Development of Attachment PDF
Development of Attachment PDF
Development of Attachment PDF
Attachment is the strong, affectionate tie we have with special people in our
lives that leads us to feel pleasure when we interact with them and to be
comforted by their nearness during times of stress.
By the second half of the first year, infants have become attached to familiar
people who have responded to their needs. Watch how babies of this age
single out their parents for special attention. When the mother enters the
room, the baby breaks into a broad, friendly smile. When she picks him up,
he pats her face, explores her hair, and snuggles against her body. When he
feels anxious, he crawls into her lap and clings closely.
Freud first suggested that the infant's emotional tie to the mother provides
the foundation for all later relationships. Although Freud was correct that the
quality of the infant—parent bond is vitally important, contemporary
research indicates that his ideas merit revision: The contribution of
attachment to long-term development depends not just on the infant's early
experiences but also on the ongoing parent—child relationship.
Psychoanalytic perspective regards feeding as the central context in which
caregivers and babies build this close emotional bond. Behaviorism, too,
emphasizes the importance of feeding, but for different reasons. According
to a well-known behaviorist explanation, as the caregiver satisfies the baby's
hunger (primary drive), the infant learns to prefer her soft caresses, warm
smiles, and tender words of comfort (secondary drive) because these events
have been paired with tension relief.
Although feeding is an important context in which mothers and babies
build a close relationship, attachment does not depend on hunger
satisfaction.
In the 1950s, a famous experiment showed that rhesus monkeys reared
with terry-cloth and wire-mesh "surrogate mothers" clung to the soft
terry-cloth substitute, even though the wire-mesh "mother" held the
bottle and infants had to climb on it to be fed (Harlow & Zimmerman,
1959).
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_O60TYAIgC4&feature=emb_logo
John Bowlby (1969), who first applied this idea to the infant—caregiver
bond, was originally a psychoanalyst. In his theory, he retained the
psychoanalytic idea that quality of attachment to the caregiver has profound
implications for the child's feelings of security and capacity to form trusting
relationships.
He believed that the human infant, like the young of other animal species, is
endowed with a set of built-in behaviors that keep the parent nearby to
protect the infant from danger and to provide support for exploring and
mastering the environment (Waters & Cummings, 2000).
Contact with the parent also ensures that the baby will be fed, but Bowlby
pointed out that feeding is not the basis for attachment. Rather, the
attachment bond has strong biological roots. It can best be understood in an
evolutionary context in which survival of the species—through guaranteeing
both safety and competence—is of utmost importance.
According to Bowlby, the infant's relationship with the parent begins as a set
of innate signals that calls the adult to the baby's side. Over time, a true
affectionate bond develops, supported by new emotional and cognitive
capacities as well as by a history of warm, sensitive care. Attachment
develops in four phases:
With age, children depend less on the physical proximity of caregivers and
more on a sense of confidence that they will be accessible and responsive in
times of need.
According to Bowlby (1980), out of their experiences during these four
phases, children construct an enduring affectionate tie that they use as a
secure base in the parents' absence. This image serves as an internal
working model, or set of expectations about the availability of attachment
figures, their likelihood of providing support during times of stress, and the
Self's interaction with those figures. The internal working model becomes a
vital part of personality, serving as a guide for all future close relationships
(Bretherton & Munholland, 1999).
With age, children constantly “update” or revise and expand—the internal
working model as their cognitive, emotional, and social capacities increase,
as they interact further with parents, and as they form other close bonds with
adults, siblings, and friends.
INFANT CHARACTERISTICS
Because attachment is the result of a relationship that builds between two
partners, infant characteristics should affect how easily it is established.
We saw that pre-maturity, birth complications and newborn illness make
caregiving more taxing.
In stressed, poverty-stricken families, these difficulties are linked to
attachment insecurity (Wille, 1991).
But when parents have the time and patience to care for a baby with special
needs and view their infants positively, at-risk newborns fare quite well in
attachment security (Cox, Hopkins & Hans, 2000; Pederson & Mofan,
1995).
Infants also vary considerably in temperament, but its role in attachment
security has been intensely debated. Some researchers believe that infants
who are irritable and fearful may simply react to brief separations with
intense anxiety, regardless of the parent's sensitivity to the baby (Kagan,
1998). Consistent with this view, emotionally reactive, difficult babies are
more likely to develop later insecure attachments (van IJzendoorn et al.,
2004; Vaughn & Bost, 1999).
But other evidence suggests that caregiving is involved in the relationship
between infant difficultness and attachment insecurity.
In a study extending from birth to age 2, difficult infants more often had
highly anxious mothers, a combination that often resulted in a
"disharmonious relationship" in the second year—characterized by both
maternal insensitivity and attachment insecurity.
Infant difficultness and maternal anxiety seemed to perpetuate one another,
impairing caregiving and the security of the parent—infant bond.
In another investigation that focused on disorganized/disoriented babies,
negative emotional reactivity increased sharply between 12 and 18 months.
Attachment disorganization was not caused by a difficult temperament;
instead, it preceded difficultness and seemed to promote it (Barnett,
Ganiban, & Cicchetti, 1999).
Indeed, extensive research confirms that caregiving can override the impact
of infant characteristics on attachment security.
When researchers combined data from more than a thousand mother—infant
pairs, they found that maternal problems—such as mental illness, teenage
parenthood, and child abuse—were associated with a sharp rise in
attachment insecurity. In contrast, child problems—ranging from
prematurity and developmental delays to serious physical disabilities and
psychological disorders—had little impact on attachment quality (van
IJzendoorn et al., 1992).
Finally, if children's traits determined security of attachment, we would
expect attachment to be at least moderately heritable, just as temperament is.
Yet twin comparisons reveal that the heritability of attachment is virtually
nil (O'Connor & Croft, 2001). Identical twins are not more alike than
fraternal twins (or other siblings) in attachment security. About two-thirds of
siblings—whether identical twins, fraternal twins, non-twin siblings,
unrelated siblings, or foster infants—establish similar attachment patterns
with their parent. Yet these siblings often differ in temperament (Dozier et
al., 2001; van IJzendoorn, 1995). This suggests that the strongest
environmental influences on attachment security are non-shared experiences,
reflecting most parents' efforts to adjust their caregiving to each child's
individual needs.
FAMILY CIRCUMSTANCES
As we have indicated in this and previous chapters, quality of caregiving can
be fully understood only in terms of the larger context of the parent—child
relationship. Job loss, a failing marriage, financial difficulties, and other
stressors can undermine attachment by interfering with the sensitivity of
parental care. Or these stressors can affect babies' sense of security directly,
by exposing them to angry adult interactions or unfavorable child-care
arrangements (Thompson & Raikes, 2003).
The arrival of a new sibling illustrates how family circumstances can affect
attachment quality.
In one study, first-born preschoolers who declined in attachment security
after the birth of a baby had mothers who were depressed, anxious, or hostile
before the birth. These symptoms were associated with marital friction
(which the first-borns probably sensed) as well as with unfavorable
mother—first-born interaction. When mothers had cooperative marriages,
coped well with the second birth, and stayed involved with their older child,
preschoolers maintained a secure attachment bond (Teti et al., 1996).
The availability of social supports, especially a good mother—father
relationship and mutual assistance with caregiving, reduces family stress and
predicts greater attachment security (Owen & Cox, 1997).