The Effects of Short-Term Sensory Deprivation On Heart Rate in Humans
The Effects of Short-Term Sensory Deprivation On Heart Rate in Humans
The Effects of Short-Term Sensory Deprivation On Heart Rate in Humans
RATE IN HUMANS
1
Abstract
To find the effects of sensory deprivation on human heart rate, 16 college students
had their heart rate measured before and after two walks around the atrium of the Beloit
College Science Center—one without and one with a blindfold. The results showed no
definite pattern, and did not lead to any conclusion in regard to the actual effects of
sensory deprivation on heart rate but did lead to much theorizing on why the results were
this way. It was concluded that in order to observe the effect of sensory deprivation,
subjects must be observed in a closed space for extended periods of time and not exert
themselves physically.
Introduction
The aim of this experiment was to find the effect of sensory deprivation on heart
United States and other governments. We also were interested in human phobias, but
perspective. Though the experiment had several variables, we chose to focus on only one
half of the trials so that the only variable would be visual deprivation. Much of the
previous research that we examined was found in John P. Zubek’s Sensory Deprivation:
Fifteen Years. Though this book is dated, it provides an extensive amount of information
on sensory deprivation and brings together many different studies from various sources
that proved relevant to our experiment. Another useful source was a 1990 article from the
2
Occupational performance of a paced secondary task under conditions of sensory
deprivation. I. Heart rate changes in train drivers as a result of monotony, which offered
insight into the real causes of perceived rises in heart rate from sensory deprivation.
COPD, from the journal Chest, concluded that sensory deprivation raises heart rate when
combined with physical exertion. Most of the information we gathered implied that heart
short-term sensory deprivation would raise both heart rate and blood pressure as a result
of anxiety, as we did our outside research after the study was completed.
The test group for the study consisted of sixteen college students ages 18-22
enrolled in Human Biology. Each student went through two circuits of the experiment. A
walking loop was designed in the atrium of the Beloit College Science Center where test
subjects were led by the Teaching Assistant, Leah. The first time the subject walked
without a blindfold but agreed to keep their eyes on the ground. The second time they
walked the loop with a blindfold. A scarf was used as the blindfold. Eight of the trials
climaxed in a ‘scare’ where a student participant would jump out at a test subject and
surprise them during their walk. Every student was aware that there was a 50% chance
they would be ‘scared’ during their walk. Every subject had their blood pressure and
heart rate taken before they began walking the loop. They had these measurements taken
again directly after being scared or at the end of the loop if there was no scare. We used a
digital sphygmomanometer to test the blood pressure and heart rate. Heather was in
3
charge of taking and writing down the data. The experiment was not conducted in an
isolated environment; there were students and faculty walking through the Science Center
Results
After completing our experiment we assessed that there was no substantial trend
in heart rate change. We felt that results were inconclusive in regards to change in heart
We arranged our data on heart rate and blood pressure (systolic and diastolic)
based on gender, and whether the subject was scared and/or blindfolded. We specifically
examined the subjects’ change in heart rate in no-scare trials, with and without blindfold,
as shown in Fig. 1. Each of the eight no-scare participants in the experiment went
through the loop with and without a blindfold. The blue columns represent the change in
heart rate after loops done without a blindfold and the red columns represent the change
Fig. 1
4
Discussion
We found in our results that the change in heart rate varied greatly between each
change in heart rate during the control trial. In Sensory Deprivation, Zubek cites a study
in which the heart rate of participants dropped significantly while they were subject to
sensory deprivation. (Zubek, 61) However, the study that Zubek cites took place over the
course of 8 hours while our trials took at most under 2 minutes. In addition, because 5 out
of 8 of the participants experienced a rise in heart rate after the non-blindfolded trial, we
must assume that the physical exertion of walking around the atrium was responsible in
part for any rise in heart rate, and therefore it cannot be determined how much of the
We feel there were many ways the study could have been improved. We decided
to specifically examine the no-scare test group because we felt that the ‘scares’ were not
approaches to scaring and depending on the person and the sound they chose to make
there was a large variation in how frightening the situation was. We also feel that the
Science Center atrium is not an isolated enough environment to hold a successful sensory
deprivation experiment in because there were too many outside stimuli. We also think
that both the short-term nature of the deprivation and the amount of time it takes to
We also think that a more effective study would use multiple-sense deprivation.
We are curious to see the effect on heart rate if a person were led with their sense of
sight, smell and hearing all blocked. The experiment would no longer include the ‘scare
5
factor,’ as frightening a person who cannot see, hear or smell would be difficult, but we
6
Literature Cited