What We Feel and Why We Buy: The in Uence of Emotions On Consumer Decision-Making
What We Feel and Why We Buy: The in Uence of Emotions On Consumer Decision-Making
What We Feel and Why We Buy: The in Uence of Emotions On Consumer Decision-Making
net/publication/293801220
CITATIONS READS
41 9,390
4 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
The Impact of Emotions on the Persuasiveness of Detection versus Prevention Appeals View project
All content following this page was uploaded by Chethana Achar on 23 April 2018.
PII: S2352-250X(16)30001-X
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.01.009
Reference: COPSYC 254
To appear in:
Please cite this article as: C. Achar, J. So, N. Agrawal, A. Duhachek, What We Feel and
Why We Buy: The Influence of Emotions on Consumer Decision-Making, COPSYC
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.01.009
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication.
As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript.
The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof
before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process
errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that
apply to the journal pertain.
HIGHLIGHTS
making.
t
Consumers are influenced by emotions that are both related and unrelated to the decision.
ip
Emotions impact consumer decisions through cognitive appraisals.
cr
us
an
M
ed
pt
ce
Ac
Page 1 of 17
2
Decision-Making
t
ip
cr
us
Chethana Achara, Jane Soa, Nidhi Agrawala and Adam Duhachekb
an
M
ed
Addresses:
pt
a
Foster School of Business, University of Washington, PACCAR Hall, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
ce
b
Kelley School of Business, Indiana University, 1309 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47405,
USA.
Ac
Page 2 of 17
3
Abstract
Each specific emotion is associated with a set of cognitive appraisals that drives the influence of
integrated view of the current literature on how emotions - both related and unrelated to the
t
decision at hand - play an important role in shaping consumer decision-making. Emotions
ip
embedded in marketing stimuli influence decision-making via processes driven by cognitive
cr
appraisals. Emotions that are unrelated to the decisions influence decision-making via carried
us
over appraisal tendencies. We present perspectives on why and under what conditions emotions
serve as antecedents to decision-making, and call for future research to examine how emotional
an
influences can both undermine and help consumer decision-making.
M
Introduction
ed
Consider the following situations: Requesting a nicotine patch after seeing a fear-
inducing anti-smoking flier in your doctor’s office, donating money to a charity after eating a
pt
sinful chocolate cake, holding on tightly to your bottle of Coke (as an object of attachment)
ce
while watching a horror movie. Each of these situations provides an example of how emotions
consumer’s incidental emotional state affects the decisions she or he might make. Before delving
into the psychological processes by which emotions influence consumers, we review the current
Page 3 of 17
4
relationship to their social and physical surroundings as well as their interpretations regarding
these relationships [1,2]. Each specific or discrete emotion is associated with a profile of
t
cognitive evaluations called ‘appraisals’. Early research on specific emotions was aimed at
ip
identifying and classifying emotional appraisals. Smith and Ellsworth [2] classified cognitive
cr
appraisals associated with fifteen common specific emotions along six dimensions (i.e.,
us
pleasantness, certainty, self-responsibility, anticipated effort, attention and situational control).
The more recently developed Appraisal Tendency Framework (ATF) [3,4], suggests that
an
appraisals associated with the experience of a specific emotion can ‘carry over’ by predisposing
individuals’ view of other, unrelated events in line with the preexisting appraisals [5]. For
M
example, fear is an emotion that arises from the appraisal of low individual control over an
unpleasant event whereas anger is associated with high individual control [2]. Thus feelings of
ed
fear or anger lead to different subsequent risk perceptions, such that fear leads to pessimistic risk
perceptions (i.e., perceptions of low control) and anger leads to optimistic risk perceptions (i.e.,
pt
Based on the source of consumers’ emotional experiences and its relationship to the
decision at hand, emotional influences are classified into two broad categories: Integral emotions
Ac
and incidental emotions. Integral emotions are experienced when marketers embed emotions in
the marketing stimuli or contexts with the intention of influencing a particular decision [7].
Incidental emotions arise from sources that are unrelated to a particular decision, yet their
influence carries over to that subsequent decision [7]. This review is organized around such
Page 4 of 17
5
consumer decision-making, followed by sections that detail the specific psychological processes.
t
ip
Decision-Making
Appraisals associated with emotions influence judgments in two ways. One, the nature of
cr
marketing appeals and marketing contexts (i.e., advertising, brand, category) can elicit emotions
us
(i.e., integral emotions), which influence consumers’ decision-making processes via cognitive
appraisals of the object or the event. Two, unrelated environmental factors, such as prior events,
an
or consumer’s personality might elicit emotions (i.e., incidental emotions), which affect how
consumers make that decision. Such incidental emotions activate appraisal tendencies related to
M
the emotion and are carried over to decisions about another object or situation. Finally, integral
and incidental emotions can jointly influence decision-making via interaction of cognitive
ed
product packaging, positioning, events, etc. [8,9]. Brands and marketing contents systematically
portray and evoke emotions that encourage desired consumer responses. There is a small stream
Page 5 of 17
6
of research that explores how integral emotions influence decision-making. Here, we review this
area of research in the hope that it will serve as a call for more scholars to examine the role of
t
appeals. In a field experiment, anti-child abuse public service announcements that used
ip
empathetic (vs. rational) appeals were more effective [10]. Following multiple demonstrations
cr
that emotional appeals could be effective alternatives to rational appeals in influencing consumer
us
behavior, most research has focused on understanding how and when integral emotions make
processes that drives consumer reactions to emotional appeals. Certain emotions increase the
M
motivation to act in compliance with an appeal via accountability appraisals. For example,
sunscreen ads that used specific emotions with appraisals of self-accountability (such as regret,
ed
guilt, and challenge) increased individuals’ intentions to use sunscreen, relative to appeals that
used emotions with low self-accountability (e.g., fear, hope) [11]. Emotional appeals could also
pt
influence subsequent decision-making through modifying one’s concept of self relative to others.
ce
For example, the emotion love expands the boundary of caring toward others and extends
likely to donate to an international (vs. local) charity [12]. Emotions may also influence
consumer behavior through contagion effects; individuals experienced more sadness when
viewing a charitable appeal with victims’ faces showing sad (vs. happy) expressions and hence
Page 6 of 17
7
Some emotional appeals influence consumers by changing how the information provided
in the appeal is processed. For example, high fear-arousing appeals (i.e. anti-smoking ads) are
likely to be defensively processed by consumers. Hence, they were effective only when the
message reduced the problem elaboration by using others as references [14]. Another processing
t
mechanism that could enhance the effectiveness of emotional appeals is metacognitive fluency
ip
facilitated by message framing [15]. A guilt-inducing anti-drinking appeal cast in a gain (rather
cr
than loss) frame was processed more fluently and was consequently more effective in decreasing
us
binge drinking intentions. This is because guilt functions through problem-focused coping – as
facilitated by a gain frame – and shame functions through emotion-focused coping, facilitated by
depend on their compatibility with many consumer-related factors, such as culture, consumers’
ed
salient self-identities, and their incidental emotional states. For example, the influence of
different discrete emotions across people belonging to different cultures varies; emotional
pt
appeals that are not compatible with the participants’ culture may be more effective due to their
ce
novelty [16]. For example, members of a collectivist (vs. individualist) culture were more
persuaded by emotional appeals that were ego-focused (e.g., pride) rather than appeals that
Ac
used other-focused emotions (e.g., empathy) due to a sense of novelty [16]. In another
Williams [17] show that individuals prefer emotional messages that are compatible with their
salient self-identity. Individuals primed with their athlete identity were most persuaded by anger-
Page 7 of 17
8
based advertisements because anger is consistent with the stereotypical social identity of being
an athlete.
thinking towards appraisals associated with those emotions such as higher self-responsibility
t
(e.g., regret) and greater inclusion of other in self (e.g., love). Appraisals may also interact with
ip
decision contexts and consumer characteristics to enhance effectiveness of marketing through
cr
various processes such as identity-compatibility, reduced defensive processing, and novelty.
us
While existing research has documented the effects of both compatibility and incompatibility of
emotional appeals with culture [17], future research could explore when compatibility versus
Emotional influences in the marketplace are not limited to those intentionally set up by
ed
marketers. Even incidental emotions may affect a variety of consumer responses such as
perception, brand choice, information processing, risk taking, etc. [3,18-22]. Incidental emotions
pt
evoked from a previous experience (e.g., watching an ad that portrays hope while watching TV)
ce
may influence subsequent, unrelated decisions (e.g., how many chips you may eat while
watching TV). Recent research has documented effects such as incidental pride affecting
Ac
consumers’ uniqueness-seeking tendency when consumers attribute the feeling of pride to their
personal traits (vs. people who attribute pride to their effort) [23]. In this section, we review
some current views on how and when such incidental influences occur.
How do incidental emotions influence decisions? ATF [7] has helped researchers
understand the mechanism through which even unrelated (i.e., incidental) emotions influence
Page 8 of 17
9
decisions. The appraisals associated with incidental emotions predispose individuals to perceive
unrelated events in ways that are consistent with appraisal dimensions. This influence of
emotional appraisals can explain why effectiveness of the same message may differ, depending
on the emotion being incidentally experienced. For example, among people incidentally feeling
t
positive emotions (e.g. happiness, calmness), compatibility between emotional appraisals and
ip
referent in a message enhances message effectiveness [24]. In addition to some of the appraisals
cr
discussed previously, recent research has identified new appraisals to provide evidence for
us
differential effects of the same incidental emotions. For example, guilt arises from a behavior-
specific appraisal (e.g., I did not study hard) whereas shame arises from a global self-appraisal
an
(e.g., I am not an intelligent person). Thus, feelings of guilt lead consumers to adopt lower-level
construals and value secondary features (e.g., direct camera upload in an MP3 player) whereas
M
feelings of shame lead consumers to adopt higher-level construals and value primary features
(e.g., storage capacity in an MP3 player) [25]. Thus, guilt-laden consumers chose a product with
ed
unattractive primary features but attractive secondary features but shame-laden consumers chose
the reverse. This illustrates that incidental emotions can influence the construal level of
pt
consumers’ mindsets [25]. Another example of an influential appraisal is the temporal focus of
ce
the emotion. Emotions associated with future-focused appraisals (e.g., hope) increase consumers’
self-control and lead them to make healthier choices relative to emotions associated with present-
Ac
focused or past-focused appraisals (e.g., pride) [26]. In summary, incidental emotions can
influence unrelated decisions via processes such as enhancing message compatibility, changing
When do incidental emotions influence decisions? Sometimes, the surprising effect of the
same emotion having different effects can be explained by minor differences in the nature of
Page 9 of 17
10
incidental emotions and their interactions with contextual factors. For example, pride arising
from different sources affects decision-making differently. Pride experienced when self-
with their goals. However, pride experienced due to the appraisal of self-achievement leads to
t
indulgent choices because consumers disengage with the goal [27]. In a similar vein, presence of
ip
a goal can moderate the effect of incidental emotions on decisions [28,29]. Incidental sadness
cr
leads to more hedonic consumption because consumers are trying to regulate their feelings.
us
However, this effect is mitigated when a hedonic eating goal is salient because consumers try to
where the product is displayed, moderate the effect of incidental emotions [30]. For example,
M
incidental fear leads people to sell stocks early when individuals are led to believe that a peer
decides the value of the stock; this effect of fear on stock-selling does not occur when they are
ed
told that a computer decides stock values [31]. This is caused by ‘social projection’ that is,
people’s tendency to judge others’ state of mind based on their own state. Product display or
pt
physical placement can affect the evaluation of a product when consumers are feeling disgusted
ce
by a nearby, unrelated product [32]. When a disgust-eliciting product (e.g., sanitary napkin) was
placed in such a way that it touches a target product (e.g., a package of notebook paper),
Ac
consumers see the target product as contaminated and had less favorable evaluations of it,
In summary, emotions can affect decisions that are seemingly unrelated via appraisal
tendencies. Appraisals associated with each emotion shape consumers’ decisions by processes
such as influencing consumers’ construal levels or temporal focus. The effect of incidental
Page 10 of 17
11
emotions varies by decision contexts such as source of the emotion, presence of goal or physical
proximity.
t
An important but relatively understudied area in emotions research is how and when
ip
integral emotions and incidental emotions jointly influence consumer decisions. Emotional
cr
stimuli could interact with incidentally experienced emotional states and influence the manner in
us
which information is processed. Agrawal and Duhachek [33], for example, showed that anti-
drinking appeals that exacerbate guilt or shame were less effective among participants
an
incidentally feeling guilt or shame. When the emotional appeal exacerbated the emotional state
participants were already feeling, they defensively processed the information. This finding
M
demonstrated that negative emotional appeals could backfire when they were compatible with
incidental negative emotions. There has been limited research on this area, but future research
ed
can explore how integral and incidental emotions can interact as well as how they can contrast.
pt
both when embedded in marketing efforts and when carried over to the decision-making context
Ac
from an unrelated source. Under some conditions, the two forms may interact to jointly influence
consumers. Future research should delve into studying the influence of integral emotions on
consumers and contrast the effects of integral versus incidental emotions [7].
meaningful future direction is to explore when and why emotions help or hurt consumer
Page 11 of 17
12
decisions. A commonly held view is that emotional influences hurt consumers, such as incidental
fear or anger leading to risky decisions [31,33-35]. However, many findings reported in this
review show that emotions can have beneficial influences such as aiding self-regulation and
t
This article has summarized recent research that views emotions as antecedents to
ip
decision-making. In contrast to this ‘emotions as antecedents to decisions’ approach, an
cr
important stream of literature has examined how emotions can be outcomes of decision-making
us
and consumption [38-46]. Eating chocolates or buying gifts or purchasing experiences rather
than material products has been shown to make people happier [39-41]. Some of these findings
an
hint that consumers strategically make decisions to manage their emotions. The prevalence of the
phrase ‘retail therapy’ implies that consumers use consumption to manage their emotional states.
M
Blair and Roese [47] show that consumers attempt to reduce embarrassment induced by purchase
products. A related idea is that negative emotions are undesirable and should be repaired where
as positive emotions are desirable and should be increased or sustained [15, 39-41]. A fruitful
pt
line of enquiry is to study why and when consumers may do the opposite, that is, try to reduce a
ce
is rich scholarly potential in studying the processes through which emotions influence decision-
Page 12 of 17
13
REFERNCES:
1. Lambie JA, Marcel AJ: Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a
t
theoretical framework. Psychol Review 2002, 109(2):219-259.
ip
2. Smith CA, Ellsworth PC: Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol
1985, 48(4):813-838.
cr
3. Lerner JS, Keltner D: Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on
judgement and choice. Cog & Emo 2000, 14(4):473-493.
us
4. Lerner JS, Keltner D: Fear, anger, and risk. J Pers Soc Psychol 2001, 81(1):146-159.
5. So, J, Achar C, Han D, Agrawal N, Duhachek A, Maheswaran D: The psychology of
an
appraisal: specific emotions and decision-making. J Cons Psychol 2015, 25(3): 359-
371.
**This publication presents a framework of how appraisals associated with specific
M
emotions may influence decision-making. The authors highlight some nuanced
approaches of studying emotions and provide directions for future research in the domain
ed
of discrete emotions.
6. Han, S, Lerner, J S, & Keltner, D Feelings and consumer decision making: The appraisal-
tendency framework. J Cons Psychol 2007, 17(3): 158-168.
pt
7. Pham MT: Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical
evidence. Rev Gen Psychol 2007, 11(2): 155-178.
ce
9. Lee CJ, Andrade EB, Palmer SE: Interpersonal relationships and preferences for mood-
congruency in aesthetic experience. J Con Res 2013, 40(2):382-391.
10. Bagozzi RP, Moore DJ: Public service advertisements: emotions and empathy guide
prosocial behavior. J Mark 1994, 58(1):56-70.
11. Passyn K, Sujan M: Self-accountability emotions and fear appeals: motivating
behavior. J Cons Res 2006, 32(4):583-589.
Page 13 of 17
14
12. Cavanaugh LA, Bettman JR, Luce MF: Feeling love and doing more for distant others:
specific positive emotions differentially affect prosocial consumption. J Mark Res
2015, 52(5):657-673.
*This work demonstrates the differential effects of positive emotions in the context of
prosocial behavior. The authors demonstrate psychological processes through which
t
specific positive emotions may increase or decrease prosocial behavior towards different
ip
beneficiaries.
13. Small DA, Verrochi NM: The face of need: facial emotion expression on charity
cr
advertisements. J Mark Res 2009, 46(6):777-787.
14. Keller PA, Block LG: Increasing the persuasiveness of fear appeals: The effect of arousal
us
and elaboration. J Cons Res 1996, 22(4):448-459.
15. Duhachek A, Agrawal N, Han D: Guilt versus shame: coping, fluency, and framing in the
an
effectiveness of responsible drinking messages. J Mark Res 2012, 49(6):928-941.
16. Aaker JL, Williams P: Empathy versus pride: the influence of emotional appeals across
cultures. J Cons Res 1998, 25(3), 241-261.
M
17. Coleman NV, Williams P: Feeling like myself: emotion profiles and social identity. J Cons
Res 2013, 40(2):203-222.
ed
*This work brings together the literatures on consumers’ self-identity and emotions. This
is a good example of integration across contemporary research streams.
18. Agrawal N, Han D, Duhachek A: Emotional agency appraisals influence responses to
pt
preference inconsistent information. Org Beh Human Decision Proc 2013, 120(1):87-
97.
ce
* This research brings together the literature on discrete emotions and biased processing
of information. Studies provide evidence that incidental emotions differ in their response
Ac
Page 14 of 17
15
t
23. Huang X, Dong P, Mukhopadhyay A: Proud to belong or proudly different? Lay theories
ip
determine contrasting effects of incidental pride on uniqueness seeking. J Cons Res
2014, 41(3): 697-712.25.
cr
24. Agrawal N, Menon G, Aaker JL: Getting emotional about health. J Mark Res 2007,
us
44(1):100-113.28.
25. Han D, Duhachek A, Agrawal N: Emotions shape decisions through construal level: the
case of guilt and shame. J Cons Res 2014, 41(4):1047-1064.
an
*This paper shows that specific emotions can systematically alter individuals’ construal
level (i.e., abstract from shame and vs. concrete from guilt) due to the appraisals
associated with those emotions. This is a demonstration of how emotions not only
M
provide information and motivate, but affect general psychological processes.
26. Winterich KP, Haws KL: Helpful hopefulness: the effect of future positive emotions on
ed
28. Salerno A, Laran J, Janiszewski C: Hedonic eating goals and emotion: when sadness
decreases the desire to indulge. J Con Res 2014, 41(1):135-151.
ce
29. Salerno A, Laran J, Janiszewski C: Pride and regulatory behavior: the influence of
appraisal information and self-regulatory goals. J Cons Res 2015, 42(3): 499-513.
Ac
30. Garg N, Inman JJ, Mittal V: Incidental and task-related affect: A re-inquiry and
extension of the influence of affect on choice. J Cons Res 2005, 32(1):154-159.
31. Lee CJ, Andrade EB: Fear, social projection, and financial decision making. J Mark Res
2011, 48(SPL):121-129.
32. Morales AC, Fitzsimons GJ: Product contagion: changing consumer evaluations through
physical contact with “disgusting” products. J Mark Res 2007, 44(2): 272- 283.
Page 15 of 17
16
t
35. McFerran B, Aquino K, Tracy J: Evidence for two facets of pride in consumption:
ip
findings from luxury brands. J Con Psychol 2014, 24(4):455-471.
36. Gardner MP, Wansink B, Kim J, Park SB: Better moods for better eating? How mood
cr
influences food choice. J Con Psychol 2014, 24(3):320-335.
37. Laran J: Consumer Goal Pursuit. Curr Opinion Psychol 2015, 10(Aug): 22-26.
us
38. Chandran S, Menon G: When a day means more than a year: effects of temporal framing
on judgments of health risk. J Con Res 2004, 31(2):375-389.
an
39. Dunn EW, Aknin LB, Norton MI: Spending money on others promotes happiness.
Science 2008, 319(5870):1687-1688.
40. Dunn EW, Gilbert DT, Wilson TD: If money doesn't make you happy, then you probably
M
aren't spending it right. J Con Psychol 2011, 21(2):115-125.
41. Bhattacharjee A, Mogilner C: Happiness from ordinary and extraordinary experiences. J
ed
43. Lee S, Winterich KP, Ross Jr WT: I’m moral, but I won’t help you: the distinct role of
empathy and justice donations. J Con Res 2014, 41(3):678-696.
ce
44. Luce MF, Payne JW, Bettman JR: Emotional trade-off difficulty and choice. J Mark Res
1999, 36(1):143-159.
Ac
Page 16 of 17
Figure 1. Integrative model of emotional influences on consumer behavior. Both integral
and incidental emotions influence decision-making via cognitive appraisals.
t
ip
cr
us
an
M
ed
pt
ce
Ac
Page 17 of 17