC patterns of the amygdala (Makovac et al., 2016; Roy et
al., 2009).
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
The sample reported here is identical to that reported in Torrisi et al., 2015. Twenty-nine
right-handed volunteers were recruited via flyers, print advertisements, and internet
listservs for participation in this study. Informed written consent was obtained from all
participants and approved by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Combined
Neuroscience Institutional Review Board. All participants were free from the following
exclusionary criteria: (a) current or past Axis I psychiatric disorder as assessed through
a clinician administered SCID-I/NP (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2001), (b) first-
degree relative with a psychotic disorder, (c) a medical condition conflicting with safety
or design of the study, (d) brain abnormality on MRI as assessed by a radiologist, (e)
positive toxicology screen, or (f) MRI contraindication. Two participants were omitted
from analyses due to excessive head motion (i.e., more than 15% of acquired TRs with
a frame-to-frame Euclidean norm motion derivative greater than 0.3 mm) leaving a final
sample of 27 participants (M = 27.3 ± 6 SD years; 14 women).
As shown in Figure 1A, the left and right CeA seed regions of interest (ROIs) were
defined using the publicly available probabilistic atlas of Tyska and Pauli (Tyszka &
Pauli, 2016), and thresholded at 20%.
Left and right BNST seed ROIs (Figure 1B) were manually prescribed on each
participants' T1-weighted image in native space as described previously (Torrisi et al.,
2015). Briefly, three authors independently demarcated the boundaries of the BNST for
each participant based on anatomical criteria. The three binary masks were averaged
and final ROIs were determined by majority consensus (i.e., thresholded at .667).
The volume of CeA seeds (left = 58 voxels; right = 46 voxels) and BNST seeds was
similar (left = 35.41 ± 9.7 voxels; right = 38.44 ± 8.4 voxels). However, follow-up
analyses demonstrate that our results are largely unchanged when the CeA seed is
thresholded to best reflect a priori neuroanatomical estimates of volume (for details, see
Supplemental Materials).
The following nuisance signals were regressed from the functional volumes: 1) six head
motion parameters and their first derivatives; 2) eight slice-based cardiac regressors
(Glover, Li, & Ress, 2000); 3) five slice-based respiration regressors (Birn, Smith,
Jones, & Bandettini, 2008; Glover et al., 2000); 4) time series extracted from the lateral,
3rd, and 4th ventricles; 5) 0.01–0.1 Hz bandpass filter regressors; and 6) time-series from
local white matter within a 13-mm radius sphere surrounding each voxel using the
ANATICOR approach (Jo, Saad, Simmons, Milbury, & Cox, 2010). Average time series
were extracted for both bilateral seed ROIs, and separate seeds for each hemisphere
from the residualized functional images within the CeA and BNST. Pearson correlations
between each time series and all other voxels in the brain were calculated and Fisher r-
to-z transformed for group analyses.
Paired t-tests were used to contrast CeA-iFC to BNST-iFC. The variable used was the
Fisher-transformed statistical variable reflecting the iFC of the bilateral CeA and bilateral
BNST.
Differences between CeA and BNST iFC can be driven by two patterns: 1) the iFC for
one seed is significantly greater than zero, whereas the iFC for the second seed is not
significantly different from zero, leading to significant greater iFC for the first seed
compared to the second; and 2) the iFC is significantly greater than zero for both seeds,
yet the mean iFC for one seed is nonetheless significantly greater than the other. In
order to distinguish between these two patterns, clusters identified as exhibiting different
iFC between CeA and BNST were characterized as being located within areas that
exhibited iFC with each seed, or areas in which the iFC for the two seeds overlapped.
Following the identification of the voxels which exhibited significant iFC with the CeA
and BNST respectively, results from each one sample t-test were binarized, such that
all voxels that survived correction for multiple comparisons were assigned a value of 1,
whereas all other voxels were assigned a value of zero. The resulting binarized images
were then multiplied together to reveal the conjunction of CeA and BNST iFC patterns.
Here, the term conjunction refers to the logical operator “AND” (Nichols, Brett,
Andersson, Wager, & Poline, 2005). Specifically, the logical conjunction of A and B is
true, when both A is true and B is true.
All group-level statistical maps were thresholded (p<0.05, whole-brain corrected) based
on cluster extent, using a cluster-forming threshold of (p < 0.0005, k = 21 (p = 5 × 10^−4))
via updated versions of 3dFWHMx and 3dClustSim. These updates incorporate a mixed
autocorrelation function (acf) that better models non-Gaussian noise structure (Cox,
Reynolds, & Taylor, 2016; Eklund, Nichols, & Knutsson, 2016).
3. Results
3.1. Bilateral CeA and BNST iFC
The CeA showed significant iFC with cortical and subcortical regions, including
distributed regions of the cortex, medial temporal lobes, BNST, striatum, PAG, and
cerebellum (Supplemental Figure 2).
The BNST iFC has been previously reported for these participants (Torrisi et al., 2015).
In brief, the BNST was intrinsically coupled with cortical and subcortical regions, notably
the CeA, striatum, PAG, and cerebellum (Supplemental Figure 3).
3.3. Target regions that differ between bilateral CeA and BNST iFC
3.3.1. Regions more strongly connected to CeA
To fully characterize differences between CeA and BNST iFCs, the location of each
significant cluster was carefully assessed in relation to the map of shared iFC
(conjunction analysis). The cluster located within the post-central gyrus did not overlap
with either the conjunction analysis or the areas identified as exhibiting iFC with CeA via
one-sample t-test. All clusters not centered on the right or left amygdala, including all
clusters in the insula, thalamus, supramarginal gyrus, and fusiform gyrus (Table 2)
existed outside of the areas identified through the conjunction analysis. The majority of
the voxels in these clusters overlapped with areas identified as exhibiting iFC with CeA
via one-sample t-test (average cluster overlap = 86.8% of voxels, range = 68.8 – 100%).
Meanwhile, of the voxels centered on the amygdala which were more strongly coupled
with the CeA, approximately 30 percent were located within the conjunction analysis
(left: 203 voxels, right: 222 voxels) and mostly centered within the dorsal amygdala. The
remaining voxels within the amygdala which were more strongly coupled with the CeA
existed outside of the conjunction mask (left: 488 voxels, right: 534 voxels) and
centered mostly within the ventral and lateral aspects of the amygdala.
Compared to the CeA, the BNST was more strongly coupled with the paracingulate
gyrus (on the border of BA9/32) and posterior cingulate cortex (BA23) (Figure 3E). The
BNST was also more strongly coupled with a cluster in the head and body of the
caudate nucleus bilaterally, extending into the ventral striatum (Figure 3F).
Notably, the clusters located within the paracingulate gyrus and posterior cingulate
cortex did not fall within the conjunction mask. The voxels within these clusters partially
overlapped with areas identified as exhibiting iFC with BNST via one-sample t-test for
both the paracingulate gyrus (63.5%) and the posterior cingulate cortex (36.4%).
Meanwhile, approximately 12 percent of the voxels within the contiguous clusters
centered on the BNST fall within the conjunction mask (left: 103 voxels, right: 135
voxels) centered on the BNST and ventral striatum, whereas the remaining voxels in the
cluster (left: 860 voxels, right: 988 voxels) fall outside of the conjunction analysis within
the dorsal striatum and caudate.
Notably, the vmPFC (Figure 4) cluster exhibiting an interaction between seed ROI and
hemisphere was located within the conjunction mask and was also located within an
area that demonstrated an effect of hemisphere, specifically left CeA > right CeA, after
correction for multiple comparisons (for details see, Supplemental Tables).
For completeness, we report results from paired t-tests assessing differences between
left and right hemispheres, for both CeA and BNST seeds separately, within
Supplemental Materials (for details, see Supplemental Table 2 and 3).
4. Discussion
In line with expectations, the whole-brain iFC maps of the CeA and BNST exhibited
broad overlaps, particularly regarding connectivity with downstream neural effectors for
defensive responses, such as the PAG and areas of the midbrain, and with upstream
structures involved in threat-related information processing, such as the mPFC,
hippocampus, and thalamus. The main and novel findings concern the regional
differences in whole-brain iFC of these two structures. The CeA was more strongly
connected to the insula, a region involved in internal-state information processing
(Craig, 2002), and to regions supporting sensory processing, i.e., thalamus and fusiform
gyrus (Alitto & Usrey, 2003; Tyler et al., 2013). Conversely, the BNST was more
strongly connected to regions involved in the conscious appraisal of threat and decision-
making, i.e., dorsal paracingulate gyrus (Mechias, Etkin, & Kalisch, 2010; Venkatraman,
Rosati, Taren, & Huettel, 2009), motivated behavior, i.e., striatum (Liljeholm &
O’Doherty, 2012), and self-referential processes, i.e., posterior cingulate cortex (pCC)
(Northoff et al., 2006). Additionally, our results further demonstrate that the relative iFC
strength of the CeA compared to the BNST within the vmPFC, inferior frontal gyrus, and
middle temporal gyrus, vary between left and right hemispheres of the brain. These
differences in iFC between the CeA and BNST will be discussed within the framework of
the ongoing debate about the relative role of these structures in defensive responses
(e.g. Davis et al., 2010; Shackman & Fox, 2016).
In agreement with findings from anatomical studies, both the CeA and the BNST were
intrinsically connected to the PAG and areas of the midbrain. The PAG mediates
physiological and behavioral defensive responses (Bandler, Keay, Floyd, & Price, 2000;
LeDoux, Iwata, Cicchetti, & Reis, 1988) and the overlapping intrinsic connectivity of the
CeA and the BNST with this structure may reflect their common role in initiating
defensive responses to environmental threat.
Additionally, both the CeA and BNST exhibited intrinsic connectivity with multiple areas
of the midline thalamus which previous research has suggested are important for the
processing of threatening information. Both the CeA and BNST were intrinsically
connected to an area of the thalamus consistent with the interthalamic adhesion. The
interthalamic adhesion contains the nucleus reuniens, which is known from non-human
research to receive afferents from both the CeA and BNST (Aggleton & Mishkin, 1984;
Dong & Swanson, 2004). Additionally, areas of the medial dorsal thalamus likewise
exhibit functional connections with both the CeA and BNST. Stimulation of the medial
dorsal thalamus results in defensive behavioral responses (Roberts, 1962), and
previous research suggests that the medial dorsal thalamus is involved with cued and
context conditioning (Antoniadis & McDonald, 2006; Sarter & Markowitsch, 1985).
Although our observed results do not extend into the lateral geniculate nucleus, the
anatomical specificity of our findings suggests that patterns of iFC likely reflect the
functional role of these thalamic nuclei in processing threatening emotional information.
Lastly, the CeA and BNST were both intrinsically connected to the caudate and regions
of the ventral striatum, hippocampus, and large regions of the medial prefrontal cortex.
Specifically, areas of the medial prefrontal cortex included regions of vmPFC (BA 11)
and more dorsal areas of the medial frontal lobe (BA 9/10), but few clusters were
observed more caudally (i.e. BA 8/32). Exposure to threat can impact operant
responding (Jaisinghani & Rosenkranz, 2015), in addition to influencing sensory
(Grosso, Cambiaghi, Concina, Sacco, & Sacchetti, 2015) and cognitive processes
(Okon-Singer, Hendler, Pessoa, & Shackman, 2015). The present results suggest that
the CeA and BNST both interact with the neural networks that support instrumental,
sensory, and cognitive responses to threat.
4.2. CeA: preferential coupling with somatic and sensory networks
The CeA, relative to the BNST, exhibited stronger coupling with targets involved in
sensory processing. These regions consisted of the ventral posterior thalamus, the mid-
insula, the fusiform gyrus, and areas of the post-central gyrus.
The thalamus is a complex multi-nuclei structure, and the ventral-posterior region of the
thalamus processes noxious somatosensory information (Casey & Morrow, 1983).
Fibers from the ventral-posterior thalamic region also co-terminate in the amygdala
together with with input from other perceptual systems (Paré, Quirk, & Ledoux, 2004).
The co-occurrence of action potentials from neurons responding to noxious stimuli and
other neurons responding to perceptual qualities of originally neutral stimuli may form
the basis of aversive stimulus-stimulus associations (Blair, Schafe, Bauer, Rodrigues, &
LeDoux, 2001), and the greater iFC between the CeA and the ventral-posterior thalamic
region may reflect the role of this thalamic subregion in facilitating fear conditioning.
The insula is a primary hub for processing interoceptive information (Craig, 2002). The
iFC networks of the insula can be divided into three functional subdivisions (Deen et al.,
2011). The dorsal anterior insula is functionally connected to the dACC and cognitive
control network, the ventral anterior insula is connected to the pregenual cingulate
cortex, whereas the posterior insula is functionally connected to the primary and
secondary somatomotor cortices. Notably, the clusters more strongly connected to the
CeA, compared to the BNST, were distributed broadly throughout the insula and
appeared to be located in the posterior as well as the dorsal and ventral anterior
subdivisions. This differential coupling suggests that, compared to the BNST, the CeA is
more strongly communicates more strongly with regions involved with interoceptive
processing within multiple functional domains.
The fusiform gyrus is part of the ventral visual processing stream and forms the core
system for face perception and decoding, essential for social interactions (Dziobek I,
Bahnemann M, Convit A, & Heekeren HR, 2010; Tyler et al., 2013). Previous research
suggests that the amygdala interacts with the ventral visual stream during the
processing of emotional information (Vuilleumier & Driver, 2007) and the enhanced
coupled of the CeA and fusiform may reflect the well-established role of the amygdala in
responding to discrete biologically relevant stimuli, such as human faces.
Collectively, these results suggest that the CeA is functionally connected to regions
which process visual, interoceptive, and somatosensory information more strongly than
the BNST. This finding may reflect the particularly important generic role of the
amygdala in integrating salience and valence of stimuli for adaptive behavioral
responses. Although speculative, this relatively stronger functional association with the
coding of sensory characteristics of stimuli and their associated affective value might
reflect a closer link with the building blocks of fear learning. However, it should also be
noted that the CeA was also more strongly connected to the right supramarginal gyrus,
which is involved with a wide array of visuo-spatial processes including attention,
saccade eye movements, and grasping (Simon, Mangin, Cohen, Le Bihan, & Dehaene,
2002). As such, it is unlikely that the preferential role of the CeA, compared to the
BNST, be limited to strictly sensory domains.
The BNST, relative to the CeA, exhibited stronger coupling with regions involved in the
anticipation of negative events and in the integration of self-referential values to
behavior. These targets consisted of clusters in the ventral striatum, paracingulate
gyrus, and posterior cingulate cortex (pCC).
The nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus are critical for the acquisition and
performance of instrumental actions (Berridge, Robinson, & Aldridge, 2009). Although,
there is no clear evidence that the BNST has direct anatomical connections with the
caudate nucleus, recent research from independent samples has demonstrated that
these two structures are strongly coupled at rest (Avery et al., 2014), and that fiber
paths from the BNST to the medial prefrontal cortex travel through the head of the
caudate (Krüger, Shiozawa, Kreifelts, Scheffler, & Ethofer, 2015). Taken together, the
strong coupling of the BNST with the striatum might represent one mechanism
underpinning how anxiety can impact motivated behavior (Avery, Clauss, & Blackford,
2016; Stamatakis et al., 2014).
The paracingulate region more coupled with the BNST than CeA was located within the
rostral dorsal mPFC. Specifically, this cluster was located on the border of BA 8/32,
caudally to areas identified through the conjunction analysis to be coupled with both the
CeA and BNST. Meta-analyses have suggested that a similar area of the rostral dorsal
mPFC is the most consistently activated region during the conscious appraisal of threat
in instructed-fear conditioning paradigms (Mechias et al., 2010). This region has also
recently received much attention with regards to negative affect, including anxiety,
linking it to the generation and maintenance of threat appraisal (Bijsterbosch, Smith, &
Bishop, 2015; Kim, Gee, Loucks, Davis, & Whalen, 2011; Shackman et al., 2011).
Meanwhile, the pCC, is more strongly coupled with the BNST than CeA. The pCC is
involved in self-referential mental processes and mind-wandering (Northoff et al., 2006).
Taken together, this differential connectivity pattern with structures involved in self-
generated thoughts, typical of worry, may suggest a preferential involvement of the
BNST with the cognitive aspects of anxiety, relative to the amygdala. However, pCC
activity has been linked to other mental activities, including autobiographical memory
(Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2008) and researchers have suggested that the pCC may
promote internally-directed cognition or may serve to regulate the focus of attention
(Leech & Sharp, 2014). As such, our interpretation that iFC between the BNST and pCC
reflects self-referential activity is speculative, and this enhanced connectivity may reflect
other internally directed mental processes.
Finally, contrary to our expectation, the hippocampus was not found to be more strongly
connected with the BNST than the CeA. The hippocampus is critical for contextual
conditioning (Maren et al., 2013), which serves as a model for anxiety (defensive
response to diffuse threat) (Davis et al., 2010), and therefore, was expected to be more
closely associated with the BNST than the CeA. Although the BNST is strongly coupled
with the anterior hippocampus, our results do not support the hypothesis that, compared
to the CeA, the BNST is more strongly connected with the neural systems that support
contextual threat learning at rest.
We hypothesized that differences in iFC between the CeA and BNST would vary across
hemispheres, however, we made no a priori hypotheses concerning the location or
directionality of such effect. Although previous research has addressed differences
between the iFC of the left and right amygdala, the majority of studies focus on
associations between iFC and between subject factors such as gender or mood
diagnosis (Hahn et al., 2011; Kilpatrick, Zald, Pardo, & Cahill, 2006; Makovac et al.,
2016). Further, prior work which has formally tested for differences between amygdala
subregions do not report whole brain analyses testing for effects of CeA laterality (Roy
et al., 2009). Nonetheless, findings from animal models and human neuroscience can
provide insights into our results.
Research in animals suggests that dopamine and serotonin signaling within the left and
right amygdala differentially impact behavior, and that pain processing within CeA,
specifically, is lateralized (Andersen & Teicher, 1999; Bradbury, Costall, Domeney, &
Naylor, 1985; Ji & Neugebauer, 2009) Additionally, meta-analyses have demonstrated
that the left amygdala is more strongly activated than the right amygdala regardless of
paradigm and stimulus type (Baas, Aleman, & Kahn, 2004). During the repeated
presentation of emotional stimuli, the left amygdala habituates more slowly than the
right amygdala which has led researchers to suggest that the left amygdala is
specialized for sustained attention to discrete stimuli, whereas the right amygdala is
specialized for the dynamic detection of emotional cues (Wright et al., 2001).
Meanwhile, research has consistently demonstrated that the vmPFC is critically
involved with the extinction of discrete conditioned stimuli during cued aversive
Pavlovian learning (Ahs, Kragel, Zielinski, Brady, & LaBar, 2015; Milad et al., 2007;
Phelps, Delgado, Nearing, & LeDoux, 2004), and it is possible that the enhanced
connectivity between the left CeA and vmPFC reflects the need for enhanced regulation
of sustained CeA responses in the left hemisphere.
Alternatively, lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex in primates reduce BNST metabolism
and decrease freezing behavior (Fox et al., 2010). In line with this evidence, humans
who have sustained damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex exhibit reduced
resting metabolism within the BNST (Motzkin et al., 2015), suggesting that in contrast to
its role in down-regulating amygdala responses, the vmPFC may serve to up-regulate
activity within the BNST. Intriguingly, research suggests that vmPFC lesions impact the
metabolism of the right, but not left BNST (Motzkin et al., 2015), and it is possible that
the role of the vmPFC in generating BNST responses is reflected in the preferential iFC
between the vmPFC and right BNST.
Nonetheless, interactions between seed ROI and hemisphere were also observed
within the inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus, and as such it is unclear
whether this pattern necessarily reflects the differential impact of regulatory structures
on CeA and BNST responses. Future research will be necessary to confirm these
patterns and shed light on their functional significance.
Moreover, it is important to keep in mind that the resting-state fMRI approach only
informs connectivity patterns at rest. Patterns of connectivity during a task may
complement these resting-state maps by challenging the networks of these maps and
by providing functional significance. For example, the relative strength of the functional
connectivity of the BNST and CeA may appear different during acute threat vs.
sustained threat. The present work will help guide future experiments that test functional
probes of the BNST and CeA networks. Lastly, findings in healthy participants will need
to be contrasted with findings in patients suffering from anxiety or mood disorders, given
the central role of the CeA and BNST in emotion perturbations.
4.6. Conclusion
Collectively, the findings of this study suggest that the human CeA is most strongly
coupled with the neural systems that coordinate responses to sensory stimuli, whereas
BNST is preferentially coupled with regions that support self-representational processes
and goal-directed behavior. Additionally, these findings provide initial evidence that the
relative strength of CeA and BNST iFC differ across left and right hemispheres, which
may suggest differences in the lateralization of function between these nodes of the
central extended amygdala. The relative strengths of iFC networks are thought to reflect
the history of usage of these networks as well as structural connections between brain
regions (Deco et al., 2011; Raichle, 2009). As such, these results can inform
hypotheses about the organization of functional resting state networks and may shed
light on the relative roles of the CeA and BNST within distributed neural networks that
coordinate sensory, emotional, and cognitive responses to threat exposure.
5. Acknowledgements
Special thanks to Katherine O’Connell, Andrew Davis, and Brendon Nacewicz. This
work utilized the computational resources of the NIH HPC Biowulf cluster
(http://hpc.nih.gov). This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of
the National Institutes of Mental Health, project number ZIAMH002798 (clinical protocol
02-M-0321, NCT00047853) to CG, and research grants from the National Institutes of
Health (DA040717 and MH107444) to AJS. The authors report no competing interest.
The author(s) declare that, except for income received from the primary employer, no
financial support or compensation has been received from any individual or corporate
entity over the past 3 years for research or professional service and there are no
personal financial holdings that could be perceived as constituting a potential conflict of
interest.
References
AAhs, F., Kragel, P. A., Zielinski, D. J., Brady, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2015). Medial prefrontal pathways for the
Aggleton, J. P., & Mishkin, M. (1984). Projections of the amygdala to the thalamus in the cynomolgus
https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902220106
Alitto, H. J., & Usrey, W. M. (2003). Corticothalamic feedback and sensory processing. Current Opinion in
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (1999). Serotonin laterality in amygdala predicts performance in the
Antoniadis, E. A., & McDonald, R. J. (2006). Fornix, medial prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and
Avery, S. N., Clauss, J. A., & Blackford, J. U. (2016). The Human BNST: Functional Role in Anxiety and
Avery, Suzanne N., Clauss, J. A., Winder, D. G., Woodward, N., Heckers, S., & Blackford, J. U. (2014).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.017
Baas, D., Aleman, A., & Kahn, R. S. (2004). Lateralization of amygdala activation: a systematic review of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.02.004
Bandler, R., Keay, K. A., Floyd, N., & Price, J. (2000). Central circuits mediating patterned autonomic
activity during active vs. passive emotional coping. Brain Research Bulletin, 53(1), 95–104.
Berman, M. G., Peltier, S., Nee, D. E., Kross, E., Deldin, P. J., & Jonides, J. (2010). Depression, rumination
and the default network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsq080.
https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq080
Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., & Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: “liking”,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014
Bienkowski, M. S., & Rinaman, L. (2012). Common and distinct neural inputs to the medial central
nucleus of the amygdala and anterior ventrolateral bed nucleus of stria terminalis in rats. Brain
Bijsterbosch, J., Smith, S., & Bishop, S. J. (2015). Functional connectivity under anticipation of shock:
Correlates of trait anxious affect versus induced anxiety. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., McFarlin, D. R., Rogers, G. M., … Kalin, N. H.
Birn, Rasmus M., Smith, M. A., Jones, T. B., & Bandettini, P. A. (2008). The respiration response function:
Blair, H. T., Schafe, G. E., Bauer, E. P., Rodrigues, S. M., & LeDoux, J. E. (2001). Synaptic plasticity in the
lateral amygdala: a cellular hypothesis of fear conditioning. Learning & Memory, 8(5), 229–242.
Bradbury, A. J., Costall, B., Domeney, A. M., & Naylor, R. J. (1985). Laterality of dopamine function and
https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3908(85)90149-2
Casey, K. L., & Morrow, T. J. (1983). Ventral posterior thalamic neurons differentially responsive to
Cohen, A. L., Fair, D. A., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Miezin, F. M., Dierker, D., Van Essen, D. C., … Petersen, S. E.
(2008). Defining functional areas in individual human brains using resting functional connectivity
Cooney, R. E., Joormann, J., Eugène, F., Dennis, E. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Neural correlates of
https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.10.4.470
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance
Cox, R. W., Reynolds, R. C., & Taylor, P. A. (2016). AFNI and Clustering: False Positive Rates Redux.
bioRxiv, 065862.
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body.
Cullinan, W. E., Herman, J. P., & Watson, S. J. (1993). Ventral subicular interaction with the hypothalamic
paraventricular nucleus: Evidence for a relay in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. The
Davis, M., & Shi, C. (1999). The extended amygdala: are the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed
nucleus of the stria terminalis differentially involved in fear versus anxiety? Annals of the New
Davis, M., Walker, D. L., Miles, L., & Grillon, C. (2010). Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2961
Deen, B., Pitskel, N. B., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2011). Three Systems of Insular Functional Connectivity
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq186
Dong, H.-W., & Swanson, L. W. (2004). Organization of axonal projections from the anterolateral area of
the bed nuclei of the stria terminalis. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 468(2), 277–298.
Dziobek I, Bahnemann M, Convit A, & Heekeren HR. (2010). THe role of the fusiform-amygdala system in
https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.31
Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E., & Knutsson, H. (2016). Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent
have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(28),
7900–7905. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1602413113
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (2001). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR
axis I disorders—non-patient edition. New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., … others. (2002). Whole brain
33(3), 341–355.
Fonov, V., Evans, A. C., Botteron, K., Almli, C. R., McKinstry, R. C., Collins, D. L., … others. (2011).
Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. NeuroImage, 54(1), 313–327.
Fonov, V. S., Evans, A. C., McKinstry, R. C., Almli, C. R., & Collins, D. L. (2009). Unbiased nonlinear average
cortex lesions alter anxiety-related activity in the primate bed nucleus of stria terminalis. The
Fox, M. D., & Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional
Fullana, M. A., Harrison, B. J., Soriano-Mas, C., Vervliet, B., Cardoner, N., Àvila-Parcet, A., & Radua, J.
(2016). Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis
Glover, G. H., Li, T.-Q., & Ress, D. (2000). Image-based method for retrospective correction of
physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 44(1), 162–
167.
Grosso, A., Cambiaghi, M., Concina, G., Sacco, T., & Sacchetti, B. (2015). Auditory cortex involvement in
Hahn, A., Stein, P., Windischberger, C., Weissenbacher, A., Spindelegger, C., Moser, E., … Lanzenberger,
Hurley, K. M., Herbert, H., Moga, M. M., & Saper, C. B. (1991). Efferent projections of the infralimbic
https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903080210
Hutton, C., Josephs, O., Stadler, J., Featherstone, E., Reid, A., Speck, O., … Weiskopf, N. (2011). The
Jaisinghani, S., & Rosenkranz, J. A. (2015). Repeated social defeat stress enhances the anxiogenic effect
of bright light on operant reward-seeking behavior in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 290,
172–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.048
Ji, G., & Neugebauer, V. (2009). Hemispheric Lateralization of Pain Processing by Amygdala Neurons.
Jo, H. J., Saad, Z. S., Simmons, W. K., Milbury, L. A., & Cox, R. W. (2010). Mapping sources of correlation
in resting state FMRI, with artifact detection and removal. Neuroimage, 52(2), 571–582.
Kilpatrick, L. A., Zald, D. H., Pardo, J. V., & Cahill, L. F. (2006). Sex-related differences in amygdala
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.065
Kim, M. J., Gee, D. G., Loucks, R. A., Davis, F. C., & Whalen, P. J. (2011). Anxiety dissociates dorsal and
ventral medial prefrontal cortex functional connectivity with the amygdala at rest. Cerebral
Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry of Addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 217–
238. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.110
Krüger, O., Shiozawa, T., Kreifelts, B., Scheffler, K., & Ethofer, T. (2015). Three distinct fiber pathways of
the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis to the amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Cortex, 66, 60–68.
LeDoux, J. E., Iwata, J., Cicchetti, P., & Reis, D. J. (1988). Different projections of the central amygdaloid
nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear. The Journal of
Leech, R., & Sharp, D. J. (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain,
Liljeholm, M., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2012). Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.007
Makovac, E., Meeten, F., Watson, D. R., Herman, A., Garfinkel, S. N., Critchley, H. D., & Ottaviani, C.
786–795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.013
Maren, S., Phan, K. L., & Liberzon, I. (2013). The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning,
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3492
Mechias, M.-L., Etkin, A., & Kalisch, R. (2010). A meta-analysis of instructed fear studies: implications for
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.040
Milad, M. R., Wright, C. I., Orr, S. P., Pitman, R. K., Quirk, G. J., & Rauch, S. L. (2007). Recall of Fear
Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Oler, J. A., Kalin, N. H., Baskaya, M. K., & Koenigs, M. (2015). Ventromedial
prefrontal cortex damage alters resting blood flow to the bed nucleus of stria terminalis. Cortex,
Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., & Poline, J.-B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-
referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage,
Okon-Singer, H., Hendler, T., Pessoa, L., & Shackman, A. J. (2015). The neurobiology of emotion–
cognition interactions: fundamental questions and strategies for future research. Frontiers in
for coordinated functional activity within the extended amygdala of non-human and human
Oler, J. A., Tromp, D. P. M., Fox, A. S., Kovner, R., Davidson, R. J., Alexander, A. L., … Fudge, J. L. (2016).
Connectivity between the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria
terminalis in the non-human primate: neuronal tract tracing and developmental neuroimaging
Olmos, J. S., & Heimer, L. (1999). The concepts of the ventral striatopallidal system and extended
Paré, D., Quirk, G. J., & Ledoux, J. E. (2004). New vistas on amygdala networks in conditioned fear.
Paulesu, E., Sambugaro, E., Torti, T., Danelli, L., Ferri, F., Scialfa, G., … Sassaroli, S. (2010). Neural
correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: a functional MRI
Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., & LeDoux, J. E. (2004). Extinction learning in humans: role of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042
Raichle, M. E. (2009). A Paradigm Shift in Functional Brain Imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(41),
12729–12734. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4366-09.2009
Roberts, W. W. (1962). Fearlike behavior elicited from dorsomedial thalamus of cat. Journal of
Roy, A. K., Shehzad, Z., Margulies, D. S., Kelly, A. M. C., Uddin, L. Q., Gotimer, K., … Milham, M. P. (2009).
Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 45(2),
614–626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.030
Sarter, M., & Markowitsch, H. J. (1985). Involvement of the amygdala in learning and memory: A critical
https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.99.2.342
Shackman, A. J., & Fox, A. S. (2016). Contributions of the Central Extended Amygdala to Fear and
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0982-16.2016
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2011). The
integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews
Shen, X., Papademetris, X., & Constable, R. T. (2010). Graph-theory based parcellation of functional
subunits in the brain from resting-state fMRI data. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1027–1035.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.119
Shergill, S. S., White, T. P., Joyce, D. W., Bays, P. M., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2013). Modulation of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.043
Shi, C.-J., & Cassell, M. d. (1998). Cortical, thalamic, and amygdaloid connections of the anterior and
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19981005)399:4<440::AID-CNE2>3.0.CO;2-1
Shin, L. M., & Liberzon, I. (2009). The Neurocircuitry of Fear, Stress, and Anxiety Disorders.
Simon, O., Mangin, J.-F., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Topographical Layout of Hand,
Eye, Calculation, and Language-Related Areas in the Human Parietal Lobe. Neuron, 33(3), 475–
487. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00575-5
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. N. (2008). The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory,
Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis.
Stamatakis, A. M., Sparta, D. R., Jennings, J. H., McElligott, Z. A., Decot, H., & Stuber, G. D. (2014).
Amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis circuitry: implications for addiction-related
Strange, B. A., Witter, M. P., Lein, E. S., & Moser, E. I. (2014). Functional organization of the hippocampal
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3785
Sullivan, G. M., Apergis, J., Bush, D. E. A., Johnson, L. R., Hou, M., & Ledoux, J. E. (2004). Lesions in the
bed nucleus of the stria terminalis disrupt corticosterone and freezing responses elicited by a
contextual but not by a specific cue-conditioned fear stimulus. Neuroscience, 128(1), 7–14.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.06.015
Torrisi, S., O’Connell, K., Davis, A., Reynolds, R., Balderston, N., Fudge, J. L., … Ernst, M. (2015). Resting
state connectivity of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis at ultra-high field. Human Brain
Tyler, L. K., Chiu, S., Zhuang, J., Randall, B., Devereux, B. J., Wright, P., … Taylor, K. I. (2013). Objects and
Categories: Feature Statistics and Object Processing in the Ventral Stream. Journal of Cognitive
Tyszka, J. M., & Pauli, W. M. (2016). In vivo delineation of subdivisions of the human amygdaloid
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hbm.23289/full
Venkatraman, V., Rosati, A. G., Taren, A. A., & Huettel, S. A. (2009). Resolving response, decision, and
strategic control: evidence for a functional topography in dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. The
Vuilleumier, P., & Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows
on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2092
Waddell, J., Morris, R. W., & Bouton, M. E. (2006). Effects of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis lesions
https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.120.2.324
Wright, C. I., Fischer, H. akan, Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., & Rauch, S. L. (2001).
# of
Coordinates voxels
Subcortical X Y Z
Right medial temporal lobe 20.7 -9.8 -11.3 1660
-contiguous cluster including the
hippocampus, amygdala, basal
forebrain, striatum, and thalamus
Table 2. Spatial coordinates and statistics from peak voxels exhibiting significantly
different intrinsic connectivity (CeA vs. BNST) as determined by paired t-tests
# of
Coordinates T statistic voxels
BNST >
CeA X Y Z
Right BNST, caudate 6.8 4.3 1.4 18.33 1123
Posterior cingulate
cortex 4.1 -26.8 31.3 5.4 33
CeA >
BNST
Right amygdala 26.2 -10 -11.5 16.54 756
Right supramarginal
gyrus 62.6 -13.8 14.4 5.68 32
# of
Coordinates F statistic voxels
Seed ROI x
Hemisphere
Right BNST 6.8 5.6 -1.2 225.31 204
Ventromedial
prefrontal cortex -1.1 35.6 -23.2 30.46 31
Figures
Figure 1. Seed ROIs for intrinsic connectivity analyses. A): central nucleus of the
amygdala (CeA); B): bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST).
Figure 2. Shared coupling. Regions showing significant iFC with both the CeA and
BNST are depicted in red (p<.05, whole-brain corrected). A) Thalamus and
hippocampus. B) Amygdala, including regions of the lateral and basal nuclei. C)
Striatum, including the caudate, putamen, and ventral striatum. D) Midbrain, including
the PAG. E) mPFC. F) Superior temporal sulcus.
Figure 3. Distinct connections. Regions with greater CeA iFC are depicted in red-yellow,
whereas regions with greater BNST iFC are depicted in blue (p<.05, whole-brain
corrected). CeA-preferring regions include the (A) middle insula, (B) ventral posterior
thalamus, (C) postcentral and supramarginal gyri, and (D) and fusiform gyrus. BNST-
preferring regions include the (E) paracingulate gyrus and posterior cingulate and (F)
head and body of the caudate nucleus bilaterally, extending into ventral striatum.
Figure 4. Seed ROI x Hemisphere Effects within Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. (A)
Statistical image depicting results within the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)
that exhibit a significant interaction between seed ROI (CeA vs. BNST) and hemisphere
(left vs. right). (B) Bar graphs depicting mean iFC values from the vmPFC as a function
of ROI and hemisphere. iFC values are reported in arbitrary units (A.U.). Error bars
reflect 1 standard error.
Highlights
Supplemental Methods:
Our rationale for thresholding the amygdala ROI to match the size of the BNST was based on
the idea that different numbers of voxels within an ROI may lead to changes in the signal to
noise ratio of the ROI as a whole, which could potentially impact our reported findings. To
ensure that this decision did not impact our pattern of results, we conducted follow up analyses
wherein the CeA ROI was thresholded to best match a priori neuroanatomical estimates.
Previous post-mortem research has suggested that the volume of the CeA is approximately 50
ml in volume (Herzog & Kemper, 1980; Tyszka & Pauli, 2016). We therefore thresholded the
probabilistic CeA ROI at 60% resulting in CeA ROIs that closely approximate previous estimates
(left CeA: 30 voxels, 65.9 ml; right CeA: 22 voxels, 48.3 ml). These CeA ROIs were used to
calculate iFC, using steps identical to those reported in our Methods (for full details, see 2.5.
BOLD fMRI Data Preprocessing and Analysis). Paired t-tests subsequently demonstrated that
the decision to use a relatively larger CeA ROI did not meaningfully impact our results (Table
S1.)
Supplemental Results:
The average intrinsic smoothness of our iFC images was calculated to be 4.48 mm, using the
“acf” calculation in 3dFWHM. Subsequently, we calculated the Euclidean distance between our
seed ROIs, and clusters observed within the caudate and basolateral amygdala. For the
caudate, coordinates were selected to occupy the most rostral extent of the cluster, while for the
basolateral amygdala, coordinates were selected based on the most inferior extent of the
cluster. For the coordinates of the CeA and BNST seed ROIs, we calculated distance based on
both the center of mass, and for the most rostral edge for the BNST, and the most inferior edge
for the CeA. All distances were greater than 12 mm (range= 12.64 – 22.07 mm; mean = 17.77
mm), suggesting that the observed clusters were not due to smoothing, or the spatial proximity
to the seed region.
Supplemental Tables:
Table S1.
# of
Coordinates T statistic voxels
BNST > X Y Z
CeA
Right BNST, caudate 6.8 4.3 1.4 18.57 1235
Posterior cingulate
cortex 4.1 -26.8 31.3 5.43 104
CeA >
BNST
Right amygdala 24.9 -8.7 -11.5 17.08 584
Right insula 36.6 0.4 15.8 5.60 48
40.5 6.9 10.5 4.94 41
Table S1. Spatial coordinates and statistics from peak voxels exhibiting significantly different
intrinsic connectivity (CeA vs. BNST), stemming from the follow up analysis where CeA volume
was matched to a prior anatomical estimates.
Table S2.
# of
Coordinates T statistic voxels
Left CeA
> Right
CeA X Y Z
Left amygdala
-23.2 -8.7 -10.2 24.36 270
Posterior cingulate
cortex -7.6 -52.8 -31.3 6.10 62
Middle temporal
gyrus -71.2 -17.8 -10.2 5.91 47
-59.6 -32.1 -6.3 5.89 45
Ventromedial
prefrontal cortex -2.4 39.4 -24.5 5.83 21
Right
CeA >
Left CeA
Right amygdala
24.9 -8.7 -10.2 32.97 185
Table S2. Spatial coordinates and statistics from peak voxels exhibiting significantly
different intrinsic connectivity between left and right CeA as determined by paired t-test
Table S3.
Coordinates T statistic # of voxels
Left
BNST >
Right
BNST X Y Z
Left BNST
-6.2 -3.1 0.2 18.51 298
Right
BNST >
Left
BNST
Right BNST – extending
into caudate 6.8 4.3 -1.2 16.21 298
Table S3.. Spatial coordinates and statistics from peak voxels exhibiting significantly
different intrinsic connectivity between left and right BNST as determined by paired t-
test
Supplemental Figures:
Supplemental Figure 1. Group level results of (CeA vs. BNST) paired t-test at statistical
threshold of p=1.0, to display the EPI voxel coverage of 95% or more of the subjects.
Supplemental Figure 2. Voxels exhibiting intrinsic connectivity with the CeA as determined by a
one-sample t-test
Supplemental Figure 3. Voxels exhibiting intrinsic connectivity with the BNST as determined by
a one-sample t-test
References:
Herzog, A. G., & Kemper, T. L. (1980). Amygdaloid changes in aging and dementia. Archives of
Tyszka, J. M., & Pauli, W. M. (2016). In vivo delineation of subdivisions of the human