Developmental Psychology
Developmental Psychology
Developmental Psychology
developmental
PSYCHOLOGY
Editors: Jacki Watts, Kate Cockcroft and Norman Duncan
Developmental Psychology
First published 2002 by UCT Press, an imprint of Juta & Company Ltd
Reprinted January 2004
Reprinted January 2009
Second edition June 2009, ISBN 978-1-91989-515-4
Reprinted 2013 by Juta & Company Ltd
Reprinted 2015
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means,
electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage or retrieval system,
without prior permission in writing from the publisher. Subject to any applicable licensing terms and conditions
in the case of electronically supplied publications, a person may engage in fair dealing with a copy of this pub-
lication for his or her personal or private use, or his or her research or private study. See Section 12(1)(a) of the
Copyright Act 98 of 1978.
Project Manager: Debbie Henry
Editor: Juliet Haw
Proofreader: Mariana Swart
Indexer: Sanet le Roux
Typesetter: Petal Design
Cover designer: Petal Design
Cover Illustration: Owned by Jacki Watts, artist unknown.
Notes on contributors vi
Acknowledgements x
Introduction 1
Jacki Watts
6. Wilfred Bion: Thinking, feeling and the search for truth 112
Gavin Ivey
References 639
Index 678
vi Developmental Psychology
Notes on contributors
The editors
Dr Jacki Watts is a clinical psychologist in private practice in Johannesburg. She was a senior lecturer
in the psychology department at the University of the Witwatersrand where she ran the Clinical
Psychology Professional Training programme for six years. Her primary academic interests are within
the arena of psychopathology, relating to its development and to clinical intervention problems, areas
in which she has published. She is actively involved in the psychoanalytic community in Johannesburg.
She is a keen gardener, assisted by her cats and disadvantaged by her dogs. Unlike some of the
contributors she thought of running a marathon but sanity prevailed.
Associate Prof Kate Cockcroft is an associate professor at the University of the Witwatersrand where
she works as an academic/psychologist. She has published both nationally and internationally in the
field of cognitive psychology, especially language, intelligence and memory. Her original career goal
was to be a graphic designer but fate intervened and she has found her niche in academia. She has,
however, been going to art classes with two professional artists for more than 20 years and considers
herself ‘still an amateur artist’.
Prof Norman Duncan holds a professorship in psychology at the University of the Witwatersrand,
where he currently also serves as the Head of the School of Human and Community Development.
His research and publications are primarily in the fields of racism and community psychology. He has
co-edited a range of volumes, including ‘Race’, racism, knowledge production and psychology in South
Africa (Nova Science Publications, 2001).
The contributors
Dr Derek Hook is a lecturer in the Institute of Social Psychology at the London School of Eco-
nomics and Political Science. He is also a research fellow in psychology at the University of the
Witwatersrand. The overarching focus of his research concerns the attempt to develop an ‘analytics
of power’ that is able to grapple with the unconscious and psychological dimensions of racism
and ideological subjectivity. He is the author of Foucault, Psychology & the Analytics of Power
(Palgrave, 2007), the editor of Critical Psychology (University of Cape Town Press, 2004) and the co-
ordinator of Psychoanalysis@LSE, a multi-disciplinary research group that aims to advance the use
of psychoanalysis as a distinctive means of sociological and political analysis. Dr Hook was the
recipient of an LSE teaching award in 2008, and is one of the founding editors of the Palgrave-
Macmillan journal Subjectivity. Aside from the distractions of academic life, Derek is a taster of
wines, shiraz, merlot and cabernet being his favourite cultivars and is a half-marathon runner.
Prof Cora Smith is a clinical psychologist and an adjunct professor in the Department of Neurosciences,
Division of Psychiatry at the University of the Witwatersrand. She coordinates the Wits Clinical
Internship Programme within the Faculty of Health Sciences. She obtained her PhD and MA (Clin.
Psych.) at the University of the Witwatersrand. Her interests lie in the development and treatment of
personality pathology while her passion lies in driving sexy sports cars.
Prof Gavin Ivey is an associate professor and coordinator of the PhD Programme at the University
of the Witwatersrand. He is the current editor of the journal Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy in
Notes on contributors vii
South Africa. He is a member of the Institute for Psychodynamic Child Psychotherapy, where
he runs seminars on W R Bion’s contribution to psychoanalysis. He has published papers on
psychoanalytic theory and therapy in The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, British Journal of
Psychotherapy, British Journal of Medical Psychology, American Journal of Psychotherapy and Psycho-
therapy: Theory/Research/Practice/Training. He is a keen runner and yoga enthusiast but has yet to master
the headstand.
Dr Carol Long is a senior lecturer at the University of the Witwatersrand where she co-ordinates the
Clinical Masters professional training programme. She is also a practising clinical psychologist. Her
recent publications include papers on psychoanalytic theory and practice, on HIV-positive motherhood
and on the relationships between gender and race. She is interested in studies of identities, particularly
marginalised identities, and in psychoanalytic theory and practice. She loves cooking and painting.
Her loved ones enjoy her cooking and tolerate her paintings on the walls.
Associate Prof Sally Swartz is an associate professor in the Psychology Department of the University
of Cape Town, where she is both an academic and a psychotherapist. Sally has published widely
in the areas of psychotherapy and the history of psychiatry in South Africa. She has particular
interest in the challenges of working as a psychoanalytic psychotherapist in South Africa, and her
work explores the effects of race, class and gender divides on experiences of trauma and healing. She
has been involved with clinical psychology training at the University of Cape Town for many years
and, more recently, has become involved with senior leadership in her university, first as Head of the
Department of Psychology, and more recently as Deputy Dean in the Faculty of Humanities. She is
managed at home by two demanding dogs that walk her regularly on the mountain. Free head-space
is used for writing poems.
Dr Lee Senior is a clinical psychologist who holds a PhD from the University of the Witwatersrand.
She has pursued her interest in children’s emotional, social and intellectual development through her
work in private practice and within a remedial school environment, and has lectured in the area of
personality development. Lee assists at ICAS as a researcher. She has a family and is a devoted mother
of beautiful children.
Nicky Israel is a senior tutor in the Department of Psychology at the University of the Wit-
watersrand. She has published locally with her special areas of interest being research design,
psychometrics and assessment, intelligence and cognition, teaching and learning, particularly
learning styles and strategies, motivation and performance and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder.
Aside from her passion for academics she has seen Star Wars: Episode IV over 100 times, a feat not many
could match!
James Grant B Proc LLB, BA Hons (Psyc), PhD (Candidate), advocate of the High Court of South
Africa and senior lecturer in the School of Law at the University of the Witwatersrand. James lectures
and specialises in criminal law. He completed his psychology honours on the moral reasoning of Moses
Sithole, South Africa’s most notorious serial killer, and continues this research presently. His PhD is
entitled ‘The Responsible Mind in South African Criminal Law’ and concerns an investigation of the
foundation for the insanity plea and, more generally, conditions for criminal responsibility in criminal
law known as the requirement of voluntariness and criminal capacity. He has published in the areas
of psychological development, the law of delict and criminal law, and has addressed international con-
ferences on the compatibility of determinism, neuroscience and criminal responsibility, and the effect
of emotion on moral reasoning and criminal responsibility. He lives in the country with his wife, his
dogs, a pony and many chickens.
viii Developmental Psychology
Michael Greyling, MSc [Statistics], BSc Hons [Industrial Psychology] is a lecturer in the Department
of Psychology at the University of the Witwatersrand. He is a statistician seconded to the Psychology
Department to lecture on Statistics and Research. He has published widely on various research topics.
His primary academic interests are within the field of applied cognitive psychology and problems of
applied statistical analysis. He is currently doing a PhD that looks at a longitudinal analysis of person-
ality as it applies to trauma experiences. He acts as a consultant to businesses, using statistical tools to
understand various organisational problems. Mike trains dogs [and handlers] on the weekend and his
Boxer competes in working trials.
Dr Kevin Thomas is a senior lecturer in the Department of Psychology at the University of Cape
Town. He teaches neuropsychology to undergraduates, and research methods, neuropsychology and
cognitive-behavioural therapy to postgraduates. His primary academic interests and areas in which
he has published include the neuropsychology of spatial cognition and the effects of stress/trauma on
various aspects of memory. He is a member of the Executive Committee of the South African Neuro-
psychological Association. Outside of academia, he is a warden at one of UCT’s student residences,
and he works with his wife to maintain the delicate balance of power between Buddy, the prince of
Toy Poodles, and Suzi, the Maltese princess.
Marianne Ball is a postgraduate student in psychological research at the University of Cape Town. She
is presently studying neuropsychology and doing research in the field of forensic psychology, investi-
gating the experiences of child witnesses. She is very interested in developmental issues and has spent
time working with children with behaviour disorders in school environments. She is an avid traveller,
gardener and yoga enthusiast.
Michelle Robberts is a postgraduate student at the University of Cape Town. She completed her BSc
at Stellenbosch University. She currently studies research psychology and has a particular interest in
developmental neuropsychology and cognitive psychology. Her research focuses on the development
of theory of mind in children, particularly children with autism. She waits for the day that she can get
a genetically modified hypo-allergenic cat.
Dr Gillian (Gill) Mooney is a lecturer in the Department of Psychology at the University of the Wit-
watersrand. Gill teaches Human Development to undergraduate students. In terms of postgraduate
commitments, she teaches Gender to the MA Research students, Graduate Writing Skills to Honours
and Masters students and is involved in the bursar training programme, ie both tutoring and marking.
She has published in the areas of teaching and learning in higher education. In the department, Gill is
thought to have some idiosyncratic value because of her wicked wit.
Notes on contributors ix
Dr Garth Stevens is a clinical psychologist and senior lecturer at the University of the Witwatersrand.
His research interests include violence and its prevention, as well as studies in social inequality and
difference in the context of racialised, gendered and classist social formations. He has published and
presented much of his research both nationally and internationally, and is a co-editor of the book,
A RACE against time: Psychology and challenges to deracialisation in South Africa (University of South
Africa Press, 2006). A Capetonian by birth, he now resides in Johannesburg with his partner and two
children. During his free time he actively fantasises about grand schemes that will one day allow them
all to spend long, lazy days on white, sandy beaches with crystal-clear, blue waters.
Patrick Connolly has taught psychology at university level since 2004 and currently holds a lectureship
at the University of the Witwatersrand. He has a Masters degree from Wits in community-counselling
psychology and is registered as a counselling psychologist. He trains on the Clinical Masters programme
at Wits, and supervises students’ work at the Centre for the Study of Violence and Reconciliation (CSVR),
which is a trauma clinic in Johannesburg. He also trains student counsellors on the Wits trauma model.
Since 2002 he has worked at Gateway House, a residential psychiatric commune, and continues to provide
psychotherapy and training services to the organisation. His fields of interest include trauma studies,
psychoanalytic studies and psychopathology, particularly schizophrenia and bipolar mood disorders. He
currently runs a part-time private practice. His hobbies include Kung-fu, Tai-chi, and he can also play a
fairly serious game of chess. He is currently unmarried and is unsure of how long this will last!
Gillian Eagle is a professor of psychology in the School of Human and Community Development at
the University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg. She is a qualified clinical psychologist and has been
involved in academia for over 20 years. She has a particular interest in traumatic stress and in gender
issues and has published a number of articles in these areas as well as co-edited a book with Derek Hook.
She runs a small private therapeutic practice and has done extensive work in the NGO sector, primarily
involving training, supervision and consulting in the traumatic stress field. Gill is a very accomplished
potter. She also loves to dance and given the right music can get a party mood going.
Dr Mambwe Kasese-Hara is a senior lecturer at the School of Human & Community Development,
at the University of the Witwatersrand. She holds a PhD from Manchester University. Her teaching
and research interests lie in the areas of developmental and health psychology, specifically maternal
and child psychology, race and cultural issues, and HIV/AIDS. Her interests in psychology are broad-
based and reflect a study and work background of over 15 years in South Africa, Zambia and the
United Kingdom. Apart from her academic interests she loves to tend to her garden, and loves a good
sitcom or stand-up comic as her philosophy is that a good laugh helps one to unwind.
Peace Kiguwa lectures in gender studies and critical social psychology in the School of Human and
Community Development at the University of the Witwatersrand. She is co-editor of Critical Psychology
and The Gender of Psychology (both pubished by UCT Press). Her research interests include critical
race theories, critical psychology, gender and teaching and learning. She is also an avid photographer
and is known to inundate friends and family with rather ‘interesting’ snapshots of life.
Prof Catriona Macleod is head of the Department of Psychology at Rhodes University. She has
published widely in national and international journals on teenage pregnancy, abortion and feminist
theory. She has two children and practises yoga when time allows.
Acknowledgements
Illustrations and Photographs:
Belinda Karpelowsky: pp 37, 73, 138, 215, 261, 283, 324, 346, 347, 354, 358, 379, 380, 396, 458, 501.
Andre Plant: pp 10, 57, 234, 237, 247, 330, 336.
We would like to thank the following photographers for allowing us to use their work:
Bridget Corke at Bridget Corke Photography
iAfrika
Cha Johnston
David Lurie from his title, Cape Town Fringe
MediaClub South Africa
Zoë Moosman
Guy Stubbs
Michele Vrdoljak
Text:
Pg 172: Borderline organisation in the nursery: Tinker Bell
HarperCollins Publisher Ltd © James, L (2007). Tigger on the couch: The neuroses, psychoses,
disorders and maladies of our favourite childhood characters.
Pgs 581-582:
This Be The Verse from Collected Poems by Phillip Larkin. Used by permission of Faber and Faber Ltd.
Excerpt from “This Be The Verse” from COLLECTED POEMS by Philip Larkin. Copyright ©
1988, 2003 by the Estate of Philip Larkin. Reprinted by permission of Farrar, Straus and
Giroux, LLC.
Introduction
Jacki Watts
you think through the basic assumptions you will see that these assumptions still hold: the
foundation is firm but the style of the building has been revised and/or adapted to meet
ever-increasing knowledge and experience which affects how we see and understand the
human psyche. Nonetheless the role of the unconscious remains as the principle under-
pinning to human functioning. This section reflects on various ways of conceptualising
how the unconscious is lived, influenced and structured through development especially
during the formative years of childhood, and how the environment may assist or hamper
relatively optimum development.
We focus on Melanie Klein and Wilfred Bion as the purists of object relations
thinking; on the work of Ronald Dodds Fairbairn who essentially worked alone and
yet effected a significant influence upon the development of psychoanalytic theory; on
Donald Winnicott, the charismatic paediatrician who changed forever the way
psychoanalysts think about mothers and babies. We then go to the United States and
consider two of the most influential American psychoanalysts, Otto Kernberg and Heinz
Kohut. We also look at Erik Erikson and his conceptualisation of development across
the life-span. We cross back to France and take a look at one aspect of Lacan’s theorising:
what he terms the ‘mirror phase’ of development. We also examine how psychoanalytic
theory has influenced thinking about the role of attachment and its importance in the
development of the personality.
As you go through the Section One it will be very evident how far psychoanalytic
theorising has moved from Classical Psychoanalysis. This divergence is not new to the
history of psychoanalysis. From its inception, despite Freud’s firm attempts to prevent
his disciples straying from the mainstream of his thought, there have been significant
breakaways. Take a look at the genealogy of psychoanalysis in Chapter 3 to see quite
how expanded and diverse the family tree has become. One of the most significant, and
perhaps the most emotionally troubling, events for Freud was his break with Carl Jung,
whose theory is also described in this book.
This broad overview indicates very clearly the depth and diversity of psycho-
analytic thought and hopefully motivates you to pursue avenues which have caught
your imagination.
Section Two, on Cognitive Development, reflects on the hows and whys of the
development of a thinking mind. Explanations are given of how humans develop their
ability to function cognitively, based on the notion that humans are active processors of
information, not passive receivers of information. Thus the theories focus on the ways in
which people acquire various cognitive abilities and how these abilities change in structure
and function over time. In this overall understanding one has also to consider individual
differences in cognitive performance which cover not only differences with regards to age
but also alternate patterns of development and alternate developmental outcomes.
This section examines three theorists, namely Piaget, Vygotsky and Kohlberg. In
addition it investigates a number of ‘themes’ related to cognitive development.
With regard to the theorists, Piaget’s constructivist theory of cognitive development
concerns his understanding of how we start life with limited cognitive capacities and
hopefully, and eventually, reach a level of being able to think symbolically. His theory links
our cognitive development to adaptations to the environment. Vygotsky offers a socio-
historical account of cognitive development, postulating the importance of internalised
cultural and environmental mores and values while Kohlberg focuses upon moral
development as an aspect of cognitive development and thus offers an account of the
development of moral reasoning based upon levels of cognitive capacities.
Introduction 3
The thematic approach offered in this section considers the development of memory,
language and intelligence, each within a chapter devoted to the exploration of each
theme. Two additional chapters lead to a deeper understanding of cognitive psychology:
the chapter on Evolutionary psychology, which considers the impact of our evolutionary
history on our cognitive development. (Although a challenging chapter it provides some
intriguing links to both cognitive development and psychoanalytic postulations.) Finally
there is the chapter that explores the contributions of cognitive science approaches to
cognitive developmental psychology.
Section Three, on Psychosocial and socio-political contexts of development has two
foci. One highlights particular areas of concern as they impact upon development, such
as violent crime, childhood trauma, gender and race and culture in the South African
context. The authors consider how development may be compromised by experiences of
these difficult contexts.
The second focus is theory-based. The authors reflect on Bronfenbrenner’s ecological
theory of development, overview critical issues inherent in developmental psychology,
and offer a review of theory and South African developmental psychology research and
literature.
I would like to make brief mention here of the use of the male pronoun in many
chapters of this textbook. When denoting the baby or child we have used the pronoun
‘he’ as opposed to ‘he or she’ or ‘it’. We felt that using the format of ‘he or she’ or ‘he/she’
is clumsy and distracting, and therefore have opted to use only ‘he’. We hope that this
does not offend any readers.
CHAPTER
1
Basic concepts and
principles in developmental
psychology
Derek Hook and Kate Cockcroft
The next five or six decades [after childhood] are every bit as impor-
tant, not only to those adults who are passing through them but to
their children, who must live with and understand parents and grand-
parents. The changes in body, personality and abilities through these
later decades are great. Developmental tasks are imposed by marriage
and parenthood, by the waxing and waning of physical prowess and
of some intellectual capacities, by the children’s flight from the nest, by
the achievement of an occupational plateau and by retirement and the
prospect of final extinction. Parents have always been fascinated by
their children’s development, but it is high time adults began to look
objectively at themselves, to examine the systematic changes in their
own physical, mental and emotional qualities, as they pass through the
life-span, and to get acquainted with the limitations and assets they
share with so many others of their age.
Theseus was the captain of an old ship substance. In time he even lengthened the
that he loved dearly, and that he had boat, and exchanged the sails for a
sailed for many years. His ship was rudimentary engine. Eventually the ship
dilapidated, however, and badly in need looked totally different to the vessel he
of repair. Unfortunately Theseus did not had begun with—in almost every way.
have much money. What he opted to do His friends would joke with him, teasing
was to slowly repair the broken parts of him that he now had a new ship after all.
the vessel, when he had the money. His Theseus was always dismissive of these
friends often asked him if he dreamed of a jokes, saying that despite many changes it
new ship as complete and sturdy as his was still his original boat, in all the ways it
own ship was flimsy and in need of repair. counted most. Several years later it came
Theseus always laughed at this, saying to pass that every original element of his
that he would never want a new vessel, boat had been replaced—not one tiny
because he very much loved his own ship, part of the original vessel remained. One
and felt very loyal to it. Nevertheless, he of Theseus’s friends brought this to his
made repairs when he could afford to do attention, and again asked him: ‘Do you
so. First he bought new sails. Then he still believe that is your original ship?’
pulled down the old wooden mast and Theseus could not answer him.
replaced it with a fibreglass substitute. He What would your answer be? Is this still
ran short of money for a while, and could the original ship or is it a totally different
not make many more changes. vessel? Reflect on how this story might be
A few months later, however, he an effective analogy for personal develop-
inherited a substantial amount of money ment across the life-span. To what extent
and was able to replace many of the do our bodies, our identities and our
broken parts of the ship. The first things personal psychologies change over the
to go were the old rudder and the old life-span? Do we remain the same person
anchor; they were replaced with newer before and after birth, before and after
and far more sophisticated pieces of puberty, after the birth of a child, after
equipment. He changed the wooden the death of a parent? How much do
floor of the vessel for a stronger lighter we change across our life-span?
Neonate. A baby in the Research has disproved the old idea that neonates, or newborn
first month of life. babies, do nothing but eat, sleep and cry, while remaining oblivious
to the world ... newborns come equipped with a number of useful
reflexes ... such as those that control breathing ... Some enable
babies to nurse. The rooting reflex causes them to turn the head
towards the touch of a nipple on the cheek and grope around with
the mouth ... the swallowing reflex enables them to swallow milk
and other liquids without choking ... The grasping reflex causes
newborns to cling vigorously to an adult’s finger or to any other
object placed in their hands. The stepping reflex causes very young
babies to take what looks like walking steps if they
are held upright with their feet just touching a flat
surface.
If you or I were in the womb, what would it senses, although at this point they are
be like? Well, it would be like being in the totally blurred:
middle of someone else’s body. Their heart,
which is bigger than you, would be Instead of having separate senses ...
thundering away. Around you would be [it seems] ... that a fetus’s senses are much
metres and metres of arteries and veins filled less distinct. Sounds, for instance, may be
with someone else’s blood. How would you ‘felt’ through the skin, as well as heard by
get any peace when, above you, two the ears. Likewise, changes in the fluid
cavernous lungs worked day and night? which surrounds the fetus will be as much
Worse still, right next to you would be the smelled as tasted. Sounds from the outside
biggest distraction of all ... [the stomach]. world will be filtered through the mother’s
Three meals a day and who knows how many body. So although no doubt a fetus will be
ice-cream and gherkin sandwiches have got familiar with the sound of its mother’s
to go somewhere. And to top it all, you voice, this voice will sound, relative to our
would be growing all the time ... (Dale, own ears [somewhat] ... distorted (Dale
De’ath, Evans, Thompson, Georgi & Spencer, et al, 1998).
1998).
It is for these reasons that infants are
In other words, the world inside the womb said to have some familiarity with their
is anything but simple, serene, silent and mother’s voice, even before they are
devoid of stimulus. born. Similarly, it is for these reasons
that rhythmic sounds, such as a vacuum
The real world inside the womb is dynamic cleaner or music, often prove soothing
and bustling ... Instead of lying quietly ... to newborns after birth even if they are
[the fetus] is having a go at a kick, or even fairly loud because they remind the child
a somersault. It gulps, and swallows up to of the womb. Indeed, in the womb,
half a cup of amniotic fluid every day. From the volume of noise around the foetus is
quite early on it sucks its thumb, a habit able to reach the formidable level of
which may take years to break ... (Dale ninety-five decibels with each beat of
et al, 1998). the mother’s heart.
This is part of what we know of the Source: The Human Body, BBC Films In
foetus around the time of mid-pregnancy. association with the Learning Channel
But what does it know? By this time it has (Dir. Dale, De’ath, Evans, Thompson,
begun to develop the basics of all five Georgi & Spencer, 1998).
Ontogeny. Application Although people typically proceed through the same general
of developmental sequence of development, there is a wide range of individual dif-
psychology to the ferences. Only when deviation from a norm is extreme is there
understanding of the
cause to consider a person’s development exceptionally advanced
individual development
of the specific child
or delayed.
or person.
Because people differ so widely in height, weight and build, in con-
stitutional factors such as health and energy level, and in emotional
reactions, there can be great deviance from norms across individual levels
of comparison. It is important to note here that individual differences
also increase as people grow older. Whereas many changes in childhood
seem tied to maturation of the body and brain (the unfolding of a definite
sequence of physical changes and behaviour patterns), later life changes are
more contingent on life experiences (Papalia et al,1998).
Critical period. A specific
time during development Critical periods
when a given event has Another important concept in developmental psychology is that of critical
its greatest effect. periods. A critical period is a specific time during development when a given
Basic concepts and principles in developmental psychology 13
event has its greatest impact. Lenneberg, for example, proposed a critical
period—before puberty—for language development. As another example,
some ethologists believe that the first three years of life are the critical
period for the development of emotional and social responsiveness in
humans (Papalia et al, 1998). The concept of the critical period expresses the
crucial nature of timing, and specifies a timespan when—and only when—a
particular environmental factor can have an effect (Craig, 1996).
Normative history-graded
influences are common to a
particular cohort, which is a
group of people who share
a similar experience—in
this case, growing up at the
same time.
14 Developmental Psychology
What is normal behaviour for a five-month-old child? How should one raise a little girl as opposed to a little boy,
and why?
Basic concepts and principles in developmental psychology 15
that boys, when left to play in a sandpit, were prone to build towering, phallic-
like structures. Girls, by contrast, would build hollow, enclosing, womb-like
structures (Erikson, 1963). Erikson is clearly showing his psychoanalytic
loyalties here. One cannot help wondering whether his dedication to certain
of these psychoanalytic ideas is affecting how he observes the world, not
only in terms of how he understands what children are building, but also
in terms of how he perceives gendered difference between what boys and
girls do. Therefore we must ask: Is Erikson correctly observing a gender
difference, or is he already, on the basis of prior assumptions, perceiving
little boys and little girls differently?
This presents a philosophical problem: At what point are we truly
observing what is out there (as a way of building explanations and theories),
and at what point are we already drawing implicitly on preconceived
explanations and theories (in our act of perception)? The implication
here is that we always need to be aware of how our prior conceptions
influence our perceptions of the world. This is the reservation we need to
keep in mind: rather than producing knowledge perhaps we are merely
reproducing certain ideological viewpoints. This is particularly important
in developmental psychology, where the knowledge of psychologists has
a very real and concrete bearing on the lives of people. If we reproduce
various forms of prejudice or discrimination in the scientific knowledge
we are producing, then clearly we are participating in forms of social
injustice, in an inequitable ‘science’ that selectively disempowers certain
members of society.
(Gormly, 1997).
Basic concepts and principles in developmental psychology 17
Of the two examples from Papalia et al (1998: 9) that follow, the first presents
an apparent cultural universal, and the second illustrates a cross-cultural
comparison:
... no matter where children live, they learn to speak in the same
sequence, advancing from cooing and babbling to single words and
then to simple combinations of words. The sentences of toddlers
around the world are structured similarly, though the words might
vary. Such findings suggest that the capacity for learning language
is inborn. On the other hand, culture can exert a surprisingly large
influence on early motor development. African babies, whose par-
ents often prop them in a sitting position and bounce them on their
feet, tend to sit and walk earlier than American babies ...
* The human baby at birth is less flows through their open mouths into
matured than any other species. In the their stomachs where their lungs shut
first few months after birth the human off automatically, protecting them from
newborn seems woefully inept; other drowning. No one is exactly sure why
animals get up and walk within seconds babies have this skill; it may be a
of being born but the human infant can residual ability from their time in the
barely control its own twitching limbs. womb, or an ‘evolutionary echo’ from
our distant amphibious past.
* Babies have certain instinctual skills that
adults have lost. For example, all babies * Babies have already had a certain
can swim from the moment they are amount of kinaesthetic experience in
born—they instinctively kick to propel the womb; it is here that they first start
themselves forward in water. Water learning to move.
>>
18 Developmental Psychology
<<
* Even above the constant ‘roar’ of the ‘in-built’ skill to control others: their
mother’s heartbeat, the in utero child (generally) cute appearance and
hears, and reacts to, his mother’s voice. behaviour and, importantly, their crying
At this early stage there is even some help ensure that they soon become the
rudimentary interaction between centre of family life. Crying is, in fact,
mother and child. This interaction the key to the survival of infants; it
between the baby and his surroundings triggers a physiological response that
fuels the baby’s own development. their mothers find practically impossible
Although babies are not simply ‘hard- to ignore, and which (among other
wired’ to become human, they are in a factors) stimulates their mothers’ breasts
sense ‘programmed’ to seek out the to produce milk. Crying is more than a
experiences that will transform them. ‘meal-ticket’ for babies; they cannot
experience the world by themselves as
* Babies trigger their own birth. Once the yet, and for now others must ‘bring the
baby is ready to survive in the outside world’ to them. Crying, then, is also the
world, he releases a hormone into his key to early learning.
mother’s bloodstream and the resulting
muscular spasms lead to the birth * At three weeks, a baby’s most active
process. Birth is even more traumatic for muscles are those that control his eyes.
the baby than it is for the mother. The Vision, however, is still less than perfect;
child’s adrenalin levels are twice that of the baby can pick up only the strongest
hers, even higher than that of an adult of contrasts, and can make out objects
experiencing a heart attack. that are only less than a metre away.
Because the baby has not yet combined
* In the first few minutes after birth, the the bifocal images of his two retinas, at
newborn infant’s senses are bombarded first he sees everything in double. His
with new sights, sounds and feelings. eyes are drawn to moving objects; this
Within the first few hours after birth sensitivity to movement draws the baby
infants can mimic certain facial to look at those things which best
expressions; this is not conscious stimulate the developing visual areas
mimicry, but babies are able to translate of the brain. This rudimentary vision is
what they see into similar actions. ideally designed to study moving, high-
Imitation is one of the best learning contrast human faces. In the first two
strategies babies have. months babies are ‘programmed’ to
search out and stare at the human,
* The brain of the newborn human child because it is from people that they
is less organised at birth than the brains learn the most.
of virtually every other species. This
sounds like a handicap but in fact it is a * Just as babies’ eyes are ‘programmed’
great advantage. Because human to seek out human faces, so their ears
babies’ brains are flexible, essential are drawn to the sound of voices.
connections between cells can develop Infants are so attuned to human speech
as a result of a baby’s experience and so that they are able to recognise their
facilitate the process of learning. mothers’ voices within the first week
after birth. It is therefore not unusual
* Although newborn infants are physically that comfort for infants often comes in
helpless, they are equipped with an the form of a mother’s soothing voice.
>>
Basic concepts and principles in developmental psychology 19
<<
* Within weeks of birth, babies are said Moreover, at eight weeks, infants are
to be able to tell the difference between said to prefer the sound of their native
one language and another (their brains tongue to other languages.
register familiar streams of words).
Babies are able to pick out the patterns
and intonations that make their own Source: Baby It’s You, Channel 4 Television
language different from others. Films (Dir. Klein & Hickman, 1994).
Conclusion
This chapter has provided a brief overview of important concepts and ten-
sions in the study of developmental psychology. These basic concepts and
debates are certainly worth reflecting on, both on a personal level and as a
critical lens through which developmental theory can be analysed and ap-
plied. This chapter has maintained a largely meta-theoretical focus in order
to survey key debates in the field of developmental psychology. Our aim in
this has been to extend the reader’s conceptual vocabulary in this field, and
to provide a basis with which to engage, compare and relate within context
the various theories of development presented in the chapters that follow.
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➊ With reference to one of the theorists discussed in this book, explain how the cultural
perspective of that theorist and of the people he or she studied could have affected the
theory that was subsequently developed.
➋ Choose one of the theorists discussed in this book, and describe that theorist’s particular
standpoint with regard to the major debates in developmental psychology (that is,
stability versus change; nature versus nurture; continuity versus discontinuity; ontogeny
and phylogeny; critical periods; and non-normative and normative influences).
You might also try keeping a journal throughout the reading of this text and assess how
the various theories might apply to you.
SECTION ONE
Psychoanalytic approaches to development and personality
CHAPTER
2
A basic introduction
to psychoanalytic thought
Jacki Watts
A cautionary note:
This section covers definitive aspects of the theories of some major con-
tributors to psychoanalytic thought. It does not, however, do justice to
the breadth of the theories covered or to all the major theorists who have
contributed and continue to contribute to the development of psycho-
analytic thought. It is hoped that by offering you insight into the key
concepts you will have the confidence and curiosity to further explore the
theorists who fire your imagination, and to understand psychoanalytic
thought more broadly.
A basic introduction to psychoanalytic thought 23
Elusive Bion
The biographical details of Wilfred Bion’s life are found in his
posthumously published autobiographies The long weekend (1982) and
All my sins remembered (1985). These two books provide a wonderful
insight into not only the developmental influences on Bion’s life but also
the operation of his own psyche. Bion’s own presence and actions, his
own motivational strivings, are never the substance of his autobiography.
A major focus of his theory concerns containment and the functions of
containment. Bion writes as if he is a ‘pure container’, revealing little, if
any, of his own responsiveness to the world or people.
He was born in India in 1897. He and his younger sister both received
private tutoring in India. As was the practice for British colonials of the
time, Bion was sent to England at the age of eight to receive his education
at an all boy’s school, leaving his family behind in India. He did extremely
well at sport but seems to have been a lonely, intellectual child.
With the outbreak of the 1st World War, he attempted to enlist as
an officer but was refused something to do with his lack of social status.
Due to the interventions of a contact and the wonders of the British social
class, he was eventually enlisted as a tank commander where as an 18 year
old he was in command of men aged 30 or more. He relates scenes of
absolute fear and horror experienced during the war with a clinical eye
for observation. He was awarded the DSO and the Legion of Honour for
his bravery.
After the War he read for history at Oxford, an influence which is felt
in his theoretical writings, together with a strong philosophical background.
He then studied medicine at University College, London. A growing
26 Developmental Psychology
Some Winnicott
Donald Winnicott was the cherished son of an upper-class British family.
His father, Sir Frederick Winnicott, was the Lord Mayor of Plymouth.
Winnicott was the youngest child, with two elder sisters who were devoted to
him. He was an excellent scholar and athlete, growing up to be a handsome,
cultured and enigmatic man. He started his career as a paediatrician and
maintained an interest in children throughout his life. As a theorist he
devoted his life to exploring the implications and importance of the bond
between mother and child. He entered the British Psychoanalytic Society
The Bloomsbury group. at the time of its most creative and open debates.
A literary, artistic and Ernest Jones (who invited Klein to lecture in London) was then
intellectual circle of
president of the Society. James and Alix Strachey, and Adrian and Karen
friends who met at one
another’s homes in and
Steven represented the Bloomsbury influence. (The first of Winnicott’s
around the Bloomsbury two long personal analyses was with James Strachey.) Other influential
area of London in the people at the time were John Rickman and Sylvia Payne, who worked
early decades of the 20th with shell-shocked troops from World War I. There were also those who
century. At its core were were to become loyal to the Kleinian tradition: Joan Riviere, Susan Isaacs
the sisters Vanessa Bell and Paula Heimann, among others. (Winnicott had his second analysis
and Virginia Woolf. It also
with Joan Riviere.) The arrival of the Freud family in 1939, and the
included the novelist E. M.
Forster, the artist and critic
ensuing conflict with Klein, was to change the quality of the debates to a
Roger Fry, John Maynard more intense and acrimonious level.
Keynes (the influential Winnicott appears to have remained aloof to these ‘intrigues’,
economist), Victoria remaining a profoundly individualistic person. He gave innumerable talks
Sackville-West (the poet to a wide-ranging cross-section of people, from social workers, for instance,
and writer) and Lytton to clergymen, to mothers themselves. He preferred these forums where he
Strachey, the biographer.
could engage with his audience rather than the more cerebral discussions of
In its broad artistic and
philosophical range, the
academia and psychoanalysts. Winnicott never aligned himself with either
Bloomsbury group had party in the Freud and Klein wrangles, remaining clinically objective,
a profound influence on and providing the model for the independence and creative thinking that
English cultural life. characterises the Middle or Independent tradition. He was to have a profound
A basic introduction to psychoanalytic thought 27
Phantasy. The mental Freud postulates that the progressive physiological complexity of the
activity that is mutually child’s development interacts with cultural prohibitions (in Richards
bound up with the instincts & Dickson, 1977). This process leads to an evolution whereby the child
and gives them a mental
comes to progressively harness and curb his instinctual nature, eventuat-
representation
(Isaacs, 1948).
ing in a mature capacity to love and work. Klein (1946) proposes that the
infant, initially encapsulated within an internal world of phantasy, is faced
Psychopathology. Mental with the task of moving from this internal world into a world of shared
functioning that is deemed reality. Klein’s focus is on early infancy and the internal processes that
abnormal as judged by occur to lay the foundations for future cognitive and emotional capaci-
the norms of the subject’s ties. Bion (1967) looks at the role of the mother’s containment, that is, the
culture and society. In
functioning of her mind in bringing the infant’s mind into operation and
studying psychopathology
one is concerned with
reality contact.
understanding the Winnicott (1958) theorised about the significance of the mother/child
origin, development and bond. He saw this bond as essential to the future capacities of the child to
characteristics of its various integrate and be able to relate to others in mature and fulfilling ways.
manifestations. Jung (1986) also asserted the importance of childhood. He felt, however,
that this terrain of theoretical understanding had been well documented
Psychoanalytic psycho-
therapy. A modification of by Freud. Jung’s focus was on development during adulthood. He
psychoanalysis. Whereas postulated that significant developmental challenges faced humankind
psychoanalysis is based throughout life and these were the challenges involved in the journey
upon at least thrice of individuation, a universal development towards integration of the
weekly sessions, which personality. Lacan (1977) postulated that it was important to understand
are only transference how the symbolic world of language and culture acted upon the child and
based, psychoanalytic
imposed an alienation from the self that the child can never overcome.
psychotherapy is
conducted less often and
(while transference based) Normal and pathological development
it also deals with reality- Psychoanalytic theory arose from work with pathological functioning.
based interventions. This theory derives from work within the clinical field and from work
Personality disorder.
with individuals who are struggling or suffering with various states of
Deeply ingrained and psychic pain (psychopathology). One could argue then that these theories
maladaptive patterns of are only apposite to pathology. Certainly, psychoanalytic theories have
relating to, thinking about, given clinicians the understanding and insight to treat psychological
and perceiving the world conditions that had previously been considered untreatable. For example,
and oneself. These patterns psychoanalytic psychotherapy is the treatment of choice when one considers
lead to impairment in
conditions such as personality disorders. Freud’s theoretical paradigm
functioning and distress.
Personality disorders can
is concerned with Oedipal disorders, which are termed neuroses. Post-
often be diagnosed by late psychoanalytic theory has offered insights into pre-Oedipal disorders.
latency or adolescence and These have come to be seen as disorders of the development of the self.
continue throughout life Psychoanalytic theory, in considering the development of pathology, has
unless treated. They are also inevitably offered clinicians and theorists the opportunity to see very
termed disorders of the self. clearly those contexts which foster development of children and those
Long-term psychoanalytic
which lead to pathology and psychological damage. In this sense, through
psychotherapy is the
treatment of choice.
psychoanalytic theory, one is able to trace both ‘normal’ and pathological
development. The theorists covered in this section all postulate that there
Psychic development.
are certain optimum conditions that foster psychological growth. Freud (in
The process whereby the
mental life of an individual
Richards & Dickson, 1977) and Klein (1946) tend to focus on the internal
becomes constituted to be world and conditions that impede or foster psychic development. Winnicott
able to function within both (1958) focuses on what is required from the primary caregiver to foster the
internal and external reality. child’s optimum development. Within this context he analyses those con-
A basic introduction to psychoanalytic thought 31
ditions that lead to compromises in the child’s development. Jung (1986) Stages. Development
traces the processes whereby integration and balance between opposing that is built upon and
forces lead to integration of the personality. Each theorist, in postulating a dependent on the
attainment of prior
theory of pathology, also postulates a theory of normal development and
developmental aspects.
the conditions necessary to foster this development. Most see the role of
the environment as important in the development of pathology. Some give Positions. Independent
more priority to innate factors such as the role of innate aggression. phases that can be
returned to at various
times throughout
Stages of development development.
All psychoanalytic theories see development as proceeding through a
sequence of stages. There are differences in terminology and even subtle
The realm of the dream world and the unconscious underpins all psychoanalytic theory.
A basic introduction to psychoanalytic thought 33
Freud
Psychodrama
Sullivan Bioenergetics
Fromm-Reichman Ida Rolf [Rolfing]
Eastern philosophies
Searles Ego Psychology and practice Zanov
Hartman Yoga/martial arts [Primal therapy]
Laing
1 Classical Fairbairn
Psychoanalysis 2 Object Relations Guntrip
Anna Freud Klein
Isaacs Kernberg Balint 4 Self Psychology
Segal Kohut
French ORT Bion 3 Middle School Wolf
Green Rosenfeld Winnicott Intersubjectivists
Rey Heimann Khan Chessick
A basic introduction to psychoanalytic thought 35
clinical case studies. All these fields offer wide-ranging support for the
importance of childhood, for the role of parenting, and for the significance
of unconscious motivations in innate emotional capacity and in adult
personality functioning. Look at the following chapter on Freud for a
more elaborated discussion of these issues.
A more difficult critical claim concerns the deterministic nature
of psychoanalytic theory. The claim is made that the theory suggests
psychological development of the individual to be largely complete by the
time of puberty and that the individual is then imprisoned by his or her
development history. A way to think about this claim is to consider that
the theory is deterministic about the development of psychic apparatus
during certain times and conditions. However, it is not so when one comes
to the particulars of an individual’s life. The theory is not predictive. All
it claims is that at certain developmental stages, certain developmental
achievements occur, which lead to the maturing of the human organism.
Clearly all theory is worthy of critique. Nevertheless, one’s critique
must respond to the current state of theory.
Specific tasks
➊ Once you have reviewed the whole of the Psychoanalytic Section, go back and look at
the pages dealing with the assumptions of psychoanalytic theories. You will notice that
Kernberg, Kohut and Erikson are not covered. How would you capture their central
theoretical foci under these assumptions?
➋ Here is a list of serial killers. Research the development history of one of these
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
individuals and apply the developmental concepts of one or more of the theories in
this section. Consider whether the theory allows you an understanding of the level
of hatred and aggression expressed by the killer.
Albert DeSalvo, the Boston Strangler; Ed Gein, the man upon whom The Texas
Chainsaw Massacre movie was based; Ted Bundy; David Berkowitz, Son of Sam;
Aileen Wuornos, upon whom the movie Monster was styled; Kobus Geldenhuys,
the Norwood killer, and Moses Sithole, two of South Africa’s most notorious serial killers.
➌ Once you have reviewed the whole of the Psychoanalytic Section, consider in what
ways the theorists covered offer complementary and/or divergent views of development.
➎ Give careful thought to the concept of the unconscious: What evidence can we find
for the existence of this ‘area’ or aspect of psychological functioning? Again refer to
personal references drawn from your own life. >>
36 Developmental Psychology
<<
➏ If you were formulating a theory of development, what assumptions would
underlie your theory?
➐ What do you think are the strongest critiques of the psychoanalytic approach and why?
➑ Give some thought to how the basic psychoanalytic principles discussed above
inform the clinical practice of psychoanalysis.
Recommended readings
Freud, S (1977). ‘Three essays on the theory of sexuality’. In A. Richards & A. Dickson (eds),
The Penguin Freud Library. Volume 7 on sexuality: Three essays on the theory of sexuality and
other work. London: Penguin.
(Freud lays out his theory of infantile sexuality in ways that make the theory accessible.)
Gay, P (1988). Freud, a life for our times. London: Papermac.
(A wonderful insight into the life and times and the theory of Freud.)
Grosskurth, P (1986). Melanie Klein. Her world and her work. London: Jason Aronson.
(A thorough biography of Klein. Gives insight into her life and motivations and gives human drama
to her theory.)
Isaacs, S (1948). ‘The nature and function of phantasy’. In M. Klein (ed), Contributions to
psychoanalysis, 1921-1945. London: Hogarth Press.
(The first exposition of the Kleinian position on phantasy. Rather long-winded but definitive for
the serious student.)
Jung, C G (1963). Memories, dreams, reflections. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
(An autobiography that gives one a personal glimpse of how Jung thought about his life.)
Jung, C G (1986). Analytic psychology: Its theory and practice. London: Ark Paperbacks.
(A very readable overview of Jung’s theories. It is based upon his lectures to the Tavistock.)
Klein, M (1946). ‘Notes on some schizoid mechanisms’. International journal of psychoanalysis, 27, 99-110.
(Her definitive paper. It may be a bit hard but well worth the effort to serious students.)
Kohon, G (ed) (1986). The British school of psycho-analysis: The independent tradition.
London: Free Association Books.
(This collection has some wonderful essays which illustrate the creativity characteristic of the
Independent tradition.)
Lacan, J (1979). The four fundamental concepts of psychoanalysis. London: Penguin.
(A difficult read, but for the serious student it covers Lacan’s reading of four of Freud’s basic concepts.)
Segal, H (1964). Introduction to the work of Melanie Klein. New York: Basic Books.
(One of the best overviews of Klein’s work.)
Winnicott, D W (1958). Collected papers: Through paediatrics to psycho-analysis. New York:
Basic Books.
(As always, a wonderful experience.)
CHAPTER
3
Freud’s psychoanalytic
theory of development
and personality
Jacki Watts and Derek Hook
Rather, it is socialisation that has imposed the need for some feelings
and actions to remain unconscious to our functioning. As such, these
feelings and actions are associated with our natural instinctual life—our
inborn patterns of behaving towards certain stimuli. These may be sexual
and aggressive feelings. Social living dictates that some of this material
needs to remain unconscious to safeguard the coherence of the society. It
is upon this premise that Freud postulated our development towards a Soma/somatic. The body/
relatively civilised state. of the body.
Trieb
In considering the conditions that might sexuality, Freud postulates the infant in
render psychic material unconscious, Freud an initial polymorphous state, that is,
postulated the operation of the instinct or infantile sexuality occurs in several distinct
drives. Freud wrote in German, using the forms - oral, anal, phallic and genital. This
terms instinkt and trieb. English translations state is merely concerned with the aim of
have used the term instinct for both these eliminating tension at the somatic source.
terms, leaving the risk of confusing the The source could be oral, anal or phallic.
two meanings that Freud intended. In The instinct would thus attach itself to
his writing they are used in quite distinct any object that resulted in satisfaction.
ways. Instinkt is used in the classical sense Fixations at this stage of polymorphous
of animal instinct, such as an instinctual infantile sexuality could lead to what Freud
recognition of danger. His use of trieb defines as perversion in adult sexuality.
designates a dynamic, that is, something What he demonstrates in his study of
which is alive. This dynamic describes sexual perversion is that the object is
a relationship: the instinct as having a variable. It is chosen because of the
source, an aim and an object. vicissitude of the person’s history and
Freud’s concept of trieb has commonly where the fixation may lie.
come to be termed instinct, which is an In the relative normal development
unfortunate simplification. It has led some of sexuality, the aim of the instinct only
to thinking that for Freud ‘sex is all’. This becomes subordinate to the genital zone
is not so. It is instinct as designated by at the end of a complex evolution through
trieb, which underlies Freud’s complex childhood. This evolution is dependent
understanding of psychic motivation. upon biological maturation but its course
The instinct has its source in the bodily is influenced by complex forces of repres-
sensation, such as an oral need. The aim sion that may result in fixation at any of
of the instinct is then to discharge the the psychosexual stages. Thus one can see
state of tension created by the need, that, in and of itself, the instinct has no
either through eating, drinking, talking, moral rightness or wrongness about it.
smoking or screaming, etc. The object The polymorphous nature of infantile
is the means by which this may come sexuality is pressured through the
about, for example food, cigarettes, progressive demands of socialisation into
drugs or words. a genital state and the variable infantile
The popular view of sexuality is that motivations inherent in the instinct are
it means heterosexual genital contact. repressed into the unconscious.
In his study of perversions and infantile (Laplanche & Pontalis, 1973)
40 Developmental Psychology
stinct is particularly drawn is not defined. As a result, any one of a variety Polymorphously
of activities that provide bodily sensations of pleasure—such as looking, perverse. The phrase
touching and even the exchange of pain—can be the sexual aim for the Freud uses to describe
the sexuality of the infant
infant, or an adult functioning from a pre-genital stage of development.
before the hierachy of
Infants, therefore, are ‘polymorphously perverse’ (Freud, 1977). This term component instincts has
indicates that they have no hierarchies or ordering of sexual instincts. been established (oral,
Mature sexuality, by contrast, requires that the adult should subordinate anal and phallic) under
all the other sexual instincts (oral, anal and visual satisfaction) to the sexual the subordination of all
priority of penetrative, genital sex. This does not mean that all our other other sexual instincts to
sexual instincts disappear as we reach mature sexuality. Oral and anal sex the priority of penetrative
genital sex.
(and visual stimulation) still remain potentially pleasurable for adults; it is
just that these sexual pleasures generally become subordinated, as ‘fore-
play’ activities, to the priority and focus of genital sex.
The object:
The second important difference between infantile and adult sexuality
is that the infant, unlike the adult, has not channelled the sexual instincts Sexual object/object-
towards a specific sexual object (Freud, 1977). Freud used this term to choice. Freud’s term for
denote one’s preferred type of sexual partner, the person whom one finds one’s preferred type of
sexually attractive. Freud maintains that settling on a secure object-choice sexual partner or the
is a characteristic only of mature adult sexuality. Thus the infant is not person whom one finds
only polymorphously perverse but also inherently bisexual. The infant has most sexually attractive.
no innate predisposition to any one gender as more sexually desirable. For
Bisexuality. Having
Freud, in short, there is no genetic preprogramming making the infant no sexual preference for
male prefer females, or the female prefer males. Sexual preference, the either exclusively male
making of a stable object-choice (be it heterosexual or homosexual), is thus or female categories of
only an outcome of the process of psychosexual development. object-choice.
42 Developmental Psychology
‘Perversions’
Infant ➞ Adult
‘Polymorphously perverse’ ➞ ‘Genital sexuality’
(all erogenous zones are sources ➞ (erogenous zones are subordinated to the
of sexual pleasure) primacy of the genitals)
no sexual object-choice has been made ➞ secure object-choice has been made
(any person is as good as another as a source
of sexual pleasure and desire)
➞ (a certain specific type of person is preferred
to others as a source of sexual pleasure)
Figure 3.1 Conceptualising the differences between infant and adult sexuality. (Sue van Zyl)
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 43
Scopophilic drive. The stage of development. Children are very curious about the sexual differ-
desire to see another’s ences of the genders and about ‘where babies come from’. This is the time
sexual organs, manifesting of games such as ‘doctor doctor’ which manifest the scopophilic drive of
in active and passive
this stage. This is the desire to see another’s sexual organs. The scopophilic
guises respectively as
exhibitionism (the
drive manifests in an active way as with exhibitionism (the sexual desire to
sexual desire to show show one’s genitals to another) and in passive ways, such as voyeurism (the
one’s genitals to others) desire to see other people naked or engaging in sexual activity).
and voyeurism (the desire It is at this stage also that girls and boys discover their anatomical sex-
to watch other people ual differences. Following the perspective of the little boy, Freud claims
naked or engaging in that it is the little boy’s assumption that all other living beings possess a
sexual activity).
penis similar to his. Inevitably though, the boy discovers that girls lack a
Exhibitionism. The
penis. The childhood reaction to this discovery in boys is one of fear, the
sexual desire to show fear that they might lose their penis, that ‘someone might take it away’. In
one’s genitals to others. girls the discovery of their lack of a penis leads to reactions of jealousy; they
see the little boy has something that they do not have, and they instantly
Voyeurism. The desire want it. These are the two respective bases for the formation of castration
to watch other people anxiety and penis envy. Castration anxiety in males is where there is either
naked or engaging in
a literal or a figurative fear of, in some way, losing the penis, or manhood
sexual activity.
generally. Penis envy in females is the desire to possess a penis, or to attain
Castration anxiety.
a certain masculine status. Both of these phenomena will be discussed in
The literal or figurative more detail in relation to the Oedipus complex.
unconscious fear in males Consider the number of ways in which Freud’s understandings about
of in some way losing having or not having a penis become manifest in the world. 1: Having
their penis, or their or not having a penis is permeated with cultural values and meanings.
manhood generally. 2: In patriarchal societies men have greater power, and little boys are ac-
corded more importance than little girls. Girls may even be killed at birth.
Penis envy. The literal
or figurative unconscious 3: The rise of feminist thought attests to the theorising that has attempted
desire in females to to understand the dominance of the male.
possess their own penis, The significance of the penis is not wholly determined by culture.
or to attain a certain The value of having a penis is also supported by a diverse range
masculine status. of influences. Consider, for example, issues of active and passive
behavioural roles where the act of penetration by the penis, by definition,
implies greater power than the more passive act of incorporation by the
vagina. Common judgements around ‘absence and presence’ are also
made where the ‘having’ of something is preferable to the ‘not having’
of it. There are also judgements around size, where ‘bigger’ or ‘more
noticeable’ is also seen as better. Within these modes of thinking, it is
understandable that male genitals are seen as preferable to the discreet or
smaller sexual organs of women.
The active-passive polarity of this phallic stage corresponds to the
penis versus non-penis, male-genital versus castrated polarity, or put
more simply, to the distinction between presence and absence. For Freud
this presence-absence distinction has important ramifications for the way
children come to understand their gender roles in society, and how they
come to be gendered into an active position or a passive position. The
parental ‘training’ that accompanies this stage is the prohibition around
masturbation and appropriate sexual behaviour. The prohibitions may or
may not be accompanied by threats of punishment. We will discuss the
implications of the prohibition as part of the Oedipus complex.
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 47
The Oedipus complex has been instrumental in instituting a non- Genitally-led. The phrase
incestuous, (generally) heterosexual, genitally-led eroticism that serves Freud uses to describe the
the purpose of reproduction. Also of great importance is the fact that sexuality of the adult after
the various component
the sexual instincts become absolutely directed towards a sexual object.
sexual instincts have
Instead of being preoccupied with his own sexual gratification, the been subordinated under
individual now develops the desire to share mutual sexual gratification the sexual priority of
with someone else. penetrative genital sex.
Whereas the phallic stage was dominated simply by the sensory organ
stimulus of the penis and the clitoris, the genital stage is dominated by the
goal of orgasm through a particular form of stimulation of the penis and
the vagina. The overarching sexual aim of this final phase is, for the male,
to penetrate the vagina with his penis and, for the female, to be penetrated
in the vagina by the penis. The ability to achieve full and free orgasm
with a heterosexual partner on an equal basis is an important foundation
for intimate relationships and life-partner choices from here on. By now,
individuals are able to share erogenous pleasure with others in a warm
and caring way, to be concerned for them, to feel love for them.
Individuals generally now have better control over their sexual and
aggressive instincts and have been transformed from pleasure-seeking,
self-centred infants into socialised and caring adults. As we know, not
all people succeed in properly entering the genital stage. Many of us
have less than adequate control over our sexual and aggressive instincts
and are also unable to gratify our sexual desires in a completely satis-
fying and acceptable way. So although the genital stage represents the
perfect culmination of psychosexual development it is, in many ways,
more of an ideal to strive for than an end that may be taken for granted
(Fenichel, 1945).
Is Freud homophobic?
It is important to be clear about Freud’s the end of a child’s development. These
views on homosexuality. For Freud (1977) challenges are the transformation of the
homosexuality is not abnormal in the auto-erotic, incestuous, polymorphously
morally evaluative or stigmatised sense perverse and bisexual instincts, and the
of the term. He claims in fact that: resolution of the Oedipus/Elektra complex.
Nevertheless, it is undeniable
Psychoanalytic research is most decidedly that although Freud can explain the
opposed to any attempt at separating off occurrence of the homosexual object-
homosexuals from the rest of mankind as choice, he cannot avoid seeing it as in
a group of special character. By studying some ways deviant or aberrant. For Freud
sexual excitations other than those that (1977), homosexuality stems from three
are manifestly displayed, it has found that principal sources: 1) a narcissistic object-
all human beings are capable of making choice, where one takes one’s self, or one’s
a homosexual object-choice and have in own sex as an object of sexual desire, 2)
fact made one in their unconscious (Freud, a problematic negotiation of the Oedipus
1977: 56). complex, where the individual comes to
both desire and identify with the same
This in fact is an extraordinarily sex, rather than desiring the opposite
progressive view on homosexuality, sex and identifying with the same sex, as
particularly considering the predominance occurs in the successful resolution of the
of conservative attitudes towards sexuality Oedipus complex, or 3) an early fixation
in late 19th-century Western Europe which has prevented the individual from
when Freud was writing. Indeed, Freud attaining the full genital psychosexual
(1977) rejects outright the notion that stage of development
sexual deviation should be couched in It is important to note that homo-
terms of ‘degeneracy’ or ‘disease’, or that sexuality is only psychopathological if an
the term ‘perversion’ should be used as individual experiences his object-choice as
a term of reproach. For Freud (1977), conflictual on the level of personality. This
every individual’s sexuality, like their means that there is ego/superego conflict
psychological adjustment more generally, attached to the object-choice. For Freud, it
deviates somewhat, however slightly, from is possible to be a well-adjusted and non-
the ideal norm. There is, in his words, ‘no psychopathological homosexual. Moreover,
healthy person that fails to make some it should be noted that Freud’s theory is
addition that might be called perverse to led by the evolutionary priority of species-
the normal sexual aim’ (Freud, 1977: 74). preservation as espoused by Darwin. Freud
If we consider how different and takes the stand that nature and, following
idiosyncratic each individual’s path from it, culture, demands that heterosexual,
through the zonal stages and the penetrative, genital sex be the norm within
Oedipus complex must be, we then society, for this was the only way in which
start to appreciate that each instance of the reproduction of the species could
sexuality is in some way distinct. In fact, be assured (Freud 1977). It would be
considering the challenges that Freud sees fascinating to speak to Freud today now
confronting the task of ‘normal’ sexual that medical science offers other means of
development, it is almost a miracle that ensuring the preservation of the species
a vaguely ‘normal’ sexuality emerges at for example in vitro fertilisation.
>>
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 51
<<
There is now general and widespread psychopathology, is a derisive, value-laden,
support for the view that describing and a damaging approach to a healthy and
homosexuality in terms of deviance, loving form of sexuality.
abnormality, aberrance and particularly Derek Hook
Castration anxiety
It is important to reiterate that the Oedipus complex overlaps, and is an
integral part of, the phallic stage, that period when children are insatiably
curious about the genitals of others. At this time, given his curiosity, the
boy notices that a girl or, for that matter, his mother does not have a penis.
This has an enormous effect on the little boy, who begins to think that
he could lose his penis as well. The boy automatically assumes that the
female must once have had a penis and that she has been castrated, that
someone has ‘taken it away’. The fear of losing his penis develops into
52 Developmental Psychology
Is Freud a chauvinist?
Many feminist writers have taken excep- superego as do men (see below), paints
tion to the way Freud has characterised a picture of women as less moral and far
women (Horney, 1967; Chodorow, more likely to disturb the moral order
1978). The focus of the feminist critique than men. This fear is seen in many
of Freud is often the notion of penis aspects of myth, religion and art. It
envy, which sees women as haunted was Eve, for example, who brought
throughout their lives by feelings of the downfall of paradise.
emptiness and jealousy stemming from In Freud’s defence, some argue,
their earlier ‘castration‘. The implication perhaps superficially, that it should be
of this idea is that women always will noted that he lived in a very different
be inferior in some way and that they historical and cultural era to our own,
always (unconsciously or consciously) will where equal rights and equality were
want to be like men, or want to possess in no way as prioritised as they are
men. It is argued that this is a demeaning now. In this way, rather than being
and unbalanced view of women, which automatically sexist, Freud has shown us
portrays women from an insulting, and how certain psychosexual developments
irredeemably masculine, point of view. and anatomical distinctions came to be
It is also a point of view that serves male particularly culturally loaded in the era in
interests (take, for example, the idea that which he lived. In fact, Freud (1977) does
women want, because of penis envy, to suggest that what is important in the little
incorporate the penis into their body girl’s evaluation of and desire for the penis
through intercourse). Peterson (1980) is not merely her perception of the penis in
has humorously mocked the idea of penis isolation but reflects the social desirability
envy by suggesting that today men and privilege of the male penis.
seem to exhibit what she calls ‘vagina It can also be argued that placing
envy’. This idea makes a certain amount Freud merely within his time and ‘excusing’,
of sense when one considers that female as it were, his difficult concepts as merely
anatomy enables women to bear children, products of his era negates the deep
an ability that, in some cultures, makes significance Freud gave to the unconscious.
women more socially powerful than men. Freud’s work and that of subsequent
In Western ‘First World’ societies, it may psychoanalysts has become an important
be argued that this ability is sometimes basis for much critical thinking in the
thought to disempower women, and intellectual world. One such theorist is
to put them at a disadvantage in a Juliet Mitchell who has used psychoanalysis
professional working environment. to engage critically with issues of
Similarly, the idea that women do feminism, gender, patriarchal power
not experience castration anxiety, and and politics.
therefore do not develop as powerful a Jacki Watts and Derek Hook
Conscious
Preconscious
Unconscious
(Unconscious contents: representatives
of the instincts.
Figure 3.2 Freud’s topographical model of the mind, which divides the mind into conscious, preconscious
and unconscious parts. Note that whereas material can pass relatively freely between conscious and
preconscious portions of the mind, unconscious material cannot pass freely into consciousness (hence the
idea of the one-way gate). It is only under special conditions that any unconscious material can impinge
upon the conscious mind. Dreaming is one such special circumstance, where especially disguised and
moderated unconscious material is able to have some entry into the preconscious. Because the individual
is asleep, this material does not impinge directly on consciousness.
is, by the idea that all needs (for example, hunger or the need to urinate)
Primary process. The should be satisfied immediately. According to the pleasure principle we
id’s main mechanism for should eat at the first feelings of hunger or should seek sexual gratifica-
dealing with the build-up tion at the first twinges of sexual arousal. If such needs are not satisfied
of tension. The primary
quickly, then there is a resulting build-up of pressure, which the child
process involves the
conjuring up of a mental experiences as discomfort (un-pleasure). It is this discomfort that the
image that would satisfy pleasure principle hopes to avoid. The pleasure principle is clearly prob-
the instinctual need that is lematic, however, especially if one considers what would happen to us if
building up. we went around acting on our slightest desires, demanding gratification
in the most immediate fashion. The pleasure principle, in short, does not
Hallucinatory take into account reality, or what the outcome might be of acting in such
gratification. The
ways; it is unconcerned with what is rational or appropriate.
conjuring up of a mental
image that would provide The id’s main mechanism for dealing with the build-up of tension is
some (limited) satisfaction called the primary process. The primary process involves the conjuring up
of the instinctual need that of a mental image that would provide hallucinatory gratification of the
is building up. instinctual need that is building up. The hungry child may imagine the
mother’s breast, just as we might imagine a fantasy image of something
Wish fulfillment. The that we desire sexually, or the image of a person close to us whom we have
experience of generating
not seen for a long while. This experience of generating an image that
an image that would fulfill
a pressing need and the would fulfill a pressing need is termed wish fulfillment by Freud, and it is
basis of dream activity. the basis of dream activity. Wish fulfillment activity does not bring about
a real end to the need in question. No matter how much a hungry person
imagines food, that imagined food will not satisfy his hunger. This is yet
another demonstration of how the id—and the pleasure principle—do
not take reality into account in their functioning.
The ego
As the child gets older, it becomes increasingly important that he is able
to adapt to objective reality as this is vital for the survival of the child. It is
because of this need that the ego develops. The ego then arises out of the
inadequacy of the id, as a ‘negotiator’ between the id and the requirements
of external reality. The ego develops out of the id, and it harnesses part
of the id’s energy for itself. Thus part of the ego is also unconscious.
However, because the ego is also involved with transactions with the
outside world, it also needs to exist in the conscious and preconscious
levels of the mind.
Reality principle. The ego is regulated by the reality principle, which is the idea that
Regulates the ego, behaviour must adapt to the state and conditions of the external world
and aims to adapt the rather than just obeying the instinctual needs and urges arising from
instincts and impulses
within the individual (Freud, 1991). The reality principle introduces the
of the organism to the
state and conditions
standard of rationality into behaviour and leads one into considering
of the objective and the consequences of one’s actions within a certain environment. Rather
external world. than obeying the pleasure principle, to steal food, for example, when we
are hungry, the ego alerts us to the fact that such an act may well have
undesirable consequences. The ego attempts instead to delay gratification
and to redirect the individual to attain food in a more appropriate or
legal manner.
Delaying gratification to a more opportune, safe, sensible or later time
is hence one of the overriding functions of the ego. In this respect the ego
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 59
The superego
The superego is the last part of the personality to develop. It arises as a Ego-ideal. The positive
result of the resolution of the Oedipus complex and is largely the product side of the superego,
of the internalisation of parental authority. It also involves identification the ego-ideal stems
with the influence of other authority figures, such as teachers, heroes from the individual’s
and the like, as well as more general social values, such as religion. It is identification with their
same-sex parent, but more
initially the parents who determine what is right and wrong for the child.
generally embodies all
Identification with these parental roles allows the child to act increasingly of the highly respected
in a moral fashion and to develop the superego. The superego has both values and standards of
positive and negative sides, which stem from the resolution of the Oedipus excellence that the child
complex. The positive side of the superego is called the ego-ideal. It stems has inherited from both
from identification with the same-sex parent as well as embodying more parents. Strongly felt
generally all of the highly respected values and standards of excellence moral standpoints come
from the ego-ideal, and
that the child has inherited from both parents (Freud, 1991). Strongly felt
one feels a sense of pride
moral standpoints come from the ego-ideal, and one feels a sense of pride and identity when one
and identity when one engages in behaviours that are congruent with the engages in behaviours that
ego-ideal. are congruent with the
The negative side of the superego develops from the threat of pun- ego-ideal.
ishment that parents use to discipline their children. The child can also
develop a particularly harsh superego due to either innate or heightened
fantasies of aggression and damage to others. This aspect of the superego
60 Developmental Psychology
Conscience. The negative is called the conscience, and it consists of a collection of rules and prohibi-
side of the superego, tions about what is and what is not allowed. If an individual indulges in
which develops from the bad acts or thoughts, the superego punishes him with feelings of guilt.
threats of punishment that
Remember that Freud theorised that because males have a very severe
parents use to discipline
their children, it consists and frightening form of prohibition in the form of castration anxiety,
of a collection of rules and they have a more fully developed superego than women do. If, for what-
prohibitions about what is ever reason, an individual does not properly develop a superego, he will
and what is not allowed. lack respect for social laws and order. People suffering from antisocial
If an individual indulges in personality disorders (once called ‘sociopaths’ or ‘psychopaths’) have
bad acts or thoughts, the insufficiently developed superegos.
superego punishes them
The primary function of the superego is to inhibit and squash any un-
with feelings of guilt.
conscious impulse of the id. It also tries to make the ego act in a moral way,
to take moral as well as rational considerations into account when decid-
ing how to act in a certain situation. Lastly, the superego tries to guide the
person towards perfection in what he says, does and thinks. In this regard
the superego is a hard taskmaster. It is not realistic but perfectionist in the
demands it makes upon the ego, and it can be vindictive and even sadistic
in punishing the ego when it acts in a wayward manner. The superego op-
erates on all levels of the mind: conscious, preconscious and unconscious.
Ego strength. The term The dynamic interaction of id, ego and superego
used to describe the ability Having briefly sketched the three agencies of the mind one can see that
of the ego to moderate and the ego has a difficult task in attempting to balance the demands of the
to deal with the effects of
superego and the id, and to keep them both congruent with the conditions
the opposing forces of the
id, the superego and reality.
of external reality. Conflict is unavoidable. This is easy to see if one
considers how strongly opposed the objectives of the id and the superego
actually are. How well the ego copes is dependent upon ego strength. This
is the term that has been used to describe the ability of the ego to moderate,
and deal with, the effects of these opposing forces (Freud, 1991). The more
ego strength one has, the more one is able to deal with these competing
pressures. Conversely, the less ego strength one has, the more one is at the
mercy of these conflicting forces. The clinical objective for an individual
should thus be to increase his ego strength so that there is a healthy balance
between the forces of their personality.
Types of defences
A defence mechanism is a specific, the attributes that are admired or envied,
unconscious, intrapsychic adjustment that but can also be those which are feared.
occurs in order to resolve emotional conflict
and to reduce an individual’s anxiety. A Introjection: the symbolic internalisation
defence can be called a mental mechanism, or assimilation of another person who is
an ego-defence mechanism or an adjustive either loved or hated.
technique. A few of the major defences that
have been identified are: Projection: attributing to another person
or object one’s own thoughts, feelings
Repression: the involuntary and automatic or unacceptable impulses.
placing of unacceptable impulses or feelings
or images into the unconscious. That is, an Rationalisation: an unconscious
unconsciously motivated forgetting. mechanism which can be thought of
as retrospective justification. One gives
Suppression: the voluntary, intentional acceptable motives to what essentially
putting of unacceptable feelings, etc. does not have recognisable motives.
into the preconscious. In other words,
intentional forgetting. Idealisation: overestimation of the
qualities of another, while devaluation
Regression: the unconscious return to an is the underestimation of such qualities.
earlier level of emotional functioning.
Intellectualisation: using intellectual
Reaction formation: behaviour or attitudes concepts and words to avoid experiencing
that are the opposite of unacceptable or expressing emotions.
conscious or unconscious impulses.
Dissociation: the separation of a group of
Fixation: the arrest of maturation at usually connected mental processes, such
an immature level of psychosexual as emotion and understanding, from the
development. rest of the mind.
Preconscious
Unconscious
Instinctual impulses
Figure 3.5 Model of symptom formation. The model shows the experience of signal anxiety indicating the
press of instinctual impulses. Defences are then brought into operation, which, being successful, reduce the
anxiety and push the instinctual impulse back into the unconscious. When the defence is not effective, then
the impulse continues to press for discharge into consciousness and a symptom develops in order to hide
the meaning of the impulse from becoming known to consciousness.
Jacki Watts
Compromise formation. to rationalisation (I am only angry because you provoked me). The defence
The form taken by of sublimation is a constructive defence, allowing one to channel, rather than
repressed material in repress, sexual and aggressive energies into socially acceptable forms of
order for it to be admitted
discharge, such as sport, arts, learning and love. In our example, sublimation
into consciousness. The
repressed idea becomes might have led you to go for a run or complete an amount of work.
so distorted as to be
unrecognisable. Thus both Pathology and neurosis
the unconscious urge Defences may not be able to cope with the pressure of the instinctual urges.
and the demands of the This may be due to the intensity of the urge or a weak ego structure. The
superego can be satisfied. ego is then forced to form a compromise between the id and the superego.
This is a compromise formation, which safeguards against being over-
Symptom. The (neurotic) whelmed by the id as well as satisfying the superego demands. The com-
compromise formation promise formation is the form taken by the repressed material (such as
taken by repressed
anger or sexuality) in order for it to be admitted into consciousness. Thus
material in order for
it to be admitted into the compromise formation is the formation of a symptom. The symptom,
consciousness. for example obsessive behaviour, is a symbolic expression. Symbolically,
the wish of the id is allowed, hence satisfying the id, while at the same time
Neurosis. An emotional it is not allowed, hence satisfying the superego. The symptom both hides
disorder; the displaying of and reveals the psychic conflict.
emotional distress via the Classic psychoanalysis is concerned with the manifestation of symptoms
formation of a symptom.
as indicative of neurosis. A neurosis is an emotional disorder. Thus when
It is a functional disorder,
a conflict phenomenon
we speak of a neurotic we mean someone who is displaying emotional
involving the thwarting distress via the formation of a symptom. The symptom could range from
of an instinctual urge. anxiety, panic attacks or depression, to lying or promiscuity, amongst other
Neurosis is understood symptoms. The psyche is amazingly creative in both hiding and revealing
as a psychogenic state. the nature of the emotional pain with which the person is struggling.
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 63
conscious meaning of
the dream (the latent
content). The result is that
consciousness has access
to the dream, but only in
its surface meaning, or
manifest content. The
manifest dream, which is
remembered in conscious-
ness, is sparse in compari-
son to the richness of the
latent content. The ego
disguises the dream prin-
cipally through the pro-
cesses of displacement,
Dreams, for Freud, are condensation and symbolism, which operate as follows:
‘the royal road to the ● Displacement occurs when the meaning of an image or idea becomes
unconscious’.
detached from the image and is passed onto or displaced onto another
image or images. The new image is related to the first idea through
Latent content.
a chain of associations. The classic example would be dreaming of
The unconscious content a steeple or a cigar, which could be interpreted as the penis having
of a dream as revealed by become displaced onto the image of the steeple or cigar.
interpretation. ● Condensation is an unconscious process whereby two or more ideas
conflict or wish.
Psychoanalysis is based upon the premise the free association of the patient. Over a
that the patient is always communicating period of time, latent meanings and nodal
at an unconscious (latent) level to the points of censorship start to cohere around
psychoanalyst. In fact all psychoanalytic the fixation issues of the patient.
psychology holds to the idea that all our It can also be observed how the
conscious thought processes are linked to patient attempts to draw the analyst into
unconscious meanings and motivations. certain repetitions of the past (the
Freud discovered that by remaining neutral repetition compulsion). Through these
and listening to these communications, the attempts to repeat and heal the past, it
latent meanings were eventually revealed. becomes clear where and why there is
The method employed is that of free fixation at certain psychosexual stages.
association. The patient is encouraged to As the analyst comes to understand these
speak without censorship about whatever meanings, he is able to offer interpretations
comes into his or her mind. By remaining about the unconscious operation of the
neutral and observing, the psychoanalyst patient’s psyche.
can note a number of latent aspects to the
patient’s speech which are embedded in Derek Hook
A fictional vignette
Henry is 28 and presents himself for to attain autonomy are symbolised in his
psychotherapy with a number of obses- mother’s frequent invasive attempts to empty
sional complaints. These include his inability and control his ‘bad’ insides. Henry is left with
to leave the house without first washing his pathological fantasies of the dirtiness of his
hands for about an hour. He has become insides (his dirty urges) and with anal
seriously constipated. He also has never retentive attempts to control these instinctual
been able to have sex with a woman urges. The anal fixation has precluded him
because he is repulsed by how dirty it is. It from attaining full genital sexuality. His
is revealed that Henry’s father left the sexuality is still of an infantile nature, with
family when Henry was about two-and-a- the sexual aim satisfied through a fetish.
half—at the time his mother started his
toilet training. His father moved in with a Neurotic dynamics
woman whom Henry’s mother referred to The neurotic symptoms (hand washing,
as ‘the whore’. Henry has an enmeshed constipation and fetishism) arose as a
(overly close) relationship with his mother consequence of Henry’s weakened ego
and the two of them have lived alone since being unable to negotiate between the
his father left. Neither Henry nor his id’s demand for sexual discharge and
mother has had any significant relationship the severity of the superego’s prohibition.
outside of their mother and son unit. His Henry’s superego identification appears
mother is a very religious and controlling to be based upon his mother’s moralistic
woman. She instilled into Henry the idea and sadistic attitude towards bodies and
that he must be a ‘good, clean boy’. She sexuality, viewing instinctual life as dirty
gave him regular enemas to ensure that he and bad. Henry’s attempt at sublimation,
was ‘clean inside’. It is further revealed that the move to masturbation fetishism, was
Henry has recently met a woman at work not a successful enough compromise for the
to whom he finds himself attracted. There superego. In fact it only served to increase
is great difficulty and shame in revealing the tension, with the masturbation providing
that he has been having sexual thoughts evidence of the ‘dirty contents’ of the insides
about this woman and masturbating into of his body. All other defences having failed,
his mother’s panties. These activities leave the symptom formation of hand washing
him feeling dirty and ashamed. and constipation served as the compromise
between the id and the superego. The
Character diagnosis constipation prevented any discharge of
Analysis of the clinical material suggests that dirty contents and the hand washing
Henry has strong anal personality traits cleansed and punished the dirty act of
stemming from fixation at the anal stage masturbation. Both these symptoms reveal
of psychosexual development. His infantile the hidden desires to discharge aggressive
attempts at autonomy became identified and sexual impulses towards his mother. The
with the needs of his mother for cleanliness. use of her panties highlights his fixation on
He also became her substitute love object. his mother as the love object. The symptoms
Her controlling and intrusive attitude towards also hide the nature of the psychic conflict
him left him with passive anal retentive from Henry’s consciousness.
tendencies and an inability to assert his
separateness. His initial struggles and failure Jacki Watts
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 67
Cultural bias
A second very general criticism of Freud is that his theories emerge from a
particular socio-cultural and historical location and hence exhibit a strong
cultural bias (Cloninger, 1996; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1988). In Europe at the
end of the 19th century, when Freud wrote, men had far greater powers
and importance within society than did women. Likewise, homosexuality
was strongly frowned upon. The critique suggests that
comes from within a tradition that has rejected the notion that humans
have the capacity for innate aggression. Humanistic views of people are
imbued with optimism but are challenged to account for the widespread
horror that has characterised much of humankind’s history. Freud’s
theorising arose within a context of racial hatred and two world wars. He
was, as a Jew at that time, almost forced to theorise about the potential
inhumanity of man to man. Humanistic views appear loath to make
human beings responsible for the horror of which they are capable in the
way that Freud proposed.
This criticism does not take into account Freud’s basic principle, which
is about the dynamic nature of the psyche. The psyche is always attempting
to negotiate and re-negotiate itself in relation to both external and internal
realities. It is only in psychosis that external reality does not impact upon
the ego’s negotiations with the demands of our social and cultural milieu.
What Freud postulates is that where there has been fixation, the conflictual
aspects of the fixation will impact upon the individual and his external
reality, influencing the ways in which ego negotiations operate. Freud
does not hypothesise men or women as encapsulated psychic automatons
that act only from internal motivations. These motivations are always
in contact with the demands of external reality except, as mentioned, in
psychotic states.
In assessing the cogency of any criticism, it is useful to identify and examine the assumptions
that underlie the specific criticism. This will reveal the philosophical context from which the
criticism is launched and provide a basis for establishing the relative power of the criticism to
render a theory unsubstantiated, irrelevant or invalid.
<<
The empiricist approach is only one support aspects of Freudian theory
among many in the debate around (Western, 1998; 1999).
what constitutes scientific evidence.
Consider the theory of gravity as an * Current psychoanalytic thinking still
example—the only evidence for gravity prioritises the body as the vehicle
(an intangible causal agency) is the through which the infant engages the
tangible effect it produces. Gravity itself world, and by which meaning and
is thus not directly measurable, and personality are given coherence. Infant
can only be evidenced by its effects. Yet observation studies support the notion
the theory of gravity has the status of a that the infant comes to understand
fully credible scientific theory. The same itself initially through the body.
consideration can be applied to Freud’s
theories of the unconscious or of the * Even if there were no empirical
psychic apparatus. There is no directly support for the universal applicability of
measurable evidence for either, but the Freudian theory, this would not
visible effects can be accounted for by necessarily demonstrate its inherent
invoking Freudian theory. cultural bias. Consider the following
problem: to hold that there are no
2. Freudian theory is culturally relative universally applicable conditions is itself
A consistent criticism of Freudian theory a universal statement, and therefore
is that it is steeped in 19th century socio- a contradiction in terms. We must
cultural and political thought and as such is acknowledge at least the possibility of
more of a biased intellectual legacy from a universally applicable conditions if we
particular historical period than a universally are to remain logically coherent, and
applicable theory. In particular, Freud’s view so entertain the possibility that Freudian
of human sexual development is regarded as theory is universally applicable.
a product of the restrictive Victorian attitude
towards sex. 3. Freudian theory is deterministic
The perspective that culture determines This criticism stems from the perception
thought has recently been restated with that Freud regarded the psychological
the rise of post-modern theory, which development of the individual to be largely
claims that reality is socially constructed complete by the onset of puberty. By this
via language and culture. In terms of measure, the individual is imprisoned by his
post-modern theory, there are no universal developmental history and significant
realities, only contextual ones, and thus change is impossible. The assumption that
Freudian theory is only a representation underlies this criticism is that human
of a cultural discourse of that era. The development is not determined only by
fundamental assumption underlying this prior events, nor can it be reduced to
perspective is that reality is culture- and instinctual biological drives. The problem
language-dependent and not objectively with this criticism is that it fails to take full
defined. In assessing the validity of this account of Freudian theory and thus
view, consider the following points: misrepresents the theory to a certain
extent. Consider the following aspects of
* Cultural bias may well inform some Freudian theory:
aspects of Freudian theory, but this issue
needs to be evaluated in terms of current * Freudian theory is deterministic about
cross-cultural research, which seems to the development of the psychic
>>
70 Developmental Psychology
<<
apparatus (id, ego and superego), development. The underlying assumption
but not about the particularities that here is that human beings naturally tend
transpire in an individual’s internal and towards growth and self-actualisation and
external experience of life once these consequently the capacity for aggression
structures are in place. is not innate, but a response to the
environment. In assessing the validity of
* The contents of the psyche are unique to this perspective, it is useful to consider
each individual, even though the basic the following points:
structure of the psyche is the same for all.
* Freudian theory does postulate a
* The dynamic nature of the psyche means hidden relation between human
that an individual is always negotiating behaviour and motivation, namely
and renegotiating the self in relation to unconscious instinctual drives. Yet this
both external and internal realities. The does not imply that all human
instinctual motivations are always in endeavour is reduced to instincts.
contact with the demands of external Freudian theory places great emphasis
reality and always tempered by cultural on the role that culture (external reality)
injunctions. Thus internal psychological plays in curtailing and modifying these
processes (such as sublimation, defences instincts (including aggression), and on
or associations) occur continuously the role of defence mechanisms in the
throughout the life-span in response to service of making communal life possible
external realities and these processes and cultural and creative life pleasurable.
produce changes in behaviour, cognition
and affect. Freudian theory can therefore * If human aggression is only a
be described as deterministic at the level response to the environment, there
of psychic structure, but not at other must be an innate capacity for people
levels of functioning. to be aggressive, much as there is an
innate capacity for the acquisition of
4. Freudian theory reduces human language. Innate capacity for aggression
activity to biological instincts can partly explain the widespread horror
This criticism comes from the humanist and destruction that has characterised
tradition, which rejects Freudian theory the history of humankind since the dawn
on the grounds that it reduces the finest of time. By rejecting the notion of innate
qualities of human achievement to uncon- aggression, the humanist perspective
scious instinctual drives and, by so doing, fails to hold humankind responsible
fails to account for human agency and for the horror of which it is so
choice as a factor in psychological obviously capable.
Sue Williamson
F r e u d ’s p s y c h o a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 71
Specific tasks
➊ In the clinical vignette, there is no report of a male authority figure or role models.
Consider how this lack might impact upon Henry’s psychosexual development and
later symptoms.
➋ Freud contends that the ego is a ‘slave of three masters’. Apply this statement to a careful
consideration of Henry’s personality structure as exhibited in his neurotic symptoms.
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➌ Do you feel that Freud’s ideas of penis envy and his general portrayal of women within
his developmental theory are reasonable? Try to consider both sides of the argument.
General tasks
➊ Think about a character in a movie or a novel or even someone you know well who
has done something extraordinary. Having worked through this explanation of Freud’s
developmental theory, write an essay that will attempt to account for this extraordinary
action. Involve an understanding of the psychosexual stages of development, the
Oedipus complex, and the ego/id/superego structure of mind and what use has been
made of defences in your answer.
➋ In what ways do you think Freud’s theory might be limited? If you have completed
the specific tasks above, refer specifically to any possible limitations in accounting
for Henry’s actions. Are there gaps in the theory that need to be filled, and if so,
what are they?
➌ Give some thought to how Freud’s developmental theory applies to your own life.
Do you think that it helps explain much about you? If so, what and how? If you feel
that Freud’s theories are not able to explain significant aspects of your life, reflect on
why you think this is so.
Recommended readings
Appignansi, R & Zarate, O (1992). Freud for beginners. New York: Icon.
(True to its title, this text utilises an impressive spread of illustrations, in a near comic-book format,
to lead its readers through the theory and history of Freudian analysis.)
Freud, S (1977). ‘Three essays on the theory of sexuality’. In A. Richards and A. Dickson (eds), The
Penguin Freud Library. Volume 7 on sexuality: Three essays on the theory of sexuality and other works.
London: Penguin.
(A collection of the classic papers that laid the foundations for Freud’s distinction between adult and
infant sexuality and the theory of the psychosexual stages.)
Freud, S (1978). The interpretation of dreams. London: Penguin.
(A fascinating read. Shows Freud at his most accessible. How he develops his concepts is intriguing.
This book covers most of the outline of his understanding of the unconscious.)
Freud, S (1982). Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. London: Hogarth Press.
(Freud is very easy to read and this volume introduces you to the basic concepts of psychoanalysis.)
Gay, P (1988). Freud: A life for our time. London: Papermac.
(A wonderful tale that traces Freud’s life and theoretical developments. There are also some
intriguing accounts of the intense relationships Freud had with certain people.)
72 Developmental Psychology
Laplanche, J & Pontalis, J B (1973). The language of psycho-analysis. London: Hogarth Press.
(An academic read, defining psychoanalytic terminology.)
Osborne, R (1993). Freud for beginners. London: Writers and Readers.
(Rival publication to Appignansi & Zarate [1992].)
Storr, A (2000). Freud: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(A very concise ‘primer’ to Freudian psychoanalysis that can be read in an hour or two. It usefully
blends anecdotes from Freud’s own life with explanations of key theoretical terms and principles.)
Western, D (1998). ‘The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed
psychological science’, Psychological Bulletin, 124 (3), 333-371.
Western, D (1999). ‘The scientific status of unconscious processes: Is Freud really dead?’ Journal
of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 47 (4), 1061-1112.
Wolheim, R (1977). Freud. Glasgow: Fontana/Modern Masters.
(Nearly a classic in its own right, Wolheim’s slender volume does an admirably succinct job of accurately
tying together the main theoretical strands of Freud’s thought.)
CHAPTER
4
Klein’s object relations
theory of development
and personality
]acki Watts
1. Introductory concepts
2. Theoretical focus
3. Psychological birth and selfhood
4. The concept of an internal world
5. The concept of psychic structure
6. Innate capacity
7. The paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions
8. A critique of Kleinian theory
Introductory concepts
In this chapter we examine the ideas of Melanie
Klein whose work resulted in the development of
object relations theory. This first section introduces
the key concepts of the intra-psychic world, phan-
tasy, and object relations.
Intra-psychic. The internal of the body, in other words, the instincts, as does Freud, in the psychic
world of emotional or development of the individual. However, where Freud conceived a
mental functioning. drive model of conflicting instinctual urges as the model of psychic
functioning (Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983), Klein’s theory postulates a
relational model of psychic functioning.
Klein’s focus is on the content of the intra-psychic world. She postulates
that this content is made up of phantasy relationships that mirror or
reflect the physiological state of the infant.
Phantasy
Phantasy. The mental Phantasy refers to the intra-psychic experience of unconscious relations
correlate (equivalent) between good and bad experiences. Phantasy is postulated to be the mental
of the instincts.
correlate (the equivalent) of instinctual life (Isaacs, 1948). Phantasy is the
term used to denote the most primitive origins of thought. Klein (1932)
postulates that we have innate knowledge of certain images, such as the
breast, and it is these innate images that constitute the origins of thought.
For example, when a young infant suckles it has an innate phantasy of
the breast that correlates to the experience of suckling. Phantasy is thus a
reservoir of innate unconscious images and knowledge that has been built
up as a result of phylogenetic inheritance, just as a duckling ‘knows to
follow’ when it is born.
Pre-Oedipal. The time,
in Freudian terms, which Object relations
is focused upon the oral In phantasy the infant is able to develop an awareness of the breast. It is
and anal stages. It is the
also in phantasy that the infant has a relationship with the breast. Such
time when the child is
still within a dyadic focus
relationships are innate to our capacity to be human. Thus the experience
(the mother), and has of the body is linked to innate knowledge about what the body is
not as yet come to see experiencing. This concept is central to object relations thinking and will
the father and mother be explored in detail later.
as conflicting objects to Klein’s concepts introduced a way of understanding the individual that
love. These conflicts arise has led to creative and innovative advances in post-Kleinian writings about
with the Oedipal stage
the quality and nature of our personalities. Object relations thinking has pro-
where loving the mother
and father is no longer an
vided the major impetus for understanding the development and treatment
uncomplicated affair. of pre-Oedipal personality structures, such as borderline and narcissistic
personality disorders.
Triad. A three-person You will remember from the chapter on Freud that ‘pre-Oedipal’
relationship such as denotes a pre-genital psychosexual stage, hence infantile sexuality
the relationship that functioning. Freud’s focus had been on the importance of Oedipal issues
characterises the Oedipal
(a triadic, three-person, conflict model of understanding intra-psychic
situation—mother,
father, and child.
functioning). His interest was in the importance of psychosexual drives
in the development of the personality and of neurosis and, of course, on
the nature of the unconscious. Freud’s contribution to understanding
development and pathology lay in his focus on the psychosexual stages
and the child’s eventual resolution of the Oedipal situation—of the
three-person relationships between parents and child. Object relations
theory focuses on the first years of development, where the infant is
Dyadic. A dyadic primarily involved in a two-person interaction, or a dyadic relationship.
relationship refers to The focus is thus on early primitive mental operations, which are
a relationship of two. postulated as largely biological in origin. These early primitive mental
K l e i n ’s o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 75
Theoretical focus
Klein’s work was with young children, the youngest being two
years old. From these analyses she worked backwards to capture the
significance of the experiences of early infancy. This method of
theory-building followed the route pioneered by Freud. Freud had
pieced together a theory of the significance of childhood psychic
development by working backwards from the free associations and
symptoms of adults.
Initially, in working with children, Klein was faced with the limita-
tions of the language of children. By chance, she came to conceptualise
play as giving us access to the internal unconscious world of children—
the manifest symbolic representations (Klein, 1926). Manifest symbolic Manifest symbolic
representation refers to the idea that what we see the child doing in play representations.
The readily seen symbolic
is a symbolic representation of latent (that is, unconscious) meanings.
presentation of something
(This is equivalent to the free association of adult patients in psycho- else such as a person,
analysis that provides clues to latent, unconscious meanings.) an idea, or an image.
Unconcious conflicts and wishes make up the world of children. The remembered dream
Play therapy allows the working through of the conflicts and wishes is a manifest symbolic
associated with significant others in the child’s life. Material from these representation of the
therapies led Klein to make theoretical formulations about the nature unconscious images.
of the early psychic life of infants. The psychoanalytic play process
Play therapy. The
revealed how the child is continually engaged in sifting ideas about self psychoanalytic method
and others. This information led Klein to closely examine the child’s of intervention that is
self-other configurations. (This is a term used to denote an intra- used with children.
psychic relational meaning. In our phantasy life we construct images The play is seen as the
(objects) of self and significant others in continually changing patterns equivalent of the free
of relationships with each other, which are not necessarily related associations of adults in
psychoanalysis in revealing
to the reality of our external circumstances. These configurations
unconscious material.
inform our perceptions of external reality and explain how, in some
circumstances, we can be so wrong about what is happening around us.) Configurations. The
Klein postulated that play revealed these configurations as patterns of composition of internal
internal relationships with phantasy objects. The nature and function object relations, which
of these internal, unconscious object relationships was to hold her consist of characteristic
theoretical interest for most of her life. (The concepts of phantasy and self-other representations.
In other words, how we
objects will be further explored later.)
experience ourselves
Klein’s concern was to understand the preverbal, precognitive world and others.
of the infant’s experience (Klein, 1975a; 1975b). The infant’s world is
initially a world of only sensate (bodily) experiences. It is through the
experiences of the body that the infant makes contact with the world. For
the infant, there is a seamless experiencing where the self and the world
are experienced as one with no boundaries or edges marking where the
baby’s body stops and the other person or the world starts. There is little
76 Developmental Psychology
Pre-personal needs. cognitive ability, and at this stage certainly no interpolated ‘I’ to mediate
The time in early infancy cognition. To capture this infantile experience in words is clearly very
when the infant is not difficult, which is why the language of object relations theory is sometimes
cognitively aware that
clumsy and odd in that it is trying to grasp, in language, what essentially
his needs are experiences
of his own body. Rather has no language.
these experiences are felt Klein examines the processes through which the infant—initially an
as impersonal experiences, undifferentiated bundle of pre-personal needs—evolves into a unique person,
which merely happen to with a distinct character, personality, and way of relating both to the world
the infant. Note the baby’s and to himself. The means through which Klein explored this focus
curiosity when he finds his was through play. The play of the children in analysis revealed a complex
hands and feet.
set of phantasy relations both with the self and with significant others—the
self-other configuration.
Klein (1946) postulates that from birth, the intra-psychic life of the Part objects. State in
infant is concerned and taken up with its relations to these good and bad which the infant is unable
cognitively to apprehend
experiences (or part objects). One can see that at this stage of development,
real people, or whole
to speak of relations to people is much too complicated given the infant’s objects. Objects can only
limited cognitive abilities. The infant can only have phantasy relations to parts be experienced in terms
of bodily experience. of their functions, such
as the giving or with-
The role of phantasy holding breast instead
The term ‘phantasy’ denotes the primitive mental functioning of the of the mother.
mind. They are not fantasies or daydreams as an adult might experience
them. Neither are they concerned with reality, as in the reality principle
described by Freud. Rather, phantasy is postulated to be the reservoir of
innate, unconscious images and knowledge that has been built up as a
result of phylogenetic inheritance.
Isaacs (1948) elaborates the concept of phantasy as constituting the
basic substance of all mental processes. She postulates that mental activity is
78 Developmental Psychology
operating from birth. The level of mental activity is, however, too primitive
to think of it as constituting ‘thinking’. Thus the term phantasy denotes
this first mental activity. As such, phantasy is the early psychic or mental
representation of the experience of the body, that is, of the instincts. Freud
had postulated that psychic motivation results from the instincts of libido
and aggression, which he later conceptualised as the life and death instincts.
What Isaacs postulates is that phantasy occurs along with instinctual life
Psyche. The psyche is and forms the psychic (mental) content of the instinct—the baby may be
the term for the mental having a good feed which feels good. The baby knows about this good feeling
processes of our mind. It because of the phantasy of having a good relationship with a good breast.
thus refers to the content Object relations theory thus moved psychic motivation out of the
and manner of operating
arena of Freud’s drive model and into a relational model of psychic
which characterises our
thinking, feelings and
functioning. In other words, object relations theory postulates that it is our
general mental life. relational needs that inform/motivate our psychic functioning rather than
our instinctual urges. Phantasy came to be seen as the primary mental
Psychic structure. Refers activity and the primary content of all thought. There is no impulse or
to the quality, as it were, of instinctual urge that is not experienced also as unconscious phantasy. All
our psyche. The contents thought, however rational, is seen to be based in unconscious phantasy. It
of our mental life built
is this concept that underlies the psychoanalytic understanding that every
up over the course of
development, based
action and thought has significance and that even feelings of emptiness are
upon processes such rich with unconscious meanings.
as internalisation and
projections of experiences, The concept of psychic structure
may lead to a sound It is important to emphasize that there is no such place in the brain as the
capacity for contact with psyche. Rather, by psyche we mean mental processes that progressively build
reality and thinking or
up through cognitive development and make possible the ability to think, feel,
it may lead to a fragile
contact with reality and
and have a mental life. These processes are conceptualised as the building of
psychotic thinking. psychic structure, the gradual building up of the contents of an internal object
relations world and the particular quality of these relations. Early structure is
Introjection. The taking
based upon physiological experience and the accompanying phantasy relations
in of experiences. In
psychoanalytic terms it
that lay the foundation for an individual’s particular psychic life. How this
refers to the internalisation happens is initially through introjections (taking in) and projections (expelling
of others and their values out). The earliest interactions that the child has with the world are through:
to build psychic structure.
1. Introjection of the world through the senses. This process starts
Projection. A mental with the internalisation of the world through the physiological
mechanism by which the
experiences of the body, such as feeding.
infant expels unwanted
or terrifying aspects of his
internal world. In Kleinian 2. Projection occurs when unwanted, or even valued, bodily sensations
theory, projection is are projected out. This occurs through physiological processes of
initially a developmentally the body, such as defecation, crying, and screaming. With feeding
normal mechanism that the infant takes in (introjects) the good or bad milk. With defecation
assists in the safe-keeping the infant expels (projects) what is either good or bad. These
of the good object. With
physiological experiences occur concomitantly with phantasy, that
development, its use
comes to be associated
is, phantasy accompanies the physiological experiences. A baby may
with a defence mechanism be having a good feed. In phantasy there is introjection of the good.
whereby one’s own If the feed feels bad, then in phantasy the baby may expel the bad
traits and emotions are by screaming. At this early stage of development the experience
attributed to someone else. is of taking in or expelling the good or bad part object, for example
K l e i n ’s o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 79
Innate capacity
Klein’s major focus is on the intra-psychic world. While she acknowledges
the role of the environment (the mother) in influencing this capacity, it was
only in the last phase of her career that she started to explore the implications
of external influences. This focus arose because of the growing interest in
infant observation in Britain. Have a look at the box on infant observation
and those in Chapter 7.
Klein was greatly influenced by Freud’s concepts of the life and
death instincts. She saw these instincts as being the prime motivators for the
anxieties that characterise internal object relations. Libidinal and aggressive
phantasies are seen as the direct mental/psychic representations of the life
and death instincts (Klein, 1946).
Klein saw aggression as a fundamental human potential, in the same
Constitutional/innate. way as she saw love. Love and aggression are constitutional givens. Klein
Aspects that exist at birth (1957) postulates that the level of normality and stability of psychic structure is
but are not hereditary. dependent upon the child’s constitutional or innate capacity for aggression
They are acquired during
and love. The infant has a genetic potential for greater or lesser capacities
foetal development.
An example is the level
to love and hate. This varies from infant to infant. This means that love
or amount of innate and hate are the basis of our motivational life or, once again, that our psychic
aggression in the infant. motivation is relational. Our need to live, which is seen as a relational need,
is co-existent and in continual tension with our need to die, which we are
Paranoia. State of mind doing from the moment of birth.
characterised by feelings The death instinct is operative from birth. The infant therefore suffers
of being persecuted. In the
from paranoid anxieties and attempts to deal with these anxieties through
paranoid-schizoid position,
for instance, paranoia is
schizoid mechanisms, which consist of splitting and projective defences. The
the primary anxiety that operations of the life and death instincts are important in Kleinian theory
one is under threat. and underpin the idea that behind all psychic motivation is an element
of aggression. The implication of this concept is that an individual’s
Schizoid. A way of being psychoanalysis is not complete until the aggressive components of the
in relation to others psyche have been analysed. Klein’s conceptualisations facilitated a way of
that is characterized by
under-standing some of the anomalies of human behaviour. An example is
withdrawal and an inability
to form close relationships.
an individual who in reality is talented, attractive and admired but whose
internal phantasy object relations are suffused with aggressive impulses,
leaving them feeling worthless, unattractive and empty.
K l e i n ’s o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 81
Envy
Envy is seen as a particularly malignant form of aggression (Klein, 1957). In all
other forms of hatred, aggression is directed towards the bad object. The bad
object is hated because it is seen as persecutory and/or withholding. It is seen
as persecutory largely because it contains the projections of one’s own sadism.
When we hate someone it is because we have projected a part of our selves that
we hate. This is not to say that he may not be an objectionable person. However,
it is through the mechanism of projection that we invest emotionally in that per-
son. The hated person is invested with aspects of our own split-off hated parts.
We are linked through the operation of projection, this is why we experience
the emotional involvement or reaction. As an example, you may hate someone
because they are lazy. The interpersonal dynamic would suggest that laziness is
part of yourself, which you have denied or split off. If laziness were not a split-off
aspect of yourself you would probably not have any particularly strong feelings
about the lazy person. Thus, once you have come to terms with split-off or de-
nied aspects of your psyche there is no energy or need to hate the other person.
Envy, however, is different to hatred. It is directed towards the good
object. It hates the goodness that is possessed by the good object and wishes
to destroy the very goodness that it envied. Klein (1975b: 56) makes some
interesting distinctions between envy, greed and jealousy:
A case of envy
Mary appears to be a successful accountant. happen and she could enjoy the success,
She has a husband and two children yet she she is even more unhappy, feeling that it
is unhappy with her life and feels that she was all worthless and not satisfying. The
always has to prove that she has the best of dynamic here is that she is envious of the
everything. Consequently she pushes both good of things and when she achieves
herself and her family to be the best at the good, she destroys its meaningfulness
everything and is most upset when this does and is left feeling empty and greedy.
not happen. Significantly, when it does Developmentally, Mary has a sister who was
>>
82 Developmental Psychology
<<
her mother’s favourite. The two girls were her mother. Klein would propose that
not that close as children but managed to Mary’s innate capacity for envy was evoked
get along well enough. But in relation to the by her relationship with her mother. A child
mother, Mary was a rebellious child. She with less envy would perhaps have directed
behaved as if she did not care about her her aggression, in the form of jealousy,
mother and went out of her way to upset towards the sister.
Positions/stages. In
Kleinian terms, positions In envy, destruction is the motive. In envy there is a dyadic, two-
refer to a particular person relationship where you wish to destroy the goodness of the
arrangement of object other. Thus in envy, a sibling would hate the mother rather than
relation configurations experience jealousy of another sibling.
to which one returns
depending upon Positions
developmental and
Klein postulated the concept of positions rather than stages. The concept
contextual issues. For
example, a person in the of positions emphasises that there is a process that persists throughout life
depressive position may and that one may alternate between the positions. Klein (1946) postulated
return to the paranoid- two positions, an early paranoid-schizoid position and a depressive
schizoid position if there is position. The depressive position never fully supersedes or overcomes the
trauma in their life. A person paranoid-schizoid position and throughout life we may oscillate between
in the paranoid-schizoid
the two. These positions provide the phantasy contents or object relational
position may move to the
depressive position by
dynamics that constitute the matrix through which we will negotiate our
working through various intra-psychic (internal to the mind) and inter-personal (external to the
psychological conflicts. mind) relations to the world.
Freud’s stages refer to
distinct time periods which The paranoid-schizoid position
are sequential and which The paranoid-schizoid position is the earliest position, laying foundations
affect the quality of the
from birth to approximately three months. In the infant’s experience, plea-
next stage.
sure (satisfaction) and un-pleasure (frustration) experiences are either good
or bad bodily sensations. An experience is either good or bad,
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON
concrete object. The attack must be defended against. This is achieved Re-introject. Psychic
through the defence mechanisms of projection and splitting. However, aspects that had initially
the projection increases persecutory anxiety due to the increasing fear that been projected. They are
then re-incorporated into
the bad object will retaliate and return. This anxiety is about the return
one’s inner world.
of the bad through re-introjection. The function of splitting (splitting the
good from the bad) allows the good to be protected from aggressive attack
by splitting it off to safety.
While part objects prevail, the infant’s phantasy relations are domi-
nated by paranoia anxieties. There is anxiety about the potential dangers
of the bad objects. By keeping the part objects split and separate the infant
is able, in phantasy, to keep the bad part objects separate and isolated from
the good part objects.
The paranoid-schizoid position is characterised by paranoid anxieties
and by the most primitive mechanisms of splitting, projection, denial and
projective identification. Splitting has two functions. One is as a defence
mechanism, which keeps good and bad separate. The other is as a normal
mechanism of development. It is through successful splitting, keeping the
good safe from attack, that the infant can accumulate ‘good’ experiences.
This accumulation allows the infant to develop a psychic structure that is
stable, with a coherent ego, able progressively to survive attack and acquire a
sense of continuity of self through time.
Projection is concerned with expulsion. It is usually expulsion of the ‘bad’
in order to rid the self of the bad object, for example there may be an attempt to
expel the physical sensation of hunger through screaming. Sometimes the ‘good’
is expelled, if the ‘good’ is felt to be under threat by attack from the ‘bad’.
Klein introduced the concept of projective identification to describe
the psychic phenomenon of putting as aspect of self experience into the
other. She did not elaborate this concept herself but later object relations
theorists have demonstrated that projective identification may be used as
an unconscious means of communication or as a means of unconsciously
getting the other to contain a projected part. As an example of projective
identification as a communication, we might be moved to feel sadness
when seeing someone else in distress. This emotion is not ours but one
which has been induced in us. This is the basis of empathy. An example of
projective identification as a way of getting the other to contain a projected
part, might be listening to someone speaking of a traumatic experience
that they have had. You feel overwhelmed by the tale yet the speaker is
unmoved. In this instance you feel an emotion that they are at the time
unable to experience.
The operation of envy undermines development, with serious conse-
quences. Splitting is a necessary and healthy aspect of development
but due to the operation of envy, the good object is no longer safe from
attack. The good object is attacked in phantasy in an attempt to destroy the
envied good. Splitting is then no longer an effective method for protecting
the good object. As a consequence, persecutory anxiety increases because
clearly there can be no good, only bad. Envy destroys hope because the very
goodness inherent in hope is destroyed. You will notice that envious people are
also paranoid people who find little good in the world.
84 Developmental Psychology
Manic defence
(denial)
Paranoid-schizoid position
Omnipotence
Paranoid anxieties
Depressive position
Loss
Whole object relations Failure
Damage
Phantasy
Guilt
Reparation
Manic defence
Success
Damage
Denial
Depressive anxieties Anxiety
Specific tasks
➊ Find a biography or an autobiography of someone. Examine their early life and see if
you can make any links or postulations about what influences may have dominated in
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
their early object relations. In other words see if you can identify the matrix through
which they then developed through life.
See if you can identify whether they are operating in the paranoid-schizoid or
depressive position and say what gives you this information.
➋ Think about Mary in the ‘A case of envy’. Using your imagination, write a case study
describing other difficulties Mary might have, given that envy is a strong motivator in
her psychic life. Think about Klein’s postulation that we are motivated by relational
needs yet envy attacks the good and undermines the development of psychic structure
and hence the possibility of functioning from within a dominant depressive position.
General tasks
➊ Organise an infant observation over a period of about six weeks. In the observation,
be sensitive to the baby’s responses to the world. See what deductions you can make
about the internal experience of the child.
>>
K l e i n ’s o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y o f d e v e l o p m e n t a n d p e r s o n a l i t y 87
<<
➋ Splitting and projection are two defence mechanisms that are quite easy to see in
operation. While they are characteristic of the paranoid-schizoid position, they also occur
in the everyday functioning of people who operate mostly in the depressive position. Try
to identify instances of splitting—when a situation or an emotion is split—for example,
‘I am good and they are bad’. What do you understand about the need to behave in this
way? You might also see instances of projection, when the subject attributes to others
his own personality traits or emotions. It might be more difficult to see splitting and
projection operating in yourself because of their unconscious function, but you might be
able to become aware of engaging in such defences.
Recommended readings
Bion, W (1967). Second thoughts. New York: Jason Aronson.
(Bion is one of the most prominent object relations theorists. He is difficult and challenging. To consider
yourself a serious student you will have to read him at some stage.)
Greenberg, J K & Mitchell, S A (1983). Object relations in psycho-analytic theory. London: Harvard
University Press.
(A scholarly but readable examination of the theoretical developments among the post-Freudian
theorists. Covers theorists such as Klein and Winnicott to Kohut and Kernberg, among a number
of other significant figures.)
Grosskurth, P (1986). Melanie Klein: Her world and her work. London: Jason Aronson.
(A thorough biography of Klein. As the title implies, the book gives access to Klein’s life and motivations
and gives human drama to her theory. This is the only complete biography on Klein. It is an excellent read.)
Isaacs, S (1948). ‘The nature and function of phantasy’. In M. Klein (ed.), Contributions to psychoanal-
ysis, 1921-1945. London: Hogarth Press.
(The first exposition of the Kleinian position on phantasy. Rather long-winded but definitive for the
serious student. Contributions to psycho-analysis contains a collection of papers from the greats of early
Kleinian theorising. It is interesting both from a historical point of view and for laying the foundations
of Kleinian thought.)
Klein, M (1946). ‘Notes on some schizoid mechanisms’. International journal of psycho-analysis, 27, 99-110.
(Her definitive paper. It may be a bit difficult but well worth the effort as it summarises her most
salient concepts and gives an overview of the two positions.)
Klein, M (1948). ‘Mourning and its relation to manic-depressive states’. Contributions to psycho-
analysis, 1921-1945. London: Hogarth Press.
Klein, M (1957). Envy and gratitude: A study of unconscious forces. New York: Basic Books.
Klein, M (1975a). Love, guilt and reparation and other works, 1921-1945. London: Hogarth Press.
Klein, M (1975b). Envy and gratitude and other worlds, 1946-1963. London: Hogarth Press.
(These two 1975 collections of papers cover the essential concepts explored by Klein.)
Kohon, G (ed) (1986). The British school of psycho-analysis: The independent tradition. London:
Free Association Books.
(Has some wonderful essays from the Independent School and an informative introduction that details
the development of the Middle School, also known as the Independent School.)
Segal, H (1964). Introduction to the work of Melanie Klein. New York: Basic Books.
(One of the best overviews of the work of Klein.)
CHAPTER
5
Fairbairn’s contributions
to object relations theory
Cora Smith
1. Introductory concepts
2. Infantile dependency and the theory
of motivation
3. Theory of development and primary
identification
4. Endopsychic structure and the schizoid condition
5. Fairbairn’s model of psychopathology
6. Treatment implications
7. A mind map of Fairbairn’s ideas
8. Limitations and achievements of
Fairbairn’s theory
Introductory concepts
In this chapter we will be introduced to the ideas of W R D Fairbairn
(1941-1963) and how they differed from the mainstream body of British
psychoanalytical thinking at the time.
Fairbairn’s core contributions can be summarised as follows:
1. Fairbairn was the first to formulate a true object related nature of the
self. Fairbairn believed that the ego or self was present at birth and
its prime aim was to attach to a parent figure. Fairbairn argued that
the infant needed to be treated as a person in his own right. When he
experiences significant parental failures, the unacceptable portions of
the parent are internalised in order to be controlled. Thus for Fair-
bairn, ‘object relations’ relates to failed interpersonal relations with a
F a i r b a i r n ’s c o n t r i b u t i o n s t o o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y 89
needed parent. However, it is the external person who is the disap- True object related
pointing object. Although Fairbairn emphasised the importance nature of the self.
of the real relationship he nevertheless retained the value of the Fairbairn believed that the
ego or self was present at
phantasied relationship.
birth. Its prime aim was to
attach to a parent figure.
2. Fairbairn argued against primary auto-erotic infantile sexuality
in favour of the concept of infant dependence. Infant dependence Infant dependence. The
refers to the infant’s dependence on a real external parent figure. infant’s dependence on a
He asserted that libido is primarily object seeking rather than real external parent figure.
pleasure seeking and discharging. The infant is primarily object
Object seeking. The
seeking because Fairbairn regarded the infant as driven to seek an infant is primarily object
attachment with a real external parent figure in order to survive. seeking. Fairbairn regarded
Implicit in this assumption is the absence of conscious or uncon- the infant as driven to seek
scious destructiveness within the infant. The infant is regarded an attachment with a real
as innocent and experiences automatic entitlement to a breast or external parent figure in
mother figure whose responsibility it is to provide nurturance. order to survive.
Schizoid personality. The concept of the schizoid personality can thus be understood
A discrete personality both as a discrete personality entity that is characterised by with-
entity characterised by drawal, alienation and dissociation or as referring generally to self
withdrawal, alienation
and object relating issues within an individual. Fairbairn’s theory
and dissociation or as
referring generally to self
emphasised the schizoid and dissociative aspects of individual
and object relating issues suffering in contrast with the more melancholic focus of Klein.
within an individual.
6. Fairbairn viewed the infant as object seeking and dependent on his
objects for survival. By contrast, Freud’s infant was far more ‘demonic’
and instinct-driven. Klein’s infant remained a ‘demon’ and was driven
by the death instinct and its derivatives, namely envy, aggression
and omnipotence. Winnicott however extended Fairbairn’s views
even further by defining the positive aspects of infant entitlement
as ‘primary creativity’. In sum, it may be considered that while
Freud emphasised the father and Klein the mother, Fairbairn and
Winnicott emphasised the child.
Concious
Unconcious
Idealized Object Central ego
rejecting of the child’s needs. The antilibidinal ego is connected to the Antilibidinal ego.
rejecting object by affects of rage and frustration. The memory traces in This refers to the aspect
the sub-ego of the mother figure, which represents hope and promise of of the ego that remains
identified with the
future gratification, constitute the libidinal ego. These are attached to the
rejecting aspects of
exciting or alluring object by affects of craving and longing. The rejecting the internalised object.
object represents the internalised aspects of the actual rejecting or frustrating This aspect of the ego
parent figure on whom the infant depends. The exciting or alluring object is therefore hostile and
represents the internalised aspects of the object that promise but frustrates devaluing toward any
delivery of nurturance to the infant. potential contact
Fairbairn theorised that ‘bad’ internal object relationships, which or gratification.
represent the internalisation of painful or frustrating interactions with
Libidinal ego.
parental figures, become split off from the central ego and repressed by it. This refers to the aspect
Consequently, an aspect of the ego is split off in relation to split-off and of the ego that remains
repressed objects and associated affects. The libidinal and antilibidinal identified with the
selves, by virtue of their existence, limit the range and depth of the conscious exciting aspects of the
functioning open to the central self. internalised object.
The size of the sub-egos is reciprocal to the size of the central ego and This aspect of the ego
is therefore perpetually
is thus a barometer of the severity of pathology. The sub-egos are based on
seeking and longing
actual interpersonal experiences between the child and the object. Each sub- for the promise
ego represents accumulated memories of rejecting or alluring experiences. of relatedness.
Should a growing child be subject to an overwhelming amount of frustration,
abandonment or abuse, then a densely occupied antilibidinal ego would, as Central ego.
a result, contain a vast repository of rage, disappointment and grief. In a This is responsible
similar vein, the libidinal ego will need to be equally invested with hope and for learning, thinking
and feeling in relation
longing in order to ensure continued attachment to the object.
to others, as well as
For continued attachment to the bad object to be maintained, a balance for repressing the
is found between the libidinal and antilibidinal egos. It is clear that an frustrating or painful
overwhelming antilibidinal ego would result in increased anxiety and object relation
ultimately in abandonment. Where a child is forced to continue attachment to systems, in order to
a rejecting mother figure, the emotionally deprived libidinal ego is compelled preserve attachment.
to expand itself by overvaluing those rare experiences of gratification that
actually do occur. An example would be that of a neglected child in a
children’s home who carries a birthday card sent some years earlier from
a parent, as evidence of the parent’s love and commitment. In this way the
child can remain attached to the neglectful parent, which he needs to do for
emotional survival. This is how Fairbairn accounted for the phenomenon of
repetition compulsion. There exists in the very structure of these subsidiary Repetition
selves an attachment to some negative aspect of experience that is felt as part compulsion.
of the definition of the self. As a result the individual continues to seek out An attachment to some
negative aspect of
unsatisfactory relationships in later life.
experience which is felt
In situations of extreme deprivation the antilibidinal ego may become as part of the definition
oversupplied with abusive or toxic memories leaving the child to surrender of self. As a result the
attachment to the frustrating or alluring bad object. Such individuals may individual continues to
become so resentful or disillusioned that they compensate through an unreal- seek out unsatisfactory
istic and grandiose investment in themselves. These individuals are usually relationships in
overly independent and self-reliant. later life.
The configuration of both the rejecting internal object relations system
and the exciting object relations system occurs in order to avoid awareness of
pain and threat of abandonment. It is important to recognise that the exciting
98 Developmental Psychology
The moral defence against ‘bad’ objects represents the child’s denial
or repression of any memories of a frustrating object in order to retain
their dependency on that frustrating object and retain the hope of future
needed fulfilment. In addition, the child solves the fundamental problem
of staying attached to a frustrating or abusive parent figure without
becoming overwhelmed by rage or anxiety through internalising the ‘bad’
object. This internalisation transforms the child into a ‘bad’ object and
thereby keeps the parent ‘good’.
Fairbairn indicated that the self that emerges from a childhood of
deprivation or frustration is ‘bad’, for three essential reasons:
2. The self is intrinsically bad because the child has internalised the
‘badness’ of the frustrating object in order to keep the parent ‘good’.
3. The child has not been loved by the parent in his own right and
100 Developmental Psychology
1. The extent to which bad objects have been installed in the uncon-
scious and the degree of badness by which they are characterised.
3. The nature and strength of the defences which protect the ego
from these objects.
Basic endopsychic In his later work, Fairbairn indicates that the ego is universally split,
situation. The splitting resulting in the basic endopsychic situation. This splitting of the ego into a
of the ego into a libidinal libidinal ego, antilibidinal ego and a central ego underlies all psychopathol-
ego, antilibidinal ego and
ogy. Fairbairn’s explanation of the universality of schizoid phenomena is
a central ego underlies
all psychopathology.
that their primary determinants comprise splits in the initially whole ego,
Fairbairn’s explanation of a progression from which no one essentially escapes.
the universality of schizoid The more serious development of schizoid features manifests with
phenomena is that their the presentation of superiority and a narcissistic inflation of the ego. This
primary determinants is due to the identification with idealised parental objects and the over-
comprise splits in the valuation of the contents of the self. Fairbairn regarded these attitudes
initially whole ego, a
as both defensive and as compensating against the experience of
progression from which no
one essentially escapes.
intrinsic badness or worthlessness. These formulations have formed the
groundwork of our current understanding of narcissistic and borderline
personality disorders as well as depersonalisation and dissociated states of
multiple personality disorders.
<<
returned to the fulltime care of her parents, described her mother as follows: ‘She is a
she retained contact with them through caring woman, she’s always got nice food
weekend and holiday visits. Several to eat and she’s got the house clean.
attempts to place her in foster care were She’s willing to help. I wish that she will
unsuccessful and largely thwarted by her always be there for me when I am old’.
mother, who regarded any attachment to Her description of her father was as
such parents as an act of disloyalty on E H’s follows: ‘My Dad, well I think you know
part and subject to the threat of severing he takes drugs but he limits them
contact with her. E H was the second nowadays. He is a very caring person as
youngest of several siblings, six boys and well, he says he loves us’. When
one girl. Notably, the youngest male describing her greatest fear, E H said it
sibling had been permitted to remain in would be, ‘to lose my family’. She
the care of his parents and appeared to be indicated further that once adult herself
a favoured child. Two older siblings who she would, ‘like to give my parents back
were now adults had returned to the what they gave me’. E H described herself
parental home after they were released as motivated at school and distant from
from state care and had found her peers. She added that she feared that
employment. Their presence in the she was a disappointment to her parents
household was conditional on their for her lack of achievement and
contributing to the financial upkeep of the popularity. In addition E H indicated that
home. Needless to say, neither parent was her aggression, lying, stealing and truant
employed and the burden of financial behaviours had understandably offended
support resided entirely on these adult her parents and contributed to their
offspring. E H’s father was described in reluctance to allow her to return home.
numerous court reports as unstable, This also accounted, in her view, for her
abusive, drug-dependent and seldom, or parents’ preference for her younger sibling
dubiously, employed. He appears to have who was living in the home. E H reported
been rather immature, self-involved and that she responded very negatively to
without concern for the needs or welfare criticism levelled at her by persons, other
of his children. The mother similarly was than her parents, stating, ‘When I am
described as always unemployed, criticised, I get angry and throw that
emotionally dependent, inadequate and criticism back in their face’. She added,
unreliable. Despite her obvious ‘I could hate someone who doesn’t
insufficiency as a mother, she behaved as respect me as a person’.
though her children owed her a debt of It seems a rather poignant observation
gratitude for her imaginary efforts. to note this latter comment in the light of
Although E H had requested repeatedly to Fairbairn’s observation that an emotionally
move back home, her mother refused to deprived child comes to feel that, ‘he is
take her in, having stated that she would not really loved for himself as a person by
be ‘too much to handle’. Despite his mother’ (1940: 17). Despite precious
numerous open statements of rejection little care and nurture from her parents,
from her parents, E H regarded the and evidence of significant neglect, abuse
obstacle to her return home to be due to and rejection, E H had managed to repress
the resident school social worker and her her hatred for, and her fear that she is
own bad behaviour. dependent upon, dangerous and
During a background history and unreliable objects. Her dependency and
psychotherapy assessment interview, E H fear of abandonment have rendered her
>>
102 Developmental Psychology
<<
repression of any negative attributes bad himself than have bad objects; and
in the internalised parent objects. The accordingly we have some justification for
internalisation of these bad objects is an surmising that one of his motives in
attempt to control these objects where becoming bad is to make his objects
external influence of their treatment of “good”’ (1943: 65). E H’s fear of
her is beyond hope. Notably, E H regards abandonment and rejection by her objects
herself as intrinsically bad and the cause is so acute that she has demonstrated
of her parents’ rejection of her, illustrating from a young age an unwillingness to risk
her use of the moral defence. This her mother’s wrath should she engage in
defence allows her to blame herself for the a relationship with foster parents. E H
failures of her objects, thereby keeping currently remains loyal to her mother
them ‘good’. through her schizoid withdrawal from her
Fairbairn noted, ‘It becomes obvious present peer group. She also can then
therefore, that the child would rather be protect herself from further rejection.
Treatment Implications
Although Fairbairn (1958) wrote extensively on the limitations of classical
psychoanalytic theory and contributed to a radical transformation of
psychoanalytic thought, he only addressed clinical treatment implications
of his work briefly in his final paper. Unfortunately, his ill health and
untimely death foreclosed the development of his psychotherapeutic
endeavour. Because of limited publications by Fairbairn on the clinical
application of his theory, a great deal of reliance is placed on Guntrip’s
(1975) description of his own analysis with Fairbairn. However, his per-
sonal interpretation of Fairbairn’s theory has been criticised as being
both distorted and contradictory and therefore cannot be regarded as an
accurate record of Fairbairn’s work.
Nevertheless, Fairbairn himself maintained that rigid adherence to
the details of classical psychoanalytic technique, as standardised by Freud,
was not only unjustifiably defensive of the analyst but also exploitative
of the patient. Fairbairn was critical of the requirement, by traditional
psychoanalysts of that time, that the analysands [patients] should possess
a relatively mature, strong and unmodified ego. He reasoned that such
an individual, if one existed, would be unlikely to seek psychoanalysis.
Within the classical psychoanalytic model, structural defects were not
considered analysable because only the id was seen as dynamic, with the
ego considered static. Fairbairn obviated this problem with his reformula-
tion of the concept of dynamic structure, wherein aspects of the ego are in
dynamic conflict with each other and possess a dynamic capacity. Analysis
aims at resolving the conflicts between the libidinal and antilibidinal egos
and thus healing the ego splits. According to Fairbairn, because structure
itself is dynamic, the difficulties and conflicts within it are thus accessible
to analysis. Fairbairn argued that patients seek psychoanalytic treatment
because they become aware of their own suffering. Fairbairn’s notion of
the universal nature of schizoid phenomena ultimately led to the broaden-
ing of the concept of pathology and has given a wider collective of patients
access to psychoanalytic treatment.
F a i r b a i r n ’s c o n t r i b u t i o n s t o o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y 103
bring the analyst into his inner world (Fairbairn, 1958: 378). The patient
maintains the internal configuration because he is attached to the internal
objects represented within. While the analyst attempts to penetrate this
closed system, the patient clings to his transference perception that is in
accordance with his internal world. The analyst’s leverage is derived
from his capacity to develop a personal relationship with the patient that
challenges the long-held transference perceptions. Fairbairn regards
transference analysis as therapeutic when it develops a new relationship
that impedes the patient’s previously closed system of object relationships.
Despite its lack of completion, Fairbairn’s model of psychoanalytic ther-
apy based on his own object relation’s theory was revolutionary. Subsequent
theorists have developed and expanded on Fairbairn’s ideas in no small
measure (Kernberg, 1980; Kohut, 1977; Masterson, 1981; Rinsley, 1988).
Guntrip (1975) described his own analysis his therapy was to attribute it to
with Fairbairn, which he contrasted with Fairbairn’s reticent personality and the
the one that followed with Winnicott. ongoing illness that plagued Fairbairn
Ironically, Guntrip found that Fairbairn was throughout the analysis. Guntrip felt that
more orthodox in practice than in theory, his negative transference in sessions was
while he thought Winnicott was more fostered by Fairbairn’s intellectually precise
revolutionary in practice than in theory. interpretations. He also found it odd that
Guntrip’s account of his analysis with the patient’s couch in Fairbairn’s drawing
Fairbairn describes him as orthodox, room had its head away from the desk
formal and lacking in personal relatedness behind which Fairbairn sat. He only later
in his sessions. Guntrip’s explanation of realised that there were other chairs facing
the contradiction between his theory and Fairbairn, which he could have chosen.
• Impulses exist only through the activation of the ego or self and
are not conceived of as sources of energy existing independently
from structures.
F a i r b a i r n ’s c o n t r i b u t i o n s t o o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y 105
• Each of these sub-egos contains the object and the part of the ego
related to it.
• The superego comprises the ego ideal, the antilibidinal ego and
the rejecting object.
Specific tasks
➊ Watch the film Shine or any equivalent film which shows the early experiences
of the protagonist [the central character]. How would Fairbairn’s theory formulate
the development of endopsychic structure or constellation of sub-egos in the case
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
of the protagonist?
➋ How would you use Fairbairn’s theory to describe the protagonist’s relationship with
his or her mother?
➌ Using Fairbairn’s theory give examples of schizoid personality formation which you
see in the protagonist’s various relationships.
General tasks
➊ Given the common practice of many South African families in which children are
initially reared by their grandmothers or aunts and then later by their biological
mothers (who have spent the child’s formative years either working or perhaps
studying and developing their careers), what impact do you think this would have
on primary identification and the development of the internal endopsychic structure
of the self?
>>
110 Developmental Psychology
<<
➋ Many South African infants and toddlers are being orphaned by the HIV/
Aids pandemic. What impact do you think this phenomenon could have on the
development of the libidinal and antilibidinal selves as proposed by Fairbairn?
Recommended readings
Fairbairn, W R D (1940). ‘Schizoid factors in the personality’, In Psychoanalytic Studies of the
Personality. London: Tavistock, 3-27.
Fairbairn, W R D (1941). ‘A revised psychopathology of the psychoses and psychoneuroses’,
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 22, 250-279.
Fairbairn, W R D (1943). ‘The repression and the return of bad objects (with special reference
to the ‘war neurosis’)’, British Journal of Medical Psychology, 19, 327-341.
Fairbairn, W R D (1944). ‘Endopsychic structure considered in terms of object relationships’,
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 70-73.
Fairbairn, W R D (1946). ‘Object relationships and dynamic structure’, International Journal
of Psychoanalysis, 27, 30-37.
Fairbairn, W R D (1958). ‘On the nature and aims of psychoanalytic treatment’, International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 374-386.
Fairbairn, W R D (1963). ‘Synopsis of an object relations theory of personality’, International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 24, 224-225.
(These seven papers provide the essential coverage of Fairbairn’s theory. Although his writing style is
rather dry and abstract, his papers are thankfully short. They are essential reading and will assist you in
discriminating between what Fairbairn actually said and what some erroneously attribute to him.)
Grotstein, J S (1993). ‘A reappraisal of W.R.D. Fairbairn’, Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 57, 421-449.
(A sound, succinct, summary of Fairbairn’s work applied in the contemporary setting.)
Grotstein, J S & Rinsley, D B (eds). (1994). Fairbairn and the origins of object relations. New York/
London: Guilford Press.
(A comprehensive collection of papers reviewing the contribution of Fairbairn by various
interesting theorists.)
Kernberg, O F (1980). ‘The object relations theory of W. Ronald D. Fairbairn’, Bulletin of the
Association of Psychoanalytic Medicine, 19, 131-135.
(A sound critique of Fairbairn’s theory.)
Rinsley, D B (1979). ‘Fairbairn’s object relations theory: A reconsideration in terms of newer
knowledge’, Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 43, 489-514.
(An interesting application of Fairbairn’s theory to the contemporary clinical personality theory.)
Rinsley, D B (1988). ‘Fairbairn’s ‘Basic Endopsychic Situation’ considered in terms of classical and
‘deficit’ meta-psychological models’, Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 16, 461-477.
(This paper places Fairbairn’s theory in the map of psychoanalytic debates.)
F a i r b a i r n ’s c o n t r i b u t i o n s t o o b j e c t r e l a t i o n s t h e o r y 111
6
Wilfred Bion: Thinking,
feeling and the search
for truth
Gavin Ivey
1. Introduction
2. The significance of thinking in normal and thought-
disordered people
3. The development of thinking and the capacity for thought:
the role of maternal reverie and containment
4. Projective identification and the intersubjective origins
of thinking
5. The application of Bion’s container-contained
model: Infant observation
and child and adult psychological
impairment
6. Love, hate and the search for
psychological truth
7. Understanding experience and
relationship interaction in terms of
linking, containing and paranoid-
schizoid↔depressive position interaction
8. A psychoanalytic theory of psychotic
mental functioning
9. Psychotic and non-psychotic parts of
the self
10. Psychological growth and mental health
11. Critiques of Bion
Wilfred Bion
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 113
Introduction
W R Bion (1897-1979) is one of the foremost psychoanalytic development
theorists. This is because of his efforts to understand the origins of the
human mind and the ability to think—about our own experience, that of
other people, and about the nature of external reality. For Bion, nothing
was more important than the ability to think one’s own thoughts, and to
strive against the grain of received opinion, accepted theories, and personal
internal resistance to acknowledging painful realities.
Despite being one of the most radical and innovative psychoanalytic
theorists, Bion’s points of reference throughout his career remained Freud
and Klein. (His work builds on and extends their theories, therefore it is
important to be familiar with the two previous chapters on Freud and
Klein before reading this chapter.) Freud was concerned with the nature,
origin, and fate of thoughts that were repressed because of the anxiety
they evoked in the thinker. Klein was concerned with how thoughts
attached to parts of the self were split off and located outside of the self,
and what impact this had for the thinker’s internal world. Both Freud
and Klein thus took the ability to think largely for granted. Bion’s unique
contribution to psychoanalysis was to investigate the fundamental nature
of ‘thinking’, and what made it possible. To do this he studied a ‘thought
disordered’ clinical population, that is, psychotics, whose thoughts were
distorted by primitive defensive processes, or who used their minds to
destroy the very capacity for thought. It is this inquiry into thinking—or
the refusal to think as a means of engaging with, or disengaging from,
psychic reality, that is the key to understanding how the various aspects of
Bion’s work fit together.
This chapter begins by exploring what thinking is, how it becomes
possible and how the capacity to think evolves in the interpersonal
context of the mother-infant relationship. This will entail a discussion of
Bion’s ‘interpersonalisation’ of Klein’s concept of projective identification,
and the container-contained model that Bion devised to understand how
we acquire the internal space in which to ‘house’ thoughts. Bion did not
separate thinking and feeling; in fact, he argued that thinking arises in
order to manage frustrating emotional experience. For this reason Bion is
not simply another cognitive theorist. The significance of this for living
authentically and staying aware of our internal and external reality will be
outlined in this chapter.
Bion’s theories have had a profound influence on adult and child
psychoanalytic psychotherapy and the training of psychoanalysts. In
Britain an extended period of infant observation is a necessary part of
psychoanalytic training. Infant observation and how Bion’s theories are
used to make sense of early childhood experience will also be described.
Bion had a profound understanding of psychotic experience, and
his unique contribution to understanding why ‘thought disorder’ is
the hallmark of much serious psychopathology will be outlined. Bion
also accorded central importance to the role played by truth—the
ambivalent need to both know it and flee from it—in human existence.
The chapter concludes with a critique of Bion’s theories and his practice
of psychoanalysis.
114 Developmental Psychology
Beta-elements. called these raw sensations beta-elements. Beta-elements are not felt as
These are sense comprehensible phenomena that may be labelled and understood, but
impressions and primitive
rather as sense impressions and primitive affects that are felt as concrete
affects that are felt
as concrete ‘things
‘things in themselves’. They are thus ‘unmentalised’ and meaningless
in themselves’. They sensations that haven’t yet evolved to the status of feelings.
are unmentalised and
meaningless sensations
that have not yet evolved
to the status of feelings.
Alpha and Beta Symbols
α The Greek sign for β The Greek sign for
Alpha, the first letter Beta, the second letter
of the alphabet. of the alphabet.
This baby is in the midst of How do thoughts and the capacity to think arise from beta-elements,
a beta experience where his which are emotionally concrete objects of raw experience that are not
feelings are overwhelming
yet symbolised, that is, given a meaningful status as cognitive referents
him. His father is attempting
to soothe him. or links to other experiences? Without any mental elaboration beta-
elements can ‘only be acted upon as things are: They are to be broken
up and thrown out; or, with some luck, sent out for detoxicating and
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 117
Here is a typical example of maternal reverie: A sleeping baby is This father has succeeded
woken by the telephone. While his mother is talking to her friend he in containing the beta
projections of the baby
notices that she is not there and starts crying. Mother hears him but
who is now soothed by an
finishes her conversation before going to him. He is now crying vigorously experience of containment.
and she picks him up and cuddles him, talking to him as she does: ‘Are This leads to an experience
you hungry, little chap?’ She then notes a distressing intensity in his crying of alpha functioning.
and that he is squirming against her cradled arms. ‘No, I don’t think
so. Did that loud noise wake you? And Mommy wasn’t here when you
woke up. Did you feel left on your own? Were you scared? And cross!
118 Developmental Psychology
Bad Mommy left you on your own and now you’re so cross’. The baby’s
crying has lessened and become more whimpering at this stage. At this
point she recalls an incident when she was pregnant, in which her husband
had to go on a business trip for a week, at a time when she was feeling very
vulnerable. She remembers feeling irrational hatred toward him and that
it had taken some time upon his return to feel emotionally reconnected
with him. She smiles now at how intensely this separation affected her
and how emotionally destabilised she had felt at the time. She returns
her attention to the baby and feels his body start to relax. She squeezes him
gently and says, ‘It’s terrible to feel left all alone, isn’t it?’
In the example above we notice how the mother puts aside an incorrect
assumption (her baby is crying because he is hungry) to become aware that
the cause of his distress may be something else. By paying close attention
to his cries and bodily tension she identifies his feeling state as distress
and tries to make sense of it. She relates it to the disruption of his sleep
and wonders if he was frightened by the noise of the telephone. Then,
however, she refines her interpretation of his distress and identifies it as
anger in response to her not immediately attending to him. This triggers
an association to a time when she felt vulnerable and was separated from
someone she loved, and had irrationally hated him for ‘leaving’ her. We
could say that this memory was triggered by her baby’s projection of his
infantile hatred toward the ‘bad’ mother for leaving him alone with his
distress. Her containing mind entertains and processes these feelings of
vulnerability, abandonment and anger, without judging them or trying to
dispel them. Her final words to her baby—‘It’s terrible to feel all alone’—
contextualises his anger toward her. She has unconsciously allowed the
baby’s temporary hatred toward her to ‘take shape’ in her mind in the form
of her reverie about an incident in which she hated her husband. Instead
of ignoring or feeling threatened by her baby’s anger, she understands this
as a normal response to feeling left alone when in a vulnerable emotional
state. Her baby, of course, does not understand what she is saying to
him but, as she vocally expresses her emerging understanding of what
his distress was all about, her quality of engagement with him will give
him some sense that previously unmanageable angry feelings have been
mindfully ‘transformed’ into something else. His previous experience
of a ‘bad-abandoning-mommy’ changes into one of a ‘responsive-and-
containing-good-mommy’ and his distress recedes, to be replaced by a
mood of relaxed contentment.
A containing mind. A containing mind provides a ‘mental skin’ in which the projected
The mother’s mind, contents of the infant’s primitive internal world are kept safe and given
which provides a meaning. Repeated experiences of containment will eventually result in
‘mental skin’ in which
the child’s internalisation of mother’s containing function. As Hobson
the projected contents
of her infant’s primitive
(2004: 257) succinctly puts it, the developmental accomplishment of
internal world are kept thinking is due to a ‘process by which things that happen between
safe and given meaning. people become things that happen in the individual’s mind’. This will
later be evident in the child’s growing capacity to tolerate frustration, to
soothe himself when distressed, and to think about and make sense of
his emotional experience, rather than evacuating it through projective
identification or impulsive behaviour.
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 119
While Winnicott’s ‘holding’ and Bion’s mental activity in which the mother uses
‘containing’ both describe maternal her mind to process, mentally digest and
functions developmentally necessary for give meaning to her infant’s frightening
normal infantile psychic growth, there phantasies. While the end-stage of
are important distinctions between them. containing will involve some appropriate
These distinctions are based, at least partly, physical activity based on the mother’s
on different theoretical models of infantile understanding of her infant, the core
mental life. reverie phase involves meaning-making,
Firstly, holding is considered a necessary rather than sensory-physical handling
prelude to confronting the ‘not-me’ of the baby’s psychosomatic needs.
world of external objects, whereas Containing is thus a more psychologically
containing presupposes an awareness specific activity.
of something not-me (a container) into Thirdly, while holding is something the
which primitive experience may be mother does for her infant, containing is
projected. Containment and projective a joint product of an interaction between
identification go hand-in-hand, whereas the container (the mother’s mind) and the
holding does not presuppose infantile contained (the infant’s projections). While
projective identification. Thus, instead of holding is thus always mentally growth-
needing a mind into which to project, the inducing, some container-contained
Winnicottian baby needs to feel initially dynamics may be destructive, either
that his mother is an extension of him, because of excessive infantile hatred or
and hence not separate from him. While envy directed towards the container, or
containment also presupposes a blurring because maternal emotional difficulties
of the separateness of self and object, since create a pathological container. Related
projective identification locates ‘parts’ of to this is that while the baby-being-held
oneself in the mother, the Bionian baby does not have to do anything, the baby-
has an innate awareness of his mother as being-contained does, namely to take back
something beyond or outside himself that or re-introject the projections that have
may contain his projections. been made more ‘digestible’ by mother’s
Secondly, containment is more of a meaning-making mental processing.
Projective identification
and the intersubjective origins of thinking
Whereas for Melanie Klein projective identification was a purely
defensive and intrapsychic process, for Bion it was also communicative
and interpersonal, insofar as the mother feels her infant’s projected distress
registering in her own affective experience. She is thus called upon to
mentally process and give meaning to the projected meaningless beta-
elements of the infantile experience, using her alpha function to think
about what is as yet unthinkable for the infant.
Nascent capacity for
Bion’s contention that the infant’s nascent capacity for thought is a thought. This capacity
product of the mother’s mind is important for three reasons. Firstly, it is as yet more a product
addresses a significant criticism of Klein’s model of mind, that is, that of the mother’s
psychological life is from the beginning an intrapsychic phenomenon ‘container’ mind.
120 Developmental Psychology
and that the experiences of objects in the external world are primarily
Interpsychic. determined by phantasy. For Bion, psychological life is interpsychic
The interaction between from the beginning, and the infant’s mind only develops to the extent
separate psyches, rather that the external mother can sensitively lend her mind (the container) to
than between parts
the processing of her infant’s primitive experience (the contained). The
of the same psyche
(intrapsychic).
development of a reflective psychic space to contain thoughts is thus an
interpsychic accomplishment rather than an automatic consequence of
individual neurological development.
Secondly, it provides a relational model of projective identification,
in which projective identification bridges the intrapsychic and inter-
personal domains by linking the infant’s projective phantasies to the
affective responsiveness of the mother. Projective identification is thus
not merely a primitive defence (as Klein contended) but also the earliest
communicative modality we have, a non-verbal means of alerting external
others to our internal events. As noted above, containment is essential to
the transformation of the infant’s terrifying phantasies and experiences.
Bion describes this process as follows:
‘(T)he infant projects a part of its psyche, namely its bad feelings
into a good breast. Thence in due course they are removed and
reintrojected. During their sojourn in the good breast they are
felt to have been modified in such a way that the object that is
reintrojected has become tolerable to the infant’s psyche. From
the above theory I shall abstract for use as a model the idea of a
container into which an object is projected and the object that can
Pathogenic.
be projected into the container; the latter I shall designate by the
Having a pathological
influence. term contained’ (Bion, 1962:90).
PHOTO: JACKI WATTS
to mentally elaborate and think about (give meaning to) the emotional
stirrings inside him. If mothers, because of their own anxieties, cannot be
mindful of their infants’ experience, then infants cannot develop minds
of their own to think about feelings; instead, they simply discharge these
emotional ‘happenings’ in the form of projective identifications. Whether,
and how, psychologically troubled adults think about their experience tells
us a great deal about their earliest relational transactions with their maternal
environments.
‘It is a rainy day. Mother is very tired and has a cold. She is un-
characteristically giggly and a little giddy on the day of this obser-
vation. Baby N is lying on his back on a blanket on the floor. His
movements are jerky and asymmetrical. Mother is sitting nearby
eating some toast. When N cries she picks him up and offers him
the breast. He latches on, but after a few seconds he startles and
throws his head back. Without hesitation, Mother stands up and
gets a pacifier which the baby sucks on briefly then rejects. Mother
offers the other breast but N cannot settle down. The observer feels
122 Developmental Psychology
tension between Mother and herself which she has never felt
before. Mother, standing, bounces the baby vigorously up and
down. His body seems stiff and his hands are in fists. His body
relaxes a little, but he startles violently again, then stiffens. Mother
continues to bounce him and tells the observer that N is fighting
sleep and that she herself used to do this when she was a baby’.
intellectual skills was the mother’s ability to focus on her two month-old’s
experience and to sustain the infant’s attention in early mother-infant
interaction’ (Hobson, 2004:136).
If this is the observable impact of poor containment on infants between
two and 18 months old, what about the longer-term effects on adult
psychological adjustment? We often see evidence of defective containment
in the form of adult psychological disorders. For example, a young woman
in therapy was unable to experience and acknowledge feelings of loss
and aloneness following the death of her mother, with whom she’d had
a complex and problematic relationship. Instead, years later, she would
feel compelled to get drunk, take drugs or impulsively engage in reckless
sexual behaviour, without being able to resist or think about these impulses.
Despite being intelligent she was extremely concrete in her thinking and
struggled to understand her therapist’s simplest comments about what she
might be feeling. Only after years of treatment, in which she gradually
started to tolerate, name and think about her feelings, did this impulsive
behaviour cease. As an adult, she thus had to internalise her therapist’s
containing alpha functioning in a way that she had not been able to do as
a baby with her mother.
Bion famously claimed that mental growth depends on truth in the same
way that living organisms depend on food. Truth here does not imply the
intellectual pursuit of abstract knowledge, but the emotional truth about our
own experience of ourselves and of others. Psychological growth and health
is thus dependent on our capacity to continually think about our feelings,
despite the discomfort this may cause. Psychoanalytic therapy, defined as
the pursuit of inner truth, is thus an extraordinary historical manifestation
of a universal human disposition. K is directly related to healthy containing
because containing is an expression of a K mental attitude or state-ofmind. A
containing mother uses her mind as an instrument to know and understand
what her baby is experiencing. When the baby grows up and becomes curious
abut his own feelings and ways of relating to people, this is an expression of
his internalisation of the inquiring or knowledge-seeking function of his
mother’s early relationship with him. When we find ourselves tolerating or
suffering emotions that would otherwise feel intolerable, this is due to an
internalised containing object, the intrapsychic manifestation of a container-
contained dynamic derived from our mothers’ thinking about our feelings
before we could do this for ourselves.
However, human beings are not simply truth-seeking organisms; at
any one time we experience a conflict between knowing the truth and
distorting it. This is how we may explain the fact that so many people
seem invested in fleeing from, rather than seeking knowledge of, their
inner life. The emotional knowing that is a hallmark of K needs to be
distinguished from the defensive use of intellectual knowing that is used
Curiosity. How we to evade pain and anxiety, and thereby mental growth. Curiosity, which
experience the desire is how we experience K, is always in the service of the reality principle.
to know the truth about Thinking can only be K when it is emotionally informed and driven
ourselves and the world
by the search for truth about ourselves and our environment. However,
we inhabit.
Bion’s theory of emotional links is complicated by the realization that L, H,
and K have their opposite negative equivalents, namely –L, –H, and –K.
The privilege Bion grants to K in his theory of psychic functioning arises
from two observations: Firstly, the universal developmental significance
of thinking as a means of tolerating and modifying painful sensory
experience, need frustration and object loss characteristic of infancy and,
indeed, the whole life-span. Secondly, the peculiar and disabling absence
of the capacity for thoughtful mental functioning implicated in psychotic
and borderline-psychotic psychological disorders. Bion’s curiosity about
the ‘thought-disordered’ aspects of psychotic mental processes led him to
formulate a ‘link’ that is the opposite of K, namely –K. K is what allows
us to perceive meaning, to connect different experiences, to distinguish
phantasy from reality, internal objects from external objects, and to learn
from experience. When envy or hatred predominates, or a containing
function has not been internalised, some people refuse or are simply
unable to think about feelings. Alpha-elements are transformed into beta-
elements, rather than the other way round, resulting in the destruction
of meaning and the evacuation of feelings. For example, a psychotic
person may turn his aggression into a delusion that a demonic presence
is attempting to control his mind, thereby ‘solving’ the problem within
him by locating it outside of him in the form of an evil entity. In another
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 125
states and perceptions of yourself and others in the course of a typical day,
you should start to see evidence of these dynamic psychological processes
at work in you and your relationships.
A psychoanalytic theory
of psychotic mental functioning
Psychotic psychological disorders are identified by impaired reality
testing, as evidenced in delusional thinking and/or the experience of
hallucinations. As was noted earlier, Bion was very invested in developing
a psychoanalytic understanding of psychotic psychological disorders and
their developmental origins. This was partly motivated by his commitment
to providing psychoanalytic treatment to a population Freud had believed
were unanalysable, and partly because the developmental distortion of early
infantile life that gives rise to psychotic mental processes simultaneously
reveals the early origin of normal psychological development. It is difficult
for someone with a normally functioning mind to imagine the horrifying
nature of a psychotic person’s internal world. Bion not only vividly described
Psychosis. A pathological the experiential reality of psychosis but provided us with a powerful
mental state characterised explanation of the mental processes giving rise to psychotic experience.
by impaired reality testing The infant destined for psychotic disturbance cannot tolerate the frust-
in the form of hallucina-
ration of early experience and so develops a mind invested in ridding
tions or delusions.
itself of experience, rather than processing and modifying it through
Thinking apparatus. the development of a thinking apparatus. Failed containment results in
Bion’s metaphor for a persecutory anxiety of the violent re-entry of the bad experience that was
functioning mind. temporarily expelled through projective identification. This mobilises
further destructive phantasy-based attacks on the frightening ‘invaders’,
thereby resulting in an increasingly vicious cycle that, at best, results in a
primitive and savage superego that interferes with the ability to think about
experience. At worst, it results in chronic psychosis that not only destroys
thoughts but the very mental apparatus that makes thinking possible. The
psychotic, in other words, mentally attacks his own mind so that its fragile
ability to make meaning out of experience is progressively eroded.
Bion did not dwell on the issue of what mothers bring to these failed
projective-introjective transactions, although he was not oblivious to the role
of the environment (mother) in the origin of psychosis. For Bion, the crucial
issue is the infant’s capacity for frustration tolerance. An infant able to tolerate
frustration will, to a degree, be able to manage the frustration arising from less
than optimal maternal care. He will experience some containment and inter-
nalise a containing alpha-function. However, an infant with poor frustration
tolerance will not be able to endure the inevitable distresses of baby sensations
and anxieties, even when mother’s containing abilities are adequate.
Bion contended that there are innate personality features that predispose
individual infants to excessive projection. These include a preponderance of
innate destructive impulses, which smother impulses to loving linkage with
objects (L) and curiosity to know reality (K). These are replaced by sadism and
an envious hatred of reality, both internal and external, which extends to every
thing which would make for an awareness of reality, that is, thinking. Terror
is the consequence of the failure to transform bad object experience into some-
thing ‘thinkable’, and the psychotic lives in dread of imminent annihilation by
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 129
what Bion called an ‘ego-destructive superego’, an amalgam of attacking bad Ego-destructive superego.
objects that impedes any efforts to understand one’s experience. A hostile internal presence
The question of which comes first, excessive infantile aggression or that attacks one’s attempts
to think realistically about
the maternal failure to contain destructive projective identifications, did
an experience.
not concern Bion. His focus was on the end result of the failed containing
process. What is important is not the origin of these experiences, but whether
these experiences can be endured and given meaningful representation,
rather than evacuated as unthinkable horrors. The evacuation of the
psyche of undigested experience and intolerable sensation is the opposite
of thought, that is, ‘anti-thought’. These ejected beta-elements, because
they cannot be processed and thought about, are experienced as destructive
external forces or physical objects attacking and invading the self. Internal
experience identified with these objects is split, along with the self’s nascent
capacity for awareness of reality. Anti-thought manifests as ‘attacks on
linking’, whereby all mental connections (implicit in memory, symbolism,
word and sentence construction, and so on), are obliterated because they
are associated with bringing dangerous parts of the mind into even more
terrifying conjunctions. In this pathological variant of the paranoid-schizoid
position, projective identification is used solely for ridding the mind of all
content, rather than for communication. Rather than representing mental
contents through symbols, these contents are instead manipulated through
increasingly desperate and chronic projective identification. Unthinkable
experience becomes something unthinkably alien and frightening, fit only to
be expelled from the mind. For example, the psychotic’s destructive impulses,
instead of being perceived as aggressive thoughts to be owned or disowned,
are transformed into visual hallucinations of savage reptilian creatures
waiting to attack him. He has evacuated the internal terror, the emotion as
object, as a hallucination of an external threat.
To review, whether due to excessive infantile destructiveness, poor
frustration tolerance, failure of maternal containment, or some combination
of these, where the operation of alpha function is disturbed, the sense impres-
sions of which the infant is aware, and the accompanying emotions, are not
worked over. They remain ‘undigested’ (and undigestable) beta-elements,
which become discharged as malevolent projective identifications. Any
contact with the receptacle of these toxic projections is dreaded, leading to
attacks on the link between infant and breast. The desire to know and to be
curious, on which all learning and self-understanding depends, is destroyed.
Lack of containment of the infant’s primitive emotional life leads to emo-
tions themselves becoming hated and, because external reality stimulates
emotions, reality also becomes hated, leading to hatred of life itself.
Schizophrenia is a severe consequence of the excessive use of uncon- Schizophrenia.
tained projective identification, with consequent fragmentary splitting of A chronic psychotic
the ego and objects. This type of splitting, under the influence of excessive disorder characterised
by fragmentary splitting
destructive impulses, produces the characteristic psychotic experience of
and massive projective
personality fragmentation and the destruction of the capacity for coherent identification, resulting in
verbal thought, hence the common reference to schizophrenia as a ‘thought bizarre delusions
disorder’. Furthermore, the typical paranoid delusions and hallucinations or hallucinations.
that are hallmarks of psychosis indicate the extreme deployment of
projective identification as a defence against the psychotic’s unmanageable
130 Developmental Psychology
sadism and envy. Bion made intensive use of his own emotional experi-
ences with psychotic patients to recognise and identify the operation of
projective identification into the analyst. According to Bion, in the primitive
world of paranoid-schizoid phenomena the infant relates to his own
perceptual capacities and functions as part-objects. Thus seeing, hearing,
feeling and, later, thinking are felt to be concrete parts of the self to which
the infant relates. They may thus be subject to splitting and introjective-
projective identification, as we will see in the following section.
Lebo wakes up feeling a bit tired and irritable. She recalls a bad
dream in which she was attacked by a vicious dog. She hates dogs
and consciously pushes away further thoughts of the dream. A hot
shower revives her and she goes to the kitchen for breakfast. She
finds her husband reading the newspaper and, feeling intensely
irritated, wonders why he doesn’t want to talk to her instead. He
smiles at her and she feels a little reassured. He reads her a story
about a woman who was raped in her apartment, close to where
Lebo lives. She feels suddenly angry and blurts out that all rapists
should be castrated. Her husband lowers his newspaper and says,
‘Is that really a solution to…’. She interrupts him, saying that he is a
man so of course he is siding with the rapist. She feels momentarily
frightened of him, has the impulse to leave the house, and is struck
by the possibility that the person she has married has a dark side
she hasn’t seen before. Here we see the blurring of internal and
external reality, which evokes a paranoid state of mind and distorts
Lebo’s experience of and connection to a former positive internal
representation of her husband. The dream—perhaps symbolising
an aggressive, biting aspect of her own personality—and the
newspaper article did not cause Lebo’s reaction, but catalysed the
psychotic aspect of her mental functioning.
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 131
experiences and to think about these in ways that transform them into
different experiences, which in turn are tolerated and thought about.
In other words, mental growth naturally occurs as long as we are able to
think about feelings rather than doing something to evade or get rid of
them, whether through drugs, sleep, denial, projective identification,
fundamentalist religion or any of the manifold ways in which people avoid
or distort their experience of reality. Curiosity about and tolerance of feelings
results in an openness to experience and an inquiring attitude toward reality,
whether this is internal reality (what is going on inside our own minds) or
external reality (what is really happening in the world around us).
This mental attitude, which requires a mind that can contain
experience while allowing itself to be changed by experience, results in
a truthful way of relating to oneself and others and an acceptance of life
as it is. Whatever pleasures and gratifications it brings, life is intrinsically
painful and frustrating. From Bion’s perspective, a healthy mind is not a
happy mind but one that has a capacity for both suffering and tolerating
what life presents us with.
Some people are able to think about their feelings in a more-or-
less consistent way, without requiring professional help. Others need
psychotherapy, either because they lack or have a defective containing
function, or because certain ‘pockets of experience’ result in a defensive
constriction of their ability to think about particular kinds of feelings.
A capacity to contain Psychoanalytic psychotherapy, as Bion sees it, provides an interpersonal
experience while being space in which intolerable experience—and an intolerant attitude toward
able to be changed by experience—may be transformed into tolerable experience and a more
experience, results in a receptive openness to experience.
truthful way of relating
to oneself and others and
The aim of psychoanalytic therapy is to use the relationship and its
the possibility of enduring products (feelings, thoughts, utterances, silences, and suchlike) to illuminate
relationships. the truth of patients’ psychic reality, and to help them think about this truth
PHOTO: JACKI WATTS
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 133
despite the pain, anxiety and frustration this process inevitably involves.
This confrontation with intrapsychic and interpersonal truth, what the
patient really thinks, feels and does in interactions with self and others,
produces ‘emotional turbulence’ and ‘catastrophic change’. Catastrophic
change, essential for therapeutic growth, at first sounds extreme, until
we pay attention to the degree of anxiety and resistance that accompanies
people’s realisation of some significant truth about themselves. For
example, a therapy patient who considers himself gentle and emotionally
controlled recalls a disturbing dream in which a psychotic killer attacks his
family with a knife, while he remains powerless to intervene. Although
the dream is unpleasant, he relates to it as he would to a horror film he
has reluctantly watched, as an observer offended by the alien images that
another mind (the film director’s) has subjected him to. During the course
of the therapy session, aided by the therapist’s interpretations, this patient
begins to relate differently to the dream, discovering that the deranged
killer is not ‘other’, but a representation of a hateful, destructive part of
himself. If he is to entertain this idea, rather than denying, repressing or
evacuating it, the fundamental question of who he is—his very identity—has
to change, and he has to experience the guilt and anxiety that accompanies
this disturbing new understanding. Such change, says Bion, is necessarily
violent because something (a familiar picture of himself) is destroyed, and
subversive, because a comfortable and familiar self-understanding has been Resistance. The person’s
challenged and overturned. The patient understandably experiences such defensive reaction to the
a revelation of psychic truth, or even its possibility, as an emotional crisis. emergence of a personal
Resistance is the person’s resistance to the emergence of a personal truth truth because of the
because of the emotional turmoil this transformation involves. emotional turmoil this
transformation involves.
However, although this change is catastrophic, it is not disastrous
because it opens up an enlarged experience of oneself. The real danger is
that the person cannot contain this new self-understanding, and subjects
it instead to psychotic defensive procedures in order to maintain psychic
stability and avoid thinking truthful thoughts about himself.
Critiques of Bion
Most critiques of Bion have been aimed at his rather purist approach to
psychoanalytic clinical practice and the mystical aspects of his later post-
Kleinian approach to psychoanalysis. In fact, these criticisms are two sides
of the same coin. They both concern Bion’s preoccupation with truth
and what we can know about reality: psychic reality, external reality,
and ultimate reality. Whereas external reality can be known through our
senses, psychic reality (a person’s subjective experience) cannot because,
while behaviour can be observed, experience is simply not observable.
For Bion, psychoanalysis is a relationship exclusively devoted to
discovering the truth of the patient’s experience. However, the analyst’s
desire to know the patient, and his belief that he can do so by using
psychoanalytic theory and his memories of the patient’s words and actions
in previous sessions, is an obstacle to discovering the evolving truth of
the patient’s psychic reality at this moment. Bion urged psychoanalysts
to consciously suspend memory (what they remember about the patient),
desire (the personal need to cure or change the patient) and understanding
134 Developmental Psychology
There is much to admire and learn from in Bion’s unflinching and rigorous
approach to what is existentially real and true beneath the habitual patterns,
evasions, deceptions, encrusted meanings, and anaesthetised responses that
constrict our authentic individuality. However, misery is the flip-side of Bion’s
ascetic and purist stance insofar as stoical suffering—by both patient and ther-
apist—is considered the hallmark of psychoanalytic investigation. It may ap-
pear that there is little room for playfulness or rest in this austere atmosphere.
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➋ What capacities does your person demonstrate for mentalisation (the ability to
think about feelings)?
➌ Can you apply Bion’s theory of psychotic and non-psychotic parts of the self in this
case? Identify specific behaviour that points toward these personality sub-organisations.
>>
Wi l f r e d B i o n : T h i n k i n g , f e e l i n g a n d t h e s e a rc h f o r t r u t h 137
<<
General tasks
➊ Think about a recent conflictual or frustrating encounter with a friend or family member,
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
preferably one that had a positive resolution. Can you identify L, H, and K, as well as
paranoid-schizoid and depressive moments in your experience of this encounter?
➋ In what specific ways are Buddhist meditation and a psychoanalytic session alike?
In what ways do you think they are different?
➌ The next time you feel uncomfortable (you may be cold, have a headache, or be
feeling anxious or depressed) consciously avoid doing anything to alleviate your
discomfort. Instead, just allow yourself to feel and identify the various sensations
and feelings that make it uncomfortable. After a while, do you note any change in
your experience of the discomfort? How would you understand this in terms of
Bion’s theory?
➍ Observe a mother interacting with a baby for a few minutes. What aspects of the
mother’s behaviour would you identify as being either containing or uncontaining?
How does the baby appear to be responding to the mother’s behaviour?
Recommended readings
Bion’s own writing is notoriously difficult to read and understand. He did not attempt to make his
work accessible to readers, but expected them to grapple with the difficult concepts and discoveries he made.
I suggest you read some of the excellent commentaries available on Bion’s writing before reading his own
books and papers. The following are particularly useful:
Bleandonu, G (1994). Wilfred Bion: His Life and Works 1897-1979. London: Free Association.
(This is a good biography of Bion that balances an account of his life with his contributions to
psychoanalysis.)
Bion, P, Talamo, F Borgogno & Merciai S A (eds) W.R. Bion: Between Past and Future. London &
New York: Karnac Books.
(This edited book contains some very good papers. Highly recommended is C.S. Sandler’s paper, ‘What
is thinking – an attempt at an integrated study of W.R. Bion’s contributions to the processes of knowing’,
200-219.)
Grotstein, J (2007). A Beam of Intense Darkness: Wilfred Bion’s Legacy to Psychoanalysis. London: Karnac.
(Grotstein tends to write in a very poetic and mystical way, which some readers find off-putting.
However, he was analysed by Bion and is one of the leading Bion scholars.)
Sandler, P C (2005). The Language of Bion: A Dictionary of Concepts. London: Karnac.
(This comprehensive dictionary of Bion’s concepts is indispensable for those interested in thoroughly
exploring Bion’s work.)
Symington, J & Symington, N (1996). The Clinical Thinking of Wilfred Bion. London: Routledge.
(This book, although emphasising the psychotherapeutic implications of Bion’s work, is one of the most
readable overviews of his work.)
CHAPTER
7
Donald Winnicott
Jacki Watts
1. Introduction
2. Winnicott’s focus
3. Impingement and appropriate failure
4. The process of un-integration to integration
5. Holding
6. Primary maternal preoccupation
7. The role of the father
8. Good-enough mothering
9. Transitional phenomena: transitional space
and the transitional object
10. The true self and the false self
11. Critiques of Winnicott
Introduction
Donald Winnicott In this chapter we examine Donald Winnicott’s contributions to under-
standing the significance of the mother and infant bond. Winnicott’s sense
True self. The true of the importance of this relationship is fundamental to his understanding
self is based upon what of the development of the individual. He explores how the quality of this
is true about sensate relationship between mother and infant influences the degree to which
experienceing, that is,
one can become a person. We look at Winnicott’s concept of the develop-
id or instinctual needs.
By remaining in touch ment of personhood, which reflects his remarkable creativity and genius.
with this level of integrity Theorists had previously taken it for granted that one was a person.
with the body we are Winnicott did not. He suggests that we become persons, and that we
living close to the
can only achieve this state given the necessary environmental provisions
true self.
and innate potential. He would suggest that some people do not become
False self. Means that persons but remain extensions of others, that is, false selves. We shall
we’ve had to abandon the explore his conceptualisations of true and false self development.
integrity of the body and Winnicott’s writings, while stemming from a Kleinian base, shifted psy-
adapted to the demands of
choanalytic emphasis from the intrapscyhic which is the mental activity that
external reality, eg implies
cleanliness is the only issue occurs within the personality, to the interpsychic or the interpersonal realm.
as it were. Interpsychic is the psychic activity that involves relations between people.
Within this focus, his exploration of the mother-infant relationship has
provided invaluable insight into infant development. His understanding
D o n a l d Wi n n i c o t t 139
of the role of the mother has also provided a model for therapeutic Free association. Speaking
intervention. The classic free association of Freudian analysis is indicated for about the first things that
intervention with neurotic conditions, but it is not applicable in personality come into your mind
without censoring them.
disorders. Individuals with personality disorders cannot withstand the
Psychoanalysis is based
frustrations and lack of gratification that occur in a psychoanalytic analysis. upon this method as it
While the advances offered by Kleinian theory have opened avenues becomes clear when it is
for intervention with self-pathology, Winnicott’s model has suggested a difficult for the patient to
new metaphor for psychotherapy intervention. This model is based upon speak, indicating conscious
modification of the strict psychoanalytic method and offers a focus on certain or unconscious censorship.
specific aspects of the relationship between therapist and client. It allows Personality disorders.
the individual to re-visit, within the relationship to the therapist, the early Enduring, maladaptive
damage of the pre-Oedipal development stage. ways of relating in
the world that affect
perceptions, thinking and
Winnicott’s focus interpersonal relations.
Winnicott (1945; 1960), like Klein, was concerned with examining the Such individuals struggle
processes through which the little bundle of sensate experiencing becomes with damage to their
a person. Winnicott focused upon the relationship between the mother and personality structure and
the infant. While guided by Klein’s (1946) understanding of the internal phan- have not fully developed
tasy world of the infant, he gave central importance to the reality of the external their ‘personhood’.
Developmentally, they
mother. Winnicott saw the mother as influencing both the extent to which one
have not had optimum
could become a person—that is, living one’s life in relation to one’s true self— conditions to develop
and the stability of the person. We shall discuss the idea of the true self presently. stable personality
(Winnicott’s term ‘mother’ refers to the real mother of the child. However, the structures and struggle
term can also be applied to the function served by the primary caregiver. This with problems of the ‘self’.
extended understanding of the term has implications for South Africa where a Self-pathology.
large proportion of children are reared within extended family contexts.) Personality pathology that
results from disruptions
Psychological birth and personhood prior to the Oedipal years.
Freud took the entity, or even the existence, of the ‘person’ of the individual The disruption occurs due
to compromises within
for granted. This was not the case with Winnicott. He was fascinated
the infant/mother/primary
with what enabled the making or ‘becoming’ of the person. Winnicott caregiver relationship.
was foremost a paediatrician and he saw the mysteries of the mother- These lead to unstable
infant bond as fundamental to understanding what was involved in development of the child’s
becoming a ‘person’. Winnicott coined the famous clinical understanding sense of self.
that there is no such thing as an infant; there is only a mother and infant. Grandiosity. The exag-
This formulation means definitively that, where there is an infant, there gerated belief in one’s own
is maternal care (1956). If maternal care is not there, there is no infant— importance or power. For
the infant will die. What is unique about Winnicott is that he took this ob- the infant this is an essential
vious fact and explored its implications in a creative and distinctive way. experience as it allows a
The basic understanding is that initially the infant is in a state of safeguarding of the infantile
ego against its own reality-
omnipotence. It does not have the cognitive capacities to understand that
based helplessness.
the world and the mother are separate entities. Rather, for the infant, the
world and the mother are products of his own making. This experience is Wish fulfilment.
an aspect of the infant’s initial hallucinatory relationship to the world and The state in which the
imaginary wish seems to
people. The world and the infant are all one. Through this experience the
have been fulfilled. Thus
infant’s grandiosity and wish fulfilment remain intact, safeguarding the dreams, symptoms and
stability of the infantile ego. Winnicott draws attention to the need for an hallucinations are all wish
infant not to experience frustration that goes beyond the capacities of the fulfilment in which the
infantile ego to bear. Thus when a baby is hungry the baby needs to be fed. wish has found expression.
140 Developmental Psychology
To not feed the baby and make the baby wait would constitute an
Impingement. Demand impingement upon the infantile ego.
made by the environment
(the mother) upon the Impingement and appropriate failure
infant to be separate from The distinction that Winnicott makes between impingement and appro-
her. The infant is unable to
priate failure is central to his understanding of the role of a good-enough
meet the demand due to
the unintegrated state of mother (Winnicott, 1958). Winnicott maintains that the initial state of the
the infantile ego. infant’s ego is one of fragility and fragmentation. In this state, the ego cannot
tolerate impingements. An impingement is experienced when the mother
Appropriate failure. acts towards the infant as if the infant were able to tolerate degrees of
Failure of the environment frustration and separateness from her, which he is not capable of tolerating.
(the mother), under the Winnicott defines this experience as a demand made by the environment (that
controlled conditions
is, the mother), which the infant is unable to meet due to the unintegrated state
of holding, to meet the
infant’s needs. This kind of the infantile ego. The experience is a demand made upon the infant to be
of failure is necessary in able to be separate from the mother and to be able to react to this separateness.
order to stimulate the Remember that psychoanalytic theory sees the infant’s experience as initially
infant’s development of an an omnipotent state where the infant and the world are experienced by
active and positive sense of the infant as one, a seamless experiencing where there is no experience of
separate being, leading ‘something else’ outside of the infant. When the environment brings to the
to a sense of ‘me’ and
baby’s experience the fact that there is ‘something else’ then the infant has
‘not me’.
to react. The ‘something else’ may be that the baby is hungry and the milk
Omnipotence. State of does not appear seamlessly in the baby’s omnipotent world. The need for
believing that one has the infant to react indicates that there has been maternal failure to meet his
unlimited powers. needs. In short, there has been impingement.
Effects of impingements
An example of an impingement is a will fall into despair. Winnicott (1952)
hungry infant who is not fed. The child refers to this experience as ‘nameless
will experience the hunger as a threat dread’. Many adults are familiar with the
or an outside demand that threatens anxiety of what is essentially associated
his cohesion. There is not the cognitive with nameless dread. It is an experience
ability to know what the experience is, or of an acute anxiety that one will fall to
that eventually the mother will come to pieces. This experience stems from such
feed the child. For the infant, the demand early impingements when the child was
is terrible and life-threatening. The lack unable to cope with the experience of
of feeding makes a demand upon the separateness from its mother. With such
child for frustration tolerance that the experiences, the infant must deny its
infant is incapable of meeting. The child hunger and the infant begins to develop
will reach a peak of desperation, and compliance to the outside world. This is
then when the feeding does not come, a false self compliance.
If the demand is persistent, the infant then has a need for some armour. ‘The
infant that is disturbed by being forced to react is disturbed out of a state of
being’, (Winnicott, 1960).
Winnicott does not imply that the pair must always have this mother-
infant unit whereby the infant can live in a hallucinatory relationship forever. Hallucinatory
Rather, he proposes that failure must occur but that it must occur within relationship. A false
specific parameters (Winnicott, 1960). He terms this kind of failure, appropriate sensory perception that
is not based upon reality.
failure. As the infant develops there must be the experience of separateness from
In terms of infantile
the mother. This experience of separateness allows the infant to develop a development this is an
sense of its own being as a separate and independent person in the world. extremely important
These experiences must, however, be appropriate to the capacity of the infant experience as it safeguards
ego to experience separateness. When this is so the infant can experience the infantile ego from
his separateness from the environment as building up the ego rather then fragmentation.
breaking it down.
Therefore, providing that the environment is sensitive to adapting to the
needs of the infant’s experience of going-on-being—not putting the infant in
the situation where it will ‘fall to bits’—the demand for separateness will not
be impingement but will be appropriate failure.
Is birth an impingement?
Winnicott did not see birth as an impinge- constitutes the impingement; rather it is
ment upon the infant. He saw birth as the way that the environment may fail
something for which the infant has been the infant. This could occur through the
innately prepared. He was too much of alienating processes that characterise some
a pragmatist to hold that something as gynaecological hospitals, or through the
natural and normal as the birth process nature of the mother’s bonding with her
could be construed as traumatic for infant in the first days after birth.
the infant. It is not the actual birth that
Holding
Holding is a fundamental concept of Winnicott (1958). By holding he means
the quality of the mother’s provision of infant care. Without holding the
inherited potential of the infant cannot come into being. The quality of
the mother’s holding has a fundamental influence upon the actualisation
of the infant’s selfhood. Winnicott means by this that literally the infant
A young boy emerging cannot start to be, except through the holding of the mother. The quality of
into selfhood. the infant’s being will be largely dependent upon the quality of the mother’s
holding capacities. For the infant, where holding confirms the aliveness of
Holding. The quality of
the mother’s provision
its body, it has experiences of the aliveness of the true self. This means that the
of infant care. infant then has an experience of existing. Thus infant care, or holding,
which confirms the bodily experience of the infant rather than substituting
the mother’s experience, results in experiences of existing. An example that
illustrates this concept is an infant who is fed when hungry. The feeding—
that is, holding—will confirm the experience of a hungry body. On the other
hand, a hungry child who has his nappy changed will not have the experi-
ence of the body confirmed.
Empathy. The ability to Winnicott maintains that holding is based upon empathy and not upon
understand and enter into understanding. Thus Winnicott’s concept of mothering has nothing to do
the feelings of another. with intelligence or education. Good mothering is about a mother’s capacity
to feel her way appropriately into her child’s experience. Winnicott (1958)
classifies holding into three stages:
hungry; she warms when the infant is cold. How a mother knows these
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON
things about her infant is through her ability to be attuned to her infant, to
be in a state of primary maternal preoccupation.
If the mother can be in this state and adapt appropriately to the needs
of the infant, there is little disturbance and need for reaction. The infant’s
existence remains intact and relatively seamless. When the mother gets it right,
there is no awareness of the mother and the infant’s developmentally needed
state of omnipotence remains intact. Where a mother is not psychologically
healthy enough to move into a state of primary maternal preoccupation, the
result is that she enforces impingements upon the infant. She substitutes
Primary maternal her own needs and understandings onto the id needs of the infant. When
preoccupation. For the last this happens the id experiences (the body experiences) of the child are not
few weeks of pregnancy and given meanings that make sense of the child’s experience. The result is that
the first few days after birth,
the mother loses her interest
the potential aliveness of the body is killed and the child must then adapt to
in the outside world and the meanings imposed by the external world to understand his experience.
becomes preoccupied with Should this happen at this early stage of the infant’s life it is least able to cope
the baby. with such failure. The impingements interrupt the going-on-being of the
infant and produce the threat of annihilation. For Winnicott (1945) the fear
of annihilation is the most primitive anxiety.
Eating disorders
It is a common experience of people with own needs and not the hungry needs of
eating disorders to use food to calm or the baby. The child then becomes confused
comfort themselves. They have learned and learns to associate emotions with
to see food as a substitute for emotional food. The basis of the establishment of a
holding. Developmentally we can postulate stable ego is a sufficiency of the experience
that in their childhood their mothers of going-on-being. For the infant, the
were either anxious or unable to meet possibility of this state of going-on-being is
their emotional needs. Instead food was totally dependent upon the quality of the
given as a substitute. The mother might mother’s management of infant care, that
misinterpret the baby’s needs or the is, her capacity to hold the infant’s id and
mother might feed the baby to satisfy her developing ego-needs.
Good-enough mothering
Winnicott postulates mothering as a process, which matches the changing Good-enough mothering.
needs and cognitive development of the infant. While a period of almost The mother’s capacity
perfect attunement—that is, the state of primary maternal preoccupation—is to fail appropriately in
meeting the infant’s needs.
necessary for the infant’s initial development, this harmony between infant
Winnicott makes the point
and mother needs to come to an end. If the mother continues in this state clear that it is ‘appropriate’
and continues to pre-empt the infant’s needs, the infant will not have the failure that is needed,
opportunity of experiencing where the mother leaves off and the infant not impingements. These
himself begins. The infant must begin to have experiences that define it as an failures give the infant
entity, separate from the mother. For this to happen, the mother must begin slow, measured, small
to appropriately fail in meeting the infant’s needs. Winnicott points out that it doses of frustration and
anxiety. These experiences
is ‘appropriate’ failure that is needed, not impingements. These failures give
allow the infant to
the infant slow, measured, small doses of frustration and anxiety. This anxiety experience separateness
begins to disrupt the infant’s omnipotence in ways that do not threaten it from the mother without
with annihilation. being overwhelmed by
The infant begins to experience that he is not the whole world. the experience.
Appropriate failure disrupts this omnipotent phantasy, forcing upon the
infant experiences that start the rudimentary distinctions between me and
not me, between inside and outside. Appropriate failures allow the infant to
come slowly to experience an outside reality over which he does not have
omnipotent control. Each failure results in bodily tensions and experiences of Daddy and baby bonding
which not only assists
relative terror and disintegration. Survival of each of these experiences leads
the child’s emotional
to a growing stability and trust in a sense of going-on-being. These experi- development but also
ences allow the infant to experience separateness from the mother without enhances mother’s
being overwhelmed by it. capacities to mother.
For this developmental transition to occur appro-
PHOTO: BRIDGET CORKE PHOTOGRAPHY
the infant from ‘falling to bits’, or ‘disintegrating’. The infant’s initial sense
of omnipotence is a vital stage of his initial sense of going-on-being and
psychic growth. The good-enough mother has an intuitive knowledge of
this state and does not challenge or disrupt the infant’s omnipotence. The
mother who is not ‘good-enough’ induces traumatic and inappropriate
failures that lead to traumatic breakdowns of omnipotence and the
infantile experience of ‘falling to bits’.
A few possible situations which could constitute inappropriate failures
for the infant’s wellbeing range, for example, from the hospitalisation
PHOTO: MICHELE VRDOLJAK
Mother/Infant unit
FAILURE SUCCESS
Father
impingements/appropriate failure
good-enough mothering
holding/stage of dependence
inner reality
capacity to be alone
– personhood +
Critiques of Winnicott
A major critique of Winnicott is concerned with his writing style. While
he claims to be within the psychoanalytic paradigm, it is difficult to find
150 Developmental Psychology
<<
not write anything down. He has to find a would be to describe the development
position from which he can be receptive, of interpersonal relationships between
which means not becoming part of, an infant and others, including the
interfering or intruding in the family group. observer. The possible meanings of
As soon as possible after the visit to the these careful observations are then
family, the observer is asked to record as discussed in the seminar group who
accurately and in as much detail as possible, meet weekly, observers taking turns
what has been observed. Observers are to present observational material.
often worried that they will not remember Gradually, observers are asked to consider
well enough to report the observations the emerging picture of an infant’s
of a full hour. This is a skill that gradually personality, what might be the subtle
develops, and observers are often surprised interaction between constitutional and
to find how much detail they are able to developmental factors, and how these
gradually remember and record. may be supported by the environment.
The observer, in this new setting, is There is therefore some interest in
in some sense in a position similar to discussing the extent to which the
that of new parents. He is entering into environment is holding of the infant’s
a situation of ‘not knowing’ and hopes experience. Because of the sharing in the
to gradually learn something about seminar group, each observer has the
babies and their relationships to their opportunity to get to know their observed
families, approaching the situation with baby very well, but also to know the four
an open mind, as free of judgements other babies being observed. This often
or preconceptions as possible. Whilst highlights the uniqueness of each baby,
often an observer is observing a first child mother and family situation.
and mother, sometimes there are other What one might learn from this situation:
children, or a father, present. These family An observer, in learning about an
members would be recorded as part of actual baby, is also learning about his
the observation. own responses to the observation. The
The written reports are then shared experience of an observation therefore
with a small group of usually five fellow offers opportunities for personal and
students in a regular seminar. The group professional growth. Theoretical
functions as a container for the experience knowledge is explored with the actual
of the observer himself, who may use experience of being with a child.
the group to consider the emotional The position of the observer demands
impact of a new baby on a family, but that he learns to contain and bear intense
also on himself. It is acknowledged that feelings without acting on these. One
observers, in experiencing so directly this is gradually asked to develop the mental
intense and emotionally charged event space to observe what is happening
in the family’s history, are also deeply within oneself, as well as to consider
effected by the experience. This can be what observed events might mean for a
a particularly painful if the observer is mother, father and baby. This stance is
exposed to experiences reminiscent of helpful in clinical practice, particularly in
his own early infantile experiences. For work with children.
this reason, people undertaking an infant Skills like observing accurately, and
observation are encouraged to be in their being able to remember and record the
own analysis or psychotherapy. finest details of a situation are also skills
The task of an observation, therefore, valuable in clinical practice.
Shayleen Peek
152 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
➊ Research or think about which countries have provided great literature or art, great
philosophers, musicians, political thinkers etc. What conditions would you think contri-
buted to these developments? Do they fit with Winnicott’s theory of transitional space?
➌ Consider the hypothetical case of a sexually abused child who is prematurely forced
into sexuality and becomes promiscuous. How would you explain the development of
the promiscuity in Winnicott’s terms? If you have done the chapter on Fairbairn, how
would his theory offer additional insight?
General tasks
➊ Consider in what ways you may be acting from a false self position. Remember that the
false self is largely an unconscious way of being in the world, but it is possible to have some
knowledge of when we act in ways that are adaptations to the necessary social demands of life
[healthy false self functioning], to those times when we make adaptations to those around us
rather than living in a more true manner with what we actually think or feel.
➋ Take a survey of people to find out how many of them had transitional objects in their
childhood. Find out what the objects were and what happened to them. See if your
findings fit Winnicott’s theory of transitional phenomena.
➌ Observe two mothers, one whose baby is only a few days old, and the other whose baby is a
few months old. Make notes about the differences between the ways they react to their infants
and look at the Ester Bick interest box on page 150 earlier for some important guidelines.
Recommended readings
Goldman, D (ed) (1993). In one’s bones: The clinical genius of Winnicott. London: Tavistock.
(A collection of papers by Winnicott and others that highlights the range, creativity and unique contribution
of Winnicott.)
Kohon, G (ed) (1986). The British school of psycho-analysis: The independent tradition. London: Free
Association Books. (A wonderful introduction to the writings of some of the Independent tradition
theorists. You will get a good sense of the way they think about their world and their patients.)
Winnicott, D W (1958). Collected papers: Through paediatrics to psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books.
Winnicott, D W (1964). The child, the family and the outside world. London: Penguin.
Winnicott, D W (1965). Maturational processes and the facilitating environment. New York:
International Universities Press.
Winnicott, D W (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.
(These four books provide the essential coverage of Winnicott’s concepts. They are all highly readable and most
enjoyable. Be aware that Winnicott’s idiosyncratic style is deceptively easy. When you try to capture what he has
said you will struggle!)
CHAPTER
8
Kernberg’s theory of
normal and pathological
development
Carol Long
1. The development of the relationship between our affects, self repre- Basic units of experience.
sentations and object representations form our basic units of experi- The relationship
ence. As these grow more complex, so our developmental processes between our affects,
self representations and
become more sophisticated and our psychic structures develop.
object representations.
2. The development of the ways in which we internalise our expe-
riences, and the processes of internalisation, also become more
sophisticated as we develop.
PROCESSES OF INTERNALISATION
Ego Identity
4. Integration of self & object representations
Higher structures
Introjection
1. Undifferentiated
STAGES OF DEVELOPMENT
Self Object
Affect
Representations Representations
Now the baby has a more sophisticated sense of what is going on. It also
becomes clearer that the same experience might be experienced differently
by different babies, or by the same baby at different times. There is also a
Self representation. rudimentary sense not only of the feeling but of something happening outside
The representation of the the baby and something being experienced inside the baby. In other words,
self in the internal world. the baby has a rudimentary sense of self representation, in this case a sense of
K e r n b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f n o r m a l a n d p a t h o l o g i c a l d e v e l o p m e n t 157
sharing in the power or being at the mercy of it, and object representation— Object representation.
the feeling of being lifted up. Even though tiny babies don’t really know The representation of
who they are or that mother is separate from them, there exists some image others (such as the mother)
in the internal world.
in their internal worlds of self and object. These representations, over
time, start to become coloured by their affective experiences. The whoosh,
and the resultant affect, self representations and object representations
associated with it, become stored in affective memory.
The basic units of development, then, continually interact with one
another as various affect states become attached to self and object represen-
tations. The initial category of pleasure and un-pleasure begins to include
degrees of each of these emotions, and the self and object representations
become coloured as ‘bad’ or ‘good’. If the baby likes the whoosh, this may
build upon his existing self representation (‘‘Oh! I like that, it’s thrilling, I
feel grand and do wonderful things’’ (Klein, 1987:128)). If the baby doesn’t
like the whoosh, this may build upon his existing object representation
(‘Oh! That was horrid. Horrid mommy is doing horrid things to me’).
In other words, as the basic affects of pleasure and un-pleasure develop,
they begin to colour internalised self representations and object representa-
tions. Over time, these self representations and object representations cluster
around ‘good’ and ‘bad’. A young child will have a ‘bad’ mother image and
a ‘good’ mother image in their internal world, as well as ‘bad’ and ‘good’
self images. The implication of this is that affect is always embodied in a
self-object relationship. These interrelationships develop into the structures
of our mind (id, ego and superego). At first, the affects are very intense and
the self and object representations very unclear. As the child grows, the self
and object representations become more associated with affect and the child
becomes more able to distinguish between self and other. As this process
unfolds, the affects usually lose their early primitive intensity.
For Kernberg, the initial affects of pleasure and un-pleasure develop into
what he calls drive derivatives. Freud postulated two basic human drives: Drive derivatives.
The derivatives of basic
libido and aggression. Drive derivatives, in Kernberg’s theory, are the derivatives
pleasure and un-pleasure,
of basic pleasure and unpleasure, complexified through experience, that have complexified through
become libido and aggression. Affects of pleasure develop into libidinal drive experience, that
derivatives while affects of un-pleasure develop into aggression. It is important have become libido
to understand that drive derivatives are located in the internal world but come and aggression.
<<
that moment, nothing else exists for the self and object representations (good
baby except for this intensely pleasurable and bad are more defined while self
experience. and object are more differentiated).
In the second scene, later in the film, Because of this, the experience would
Tsotsi opens the bag where he has put be more strongly related to the child’s
the baby to find that he has forgotten existing self and object representations.
the tin of condensed milk, which ants If, for example, bad self and object
have found. We see an image of the representations predominated in the
baby covered in condensed milk, ants child’s internal world, he might be
all over his face as they bite him to get more likely to internalise the second
to the condensed milk. The scene is so episode. If good self and object
powerful because the baby’s distress representations were stronger, he
is heartbreakingly obvious. In that might be more trusting of Tsotsi and
moment, the baby feels as though the forgiving of his bad experience. This
world is attacking him and his extreme would depend not only on his prior
un-pleasure is all-encompassing. experiences in the external world, but
Kernberg would predict that this also on his own innate combination of
small baby would not at first connect aggression and libido.
these two scenes. There would be Returning to the baby in the film,
no understanding that Tsotsi gave what impact would Tsotsi’s actions have
baby something good and then made on this baby’s future development? This
something bad happen. All the baby would depend upon how frequently
would know would be confined to the these experiences were repeated. It
moment in time where either pleasure would also depend upon how the
or un-pleasure predominated. Both baby initially experienced them and
of these experiences would be stored this more generally would be related
in affective memory and both would to the climate of the baby’s internal
colour self and object representations. world. A very aggressive baby would
From these two experiences, the baby be expected to respond to this
would not know, in reality, whether adverse circumstance with much more
Tsotsi was caring or uncaring. aggression than a less aggressive baby.
Imagine, however, if Tsotsi had The chances are, however, that no one
found a five-year-old child in the car, event would determine the baby’s later
and the same events had occurred. The development but that it would depend
five-year-old has more mature affects as on the general ambience of the baby’s
well as more defined and differentiated internal and external experiences.
into being through the interaction between the internal and external world.
Kernberg believes that we develop through an interaction of our inborn propen-
sities and our external experiences. Some people, for example, build up strong
affects of aggression largely as the result of bad experiences while others build
up strong aggression because they had a large amount of inborn aggression in
the first place. Experience, for Kernberg, is always a combination of the internal
and external world: we can only ever experience what happens to us through
our own subjective feelings. It is clear, however, that our first dyadic relationship
(mother and infant) is the most important site for the development of basic units
of development. Our early primitive affect states, and the ways in which these
colour our self and object representations, provide the basis for later experience.
K e r n b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f n o r m a l a n d p a t h o l o g i c a l d e v e l o p m e n t 159
Processes of internalisation
If affects colour our self and object representations, how are each of
these internalised? How do we build up or populate our internal worlds
with self and object representations? Kernberg believes that processes of Processes of
internalisation are crucial for helping us to complexify our basic units internalisation.
Crucial for helping us to
of development. It is these processes of internalisation that provide us
complexify out basic units
with the momentum for developing. Kernberg identifies three kinds of of development.
internalisation, which develop one after the other. First come introjections,
followed by identifications and finally by ego identity. We all use all Three kinds of
forms of internalisation. However, if we have developed satisfactorily, ego internalisation.
identity predominates. Introjections, identifications
and ego identity.
Introjection
Introjection is the earliest and most basic form of internalisation. It Introjection. The earliest
can first be found in the small baby who has not yet clearly differenti- and most basic form of
ated self representations from object representations and whose affects internalisation whereby
the baby takes into the
are still very intense, primitive and diffuse. The baby introjects (takes
unconscious mind object
into the unconscious mind) object representations and representations representations and
of self in relation to the object. What gets introjected depends funda- representations of self in
mentally on affective valence: on how self and object representations relation to the object.
are coloured by affect. At first the baby takes in chaotic impressions
related to his experience of the world. Remember, however, that these Affective valence.
chaotic impressions are largely divided into pleasure and un-pleasure. The extent to which an
experience is coloured
As the baby becomes more mature (and there is increasing differentia-
by positive (pleasurable),
tion of self from other), the chaos starts to take on a pattern. Introjects negative (unpleasurable)
of similar valence (that is, pleasure or un-pleasure) become clustered or neutral affects.
together. These experiences then fuse to form good internal objects
(internal objects coloured by pleasure) and bad internal objects (in-
ternal objects coloured by un-pleasure). Just as Melanie Klein’s good
and bad breasts represent images within the child’s psyche and not the
actual physical breast, so too does Kernberg believe that we take in
objects which are defined, not by their real qualities, but by the child’s
experience of good and bad.
These introjections are important for development. Firstly, they
help to organise the chaos of the baby’s experience, albeit unconsciously,
so that the baby can begin to make sense of the world and his place
within it. Secondly, the process of introjection, with its fusion of similar
introjects, clusters good together and bad together. This helps to keep
them separate from one another and thereby helps to prevent bad from
overwhelming good. Thirdly, in this process of fusion, the raw intensity
and diffuse confusion of early experience starts to become progressively
less intense and diffuse.
It would be terrible if we continued to experience our affects as acutely
as a baby does. It would also be very difficult to develop at all since we
would be paralysed by our affects. We do use introjections defensively
(particularly when splitting, as will be discussed under abnormal develop-
ment) when, for example, we feel the need to keep good safe from bad.
However, introjections also propel development forward by making the
internal world of the small baby more manageable.
160 Developmental Psychology
Ego identity
Ego identity is the highest level of internalisation because
it is the most mature and most approximates reality. It is
still strongly influenced by the child’s fantasies and internal
Two little boys obtaining pleasure from experiences but becomes more realistic as the child grows
identifying with a male figure. In the top
picture the boy enjoys modelling himself
older. Ego identity begins around the end of the third
on the man while in the bottom picture the year (Stage Four and Five in Kernberg’s classification of
son is sharing a relationship with his father. development—see Stages of normal development) but starts
K e r n b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f n o r m a l a n d p a t h o l o g i c a l d e v e l o p m e n t 161
Stage One. Affects of therefore instructive to read these stages as Kernberg’s unfolding story of
pleasure and unpleasure. development rather than as five separate boxes of experience. As you read
Not yet differentiated
this story, pay particular attention to what happens to categories of ‘self’
good versus bad or self
versus object.
and ‘object’ as well as to categories of ‘good’ and ‘bad’.
The baby is so small that he has not yet had sufficient experi-
ences of pleasure and un-pleasure to build up self and object
representations. He doesn’t really know who or where he is and
what is going on. The very small baby simply experiences what
comes at him. At this point the baby is undifferentiated: the
baby has not yet separated self from object or good from bad.
If all goes well, the baby will experience sufficient pleasurable,
gratifying experiences (for example enough good feeds; enough
warmth and comfort) to begin to build up a ‘good’ self object
representation. This requires a caring relationship as well as ad-
equate attention to physical needs. Armed with the beginnings
of a good representation—having taken enough good inside—
the baby will be ready for the next stage.
Stage Three: Differentiation of self from object Stage Three: Self and
representations object become more
This stage begins when the baby has a differentiated sense of good self and differentiated. Good and
bad are first split then later
good object, usually around six to eight months. This is a very important
come closer together so
stage because the differentiation of bad self and bad object is about to that total self- and
begin more fully. The task of this stage is first to separate good and bad, object-representations
as well as to separate self and object. At first, the baby’s ego is very fragile are formed.
so it is easy to feel assailed by good and bad, and to feel confused about
where these experiences are coming from. As this stage progresses, the
baby’s defence mechanisms develop in order to deal with this situation. Defence mechanisms.
The primary defences used at this stage are designed to keep good and bad Unconscious, intrapsychic
separate: splitting, projection, omnipotence, denial and idealisation (these reactions aimed at
diminishing anxiety.
concepts are similar to those of Melanie Klein and Freud). Good and bad
are kept separate at all costs, so the child fluctuates between the two.
This frightening situation is greatly helped by the baby’s growing
recognition of his mother. This helps the baby to become clearer about
the distinction between self and other, which in turn results in more
sophisticated self and object representations, characterised by a broader
range of affects. As baby comes to know mother, he also comes to know
himself. As the baby’s ego becomes stronger, the need for splitting
should naturally decrease: good and bad self and object images become
more defined which means that good and bad can be allowed closer
together. The baby may still see his own hostility as coming from ‘out
there’ (how can something so bad come from inside?) but, over time,
there is increasing differentiation of self representations from object
representations as good and bad become less polarised. In this process,
the baby is more able to develop a sense of self and also more able to
understand other people. This whole process takes the baby to the age
of 18 months to three years, but should be complete before the now
growing child enters the next stage of development.
Right at the end of this stage, the difference between good and bad,
self and other, starts to make sense to the baby. Good and bad, at this point,
have become sufficiently integrated so that self representations and object
representations become more distinct. Rather than a stark separation, for
example, of good self and good other, there is the beginning of a sense that
both self and object representations are both good and bad. This paves the
way for the beginning of Stage Four.
These are:
1. Early sadistic superego forerunners
The child has previously internalised ‘bad’ object images of a very
sadistic, hostile, dangerous and persecutory nature. These are the
forerunners of the superego and the basis upon which the superego
starts to develop. Everybody has early sadistic superego forerunners,
but these can be particularly sadistic if development is accompanied
by internal or external frustration and aggression.
2. The ego’s ideal self and ideal object representations
The second component of the superego is the ego ideal. This is the
internal image of all the wonderfully good, magical and wishful
ideal images of self and object—the fantasies of a perfect, benign
self and other. The ego started off with very good and very bad
self- and object images and was required to integrate these into
something more realistic. The superego is in for a similar fate.
In Stage Four the superego begins to integrate the early sadistic
superego forerunners (the bad) and the ego ideal (the good) into
something that is more toned down and realistic.
3. Realistic demands and prohibitions of the parental figures
With the toning down of components one and two of the developing
superego, it becomes possible to internalise (rather than introject) the
parental figures. The third level of the superego structure consists of
internalised parental demands and prohibitions. These more realistic
identifications form a bridge between the sadistic superego forerun-
ners and the ego ideal.
Once the sadistic and ego-ideal forerunners are integrated with realis-
tic internalisations of the parents, it becomes possible for the child to move
from identification to ego identity. With a superego now in place, the ego
becomes even more proficient at synthesising good and bad, self and other,
resulting in a stronger sense of ego identity. The child is now ready to
consolidate these achievements in Stage Five.
<<
* Correspondence of behaviour and maturity, however, is characterised by
internal world. Sometimes our a closer match between the internal
behaviour or our reading of reality does and external world, as well as between
not actually correspond to reality. We our experiences and our behaviour. We
are misinterpreting, and this usually find ourselves less frequently grossly
happens because something from our misreading a situation or behaving in
internal world has coloured our reading a way that doesn’t fit with our sense
of the external world. This happens to of self. When this does happen, more
everybody because we cannot (and mature people are also more likely to
do not necessarily want to) remove be able to recognise the mismatch and
ourselves from our fantasies. Growing to have insight as to why it happened.
Pathological development
What happens when something goes wrong in development? How is it
that we develop symptoms that negatively impact upon our lives? As we
mentioned earlier, Kernberg believes that things may go wrong because
the child has imbalances in libido and/or aggression and/or because object
relationships are faulty. In either case, this could be caused by the child’s
constitution (for example the amount of aggression or envy the baby is
born with) or because of failures in parenting. Often, it is a combination of
the two that causes problems.
Kernberg believes that developmental failure alters the development
of our psychic structures and this is what leads to psychopathology. If Psychopathology.
something goes wrong then the result is not that we become arrested (get Psychological
stuck or fixated) at a particular developmental stage but rather that the malfunctioning, indicated
by abnormal, maladaptive
developmental failure results in the development of faulty structures,
behavior or mental activity.
and we therefore continue our development along a different line to the
normal developmental path. The earlier this happens, the more severely
our faulty structures influence our lives.
Kernberg therefore believes that severe forms of psychopathology
are a result of early developmental failure and less severe forms are a
result of later developmental failure. For example, failure in Stages One
or Two of development (before the sixth to eighth month of life) results in
psychotic kinds of psychopathology. Failure in Stage Three or early in
Stage Four results in severe kinds of character pathology, while failure
later in Stage Four or Stage Five results in less severe character pathology
or, more typically, neurotic conditions. The pathways of developmental
failure are summarised in the table on the next page.
168
1. Normal ‘Autism’ Affects of pleasure Beginnings of the No clear structures as Unable to take in enough ‘good’. Autistic psychosis.
and un-pleasure. introjection of yet: just basic units. Inability to establish a symbiotic
Not yet differentiated ‘good’ experiences. relationship with mother.
‘good’ versus ‘bad’
or self versus object.
Developmental Psychology
2. Normal Pleasure becomes Increasing Nucleus of ego forms Central problem: loss of Children: symbiotic
‘Symbiosis’ invested with libido introjection of from ‘good’ self and differentiation of ego boundaries: psychosis.
and un-pleasure ‘good’. Beginnings object images. • Extreme regression to ‘good’ Adults: psychotic
with aggression. of introjection of when trauma is present. schizophrenia or
Differentiation of ‘bad’, with attempts • Excessive activation of psychotic
‘good’ self and to expel. ‘bad’ in this stage results depression.
object images. in severe anxiety.
Un-pleasurable • ‘Good’ and ‘bad’ are not
experiences cluster sufficiently differentiated
into ‘bad’ internal while differentiation of ‘good’
images. self from ‘good’ object does
not take place.
3. Differentiation of Self and object Introjection becomes Ego boundaries Weak ego boundaries because Borderline
self from object become more more sophisticated become more firmly of severe frustrations prompt personality
representations differentiated. and is accompanied established. the person to defensively retreat organisation.
‘Good’ and ‘bad’ by defences such as to primitive ‘all good’ self and
are first split then splitting and No developed object representations.
later come closer projection. Splits superego, but cruel, Splitting increases instead
together so that gradually soften as punitive, harsh object of decreasing.
total self and object stage progresses. images constitute Identity diffusion.
representations Identification begins. superego forerunners. Differentiation is so defensively
are formed. There is also the strong that person is unable
beginnings of the ego to integrate self and object,
ideal, derived from ‘good’ and ‘bad’.
good internal objects.
Developmental Status of Basic Process of Structural Developmental Failure Pathological
Stage Units Internalisation Development Outcome
4. Integration of The child discovers Identification It becomes possible to Failure is located in: • Narcissistic
self and object that both self and (particularly during think of a structural id • Abnormal condensation of id, personality
representations; object representa- the Oedipal period). and superego. ego and superego structures (abnormal
higher-level tions can contain This then matures Superego is formed (narcissistic personality). condensation of
structures. both ‘good’ and into ego identity. from sadistic and ego • Conflicts between ego and psychic
‘bad’. Drive ideal forerunners in superego. structures).
derivatives are more combination with • Failure to integrate good and • Higher level
mature and now internalisations of bad superego forerunners— character
undergo repression. realistic parental interferes with ability to disorders (for
demands and develop value systems. example,
prohibitions. Potential to develop paranoid hysterical,
The ego strengthens characteristics. obsessive
as a result. • Integration of superego compulsive or
components is dominated by depressive-
aggression, resulting in masochistic
sadistic demands for characters).
perfection. Associated with • Neuroses.
neurosis.
5. Consolidation Mature self- Ego identity is Ego and superego Failures are normal human Everyday life!
of superego representations consolidated. consolidated and exist failures and do not have
and ego and object- in closer proximity to pathological outcomes.
integration. representations one another.
characterised by
mature affect and
appropriate
repression of
aggressive and
libidinal drive
derivatives.
K e r n b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f n o r m a l a n d p a t h o l o g i c a l d e v e l o p m e n t
169
are clustered around processes of splitting, in which ‘bad’ and ‘good’ are
kept separate from one another.
Kernberg stresses that the person does not repress a memory of
‘goodness’ whilst in the grip of ‘badness’: the person may be very conscious
of the change and yet this makes no difference to them. When the person
is in a particular ego state, it is as if the other ego state is not part of that
same person. Kernberg gives an example of someone who might alternate
between being sexually prudish and inhibited, completely split off from
her sexuality, and then very promiscuous and uninhibited. When you
point out the discrepancy, this causes great anxiety to the borderline
personality because it is so important for the person to keep these different
ego states separate. If we recall the example of the baby from Tsotsi, it is
almost as if isolated ego states of ‘good’ condensed milk alternate with
ego states of ‘bad’ condensed milk. They cannot be linked as part of a
continuous experience and each ego state, of opposite affect to the other
one, exists in a bubble. This is because of the predominance of early
defence mechanisms such as splitting, projection, primitive idealisation,
devaluation and omnipotence.
The intensity of the person’s affects, as well as their rapidly shifting
nature, is reinforced by a lack of impulse control and poor anxiety
tolerance. For this reason, people with borderline personality organisation
are particularly prone to ‘affect storms’ in which intense emotion sweeps
them up and is often directed towards those close to them. This is related
to their pathological object relations. The link between their image of self
and others in their internal worlds and the external world is tenuous, so
that it is difficult to realistically evaluate reality or to feel realistic empathy
for self or others. In more schizoid types of borderline organisations, it
often feels like their internal world is empty of real relationships, whereas
in borderline personalities, their intrapsychic life is ‘enacted in [their]
interpersonal patterns, very often replacing self awareness with driven,
repetitive behavior patterns’ (Kernberg, 2004:18). Instead of having rich
and real interpersonal relationships, their interpersonal relationships
become the repository of their internal conflicts. This is related to another
characteristic of borderline personality organisation: identity diffusion. Identity diffusion. An
People with borderline personality organisation often have an unstable unstable and fluctuating
and fluctuating sense of who they are which is heavily influenced by sense of who one is,
heavily influenced by
their affective state. Self-destructive behaviours are common, including
one’s affective state.
self-mutilation or harming of others. In general, people with borderline
personality organisation experience severely distorted interpersonal
relationships, lack of direction and uncertainty and often pathological
and chaotic sexual lives. All in all, borderline personality organisation
causes severe difficulties in the relationships of these people, characterised
as they are by the intense and overwhelming feelings of a dangerous
internal world.
What causes borderline personality organisation? Kernberg
links its emergence to the third stage of development, where self and
object images have become differentiated but where there are initially
strong splits between ‘good’ and ‘bad’. ‘Good’ and ‘bad’ have not yet
become integrated. Difficulties at this stage result in further pathological
172 Developmental Psychology
Borderline organisation
in the nursery: Tinker Bell
James (2007) undertakes an entertaining sometimes she is all bad. Fairies have to be
diagnosis of characters in popular children’s one thing or the other, at any one time. This
books. She assigns the diagnosis of is because they are so small and therefore
borderline personality disorder (BPD) to have room for only one feeling at a time. It is
Tinker Bell, the little fairy from Peter Pan who, common for those with BPD to feel this way
James suggests, ‘is unable to form stable as they have an unstable sense of self and can
relationships and fears Peter will abandon her interpret normal, time-limited separation from
for Wendy’ (2007:85). This is what she has to individuals they care about as rejection or a
say about Tinker Bell (2007:85-7): sign that they are in some way ‘bad’.
Tinker Bell finds it seemingly impossible
Tinker Bell lives in Neverland with Peter to control her anger. She has violent rages
Pan and the Lost Boys. She would like to be in response to things that would cause
Peter’s fairy, but this is not allowed as she only minor irritation in others. Wendy’s
is female and male humans cannot have a compassion makes Tinker Bell even more
fairy of the opposite sex. jealous and angry—perhaps because she is
Tinker Bell is in love with Peter and incapable of consistently feeling such warmth
becomes extremely jealous when he for another person.
interacts with Wendy. She finds it difficult to Suicide attempts and self-harm are common
control her emotions and is often verbally in those with BPD, as are reckless and impulsive
abusive and physically violent towards acts. When Peter’s medicine was poisoned by
Wendy. On one occasion she took this to Captain Hook, Tinker Bell drank it, knowing it
extremes when she tried to get one of the might kill her. It was only Peter’s intervention
Lost Boys to murder Wendy by shooting her that saved her. While it’s impossible to say
with an arrow. When Peter heard of this he whether Tinker Bell wanted to die, by drinking
banished Tinker Bell, but Wendy, forever the medicine she behaved in an irrational way.
thinking of the welfare of others, made sure It would initially appear her motive was to save
this was only a temporary measure. Peter; equally, it could be seen as a suicide
Sometimes Tinker Bell is all good and attempt or at least a cry for attention. >>
K e r n b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f n o r m a l a n d p a t h o l o g i c a l d e v e l o p m e n t 173
<<
Although this description is closer to the Her extremes of affect express themselves
DSM-IV definition of borderline personality in both her idealisation of Peter and her
disorder than to Kernberg’s description devaluation of Wendy. One might suggest
of borderline personality organisation, that Peter treads carefully, since he could
one can clearly see the features Kernberg turn into the ‘bad’ object at any moment.
describes. The predominance of splitting Her impulsive behaviour, both in matters of
processes and the intensity of affect, as well love and in matters of her own wellbeing
as the speed with which ‘good-Tinker Bell’ are clearly linked to poor anxiety tolerance
turns into ‘bad-Tinker Bell’ suggests that and impulse control. Her unstable sense
Tinker Bell has constitutional difficulties of self is matched by the stability of her
with aggression, perhaps combined with weakened ego, and she is never sure how
traumatic experiences in her early life. to get her needs met.
her as wonderful only as far as she made him look wonderful, and he would
probably control her in an aggressive manner to make sure she fulfilled
his projection of himself. Similarly, it could be suggested that Tinker Bell
does not love Peter Pan, but rather the image of herself that she imagines
reflected through her association with Peter Pan. Peter Pan is not a real
object for her but rather a part of her internal world.
both could be useful: Kohut reminds us that the narcissist is little and fragile
underneath all that bravado and reminds therapists to be empathic rather than
punitive. Kernberg reminds us that personality structures are complicated
and that we should not be afraid of confronting issues that are clearly not
working for patients. He helps us to avoid colluding with narcissists, even
when they are stroking our own egos. Some therapists therefore use both
approaches at different points in the therapy (see Consolini, 1999). However, it
is clear that the two theorists are incompatible in a number of important ways,
and that it may be too simple to suggest that they could be combined.
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➊ Think of a political or popular figure who you think may be narcissistic.
What evidence do you have to support this contention? What hypotheses
might you make regarding the ways in which this person developed?
General tasks
➊ Is Kernberg a bad cook or a master chef?
➋ Would you prefer to have Kohut or Kernberg as your therapist? Explain why.
Recommended readings
Kernberg, O (1984). Object relations theory and clinical psychoanalysis. Northvale, New Jersey
Jason Aronson Inc.
(Provides a good description of aspects of Kernberg’s theory, including his developmental theory.
There are also interesting chapters on his theory of romantic love.)
Kernberg, O (1985). Internal world and external reality. Northvale & London: Jason Aronson Inc.
(Kernberg discusses some of the key theorists to have influenced his own theory. There is also a
discussion of pathology and treatment, as well as an application to group psychology.)
Kernberg, O (1985). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale & London:
Jason Aronson Inc.
(A thorough discussion of character pathology, including development, diagnosis and treatment.)
Kernberg, O (1993). Severe Personality Disorders: Psychotherapeutic strategies. New Haven &
London: Yale University Press.
(Another good source for his theory of character pathology. Includes guidelines for interviewing
and explorations of cases of malignant narcissism.)
Kernberg, O (2004). Aggressivity, narcissism, and self-destructiveness in the psychotherapeutic
relationship. New Haven & London: Yale University Press.
(A recently written explication of his theory, this book provides a general overview as well as
picking up specific issues. The style of the book seems more engaging and better to read than
some of his earlier books.)
182 Developmental Psychology
Bacal, H A & Newman, K M (1990). Theories of object relations: Bridges to self psychology.
New York: Columbia University Press.
(Chapter 4 includes a discussion of Kernberg. Interesting to read a description of Kernberg
from the perspective of a self psychologist, ie someone who thinks Kohut is right.)
Fonagy, P & Target, M (2003). Psychoanalytic theories: Perspectives from developmental
psychopathology. London & Philadelphia: Whurr Publishers.
(Chapter 8 includes a thorough summary of Kernberg’s theory as well as incisive critiques.
The critiques include consideration of how contemporary developmental research might
support or challenge psychoanalytic theory.)
Glassman, M (1988). Kernberg and Kohut: ‘A test of competing psychoanalytic models of
narcissism’, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 36, 597-625.
(A novel attempt to test the theories of Kohut and Kernberg. The author suggests that, while both
theories have validity, Kohut’s theory can be seen as merely a special case of Kernberg’s more
comprehensive theory.)
Greenberg, J R & Mitchell, S A (1983). Object relations in psychoanalytic theory. Cambridge,
Mass. & London: Harvard University Press.
(Chapter 10 presents a thoughtful description and evaluation of Kernberg’s theory.
Also very good chapters on other psychoanalytic theorists.)
Klein, J (1987). Our need for others and its roots in infancy. London & New York: Routledge.
(This book really talks to the reader. Chapter 8 explores Kernberg’s basic units of development,
giving a wonderful sense of the development of affects.)
McWilliams, N (1994). Psychoanalytic diagnosis: Understanding personality structures in the
clinical process. New York: The Guidford Press.
(A general psychoanalytic book with a number of references to Kernberg, including a
comparison between Kernberg & Kohut.)
Russell, G A (1985). ‘Narcissism and the narcissistic personality disorder: A comparison of the
theories of Kernberg and Kohut’, British Journal of Medical Psychology, 58, 137-148.
(Provides a thorough comparison between the two theories. Fairly even-handed but with a slight
preference for Kohut, particularly in relation to treatment strategies.)
Saperstein, J & Gaines J (1978). ‘A commentary on the divergent views between Kernberg and
Kohut on the theory and treatment of narcissistic personality disorders’, International Review
of Psychoanalysis, 5, 413-423.
(A rather philosophical but interesting comparison between Kernberg and Kohut, suggesting that
they have fundamentally different ideas regarding the intentionality and agency of the person.)
Wallerstein, R S (1995). The talking cures: The psychoanalyses and the psychotherapies. New Haven &
London: Yale University Press.
(Chapter 20 examines narcissistic and borderline personality from the perspectives of both
Kernberg and Kohut. Raises some interesting meta-theoretical questions and comparisons
between the two theorists.)
9
Heinz Kohut:
Self psychology
Sally Swartz
Heinz Kohut
184 Developmental Psychology
Mr Z is a complex case, and has been the subject of a great deal of critical
debate, not least because of the academic sleight of hand involved in describ-
ing Kohut’s own experience as a patient, without acknowledging it as such
(Schoenewolf, 1990; Strozier, 2001). However, despite its history, it remains a
significant contribution to Kohut’s theory. Its importance lies in its dramatic
presentation of the effects on the same patient of a reorientation to narcis-
sistic phenomena such as an unrealistic sense of entitlement, demands for
attention, grandiosity, all of which were previously regarded as impediments
to successful analysis. The acceptance of such phenomena as a window onto
early childhood development formed the nucleus of self theory.
Selfobject experiences
An infant’s earliest experiences of selfhood are always mediated by inter-
actions with primary caregivers. For example, early experiences of well-
being might arise from being rocked or stroked when distressed. These first
encounters of relatedness are undifferentiated and are part of a seamless
merger with the caregiver. As physical and cognitive capacity expands,
merger states give way to an increasingly differentiated sense of self-as-
agent. Admiring responsiveness from caregivers over first smiles, learning
to grasp an object, or taking first steps are the mirror in which the self as
the centre of goal-directed successful behaviour is seen. In other words,
without an admiring and containing audience, the infant has no sustained
and cohesive way of experiencing selfhood. The essential link between
self and other is captured in the term ‘selfobject’. According to Bacal &
Newman, an object is a selfobject ‘when it is experienced intrapsychically
as providing functions in a relationship that evoke, maintain, or positively
affect the sense of self’ (Bacal & Newman 1990: 229). To put it another
way, Wolf describes any experience that ‘functions to evoke the structured
self (which manifests as an experience of selfhood) or to maintain the
continuity of such selfhood’ as a selfobject experience (Wolf, 1988:.52).
Selfobject experiences do not always involve a relationship with another
person. For example, cuddly toys, dummies and soft blankets may soothe
and sustain a sense of wellbeing in an infant. However, these objects,
188 Developmental Psychology
a warmly responsive and caring attitude towards the patient. For Kohut
(1982) empathy was first and foremost a means of collecting information
about the inner world of others. It is an act of imagination, an entry into
the essentially private experience of another through immersion in it.
Empathy provides the therapist with an affective grasp, a picture beyond
words, of the patient’s experience.
Kohut (1982) defined empathy as ‘vicarious introspection’ and the
terms are often used synonymously. However, Teicholz points out that
empathic responsiveness, through vicarious introspection, involves the
therapist in the activity of being in touch with his own internal world
as well as that of the patient. She suggests that it is the therapist’s ‘con-
tact with his own subjectivity’ that forms the basis for empathy with the
patient’s inner world (1999: 27). For the therapist to imagine what an
experience might be like involves not only an attuned responsiveness to
that patient’s unique history and needs, but also to a much broader field
of experiences encountered in many situations. Empathic attunement
involves the therapist in a process of using both the patient’s verbal and
non-verbal communications and her own field of experience, to adjust
and re-adjust his grasp of a many-layered and constantly shifting message.
Schwaber calls this oscillation between the subjectivities of therapist and
patient ‘our resonance of alikeness’ (1990: 239). She goes on to caution that
‘we must not confuse patient with us; we must know that his world is not
our world, her psychic reality not ours. It is our mutuality that allows us
to discover our individuality; the more we find our echoes of alikeness, the
more we enhance the possibility of locating our differences’ (1990: 239).
The resonant understanding arrived at through vicarious introspection
may then be communicated to the patient, and if it is sufficiently accurate,
it will fulfil a selfobject need. This however, is only the first step. As Basch
puts it, ‘Empathy leads to knowledge. By itself it neither prescribes nor
proscribes behavior any more than does the knowledge gained from logical
reasoning alone. What one does with the insight provided by empathic
understanding remains to be determined by the nature of the relationship
between the people involved and the purpose for which the empathic
capacity was engaged by its user in the first place’ (Basch, 1983: 123).
While the experience of being understood is in itself helpful to the patient,
this must be followed by interpretation of current circumstances and past
experience. Kohut makes this clear: ‘Analysis cures by giving expla-
nations intervention on the level of interpretation; not by understanding,
not by repeating and confirming what the patient feels and says, that’s only
the first step’ (Kohut, 1981: 532). The timing and content of interpretations
will depend upon the therapist’s understanding of the patient’s capacity to
experience them as helpful, and to absorb their implications as part of a
joint effort at strengthening the self.
For a very fragile patient at the onset of therapy, the experience of
having his inner world heard and understood may be all that is possible.
Interpretation of the origin and problems of that inner world will follow
later. Other patients may need to approach their inner world through an
intellectual appreciation of its contours: the full affective experience may
come later.
Heinz Kohut: Self psychology 193
difficulty with writing, because she seemed so calm and so sure of her
knowledge. By contrast, she experienced herself as panicky and ignorant,
not trusting that her research was either what it claimed to be, or a real
contribution to her field. In fact she was beginning to feel that when her
work was closely scrutinised by colleagues they would see that she had
made many mistakes and erroneous assumptions, and that she would be
discovered to be a fraud. At this stage in the therapy, a twinship transfer-
ence was also fleetingly apparent in assumptions she made about sharing
her therapist’s taste in clothes, books and movies, and reactions to political
events. This twinship served to consolidate her sense of being accepted
and her capacity to trust the therapy.
The therapy shifted dramatically after Zareena had a dream in which
she was giving a public lecture in front of many thousands of people. She
described her dream-self as articulate and powerful, commanding the
attention of the entire audience, until the microphone abruptly stopped
working and she could no longer be heard. People began to leave the
hall, angry and disappointed in her, and she woke up crying. This dream
marked the activation of her grandiose self in the therapy context, and
opened the way for the exploration of her intense need for, and fear of
admiration for her achievements. She began to recognise the extent to
which her colleagues’ high expectations of her and their frequently stated
respect for her work had simultaneously gratified and scared her. This
material coincided with the establishment of a mirror transference in which
Zareena tentatively explored her fantasy that she was somehow special
to her therapist. She felt sure that her therapist had changed her sched-
ule particularly to accommodate Zareena’s need to be seen early in the
morning. She also remarked that she was sure her therapist had never
seen anyone as difficult or as complicated as she was. Soon after this, the
experienced therapist made a comment in a session that Zareena found
distressingly mis-attuned, and she reacted angrily, accusing her therapist
of not paying sufficient attention to what she was saying. A pattern of
demands to be understood and closely attended to, quickly followed by a
sense that she was boring and not worth listening to, followed. The thera-
pist and Zareena explored this pattern as an effect of a childhood in which
her mother, as a result of the onset of a chronic illness, had abruptly with-
drawn from her when she was two years old. During the next few years,
Zareena had been constantly told to occupy herself, to be quiet, and not
to make demands. Her mother was preoccupied and frequently did not
listen to Zareena’s excited stories. She received some soothing and comfort
from the calm presence of her capable, clever but somewhat unemotional
father, and it was this selfobject relationship that had been recreated early
in the therapeutic relationship.
The therapist’s acceptance and understanding, first of Zareena’s ide-
alising needs, and later of her need for mirroring in the form of close at-
tention, followed by explanation of those needs in terms of the difficulties
she had experienced in her early relationship with her mother, began the
process of integrating her frightening exhibitionist and grandiose self, and
allowed her to begin to write regularly, and to consolidate her academic
standing. In other words, she was able to experience both her ambitions
Heinz Kohut: Self psychology 197
that he was excellent at it, and then soon becoming bored and
refusing to continue to apply himself. His parents indulged this, and
did not insist that he sustain effort in any direction. Their constant
and unrealistic admiration thus became divorced both from his own
initiative, and from visible achievement. At the same time, his rather
brash style made him unpopular at school. He became overweight
and shy, and recalled his adolescence as being a time of loneliness
and depression. He responded to his parents’ attempts to encourage
him in any way as unsettling intrusions. He sought therapy during
his first year at university, at a time when he was struggling to
decide on a course of study, and was feeling lonely and depressed.
The legacy of his parents’ unmediated admiration and over-
investment in him became apparent during a brief interaction with
his therapist. During a session he told his therapist that he had just
achieved a very high mark for an examination, but was unable to
feel pleased about it. In fact, he had noticed himself becoming very
anxious about the mark, feeling he would never be able to
reproduce the mark, and that somehow a mistake had been made.
His therapist greeted the news of his exam result with a warm
smile, and then spent the session exploring Joe’s anxiety about his
achievement. The following session, Joe came in looking defeated
and withdrawn. It eventually became clear that he had responded
strongly to his therapist’s smile about his exam achievement. On the
one hand, this had led to the fantasy that he was going to have a
brilliant academic career that would make his therapist proud of
him. On the other, he became aware of a sense of resentment, both
that he had to achieve in order to be valued, and that his therapist
had not taken his anxiety and apprehension seriously.
Shane points out that ‘the traditional analyst, if any good, also waits
for something better to come along before working interpretively on a
resistance or a defence’ (1985: 81). It is standard analytic technique not
to interpret the defence prematurely without linking it to the underlying
anxiety, and the hidden impulse or need (Malan, 1979). Thus, the
insistence of self psychologists on a basic orientation to the self state, and
the selfobject need against which the defence is mobilised is not in itself
a radical departure from traditional technique. However, the empathic
stance precludes the therapist from taking up a perspective that in any way
implies knowing more about the patient’s inner reality than the patient
himself understands. Moreover, according to self theorists, traditional
defence interpretation does not necessarily facilitate increasing awareness
of previously unavailable memories, needs or beliefs; it simply teaches
the patient to recognise the processes through which these are kept from
awareness. It is through trust in the therapist’s availability to explore what
is unknown in a non-judgemental manner that allows for the emergence of
hidden layers of the self-state (Lichtenberg, Lachmann & Fosshage, 1992).
Moreover, Kohut (1971) regarded the self-in-treatment as fundamentally
disposed to seek and use needed selfobject experiences. These needed
selfobject experiences may be for admiration and affirmation, or for the
soothing effect of merger with an idealised selfobject; in either case their
initial appearance is likely to be coloured by an expectation that they will not
be empathically understood. For example, a patient seeking affirmation,
but fearing that she will be met with criticism, may become defensively
boastful, or self-denigrating, or she may disguise her pride in an achievement
within a narrative filled with digression. For Kohut, the primary aim of
therapy was to maintain a focus on the primary selfobject need, and not to
be distracted by the defences mobilised to protect the injured self. Thus,
‘the therapist’s major effort must be concentrated on the task of keeping
the old needs mobilised. If he succeeds in this, then they will gradually—
and spontaneously—be transformed into normal self-assertiveness and
normal devotion to ideals’ (Kohut & Wolf, 1978: 423).
In self theory resistance is understood intersubjectively, as a reaction to
an experience of the therapist as mis-attuned, or to a fearful retreat from
exposing aspects of the self in the therapeutic situation. While defence is
primarily seen as intrapsychic, resistance always implicates a particular
analytic context.
practice are now discernible within the loose boundaries of the self
paradigm. Although the divisions between theoretical strands are fluid,
broadly they fall into three groups. There are those whose work has
largely confined itself to the elaboration of Kohut’s original contribu-
tions, particularly in the areas of self structure, selfobject experience,
and optimal provision. This strand of self theory is closely identified
with the work of Ernest Wolf (1988), Paul and Anna Ornstein (1995),
Morton and Estelle Shane (1988), Arnold Goldberg (1988; 1990), Paul
Tolpin (1988), and Marion Tolpin (1993). Two other strands in the
development of self theory are marked by more radical transformations
of Kohut’s work, and these shall be explored briefly below.
theory developed in contexts very different from those that shape African
realities, we run the risk of inadvertently colouring the patient’s commu-
nications with fundamental assumptions embedded in that theory. Our
challenge is to recast various branches of psychoanalytic theory in ways
that speak to local race, class and gender realities. This work has been
begun locally and internationally by theorists concerned with the white,
middle-class hetero-normativity of classical psychoanalytic approaches (see
for example Eagle & Watts, 2002; Lazarus & Kruger, 2004; Swartz, 2007b;
Straker, 2004; Suchet, 2004).
Self theory facilitates this endeavour by offering user-friendly techniques
for bridging the experiential gaps created by social divides. Empathic im-
mersion in particular involves, ‘respecting the patient’s communication as
providing the essential information needed to explore his inner life at that
moment without transposing time, place, or person in a manner guided by
theory’ (Lachmann & Lichtenberg, 1992:164). To guard against a theoretical
knowingness, there must be an explicit acknowledgement that the ‘patient’s
theory about himself or herself will usually be the final arbiter of what is valid
for him or her’ (Bacal, 1995:354).
Case study
<<
nightclubs and bars, he strongly dislikes the sister. During this time John was looked
idea of going into them alone. He feels that after by his mother’s sister. He has few
unless he arrives at a place with a group of memories of this time, but remembers
friends, he will seem to everyone like ‘a sitting at his aunt’s front door, crying, and
loser, someone who can’t make it’. This is saying, ‘I want mommy back’.
why he spends so much energy on making John’s schooling was uneventful. He
his arrangements. He wishes he had one or did well in his studies and played a lot
two close friends who he could rely on to of sport. He identifies his problems as
go out with him every evening and beginning in his second year at university,
weekend, but he says he has never had when he began to drink and smoke dagga
friendships like that. He also wishes he had regularly. Since leaving university, things
a girlfriend but as soon as an intimacy have gradually felt more and more out
develops between himself and a woman, of control. He feels that his life has lost
he feels himself withdrawing and becoming purpose and that he has no real goals. He
critical of her. He occasionally sleeps with occasionally has suicidal thoughts, but says
women, but seldom more than once. he never gets as far as making concrete
John comes from a small family that plans to kill himself.
lives in the Eastern Cape. His father is a At his first interview with a female
builder, and his mother has never worked therapist, John seems anxious and
outside the home, except for occasional distracted. He says he feels depressed about
employment in her church community, his life and wants things to change. He has
with which she is very involved. He has resolved to take control of his drinking and
one sister, two years younger than himself, drug use, and discusses his plans to apply
who is married, with two small children. for a job in a small computer firm in which
John has been the only member of the he will be expected to work long hours. He
family to have tertiary education. He feels that the challenge of the new job will
completed a BA degree, and has since help him to change.
done courses in computer technology. The therapist understands John’s attack
He describes his family as close-knit of rage and subsequent fragmentation
and private. He says his father is a tense as responsive to an injury to self, caused
and rather depressed man who worries by the failure of his friends to meet his
constantly about money and drinks needs. His inability to go out alone and
heavily on weekends. He was never his need to be seen as having friends is an
approachable or very involved with the indication of the fragility of his self-esteem.
upbringing of the children, but would She also notes the addictive quality of his
occasionally punish them severely for social activity, drinking and drug-use, all
small misdemeanours. His mother, a very attempts to self-soothe, in the absence of
religious woman, would often be out of well-established patterns of mature self-
the house in the evenings, at bible or regulation. She is aware that addictive forms
church meetings. The family seldom of self-soothing, and the sense of emptiness
entertained or saw friends, and the they defend against, are often linked to
children were discouraged from bringing early mirroring failures, and she wonders
friends home. The parents’ marriage is whether John’s mother was preoccupied
stable but unaffectionate. and even depressed when John was an
His mother was hospitalised for a infant. Certainly, depression and remoteness
severe depressive episode for three characterised his mother’s behaviour later in
months immediately after the birth of his his life. The trauma of separation from her
>>
Heinz Kohut: Self psychology 211
<<
during her hospitalisation for depression, remarks about her therapy room, the
coinciding with the birth of a sibling, may building in which she works, and sometimes
have compounded an already anxious therapy itself. She anticipates that he will
attachment pattern. It also seems that his soon suggest that he terminate. With this in
father was unavailable to meet either his mind, after another cancellation, she
mirroring or his idealising needs. It seems interprets the link between his withdrawal
unlikely that either parent was able to affirm from the therapy, and his difficulty in
his achievements. maintaining intimate relationships with
There is evidence of an effective women, and suggests that both of these
compensatory self-structure, seen in John’s might be his way of protecting himself from
school, university and sporting achievements. the possibility of being hurt. She goes on to
It is possible that affirmation in these settings say that he might anticipate being hurt
met some of John’s mirroring and idealising because when he was a young child his
needs, and also that twinship selfobject ties mother was not often emotionally available
with his peers compensated for the failing to him, as a result of her church work, her
archaic selfobject matrix. The therapist feels depression, and later, her involvement with
that the compensatory self structure will his young sister.
provide a foundation for the work that needs John reacts to this interpretation with
to take place in therapy. She anticipates that hurt and anger. He says he feels that the
the therapy will need to address early therapist does not understand the
misattunements in self- and mutual urgency of his work demands, and
regulation, that John will be very sensitive to under-estimates the difficulty he has in
perceived criticism, rejection and getting away. The therapy is important to
abandonment, and that he will react to these him, and he looks forward to coming. He
with either rage or withdrawal or both. says he cannot understand why she is
In the first phase of therapy, which lasts taking his absences so personally.
for approximately four months, John At this stage the therapist realises that
establishes himself in his new job, and as he although her interpretation might have
anticipated, this substantially changes his been accurate in some ways, it also
after-hours behaviour. He has very little free constituted an empathic failure for two
time. The close relationships with new reasons. Firstly, it was not attuned to the
colleagues in a demanding and creative element of primitive grandiosity mobilised
work situation have made him feel confident by John’s new work, and his need to have
and full of energy. Although he still his work achievements, including a sense
occasionally goes to nightclubs, he is of being indispensable, admired. Secondly,
drinking far less and seldom uses drugs. He although her interpretation had linked his
expresses his gratitude to his therapist, defensive withdrawal to the underlying
telling her that therapy has changed his life. injury to self, she realised that he had felt
In the next phase of the therapy, John criticised. She thought this might well be
begins to withdraw. He often cancels linked to her own feelings of irritation,
sessions at short notice, saying that there which probably gave the tone of her
has been an emergency at work. His interpretation a critical edge. She also
therapist is aware of some annoyance in thought that he might not yet be ready to
herself about this and the rather high- consciously face the dependency needs
handed way in which he brushes aside her activated by the therapy. She noted that
attempts to explore his absences. She is also her interpretation had not taken into
aware that he is beginning to make critical account the extent to which she was still
>>
212 Developmental Psychology
<<
an idealised selfobject for John. With these certain tasks at work. The therapist then
thoughts in mind, she commented that asks how it feels when he has to miss a
perhaps it was important to John that she therapy session. He says he sometimes feels
understands how much he is needed at relieved, because he doesn’t want to be
work when there is an urgent project going dependent on it, and knows he will have
on. He accepted this with relief and in the to stop at some point. This remark allows
interaction that followed he talked eagerly them to move into an exploration of his
about how great it feels to him to know pattern of defensive withdrawal at his
that his colleagues need him to perform own pace.
Specific tasks
➊ Self theory regards rage as the outcome of an injury to the self. In psychotherapy
therefore the focus tends to be on the injury that precipitated the rage, rather than on
the rage itself. This is in contrast to Kernberg’s approach, for example, and has major
implications for work with clients whose angry outbursts are a major part of their
presenting problem. Explore some of the advantages and disadvantages of this approach
to rage.
disclosure to their clients of their own thoughts, feelings and experience. Write an essay
in which you compare and contrast the classical analyst’s neutrality with the stance of
the intersubjective analyst. What are the strengths and weaknesses of each?
General tasks
➊ Primitive grandiosity—the sense of being ‘the king of the castle’—has an important
place in the development of self-esteem in all of us. Arrange to spend some time
observing young children who are exploring the limits of their physical skills in a play
group or adventure playground. How do they express their primitive grandiosity?
How do their caregivers respond? What happens when a moment of delight in an
achievement (such as reaching the top of a jungle gym) is not met with an attuned
(mirroring) response from the caregiver?
➋ Kohut’s theory of the bipolar self suggests that ambition (a development from primitive
grandiosity) is balanced with idealism (a development from the experience of feeling
contained in a soothing (omniscient) selfobject relationship). Spend some time thinking
about the different ways in which imbalance of ambition and idealism affects behaviour.
There are many examples in public life of men and women at one extreme or another:
how does this affect those around them? How are you affected by people who are very
idealistic or very ambitious?
➌ Give some thought to how Freud’s developmental theory applies to your own life.
Do you think that it helps explain much about you? If so, what and how? If you feel
that Freud’s theories are not able to explain significant aspects of your life, reflect on
why you think this is so.
Heinz Kohut: Self psychology 213
Recommended readings
On self theory:
The following readings give a thorough introduction to basic self theory. They offer insight into the ways in which
Kohut himself wrote for a wide audience of psychoanalytic scholars, and also show how his close collaborators
understood and developed his work. For readers just beginning to engage with self theory, this is the place to start.
I suggest reading Kohut’s major works, listed in the references, after these excellent easy-to-read articles. Strozier’s
biography will give you a rich insight into Kohut’s place in the psychoanalytic world, and his struggles with intro-
ducing his theory into the conservative classical psychoanalytic world.
Bacal, H (1995). ‘The essence of Kohut’s work and the progress of Self psychology’, Psychoanalytic
Dialogues, 5, 353-366.
Kohut, H (1979). ‘The two analyses of Mr Z’, International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60, 3-27.
(This is perhaps Kohut’s most famous paper. It is also known to be autobiographical, so apart from giving
an insight into what a self approach offers, it also reveals the essence of Kohut’s capacity to reflect on his
own experience.)
Kohut, H (1981) ‘On empathy’, In P. Ornstein (ed), The Search for the Self, vol 4. (1990). Madison:
International Universities Press, 525-535.
(Kohut addressed the concept of empathy repeatedly. It is central to self technique, and is also the most
misunderstood concept in the theory, partly because it is confused with the use of the term in everyday
conversation, often as a synonym for ‘sympathy’.)
Kohut, H (1982). ‘Introspection, empathy and the semi-circle of mental health’, International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 63, 395-407.
Ornstein, P & Ornstein, A (1995). ‘Some distinguishing features of Heinz Kohut’s Self psychology’,
Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 5, 353-366.
Strozier, C (2001). Heinz Kohut: The Making of a Psychoanalyst. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Wolf, E (1988). Treating the Self: Elements of Clinical Self Psychology. New York: Guilford.
(This is an excellent readable introduction to self theory. Ernest Wolf worked closely with Kohut,
and gives an accessible and thorough introduction to his ideas.)
10
Attachment theory
Lee Senior
Introduction
Attachment theory concerns early
caregiving relationships and
the way that these relationships
support the child’s subsequent
development. The nature of the
parent-child relationship during
early childhood is believed to be
one of the central causal factors
in personality development and
interpersonal functioning, as well as
having implications for psychopathology.
Mary Ainsworth
216 Developmental Psychology
Attachment behaviour
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON
Although Bowlby asserted that the infant- which is just enough for the newborn to
caregiver relationship builds over time, he be able to recognise general features of
did suggest that, from the outset, there the face, and the newborn prefers looking
are powerful biological forces directing at human faces above any other stimulus.
the mother and infant towards mutual Newborn infants are able to differentiate
attachment. Research with newborns has between their own mother and another
confirmed the existence of perceptual mother on the basis of smell. In one study,
biases in the newborn that facilitate the significantly more babies spent more time
mother–infant bond. Newborns just a turning towards their own mother’s breast
few hours old are able to recognise their pad than towards a clean breast pad at five
mothers’ voices. Newborns’ heart rates days of age, and by six days of age were
and patterns of sucking behaviour have showing a differential response between
been shown to change in response to their own mother’s breast pad and another
their mothers’ voices. The basis for this mother’s breast pad. Breast-fed infants
recognition has been thought to be rapidly learn their mother’s characteristic
prenatal experience in hearing the mother’s olfactory signature while sucking at her
voice. The newborn infant has a visual breasts and can subsequently recognise
accommodation of about 25 centimetres, her by that unique scent.
While the set goal of the attachment system was initially regarded as
physical proximity, Bowlby (1973) later refined his definition of this goal
and emphasised the importance of the infant’s belief that the attachment
figure would not only be physically present, but be emotionally accessible and
responsive if needed. Quality of care and sensitivity of the caregiver to the
infant’s signals became the central issues of infancy.
218 Developmental Psychology
mation of mental representations or internal working models of close Internal working models.
relationships. Internal working models comprise: Internal mental repre-
sentations, developed in
early close relationships
s self model—containing perceptions of one’s own worth and
between the self,
lovability, and attachment figure(s)
s other model—containing expectations regarding the essential and the environment.
goodness, trustworthiness, and dependability of important others
in one’s social world.
1 The mother (or father) and infant are introduced into a sparsely furnished room containing
toys for the infant and chairs for the mother and another adult.
3 An unfamiliar adult enters, sits quietly, then converses with mother and eventually
engages the infant in play.
5 Mother returns and tries to engage the infant in play. First reunion episode.
6 Mother exits once more, leaving child completely alone. Second separation episode.
7 Stranger returns.
Infant observation
Naturalistic infant observation as a Fleeing Nazi persecution, Esther Bick
method of observing the development of moved, via Switzerland, from her home
an infant in the natural setting of his and early work with children in Vienna, to
home with his mother, father or other settle in England as a refugee.
caregivers was pioneered by Esther Bick in She had a rich and fascinating life, and
1948 at the Tavistock Clinic in London. her early work with children as a teacher
>>
222 Developmental Psychology
<<
and caregiver stimulated a lifelong interest the Tavistock Clinic. It was introduced at
in the state of childhood and infancy, and the Institute of Psycho analysis in London
the very subtle, primitive and intense in 1960 as part of the course for first
experiences of the infantile state of mind. year students.
Esther Bick’s early work was with children Margaret Rustin (1999) describes how
in a war-time day nursery in Salford. Between the infant observation methodology that
1942 and 1945 she worked one day a week was central to the training, led naturally to
in a child guidance clinic in Leeds. She was further innovations such as work discussion.
accepted by the Institute of Psychoanalysis This took the skills learned in observation
for training in 1947. Her supervisors were into other settings like social work,
James Strachey and Melanie Klein. Klein teaching and residential work, and
supervised her second adult and her first stimulated the interest of professionals with
child cases, and she was greatly influenced different areas of interest. Other
by Klein’s ideas. It was at this point that she professionals such as teachers, social
ended her analysis with Strachey and entered workers, child-care workers and nurses
into analysis with Klein. have subsequently undertaken
Bick was invited by John Bowlby to work observations. Interest in the observational
as a child psychotherapist at the Tavistock method spread as visiting students that
Clinic and it is as a result of her teaching had trained at the Tavistock took their skills
there that she is recognised as one of the farther afield, with the result that infant
founders of child psychotherapy. In 1948, observation became established in many
Esther Bick, together with John Bowlby, countries and cities beyond London. The
started the first training in child International Journal of Infant Observation
psychotherapy at the Tavistock. Infant and its Applications published its first issue
observation was to form a central part of in 1997 and are now holding regular
this training. international conferences that focus on
Bick considered observation a good infant observation and its teaching.
introduction for those wishing to work with In South Africa, the first non-accredited
children, allowing them to learn about the infant observation groups were started in
early emotional development of babies, as the early 1990s, with seminar groups in
well as being able to experience and learn Cape Town, Johannesburg and Durban.
more about their own emotional responses These initial groups have developed into
to this intense and vulnerable time of a more formal seminars and are now offered
family’s life. Her own careful observations as part of a Tavistock-accredited diploma
of children she had worked with—starting course in communication with children,
with initially a behavioural observation under the auspices of the Institute for
which formed the basis of her doctoral Psychodynamic Child Psychotherapy. These
research—had stimulated her interest in the groups are overseen by visiting teachers
complexities of primitive mental states, an from the Tavistock, who also assess the
interest she was to deepen and explore in required written work.
her pioneering work on the function of skin (‘Surviving Space: Papers on infant
in early object relations. observation’ (2002) edited by Andrew
Following the initial introduction of Briggs offers a comprehensive introduction
infant observation as part of a non-clinical to the life and work of Esther Bick.)
year, it has been a central part of the
training course for child psychotherapists at Shayleen Peek
Attachment theory 223
Mental health
SECURE Resilience
Attachment
Early caregiving
STRESS
Temperament
INSECURE Vulnerability
Attachment
Psychopathology
Figure 10.1 The role of attachment in the development of psychopathology. As depicted above,
temperament and early caregiving experiences interact in shaping attachment security. Attachment
patterns, in turn, may act as protective factors or risk factors, when facing adversity. Securely attached
individuals tend to have better self-esteem and to engage their worlds with confidence, are more socially
competent, independent and adaptable, and have been found to be better problem solvers. Securely
attached individuals tend to be more resilient to the effects of subsequent life stress. Insecure attachment
has negative implications for personality development and is associated with interpersonal difficulties and
social isolation, poor self-esteem and difficulty regulating emotion. Insecurely attached individuals tend to
experience and respond to life stress more negatively, and are vulnerable to developing psychopathology.
Monotropism
Another question is whether attachment theory adequately accommodates
the use of multiple caregivers, a prevalent practice in some socio-cultural
contexts. Bowlby stressed the importance of a continuous relationship with
a primary caregiver for emotional health. While Bowlby stated that infants
Attachment theory 229
will, from the outset, form more than one attachment, often establishing Monotropism. Theory
a small hierarchy of attachments, he asserted that infants have a strong suggesting that although
tendency to prefer a principal attachment figure for comfort and security, a infants have a small
hierarchy of attachments,
phenomenon known as monotropism. Authors such as Jackson (1993) have
they evidence a bias for
conducted research in cultures in which a multiple caregiver arrangement is attaching themselves
normative and question whether monotropy adequately describes attachment especially to one figure
development in the multiple caregiver context. Van Ijzendoorn (1993) asserts, at the top of the hierarchy
however, that even in contexts where infants have multiple caregivers and and will tend to seek
develop multiple attachments, a special infant-mother bond exists and infants out this figure during
are likely to prefer proximity to their mothers in stressful situations. a crisis period.
While the distribution and manifestation of secure and insecure
attachment may differ among various cultural groups, the link between
early care and infant security appears to hold across cultural contexts.
Posada, Gao, Fang, Posada, Tascon, Schoelmerich, Sagi, Kondon-
Ikemaura, Ylaland & Synnevaag (1995) found evidence of secure base
phenomena in all seven of the cultures they studied. Posada, Jacobs,
Carbonell, Alzate, Bustamante & Arenas (1999) reported strong correlations
between maternal sensitivity and infant secure base behaviour in two
different samples of Columbian infants. This relationship was evident in
both a middle-class and a very poor sample, suggesting that the association
between maternal sensitivity and infant security is not specific to middle-
class sectors of the population. Waters & Cummings (2000) argue that
while attachment theory does assume that sensitivity to infant signals,
co-operative interaction, availability, and responsiveness are important in
the development of attachment, attachment theory does not assume that
these are equally prevalent in every culture. They believe that attachment
theory is able to accommodate differing cultural contexts.
1.5
Millions
0.5
0
1990 1995 2000 2005 2010
Projections (Source: Metropolitan Life Scenario 80)
Figure 10.2 Orphans due to AIDS in South Africa, 1990–2010 (UNAIDS, 1999).
Equally, children who are themselves infected with AIDS may have
attachment difficulties. Young children with specific chronic medical
conditions were shown to be less secure as infants than children in a matched
control group. Minde (1999) argues that not only will children with existing
attachment difficulties prior to contracting a serious medical condition be
further compromised in their attachment status by events associated with
illness, but also a chronic illness is likely to stress significantly the attachment
system of many well-attached children.
Insecure attachment puts these children at risk of developing behav-
ioural and social problems, poor self-esteem, and general adjustment
difficulties, particularly when there is no improvement in the subsequent
caregiving environment.
Clearly, loss of parents due to AIDS is likely further to disrupt
attachment for these children—the association between loss of a parent
during childhood and a marked degree of insecure attachment has been
well documented in the literature. From an attachment theory perspec-
tive, the long-term developmental impact of these early upsets and losses
Attachment theory 231
Attachment theory has also been valued for having the potential for
predictive power, based on findings of continuity between early quality of
attachment and later socio-emotional development (Harwood, 1995).
Bowlby took issue with the emphasis placed by psychoanalysis on the
child’s inner fantasy world, rather than on actual life events and inter-
actions. Bowlby chose to emphasise external reality and the importance
of environmental influence in psychological development. Consequently
he has been accused by the psychoanalysts of not taking sufficient account
of the child’s inner world (Holmes, 1995) or of distortions in the child’s
perceptions of the external world (Fonagy, 2000).
Bowlby was aware of the importance of societal supports for the
development of secure relationships in childhood and later life and had a
keen interest in social policy throughout his career (Bretherton, 1997). He
championed the rights to love and care of children who were innocent
victims of war and social disruption (Holmes, 1995). Many of the revolu-
tionary changes in child care of the post-war period were partly a result of
Bowlby’s efforts. These include allowing parents into hospital with their sick
children, the emphasis on foster rather than institutional care, and political
acceptance of the need for child-care benefits to be paid to mothers. Bowlby
has, however, been criticised for placing undue emphasis upon the role of
the mother, and the concept of maternal deprivation has led mothers to feel
anxious about the potentially damaging effects of even brief separations from
their children. These concerns appear to stem from a misunderstanding of
the concept of monotropism to mean that only the mother would do.
Specific tasks
➊ Skinner & Swartz (1989) conducted research in the Western Cape examining the
psychological sequelae for the preschool child of a parent’s detention as a political
prisoner. These authors found that children whose parents had been detained in the
1985-1986 State of Emergency in South Africa suffered a range of developmental and
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➋ Consider the AIDS epidemic in South Africa and the enormous number of children
orphaned by the disease. If, like Bowlby, you were called upon to comment on the
mental health implications of the problem and to make recommendations to the
government regarding a policy of intervention, what issues would you emphasise and
what recommendations would you make?
➌ Given the increasing number of women entering the workforce, a substantial number of
children are receiving non-parental care, including that offered by crèches or day care centres,
from a very early age. What are your feelings about this trend and its impact on attachment?
➍ Can you identify the common grounds, and the points of major theoretical divergence,
between attachment theory and psychoanalysis?
Attachment theory 233
Recommended readings
Bowlby J (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1): Attachment. London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby J (1973). Attachment and loss (Vol. 2): Separation: Anxiety and anger.London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby J (1980). Attachment and loss (Vol 3): Loss, sadness and depression. London: Hogarth Press.
(The original Bowlby trilogy presenting his theory of attachment. The first volume examines the nature
of the child’s ties to the mother, focusing on instinctive behaviour and the development of attachment
behaviour. The second volume focuses on separation and separation anxiety. The third volume looks at the
effects of a death in the family on the lives of children and adults. Bowlby’s writing is fairly accessible.)
Cassidy, J & Shaver, P R (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications.
New York: Guilford Press.
(A comprehensive volume, with contributions from the pre-eminent authorities in the field, presenting
the current state of knowledge about attachment, and the continuing development of attachment theory
and its clinical applications. Highly recommended for those with a particular interest in broadening their
knowledge within this field.)
(Peter Fonagy’s articles comparing attachment and psychoanalytic theory may be of interest. He has
written a chapter in Cassidy & Shaver’s Handbook of attachment, entitled ‘Psychoanalytic theory
from the viewpoint of attachment theory and research’. Alternatively, his paper entitled ‘Points of con-
vergence and divergence between psychoanalytic and attachment theories: Is psychoanalytic theory truly
different?’ may be downloaded at http://www.psycholucl.ac.uk/psychoanalysis/confpapers.html)
CHAPTER
11
Jung’s analytic theory of the
development of personality
Jacki Watts
Introduction
It is useful at the start to indicate that theorists who
follow Freudian assumptions, however unrecogn-
isable they might appear [for example Winnicott’s
theories], are seen to be part of the psychoanalytic
tradition. Theorists who follow Jung’s assump-
tions are seen to be part of an analytic tradition.
Jung’s theoretical break from Freud arose
from differences between the two men over the
role of spirituality in the human psychic world.
Jung was well aware of the significance of child-
hood for the formation of personality. In fact, he
maintained that Freud had clearly mapped out the
psychology and the appropriate intervention when
he theorised that the problems of the individual start with Carl Jung
problems of separating from the influence of home and parents (Jung, 1983).
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 235
However, we shall see that the issue of spirituality presents a major differ- Individuation. The innate
ence between Freud and Jung. We shall see that a spiritual dimension is and lifelong process of
important to an overall understanding of Jung’s view of humanity and his becoming an individual,
the move towards
conceptualisations of psychic motivation and growth.
wholeness through the
Central to Jung’s theory is the concept of the individuation of the self as integration of conscious
an innate process. Jung sees all of humankind as located within a shared ex- and unconscious aspects
perience of humanness and guided by a universal collective unconscious. of the personality.
He sees the individual, within this context, as having an innate need to seek
growth. Growth occurs as a result of the workings of the self, which has access
to the wisdom of our collective unconscious. This wisdom becomes accessible
‘We become what we
to us through our dreams and the symbols of our culture.
dream ... We achieve
Individuation is a lifelong process that is never completed. We start
in reality, in substance,
life with the self as the centre. The self is paradoxically both the centre and
only the pictures of the
the goal of life. Throughout development the self unites and organises our
imagination.’
experiences towards its goal of progressive integration and wholeness. The
Lawrence Durrell
soul is an important concept in Jung’s theory, and appears to be the same
concept as the self—Jung sees the self as the closest we come to an experience
of God. This short description indicates how spirituality is central in Jung’s
thinking. We shall go on to examine these ideas and illustrate his theory
of individuation.
Consciousness. The ego arises out of the id and acts as the mediator between the tensions
The range of experience of the id and the superego, which is the individual’s conscience. These
of which one is aware. three levels are distinct but are dynamically interrelated in the resolution
These experiences come
of drives. This means that Freud’s model is a conflict model, based on the
together to form a sense of
our continuity in time and
relationships (dynamics) between the three levels as they try to negotiate a
place. Consciousness is a process for psychic equilibrium around drive conflicts. Jung’s concept of the
reflection of the ego, structure of the psyche is quite different. His is not a conflict model, rather he
the ‘I’ of our being in sees the elements of the psyche as working together in a progressive journey
the world. of growth towards individuation.
Ego. The ego or
Consciousness
‘I’ is the centre of
consciousness, and as
Consciousness is the range of experience of which we are aware (Jung 1986).
such it constitutes the These experiences come together to form a sense of our continuity in time
contents of consciousness. and place. Consciousness is a reflection of our ego, the ‘I’ of our being in the
Consciousness is only world. The ego or I is the centre of consciousness, and as such it constitutes
aware of the experiences the contents of consciousness. Jung calls this the ego-complex. (We will
of the conscious ego. discuss his idea of complexes when we look at the relationship between the
Post-analytic thinking has
complexes, the personal unconscious and the archetypes.) In other words,
taken the concept further
and postulates, like Freud,
consciousness is only aware of the experiences of the conscious ego. Post-
that the ego is also partly analytic thinking has taken the concept further and postulates, like Freud,
unconscious. that the ego is also partly unconscious (Jung, 1983; 1986).
phylogenetically
constituted id, Jung’s
concept is ontogenetically
constituted and can,
in principle, be
made conscious.
Phylogenetic.
The development of
a species or group.
Marriage is an initiation rite symbolising separation from parents and the start of a
new procreative unity.
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 237
within its cultural taboos, Jung’s ontogenetic focus emphasises the indi- Collective unconscious.
vidual’s development. Jung’s focus is upon the personal context for what has An inherited part of the
psyche, which is shared
become unconscious.
by all humans and which
is responsible for the
Collective unconscious patterning of behaviours
The idea of the collective unconscious (Jung, 1983) is Jung’s unique con- that occurs across cultures
tribution. By this concept he postulates an inherited part of the psyche, which and races. Jung postulates
is shared by all humans. It is a patterning of behaviours that occurs across this concept in an attempt
cultures and races. He postulates this concept in an attempt to account for to account for the striking
structural similarities in
the striking structural similarities in behaviour and experience that we see
behaviour and experience
across all cultures. Regardless of where one is in the world, there are human that we see across
interactions and behaviours that are universally common. This suggests that all cultures.
there are organising principles that account for us being human. These are
revealed in events such as the initiation rituals of birth, entry into adult-
hood, marriage and death.
By definition then, the universality of the collective unconscious is of Consciousness
limitless extent and depth and possibility. We are born deeply unconscious
and this experience provides the matrix out of which consciousness, and
thus the ego, will develop. It is also the matrix to which we return in sleep. Preconscious
Sleep is the place of dreaming. Through the dream experience we are in complexes
contact with the wisdom of the collective unconscious. Jung’s positive view
of man’s unconscious sets him apart from Freud and other psychoanalytic
Collective unconscious
theorists. He postulates that forces within the collective unconscious guide
archetypes
a journey towards individuation. (These ideas will unfold as we go along.)
Dreams, for Jung, are thus the fundamental processes through which
personal growth occurs, as it is through our dreams that we have access
Figure 11.1 The three
to the guiding forces of archetypal energies—the wisdom and history of levels of the structure
our ancestors. of the psyche.
Symbols
The circle is the most universal of all symbols. The unbroken line
represents perfection, eternity, and the never-ending cycle of
creation, death and regeneration.
The Tai-chi is a circle enclosing the yin and yang, the two
opposing but complementary forces of creation. They are often
thought of as male and female. Around the Tai-chi are the Eight
Trigrams. In Chinese divination, these are the magic symbols
used to determine the balance of cosmic forces.
The uroborus is a symbol of a dragon or a snake swallowing its
tail. It is an ancient symbol that is found as far apart in the world
as West Africa and central America. With the beginning (the
mouth) and the end (the tail) at the same point, it represents an
eternal cycle of destruction and simultaneous regeneration.
238 Developmental Psychology
Complexes. Complexes
The personalised psychic The idea of complexes arose from Jung’s work with word association.
structures, found within He found that individuals displayed specific identifiable themes, unique
the personal unconscious,
to each individual, in their associations to stimulus words (Jung, 1986). The
which act as organising
principles. A complex
concept of the complex was Jung’s way of linking some fascinating observations.
clusters the effects of He noted that there appear to be universal themes common to all humankind,
several archetypal patterns such as loving, hating, desiring, raging, abandoning, engulfing, etc. He
with personal experience postulated that these strata of experiencing arose from a deeply unconscious
and affect (emotion). level of experience, which is the collective unconscious. Jung also noted that each
individual experienced these common themes in completely individual
ways. Therefore a system was in place that allowed these collective themes
to be expressed in an individual fashion. Thus he postulated that a personal
unconscious exists and that the individual nature of these universal themes is
expressed from this personal level of experiencing.
The personal unconscious is thus the centre from which the organisation
of the complexes occurs. The complexes co-ordinate the personal dimension
of being human with the collective dimensions of being human. Jung
postulated that personal experiences throughout life cluster around archetypal
energies. Events in childhood, and especially internal conflict, provide the
personal aspects of the way the archetypal core is organised and experienced
in adult life.
Thus we may define the complexes as the personalised psychic struc-
tures, found within the personal unconscious, which act as organising
principles. A complex clusters together the effects of several archetypal
patterns with personal experience and affect (emotion). Experience tends
to gather round the complexes and is mediated (organised and understood)
through them. Complexes are the personalised and integrated psychic
organising principles that emerge out of the relations between archetypal
potentialities and the personal experiences of the personal unconscious. As
a personalised and integrated psychic structure, a complex is an organising
function that makes sense of particular archetypal energies in the themes
of everyday life. Jung emphasises that the themes will accord with the
experiences of an individual, that is, within a particular life experience.
Thus a complex results from a blend of archetypal core experience and
human experience and it is through the complex that meaning is given to
certain archetypal energies.
This notion of the complexes links Jung with all other psychoanalytic
theorists who postulate that there are central internal organising princi-
ples that influence the way we are able to live our lives and interact with
others. For Freud, these were the influences of fixations, for Klein the
relative strengths of the paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions, and for
Winnicott the mediation of the false self.
In line with his positive view of humankind’s potential, Jung saw
growth as occurring through the process of bringing to consciousness the
archetypal energies that organise or structure our existence. As long as the
complexes remain unconscious they will exert unconscious influences on
our lives and we will remain unaware of them. Jung maintains that an
individual’s most habitual complexes are the ‘tender spots of the psyche,
which react most quickly to an external stimulus or disturbance’ (Jung,
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 239
1990: 28). Free association or word association (his old research tool) will
thus inevitably lead one to the ‘critical secret thoughts’ of the individual.
Archetypes
Jung (1986) initially used the term ‘primordial images’. His thinking included
the idea that in the development of the human mind we still retained the
fundamental biological, prehistoric and unconscious aspects of archaic
humanity. These archaic remnants would provide our most primitive
but essential foundations of how to be human. He termed these remnants
‘projected memories’. (Jung’s concept of memories is similar to Klein’s
notion of phantasy, that is, a reservoir of innate knowledge of the object of
the instincts, such as breasts, nipples, and suchlike.) He conceptualised this
archaic foundation as providing primordial or archaic tendencies to organise
ourselves as human. These tendencies, he postulated, are observable as inherent
tendencies to form representations or images or symbols about experience
and behaviour. Importantly, it is not the representations that are inherited.
What is inherited is a tendency to organise behaviour or experience in certain
ways. Thus these tendencies influence each culture to form representations
of what is essentially our primordial or archaic inheritance. The tendency
to represent leads to images that indicate universal modes of experience and
behaviour. He was later to term these influences archetypes. Archetypes.
Jung (1990) explains that, in archaic times, man did not reflect upon his The typical patterns of
symbols; symbols were lived and were unconsciously animated by their meaning. human adaptations,
which have been passed
Think of any symbol that permeates your culture. You will find that once you
on through phylogenetic
start to investigate the mythology of the symbol, its origin proves to be rooted history. The actual form
in some action or deed. Bushman cave paintings capture this dynamic, that is, in which the archetype
deeds [hunting] originally motivated by affect (emotion). Another example is will be realised or lived
that fear of a new guest at the supper table, who might stab you for your lands is dependent upon the
or your wife and child, led to the custom of placing all weapons on the right- environmental and
hand side of the eater. In that way any attempt to use the weapons would be historical contexts of the
individual. Thus, while the
visible. It is now customary in Western culture to place the table knife on the
archetypes have unlimited
right hand side. universal potentialities,
The closer the image is to an archetypal core, the greater will be the how they come to be
emotional impact. Perhaps one of the most emotionally charged archetypal lived in an individual's life
behaviours is contained within the ceremonies of a wedding. Clear symbolic is always defined by the
acts that are linked to the fears and anxieties of a tribe are revealed in the culture and the particulars
ceremonies. For example, the couple needs to be protected in their separation of that individual's
personal life history.
from the parents, hence the giving of gifts. The vulnerability of a woman
in defending herself is symbolised in the vows exchanged in traditional
marriages where the husband is expected to look after her. The woman
must protect the working of the marriage for the peaceful continuance of the
tribe; she thus makes vows of obedience and commitment to her husband
and his will. Another rich example is death and its elaborate ceremonies that
attempt to allay our fears of dying and confirm our hope in the afterlife. Here
we see formalised partying, eating and giving of gifts either from the dead
person, as in bequests, or gifts to the dead as in the laying of flowers or fun-
eral wreaths. All these actions symbolise our hopes and fears about dying.
By 1919, Jung was using the term archetype to designate the inherited,
unknowable nucleus, ‘a system of readiness for action’ (1983). The arche-
240 Developmental Psychology
types are the typical patterns of human adaptations that have been passed on
through phylogenetic history. The actual form in which the archetype will be
realised or lived is dependent upon the environmental and historical contexts
of the individuals. Thus, while the archetypes have unlimited universal
potentialities, how they come to be lived in an individual’s life is always
structured through the culture and the particulars of that individual’s personal
life history. The complex bridges the universal and the particular. Jung (1990)
gives the example of two men he was seeing as patients. One of them, a shy
young man, dreamed of jumping over a wide water channel. Other men in his
dream fell into the water. The other, a convalescent old man, who was proving
to be a very difficult patient, had a very similar dream of jumping over a water
channel. Jung observes that the dream of the young man encourages him in
the belief that he can do something that he consciously thinks he cannot.
The dream of the old man, on the other hand, offers a different wisdom. The
dream indicates that what he is doing is foolhardy in relation to his being an
old man and that he is not acting in ways that are appropriate. It signifies that
being a difficult patient is foolhardy.
In studying myths and folklore you will Anansi, the spider trickster who, while his
see that the same themes occur across all behaviour is not always moral, will usually
cultures. Myth is thought to capture manage to outwit his opponents
humanity’s attempts to tell the stories of (Dagomba people from West Africa); the
its development. These stories present Aboriginal myth of Malu, the red Kangaroo
evolutionary themes about how people who left caves, rocks and creeks to mark
have managed to master and engage with his journey across Australia; the Chinese
their surroundings. In this they convey Hare who mixes the elixir of life with his
the archetypal themes that have patterned mortar and pestle, and the North
humanity’s development. American tale of Coyote who stole fire
One such theme is the construction of from the gods to give it to humans.
the various myths about heroes or Shakespeare’s plays also contain the
tricksters. They are usually stories about trickster in such characters as the clown
animals that have heroic characteristics but in Twelfth Night, Trinculo the jester in
also tend to be cunning tricksters. Some The Tempest and Touchstone the clown
examples of tricksters include the tale of in As You Like It. These characters, while
the Praying Mantis who brought fire to acting as fools or jesters, articulate the
humanity (South West Africa); the tale of wisdom or moral of the play.
A very important distinction that Jung (1983) makes is that there is a sharp
distinction between the archetype and the archetypal image. The archetype, as
such, is never knowable. It is part of the collective unconscious and offers
only a tendency with regard to the organising of experience. The archetypal
image, however, is knowable. It is the means whereby the tendency is
given expression. This expression is made manifest through such media as
ceremonies, behaviours, symbols, etc. An example of such an archetypal
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 241
Hierarchy of archetypes
Since archetypes provide the organising tendencies for being human, we
can review the archetypal images by examining the developmental hier-
archy of archetypal influences. The infant, initially, is absorbed within the
total and unconscious self archetype, part of the collective unconscious. The
self is thus the very centre of being. Out of this centre arises the ego archetype.
The ego slowly develops out of the collective unconscious, that is, from
the totality of the self. The ego emerges as fragments from the collective
unconscious and gradually coheres with ‘good-enough’ interpersonal
experiences. Such coherence entails the integration of psychic functions,
the personalisation of archetypal images and themes, and the development
of a personal identity and boundary. The ego comes to contain what is
conscious and what is concerned with living with others in a shared world.
The ego presides over the processes of becoming conscious of thinking,
knowing, and being able to live autonomously. The ego allows desire
and ambition to be fulfilled and takes us into a world where we share the
collective ideals of our culture (Jung, 1983; 1986; 1990).
In childhood, archetypal images are related to in projected form and in a Projection. The psychical
concrete way. The child is unable to see the mother in her unique personality. process whereby qualities
To the child, the mother is the archetypal mother; she is the incarnate of all of the self are seen as
embodied in another.
that mothers ought to be. Much of the work in psychotherapy deals with the
difference between the client’s relations to images of the archetypal mother,
and their actual relations to the real mother. It is sometimes the case that
great disappointment in the mother is related to fantasies of the archetypal
mother, who is experienced as all good or all bad. Once these archetypal
influences have been worked through, the client is able to appreciate that the
mother, while fallible, was still a loving mother.
Persona
Through development, the child must learn to conform to societal Persona. This archetype
norms. The persona archetype is the social mask that develops in order to is the social mask that
enable the containment of strong, primitive emotions and impulses. develops to enable the
containment of strong,
There is a need for the persona archetype, as it allows one to adapt to the
primitive emotions
demands and cultural needs of one’s society and culture. The danger of and impulses.
the persona is that one can become too closely identified with the mask and act
as if it were the real self. In such an instance one has become inflated with Shadow. The archetype
the persona and may lose the capacity to integrate the wholeness of one’s self. that contains whatever
(Inflation will be discussed when we look at pathology.) is unacceptable to one’s
culture and also to
Shadow one’s self, such as moral
issues like greed, envy,
In our attempt to adapt to our cultural norms, we also develop the shadow
prejudice and racism.
archetype. The shadow contains whatever is unacceptable to our culture and It also contains repressed
also to our self. These unacceptable aspects often entail moral issues such as aspects of instinctual life
greed, envy, prejudice and racism. The shadow also often contains repressed such as sexuality
aspects of instinctual life that are intended for survival, such as sexuality and aggression.
242 Developmental Psychology
and aggression. These are perhaps aspects about which we are ashamed
and which we attempt to hide, both from others and ourselves. As shadow
aspects we often project them onto others, seeing others as the embodiment
of those aspects that are too difficult for us to face in our selves. Life is full
of examples of people who project their shadow onto others. Prejudice is a
process whereby aspects repressed within one’s own sense of self are projected
onto another race or culture. You can come to know about your own shadow
by taking note of any strongly held negative belief about other people. These
negative beliefs are the basis of your own shadow, which you have projected
onto others. The therapeutic process attempts to move away from judgemen-
tal attitudes towards the shadow and to try to integrate these qualities into
the self. Integration of one’s shadow is an aspect of the individuation process.
A dreamer dreams that she has been abandoned by their families and that they
cornered in a cul-de-sac by a group of must suffer great despair. In relation to her
street children. Her immediate thoughts are own life, she too had shown remarkable
that such children are dirty and are thieves. resilience in overcoming severe childhood
Further exploration reveals that she also trauma. She had split off and projected
sees that they show remarkable resilience onto the street children her own abandon-
in surviving their harsh lives. What finally ment and despair. These were shadow
emerges in her amplifications on the street emotions, which she had been too afraid to
children is that they have been fatally embrace in response to her own situation.
Jung identified only a small number of often the subject of stories, myths and art.
archetypes. Post-Jungian writers have Read the myths of a number of cultures
identified many more, such as the divine and you will find that they contain stories
child, the eternal youth, the Oedipal child, that have the same themes, such as
the earth mother, the hero, death, birth creation, keepers of light and darkness,
and rebirth, the wise old man or woman fertility stories, elements of nature, heroes
and the wounded healer. These archetypes and tricksters, death and rebirth. These
are universal potentialities but their realisa- myths chronicle humankind’s continual
tion occurs within a cultural context and is quest to understand the organising
further differentiated by the particulars of principles that govern the condition of
family life and experience. These images are being human or the meanings of life.
244 Developmental Psychology
Psychic activity
Archetypes express built-in polarities between positive and negative
aspects of experience and emotion. Consider for example the persona
and the shadow, the animus and the anima. The impact of the archetypal
image depends to a great extent on the way in which environmental
experience blends with or mediates the archetypal imagery. In ordinary
development, such mediation will prevent too extreme a concentration at
one or other end of the polar continuum—either inflation or alienation. Inflation.
(We shall examine these conditions presently.) If real experience rein- Inflation develops when
forces either extreme, the individual is hooked onto only one end of the the environment has
reinforced one extreme of
range of archetypal possibilities. An example of this might be the belief
the archetype. This is the
that the persona is the real self. By contrast, the resolution of opposites situation in which the ego
brings about individuation. An example of such resolution would be the has become too identified
integration of the shadow and persona, or of the animus and anima. Jung with the archetypal
(1986) maintains that difficult times in one’s life are often turning points energy; the identification
when one is working out archetypal activity towards a resolution. is of such an extent that
the ego is engulfed by
the archetype.
Individuation and the self
Individuation is the process of becoming an individual, the move towards Alienation. The situation
wholeness through the integration of conscious and unconscious aspects of in which the ego becomes
the personality. Conflict is an inevitable condition of the polarities of life. detached from the life-
Individuation occurs through acceptance and integration of these polarities, giving archetypal energies
such as the shadow and the persona, and the two contra-sexual aspects. and life loses its meaning.
Individuation for Jung was not an elimination of conflict, but rather
an increased consciousness of conflict and its potential. The self becomes an
image of a more complete person, as well as being the goal of life. This is the
attaining or realising of one’s self (Jung, 1983; 1986; 1990).
In the process of individuation, the ego ideal is given up in favour of
self-acceptance. Remember that the ego is the centre of consciousness. Thus
the ego ideal is taken up with the ego archetype. It is concerned with the
outer aspirations of the ego, with collective aspirations. These could be aspects
such as ambition, achievement, family, and friends. With individuation, there
is withdrawal from these collective foci of the ego. The collective ego norms
are replaced with the self as an inner guide.
There are basically two stages of adult development—the morning and
afternoon of life. The transition from one to the other is often, but not always,
heralded by a mid-life crisis. In the morning of one’s life the focus is on the de-
velopment of the ego. The ego, the ‘I’, will establish a social identity (persona)
and forge a place in the world, independent of parents. The ego has primacy
and functions with a consciousness immersed in the collective consciousness
of social conformity. The ego is thus taken up with collective norms. The
morning of one’s life lasts until about the mid-forties or fifties (Jung, 1983).
In the afternoon of life there is a move towards the realisation of the self,
and the self becomes the new centre of psychological life. Individuation
is often associated with this latter stage of life, with realisation of the self as
its goal. Just as the depth of the collective unconscious is unfathomable and
246 Developmental Psychology
opens up the triumph of rationality over instinct and the child’s appro-
priation of the human cultural order.
The development of the persona is the compromise that links identity
with the needs of society. Where there is too close an identification, there is
an immersion in the anonymity of collective life. True individuality and
consciousness is founded upon dialogue with the self. Severe and persistent
identification with the persona leads to crisis. Jung postulates that this
crisis will tend to happen in mid-life when the individual realises that who
they are in a world of collective consciousness is no longer fulfilling. Many
individuals attempt to solve the crisis by changing their circumstances. This,
however, does not solve the problem. It is just more of the same. Jung suggests
that the crisis is actually the call to give up this immersion in a life of social
consciousness—that is, the collective norms of the ego—and embark upon a
journey of self-discovery. This is essentially a journey of aloneness, where one
finds values and meanings that are personal and individual. The essence of
these values and meanings, Jung postulates, will be spiritual in nature. This is
based upon the notion that the self is indistinguishable from the God-image
and the goal of individuation is spiritual wholeness.
Types of pathology
There are several possibilities for pathology, each of which reflects imbalance
or lack of integration in the relationship to archetypal energy (Jung, 1983).
Inflation
Where the environment has reinforced one extreme of the archetype,
inflation develops. This means that the ego has become too identified with
the archetypal energy. The identification can be so complete that the ego
is engulfed by the archetype. Psychosis would reflect this state, in which the
ego is engulfed and fragmented, and at the mercy of archetypal powers.
In psychosis the individual is so inflated with the archetype that contact with
reality is lost. This is the condition seen in schizophrenia.
In less extreme forms of inflation, the person acts out the archetypal
energies. These energies define the person and preclude the possibility of
integrating the total person. An example that we may find in everyday life
is the woman who has no life other than in her children. She is inflated
with the mother archetype and is not connected to her larger personality or self.
Another common example is the man who is a philanderer; he is unable to
make commitments and may be inflated with the peura eterna archetype—the
youth who never grows up. Inflation with the persona would indicate that
Participation mystique.
The situation in which the
one has become identified with the persona as the whole of the personality.
ego becomes fused with
archetypal reality. The Participation mystique
ego struggles to find an Jung’s ideas offer a helpful approach to certain pathological phenomena such
adequate differentiation as cult involvement and cult leaders. He terms this pathology participation
between inner and outer mystique. Participation mystique reflects a situation where the ego becomes
reality. The conscious
fused with archetypal reality. Unlike inflation, where the ego is potentially
capacities of the ego
remain but they are fused
engulfed and disintegrated by the archetype, in participation mystique the ego
with the unconscious struggles to find an adequate differentiation between inner and outer real-
realities of the archetype. ity. The ego becomes fused with the archetype. Thus the conscious capacities
of the ego remain but they are fused with the unconscious realities of the ar-
Neurosis. The concept of chetype. An example of participation mystique is a cult leader who becomes
neurosis, for Jung, refers fused with (or participates in) a myth. The theme of the myth is usually about
to the situation in which
a saviour or a great leader. The cult leader becomes fused with this myth
the ego misguidedly
but heroically fights
and lives as if he or she were this mythical leader.
the changes that the
archetypes are trying Neurosis
to bring about within the Freud sees neurosis as the ego’s compromise between the strivings of the
total personality structure. id and the censorship of the superego. As the id presses for discharge,
Through the ego’s anxiety signals the ego into action. When such action does not resolve
resistance of
the problem, the symptom then becomes the disguised means whereby
the changes that are
indicated, the potentialities
the ego can satisfy both the id and the superego. The symptom thus
of the archetypal core are holds the meaning of the conflict.
prevented from Jung’s (1986) formulation is similar to Freud’s. Jung also saw the
being realised. symptom as holding the secret of cure in its meaning. However, Jung’s
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 249
Alienation
In alienation the ego becomes detached from the life-giving archetypal
energies and life loses its meaning. Jung believed that the plight of modern
society is that we have become alienated from our collective unconscious
and our spiritual domain. Suicidal people have become alienated from
archetypal energies. They no longer feel that the patterns and organising
symbols of life have any meaning. Adolescence is a time of potential
alienation where adolescents question the received wisdom of the culture
and society. Adolescence is the period of life when most suicides occur.
The integrating of despair and alienation offers a renewed vigour and
optimism about life as a young adult.
Psychological types
Jung (1983) postulates that each individual can be characterised as
either primarily outwardly or inwardly oriented. Extroversion and
introversion, respectively, are his terms for these attitudes towards
stimulation (or the world). Clearly it is rare for an individual to be
only extroverted or only introverted. However, individuals may tend
towards one or other habitual attitude.
Rational functions
The functions of thinking and feeling are rational functions that arise from
the level of consciousness.
Thinking
Where thinking is the dominant function, we may see the extroverted
thinker as characterised by:
• a tendency to value ideas over facts, the inner over the outer,
• a quality of being at odds with the world, especially since they
often pay little attention to nurturing their general relationship
to the outside world.
Feeling
Jung has an interesting understanding of feeling. Feeling is not an
emotion; rather it is a function by which values are weighed, accepted, or
refused. (Any function can lead over to emotion, which is then a conse-
quence, but not the function.) Thus to say, ‘it feels right’ is to be using the
feeling function.
Where feeling is the dominant function, we may see that the extroverted
type is characterised by:
Irrational functions
The functions of sensation and intuition are the irrational functions as they
arise from the level of the unconscious.
Sensation
Where sensation is the dominant function, we may see the sensation type. These
are those individuals who experience things as they are, without imaginative
trimmings. What counts for the sensation type is the strength and pleasure of
the sensation. They can be mistaken for rational because of their calm, even
phlegmatic natures. They are frequently easy and jolly with a great capacity
for enjoyment. The danger lies in overvaluation of the senses. There is the
danger of degenerating into unscrupulous and hedonistic pleasure-seekers.
For extroverted sensation types, the object is important. For introverted
sensation types, the experience is important.
Intuition
Intuition is a perception of reality that arises from the unconscious. Extro-
verted intuition types are characterised by the attitude that all things are
possible, and by an intense dislike for what is known, familiar, safe and well
established. Extroverted intuitive types exhibit little respect for customs and
typically show ruthlessness towards the feelings of others when they feel
they are ‘onto something’. They are the classic ‘sowers of seeds’ but not the
reapers; it is others who benefit from their energy and enterprise.
In Jungian therapy, which deals extensively favourable conditions they may enter into
with dreams and fantasies, a dialogue is the individuation process: a lengthy series
set up between the conscious mind and of psychological transformations
the contents of the unconscious. Patients culminating in the integration of opposite
are made aware of both the personal and tendencies and functions and the achieve-
collective (archetypal) meanings inherent ment of personal wholeness.
in their symptoms and difficulties. Under
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 253
Dream-work, or amplification
Jung conceptualises dreams as the psychic seat of wisdom. The dream
is as much concerned with the direction in which we are going, as with
the experiences from which we have come. The implication is that the
unconscious is creative and that it contains a moral component. Dream
symbols may have rich symbolic significance but this significance,
Amplification. The process Jung cautions, is personal. Dream-work, or amplification, is required
whereby associations are to access the personal significance of a dream symbol. Amplification
made to each of the dream
is a process of associations that are made to each of the dream images
images so as to establish
the context of the dream
themselves. This process establishes, with great care, the context of the
images. Amplification dream images. Amplification broadens and enriches the dream con-
broadens and enriches tent with analogous images and personal associations of the dreamer
the dream content with and the therapist. Together they move towards establishing a correct
analogous images and interpretation which the dreamer finds convincing. Amplification is
personal associations conducted first on the personal level. Only when the personal concerns
of the dreamer and
and conflicts have been redressed, does it move to an archetypal focus.
the therapist.
A knowledge of myths and symbolism is necessary for the therapist
when working at the archetypal level of understanding.
Jung maintains that free association, as used
B C by Freud, will always reveal the complexes.
However, it will not necessarily reveal those
connected to the dream image. Therefore it
is necessary to stay close to the dream images
themselves.
A THEMES D Another source of amplification is a
series of dreams. Over a series we are able
to recognise important contents and themes.
Every interpretation is only an hypothesis, but
F E
a degree of relative certainty can be achieved
when working with a series of dreams where
Figure 11.2 Conceptualising a series of dreams the dream continues like a monologue. The
around a central point. series can be conceptualised around the theme
as a central point.
When the central point is revealed to consciousness, a new point
arises. It is therefore important to keep dream books for recording
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 255
1) What is the time and place of the setting? What thoughts might
one have about why the dream needed to be placed in this setting?
What wisdom is there in this setting? Are things still fantastical or
‘All human beings are
are they more reality-based?
also dream beings.
2) Who are the dramatis personae? What archetypal figures occupy the Dreaming ties all
dream? What does this tell you about the psychic tensions and con- mankind together ’
flicts of the dreamer? Jack Kerouac
3) What are the characteristics of the archetypal figures? These are the
amplifications of the archetypes. Are they inflated, alienated, etc?
4) What is the ‘plot’ or the ‘problem’ of the dream? Here the unconscious
gives the archetype or complex a visible form that can be worked
upon by the dramatis personae. How does this problem relate to
the conscious problems of the dreamer? Why has the dream offered
this ‘problem’?
5) What is the quality of the ‘play’, the emotional tones, the weaving
of the plot, the intensification of the events? Is the dream filled with
affect (emotional appeal) or is it flat? Why does the ‘problem’ need
to be approached in emotional ways or non-emotional ways?
256 Developmental Psychology
A dream analysis
‘I am walking through a magical land. Everyone is ful.’ This is confirmation of the identification
smiling and happy and there are many children with the Great Mother archetype and the
running about. I feel as if they are all my children anima. ‘Suddenly I see a huge lion, it is a
and that I am the perfect mother. Everything is male lion and it wants to eat me.’ Masculine
beautiful. Suddenly I see a huge lion, it is a male power and aggression are symbolised by the
lion and it wants to eat me. I run away, but it lion. Instinctual and shadow aspects are
chases me. I realise that I have nowhere to run primitive as seen in the symbol of an animal.
and that it will catch me. I stop and turn. I feel This indicates the lack of current integration
terrified but look at the lion. Suddenly the lion of the animus, that is, the animus is still at
turns into a horse, a beautiful white horse. I get the instinctual level. Lack of integration is
on the horse and we gallop over the land. I find indicated in the desire to flee and in the fear
myself at the office having a meeting with A, a of the lion, the shadow aspect.
work colleague. He usually frightens me but we ‘I run away, but it chases me.’ This illus-
are discussing things and I am very strong.’ trates the dream’s purposive function—if
integration is run away from, it will follow
Overarching understanding: and be destructive. ‘I realise that I have
This is a compensatory and purposive nowhere to run and that it will catch me.’
dream. It gives the woman insight into her Here is realisation and acceptance of the
unbalanced identification with the feminine need for integration.
(purposive aspect) and shows her what ‘I stop and turn. I feel terrified but look at
might be possible were she to integrate the the lion.’ A moment of possible integration
masculine (compensatory aspect). is attempted, even in fear. The dream’s
‘I am walking through a magical land.’ purposive function is showing what might
This is not based in the real world; it is a be possible. ‘Suddenly the lion turns into
fantasy component. ‘Everyone is smiling a horse, a beautiful white horse.’
and happy and there are many children A transformation occurs with the facing of
running about.’ This evokes a scene of the animus. The primitive and fearsome lion
motherhood and child bearing. It suggest transforms into a horse, which is symbolic in
a great mother archetype and identifica- evolutionary terms—it has assisted human-
tion with the feminine [anima]. kind to evolve and develop in ways not
‘I feel as if they are all my children and I possible before it arrived. White indicates
am the perfect mother. Everything is beauti- purity and the possibility of transformation.
>>
J u n g ’s a n a l y t i c t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f p e r s o n a l i t y 257
<<
White is symbolic of new beginnings. things and I am very strong.’ This is the
‘I get on the the horse and we gallop compensatory function of the dream
over the land.’ Newfound possibilities of showing how, when integration of the
covering new terrain and psychic ground. animus occurs, there is the possibility of
‘I find myself at the office having a being strong and equal in relation to
meeting with A, a work colleague. He masculine energies, which are no longer
usually frightens me but we are discussing experienced as frightening.
Critiques of Jung
Clearly the same kinds of critiques that have been applied to Freud have
been levelled at Jung’s theories, though his theories have perhaps drawn
more criticism because of their esoteric or spiritual postulations. The
arguments that have been raised against Freudian conceptualisation
could be applied to Jung. Look at the critiques of Freud (Chapter 3) and
apply them to thinking about Jung’s theories.
Psyche
Complexes Archetypes
Process of individuation
Integration of opposites
Hierarchy of archetypes
Dreams
Persona
Function of dreams
Shadow
SELF
Animus/Anima
Psychological types
Etc.
Specific tasks
➊ Considering that Jung’s theory is principally about adult development, how does it
provide an understanding of the behaviours and motivations of a person such as Mugabe?
Think about the archetypal energies that might be involved and how, or if, these energies
constitute a pathology. You would need to consider the role of the complexes in trying to
locate the workings of the archetypal energies in his personal life.
➋ Consider what particular psychological function is dominant in a man such as Mugabe. Think
about how the dominant and inferior function interact to perhaps account for his behaviour.
➌ Jung suggests that archaic man did not reflect upon his symbols, but rather lived them,
and was unconsciously animated by their meaning. Investigate the origins of a symbol or
symbolic behaviour within your culture. Whatever symbol you choose, be it marriage,
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
the Star of David, initiation rites, this symbol is the link to archetypal organising energies.
Consider how it was originally lived and what function it serves in ordering our humanity.
➍ Consider the four major patterns of pathology and provide an example of people or events
that illustrate Jung’s understanding of each of these pathological possibilities.
General tasks
➊ Make a family tree of your family. Provide pseudonyms to ensure confidentiality. Identify what
patterns emerge. It may be that the women or the men of the family follow similar patterns or
have similar characteristics, or that particular events or characteristics are seen in the family. Trace
these patterns and identify which of the archetypal images are being activated within this family.
How do you understand this activation?
➋ Consider the stages of the individuation process. Discuss where you are in this process and
what archetypal themes may have already been integrated into your life and what still lies
ahead. Remember that individuation is a lifelong process.
➌ Describe your psychological type and which function is dominant for you. Which function
are you most challenged to develop? Explain in your answer why you think these observa-
tions apply to you.
➍ Keep a dream journal for a month. Trace the themes and symbols. You’ll probably find
that the themes make sense of things happening in your life at the moment. If there are
some strange symbols look them up in a book of symbols.
➎ Think of an example of a leader of a religious cult (ie David Koresh, the charismatic cultist
involved in the infamous Waco incident in the USA). Consider how Jung’s concept of
participation mystique may be applied in this case.
260 Developmental Psychology
Recommended readings
(Brooke, Samuels, and Jung’s collected works are probably for the more serious scholar of Jung, but they are
worth a try. The other books make quite accessible reading.)
Jung, C G (1963). Memories, dreams, reflections. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
(This is Jung’s autobiography. It gives an insight into Jung’s life and the particularities of his personality.
There is a fascinating introduction by Donald Winnicott, which is a gem on its own.)
Jung, C G (1972). The Collected Works of Carl G. Jung. 20 vols. (1953-75). London: Routledge.
(Routledge published the first complete English edition of Jung’s work. It is worthwhile going to the
original works to follow Jung’s own elucidation of his theory.)
Jung, C G (1983). Jung, selected writings. Introduced by Anthony Storr. London: Fontana.
(A wonderful selection of Jungian concepts that Storr brings together to elucidate the development of
Jung’s thought.)
Jung, C G (1986). Analytic psychology: Its theory and practice. London: Ark Paperbacks.
(This slim volume covers Jung’s 1935 lectures to the Tavistock. They are published in their lecture
format and give a wonderful sense of how Jung thought and spoke about his concepts.)
Jung, C G (ed) (1990). Man and his symbols. New York: Aldus Books.
(A beautifully illustrated book with essays by Jung and others on various aspects of mythology
and symbolism.)
McGuire, W & Hull, R C G (eds) (1980). Jung speaking: Interviews and encounters. London: Picador.
(The book presents interviews with a vast number of people who knew or met Jung, covering his whole
life. A fascinating portrait of the man develops through the eyes of those who knew him.)
12
Lacan’s mirror stage
Derek Hook
Introduction
Jacques Lacan was perhaps the most controversial
‘post-Freud’ Freudian. Although a psychoanalyst
and an advocate of a return to the original works
of Freud, Lacan radically reinterpreted much of
Freudian psychoanalysis and offered a number
of complementary psychoanalytic notions that
differed from traditional understandings. In fact
it was exactly this ‘radicalism’ that ultimately re-
sulted in his ‘excommunication’ from established
psychoanalytic circles. By introducing to psycho-
analysis certain important notions pertaining to
French Structuralism and Linguistics, Lacan ar-
guably extended the parameters of psychoanalysis,
and in some ways ‘modernised’ and changed the
face of the discipline.
Jacques Lacan
262 Developmental Psychology
which from that point on ‘acts as an intermediary between the id and the
external world ... [that special organisation is] ... the ego’ (Freud, 1966: 145).
Superego considerations
Freud also involved the superego in his explanation of the ego. Now
whereas the superego cannot be in any way responsible for the emergence
of the ego (the superego only develops after the ego), the ego does contain
certain elements within it that will make it a suitable place for the superego
to arise. These elements give us an important clue as to the development
of the ego. Benvenuto and Kennedy (1986:51-52) provide an extremely
helpful summary in this regard:
In other words, Freud has suggested that the superego contains, in the
‘ego-ideal’, a powerful element of narcissism, the love of one’s own ideal.
This powerful element of narcissism stems from the ego, a factor already
alluded to above, but now reiterated. For these reasons, it now becomes
crucial to involve an explanation of narcissism in the account of how the ego
emerges. Lacan will go on to do just this. In fact, his explanation for the emer-
gence of the ego will be able to account for both the elements of aggressivity
and narcissism, which—following Benvenuto and Kennedy’s (1986) thoughts
above—will be strongly present in the incipient superego.
opmental event that, according to Lacan, occurs roughly between the ages
of six and eighteen months, when the infant begins to recognise its image in
the mirror.
This formative event has been well documented in various studies of
early child and primate behaviour: the baby looks into a mirror, recognises
the image—a recognition which is accompanied by great pleasure—becomes
fascinated by it, and goes on to try and control or play with it. This is the mo-
ment, as Bowie (1991) puts it, that the child suddenly seems able to formulate,
however roughly, the propositions ‘I am that’ and ‘that is me’. A variety of
important bodily and psychological changes accompany this event:
At a certain point, around six months ... the infant becomes aware,
through seeing his image in the mirror, of his own body as a total-
ity, a total form or Gestalt. The mirror image is held together, it
can come and go with a slight change in the infant’s position, and
his mastery of the image fills him with triumph and joy. The mir-
ror image anticipates the mastery of the body that the infant has
not yet objectively achieved. He falls in love with his image and, in
contrast to the auto-erotic stage, in which he has an erotic relationship
to his fragmented body, he now takes the image of his whole body as
his love-object (Benvenuto & Kennedy, 1986: 54-55).
The ‘mirror test’ claims Evans (1996) was in the mirror and jubilantly assumes it as its
first described by the French psychologist own image, whereas the chimpanzee quickly
and friend of Lacan, Henri Wallon in 1931: realizes that the image is illusory and loses
interest in it (Evans, 1996:15).
It refers to a particular experiment that can
differentiate the human infant from his Benvenuto & Kennedy (1986) likewise
closest animal relative, the chimpanzee. make reference to the use of this ‘mirror
The six-month-old child differs from the test’ in early chimp experimentation, and
chimpanzee of the same age in that the quote at length the work of Kohler:
former becomes fascinated with its reflection Rana [a chimpanzee] ... gazed long and
>>
268 Developmental Psychology
<<
intently into the mirror, looked up and possible behind her back, gazed with an
then down, put it to her face and licked it air of indifference at the other animal,
once, stared into it again, and suddenly then suddenly made a pounce with her
her free hand rose and grasped as free hand. However, she and the rest
though at a body behind the mirror. But soon became used to this side of the
as she grasped emptiness she dropped affair and concentrated all their interest
the mirror sideways in her astonishment. on the image; this interest did not
Then she lifted it again, stared fixedly at decrease ... but remained so strong that
the other ape, and again was misled into the playing with reflecting surfaces
grasping into empty space. She became became one of the most popular and
impatient and struck out violently at the permanent of their fashions (Kohler, cited
mirror ... She held the mirror still in one in Benvenuto & Kennedy, 1986).
hand, drew back the other arm as far as
Indeed, as might well be pointed out, not all infants have access to
mirrors. Such an awareness of one’s own image—an image that is as a reflec-
tion of self—may occur in different ways. One example lies in the infant’s
awareness of how the mother’s gaze—or for that matter, the mother’s
voice—reflects its own current emotional state. In fact, intersubjective
moments more generally, where individuals gain a sense of self through
multiple kinds of interactions with others, are all an important source of
reflected ‘images’ of self. The same holds for interactions with ‘like’ indi-
viduals, such as children of the same basic age as the infant. As Bowie (1991)
notes, a roughly equivalent experience to beholding one’s mirror image is
that of seeing one’s own behaviours reflected in the imitative gestures of an
adult or another child. The important point is that the child will come to
understand himself as a coherent, unified being via these reflected images of
the self. (This also occurs through the fact that parents, adults, and siblings
treat the child as if it were a unified and coherent being. Hence he is sent
messages about his coherence and unity, both bodily and psychologically,
from the various responses of caregivers in addition to glimpses of his own
physical appearance in the form of visual reflections.)
parts. Indeed, how else is the child to ‘see’ his whole body—his face and
eyes as attached and related to his torso, legs, and arms—apart from seeing
an image of itself? This is indeed a momentous event for the child. It starts
to have some understanding of how his body fits together, how it might
control it, and where it ends and the world begins. A part of this is realising
that whereas it can control his arm, for instance, it cannot control the breast,
or other external objects. This control of the body, and his accompanying
sense of separateness from the world, is something that we as adults typically
take for granted. Hence it is certainly worthwhile considering, as we shall
do, the psychological gains that are associated with these newfound levels
of mastery.
In Greek mythology Narcissus was the image, he was unable to pull himself
handsome son of the river god Cephissus away from it, and eventually pined away.
and the nymph Liriope. For rejecting the His body was transformed into the flower
love of Echo he was punished by of the same name, which was said to be
Aphrodite by being compelled to fall in the last flower plucked by Persephone,
love with his own reflection in a pool of the goddess of fertility, before she was
water. Hopelessly enamored with his own carried off to the underworld.
disunity of the subject’s real body, and thus seems to threaten the
subject with disintegration (Evans, 1996: 120).
Lacan notes, the sight of its own mirror reflection is enough to ensure this
development. Lacan argues that for humans the body image also produces
instinctual responses, especially sexual ones, such as triggering the end of
auto-eroticism and the beginning of desire for an external object.
Another powerful example of the instinctual importance of the image
is to be found in Lorenz’s famous experiment of ‘imprinting’:
Lorenz had put his Wellington boots next to duck eggs. As the
ducklings hatched out and saw the boot, they became ‘imprinted’
with its image; wherever that boot went, the little ducks would
follow. They mistook Lorenz’s boot for their mummy. When
Lorenz wore his Wellingtons he was slavishly followed by a trail
of ducklings, each of whom [was] captivated by the image of the
boot (Hill, 1997:11).
Mirror anecdotes
There were pillars lined with full-length Harry (a year older). While we were
mirrors at the department store where opening presents, Ben started pointing
I used to work. Young children always at the full-length mirror which was
seemed drawn to the mirrors. I remember balancing against the mantelpiece and
one little girl who kissed her reflection excitedly identified ‘Harry, Harry’, even
and pressed her face against the glass, though Harry was at the other end of the
forcing air out of her cheeks at the same room. Ben toddled up to the mirror and
time. Others would pull faces and make started to pull at it, trying to climb
gurgling sounds, whilst playing with behind it, all this time calling Harry.
‘themselves’ in the mirrors. There were Eventually I asked Ben, curious to see
nearly always little hand marks at the whether he was identifying with the
bottom of mirrors at the end of the day mirror image, ‘Where is Ben?’ Ben then
when I went home. hid behind the couch, eventually peeking
I remember a separate occasion when I around the corner. ‘Ben’ he identified,
shared Christmas with another family, pointing to himself, joining in the game.
who had two young boys, Ben (who was
18 months old) and his brother Michele Vrdojak, University of the Witwatersrand
relationship of the subject with his or her own body. We know from the above
discussion that identification with the image is an enabling factor, which gives
the child increased control over his motor functions, among other things.
But what does it mean that the ego is formed on the basis of an imaginary
relationship with the body, and that all future engagements with reality will
be marked by imaginary anticipation?
Here it is important to reconsider certain implications of the image as
gestalt—that, for example, the gestalt lends a sense of completeness, closure, unity
and autonomy that is not necessarily there. In other words, there is always
something imaginary, something illusionary, in our identifications with our
own images, something deceptive, untruthful and fictitious. Lacan hints at
this in his descriptions of the mirror stage as a process of trickery, seduction,
captivation, slavery and bondage.
primary function of the ego. Hill (1997: 18) describes this deceptive
function of the ego:
In the mirror stage the infant sees its reflection in the mirror as a
whole/synthesis ... [a perception which contrasts with] the perception
of its own body ... as divided and fragmentary. The anxiety
provoked by this feeling of fragmentation fuels the identification
with the ... image by which the ego is formed. However, the anti-
cipation of a synthetic ego is henceforth constantly threatened by
the memory of this sense of fragmentation, which manifests itself in
images of ‘castration, emasculation, mutilation, dismemberment,
dislocation, evisceration, devouring, bursting open of the body’
which haunt the human imagination.
In a more general sense, the fragmented body refers not only to images
of the physical body but also to any sense of fragmentation and disunity
... Any such sense of disunity threatens the illusion of synthesis which
constitutes the ego.
the subject. In this way, claims Lacan, while there are vitally important Alienation. On a basic
achievements to be gained by the recognition of the image of self, there is also level the term refers to a
a fundamental alienation in this act, by virtue of the fact that it must always, fundamental split within
the subject, in which he
at some level, represent the inclusion of something more than is actually there.
is estranged, distanced
In other words, we might suggest that the action of identifying oneself with from his self. Alienation is
the image must always represent the internalisation of something external. the unavoidable result of
The infant’s prospective mastery, through the mirror image, of basic the process by which the
motor and physical co-ordination must always come from outside of itself; he ego is constituted through
is not yet really the master of his own movements. He can only see his form as identification with the
unified—as a more or less total image—in an external image. This external image external counterpart.
is a virtual, alienated, ideal unity that cannot actually be touched (Benvenuto
& Kennedy, 1986). Indeed, the mirror image is always a paradox; it is always
two contradicting things at once. It seemingly is the child, by virtue of the fact
that it is exactly the child’s reflection. But by being a reflection, and a reflection
only, it is also essentially not the child but something outside of him.
Conclusion
The mirror stage describes the formation of the ego via the process of
identification with an external image. The child identifies with an image
outside himself, which may be a real mirror image or simply the image of
another child. The apparent completeness of this image gives the child a new
mastery over his body, a new awareness of his capacities, which, through a
kind of mimicry, precipitates the child’s actual control over himself. This
identification with an external image or other party enables the child to do
things that he could not do before. Importantly, this identification with an
external image is more than a single moment in childhood: it is an organising
principle of human development generally. As Evans (1996) emphasises, it is
not simply a moment in the life of the infant, rather it represents a permanent
structure of subjectivity—a structure in which the subject is permanently
caught and captured by its own image.
For Lacan, we only ever However, this mastery of motor functions, this entry into the human
realise ourselves through an world of space and movement, comes at a price: the subject is captivated
external image or reflection, by the image and becomes fundamentally deluded by it—with significant
and accordingly there is an
‘inbuilt’ sense of alienation
consequences for the rest of life. The image as gestalt possesses an illusory
that cuts across all our self- completeness and unity that the infant does not possess in terms of his
understandings, no matter physical or psychological conceptions of self. In fact, in total contrast to
how deep or how arbitrary. this, the infant exists in a disturbing state of fragmented and disconnected
physicality, where he perceives his body as in pieces. Because the image
enables him to gain a sense of the physical integrity of his body, along with
a basic sense of ‘this is me’ apart from the outside world, it comes to love
and identify with the image. This image is also the form of the first rival,
however, because it contrasts with the uncoordinated disunity of the subject’s
real body. The image accentuates the subject’s feelings of fragmentation and
For Lacan, the mirror stage gives rise to an aggressive tension and rivalry between the subject and the
represents a permanent image. The erotic/aggressive identification with the mirror image will form
structure of subjectivity, a the basic template for all future relations of love and hate.
structure in which the
subject is permanently
Since the mirror image always comes from ‘the outside in’—and since
caught and captured by his the recognition of the image of self is in some ways the founding point of
or her own image. the ego—it stands to reason that there is always something fundamentally
P HOTO: GILL M OONEY
L a c a n ’s m i r r o r s t a g e 281
alien, something external, at the basis of the ego, at the basis of our most
fundamental understandings of self. In this way, a sense of identity only
comes at the price of a fundamental alienation. We only come to know
ourselves, and to develop our efficacy in the world, in terms of what is not
us, in terms of something other, and apart from ourselves. Hence our sense
of self is always imbricated (enmeshed, interwoven) with the other (the
position of someone outside of us), just as the other is always imbricated
in our sense of self.
The primary function of the ego then, based as it is on imaginary
images, is deception, mis-knowing, meconnaissance. It is not to be trusted,
and treatment should not seek to strengthen or develop it, or to help the
subject know him- or herself better. These attempts to find a unified
and total person are futile, and doomed to end in failure. Mainstream
developmental psychology has still to ponder the full implications of this
intriguing theory for the psychosocial development of the child.
Critiques of Lacan
Perhaps the chief criticism of Lacan is one that applies to virtually all of his
writings, and not to the mirror stage alone. It is simply that Lacan seems
almost perverse in the obscure and abstruse style in which he expresses his
theories and ideas. He intentionally chose a writing style that was non-
linear and opaque, and which, as Bowie (1991) notes, is typically frustrating,
difficult, and ambiguous. At a literary level perhaps this might be rationalised
as a style appropriate to its subject matter—to the unconscious flow of
meaning. The problem, however, is that one is almost always at pains to
know whether one is accurately interpreting his ideas. The mirror stage has
been an extremely influential theory. It has been widely lauded and even
more widely applied in academic discourse, and has become emblematic
of post-structural, and postmodern debates about the fragmentation
and ‘multiplicity of self’. (This fact is lamented by Sey (1999), who sees
it as a misappropriation and ‘domestication’ of the theory.) Mainstream
developmental psychology has still to engage properly with the ideas, and to
assess their practical utility. Until then the popular jury is still largely ‘out’,
at least regarding the critical evaluation of this theory.
282 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➊ Be on the lookout for the theme of mirror images in popular magazines,
advertising and popular culture generally. Try and collect a series of such
examples. Carefully ‘read’ these examples according to your understanding of
the mirror stage, and explore the various implications of Lacan’s theory for each
of them.
➋ If possible, try to observe infants playing in front of mirrors. Do their actions seem
to affirm or refute the basic mirror stage theory? Motivate your answer.
➌ Is it true, in your opinion, that young children often ‘play out’ the theme of the
corps morcele? (Think about games that you yourself played as a child.)
Give examples to support your answer.
➍ How would you explain the disorder of anorexia nervosa in terms of the mirror
phase? Consider the usefulness of the theory for this disorder?
Recommended readings
Benvenuto, B & Kennedy, R (1986). The works of Jacques Lacan: An introduction.
London: Free Association Press.
(In a field filled with deliberately obscure styles of writing, this volume presents an invaluably
accessible primer to Lacan’s basic ideas. Highly recommended.)
Bowie, M (1991). Lacan. London: Fontana.
(Almost as accessible as Benvenuto and Kennedy (1986). Bowie’s book does a similarly admirable job
of plotting out the general shape of Lacan’s overarching psychoanalytic theory.)
Evans, D (1996). An introductory dictionary of Lacanian psychoanalysis. London: Routledge.
(A clear and thorough explication of all the major terms and concepts of Lacanian psychoanalysis, which
includes commentaries on how they have been developed and in some instances transformed across the
whole span of Lacan’s writings.)
Hill, P (1997). Lacan for beginners. London: Writers and Readers.
Lacan, J (1977). Ecrits. London: Norton.
Lacan, J (1979). The four fundamental concepts of psychoanalysis. London: Penguin.
(It seems unusual not to recommend the original writings of the author to students, but as mentioned
above, Lacan’s original writings can be hair-raisingly frustrating and esoteric. Ecrits contains the mirror
stage paper, and is probably the slightly easier of the two.)
Leader, D & Groves, G (1995). Lacan for beginners. Cambridge: Icon Books.
(Both of the above are colourful, user-friendly, comic-book style introductions to Lacan, which usefully
illustrate many of his core concepts.)
CHAPTER
13
Erikson’s psychosocial stages
of development
Derek Hook
Erik Erikson
284 Developmental Psychology
have a foundation which has not been completely built), he does allow for the
possibility that adjustments made at one stage can in fact be altered or even
reversed in later stages. Craig, (1996: 59) describes this as follows:
... children who are denied attention in infancy can grow to nor-
mal adulthood if they are given extra attention at later stages ...
adjustments to conflicts play an important part in the develop-
ment of personality. The resolution of these conflicts is cumulative; a
person’s manner of adjustment at each stage affects the way he or she
handles the next conflict.
Figure 13.1 For Erikson each stage forms the basis for the next; the successful resolution of each stage
forms the foundation for the next stage of development (Carver and Scheier, 1988).
It is in this way that Erikson notes that the first demonstration of social
trust in the baby is ‘the ease of his feeding, the depth of his sleep, the relax-
ation of his bowels’ (1963: 247).
his will reaffirmed and delineated within an adult order of things which
at the same time reaffirms and delineates the will of others ... [under] the
institutional safeguard [of the] ... principle of law and order’ (Erikson, 1963:
254). Erikson also suggests that the pattern of child training established at this
stage determines the eventual form of political authority that the child will
prefer as an adult.
Moral development
Erikson (1963) suggests that moral development in children occurs
primarily at this stage, and contends that its source (the early super-
ego) can be cruel and over-controlling—over-constricting to the point
that children develop an over-obedience more literal than the one the
parent has wished them to develop. In fact, Erikson argues that some
of our deepest regressions and longest resentments stem from parents
who themselves did not live up to the ideals of this new conscience:
‘One of the deepest conflicts in life is the hate for a parent who served
as the model for the superego, but who ... was found trying to get away
with the very transgressions which the child can no longer tolerate in
themselves’ (Erikson, 1963: 257).
The adult problems that can stem from this phase of development
are inhibition, impotence, or denial—the kinds of paralysis that result
from an individual being too scared to ‘stick their neck out’ (Erikson,
1963). On the other hand, we may find recklessness, showing-off and
gratuitous risk-taking behaviour in those who are trying to over-
compensate for such inhibitions (Erikson, 1963).
The moral conscience (or superego) is built up not only from
parental prohibitions, but also from the child’s sociocultural heritage.
Cultural values, the tastes, class standards, characteristics, and traditions
of a society all contribute to the child’s sense of morality (Erikson, 1980).
Many of the child’s wildest fantasies are now repressed and inhibited
through this new capacity for guilt, and the predominant challenge here
is to balance a newfound sense of moral order with an ongoing thirst for
initiative. The child has to negotiate the tension between an ever-present
sense of moral surveillance and the desire to gain social experiences and
knowledge. Like Freud (1991) though, Erikson (1963) suggests that the
E r i k s o n ’s p s y c h o s o c i a l s t a g e s o f d e v e l o p m e n t 293
South Africa is a violent society charac- Guilt is a major problem for abused
terised by high levels of brutal crime children. As Angless & Shefer (1997)
and physical political conflict. An often- suggest, an alarming number of children
neglected form of South African violence is who live with the threat of domestic violence
that of domestic abuse. As Angless & Shefer feel that such violence is their fault, and
(1997) note, the home—far from being come to blame themselves for ‘causing the
a safe place for children—is often riddled problem’. This can have detrimental effects
with various forms of familial violence, on the self-esteem of such children, in the
with physical and sexual abuses, which can form of shame and feelings of ‘badness’.
understandably cause a range of emotional Social isolation is a frequent outcome
difficulties in child development. The effects for children who witness or suffer domestic
of witnessing violence committed against a abuse. Angless & Shefer (1997) quote
family member by another family member, a variety of figures suggesting that the
or of being abused oneself, vary widely for children of battered women are often
different children. alienated from their peers, and do not relate
In Erikson’s psychosocial developmental to their common interests and activities.
theory it is easy to see that a violent parent, or Similarly, children who have been abused
violence in the home more generally, would themselves often have great difficulties in
pose a threat to the child’s ability to resolve, social adjustment, in ‘fitting in’ at school
in particular, the crisis of trust versus mistrust, and in maintaining productive social
but also the crises of autonomy versus shame/ interactions more generally. Difficulties in
doubt and of initiative versus guilt. If children concentration, poor academic results, school
live largely in the shadow of fear of an phobia, fighting with peers and general
abusive parent, it will be very difficult, if not rebelliousness against adults and authority
impossible, for them to form a strong sense figures may all stem from a violent home
of trust in that parent In this connection, background (Angless & Shefer, 1997).
Angless & Shefer (1997) report that children The formation of strong role models and
exposed to domestic violence often live positive gender identifications are both also
with terrible anxiety and uncertainty, and extremely problematic for children who come
exhibit symptoms of emotional distress such from violent homes. While children do not
as restlessness, nervousness and a variety want to identify with the abuser, they equally
of related somatic complaints, in addition do not want to identify with the victim and as
to not being able to trust their basic home such they may equate maleness with violence
environment—or abusive parent(s)—such and hurting women, and femaleness with
children often experience the suspicion that being hurt by men (Angless & Shefer, 1997).
they may ultimately not be able properly to Perhaps most disturbing of all is the
trust or control themselves. Abused children contention that frequent exposure to vio-
observe parents who cannot control their lence, both as a witness and as a victim,
own anger and subsequently become appears to be a predisposing factor in
concerned that they may not be able to future violence. This is an ominous sugges-
control themselves. Guilt and anxiety, and tion, particularly considering the current
auto-phobia (fear of their own behaviour) are ‘culture of violence’ in South Africa, where
generally characteristic features of this state of a climate of brutal violence is so well
fundamental mistrust. established in the history of the country.
294 Developmental Psychology
new superego of the child not only restricts the horizon of the permissible
but also fosters positive goals; it sets the direction in which the dreams of
childhood might be attached to the goals of an active adult life.
advancements made in the three previous stages. In many ways this is the
stage in which the child ‘fills in the gaps’ of those psychosocial skills, social
roles, and levels of physical growth already achieved. Although the child is
now rapidly approaching the age where it is able to be parent (in the most
rudimentary psychological and biological terms), it must first become a
worker—a productive member of society and a potential provider (Erik-
son, 1963). In short, the child must develop the virtue of industriousness.
Technological proficiency
PHOTO: MICHELE VRDOLJAK
There are many life choices to be made here that will impact on
the identity of the individual. These concern the individual’s choice of
career, the nature of personal alliances they will be prepared to enter
into with others, the degree of mutuality they will share with peers, and
their placement relative to their social roles, and with reference to the
predominant socio-political issues of the day. The formation of identity
requires a number of hallmarks of stability, and these stabilities are pro-
vided in terms of sameness and continuity—the qualities that are needed
to hold together both one’s past history and one’s possible future and
career. It is these qualities that lend a sense of confidence to the developing
identity of the adolescent (Erikson, 1963).
Role confusion. Role confusion can surface in various ways. Strong previous doubts
Inability to settle on an about sexual orientation can lead to delinquent or psychotic episodes
occupational career, (Erikson, 1963), whereas milder forms of confusion stem predominantly
a sexual object-choice or
from the inability to settle on an occupational career, and from
a fundamental social role,
leading to the inability
overidentifying practices (Erikson, 1963).
to form a secure identity.
In- and out-group identifications
Overidentification. Overidentification for Erikson (1963) refers to that overzealous adoption of
Adoption of a group a group identity that threatens to totally eclipse one’s own sense of identity.
identity that threatens to Here he means the excessive emulation of heroes or idols, involvement in
totally eclipse one’s own
particular cliques or crowds, or blind adherence to varieties of dress codes
sense of identity.
or youth conventions. Overidentification is itself part of falling in love which,
Erikson (1963) claims, is by no means simply a sexual matter at this age: ‘To
a considerable extent adolescent love is an attempt to arrive at a definition of
one’s identity by projecting one’s diffused ego image on another and by seeing
it thus reflected and gradually clarified’ (Erikson, 1963: 262).
Stability of identity is likewise secured through a consistency in the way
that others variously understand, receive and perceive one (Erikson, 1963).
For Erikson (1963), young adults at this stage are remarkably sensitive to, and
aware of the way that they appear in the eyes of others. This increased awareness
of how one is viewed by the social world can complicate the process of identity
formation, because one is involved in the attempt to define and distinguish oneself
as precisely distinct from the rest of the social world generally. The danger at this
stage is that of role confusion.
Erikson (1963) notes how youngsters at this age of development can
be remarkably cruel to those who are ‘out’—who are ‘different’ in race or
cultural background, in tastes and gifts, in dress and gesture. This stage of
development is characterised by the selection of the signs of the ‘in-group’
and the ‘out-group’. The intolerance of youths towards ‘out-groupers’ is
in large part, according to Erikson, a ‘defence against [their own] sense
of identity confusion’ (1963: 262). If children were impressionable at the
previous stages, they are in a sense more so now, because identity formation
has become such an overriding focus. Indeed, Erikson (1963) notes that
simple, cruel and totalitarian doctrines often have appeal to the youths of
countries and classes that are in the process of losing their more traditional
group identities. The mind of the adolescent is an ‘ideological mind’, ‘eager
to be affirmed and to be confirmed by social rituals, creeds, and programmes
which delineate what is evil, uncanny, and inimical, along with what is
E r i k s o n ’s p s y c h o s o c i a l s t a g e s o f d e v e l o p m e n t 299
socially valued, prized, and idealised’ (Erikson, 1963: 263). This search
for identity also means that youths often end up appointing well-meaning
people in the role of adversaries. Youths are, as Erikson puts it, ‘ever ready to
install lasting idols and ideals as guardians of a final identity’ (1963: 261).
Negative identifications
Negative identity Ego diffusion, a particularly severe form of role confusion, can lead
formation. The choosing to social alienation—to the social withdrawal and isolation of those
of an identity opposite who are unable to integrate themselves within the social structure
to the one suggested by and values of their home culture. The lack of resolution at this stage
society. A lack of identity
may be linked to the behaviours of substance abusers and antisocial
resolution is often evident
in the behaviours of
personalities such as violent criminals. For Erikson (1980) these are
substance abusers and roles of deviance and extreme non-conformity that suggest negative
antisocial personalities identity formation, or the choosing of an identity opposite to the one
such as violent criminals. suggested by society.
Stage Trust vs Mistrust Autonomy vs Shame Initiative vs Guilt Industry vs Inferiority Identity vs Role
and Doubt Confusion
Age birth to 12–18 months 12–18 months to 3 years 3 to 6 years 6 to 11 years Adolescence
Predominant Intake of food, eating. Development of muscular Locomotion and genital Child is typically engaged in Adolescent decides on
activity Oral sensory activities. control, particularly activity. Exploration forms of systematic occupation, significant
anal control. and play. instruction and education. other and identity.
Goal of stage Reduction of tension. Attaining a basic sense of Reaching out; meeting the Technological proficiencies; Integration of ego
Attainment of basic independence, free choice challenges of the social ability to use tools. identifications, personal
equilibrium, both of inner and free will from accompl- environment in a directed aptitudes and social
biological need and ishment of rudimentary tasks and purposeful way; being opportunities into
between inner and and exploratory activities. able to take responsibility solid identity.
outer worlds. for self.
Possible Psychosocial weaknesses. Aggression, cruelty, Rigidity, overdeveloped Inferiority complexes, Social withdrawal and
developmental Masochistic, depressive, intolerance, irrational fears, superego, inhibition, self- low self-esteem, feelings isolation, psychotic
maladaptations schizoid personality (social obsessiveness, stubbornness, consciousness, social of unworthiness episodes, delinquency,
detachment and restricted antisocial personality impotence; alternatively, and incompetence. substance abuse, antisocial
emotional disposition), disorder. recklessness, showing-off, personality disorder.
psychosis. gratuitous risk-taking.
Radius of Maternal person Paternal person Basic family Neighbourhood and school Peer groups, in- and out-
significant groups. Models of leadership
persons
Louw, Louw, & Schoeman (1995) have minders, it has become a pressing research
called attention to the influence of the question as to what the effects of such an
black child-minder on psychosocial child arrangement would be upon white chil-
development. Given that a large number dren. Indeed, in such a historically racially
of white South African families use black divided society, it may have been proposed
‘domestic workers’ or ‘nannies’ as child- that this was a situation that lent itself to
>>
E r i k s o n ’s p s y c h o s o c i a l s t a g e s o f d e v e l o p m e n t 303
<<
a form of racial integration. In view of the positions and disparities of power in
fact that the ‘nanny’ was taking on a partly their immediate social spheres. Erikson
maternal role in caring for the children of a (1963) predicts this in his third stage,
family, one may have supposed that strong where he warns that the increasing
affectional bonds may have been exhibited social competence of the child enables
by children in ways that transcended the their awareness of a variety of social
apartheid imposition of racial boundaries. institutions, social roles, and standings
Similarly, in view of the fact that such in society. Bronfenbrenner (1979)
‘nannies’ were entrusted with the children likewise would find the results of the
of a family, and charged with their care, above study unsurprising in the sense
one might suppose that the family as a that macrosystem influences, like broad
whole (parents included) would come to socio-political values and ideologies such
form close emotional ties with her. as racism, would permeate all the way
Ultimately, however, research into the down to the more immediate levels of
influence of black child-minders (Straker, development such as the microsystem
1990; Wulfsohn, 1988) on white children sets of face-to-face ‘nanny’ and
supported neither of these suppositions. It child relationships.
was, as Louw et al (1995) note, markedly Another significant fact stemming
not the case that extended contact from the above research is that parental
between white child and black child- influence on children is not always
minder meant that such white children benign or positive. Parents cannot take
exhibited greater acceptance of other responsibility for all social values learnt,
race groups in South Africa. especially when there is no doubt that
Similarly, such consistent daily the environment into which the child is
exposure to a member of another culture developing is a social one informed by
did not significantly influence white a particular history, and populated with
children in the sense that they became certain popular (or predominant) values,
better versed in the values of a different standpoints and ideologies. Nevertheless,
culture, or less entrenched in the values of parents certainly can take responsibility
their own culture. The black child-minder for socio-political values that are the norm
was not a central figure in the life of the at home. Given the foundational nature
child, particularly not in the emotional of immediate familial relationships within
sense and, as Straker notes (cited in Louw the context of home life (in Freud, Erikson
et al, 1995), had no power, status, or and in Bronfenbrenner) it seems that
real importance in the eyes of children. racism might well ‘start in the home’ so to
In many ways the perception of the child speak, and that parental influence would
seems to have been that the child-minder have a lot to answer for in the psychosocial
had a contractual and not an emotional development of racism in the child. This is
bond to the family. This is a view certainly an area in which more research is
corroborated by many of such children’s needed, particularly in view of the fact that
actions, where they were content to a psychosocial developmental account of
‘let the nanny do it’ rather than do it racism would seem to be able to explain
themselves (as in the case of cleaning up not only the tenacity of the phenomenon,
a mess left by the child). but also its ‘deep-rootedness’—the way
It seems in this way that even fairly it seems to function at such an intuitive,
young children were responsive to socio- immediate, almost ‘naturalised’ level
political factors in their developmental within people.
environment, and aware of social role
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON 304 Developmental Psychology
The search for the prospective The importance and risks of intimacy
significant other is central to Most young adults emerge from the search for identity with a willingness
Erikson’s sixth stage.
to fuse their identities with others, with an enthusiasm for intimacy, and
with the rudimentary ethical strength that they will need in order to abide
by the emotional commitments they make to potential significant others
at this stage. The ego needs to be strong in this phase, so as to fend off the
fear of ego loss in the case of close friendships, in the case of inspiration by
teachers, in the orgasms of sexual union, and in various close affiliations
(Erikson, 1963). The consideration here is the necessity to maintain the
integrity of the ego and not allow it to be incorporated wholesale into some-
thing or someone else. Such ego losses may lead to a deep sense of isolation
and subsequent self-absorption. (It is understandable, by this rationale,
why suicide might occur with particular prevalence at this stage of life.)
For Erikson (1963: 266) the ‘utopia of genitality’ should include the
following elements:
1. mutuality of orgasm,
2. with a loved partner,
3. of the other sex,
4. with whom one shares a mutual trust,
5. with whom one can work, procreate, and relax, and
6. so as to secure offspring to the successful stages of development.
The idea is that adults by now have often largely resolved earlier life-stage
conflicts and are hence free to direct their attention to the assistance of oth-
ers, particularly their own children (Craig, 1996). Generativity refers chiefly
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON 306 Developmental Psychology
Disgust on the part of ageing adults can often function in trying to hide
despair, even if only in the form of ‘a thousand little disgusts that cannot add
E r i k s o n ’s p s y c h o s o c i a l s t a g e s o f d e v e l o p m e n t 307
<<
Nevertheless, this author feels that the Rather, these kinds of influence might
creativity of much contemporary youth prove to be valuable resources from which
culture in post-apartheid South Africa vital South African youth movements and
should not be underestimated. Exposure cultures may emerge. Kwaito music seems
to the ‘Coca-Cola culture’ of ‘first-world’ a good example; to consider Kwaito merely
entertainment role models does not simply an alienating cardboard cut-out of American
lead to a kind of dead-end or derivative rap and dance music would seem to miss
culture of alienation and marginalisation. the vibrancy and originality of this new style.
In some ways this last stage of development links up with the very first
stage, of trust, particularly since trust is sometimes defined as ‘the assured
reliance on another’s integrity’. Accordingly, Erikson concludes with the
observation that ‘it seems possible to ... paraphrase the relation of adult
integrity and infantile trust by saying that healthy children will not fear
life if their elders have integrity enough not to fear death’ (1963: 269). The
virtue at this late adulthood stage of development is wisdom.
Idealism
Whereas Freud has been criticised for a seemingly pessimistic view of
development, it has been argued that Erikson’s theory is frequently too
optimistic and idealises descriptions of typical development (Maier, 1988).
What place is there for human tragedy in his theory? And, likewise, are there
not emotional conflicts that lead in no ‘healthy’ direction at all? It would
seem that the very essence of certain crises is that they lead to no productive
resolution, and in fact constitute a ‘waste’ of human feelings and impulses.
One needs to ask whether Erikson, in avoiding Freud’s theoretical tendency
to focus on ‘how things can go wrong’, has not perhaps given us an idealised
rather than a realistic or a pragmatic account of psychosocial development.
In a similar vein, approaches like that of Lacan (1977) would suggest that,
while Freud may appear deterministic and reductionist, Erikson, by contrast,
overvalues the adaptive and integrative functions of the ego. For Lacan (1977)
it is decidedly not the case that the ego is a basically honest mechanism that is
E r i k s o n ’s p s y c h o s o c i a l s t a g e s o f d e v e l o p m e n t 309
congruent with reality, and that places the individual in an ever more healthy
and adaptive relationship to society. For him, many of the primary functions
of the ego remain unconscious, fundamentally deceptive, and connected to
external reality in a basically illusionary manner. This view makes a stark con-
trast to Erikson’s positive understanding of ego-adaptation and development.
Allegations of sexism
Erikson, like Freud, has been criticised for treating the male as the standard
of human development, with the female featuring only as a variation, or
even deviation, of the normal path of psychosocial development. As Maier
(1988) contends, Erikson can reasonably be criticised for suggesting that
the development of ‘humankind’ is in fact the development of ‘malekind’.
Not only is it the case that Erikson’s writings reflect a male bias and
dominance, it is also the case that his clinical and historical research subjects
are all male (Maier, 1988). For this reason, Erikson’s theories may be seen
as not only a limited means of accounting for female development, but also
as a demeaning account of female development. His later stages strongly
prioritise the procreative and maternal qualities that he sees as endemic to
femininity; these qualities are indicative to Erikson of both necessary social
roles and personality development within women (Erikson, 1963). As an
extension of this view, women have, within Erikson’s theory, typically
been conceptualised as more dependent and less assertive or active than
men. This induces the suggestion that it is only through intimate relations
that women come properly to find their identity—a precondition that
does not hold for men (Maier, 1988). In this vein, and quoting Erikson to
substantiate her critique, Weisstein makes the following observation:
Erikson ... upon noting that young women often ask whether
they can ‘have a identity before they know whom they will
marry, and for whom they will make a home,’ explains ... that:
‘Much of a young woman’s identity is already defined in her
kind of attractiveness and in the selectivity of her search for the
man ... by whom she wishes to be sought...’ Mature womanly
fulfilment, for Erikson, rests on the fact that a woman’s ‘...
somatic design harbors an “inner space” destined to bear the
offspring of chosen men, and with it, a biological, psychological,
and ethical commitment to take care of human infancy’ (Weisstein,
1973: 391).
Cultural bias
If Freud can be accused of cultural bias, then so can Erikson. Erikson’s theories
may seem less biased to us purely because they are not as historically or cultur-
ally distant to us as Freud’s. However, they exhibit a number of strong late 20th
century capitalistic American values, which may limit the universality of his
theory, or its cross-cultural applicability. Certain of Erikson’s virtues, like inde-
pendence (the virtue of his third stage), initiative and industriousness sound more
like the individual qualities that are specifically desirable within a competitive
and capitalistic society than the universal virtues of healthily developing chil-
dren. In this connection, Sampson (1992) takes issue with Erikson’s approach,
310 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
CRITICAL THINKING TASKS
➊ Reflect briefly on how a punitive upbringing with frequent corporal punishment would
influence a child’s acquisition of basic trust.
➋ What parenting styles would ideally promote the development of autonomy in the
young child? Similarly, give some thought to the kinds of life events that would threaten
the development of basic autonomy in the child.
➌ In terms of Erikson’s theory, how would blindness or lack of mobility, as in the case
of the handicapped child, impact on child development within the first three stages?
➍ Try and ‘rewrite’ Erikson’s description of the fifth stage of development in the terms
of your own personal life experiences at this age. >>
E r i k s o n ’s p s y c h o s o c i a l s t a g e s o f d e v e l o p m e n t 311
<<
➎ Trace a series of parallels, where possible, between the first five stages of development as
conceptualised by both Freud and Erikson.
➏ This chapter provides a tabular breakdown of the first five of Erikson’s developmental
stages. Following the same format, try to generate a similar table highlighting the key
aspects of the last three developmental stages.
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
General tasks
➊ Research a biographical account of a famous psychopath. By systematically working
through the first five stages of Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development, compile a list
of the developmental challenges that this person appears to have been unable to meet.
➋ What are some of the developmental ‘virtues’ that this individual did not attain, and
how do you see this failure as impacting on the psychopath’s individual personality
development?
➌ What kinds of interventions or crucial differences in the psychopath’s life could have
helped to prevent his actions? What plausible life events may have aided him in
repairing early developmental deficits?
➍ Whose theory, Freud’s or Erikson’s, do you feel has the greatest explanatory power with
regard to the psychopath’s case? Explain your answer, giving concrete examples.
➎ How can the theories of Freud and Erikson, with their different focuses, be used in a
complementary way? Can you use them to build on one another, to ‘fill in each other’s
gaps’, in explaining developmental phenomena in the psychopath’s case?
Recommended readings
Erikson’s theory has proved to be an influential and popular staple of developmental psychology, and as such
it features in virtually every developmental psychology textbook. If you are interested in Erikson’s theories
though, it is worthwhile engaging directly with Erikson’s own writings, which are generally accessible,
and contain far more textured and nuanced detail than most textbook summaries of his thinking. The two
central texts to try are:
Erikson, E H (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H (1968). Identity, youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
(Both these texts provide useful perspectives and elaborations on his psychosocial stage theory.
Also interesting are the following texts, which essentially extend and apply the theory.)
Erikson, E H (1964). Insight and responsibility. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H (1969). Gandhi’s truth. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H (1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: Norton.
(If you’re interested in tackling a good critical commentary on Erikson— and it is a good idea to read
someone other than Erikson on Erikson, because, regrettably, some of his understandings of race and
gender have dated badly—then try the relevant sections of:
Maier, H W (1988). Three theories of child development, 3rd ed. New York: University Press
of America.)
SECTION TWO
Cognitive development
CHAPTER
14
Introduction to cognitive
development
Kate Cockcroft
Introduction
Attempts to understand human cognition always result in one marvelling
at how it functions. In How the mind works, Steven Pinker (1997) notes
that man’s attempts to artificially replicate how the mind works are feeble
in comparison to the intellectual feats of which even a four-year-old
child is capable. Not all of these amazing capacities arrive fully formed
in the newborn infant however, but require decades of experience and
biological maturation. Cognitive developmental psychologists are
therefore interested in how our mental processes are related to our biology
and experiences.
Going a step back, ‘cognition’ is a collective term for the processes involved
in acquiring, organising, manipulating and using knowledge. These processes
are not directly observable. Remembering, problem solving, imagining,
creating, fantasising and using symbols, for example, are all cognitive
processes. Cognitive psychologists do not see people as passive recipients of
information but as active processors of information, constantly reorganising
Introduction to cognitive development 315
and changing the information they receive from the environment via
their senses. Cognitive development therefore refers to the ways in which
people acquire various cognitive abilities and how these abilities change
in structure and function over time. In addition, cognitive developmental
psychologists realise the significance of individual differences in cognitive
performance among people of a particular age and regard these variations
as useful, providing interesting and important information about alternate
patterns of development and alternate developmental outcomes.
By now you know that development is generally thought of as the
progressive change from simple to increasingly more complex structures
and behaviours. However, this does not mean that early forms of cognition Cognition. A collective
are less effective and incomplete in comparison to adult cognition. Piaget term for the processes
was the first cognitive developmental theorist to point out that early forms involved in acquiring,
organising, manipulating
of cognitive functioning may serve a particular function, for example
and using knowledge.
the tendencies of preschool children to overestimate their physical and
cognitive abilities may serve to bolster their self-esteem.
The purpose of this section is to introduce the substantive theorists and
issues in cognitive developmental psychology in sufficient depth that read-
ers can engage with them at a variety of levels. It is hoped that after reading
this section, you will see cognitive developmental psychology as an exciting,
stimulating and practically relevant field.
A popular point of debate in the Initially, this question was the domain of
philosophy of psychology concerns philosophers and theologians. The French
whether mind and brain are the same or philosopher, Rene Descartes, argued that
not. For example, do you think that your humans have ‘mental substance’ or an
sense of self is separate from your body? immaterial, non-spatial mind in addition
>>
318 Developmental Psychology
<<
to our physical brain, which distinguishes Searle, argues that mere symbol
us from lower life forms. When manipulators (computers) cannot have
psychology was established as a semantics or meanings, and thus
discipline, questions concerning the intentionality. Searle (1980; 1992) uses
relationship between mind and brain his famous ‘Chinese Room Argument’ (of
were also asked. Descartes’ mind-brain which there are several versions) to argue
dualism was criticised, with many arguing his case. Suppose the central processing
that there is no mind over and above the unit (CPU) of a digital computer is
brain. Rather, the brain is the self programmed with the rules of Chinese
(Churchland, 1995). Churchland argues syntax. The CPU is able to string together
that all cognitive processes can be Chinese characters and output them in
explained entirely in terms of the brain. such a way that a person fluent in
For example, post-mortem examinations Chinese could read, understand and
of the brains of individuals who suffered respond to them. Even though the CPU
from Alzheimer’s Disease reveal actual gives the impression of knowing what the
plaques and tangles throughout the fine Chinese characters mean, and can
web of synaptic connections of the manipulate them according to a
neurons of the brain that embody one’s programme, it does not have
cognitive capabilities. Churchland also intentionality in the sense that it cannot
discusses how the advances in understand the world and its features. So,
neurosciences and artificial intelligence brains or computers which can only
allow for brain function to be represented manipulate symbols according to a
as the parallel distributed processing of programme, cannot have intentionality.
recurrent neural networks. Intentionality can only be had by objects
On the other hand, others argue that that have a conscious mind, such as the
computational modelling of mental Chinese speaker. Searle’s argument
processes does not demonstrate the presents a challenge to Churchland’s
uniquely human characteristic of view that the material brain is just
intentionality. The philosopher, John a neurocomputer.
Intentionality. Every
mental phenomenon has a
Evidently the cognitive revolution was a success, as cognitive psychology
content, and is directed at is today a dominant paradigm in academic psychology. Cognitive science
an object (the intentional is currently one of the fastest growing and most influential areas of study
object). Intentionality in the twenty-first century. It is an interdisciplinary approach involving
refers to the fact that every anthropology, artificial intelligence, computer science, mathematics,
belief, desire etc, has an linguistics, neuroscience, philosophy, evolutionary biology and, of course,
object that it is about: the
psychology. For more detail on this approach see Chapter 21, which deals
believed, the wanted, etc.
with the contributions of cognitive science to this field.
To explain how birds fly, we invoke principles of lift and drag and
fluid mechanisms that also explain how airplanes fly. That does
not commit us to an Airplane Metaphor for birds, complete with
jet engines and complimentary beverage service.
may test the resulting hypothesis). For example, we might observe that
it is possible to drive and talk on a cellphone provided there is no other
distraction. So, we may ask, ‘Does talking on a cellphone affect the ability
to drive?’ We can test this question by comparing two situations that are as
similar as possible. In one, the participants undertake a simulated driving
test without talking on a cellphone. The effects on their driving are noted.
In the other situation, the participants experience the same as the first
group but have to conduct a conversation on a cellphone. Any significant
difference between performance on the simulated driving test between
the two situations leads us to infer that talking on a cellphone had an effect
on driving.
Such an approach also allows the hypothesis to be tested for falsifiability.
Popper (1959) stated that if a theory is to be a good one, then it should
be possible to prove it false. While this may sound like a contradiction
in terms, consider the classic example of falsifiability, namely the white
swan hypothesis. If we think that all swans are white, we cannot prove
this hypothesis to be true. Every instance of a white swan adds to our
conviction, but without seeing every swan in existence, in the past and
future, we cannot completely prove this hypothesis to be true. But we can
prove it false if we see one black swan. In this way, we are setting up a direct
comparison between two situations (white swan-black swan; driving and
talking on a cellphone versus driving and not talking on a cellphone).
human intelligence over the life-span and one of the proponents of this
view, Reuven Feuerstein (1979), has conducted numerous studies in which
the cognitive abilities of severely retarded children are modified to become
more efficient (See also interest block in Chapter 22 on Vygotsky).
the legal system to explore aspects such as memory for crime details, the
military to explore robotic and mind-controlled machinery, health-care
providers to explore the effects of brain trauma on cognitive functioning
and educators to explore the most effective ways for teaching and learning
to occur. Thus, there is a move to demonstrate the broader applicability of
laboratory-tested experiments.
An experimental approach to research may be further justified by the
argument that it is reasonable to adopt a reductionistic approach, where an
attempt is made to understand complex mental processes by analysing them
into their constituent parts, for example studying eye movements when read-
ing. Naturally, it would also be reasonable to assume that these researchers
would eventually put the various pieces back together again and also deal with
the larger event as a whole, for example how eye movements can be corrected
to improve the reading ability of children with dyslexia. Recent developments
(see Chapter 21 on cognitive science) seem to hold just such a promise.
So, although different aspects of cognitive development will be
discussed in the chapters that follow, the cognitive system generally
operates as a whole, and it is necessary to emphasise that each aspect
must be viewed as integrally related to the functioning of all the other
parts. While the chapters may give the impression that various aspects
of cognitive development, such as language, intelligence and memory,
can be neatly compartmentalised, in reality this is not the case. Using
a language, for example, requires that we draw on aspects of attention,
perception, memory and concept formation, to name a few of the
cognitive processes involved. Language also has emotional impact. Just
think about the emotional responses evoked by the phrases ‘I love you’
and ‘I hate you’. However, due to the complexity of the cognitive system,
it is often easier to explain cognitive development by using these artificial
distinctions. Similarly, cognitive development is one of the many parts
of human development, and should be regarded as integrated with a
person’s emotional and psychosocial functioning.
Because of the tendency to focus on discrete aspects of cognitive
functioning, cognitive developmental psychology often appears to lack
integration. This is also because there is no particular framework and no
one particular theory that we can point to as the main or archetypical theory.
There is also no distinct theme. Cognitive developmental psychology is
not so much a set of theories as an approach to human development that
focuses on mental processes and how they change over the life-span and
how these changes may affect our behaviour. A benefit of this is that we
can use a cognitive approach to understand many aspects of functioning,
such as emotion and social functioning. So, a criticism that the field is
difficult to study, is also a demonstration of its flexibility.
Another major criticism of cognitive developmental psychology is
that the notion of man as computer, the metaphor often used by cognitive
psychologists, seems offensive to many, who argue that it does not take into
account the fundamental differences between human and machine. By
way of explanation, this is a metaphor, a useful way of using something we
understand (computers) to explain something that we do not (the human
mind). However, this does have limitations if it is used in an absolute way.
Introduction to cognitive development 323
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
Specific tasks
➊ How do cognitive theories help us to make predictions about development
and behaviour?
Recommended readings
Bjorklund, D F (2005). Children’s Thinking. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.
(This book is based on the premise that individual differences in cognition can be understood by
considering their developmental function. It discusses cognitive development, with a focus on the
relationship between biology and the child’s physical and social environment. It is an easy and
engaging read suitable for both undergraduate and postgraduate students.)
Blackmore, S (2005). Conversations on Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(This is a series of interesting, and often humorous, conversations between Blackmore and the world’s
leading philosophers and neuroscientists.)
Erdelyi, M (1985). Psychoanalysis: Freud’s Cognitive Psychology. New York: W H Freeman.
(This book examines the relationship between psychoanalytic theory and cognitive theory.)
Pinker, S (1997). How the mind works. London: Penguin Books.
(This book explains what the mind is, how it evolved and how it allows us to do the myriad of things
that we take for granted on a daily basis.)
CHAPTER
15
Piaget’s constructivist
theory of cognitive
development
Kate Cockcroft
Introduction
Jean Piaget (1896-1980), more than any
other person, has laid the groundwork for
many of our current beliefs about cognitive
development. From a young age he was
interested in the scientific study of nature.
When, at ten years old, he found that his
questions could only be answered by access to
the university library, Piaget wrote and signed
a short paper on the sighting of an albino
sparrow, in the hope that this would
stop the university librarian from
treating him like a child and
give him access to the library.
It worked (Papert, 1996). Later, Jean Piaget
Piaget trained as a biologist and,
P i a g e t ’s c o n s t r u c t i v i s t t h e o r y o f c o g n i t i v e d e v e l o p m e n t 325
before he was 20 years old, was well known in Europe for his work in
this field. After completing a dissertation on molluscs and receiving
his doctorate at the age of 21, Piaget decided that he wanted more
formal training in psychology. He became interested in psychoanalysis
and attended Jung’s lectures in Zurich. Shortly thereafter he started
working with Theodore Simon (who developed the Simon-Binet
Intelligence Test). During this work, Piaget discovered that whether
a child was right or wrong on a particular intelligence test item was
far less interesting than the child’s reasoning in arriving at a particular
answer, especially a wrong answer. Piaget concluded from examining
children’s answers and how they reached them that children’s thinking
was qualitatively different from adults and that it had its own special
logic. Einstein called Piaget’s discovery ‘so simple that only a genius could
have thought of it’ (quoted in Papert, 1996: 59). In one of his famous
experiments, Piaget interviewed many children to reveal how they
thought. One question that he asked them was: What makes the wind?
Adaptation. Piaget’s term Piaget devoted the rest of his life to investigating how intelligence develops.
for the ability to adjust By intelligence Piaget meant more than just what is measured by intelligence
to the demands of the
tests. According to Piaget, intelligence influences all acts of thinking—
environment, a process
during which schemes
perception, language, morality, to name but a few. This is why his theory
are elaborated, changed is often called a theory of cognitive, rather than ‘intellectual’, development.
and developed. Piaget Reflecting his past training, he saw the development of intelligence as a
separated adaptation form of biological adaptation to the environment. Remaining a helpless
into two complementary baby, totally dependent on others for survival, is not adaptive. So, for example,
components called through the process of adaptation, the baby gradually learns how to grasp a
assimilation and
bottle and satisfy his hunger, thereby decreasing his dependence on others.
accommodation.
The process of adaptation will be discussed in more detail later.
Constructivism. Piaget’s theory is essentially a constructivist one which assumes the active
The notion that reality is building up of knowledge and cognitive processes from a very basic starting
a construction based on
point, and that children at different developmental levels construct different
the information from
our environment and
realities (Papalia & Olds, 1993). Piaget referred to his approach as genetic
in our heads. So, each epistemology, where the term genetic signifies growth and development,
person constructs a rather than the action of genes, and epistemology means the study of
different understanding knowledge (Piaget, 1952). Piaget developed a philosophy of epistemological
of the world. relativism in which multiple ways of knowing are acknowledged and
examined analytically and objectively.
Genetic epistemology.
The experimental study of
the development
Basic concepts underlying Piaget’s theory:
of knowledge. schemes, operations and adaptation
Piaget called mental structures schemes. Schemes, in this definition, are
Schemes. Piaget’s term ways of processing information that change as we develop. There are two
for mental structures types of scheme: sensorimotor schemes (also known as action schemes) and
that process information cognitive schemes (also referred to as concepts). During the first two years of
from the external world.
life, the infant’s knowledge of objects and events is limited to various practi-
Schemes change as
we develop.
cal sensorimotor schemes such as grasping, sucking and looking. Thus, for
a ten-month-old baby, a fluffy teddy bear is not understood as a bear but
simply as an object that feels soft and can be cuddled or chewed. According
to Piaget, the child only shows signs of cognitive schemes at about two years
of age. Then the child becomes capable of solving problems and thinking
about objects and events without having acted on them. This means that the
child is able to represent experiences mentally and use these mental symbols
to achieve certain objectives. Piaget (1962: 63) illustrates how his 16-month-
old daughter, Jacqueline, formed a mental representation of the behaviour
of a visiting child and reproduced his behaviour the following day:
Jacqueline had a visit from a little boy (13 months of age) ... who, in the
course of the afternoon, got into a terrible temper. He screamed as he
tried to get out of a playpen, and pushed it backward, stamping his feet.
Jacqueline stood, watching him in amazement, never having witnessed
Operations.
such a scene before. The next day, she herself screamed in her playpen
Piaget’s term for
reversible mental actions. and tried to move it, stamping her foot ... several times in succession.
Operations combine
to form qualitatively Schemes are ultimately organised into operations (that is, reversible
different stages of mental actions), which combine to form qualitatively different stages of
cognitive development. cognitive development (Papalia & Olds, 1993).
P i a g e t ’s c o n s t r u c t i v i s t t h e o r y o f c o g n i t i v e d e v e l o p m e n t 327
(a) Primary circular reaction: action and response both involve infant’s own body (one to four months)
(b) Secondary circular reaction: action gets a response from another person or object, leading to
baby’s repeating original action (four to eight months)
(c) Tertiary circular reaction: action gets one pleasing result, leading baby to perform similar actions
to get similar results (12 to 18 months)
Primary, secondary, and tertiary circular reactions. According to Piaget, infants learn to reproduce
pleasing events they have discovered accidentally.
(a) Primary circular reaction: A baby happens to suck a thumb, enjoys sucking, and puts the thumb
back into the mouth or keeps it there. The stimulus (thumb) elicits the sucking reflex; pleasure then
stimulates the baby to keep on sucking.
(b) Secondary circular reaction: This involves something outside the baby’s body. The baby coos; the
mother smiles; and because the baby likes to see the mother smile, the baby coos again.
(c) Tertiary circular reaction: The baby tries different ways to reproduce an accidentally discovered
response. When the baby steps on a rubber duck, the duck squeaks. The baby then tries to produce
the squeak in other ways, perhaps by squeezing it or sitting on it.
Figure 15.1 Primary, secondary and tertiary circular responses. Adapted from Papalia and Olds (1998).
P i a g e t ’s c o n s t r u c t i v i s t t h e o r y o f c o g n i t i v e d e v e l o p m e n t 331
again (Papalia & Olds, 1993). Here there is a clear differentiation between
means and ends. When faced with a problem, such as getting a stick
through the bars of a gate, the toddler can now make subtle adjustments
to his existing schemes to work out a solution. Together with these new
intellectual tools come increased locomotive abilities, which enable the
toddler to actively experiment with the environment. At this age, problems
are solved by means of a physical trial-and-error process. So, if the toddler
wants to know how much toilet paper is on a roll and whether it will all fit
into the toilet, he has to try this out. The toddler cannot simply examine the
two objects and discern that one will easily fit into the other. At this stage
objects are understood only by acting on them (physically manipulating
them). Mental comparisons, which involve the symbolic representation of
objects and events, are not yet possible.
Object permanence
Imitation
Deferred imitation. Piaget believed that imitation plays an adaptive role in the child’s development.
The ability to reproduce Based on his observations of infants, Piaget believed that they are incapable
the behaviour of an of imitating novel responses until eight months of age. Deferred imitation, or
absent model.
the ability to reproduce the behaviour of an absent model, first appears around
12 to 18 months of age. (Jacqueline’s reproduction of her playmate’s temper
tantrum, mentioned earlier, is an example of deferred imitation.) It is only once
a child is capable of forming mental symbols or images of another person’s
behaviour, and of storing and retrieving these from memory, that he is capable
Preconceptual stage.
of deferred imitation (Ginsburg & Opper, 1969). Research has indicated that
The first phase of the
preoperational stage of infants and children are capable of imitation and deferred imitation much
cognitive development. earlier than Piaget proposed (Craig, 1996). Some newborns are able to imitate
It is characterised by the an adult sticking out their tongue, but then lose this ability and do not recover
increasing use of symbols, it for several months (Craig, 1996). Meltzoff (1988) found that nine-month-olds
including language and are able to imitate very simple acts such as pressing a button to activate a noise-
symbolic play. producing toy, a day after observing a model producing such behaviour.
Intuitive stage. The
second phase of the Symbolic Representation
preoperational stage of
cognitive development, Preoperations (two to seven years)
also known as the The period from two to seven years is a time of much intellectual curiosity,
transitional stage. During when children come up with all sorts of questions, such as ‘What makes
this phase the child you stop growing?’ or ‘Why do you have to have a man and a lady to have a
becomes less egocentric
baby?’ Piaget called this period the preoperationaI or prelogical stage because
and much better at
classifying objects on he believed that preschool children have not yet acquired the cognitive
the basis of perceptual operations that would enable them to think logically or to interpret reality
categories such as size, correctly. Piaget’s descriptions of preoperational thinking focus mainly on
shape and colour. the limitations of this stage (Piaget, 1952).
The preoperational stage is divided into two parts: the preconceptual
Transitional stage. stage (from two to four years) and the intuitive or transitional stage (from five
See intuitive stage.
to seven years). The preconceptual stage is characterised by the increasing
Animism. use of symbols, including language and symbolic play. For example, a child
The preoperational belief playing with a stick as if it were a gun is engaging in symbolic play. The use
that all moving objects of symbols enables the child to think about things that are not only in his
(and sometimes inanimate immediate environment. The child also has the power to name things that
objects) are alive. may not be immediately present. Children in the preconceptual stage still
have difficulty distinguishing between mental, physical and social reality.
Egocentricity. A view of
They may think, for example, that all objects that move are alive, including
the world that is centred
on one’s own perspective; cars and clouds. (This is called animism). The child may expect that the
a characteristic of inanimate world will obey his commands, a trait stemming partly from
preoperational thinking. the child’s self-centred view of the world (or egocentricity). Most of these
‘illogical’ ways (by adult standards) of thinking about the world result from
Transductive reasoning. the child’s transductive reasoning. Transductive reasoning entails reasoning
Reasoning from the from the particular to the particular. When any two events co-vary, the child
particular to the
assumes that one has caused the other. For example, when Piaget’s daughter
particular. Assuming, for
example, that when any had missed her regular afternoon nap one day, she said, ‘I haven’t had a nap,
two events occur so it isn’t afternoon’ (Shaffer, 1996: 256).
simultaneously, one has The transitional or intuitive stage begins around five years of age and the
caused the other. differences between the preconceptual and the intuitive stages are so slight,
P i a g e t ’s c o n s t r u c t i v i s t t h e o r y o f c o g n i t i v e d e v e l o p m e n t 333
This is because the child is focusing on the current state of the object
and not on the process of transformation. The child is also focusing
on one dimension of the clay at a time such as its ‘flatness’ or ‘fatness’.
This example also shows how the preoperational child’s thinking is
concrete, based on direct experience in the ‘here and now’, and it
shows the irreversibility of the child’s thinking—he is unable to
reverse the clay ball back to its original state.
One of the main distinctions between photographs of objects as though they were
humans and other creatures is our capacity the real object, attempting to grasp them or
for symbolic representation. This enables us pick them up off the page. Some would
to transmit information from one generation even lean over and put their lips on the teat
to the next, thus ensuring cultural of a photograph of a bottle, for example.
continuity, as well as to learn without direct They only did this, though, if the depicted
experience—we all know about dinosaurs object was highly similar to the object it
although we have never encountered one. represents (its referent) as in colour
Symbolic representation is possibly one of photographs and video footage. When the
the most important aspects of human objects bore less resemblance to the real
development, due to its fundamental role in thing, as in a line drawing, the infants
nearly everything we do. One of the first would rarely explore them. By
types of symbolic objects that Westernised approximately 18 months, infants come to
infants master is pictures. DeLoache (2006) realise that the picture only represents the
found that American infants, as well as rural real thing. They interpreted the picture
infants from a village in the Ivory Coast, symbolically, as standing for, not just being
would attempt to explore colour similar to, its referent. >>
P i a g e t ’s c o n s t r u c t i v i s t t h e o r y o f c o g n i t i v e d e v e l o p m e n t 335
<<
But researchers have found that it takes on the further assumption that such a
several years for children to completely child will be able to think of the doll as
understand the nature of pictures. Flavell a representation of him- or herself.
(cited in DeLoache, 2006) found that, Several researchers have questioned these
until the age of four, many children think assumptions, and have shown that when
that turning a picture of a bowl of preschoolers were asked to recall a visit to
popcorn upside-down will result in the the paediatrician, either using a doll or
popcorn falling out of the bowl. In young not, the children’s reports tended to be
children, symbol confusion also occurs more accurate when they were questioned
with other objects that are representative without a doll. They were more likely to
of something else, such as a model house. falsely report genital touching when a doll
Between 18 and 30 months, children was used (DeLoache, 2006). It is
typically make errors of scale with suggested that children younger than
miniature versions of real objects, for three are not able to relate their own
example attempting to sit on a miniature body to a doll.
chair or climb into a miniature car. Such The concept of dual representation has
scale errors involve a failure of dual implications for education as well.
representation—the child cannot maintain Preschool and elementary school teachers
the distinction between the symbol and its often use blocks, beads and other objects
referent. This has some important practical to stand for numerical quantities, assuming
implications. For example, the use of dolls that these concrete objects help children
when interviewing very young children in to understand abstract mathematical
cases of suspected sexual abuse, is based principles. But if children do not
on the assumption that the child will find understand the relations between the
it easier to describe what happened using objects and what they represent, such
a doll. However, this assumption is based techniques may be counter-productive.
• The one to one principle, that is, that you say only one number for each
item being counted.
336 Developmental Psychology
• The stable order principle, that is, that you say numbers in a fixed
order.
• The order irrelevance principle, that is, you can start counting with any
item and the total number will always be the same, for example, if
counting ‘Smarties’, you can begin counting with any colour ‘Smarty’,
and you will always reach the same total number (unless you eat
some in the process, of course).
• The cardinality principle, that is, the last number name that you use is
the total number of items.
• The abstraction principle, that is, that you can count all sorts of things -
sweets, people, number of times jumping up in the air (Papalia &
Olds, 1993).
more water than a wide, short glass, although the liquid was poured into
both containers from the same glass. Concrete operational children know
that the differently shaped glasses contain the same amount of liquid. They
realise how the liquid can be transformed by the shape of the container and
remember how it appeared before it was poured into the glasses. These chil-
dren display reversibility, or the ability to undo mentally the pouring process
and imagine the water back in its original container, while preoperational
children will point to one or the other glass as having more water.
Children in the concrete operational stage gradually master the con-
cept of conservation in a series of stages. For instance, the conservation of Conservation.
number and weight is usually understood first (around seven or eight A cognitive capacity
years) and conservation of area last (around 11 or 12 years). Piaget called described by Piaget as
particularly important
this inconsistency in the development of a particular cognitive ability, such
during the concrete
as conservation, horizontal decalage. Children in this stage find it difficult operational period. It
to transfer what they have learnt about one type of conservation to another refers to the ability to
type, such as from number conservation to length conservation, although the judge changes in amounts
underlying principles are the same. One explanation for this is that problems (liquid, area, volume or
of conservation, while appearing to be similar, actually differ in complexity. mass) through logical
The concrete operational child is able to understand seriation problems, deduction rather than on
the basis of appearance.
that is, he can mentally classify objects by placing them in order according
to one or more dimensions. This indicates transitive inference—the ability Seriation. The ability to
to mentally compare different objects and find similarities and differences mentally classify objects
between them. If, for example, Susan is shorter than Peter, and Peter is by placing them in order
shorter than Mary, who is the shortest? It follows logically that Susan is the (in series) according to
shortest, and the concrete operational child is able to understand the transi- one or more dimensions.
tivity of these size relationships. The child is also able to focus on more than
Transitive inference.
one feature of a problem simultaneously, an ability referred to as decentration The ability to mentally
(the opposite of centration). compare different objects
Concrete operational children, unlike preoperational children, can and find similarities and
theorise about the world. They can guess about things and test out their differences between them.
guesses, estimating, for example how many breaths of air they can blow
into a balloon before it pops or how many blue ‘Smarties’ it is necessary to
eat for their entire tongue to turn blue. But this ability to theorise is limited
to concrete objects that they can see. Children only develop theories about
abstract concepts in the formal operations stage around 11 or 12 years of age.
The skills outlined above, which are characteristic of concrete opera-
tions, do not appear all at once or over a short period of time. Piaget (1952)
maintained that operational abilities develop gradually and sequentially as
the initial, basic skills are consolidated and reorganised into increasingly
more complex mental structures.
events contrary to fact. During this stage thinking becomes more systematic
and the adolescent is able to plan and think ahead. In a study where tenth-
graders, twelfth-graders, first-year college students and final-year college
students were all asked to imagine and describe what they thought might
happen to them in the future and to say how old they thought they would be
when these events occurred, the older subjects could look farther into the
future than the younger ones and their speculations about the future were
far more specific (Greene, 1990).
Formal operational thought is characterised as a second-order process. First-
order processes of thinking entail discovering and examining relationships
between objects. Second-order processes involve thinking about your own
thoughts, looking for connections between relationships, and moving
between reality and possibility. The three main characteristics of hypothetical-
deductive formal operational thought are:
will fall back on a more concrete type of reasoning (Piaget, 1952). Does
cognitive development then end with formal operations? Many theorists
have proposed development beyond this final Piagetian stage.
* Males and females between the ages of 16 programmes for teens. Traditional
and 20 are twice as likely to be in a car intervention programmes stress the
accident than drivers aged between 20 and importance of providing information
50. Car accidents are the leading cause of about risks and allowing teens the
death among 15 to 20-year-olds in the USA, freedom to make their own decisions.
and 31 per cent of these were intoxicated at For example, it is assumed that by telling
the time. teens the risks of unprotected sex, such
* Three million adolescents contract sexually as unwanted pregnancy and HIV infection,
transmitted diseases every year. they will not engage in such behaviour.
* More than half of all new cases of HIV Reyna and Farley (2007) argue that such
infection occur in people younger than 25. programmes are flawed, not because they
* Forty per cent of adult alcoholics report that presume that teenagers will weigh risks
they started drinking between the ages of 15 against benefits and come to a rational
and 19. conclusion about their actions, but
* Ten to 14 per cent of adolescents show because the unfinished architecture of the
evidence of pathological gambling and many teen brain prevents them from thinking
report that they started gambling at 12. like adults. Recent studies have shown
(From Reyna & Farley, 2007) that teens tend to weigh the benefits
more heavily than risks when making
Part of this reckless behaviour may be the decisions. So, after carefully considering
result of brain regions associated with the pros and cons of a situation, the teen
planning, reasoning and impulse control brain tends to focus on the benefits and
only developing in early adulthood. may choose the risky situation.
This has implications for intervention
s the ability to choose the best of several possible solutions and to Relativistic thinking.
identify the criteria upon which this choice is based, and See post-formal thinking.
s recognition that a problem or solution involves inherent conflict.
described, but not organised into a sequence of within-stage levels. The same
is true of concrete and formal operations. Thus, a sequence of within-stage
levels from entry into the stage to its full mastery of its structures is lacking
for all but the first stage.
➋ How could teenagers be taught to reason like an adult in evaluating risky situations?
➌ Do you think Piaget’s theory has universal applicability? Give reasons for your answer.
Recommended readings
Piaget, J (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.
(This is quite a difficult read, in parts, but is highly recommended for the serious student of Piaget’s work,
as it gives a nearly first-hand [it is translated from the original French] description of his theorising.)
Tryphon, A & Voneche, J (1996). Piaget-Vygotsky: The Social Genesis of Thought. New York:
Psychology Press.
(This book focuses on the long and heated debate between supporters of Vygotsky and Piaget and so it is
best read after you have been through the chapter on Vygotsky’s theory. The book highlights the fact that
the two theorists actually based their theorising on a common premise, namely that knowledge is
constructed within a specific context. The book also looks at how the theories complement one another
and together provide a comprehensive account of child development. It is recommended for the
postgraduate or advanced student.)
CHAPTER
16
Intellectual development
Kate Cockcroft and Nicky Israel
Introduction
Often the terms intellectual development and cognitive development are
used interchangeably. This is because intelligence generally encompasses
all cognitive abilities (attention, perception, memory, language, concept
formation and problem solving all feed into our intellectual ability). This
chapter explores how our intellectual competence increases as we develop.
You will note, however, that a substantial portion of this chapter is devoted to
explaining the different theories of intelligence. This is necessary in order to
illustrate the extent to which the theories do or do not attempt to account for
intellectual change over time. The chapter starts with an attempt to define
what intelligence is. It then looks at the various approaches to intelligence
and its development, and finally it considers a few of the more interesting
topics that have been linked to intelligence, such as the concept of emotional
intelligence and the links between intelligence and creativity.
phers, psychologists and researchers. Here are a few of them (from Pyle,
1979: 3):
mathematical
S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S S
Special abilities
PRODUCTS
Units
Classes
Relations
Systems
Transformations
Implications
OPERATIONS
CONTENTS Evaluation
Figural Convergent production
Symbolic Divergent production
Semantic Memory
Behavioural Cognition
He argued that to group them together under broad headings such as ‘visuo-
spatial ability’ did not do justice to describing the richness and complexity
350 Developmental Psychology
1. content (what the person must think about, for example, letter
symbols or numerical symbols),
2. operations (the kind of thinking the person needs to perform, for
example, recognising information in symbolic form and holding it
in memory), and
3. products (the kind of answer that is required, for example,
speaking a word or pressing a button).
He proposed that there are five types of content, six possible operations,
and six products, which allow for up to 180 primary mental abilities (5 X
6 X 6). There is much respect for Guilford’s attempt to broaden the view
of intelligence. His model is based on evidence from genetics, neurology, the
biological sciences, and experimental psychology.
Guilford’s next task was to construct tests to measure each of his 180
mental abilities, and to date there are tests that assess over 100 of the mental
abilities in his model. However, findings have indicated that scores on these
tasks are often correlated, suggesting that the abilities they measure are not
as independent as Guilford assumed (Brody, 1992).
Fluid intelligence
Fluid intelligence or Gf .
The ability to solve novel
The final psychometric approach is that of Raymond Cattell and John Horn.
and abstract problems of They proposed that Spearman’s g and Thurstone’s primary mental abilities
the sort that are not could be divided into two major dimensions, namely fluid and crystallised
taught and are relatively intelligence (Horn & Cattell, 1982). Fluid intelligence or Gf is the ‘ability to
free of cultural influences. solve novel and abstract problems of the sort that are not taught and are
relatively free of cultural influences’ (Shaffer,1996: 336), such as verbal
Crystallised intelligence
analogies. Fluid intelligence is a measure of the influence of biological
or Gc . The ability to solve
problems that depends on
factors. It is comparable to inherited ability and is believed to flow (hence
knowledge acquired as a the term ‘fluid’) into a wide variety of intellectual activities. In contrast,
result of schooling and crystallised intelligence or Gc is ‘the ability to solve problems that depends
other life experiences. on knowledge acquired as a result of schooling and other life experiences’
Intellectual development 351
<<
relation to the suitability of IQ tests for 1992; Rushton & Skuy, 2000; Rushton,
measuring IQ in the same way across Skuy & Fridjhon, 2003 and Rushton, Skuy
different groups of people, particularly in & Bons, 2004. These researchers all
societies that are multicultural and concluded that despite the difference in
multilingual, such as South Africa. One of scores between the groups, the test
the highest priorities of practitioners and functioned similarly for the different
psychometrists in South Africa today is groups in terms of item structure, item
therefore to examine the suitability of difficulty and item loadings on ‘g’, and
specific available IQ tests for measuring IQ that therefore the test was not biased.
in a fair and unbiased way in the general Owen (1992), however, also argued that
South African population. there might be cultural differences in the
As an example, one IQ test that has way the items of the test were approached
received special attention in this regard is and solved. A preliminary study by Israel
the Raven’s Progressive Matrices, a (2006) suggested that this might be the
picture-based test that involves solving a case, as deep-seated differences in the
series of logic problems on paper. The test way the test was responded to, based on
is thus non-verbal, and is therefore seen as the home language spoken by the test
particularly useful in the South African respondent, were found. This would
context because it is assumed to be suggest that further research is necessary
reasonably free of linguistic and cultural to establish more clearly the degree to
biases. Despite this, the same difference in which both nature and nurture affect IQ
performance between white and non- and its measurement, particularly in
white groups has been found when relation to using IQ tests across different
administering the RPM to South African cultural contexts.
samples, as seen in research by Owen,
a correlation of r = 0.5 only accounts for about 25 per cent of the variation in
individual performances, leaving 75 per cent unexplained. Therefore, there
is much more to school performance than IQ. The predictive validity of IQ
tests decreases when they are used to forecast performance in later life, such as
salary, job performance, or even the likelihood of obtaining a job. Generally,
the correlations are slightly over r = 0.3, which means that the tests account
for approximately ten per cent of the variation in people’s performance, while
90 per cent remains unexplained (Ceci, 1990). Further, IQ prediction is even
less effective when populations, situations or tasks change. Fiedler (cited
in Ceci, 1990) found that IQ positively predicts leadership success under con-
ditions of low stress, but in high stress situations the tests negatively predict
leadership success. Research has shown that the IQ scores of most students
improve when they are taught to think analytically, creatively and practically
(Sternberg, 1998). Despite this, the actual contents of IQ tests have changed
very little since the beginning of the 20th century. What does an IQ represent
then, if not one’s intellectual ability? Many researchers in the field believe
that an IQ score is just an estimate of a person’s performance at one particular
point in time, that is, when the test was taken (Shaffer, 1996).
Cognitive approach.
Theories of intelligence Narrow definition of intelligence
that seek to understand Psychometric definitions of intelligence have been criticised for being too
intelligence in terms of
narrow. They focus predominantly on what the test taker ‘knows’, rather
actual knowledge and
underlying reasoning
than on the process whereby this knowledge is acquired, stored and
processes. manipulated in problem solving. More recently, tests such as The Kaufman
Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983)
and the Cognitive Assessment System (CAS) (Das, Naglieri & Kirby, 1994)
have been developed as attempts to assess the processes involved in intelligent
behaviours. However, although they are tests of intellectual ability, these
tests do not fall strictly within the psychometric domain, as they are based
on theories of cognitive processing. They form part of the growing group
of tests developed as a reaction to the narrowness of the psychometric
understanding of intelligence, and fall under the cognitive approach
to intelligence.
by previous theorists. Sternberg believes that skills necessary for effective Triarchic theory.
performance in the real world are just as important as the more limited Sternberg’s theory of
skills assessed by traditional intelligence tests. He proposes a triarchic intelligence which
emphasises three inter-
theory of intelligence that, as its name implies, emphasises three
related components of
interrelated components of intelligence. Sternberg presents these as the intelligence, designated as
componential, experiential and contextual sub-theories. the componential,
experiential and
• The componential sub-theory addresses the mental processes that are contextual sub-theories.
emphasised by most theories of intelligence, namely the ability to
acquire new knowledge and to solve problems effectively, that is, it Componential sub-
looks at what happens inside a person’s head, when he or she thinks theory. That part of
intelligently. Sternberg’s triarchic
theory of intelligence that
• The experiential sub-theory deals with the ability to adjust to new
considers the various
tasks, to use new concepts, to adapt creatively in new situations and types of ‘components’ or
to use insight. mental operations that
• The contextual sub-theory considers people’s ability to select contexts individuals use in problem
in which they can excel, to capitalise on strengths and compensate for solving. This sub-theory
weaknesses, and to shape the environment to fit their strengths includes three types of
(Sternberg 1984). components: knowledge
acquisition, performance
and metacomponents.
The componential sub-theory
The componential sub-theory is the most highly elaborated of the sub-
Metacomponents. In the
theories. It considers the various types of components or mental operations componential sub-theory
that individuals use in problem solving. The component is the basic level of of Sternberg’s triarchic
analysis in Sternberg’s theory and is defined as ‘an elementary information theory of intelligence,
process that operates upon internal representations of objects and symbols’ these components are
(Sternberg, 1985: 97). Sternberg further specifies three broad kinds of involved in planning,
component, which are interrelated: monitoring and evaluating
the processing that occurs
during problem solving.
• Metacomponents play a supervisory, decision-making role in problem
solving. They are used to plan, monitor and evaluate processing dur- Performance
ing problem solving. These components are responsible for allocating components. In the
attentional resources to various aspects of task processing. componential sub-theory
• Performance components carry out problem-solving strategies of Sternberg’s triarchic
specified by the metacomponents. They can be subdivided into: theory of intelligence,
these components are
1. Encoding components, in which the sensory information
involved in executing
is defined and represented in the information-processing cognitive tasks.
system,
2. components involved in the combination of, or comparisons
Knowledge-acquisition
between sensory stimuli, and components. In the
3. response components, which provide the necessary response to componential sub-theory
the problem. of Sternberg’s triarchic
• Knowledge-acquisition components selectively encode, combine and theory of intelligence,
compare information during the course of problem solving, thereby these components are
bringing about new learning (Sternberg, 1984). involved in the acquisition
of knowledge, which
entails selective encoding,
For example, when writing an essay, metacomponents help you to selective combination
choose a topic, organise the paper, monitor the writing and evaluate the final and comparison
work. Knowledge-acquisition components enable you to carry out research of information.
356 Developmental Psychology
for the paper. Performance components are involved in the writing of the
paper, in searching for appropriate words and phrases and retrieving them
from memory.
Multiple intelligences
Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences (MI theory) is less mechanistic Multiple intelligences.
than the triarchic theory. Gardner (1984) challenges the assumption that Gardner’s theory of
there is a single general intelligence, or g, which is believed to be reflected intelligence, which holds
that human intelligence
by an individual’s IQ. The basic tenet of the theory of multiple intelligences
encompasses at least eight
is, as its name implies, that human intelligence encompasses at least eight different kinds of
different kinds of competencies. These include what are traditionally competencies. These
regarded as intelligence, such as linguistic, logical-mathematical and spatial include linguistic, logical-
abilities, as well as other less traditional conceptions of intelligence, such as mathematical, musical,
musical and kinaesthetic capabilities. For Gardner, each form of intelligence naturalist, spatial, bodily-
represents a modular, brain-based capacity. These intelligences do not kinaesthetic, interpersonal
and intra-personal abilities.
always reveal themselves in traditional paper-and-pencil tests.
Gardner’s theory is based on extensive research into the nature of
intelligence. In order to define the various intelligences possessed by
358 Developmental Psychology
humans, he developed the following set of conditions and criteria that each
distinct intelligence had to meet:
Definition of intelligence
Gardner’s theory has been criticised in particular for including human
characteristics that are not typically considered to be mental operations, such
as athletic ability and bodily control. This returns us to the question asked at
the beginning of this chapter, namely, ‘What is intelligence?’
* An average IQ ranges between 90 and * Rather than being bad for children,
110, while MENSA level IQ (top 2 per some research suggests computer
cent of the population) is usually a games may actually help to develop
score of above 132. One report states their IQ by improving their critical
that Albert Einstein, famous for his thinking and memory skills.
theory of relativity, had an estimated
IQ of 160. * Contrary to popular belief, it is not
possible to make a baby cleverer by
* Children’s brains are far more efficient playing him/her classical music in
at learning than adults, and very the womb, and there is no scientific
recent research suggests that the first evidence suggesting exposure to
two years of life may be the most classical music has any long-term
critical learning period of all. effect on IQ.
Figure 16.3 Left: MRI scan of a normal adult brain. Right: MRI scan of a patient with Alzheimer’s disease,
showing the deterioration of the cerebral cortex.
Source: Lahey, B. B. (2001). Psychology: An introduction. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Amabile (1993) has been studying creativity in children for many years.
Her research indicates that the following circumstances are most likely to
destroy children’s natural creativity:
Specific tasks
➊ Definitions of intelligence are difficult to obtain and rarely agreed upon. Try C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
to create your own definition of intelligence. Examine your definition to try to
establish what ‘signals’ you have included and reflect on why this might be the
case. For example, if you have included academic performance in your definition,
is this because people around you consider people who do well in school to be
intelligent?
➌ Identify the main differences and similarities between the psychometric and
cognitive approaches to intelligence.
➎ Critically discuss the role each psychometric and cognitive theory presented in the chapter
might play in accounting for or explaining the process of intellectual development.
>>
364 Developmental Psychology
<<
➏ Critically analyse the similarities between aspects of Gardner’s theory of multiple
intelligences and the concept of emotional intelligence.
➐ How does creativity differ from intelligence? Think up some tasks that could be
used to assess creativity and explain whether and why these would also be suitable
for assessing intelligence as well—or not.
Recommended readings
Anderson, M (ed). (2001). The Development of Intelligence. London: Psychology Press.
(This edited collection by contemporary theorists in the field covers a wide range of theories and research
related to the development of intelligence and is an excellent field-specific resource.)
Brody, N (1992). Intelligence. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
(A detailed account of the difficulties in defining and measuring intelligence.)
Ceci, S J (1990). On intelligence, more or less. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
(A discussion of the development of theories of intelligence and their value in modern society.)
Gardner, H (1998). ‘A multiplicity of intelligences’, Scientific American, 9 (4), 19-23.
(An easy-to-read, first-hand explanation of Gardner’s multiple intelligence theory).
Neisser, U, Boodoo, G, Bouchard Jr, T J, Wade Boykin, A, Brody, N, Ceci, S J, Halpern,
D F, Loehlin, J C, Perloff, R, Sternberg, R J & Urbina, S (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and
Unknowns. American Psychologist, 51 (2), 77-101.
(Although this article does not cover the topic of intellectual development per se, it is widely recognised
as a seminal paper in the broader area of intelligence. Compiled by a number of the current leading
researchers in the field, the paper provides a comprehensive overview of the key issues and debates
coherently and sensibly, and indirectly raises a number of topics pertinent to intellectual development.)
Sternberg, R J (ed). (2000). The Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
(This edited collection provides a comprehensive overview of current research and theory in the field of
intelligence, although it does not address developmental issues specifically.)
CHAPTER
17
Memory development
Kate Cockcroft
1. What is memory?
2. Models of memory
* Schema theory
* Semantic network models
* Connectionist models
3. The development of memory
4. The development of memory strategies
5. Metacognition
6. Memory in later life
What is memory?
When I used to grieve for my mother, and later for my aunt, I told myself
that although they were certainly as dead as they were ever going to be they Short-term
were still mine, that they inhabited my interior world, which was at least memory. That
memory system
as noisy and various as life itself. From early on I valued the gift of memory
which stores
above all others. I understood that as we grow older we carry a whole information over
nation around inside of us, with places and ways that have disappeared, very brief intervals
believing that they are ours, that we alone hold the torch for our past, of time (usually
that we are as unpenetrable as stone. Memory still seems a gift to me and seconds).
I hold tight to those few things that are forever gone and always a part of
Long-term
me ... (Hamilton, 1994:39).
memory.
Information that is
People often think of scenes from their past when thinking about stored for longer
‘memory’. However, these ‘autobiographical’ memories are not the only than a few seconds.
type of memory we possess. First, there is the well-known, distinction
Episodic memory.
between long- and short-term memory. An example would be looking
Memory for personal
up a telephone number and remembering it for just long enough to be able to experiences and
dial it (short-term memory), as opposed to recalling an event from childhood events that have
(long-term memory) (Greene & Hicks, 1984). Tulving (1972) distinguished happened in an
between two types of long-term memory, namely episodic memory, which is individual’s life.
366 Developmental Psychology
Semantic memory. our memory for personal experiences and events that have happened in our
Memory of general life, and semantic memory, which contains our general knowledge.
knowledge or Long-term memory has also been separated into explicit and implicit
factual information.
memory, where explicit memory is your conscious memory of a specific event
Explicit memory.
or fact, usually required to fulfil some direct goal (for example answering the
Conscious memory of a question, ‘What was the name of that movie with Johnny Depp in it where
specific event or fact. he was a pirate?’ or remembering your first day at university). Implicit
memory, on the other hand, is memory without awareness, usually where
Implicit memory. a ‘part’ memory is triggered by a priming (triggering) event, but the actual
Memory without memory is not explicitly recalled. For example, the neurologist, Klaperede
awareness, usually
(Santrock, 1999), recounts the story of a patient with anterograde amnesia
where ‘part’ memory
can be triggered but the
who had to visit him on a weekly basis. Due to her amnesia, the patient was
actual memory is not unable to remember who Klaperede was. To test whether she had implicit
explicitly recalled. memories, Klaperede hid a pin in his hand and when he shook her hand,
she was pricked by the pin. The next day she had forgotten who Klaperede
was, and when she came into the room he held out his hand to shake hers
but she didn’t want to, even though she could not explain why. Implicitly,
she knew that something unpleasant would occur if she shook his hand.
Implicit and explicit memories are sometimes further divided into
Procedural memory. procedural and declarative memories. Procedural memory concerns knowing
An implicit memory that how to do something, and is thus usually an implicit memory, for example
involves knowing how to knowing how to drive a car. Declarative memories are conscious recollections
do something, for example
of specific facts or events, and are therefore usually explicit memories, in fact
knowing how to drive
a car. the terms are often used interchangeably. There is a further refinement of
declarative memory into episodic and semantic memories. Episodic memory
Declarative memory. refers to memories for specific personal experiences and events that have
Concious recollection of happened in your life (such as having breakfast this morning). These memories
specific facts or events. are usually linked to a specific time-frame and the ‘truth’, or validity, of the
memory depends on the strength of the individual’s belief in that memory.
Semantic memory contains your general knowledge about the world,
for example, that Nelson Mandela was South Africa’s first democratically
elected president. Episodic memories may lead to the formation of semantic
memories or be used to infer semantic memories.
EXPLICIT/DECLARATIVE IMPLICIT/NON-DECLARATIVE
(conscious) (not conscious)
Figure 17.1 Hierarchical depiction of the different types of long-term memory. Adapted from
Goldstein, EB (2008). Cognitive Psychology. Connecting Mind, research and Everyday Experience.
Instructors Resource CD. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.
Memory development 367
Models of memory
Most work on human memory has taken place within the information-
processing framework. Information-processing theorists define memory
as ‘the capacity for storing and retrieving information’ (Baddeley, 1982:
11). This framework conceptualises memory as a series of complex, inter-
connected processes that take in, store, and retrieve information about
the world. The process of taking in sensory information about the world
and converting it into a memory is referred to as encoding. Storage is the
way in which the information is represented in the brain and retrieval is Multistore model of
how information in memory is made available to the individual. Failure memory. An information
processing model of
to remember could be the result of a problem at any of these three stages: memory that proposes
encoding, storage, or retrieval. several different stores
The distinction between long- and short-term memory led within memory.
researchers to develop a multistore model of memory. It is called ‘multi-
store’ because it proposes
several different stores
within memory. The Sensory Short-term Long-term
Stimulus
most common version processes memory memory
input
(SM) (STM) (LTM)
is that of Atkinson &
Shiffrin (1968), a basic
information-processing
model that conceptualises
Response
the memory system in
terms of three main
processes or stores (see Figure 17.2 Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model of memory.
Figure 17.2).
First, information that is perceived by the senses is held briefly in the
sensory store (called iconic memory when referring to sensory visual mem- Sensory store. Information
ory, and echoic memory when referring to sensory auditory memory). The that is perceived by the
information here is in its raw state—the only thing that has been done to senses is held here very
briefly. (Sensory memory
it is to convert it from information in the environment, such as sounds or
is called iconic memory
light, into electrical activity in the brain. Information in this state is still when referring to sensory
relatively meaningless. Information from the eyes, for example, arrives in visual memory, and echoic
the brain in the form of ‘blobs’ and ‘edges’ of light and dark. Information memory when referring to
that is selected for further processing enters the short-term store, where it sensory auditory memory.)
is processed further. The short-term store, as its name implies, only holds
information briefly, between six and 12 seconds. However, this can be Iconic memory.
extended by using various strategies such as rehearsal, where you repeat See sensory store.
the contents of the short-term store either out aloud or mentally in your
head. (Memory strategies and their development will be discussed later Echoic memory.
See sensory store.
in this chapter.) The use of various strategies to increase memory reten-
tion results in the information being stored indefinitely in the long-term
store. Schema theory provides a useful explanation for the understanding Schema theory. Bartlett’s
of long-term memory. theory that we construct
schemata (units of
information about typical
Schema theory events), for example, we
Schema theory was developed by Frederick Bartlett (1932) who proposed have a schema for a typical
that memories are constructed, rather than being exact replicas of the movie theatre and what
information that is stored. The reconstructive nature of memory is occurs there.
368 Developmental Psychology
evident when you think about how two people may have experienced
the same event, yet their recollections of that event can show great
Shemata. Discrete units of deviations. Memory schemata are discrete units of information that relate
information that relate to a to a typical object or event. Schemata incorporate everything we know
typical object or event. about a subject or object, for instance by now you should have a fairly
well-defined schema about lectures, and how they typically proceed.
In addition to supplying a base of knowledge, schemata also provide a
framework within which new information can be processed. Schema-
Scripts. like memories for events (called scripts) influence how we remember
General remembered events. A script for a restaurant might include information about food,
outline of a familiar
waiters or waitresses, paying the bill, and so on.
repeated event, used
to guide behaviour.
Semantic network models
Schemata are not represented as separate from one another. Rather,
Semantic network. they are depicted in memory as a massive interconnected web of
A large interconnected information, called semantic networks. Collins & Loftus (1975) were
web of schemata. interested in the links between concepts stored in memory, rather than
the actual concepts, and this led to the development of a semantic
Nodes. The concepts
(words, numbers, ideas)
network model of memory. In semantic networks, the connections
that are linked in a between the concepts (or nodes as they are referred to) in the network
semantic network. specify the relationship between them. So, for example, the link
between ‘apple’ and ‘fruit’ would be an ‘is a’ link. The links also have
varying strengths, since concepts can be strongly, weakly, or moderately
Two people can have
distinctly different related to one another (with many gradations in between). Apples, for
memories of the same example, are always fruit, so the link between apple and fruit would
event, demonstrating be very strong. The link between ‘apple’ and ‘green’ would be weaker,
the reconstructive nature since not all apples are green.
of memory.
Semantic networks illustrate the associative and
interconnected nature of memory. All concepts are
PHOTO: KATE COCKCROFT
Connectionist models
McClelland & Rumelhart (1985) describe a basic computational model of
conceptual memory that explains how object schemata are represented in
human memory. Their model illustrates some of the important aspects of
connectionism. Just as sensory information is converted into electrical activity
in the brain, so McClelland and Rumelhart’s model processes information
in a numerical way. The network consists of 24 units, each connected to
the others by a link, which has a strength or weight. Concepts that are
recognised by the network are converted into numbers, depending on what
the concept is. Activation is also sent to all other units in the network that
PHO TO : K ATE CO CK CRO FT 370 Developmental Psychology
Memory in childhood
What is your earliest childhood memory and when did it occur? Until quite
recently, it was believed that most people are unable to recall specific events
that occurred before their third birthday. This was called infantile amnesia. Infantile amnesia.
Freud (1953: 175), who coined the term, said that the first years of life showed Freud’s term for the
‘the remarkable amnesia of childhood ... the forgetting which veils our individual’s inability to
recall specific events that
earliest youth from us and makes us strangers to it.’ He believed that the
occured before their
experiences of the first years of life are not actually forgotten, but are instead third birthday.
merely inaccessible to our conscious awareness due to repression. According
to Freud, many of our desires and fantasies are very erotic in nature at
this age and these sexual feelings conflict with the realities of everyday life.
To resolve this conflict, we repress this whole period of our lives into the
unconscious mind. However, stripped of its psychoanalytic trimmings,
Freud’s view basically maintains that infantile amnesia is due to retrieval
failure. The experiences are still represented in memory but the individual
cannot gain access to them. It is supposed that retrieval failure occurs because
the context at the time of the original learning is very different from the con-
text at the time of attempted retrieval (often decades later).
It has also been suggested that young children organise, encode and
store information in very different ways from adults. For example, adults
and older children use language to a much greater extent than do infants.
372 Developmental Psychology
Encoding specificity For instance, if asked to recall your second birthday, you would probably
principle. The likelihood have difficulty doing so because the memories were probably not originally
of retrieving information encoded and stored verbally. In order to retrieve any information about
from long-term memory
your second birthday, you will need to reinstate some of the original
will be maximised if the
conditions at retrieval
context, to look, for example, at a photo or video of your party. This
match as closely as illustrates the encoding specificity principle (Tulving, 1972), that is, that
possible the conditions the likelihood of retrieving information from long-term memory will be
present during the maximised if the conditions at retrieval match as closely as possible the
original learning. conditions present during the original learning. Remember this when
studying for exams—make your study conditions as similar as possible to
the exam conditions.
Children as young as two years old have been shown to have memo-
ries of events that occurred months earlier. However these memories often
Implicit memories. do not last for a long time, because they are implicit memories, that is,
Memories that can memories that can produce behavioural change without intentional recall
produce behavioural or conscious awareness of the memory. Examples of this are the knowledge
change without intentional
of how to kick a ball or drive a car. It is thought that the brain structures
recall or conscious
awareness of the memory.
necessary for implicit memory develop before those necessary for explicit
Examples are how to kick a memory, that is, memories for factual knowledge such as names of people
ball or drive a car. and events. Consequently, most people are unable to retrieve explicit
memories that occurred before the age of three. The rapid development
Explicit memory. of language from three to six years enables the child to store information
Memories for factual for longer periods than previously, when information was stored mainly
knowledge such as names
in a visual format.
of people and events.
that the infant lacks the capacity to recall it. It is possible that they may
not understand what is required of them, or that the experimenter’s actions
are too complex for the infant’s motor-skill level. Nevertheless, the research
indicates that the ability to recall prior experiences seems to emerge in the
first year of life and increases gradually thereafter.
While preschool children are readily able to recognise things, their recall
is much poorer but this improves with age. In a recognition task, in which
many objects were shown only once to children between the ages of two
and five years, even the youngest could correctly recognise 81 per cent of the
objects when placed with other objects, while the older children were able to
recognise 92 per cent of the objects correctly. This shows that young children
are able to encode and retain substantial amounts of information. In recall
studies, when children between two and four were asked to name objects
the experimenter had shown them, the three-year-olds could only name 22
per cent of the objects, while the four-year-olds could name 40 per cent
(Myers & Perlmutter, 1978). This finding may reflect increasing vocabulary,
which may be a confounding factor in this study.
Experiences early in life impact on how a person develops subsequently.
But, experiences can only have impact if they are stored in memory.
Consequently, infants with better memories should be able to learn from
experience better than those with poorer memories. This line of reasoning
led to the hypothesis that measures of early memory ability should be
related to later cognitive development. In support of this hypothesis, Rose &
Wallace (1985) found that recognition ability at six months was positively and
significantly related to intelligence at two, three and six years of age.
bodies by remembering where they had been at the time when he left.
Consequent to this, Simonides speculated that since his visual memory
appeared to be so good, why not use it to recall other material? He
then devised a system in which he visualised a room in great detail,
and then imagined various items in specific places in the room.
Whenever he needed to remember what the items were, he would
look into his room, and find them at the appropriate location. This
system became very popular with classical orators such as Cicero, and
has continued in use to the present day (Baddeley, 1982). Baddeley
(1982: 196) illustrates how it works:
Cultures differ in the extent
to which they encourage First of all, think of ten locations in your home, choosing them so
the use of particular that the sequence of moving from one to the other is an obvious
memory strategies.
one—for example, front door to entrance hall, to kitchen, to bed-
room, and so on. Check that you can imagine moving through your
ten locations in a consistent order without difficulty. Now think of
10 items and imagine them in those locations. If the first item is
a pipe, you might imagine it poking out of the letterbox of your
front door and great clouds of smoke billowing into the street. If
the second is a cabbage, you might imagine your hall obstructed by
an enormous cabbage, and so on.
Vygotsky made use of mnemonics to help Then we would ask him to reproduce the
him remember information. His daughter 17th, 43rd, 61st, 7th and so on, word, and he,
wrote this about his ability: without difficulty and without any mis-
takes, would do it. (Vygodskaia, 1995: 58).
Right there, by the stove, Lev Semenovich
would demonstrate to us his ability to The secret to Vygotsky’s amazing memory
remember large numbers of words. We was his use of visual imagery. Not only could
would, working together, compile a list of he easily create a wealth of images, he also
100 words and hand it over to Lev made use of synaesthesia or the ability for a
Semenovich. He would slowly read each stimulus in one sense (for example vision or
word, return the list, and then offer to recite hearing) to evoke an image in another, for
it in any order. To our amazement and joy, example associating high-pitched sounds
he would, without mistake, repeat all the with bright colours and low-pitched sounds
words on the list from beginning to end, with more sombre colours.
and then repeat them in the reverse order.
Metacognition
Metacognition. Metacognition refers to the sophisticated intellectual processes that enable
The sophisticated people to be aware of their own thinking, memory (metamemory), and
intellectual processes language (metalinguistic) abilities. Between the ages of six to 12, children
that enable people to
develop metacognitive abilities, which they use to help them with problem
be aware of and reflect
on their own thinking,
solving and decision making. A simple example of this is a study where
memory (metamemory), preoperational and concrete operational children were all given a group
and language of items to study until they felt they could remember them perfectly. After
(metalinguistic) abilities. studying the items for a while, the concrete operational children said they
were ready, and they usually were. When tested, they could remember
all of the items. The preoperational children did not perform as well,
although they also assured the researchers that they knew all the items.
Thus, their metacognitive abilities or awareness of their own cognitive
processes and ability to monitor these abilities were poorer than those of
the concrete operational children (Kail, 1979).
The major change in memory with age is a decline in the ability to recall
things explicitly. Therefore, memory for day-to-day events decreases consid-
erably and older people, in laboratory experiments, often perform poorly at
the recall of lists of words or pictures. However, recognition memory does
not show such dramatic declines with age. While explicit memory appears to
decline with age, implicit memory appears to remain intact in older people.
At the simplest level, our memories fail with increasing age because we
lose neurons, resulting in reduced processing capacity. In order to function
effectively, our mental processes must attend to what we are doing and
actively ignore irrelevant information (a process called inhibition). In relation
to memory, a deficit in inhibition could impair memory, leading people to
concentrate less effectively on what they are trying to remember. Ageing
selectively causes neuron depletion in the frontal lobes of the brain, which are
While some memory loss is
inevitable in old age, responsible for controlling inhibition (Hasher & Zacks, 1988). Another theory
staying mentally active can of ageing and memory, the processing speed hypothesis, is that our memories
considerably lessen this become poorer because our brains just get slower at operating (Salthouse,
loss. 1997). A problem with this hypothesis is that the tasks used to support it may
Memory development 377
only be measuring fluid intelligence (the ability to think flexibly and solve
problems) and not crystallised intelligence (knowledge of language and basic
concepts). Fluid intelligence is known to decline markedly with age, while
crystallised intelligence does not (Parkin & Java, 1999).
A severe deterioration in memory in old age is often a precursor
to senile dementia or organic brain syndrome, ultimately resulting in
intellectual handicap. The forgetfulness may seem at first quite mild—
forgetting where one has put objects, or the times or dates of appointments.
It can, however, become so severe that the afflicted individual is unable Senile dementia.
to lead a normal life. Baddeley (1982: 141) cites the case of an old lady Cognitive decline in old
suffering from senile dementia who agreed to have a nurse stay with age resulting in confusion,
her at night, then forgot all about this arrangement so that when the forgetfulness, and
nurse arrived, she was treated with suspicion, locked out, and the police personality change
telephoned. Senile dementia is progressive and typically occurs shortly (also known as organic
brain syndrome).
before death in very old people. Riegel & Riegel (1982) studied many
elderly people, testing their intellectual and physical abilities over several
years. They suggested that mental ability declined fairly gradually in old
age up to a point approximately a year before death, when deterioration
became rapid, a phenomenon referred to as terminal drop.
For those who are not afflicted with dementia, there is hope. Studies
of animals suggest that an enriched environment results in the loss of
fewer neurons as age progresses. A few studies have compared elderly
individuals living in institutionalised settings with those living more
active lives in the community. On various tests of memory, the active
community-dwelling elderly showed fewer decrements. The answer
then seems to be: keep mentally active.
➋ What function does memory play in cultures with a strong oral tradition?
➌ Think back to a memory of a shared event that happened at least five years ago and
write it down. Then speak to a person who shared that event and ask them to write
down their memory of it. What does your comparison of the two recollections tell
you about the reconstructive nature of memory?
Recommended readings
Baddeley, A (1982). Your memory: A user’s guide. London: Multimedia Publications.
(An accessible, introductory text to the structure and functioning of short- and long-term memory.)
Loftus, E (1994). The myth of repressed memory. New York: St Martin’s Press.
(A fascinating, and somewhat chilling, account of how people delude themselves into believing in
memories for events that did not exist.)
CHAPTER
18
Language development
Kate Cockcroft
What is language?
Language involves the use of a shared set of symbols (letters, words,
gestures, icons) for communicating information. Vygotsky (1962)
described language as one of the primary mental tools that connects the
human mind to the world. Learning to use this tool requires the child to
master an extremely complex linguistic system. This task is particularly
difficult because language is arbitrary, often with no overt connection
between the symbol (the word) and the object or idea to which it refers.
So, the meanings of words, particularly abstract ones, are often difficult
for the child to discover. Having overcome this obstacle and discovered
what certain words mean, the child then has to determine the complex
ways in which particular words may be combined to form sentences.
The child does not only have to learn how to produce sentences of his
own, but also how to discover the meaning of sentences which other
people use. Despite the difficulty of these tasks, most children learn to
talk, and by five years of age, most children are able to use language with
a great deal of skill.
Learning theory
B F Skinner (1957) extended his model of operant conditioning in
order to explain language acquisition. Operant is the term he uses to Operant conditioning.
define behavioural responses of the subject (or child) that influence his A term used by B. F.
environment. In other words, such responses are associated more with Skinner for responses
that are reinforced by
their consequences (their effect), than with their cause or origin; they may
their consequences.
be seen as emitted rather than elicited responses. (Operant conditioning
is the process by which selected operant responses are reinforced by Operant response.
the caregivers.) Skinner claimed that children learn language through This is a kind of
imitating their caregivers, who, through reinforcement, also shape the behavioural response—
child’s initially incorrect attempts at speech so that these eventually become identified and defined by
‘adult-like’. According to Skinner, utterances that are not reinforced Skinner—which leads to
a certain consequence. In
gradually decline in frequency until they stop occurring altogether. The
other words, the emphasis,
remaining utterances are then shaped with reinforcers until they are in such behaviour, is on its
correct. For example, as children grow older, parents may use shaping and effect rather than on
insist on closer and closer approximations of the word ‘chocolate’ before its cause.
supplying the requested treat.
Skinner did not believe that animals could be taught to speak
through schedules of reinforcement, because animals use vocal expres-
sion in set ways to express emotional states such as fear or rage and
they also lack the necessary articulatory organs for producing many
of the sounds in human language (Skinner, 1957). It seems that,
according to Learning Theory, most early vocabulary is learned
through imitation, with the infant trying to copy the adult, saying
for example ‘airpha’ for aeroplane. However, this approach fails
to explain how the child comes up with novel and grammatically
incorrect sayings that he could not have imitated from anyone else,
for example ‘that man growed outwards’. Also, when the child is
very young, parents tend to reinforce all utterances, even
the grammatically incorrect ones, tending to correct
the content, rather than the grammar of what is
being said. For example, the child may say ‘That
dog eated the food’, and the parent may respond,
‘No, it’s not a dog, it’s a horse’ rather than saying,
‘We don’t say eated, we say ate’ (Craig, 1996).
Nativism
Noam Chomsky (1959) held that language is creative. B.F. Skinner
This means that a person who knows a language does not just know how
to speak or understand a limited number of spoken, written or signed
messages. Rather, a language user knows how to produce and understand
an infinite number of messages, including messages that they have never
encountered previously and could therefore not have copied from their
caregivers. The creativity in language is possible because the individual
units of a language, namely words (which are finite in number), can be
combined in an infinite number of ways.
380 Developmental Psychology
<<
with a distinct area for the second multilinguals may have been depressed for
language. These findings may account for reasons other than their multilingualism,
the apparent ease with which a second such as poverty, poor education, or lack of
language is learnt in early childhood. familiarity with the culture of test taking.
Does learning two languages instead of More recent research indicates that
one during the preschool years hinder the learning more than one language in
child’s language acquisition or cognitive early childhood has cultural, linguistic
development? Early studies in the United and cognitive advantages (Diaz, 1985).
States of America and Great Britain found Young children may be at a disadvantage
that learning two or more languages at while acquiring two (or more) languages,
a young age could have a detrimental but once they have integrated the two
effect on cognitive development, languages, they often surpass their
because multilingual children scored monolingual peers linguistically and
lower on standardised English tests than cognitively (Goncz, 1988). Studies have
monolingual English-speaking children. found that bilingual children are more
However, none of these studies took into creative both linguistically and cognitively,
account differences in socio-economic or are more adept at divergent thinking, and
educational level of the children or their better at concept formation than their
parents. It is possible that the scores of monolingual peers.
Social interactionism
This approach is less clearly defined than the others we have mentioned so
far. Lev Vygotsky and Jerome Bruner are both social interactionist theorists
and stress the importance of the interpersonal context in which language
appears. According to this approach, the baby learns about language via his
382 Developmental Psychology
Connectionism
Connectionism has as its starting point the human neural system, which has
the ability to form and retain networks of interconnected neurons. Babies
are born with such a system, which continues to develop throughout life.
Lev Vygotsky
According to the connectionist view, neural systems are constructed on the
basis of what happens to the developing person’s senses. As more sensory
information becomes available, the neural system will be extended and
modified to incorporate the new sensory material. An aspect of language
that has been widely researched using connectionist models is the learning
of the English tense system. The usual explanation is that children have
to learn a rule that, in order to refer to the past, verbs must be given an
‘-ed’ suffix. They must also learn that there are many irregular exceptions
in English, for instance, ‘go’ becomes ‘went’, and ‘see’ becomes ‘saw’.
Children have to learn and store the rule and its exceptions. Children in
the early stages of learning English tend to overuse the rule and add ‘-ed’
to both regular and irregular verbs. For example they may say ‘goed’ and
‘seed’ instead of ‘went’ and ‘saw’.
b/1 c/1 m/1 t/1 a/2 e/2 i/2 n/2 t/3 n/3 r/3 s/3
Figure 18.1 An example of a three-layered connectionist network. The bottom layer contains the units that
represent particular graphemes (written units) in particular positions within a word. The middle layer
contains units that recognise complete words, and the top layer contains units that represent semantic
features concerning the meaning of the word.
Source: Cognitive Psychology: A student’s handbook by Michael W Eysenck & Mark T Keane, Lawrence
Earlbaum, 1990: 230.
Language development 383
Before words
At birth, the most obvious vocalisations that the baby makes are crying, and
by two months, also cooing. The baby’s cooing contains the basic sounds of
all languages, not just those of the baby’s native language.
PHOTO: KATE COCKCROFT
Telegraphic speech
Eventually that first wonderful word is spoken, when the child is
approximately a year old. The first words are usually concrete nouns
spoken in the presence of the objects they represent. During the next six
to eight months, children build up a vocabulary of one-word sentences Holophrases. The young
child’s first ‘sentences’,
called holophrases such as ‘up!’ and ‘more!’ They may also compound
which consist of single
(join) words such as ‘awgone’ (all gone). By the age of two years, the child words such as ‘up!’ and
is generally able to create two-word combinations such as ‘Mommy gone’, ‘more!’
which are primitive sentences and which soon expand in length. The
rules of syntax start to appear, for example English-speaking children Telegraphic speech.
always say ‘see car’ and not ‘car see’. Brown (1965) found that children at The young child’s stage
this age seize on the most important parts of speech, those that contain of language development
where simple sentences are
the most meaning, called contentives (nouns, verbs, adjectives) and tend
formed. These sentences
to omit inflections, auxiliary verbs, prepositions and articles, which he include only the most
called functors. The result is what Brown (1965) called telegraphic speech, important words, for
which omits less significant words and includes words that carry the most example ‘Mommy
meaning, for example ‘Mommy give bopple’. give bopple’.
386 Developmental Psychology
hArRoDs
earlier. The child’s responses are no longer only selected from his reading
vocabulary but may also be words that have not been learned directly or
may even be non-words, as the child is now able to sound out words. This
leads to a rapid expansion in the number of words the child is able to read
aloud correctly. This behaviour indicates a move from the discrimination-
net stage to the phonological recoding stage. In the phonological recoding
stage, the child makes use of translating letters into sounds and also learns
the visual representations of words where the spelling prevents them from
being translated directly into the correct sounds, for example with the
words ‘rough’ and ‘yacht’. Evidence shows that the tendency to translate
letters into sounds dominates at this stage (Doctor & Coltheart, 1980).
<<
tongue either a Sotho or Nguni language, methodological problems, yet the data
which are highly transparent in indicate that a stage model based on the
comparison to English, which has a very reading development of monolingual,
opaque orthography (writing system). In English children cannot effectively account
transparent orthographies, such as Italian, for reading development in EL2 children.
Afrikaans, isiZulu, Sesotho (in fact, all of Rather, theories of reading development
the indigenous South African languages), should take the form of flexible
the spelling-sound relationship is relatively frameworks that can accommodate
consistent, so that words are generally different developmental pathways related
spelled as they sound. In an opaque to different methods of instruction.
orthography, such as English, the spelling- Beyond that, models of reading
sound correspondence is more indirect, development also need to accommodate
that is, there are many words with the influence of different oral language
grapheme-phoneme correspondences that backgrounds, as well as consider the
are irregular or not 1:1, such as ‘yacht’ relationship between the child’s two (or
and ‘steak’. Depth of orthography is not more) orthographies.
absolute, but should be understood as a The results of the study also showed
continuum, with some orthographies that knowledge of letter sounds and
at the more transparent end and others at names is one of the strongest initial
the more opaque end. With this in mind, predictors of developing word and non-
the EL1 and EL2 children in the study would word reading ability in both the EL1 and
have had different skills and expectations EL2 samples. Further, letter knowledge
regarding spoken language, which were and short-term phonological memory
likely to impact on the process of learning appear to be mediating factors in the
to read in English. The children were relationship between phonological
assessed annually on reading tests as well awareness and reading development.
as on a number of measures that are Phoneme deletion or the ability to
related to reading development, namely segment words into their constituent parts
phonological awareness or one’s sensitivity emerged as a consistent concurrent and
to the internal sound structure of words, long-term predictor of later word
letter knowledge and short-term and non-word reading and of reading
phonological memory. comprehension for both language groups.
Certain tentative conclusions were The results of this study also showed that
drawn from the study, the main one being phonological awareness is a set of skills
that the EL1 and EL2 children appeared to that develops gradually and reciprocally
progress through the same developmental with learning to read. While differences in
stages of reading and phonological performance on the phonological
awareness, but at different rates. These awareness measures emerged at preschool
differences could be attributed, at least in level between the EL1 and EL2 children,
part, to the influence of the children’s these differences, which were in favour of
different home languages. Despite these the EL1 children, disappeared with
differences, neither language group exposure to the same reading instruction.
demonstrated an advantage over the The finding that the EL1 and EL2 children
other in single word or non-word reading progress through the developmental
or reading comprehension. The linguistic phases of phonological awareness but at
diversity of South Africa’s school different rates and that these phases exert
population raises a number of a changing role in early reading
>>
392 Developmental Psychology
<<
development suggests that task bilinguals, though it must be remem-
appropriateness at each developmental bered that the generalisability of these
level is an important issue. The results findings is restricted to children who
of this study indicate that some of the are learning to read in English, and
mechanisms at work in learning to read may differ for languages with different
are common to monolinguals and alphabetic characteristics.
Piaget
There has always been debate about which develops first—language or
thought—and about the exact nature and degree of interdependence
between them. According to Piaget, language is intimately related to the
development of symbolic representation, which provides the capacity for
mental representation, imagery, imitation and pretend play, as well as
spoken language. All new stages (language being one of them) must wait
for the appropriate mental structures to develop and mature. Thought
develops first, from sensorimotor activities, and creates the necessary
mental structures for language development. Piaget thus believed that
language is dependent on thought.
Language development 393
Semantic slanting refers to deliberately without which, it would seem, your life will
choosing words to evoke specific emotional be incomplete.
responses. For example, in the language of Language not only affects our choices
the military and politicians, an invasion is a but also our sensory experiences of those
‘pre-emptive counter-attack’, bombing the choices. For example, people will choose a
enemy is providing ‘air support’, a retreat is hamburger that is 75 per cent lean over one
a ’backloading of augmentation personnel’, that is 25 per cent fat. When they actually
civilians accidentally killed or wounded taste the two burgers (which are actually
by military strikes are ‘collateral damage’, two versions of the same burger), the 75
and troops killed by their own troops are per cent lean burger is rated as tasting
‘friendly casualties’. Advertisements are better (Schwartz, 2007). This suggests that
notorious for using language in evocative advertisements and descriptions not only
ways to compel you, the consumer, to part affect our decisions but also how we
with your money and buy a new product, experience the results of those decisions.
394 Developmental Psychology
Vygotsky
Vygotsky adopted a position somewhere between those of Whorf and
Piaget. Vygotsky proposed that thought and language are potentially
present, but separate, at birth and their subsequent development is not
parallel but undergoes many changes (Vygotsky, 1988). The earliest form
of thinking in infants is action-based and concerned with purposeful,
but basic, problem solving such as reaching for food or toys, or opening
a container. The earliest ‘speech’ consists of sounds produced by air
leaving the lungs and passing through the throat, nose and mouth.
This often serves as a form of emotional release, as with cries of
frustration, grunts of pleasure and screams of anger. This ‘speech’ also
serves a social function, since it keeps the infant in close contact with
his caregivers and attracts attention and help. When a relatively high
level of development has been reached in both pre-verbal thinking
and pre-intellectual speech, around the age of two years, the two
processes begin to combine and form a new kind of mental function,
verbal thinking (Vygotsky, 1988). The onset of verbal thinking is
marked by the child’s curiosity about words and the names of things,
and the corresponding increase in the child’s vocabulary. Language
develops first in social interactions with adults or peers, with the sole
objective of communicating. As language is mastered, it is internalised
in private speech, to support thought and inner speech dialogues. (See
Chapter 22 on Vygotsky for more about private speech.)
Vygotsky (1988) maintained that thought is largely the product of
language. He believed that we could only really understand a child’s
mental processes through the psychological tools such as language
that mediate them. This means trying to understand the cultural
meanings of words and language, the unique context in which they
are being used, and the range of previous experiences and motivations
that lie behind them.
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➊ Use the spellchecker on a computer word processor. How has it been organised,
and which theory(s) of language acquisition discussed in this chapter is it based on?
➌ Explain why phonological receding is not the final stage of reading, even in
languages that do not possess any irregular words.
➍ Vygotsky saw language as a tool that helps us to organise and refine our thoughts.
What are some of the ways in which language aids our thinking?
Language development 395
Recommended readings
Bjorklund, D F (1995). Children’s thinking: Developmental function and individual differences.
Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.
(An easily accessible account of how thinking (including) language develops over childhood.
It makes mention of all of the major approaches to language acquisition.)
Harris, M & Coltheart, M (1986). Language processing in children and adults. London: Routledge
and Kegan Paul.
(A detailed cognitive explanation of models of developing and mature language, as well as
disorders of language. Suitable for both under- and postgraduate readers.)
Saunders, G (1988). Bilingual children: From birth to teens. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
(An interesting book about how bilingual children’s language–and thinking–differs from that
of monolinguals).
CHAPTER
19
Kohlberg’s theory of moral
reasoning
James Grant
Introduction
How does one decide what one ought to do in circum-
stances of moral significance? Are there certain clear
rules that one must obey: don’t steal, cheat, and above all,
don’t kill. Is it that simple? Would you observe the principle not to
kill if you were attacked and the only way to defend yourself was
to kill? That is, would you avail yourself of the well-recognised
right to self-defence? Several states, including Botswana and Lawrence Kohlberg
K o h l b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f m o r a l r e a s o n i n g 397
the United States of America, still practise the death penalty in which they
kill people in the most premeditated fashion. But, you may argue that the
people they kill are bad. Well then, what about abortion? South African
law permits abortion (Choice on Termination of Pregnancy Act), but pro-
hibits the death penalty (State v Makwanyane, 1995). Is this inconsistent?
Can we sometimes kill?
What about theft? Is that not simpler? Yet here we must ask whether
Robin Hood is immoral. In the following scenario we must contemplate
what ‘Heinz’ should do:
Here we see the rule not to steal, manifested in the law, together with
the right of someone to his property, in conflict with the right to life of
another. What should Heinz do, should he steal the drug?
Lawrence Kohlberg found that people offered different answers to
questions such as these (which always set an individual in a dilemma of
having to choose some rule or right over another). There seemed to be
no one right answer. More particularly, there appeared no significant
relationship between an individual’s knowledge of moral rules or
declared morality in the sense of what someone says they will do in a
morally significant circumstance, and their ultimate conduct (Kohlberg,
1964; 1968; 1969). What he did find, however, was that the way people
reasoned about the problem tended to correlate with their ages and
degree of cognitive development, so individuals of similar age and cog-
nitive development generally considered moral problems in the same
manner. Thus, Kohlberg’s interest in posing dilemmas, such as that
of ‘Heinz’, was to elicit the reasons for the solutions that his subjects
proposed. His concern became the structure of cognition behind the
individual’s notions of right and wrong. Furthermore, since morality
involved cognition, it could not be explained by reference to fixed
biological dispositions, non-rational dispositions, such as conscience, or
the acquisition of and adherence to societal norms or cultural practices
or orientations (Turiel, 2006: 9).
398 Developmental Psychology
Piaget’s influence
Kohlberg’s observations led him to elaborate upon the two-stage
cognitive model of children’s moral judgement presented by Piaget
(1932/1965) whose theories fascinated and inspired him (Crain, 1992).
Piaget had conceived his theory by observing children’s treatment of
rules when playing games such as marbles. Moral development, Piaget
theorised, took the form of a progression from heteronomous (subject to
external rules) to autonomous (internal) moral reasoning. Heteronomous
K o h l b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f m o r a l r e a s o n i n g 399
Level One
Preconventional/
pre-moral morality.
In Level One morality is referred to as preconventional or premoral
Kohlberg’s first level morality. This means that morality resides externally: norms and principles
orientation in which are imposed from ‘above’. It is dependent upon consequences, since the
morality resides externally, individual is concerned with the avoidance of punishment or the attainment
determined by norms of reward.
and principles that are
imposed from ‘above’.
Stage One
It is dependent upon
consequences since the
The first stage is the obedience and punishment orientation (or heteronomous
individual is concerned morality) in which right is determined by the avoidance of punishment.
with the avoidance
of punishment or the For: He should steal the drug. It isn’t really bad to take it. It isn’t
attainment of reward. as though he didn’t ask to pay for it first. The drug he’d take is
Heteronomous only worth R400; he’s not really taking a R4 000 drug. Or: if you
moral reasoning. let your wife die, you will get into trouble. You’ll be blamed for not
Piaget’s notion that spending the money to save her and there’ll be an investigation of
the reasoning adopted you and the druggist for your wife’s death.
by children is initially
characterised by strict
adherence to rules and
Against: He shouldn’t steal the drug; it’s a serious crime. He didn’t
duties, and by obedience get permission, he used force and broke and entered. He did a lot
to authority. Rules of damage, stealing a very expensive drug and breaking up the
are regarded as fixed store, too. Or: you shouldn’t steal the drug because you’ll be caught
and absolute, deriving and sent to jail if you do. If you do get away, your conscience would
from some supreme bother you thinking how the police would catch up with you at
commandment (adults,
any minute.
parents, or God).
(Heteronomous means
according to various Stage Two
external laws or principles, The second stage is the stage of individualism, instrumental purpose, and
that is, laws and principles exchange and is determined by how one can attain reward as opposed to
that are imposed on one avoiding punishment. The concern remains ‘pre-conventional’ in that
from outside.) moral reasoning is that of an individual rather than a member of society.
The orientation is represented well by the notion: ‘You scratch my back
and I’ll scratch yours’.
K o h l b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f m o r a l r e a s o n i n g 401
For: It’s all right to steal the drug because she needs it and he wants
her to live. It isn’t that he wants to steal, but he has to use this way
to get the drug to save her. Or: if you do happen to get caught you
could give the drug back and you wouldn’t get much of a sentence.
It wouldn’t bother you much to serve a little jail term, if you have
your wife when you get out.
Level Two
Level Two is referred to as the level of conventional or role conformity Conventional/role
morality in which norms have become internalised and the individual is conformity morality.
concerned with his or her reputation, and how he or she is perceived by Kohlberg’s second level
orientation in which norms
others. Moral values are determined by whether one conforms to majority
have become internalised
norms, maintains the conventional social order and fulfils the expectations and the individual is
of others. concerned with his or her
reputation—how they are
Stage Three perceived by others. Moral
The third stage is oriented by mutual interpersonal expectations, values are determined by
relationships, and interpersonal conformity (also called the good-boy/ conforming to majority
norms, maintaining
good-girl orientation). Morality is now determined by an attitude of
the conventional social
approval-seeking and is attained by fulfilling the expectations of others order and fulfilling the
close to one: being ‘good’ and ‘nice’. Concerns shift from consequences to expectations of others.
intentions in that ‘meaning well’ prevails over the possible consequences
of conduct. It is a morality of love, empathy and caring for one’s
significant others.
Stage Four
The fourth stage’s orientation is based in the social system and conscience
(or authority and social-order-maintaining). It is directed by a sense of the
value in maintaining the conventional social system as a duty. In contrast to
Stage Three reasoning, in which the individual’s moral focus is his or her
significant others such as family and friends, at Stage Four the individual
becomes concerned with society and ‘the institution’. The orientation is
represented well by the notion that the system is supreme and that one
cannot simply do what one thinks is right if it conflicts with what the
system requires, because then there would be chaos.
For: You should steal it. If you did nothing you’d be letting your
wife die, and it’s your responsibility if she dies. You have to take
it with the idea of paying the druggist. Or: if you have any sense
of honour, you won’t let your wife die because you’re afraid to do
the only thing that will save her. You’ll always feel guilty that you
caused her death if you don’t do your duty to her.
Level Three
Postconventional/ Level Three is the level of postconventional or principled morality in
principled morality. which morality becomes internal and autonomous. Norms are determined
Kohlberg’s third level
upon the basis of principles of justice, fairness and dignity, independent of
orientation in which
morality becomes internal
conventions such as national laws.
and autonomous, and
norms are determined Stage Five
on the basis of principles The fifth stage’s orientation is in social contract or utility and individual
of justice, fairness and rights. Moral norms are now defined in terms of laws or institutionalised
dignity, independent rules for their social utility. Now the concern is with a good society. This
of conventions such as
sets it apart from Stage Four reasoning in which the focus was simply on
national laws.
maintaining the system. An autocracy that functions smoothly would not
impress a Stage Five reasoner. A good society is defined as one born of
consensus (the social contract), while at the same time respecting certain
rights, such as life, as inviolable on the premise that respect for these rights
is universal. Agreement in this respect is assumed.
For: The law wasn’t set up for these circumstances. Taking the
drug in this situation isn’t really right, but to do it is justified. Or:
you’d lose other people’s respect and not gain it, if you don’t steal.
If you let your wife die, it would be from fear, not from reasoning
it out. So you’d just lose self-respect and probably the respect of
others too.
K o h l b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f m o r a l r e a s o n i n g 403
Against: You can’t completely blame someone for stealing, but even
extreme circumstances don’t really justify taking the law into your
own hands. You can’t have everyone stealing whenever they get
desperate. The end may be good, but the ends don’t justify the means.
Or: you would lose your standing and respect in the community and
violate the law. You’d lose respect for yourself if you were carried
away by emotion and forgot the long-range point of view.
Stage Six
The sixth stage is the morality of universal ethical principles in which
morality is directed by self-chosen ethical principles—not dependent upon
consensus—which are assumed to be the foundation of the law, but which
predominate where the law conflicts with these personal principles. The
existence of Stage Six as a separate stage above and beyond the scope of
Stage Five is questionable. It is treated as a hypothetical theoretical endpoint
of the sequence of development (Kohlberg, Boyd & Levine, 1990). However,
it is not provided for in the most recent edition of the instrument for the
attribution of stages to subjects (Colby & Kohlberg, 1987; Colby et al, 1987).
The distinction between Stages Five and Six is not all that clear as both are
concerned with what constitutes a good society and both respect individual
rights. The difference seems to be that rights, in Stage Five, are respected on
the assumption that they are dictated by consensus. In Stage Six, however,
rights are respected irrespective of consensus; they are apparently taken as
rules of natural law (Crain, 1992). All post-conventional thought is now
assessed, however, as Stage Five reasoning (Colby et al, 1987).
Level One – Preconventional To avoid breaking rules. Backed by Avoidance of punishment, and the superior Egocentric point of view. Does not consider
punishment, obedience for its own sake and power of authorities. the interests of others or recognise that they
Stage One: avoiding physical damage to persons and differ from the subject’s; doesn’t relate two
Developmental Psychology
Stage Two: Following rules only when it is to someone’s To serve one’s own needs or interests in a Concrete individualistic perspective. Aware
Individualism, Instrumental immediate interest; acting to meet one’s world where you have to recognise that that everybody has his or her own interest
Purpose and Exchange own interests and needs and letting others other people have their interests too. to pursue and these conflict so that right is
do the same. Right is also what’s fair, what’s relative (in the concrete individualistic sense).
an equal exchange, a deal, an agreement.
Level Two – Conventional Living up to what is expected by people close The need to be a good person in your own Perspective of the individual in relationships
to you or what people generally expect of eyes and those of others. Your caring for with other individuals. Aware of shared
Stage Three: those in your role as child, sibling, friend, etc. others. Belief in the Golden Rule. Desire to feelings, agreements and expectations that
Mutual Interpersonal ‘Being good’ is important and means having maintain rules and authority that support take primacy over individual interests.
Expectations, Relationships good motives, showing concern about others. stereotypical good behaviour. Relating points of view through the concrete
and Interpersonal It also means keeping mutual relationships, Golden Rule, putting themselves in the other
Conformity such as trust, loyalty, respect and gratitude. person’s shoes. Does not yet consider
generalised system perspective.
Stage Four: Fulfilling the actual duties to which you have To keep the institution going as a whole, Differentiates societal point of view from
Social System and agreed. Laws are to be upheld except in to avoid the breakdown in the system ‘if interpersonal agreement or motives. Takes
Conscience extreme cases where they conflict with other everyone did it’. The imperative of the point of view of the system that defines
fixed social duties. Right is also contributing conscience to meet one’s defined roles and rules. Considers individual relations
to society, the group or institution. obligations (easily confused with Stage in terms of place in the system.
Three belief in rules and authority).
Level and stage Content of stage Social perspective of stage
Level Three – Being aware that people hold a variety of A sense of obligation to law because of one’s Prior-to-society perspective. Perspective of a
Postconventional or values and opinions, and that most values social contract to make and abide by laws rational individual aware of values and rights
principled and rules are relative to your group. These for the welfare of all and for the protection prior to social attachments and contracts.
relative rules should usually be upheld, of all people’s rights. A feeling of contractual Integrates perspectives by formal mechanisms
Stage Five: however, in the interest of impartiality and commitment, freely entered upon, to family, of agreement, contract, objective impartiality
Social Contract or Utility because they are the social contract. Some friendship, trust and work obligations. and due process. Considers moral and legal
and Individual Rights non-relative values and rights like life and Concern that laws and duties be based on points of view; recognises that they
liberty, however, must be upheld in any rational calculation of overall utility: ‘the sometimes conflict and finds it difficult to
society and regardless of majority opinion. greatest good for the greatest number.’ integrate them.
Stage Six: Following self-chosen ethical principles. The belief as a rational person in the validity Perspective of a moral point of view from
Universal Ethical Particular laws or social agreements are of universal moral principles, and a sense of which social arrangements derive. Perspective
Principles usually valid because they rest on such personal commitment to them. is that of any rational individual recognising
principles. When laws violate these the nature of morality or the fact that
principles, one acts in accordance with the persons are ends in themselves and must
principle. Principles are universal principles be treated as such.
of justice: the equality of human rights
and respect for the dignity of human beings
as individuals.
Transitional Stages
Kohlberg was confronted with some data (Kramer, 1968) suggesting
that some adolescents had apparently regressed from Stage Four or
Five reasoning, back to Stage Two reasoning. However, Kohlberg
noted on closer analysis that these individuals were instead in a stage
of transition between Stages Four and Five—which he denoted as
stage 4½ or 4+. These individuals had become dissatisfied with Stage
Four reasoning, but had not yet fully adopted Stage Five reasoning.
The notion of blame being apportioned by society for wrongs
committed, in line with Stage Four (law and order) reasoning, had
been replaced by a sense of contempt for society, that society was
rather to blame (Kohlberg, 1984). A Stage 4½ response to the Heinz
dilemma could be that Heinz should steal the drug because to do
so would be the a humanistic or moral act in a capitalist society, or
because a society in which one must steal to save life is unjust (Colby
et al, 1987: 11). This may easily be mistaken for Stage Two reasoning,
but is better regarded as part of the process of developing Stage
Five reasoning (Kohlberg, 1969; 1984; Kohlberg & Kauffman, 1987).
Classify the following statements according to the stage of moral reasoning that they
demonstrate:
1. Heinz should not steal because the owner worked hard for what he has and you
shouldn’t take advantage;
2. Heinz should steal the drug because certain rights have been agreed upon or defined
by us through social process or social contract;
3. Heinz should steal the drug because his wife might be a very important person;
4. Heinz shouldn’t steal because it’s selfish or deceitful to steal;
5. Heinz should steal the drug because of the marital responsibility he accepted.
Stage scores are shown at the end of the chapter, in the Critical Thinking Tasks section.
K o h l b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f m o r a l r e a s o n i n g 407
Methodological concerns
Kohlberg’s method for scoring (assessing) moral reasoning, the Moral
Judgment Interview (MJI), relies upon a subject’s free responses to
questions concerning his or her moral reasoning about moral dilemmas
such as that of Heinz. The MJI has attracted criticism for its reliance
upon an interpretative scoring system, which has been said to incur
serious threats of scorer subjectivity. The elaborate scoring system is
cumbersome, and to the extent that subjects do not offer clear stage-
related responses (due to the freedom given them to answer), inferences
must be drawn from what is said in order to identify the responses
with a particular stage or stages. The present MJI (Colby & Kohlberg,
1987; Colby et al, 1987) claims ‘semi-standardisation’ while conceding
complexity and that it may appear ‘at first unwieldy’. An aspirant
interviewer is warned to set aside a minimum of a month or two of
concentrated study and practice to learn how to score an MJI. Thus,
while the threat of subjectivity may have been reduced, the MJI remains
a complex and somewhat tortuous instrument to administer and score.
One may apprehend the difficulties inherent in scoring by considering
the statements in the box above and attempting to attribute each to a
Kohlbergian stage.
In response to the criticisms levelled at Kohlberg’s MJI, Rest developed
the Defining Issues Test (DIT) as an objective and simpler measure of
moral reasoning (Rest, 1974; Rest, Narvaez, Bebeau & Thoma, 1999;
Thoma, 2006). Using Kohlberg’s moral stages, statements were compiled
which exemplify the characteristics of the various stages. Subjects are
scored on the importance they attach to each of these statements. The
DIT, now in its second edition (DIT2), is probably the better instrument
(The Centre for the Study of Ethical Development; (see http://www.
centerforthestudyofethicaldevelopment.net/). DIT research and the
development of moral reasoning theory in response to data collected using
the DIT now represents the new face of Kohlberg’s work—referred to
as the neo-Kohlbergian model (see Thoma (2006) for a discussion of the
theoretical developments associated with DIT research).
408 Developmental Psychology
Stage Six
It must be regarded as a shortcoming that a developmental theory
proposes an endpoint which is empirically unattainable even if it remains a
hypothetical endpoint (Kohlberg, Boyd & Levine, 1990). If a developmental
stage theory posits an endpoint for development which no one, in practice,
can attain, then it has, in effect, no developmental endpoint (Lapsley,
2006). To posit a hypothetical endpoint signifies only what, in Kohlberg’s
view, people’s morality ought to become, as a philosophical dream; it does
not indicate, as is required of a psychological developmental theory, what
people’s morality does or can become. Does this mean that we cannot
empirically verify whether something is inherently right, irrespective of
the consensus regarding the issue in question? This would have dramatic
implications for the notion of universal human rights, which are said to
exist irrespective of the consensus regarding those rights; that for instance,
the majority is not always right. Is there hope from the cross-cultural
studies that autonomy and individual rights are observed throughout the
world, across cultures? (See discussion on cross-cultural universality later
in this chapter.) Of course there is, but only if the reasoning behind the
belief in those rights can be found to be independent of society or social
consensus. Perhaps the evidence of resistance to inequality and oppression
from minorities provides some hope (Turiel, 2003).
PHOTOS: CH A JOHNSTON
410 Developmental Psychology
men are responsible for the majority of violence and antisocial conduct,
that prison populations are overwhelmingly male and they ask, ‘If there
are no sex differences in empathy or moral reasoning, why are there sex
differences in moral and immoral behaviour?’ (Gilligan & Wiggins, 1987:
279). Gilligan & Wiggins present a very good question, which needs to be
considered within the framework of the multiplicity of complex factors at
play in the production of conduct in a morally significant context.
Moses Sithole’s life is littered with violations express will in handing down a sentence
of the law and the rights of those who have of 2 410 years in prison with eligibility for
encountered him. His previous convictions parole arising at 1 460 years.
begin at the age of 13, for housebreaking, Grant (1997)* examined Sithole’s
for which he admits guilt. When he was 20 moral reasoning from a Kohlbergian
he was again convicted of housebreaking perspective and was surprised that Sithole,
and admits guilt again. At 22 and again the worst serial killer and rapist in South
at 25 he was convicted of fraud; he admits African history to date, demonstrated
guilt in both these instances. At 25 he was post-conventional moral reasoning
also convicted of a rape; he insists he is not and may appropriately be assigned to
guilty of this crime however (Sithole, 1996). that level. In the face of this finding, it
When he was 30, he was again appears doubtful that moral reasoning
arrested, this time on suspicion of being can be taken as a direct indicator of an
a serial rapist and killer responsible for 40 individual’s prospective conduct in a
rapes, 38 murders and 6 robberies. morally significant context.
On 4 December, 1997 he was * The assistance of Merle Friedman
convicted on all counts and the following was invaluable to this project and
day sentenced to die in prison: the Judge’s greatly appreciated
412 Developmental Psychology
Conclusion
Kohlberg’s method is so cumbersome and prone to errors of interpretation
that James Rest’s DIT has probably superseded the MJI as the instrument
of choice for measuring moral reasoning development. Also, although
Kohlberg’s theory seems lacking in respect of its ability to explain
behaviour directly in a morally significant context, he never claimed that
it would, and his theory stands as an explanation of an integral factor
of what is required for moral conduct. Further, his claims of invariant
sequence and universality appear to have withstood empirical scrutiny
(save that in some societies, for reasons Kohlberg seems to explain,
K o h l b e r g ’s t h e o r y o f m o r a l r e a s o n i n g 415
Specific tasks
➊ What stage of moral reasoning do you adopt when resolving morally significant
problems?
➋ What does it mean to say that someone knows that what they are doing is wrong,
particularly in the case of a child?
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➌ What do you think can explain the way people conduct themselves in a morally
relevant context?
Statement stages:
1) Stage Three (p 74)
2) Stage Five (p 55)
3) Stage One (p 12)
4) Stage Three (p 74)
(it is scored as an equivalent of statement 1)
5) Stage Four (p 42)
416 Developmental Psychology
Recommended readings
Kohlberg, L & Kauffman, K (1987). ‘Theoretical introduction to the measurement of moral
judgment’. In A Colby & L Kohlberg (eds), The measurement of moral judgment. Vol 1.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
(This provides the most recent and comprehensive exposition of Kohlberg’s theory, on the authority
of Kohlberg himself.)
Rest, J R (1983). ‘Morality’. In P Mussen, J H Flavell, & E M Markman (eds), Handbook of Child
Psychology. Vol. 3. (4th ed-). New York: John Wiley.
(An accessible rendition of Kohlberg’s theory and a detailed discussion of Rest’s component analysis
of moral conduct.)
M Killen & J Smetana (eds) (2006). Handbook of Moral Development. London: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
(A compendium of current thinking on moral development by leading scholars in the field.)
CHAPTER
20
Evolutionary psychology
Michael Greyling
1. What is evolution?
2. Mechanisms of evolutionary change
3. What is an adaption?
4. Key principles of an evolutionary psychology approach
5. What evolutionary psychology is not
6. The development of the brain and the role of genes
7. Aspects of development within an evolutionary
psychology framework
8. Cognitive development and evolutionary psychology
Introduction
Evolutionary psychology is an approach to understanding modern human Nature-Nurture Debate.
behaviour from the perspective of the challenges faced by our evolutionary The debate centres
ancestors. Our bodies and brains have evolved over millions of years and around the degree to
each of us shares that heritage. This chapter will explore that communality which our behaviours,
between our evolutionary pasts and what insights we may derive from it. beliefs, values, etc, are
To begin, evolutionary psychology revisits some of the ‘Nature- things we are born with
(Nature or Nativist) as
Nurture’ debate that occupies much of psychology. While the notion of our
opposed to things which
evolutionary past must give some weight to the nature (nativist) side of the develop due to influences
equation, the debates are not as simple or as crude as they are frequently from our environment
presented. Firstly, all theorists to a lesser or greater extent subscribe to (Nurture).
some form of interactionism. Simply put, learning, growth or ‘nurture’
would not exist without the DNA, genes and abilities which humans as a Interactionism. The
species share (‘nature’). By the same token DNA and genes do not operate position that argues
that all development
in a vacuum and no development of any sort can take place without an
occurs in the context of
enabling environment. In each and every developmental step there are an interaction between
either stable or important environmental influences that interact with the the environment and
developing organism and the way this happens is to a large extent reliant our inherent or genetic
on the genetic potential carried by individuals and species. potential.
418 Developmental Psychology
Darwin noted that not all members of a generation were able to reproduce.
Some were killed before they could reproduce while others were unable to
find a suitable mate. Secondly, Darwin noted that not all members of a species
were identical. In fact there is a large amount of variation between members
of the same species. Now imagine that some members of the species had a
trait that made it more likely for them to survive (for example colouring
which provides better camouflage from predators). Then on average more
of those with better camouflage would survive until breeding age. The next
generation then becomes more likely to have a greater proportion of those
with the better camouflage. In time we would anticipate that the whole
population would come to have that trait.
It is important to remember that natural selection operates on
variability in the population that already exists and as such does not aim for
a particular outcome. Rather, the process merely assumes that on average
those whose characteristics are most suited to the task of surviving long
enough to reproduce (or to produce the most offspring) are those most
likely to form the template for the next generation, which, in turn, will be
more suited to the task of surviving.
An imaginary example will be used to demonstrate this principle. Ima-
gine a small plant that produces berries. At some period in its evolutionary
history the plant has berries that are generally quite bitter but some of the
plants have berries sweet enough to be palatable to the local birds. The birds
fly down and carefully peck the berry until they find one that is sweet enough
to eat. The berries are then either eaten on the spot or carried away to feed
the birds’ young. Berries that are carried away and dropped somewhere else
on average have a better chance of growing as they do not have to compete
with the existing plants for nutrients. Now this scenario (not implausible,
if somewhat oversimplified) is likely to lead to two consequences if played
out over time. The sweeter berries, which the birds seek out, are more likely
to be carried away and germinate and will then become more common in
the plant population. In addition, birds more adept at spotting the subtle
variations in shape or colour that reveal the sweeter berries are more likely
to feed themselves and their young and as such the birds that have these
skills are also likely to predominate.
The important lesson from this example is that evolution via natural
selection did not set out to make sweeter berries. It merely happened
that in the context of birds eating the berries more of the sweet berry-
producing plants reproduced and therefore on average the berries got
sweeter. Students of evolution are often misled by examples such as these
to assume that evolutionary change is a steady process of improvement, for
example sweeter obviously being better. It is important that the solution
found is in some way random and dependent on the available variation.
An equally plausible set of events, given the situation above, is that some
birds have a better ability to tolerate the bitter berries and that slowly the
bird population changes to one that eats/enjoys bitter berries.
Finally it is important to remember that species do not, as a rule,
exist in nice little dyads but form part of a complex ecosystem of food,
predators, prey, environment (for example temperature, water, colours,
etc), parasites and competing species. Each component of the ecosystem
420 Developmental Psychology
can change over time and in doing so make alternative variations within
a species more successful, thereby changing the parameters under which
natural selection operates.
What is an adaption?
The notion that random chance guided by natural selection is the basis
for all the diversity of life is surprising because there are so many aspects
of different animals that seem so well designed for their task. (This is an
overstatement as we have not discussed all the mechanisms of evolution-
ary change.)
Think, for example, about our eyes. They are extremely complex organs
that have lenses, sensors, muscles, and cavities to remove excess light— all
of which do a brilliant job to perform the function of seeing. In fact, even
this massively underestimates the complexity of our visual system. It just
seems ‘obvious’ that eyes were designed for seeing.The issue here, however,
is not how natural selection and variation have come to produce an eye.
Again it is important to emphasise that according to the theory, eyes were
not planned from the outset. Rather, eyes came to be the way they are, and
eyes are clearly suited to the task of seeing. Put differently, an animal that
can map objects such as food, obstacles, prey and predators at a distance
clearly has an advantage over one that cannot. The eyes are a very effective
solution to this task. Of course other solutions, such as the echolocation in
bats or the acute smell of dogs, are also effective.
The eyes provide us with so many advantages that it may be difficult
to identify the evolutionary story that resulted in them being as they are
today. However it may be interesting to consider a possible story behind
this one small feature. Why do we have eyebrows when the rest of our
face has no hair? A functional explanation would point to the idea that
a dark patch around the eyes reduces glare from excess light, much in
the same way as we might shield ourselves from the glare with our hand
when looking in the direction of the sun. Imagine now a proto-human
population, some of which had brows while others did not. Humans with
eyebrows would have slightly better vision in bright sunlight and perhaps
recognise food, predators or other opportunities/risks better than their
non-eyebrowed compatriots. On average more of them would survive,
ultimately leaving only eyebrowed individuals in the population. As such
the eyebrows would have been selected for through the process of natural
Adaption. A trait that selection, and we would call them an adaption. Adaptions then are features
exists in a population that at some time in our evolutionary history conferred an advantage on
due to the fact that it those members of our species—or a species from which we derived—who
supplied and evolutionary
possessed them, and as such were selected for by natural selection.
advantage and as such was
selected for by means of
natural selection. Not all features are adaptions
If natural selection were the only game in town then all features would be
adaptions. However this is not the case. Three important alternatives need
to be considered. Firstly, while some random changes affect the survival
of an animal, some of the changes are neutral. If a series of random neutral
changes occurs in a species this may ultimately result in new features
that were not selected for, but merely not selected against. As an example,
Evolutionary psychology 421
These are:
in the body, the timing of the developmental process and the range of
chemicals present.
How this all works, particularly in a complex structure such as the
brain, is not well understood. What is evident is that the genes exert an
enormous amount of control over development but at the same time
rely on numerous factors external to the DNA in order to function.
These include the internal and external environment of the body as
well as a fair bit of random fluctuation. The degree to which the DNA
is responsible for the building of individual neural circuits or rather
merely providing the broad structure of the brain is at the heart of the
nature/nurture debate.
s The whole object assumption: The child assumes a new word refers
to the whole object being named, not any aspect of the object. For
example, if the mother points to a dog and says, ‘dog’ the child will
assume that what is being referred to is the whole dog and not its ears
or that it is running.
s The taxonomic assumption: The child assumes that a new word for
an unknown object refers to the class of objects, not the name of
that particular object. Thus if the mother points to a dog and says,
‘Geordie’ the child will assume that this is the generis name for all
dogs, not that ‘Geordie’ is the name of just this dog. Children often
make this mistake with pets, assuming that the name for their pet is
the generic name for all such animals.
s The basic level of categorisation: Children also assume that the categor-
isation takes place at what is called the basic level of categorisation. The basic level of
Again, using the taxonomic assumption, the child will assume a new categorisation. Consider
word applied to the first dog encountered refers to the category ‘dogs’ the terms furniture, chair,
director’s chair. Chair is at
(or a least four-legged furry animals) but not a higher category such
the basic level. Furniture
as animals or pets or a lower category such as Boxers. is a higher level. Director’s
chair is a lower level
s The mutual exclusivity assumption: Once a child has a word for a categorisation. The basic
category he will assume that new words for the same category are not level is the largest level of
synonyms, but refer to aspects or features of that particular example. categorisation in which a
If the child knows the word ‘dog’ and the mother says ‘brown’ the single mental image of the
object can be formed. It
child will make the assumption that brown is a feature of the dog and
is also the largest level at
not another name for dogs. which the way we interact
physically with the object
Why and how did natural selection produce the language is the same.
facility?
The major problem with this question is that language confers so many
benefits that it is hard to single out one benefit that might have come
first. There are many theories of the evolution of language. For example,
the ecologist and science writer Jared Diamond (1991) has pointed to
the complex nature of communication in vervet monkeys as a potential
bridge between human and animal language. The monkeys appear
to have different sounds/words for different kinds of predators and
hierarchies of other monkeys. These are highly adaptive as the correct
call from a monkey will result in appropriate behaviour from the other
monkeys. Consider that the ‘leopard’ call will send all the monkeys up
into the trees, while the ‘eagle’ call has the monkeys hiding under the
nearest bush.
This is a long way from human language, but Diamond argues that
we have yet to fully understand the complexity of animal languages and
428 Developmental Psychology
the costs involved in gestation and raising of the children. He argued that
where one sex invests more than the other, there will be differences in
strategies for obtaining a mate. In particular the greater the cost to a given
sex the more careful that sex will be in choosing his or her mate.
Men Women
Men desire as many sexual partners Women seek a few partners (typically one)
as possible. who will ensure provision for themselves
and their children.
They are typically undiscriminating but Women desire men who are powerful,
their primary desire is for beautiful and wealthy and reliable.
young women.
Their fundamental concern is that they Their fundamental concern is that their
don’t raise someone else’s children. mates do not divert needed resources to
the children of other women.
They are capable of violent reprisal if they They are jealous of other women who
suspect that someone else has sexual access may seek to lure their men
to their mate. (and hence resources.)
This follows logically from the simple notion that the more one invests,
the more risk is placed on the outcome and hence the more important
the choice will be. By definition the female is the sex that has the larger
gamete (the egg is larger than the sperm) thus on average the female is
more likely to carry the heavier cost and therefore be the more careful
mate. This definition obviously only applies where there are two sexes and
there is a difference in the size of the gametes. (There are species that have
many sexes and many that have asexual reproduction.)
In humans the difference between the sexes is typically large in a
number of ways. Women produce fewer eggs and have the additional
‘cost’ of menstruation. After conception a woman carries the baby to
term and needs to provide resources for herself and her child. Even after
birth a human infant is essentially helpless and needs careful support and
resources until the child can fend for himself. Added to this, the pregnant
mother is particularly vulnerable to predators.
By contrast, men continue to produce sperm for most of their lives
and do so in large quantities. Although men are typically involved in the
raising of their children and frequently bear the cost of providing food
and protecting the mother, they carry much less personal risk of failure. In
430 Developmental Psychology
addition, if they can find other mating partners for whom they do not take
any responsibility and hence incur less personal risk, they increase their
chances of siring more children, and this has adaptive value.
In summary, evolutionary psychologists and the sociobiologists who
preceded them argue that male parental investment is much lower
than that of women. To this end, they theorise that the male strategy in
mating will be to have as many sexual partners as possible but only to
commit resources to one or a few of those partners. By contrast, because
a woman carries the foetus, she will be dependent on a male who will
provide for her and her children and will thus seek out men who are
reliable providers. They must be willing to make a commitment to her
children and have the financial, social and physical resources to do so.
Women will be cautious to ensure that their mate is committed only to
them and will thus be jealous of rivals.
Men’s choice to commit to some women will be guided by which
women are likely to produce many children and are strong and healthy
enough to raise them. Evolutionary psychologists have proposed that
the notions of beauty and youth are adaptive proxies for the underlying
need for fecundity and wellbeing. For men the worst possible outcome
would be to provide for another man’s child (a significant risk in a small
hunter-gatherer society) and they would guard jealously against this.
The above presents a grim and fundamentally transactional view of
human relations. It is important to remember the principles that were
laid out in the first section. In particular, most humans will not make
these economic calculations consciously and will emphasise concepts such
as love and attractiveness. Evolutionary psychologists would argue that
we are not aware of the unconscious calculations that underpin simple
experiences of liking and desire. The fact that we are not aware of them,
however, does not mean that these calculations are not happening at less
conscious levels.
It is also important to realise that evolution and natural selection are not
moral forces that produce desirable outcomes. It is simply that strategies
that are successful are likely to be repeated. If the above strategies were
successful for our ancestors then those are the strategies that will be passed
on to later generations. We may experience them as love, attraction, care
and passion but they mask a complex set of neural calculations that assess
the value of any potential mate.
Finally, it is important to recognise that these strategies are not
necessarily effective or desirable in modern society. As noted before,
evolutionary psychologists have distanced themselves from claims that
these patterns are how things ‘ought to be’. Nevertheless, they would
argue that we will not properly understand gendered behaviour unless
we consider the different evolutionary challenges faced by the males and
females who were our Stone Age ancestors.
There are certainly many devout religious the existence of the Creator, rather than to
individuals from all of the world’s major provide a blow-by-blow scientific account.
belief systems who accept the evidence for Certainly all religious scholars accept that the
evolution and see no contradiction with scriptures contain some figurative language
their faith. The theory of evolution does but some are still uncomfortable with a
however challenge some long-held beliefs. broad interpretation of the word ‘days’.
These challenges need to be understood if Secondly, the notion of a Creation that
one hopes to find a middle ground. is consistent with evolution is one in which
Firstly, as natural selection relies on the Creator starts off the process, but has
extremely long time periods, the theory little involvement thereafter. That is not
comes into conflict with beliefs that the to say that no further involvement could
world was created in literally seven days be entertained, but the power of natural
or some similar time period. Many Jewish selection as a theory is precisely that it
and Christian scholars, for example, are provides an explanation for life’s diversity
comfortable with the interpretation that the without postulating constant intervention
days referred to in the book of Genesis are by a grand designer. If one’s belief system
figurative and reflect periods of time rather sees only a Creator who creates each
than exact days. They would argue that individual species immediately then one’s
the primary religious function is to indicate beliefs will contradict with the theory.
>>
434 Developmental Psychology
<<
Again religious scholars have noted that However, there is no a priori reason to
the world (as created) shows regularity assume that the theory of evolution is, in its
and order, and as such there is support broadest sense, contradictory with a belief
for the notion of a Creator who may in God. For some religions it is probably
intervene but for the most part has created complementary. It is of course a topic that
a system that operates on its own, and this is hotly contested. A brief meander through
could be consistent with an evolutionary the World Wide Web will reveal many
perspective. sites that are extremely disparaging of the
Perhaps the final challenge, which may theory. While one should read as broadly
be ‘a bridge too far’ for many beliefs, is the as possible, one should also be cautious
notion that human evolution is no different and refer to more scholarly critiques.
from that of any other species. Evolution Although the content fluctuates, Wikipedia
does not regard humans as the pinnacle of often provides balanced coverage of these
evolution but rather as only one branch on debates. Theorists such as Richard Dawkins
a very messy tree. Again it may well be that and Stephen J Gould have written many
human evolution was as much a part of the excellent and accessible texts defending the
Creator’s plan as every other species, but theory of evolution.
this is speculation outside the theory.
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➊ Look over the list of gender differences in Table 20.1. Do they ring true to you?
1.1 If so, how do you respond to critiques of these differences as merely stereotypical?
Do you support the argument that science which has potentially negative social
consequences requires a higher standard of proof?
1.2 If not, is this more than a gut response? Try to construct a critique of these views
using the evidence presented by the critics.
➌ Observe an infant as it starts to learn new words. Can you see evidence of the
assumptions described in the language section above?
Recommended readings
Readings about evolution, genes and brain development:
With the exception of Darwin all of the books below are written for a popular audience. In spite of its age
Darwin’s text is both engaging and relatively easy to read.
Darwin, C (1859). On the origin of the species by natural selection. Many editions—available on
Google books.
Dawkins, R (1976; 1989). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Evolutionary psychology 435
Dawkins, Richard [1986] (1996). The Blind Watchmaker. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.
Greenfield, S (2000). Brain Story. London: BBC.
Kukonis, G and Barr, T (2008). Evolution for Dummies. Indiana: Wiley Publishing Inc. Indianapolis.
An excellent summary of the science history of DNA is available at http://www.dnaftb.org/dnaftb/
Books by evolutionary psychologists and theorists who provide evolutionary and/or human nature-
based explanations of their theories:
Buss, Cosmides and Tooby, Pinker and Wright are all strident proponents of evolutionary psychology.
Buss, M (2005). Handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons.
Cosmides, L & Tooby, J (1997). Evolutionary psychology: A primer. Retrieved July 27, 2008, from the
University of California, Santa Barbara, Center for Evolutionary Psychology Web site: http://
www.psych.uscb.edu/research/cep/primer.html
Malik, K (2000). Man, Beast and Zombie. What science can and cannot tell us about human nature.
London: Phoenix.
Pinker, S (1994). The Language Instinct. How the Mind Creates Language. London: Penguin.
Pinker, S (1997). How the mind works. London: Penguin.
Pinker, S (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Viking Penguin.
Wright, R (1994). The Moral Animal. Why we are the way we are. London: Abacus.
Diamond, J (1991). The rise and fall of the third chimpanzee. How our animal heritage affects the way we
live. London: Vintage.
Ridley, M (2003). Nature via Nurture: Genes experience and what makes us human. London: Harper.
Overview Texts:
Workman, L & Reader, W (2004). Evolutionary psychology: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Badcock, C (2000). Evolutionary Psychology: A critical introduction. Malden Mass: Blackwell.
Critical Responses:
Rose, H & Rose, S (2001). Alas poor Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary psychology. London: Vintage.
Dupré, J (2001). Human nature and the limits of science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(This book is a fairly challenging read but well worth it.)
CHAPTER
21
Contributions of cognitive
science approaches to
cognitive developmental
psychology
Kevin G F Thomas, Susan Malcolm-Smith, Marianne Ball
and Michelle Robberts
Hebb (1949), who proposed the influential concept of cell assemblies (two Psychobiology.
or more cells that are repeatedly active at the same time and thus become Also known as
associated with one another, so that activity in one facilitates activity in biopsychology or
behavioural neuroscience,
another) as the foundation for learning in the brain. It was the work of
this discipline is one
these individuals and their students that paved the way for the cognitive branch of the study of
revolution of the later part of twentieth century. brain-behaviour relations.
More specifically, it takes
The information-processing approach as its starting point some
On the one hand, what we refer to as the ‘cognitive science approach biological process (eg,
to development’ emerged from the psychobiological roots mentioned the way in which some
particular brain regions
above. It also emerged, however, from technological developments in
work together) and
telecommunications, human-factors engineering, and, particularly, digital asks how that process
computers in the 1950s. Psychologists of the time were fascinated by the contributes to higher
possibilities of such technology, particularly with regard to the notion mental functioning
that they could design computer programs to do the kinds of the tasks (eg, how a particular
we humans do. Alan Turing’s classic paper (1950) elegantly stated the key event is remembered).
issue: Can a machine show the same kind of intelligence that a human
being does? In other words, a computer would pass the ‘Turing test’ if you Turing test.
were to exchange SMSes with it and not know that it was a computer and Alan Turing’s proposed test
not a human being. of a machine’s ability to
demonstrate intelligence.
The information-processing approach, then, is very different from
More specifically, what he
the approaches outlined in previous chapters because it is based on a called an ‘Imitation Game’.
computer analogy for the way the mind processes information received There are three players: a
from the external environment. So, information-processing theorists human being, a machine,
argue that the human mind/brain (and, to a lesser degree, those of non- and an interrogator.
human animals) is a computer-like system that uses logical rules and The interrogator must put
strategic systems to receive, manage, store and send information. a series of questions to the
other two entities in order
With regard to cognitive development in children, the analogy here
to determine which is the
is at the level of hardware and software: Information-processing theorists machine and which is
hold that we are born with identical representational and computational the person. The machine’s
systems that are genetically pre-structured; that is, we all start as entry- goal is to try to make the
level computers. Just as more powerful computers are those that have interrogator think it’s
more sophisticated microchips and dual-core processors (hardware), the person; the person’s
as well as more complex and wide-ranging programs (software), goal is to help the
interrogator correctly
similarly, older children have more sophisticated interwoven neural
identify the machine. Any
systems for sensory input and motor output (hardware), as well as machine that successfully
more complex and wide-ranging logical rules and strategic systems confuses the interrogator
(software) to support input and output. This is because, during typical is said to have passed the
development, children are exposed to stimulus-rich environments and Turing test.
people, which result in the rapid development of the brain and nervous
system. As the child becomes skilled at successfully dealing with those
environments and people, he learns new problem-solving, attentional
and memory strategies, and becomes capable of performing cognitive
tasks with greater speed and accuracy. As adults, then, we are super-
computers (Shaffer, 1996).
Information-processing theorists and researchers who are inter-
ested in cognitive development use this computer-based analogy to
design and test models of the way in which, for instance, children of
different ages encode information (that is, identify key aspects of an
438 Developmental Psychology
Short-term memory
(STM)
Temporary
Sensory registers working memory Long-term memory
(LTM)
Environmental Visual Control processes: Permanent memory
input store
Auditory Rehearsal
Coding
Decisions
Haptic Retrieval strategies
Response output
Figure 21.1 Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model of memory (1968) was one of the first to depict the flow of
information through various memory systems in the form of a flowchart.
Artificial intelligence
Artificial intelligence (AI), a field closely related to information-processing
theories, developed from a more direct influence of computing technology
on the study of cognition. AI researchers program computers to simulate
human cognitive functions or processes. These computer simulations
range from the very specific (for example, producing models of circuits
related to visual pattern recognition) to the very broad (modelling global
intelligence).
A significant line in the early empirical research on information-
processing theories, then, involved attempting to produce computer
systems that would use optimal methods to solve various problems. For
instance, Newell, Shaw and Simon (1957) developed a program called the
General Problem Solver (GPS) that used a general strategy called means-
end analysis to solve a range of quite specific problems. Essentially, the
problem-solving mechanism of the GPS was to successively reduce the dif-
ference between the current state (where you are now) and the goal state
(where you wish to be). GPS, then, is an example of a computer simulation
that emphasises global processes; other simulations, such as MYCIN
(Buchanan & Shortcliffe, 1984), which was used to detect and diagnose
bacterial infections, were designed to perform the way that a human
expert in a specific domain might.
Although AI approaches have contributed not only to methodological
and empirical but also to theoretical advances in the understanding of
human cognition, they have not been immune to criticism. One of the
most serious limitations identified by critics of (especially early) AI
approaches to, and computer simulations of, human cognition is that, by
and large, computers can only process one instruction at a time. Thus, Serial processing.
Occurs when information
the early computer-based models and simulations of human cognition
is handled in linear
almost invariably relied on step-by-step, line-by-line processing of fashion; each operation on
information. This is called serial processing, and attempts to overcome the information happens
its limitations have also led to major advances in our understanding of separately and
human cognition. in sequence.
440 Developmental Psychology
Cognitive science
Contemporary research in cognitive psychology is heavily influenced by
all of the approaches mentioned above: information processing, artificial
intelligence, connectionism, psychobiology and neuropsychology. Addi-
tionally, methods and theories from outside the discipline of psychology
(for example, from linguistics, anthropology, philosophy, economics and
neuroscience) have become an integral part of investigations into the
hardware and software that make up human cognitive architecture and
processing. The term cognitive science is thus used to subsume research
that not only draws from all of the fields listed above, but that also (a) takes
as its starting point the fact that human cognitive processes are constrained
by the limits of brain architecture, and (b) is concerned with the ways in
which human thinking is influenced by emotions and motivations, and by
the affective reactions humans have to each other and to social situations.
With specific regard to cognitive development, the cognitive science
approach is a clear improvement over traditional information-processing
approaches in numerous ways. One of these is that the social and
cultural factors so emphasized in the Vygotskian approach to cognitive
development (see Chapter 22 and also Kozulin, 1990) are absent in
information-processing accounts. Cognitive science, on the other hand, is
sensitive to both anthropological and personality variables, and cognitive
scientists have, for instance, made great contributions to cross-cultural
studies of intelligence (see for example, Nisbett, 2003).
Braver & Cohen, 1999; Page & Norris, 1998). Importantly, these
computational models have remained consistent with what is known
about the neuroanatomy of short-term and working memory (see for
example, Smith & Jonides, 1998), and with research that has established
that the prefrontal cortex, a key region for working memory function,
only fully develops relatively late in adolescence (Diamond, 2006; Sowell,
Delis, Stiles & Jernigan, 2001).
This part of the chapter focuses on that aspect of long-term memory
Autobiographical referred to as autobiographical memory (AM). It uses cognitive science
memory. The memory an research into the development of AM as an example of how fruitful this
individual has of his own approach is in furthering the aims of cognitive developmental psychology.
history and life.
Here, we use ‘long-term memory’ to refer to memory for events that
occurred years, months, days and hours ago (as opposed to ‘short-term
memory’, which refers to the recall of material presented seconds or
minutes before).
and narrative coherence. See Gathercole (1998) for a review.) Even more
Hippocampus. A brain specifically, we will focus on one crucial brain structure, the hippocampus,
region, located in the following the argument of Newcombe et al, (2007) that the maturation of
temporal lobes, that plays this neural region can be linked to the move from infantile amnesia and
a critical role in learning
into childhood amnesia, and then from childhood amnesia and into adult-
and memory.
like AM capabilities.
PHO TO : ZO Ë MO O S MANN
need to be associated and linked (bound)
together to produce autobiographical
recall of an event. For instance, to recall
that ‘We went to the rugby game on Saturday’
and to produce details of that outing, one
would need to remember, among other
things, where and when the game was,
with whom one went, and what happened
at the game. In this way, then, an episode
that one recalls ‘is not a holistic entity but
[is] instead the assemblage of a net of pair-
wise associations’ (Newcombe et al, 2007: Children between two and
37). The process of binding together these pair-wise associations is thus five years can clearly recall
critical to the production of AM, and, again, the hippocampus is a neural autobiographical events (for
region critical to this process. example a birthday party),
for up to several weeks or
Although the literature on the binding function of the hippocampus even years after the event.
is less well-developed than that on the cognitive mapping function of the
structure, there is a growing body of research from neuropsychology,
behavioural neuroscience (rodent and non-human primate studies of food
caching and sequence learning), psychophysiology (single-cell recordings
during retrospective and prospective memory tasks), psychopharmacology
(drug-based interventions affecting hippocampal functioning) and
genetics (interventions targeted at suppressing specific components of the
hippocampus), suggesting that this structure is a major player in a neural
circuit underlying the ‘what-where-when’ aspects of episodic memory
(including AM; see Morris, 2007, for an extensive review).
Surprisingly, however, there has been little work on the childhood
development of binding processes. This dearth of research is particularly
puzzling given the importance of binding to episodic autobiographical
memory, and because, at the other end of the life-span, a reasonably large
number of studies suggests that age-related declines in episodic memory
are associated with binding deficits (Castel & Craik, 2003; Chee, Goh, Binding. In this context,
Venkatraman, Tan, Gutchess & Sutton, 2006). In an attempt to jumpstart refers to the way in
research on the early development of binding, Sluzenski, Newcombe which the hippocampus
& Kovacs (2006) created a task that featured pictures of animals shown might act as a device that
integrates (binds) different
against complex naturalistic backgrounds (but, importantly, not the
contextual features
environments in which the animals are usually found; for instance, an of information in our
elephant might be pictured in a hotel room). Over the course of multiple environments. Following
experiments, Sluzenski and colleagues found that four-year-olds were no this theory, separate
worse than six-year-olds or adults at recognising either the animals alone features of an object,
or the backgrounds alone, but that there were substantial differences in event, or scene might be
memory performance when binding together of animal + background processed via separate
brain pathways in the
was tested: six-year-olds were much more likely than four-year-olds to
brain, but are all bound
remember seeing the elephant in that particular hotel room; indeed, the together, eventually, by
performance of six-year-olds was more similar to that of adults than it was the hippocampus.
to that of four-year-olds.
This behavioural evidence of a developmental change in binding
ability in children from four to six years old, along with the above-cited
neurobiological and adult neuropsychological literature on binding
448 Developmental Psychology
Anyone able to represent someone else’s mental state (in this case a
belief about where the sweet is), will predict that Sally will look for the
sweet in the place she thinks it is. Predictions based on reality (that is, based
on the child’s knowledge of where the sweet actually is) will be wrong. So
being able to pass a false belief test clearly proves that the child is correctly
inferring a mental state—Sally’s false belief about where the sweet is.
The false belief test has become a standard in theory of mind (ToM)
research. Multiple studies have demonstrated that before the age of
about four years, a typically developing child will not pass this test. He or
she will make incorrect predictions based on his or her own knowledge
of reality. This developmental milestone seems to occur universally.
For example, Avis & Harris (1991) showed that Baka children living in
the rainforests of Cameroon showed the same developmental pattern
demonstrated in Western cultures: older children (around the age of five
years) were able to infer false beliefs, while three-year-olds could not.
So, even in preliterate cultures, false belief reasoning comes online at
around the age of four years. Subsequent studies have confirmed these
results, strongly suggesting that the ability to reason about other people’s
mental states may well be an innate human ability—an evolved capacity
that provides adaptive advantage.
What is this adaptive advantage? It seems clear that ToM is part of a
set of mental abilities that allow us to successfully interact with others in
our social group. Thus, research into ToM falls in the domain of social Social cognition. Refers
cognition. There is a current line of thinking that sees the evolution of to the set of abilities that
primate (and thus also human) brain size as being primarily shaped by the enable social animals to
interact successfully with
demands of living as a social being. As early as the 1970s, it was suggested
each other. It includes
that the primate social environment created the evolutionary pressures the abilities to behave
that resulted in the development of larger brains (Humphrey, 1976; Jolly, co-operatively and
1966). Living in groups clearly provides adaptive advantages in providing altruistically, as well as the
protection against predation, allowing food sharing, and so on. However, ability to detect cheating.
it brings with it the costs of competition over resources and mates. Adap-
tive social functioning requires successful social co-operation and altruistic
behaviour towards group members. It also requires the ability to detect
cheating, and the ability to predict who is likely to reciprocate rather than
default on their obligations. It is argued that social intelligence, including
ToM abilities, evolved in order to facilitate these highly complex social
cognitive skills. In other words, it is possible that the unique size and
complexity of primate brains is the direct result of us being social animals.
for instance, show sensitivity to others’ emotions: they tend to cry when
others cry, and they show some ability to discriminate sad from happy
expressions (Field & Walden, 1982). By six months, infants can definitely
discriminate facial emotions (Caron, Caron & MacLean, 1988; Nelson,
1987). By age 12 months, infants will use parental emotional expressions
to guide their own behaviour in novel situations (Feinman, 1992). From
12-18 months, infants begin to use adults’ gaze direction to determine the
object of an emotional message. For example, Moses (2001) gave infants a
novel toy and, while they were engaged with this toy, had an adult make
a negative emotional noise. The infants would look up to determine what
the adult was looking at, and would later avoid only the object at which
the adult had been looking.
With regard to the attribution of goals to others, very young infants
prefer faces to objects, and they prefer to watch biological motion rather
than random movements. For example, they prefer to watch light points
that mimic a moving body rather than random movement (Bertenthal,
Proffitt & Cutting, 1984). At six months, they also begin to understand
movement by agents. In other words, infants are surprised if an object
moves of its own accord, but are not surprised if a person moves of his
own accord. This grasp of biological movement by an agent is probably
Intentional action. a necessary precursor to understanding intentional action (Frith & Frith,
Goal-directed movement 2003; Spelke, Phillips & Woodward, 1995).
by a biological agent. Between the age of five and eight months, infants seem to develop some
This type of movement
understanding that agents have goals. When watching a human hand, they
is processed in particular
ways by the brain.
expect the hand to keep reaching for the same object even when the object
is moved to a new location. They act surprised if the hand reaches in the
same direction but for a new object. They are not surprised if the hand
changes direction to reach for the original goal object. These responses are
not present if a mechanical object is doing the reaching (Woodward, 1998).
This set of observations suggests that these infants are attributing a goal to
the agent, and using this knowledge to predict the agent’s behaviour.
With regard to the attribution of desires to others, from around the age
of 12 months an understanding of intentions and desires begins to emerge.
Infants begin to understand the relationship between someone’s directed
gaze and the object of that gaze. For instance, they can use gaze direction
and a positive emotional expression to predict that the adult will reach for
that object (Phillips, Wellman & Spelke, 2002). Furthermore, they begin
to show an understanding of joint attention (looking at an object a parent
is looking at, for example). This reveals a reasonably complex cognitive
capacity to form a representation that includes an object of attention,
another person’s directed gaze, and their own gaze. This capacity begins
with gaze following: the infant follows the gaze of an adult to an object of
mutual attention. Initially, this only happens if the object is already within
the infant’s field of view. From ages 14-18 months, however, infants begin
to turn their heads in the direction of another’s gaze (Butterworth &
Jarrett, 1991; Caron et al, 1997).
How should we understand this evidence from pre-verbal infants?
It seems clear that infants have a desire-based theory of mind: they can
use emotional expressions to infer desired objects/goals (or things to
Contributions of cognitive science approaches to cognitive developmental psychology 451
be avoided), and predict intentional actions. It may be, however, that Referential versus
these understandings are referential rather than representational (Saxe, representational
Carey & Kanwisher, 2004). A desire or a goal may be conceived of as a understanding.
Very young children
connection between a person and a real object, but toddlers may have
seem to understand the
no representational understanding of desires. This line of reasoning relationship between
would explain the long delay between the development of desire-based looking at and wanting. A
theory of mind, which begins to be evident at around 14 months, and the referential understanding
representational belief-desire theory of mind inferences that only become of wanting links the person
possible at approximately four years of age (Bartsch & Wellman, 1995). and the object; it is a
An alternative line of argument, however, is that children younger concrete understanding
of actual things in the
than four are indeed capable of representational thinking. For instance,
world. A representational
Leslie (1987) argues strongly that the emergence of pretend play at around understanding is more
age 18 months is evidence of representational theory of mind ability. He abstract or symbolic, and is
uses the following example: a mother picks up a banana, and talks into decoupled from the actual
it as if it is a telephone. Her 18-month-old child will laugh, and will not concrete objects.
become confused about what bananas are and what telephones are. Leslie
calls this evidence of decoupling: the ability to represent thoughts about
objects separately from actual representations of objects. He also points
out that this type of interaction shows the child has an understanding of
the mother’s mental state (‘pretending’).
Regardless of whether or not one accepts that children younger than
four are capable of representational thinking, empirical and observational
studies make it clear that, from 14-24 months of age, normally developing
children start to engage in pretend play, to demonstrate joint attentional
capabilities, and to show dramatic increases in language learning (Duchan,
2000; Frith & Frith, 2003). From the ToM perspective, these developments
are crucial because pretend play and joint attention are viewed as
precursors of fully developed ToM capabilities (Charman et al, 2000), and
linguistic ability has been correlated with ToM test performance (Harris,
De Rosnay & Pons, 2005; Tager-Flusberg, 2007).
between actual objects and objects of thought. For instance, if they are told
that Anne has an apple, and Jane is thinking about an apple, they know
they can only touch Anne’s apple (Wellman & Estes, 1986). Furthermore,
it seems that three-year-olds may have some understanding of false belief.
Some researchers have demonstrated that, in the false belief test, when
asked where Sally will look for her sweet, three-year-olds tend to look first
towards the place where she put it, although they incorrectly say that she will
look for it in its new location (Onishi & Baillargeon, 2002). In other words,
the idea that seeing leads to knowing is beginning to emerge in three-year-
olds, even though it is only finally fully present in four-year-olds. By age
five years, typically developing children can fully understand others’ false
beliefs, appearance-reality differences and their own previous false beliefs
(Bibby & McDonald, 2005; Naito, Komatsu & Fuke, 1994).
PHO TO : ZO Ë MO O S MANN
girls begin performing at above chance levels at the age of seven, while
boys attain this level of performance at nine. Most typically, developing
children between nine and 11 years of age perform very well on this task.
Although we have no measures of individual differences in ToM abilities
in normal adults, some interesting data have emerged from Baron-Cohen
and his group, who have developed a test for use with adults: Reading the
Mind in the Eyes. Participants are shown photos of the eye area, and must
choose from a set of four adjectives the one that best describes the expression
pictured. This is the best ToM test for adults because, unlike others that have
been developed, it does not have ceiling effects. Studies on ToM in normal
ageing are also scarce. Happé, Winner & Brownell (1998) found that older
adults (mean age 73 years) performed better on a theory of mind task than
did adolescents (mean age 14) and young adults (mean age 22). Maylor et
al (2002), however, showed the opposite pattern, with theory of mind skills Children around seven or
declining across age bands: young adults performed better than 60- to 74-year- eight years can appreciate
olds, who in turn performed better than 75- to 89-year-olds. Obviously, far language forms such as
more research is needed to clarify patterns of ToM ability in adulthood. sarcasm, for example,
‘Mom, I’m really enjoying
these sandwiches’ while his
Theory of mind in autism face clearly shows he isn’t
This is the arena where most ToM research has taken place. In a seminal really enjoying them.
1985 paper, Baron-Cohen, Leslie, and Frith showed that 80 per cent of a
sample of children and adolescents with autism (and only mild degrees of Autism. A developmental
mental handicap) failed the false belief test that is passed by most typically disorder characterised by
developing four-year-olds. This poor performance was in contrast to that a triad of impairments:
deficits in language
of a group of children with Down’s Syndrome, who had moderate degrees
development, social
of mental handicap, and yet passed the false belief test with the same ease communication and
as normal children. This finding of a ToM deficit in autism has been imaginative play.
consistently replicated; alongside these replications are equally important
demonstrations that ToM is intact in other developmental disorders where
general intelligence and attention are impaired. For instance, children
with Down’s Syndrome, Williams Syndrome, and attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) do not show theory of mind impairments.
This set of findings suggests that ToM abilities are independent of general
intelligence and attention, and that ToM deficits are a specific feature of
autistic spectrum disorders (ASDs). Baron-Cohen subsequently put forward
the conceptualisation of autism as ‘mindblindness’—the inability to interact
with others as beings possessing feelings, thoughts and beliefs of their own.
The diagnosis of autism is not simple, as features of the disorder occur
along a spectrum. However, what is striking about children with any
autism spectrum disorder (including Asperger’s Syndrome) is a marked Asperger’s syndrome.
impairment in social function. These children appear to lack empathy and Part of the autism
to dislike social contact. They actively avoid making eye contact with their spectrum, but children
with this disorder usually
parents or anyone else, and often seem to dislike close physical contact.
have normal language
They are generally socially withdrawn, and do not establish emotional development.
relationships. The idea of the primary deficit in ASD being mindblindness
thus accounts very satisfactorily for these features.
In the years following the Baron-Cohen’s original study, research on
ToM in ASD individuals has enriched our understanding of both ToM
itself, and of how deficits in ToM in autism present. It seems that these
454 Developmental Psychology
deficits are evident from the very early stages of development in ASD
children. For instance, Baron-Cohen and others have shown that children
with ASD are deficient at using direction of gaze as an indicator of desire
or intention, and are deficient in joint attention abilities. They also tend
not to engage in spontaneous pretend play, and perform at chance levels in
distinguishing real from mental objects.
So, for example, we saw that typically developing three-year-olds could
reliably say that you can only touch the apple that Anne has. In contrast,
children with ASD (even those who are older than three years) are just as
likely to say you can touch the apple that Jane is thinking about. These
children also have difficulty on tests that assess understanding of the principle
that seeing leads to knowing. Typically developing three- to four-year-olds
have no trouble knowing that, of two dolls, only the one who has looked
inside a box knows what is inside. Children with ASD (even those who are
older than three to four years old) consistently fail to make this distinction.
Furthermore, children with ASD have difficulty distinguishing
between accidents and intentional actions (that is, whether someone
meant to do something, or whether it was an accident), and they seem
unable to engage in deceit. They do not understand sarcasm, metaphor
or irony. In summary, most low-functioning autistic children (especially
young ones) show severe deficits in ToM ability that can account for the
social, communicative and imaginative deficits seen in autism.
Therefore, most children and adolescents with ASD fail false belief
tasks such as the Sally-Anne test. This implies that these individuals
might have an early delay in ToM, so that their ToM ability is equivalent
to that of a typically developing child between one and two years of age
(in other words, an individual who has not yet developed joint attentional
and imaginative play capabilities).
A significant minority of autistic individuals (between 15 per cent and
55 per cent, depending on which study one reads) do, however, pass first-
order false belief tests (Happé & Frith, 1996). These individuals are usually
older and have higher verbal mental ages (VMAs) and verbal IQs than
do autistic individuals who fail first-order false belief tasks (Ozonoff &
McEvoy, 1994). In 1995, Happé reviewed 28 studies of ToM abilities in
individuals with ASD, and concluded that a verbal mental age (VMA)
of at least 11 years was needed before someone with ASD has an 80 per
cent chance of passing a false belief test. (In typically developing children,
those with a VMA of five years perform at this level.) Thus, a relatively
high verbal ability might be necessary, but not sufficient, for children
with ASD to pass false belief tasks. One might also think about these data
this way: Autistic individuals who pass first-order false belief tests have
reached a ToM ability equivalent to that of a typically developing four-
year-old. They do, however, usually fail second-order false belief tests that
are aimed at the typically developing six- to seven-year-old child.
Baron-Cohen has demonstrated ToM deficits even in older indi-
viduals with high functioning autism or Asperger’s Syndrome. These
individuals can pass false belief tests, but the deficit becomes evident on
more complex tasks; for instance, they perform poorly on the Faux Pas
test and on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes task.
Contributions of cognitive science approaches to cognitive developmental psychology 455
This pattern of data has led researchers to suggest that individuals with
ASD have a specific developmental delay in ToM (Baron-Cohen & Swetten-
ham, 1997). The findings of a longitudinal study by Steele, Joseph & Tager-
Flusberg (2003) support this delayed development hypothesis. In that study, Delayed development.
autistic children aged four to 14 years showed significant improvement in Means an ability comes
ToM abilities over the course of one year. Of particular interest in the Steele online much later than
in typically developing
et al (2003) study is that the researchers used a developmentally sensitive
children.
battery that included tests for early-developing aspects of ToM, such as
desire-based action tasks. Indeed, they found that most of their participants’
improvement took place between early ToM and first-order ToM abilities.
The findings of other studies have provided support for a deviance, rather Deviant development.
than a delayed, account of ToM development in ASD. For instance, Peterson, Means the developmental
Wellman & Liu (2005) found that individuals with autism, aged six to 14 years, trajectory may follow
a different pattern
showed a different ToM developmental pathway to both typically developing
altogether, with abilities
children and deaf children with a ToM deficit. Specifically, although the coming online in a
participants in all three groups showed the same developmental sequence in different order to typically
the acquisition of early ToM abilities, children with ASD found the false belief developing children.
task more difficult than a hidden emotions task, while typically developing,
late-signing deaf, and native-signing deaf children found the hidden emotions
task the most difficult. So, while both autistic and late-signing deaf children
have deficits in ToM, deaf children follow the same developmental pattern as
typically developing children, whereas children with autism seem to follow a
different developmental pattern, one that may be unique to autism.
There is, therefore, empirical support for both delayed and deviant
hypotheses of ToM development in ASD. That is to say, ToM development
does happen in ASD, but it happens more slowly and might follow a differ-
ent path to that in typically developing children. Furthermore, results from
longitudinal studies by Holroyd & Baron-Cohen (1994) and Ozonoff & Mc-
Evoy (1994) point to a possible ceiling effect in ToM development in ASD.
Recently, Baron-Cohen and colleagues (2005, 2008) have proposed that
autism be understood in terms of two dimensions: empathising and sys-
tematising. They regard empathy as the foundation of ToM abilities, argue
that in autism empathy is clearly deficient. Systematising is the quest for order,
an interest in rule-bound phenomena; this tendency is present in all of us to
greater or lesser degrees, but in autism, it is pathologically strong. This lack
of balance between empathising and systematising in autistic individuals
explains other features of ASD, such as rigid, repetitive behaviour, reliance on
routine, mathematical skills, fascination with mechanical objects, and etc.
Theory. ‘Theory theory’
Theories of theory of mind states that theory of mind
No consensus exists as to how ToM should be theorised. As mentioned is a metarepresentational
previously, the evidence around a universally consistent developmental ability. We theorise
trajectory strongly suggests that it is an innate capacity. Some argue that abstractly about what
ToM is in fact a ‘folk psychological’ theory—a metacognitive ability to people think or feel.
represent and reason about others in terms of their beliefs, knowledge, ‘Simulation theory’
states theory of mind is
intentions, etc. Within this school of thought are those who see it as a
grounded in mirror neuron
dedicated cognitive module; that is, an independent set of schemas and activity, for example our
processes that act exclusively on ToM information. ability to empathise with
Another line of thinking explains ToM according to a simulationist the pain of others.
456 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
CR I T I C A L T H I N KI NG TA S KS
➋ How and why is the cognitive science approach to cognitive development superior
to information-processing approaches? Provide examples of where information-
processing approaches fall short and the cognitive science approach does not.
General tasks
➊ How do cognitive theories help us to make predictions about development and behaviour?
➋ Create your own theory of cognitive development by combining those aspects of the
theories discussed above that you feel are most important for explaining cognitive
development. Justify your choices.
Contributions of cognitive science approaches to cognitive developmental psychology 457
Recommended readings
These are classic textbooks in developmental and cognitive psychology. The reader interested in the historical
foundations of the field of cognitive developmental psychology could consult these books to gain a deeper
understanding, for instance, of how the work of major figures in developmental psychology (such as Vygotsky
and Piaget) has been integrated into a cognitive scientific approach.
General:
Bjorklund, D F (1995). Children’s thinking: Developmental function and individual differences.
Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Flavell, J H, Miller, P H & Miller, S A (1993). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice Hall.
Kozulin, A (1990). Vygotsky’s psychology: A biography of ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Piaget, J (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.
Sternberg, R J (2006). Cognitive psychology, 4th ed. Belmont, CA: Thomson-Wadsworth.
Memory:
These works provide the reader with broad overviews of theoretical approaches to the development of
autobiographical memory in infancy and early childhood, and also review major empirical studies in the field.
Bauer, P J (2006). Remembering the times of our lives: Memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nadel, L & Zola-Morgan, S (1984). ‘Infantile amnesia: A neurobiological perspective’
In M Moscovitch (ed), Infant memory, 145-172. New York: Plenum.
Pillemer, D B (1998). ‘What is remembered about early childhood events?’, Clinical Psychology
Review, 18, 895-913.
Theory of Mind:
Baron-Cohen, S (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Frith, U & Frith, C (2003). ‘Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing’, Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 459-473.
Saxe, R & Baron-Cohen, S (eds) (2007). Theory of mind. London: Psychology Press.
CHAPTER
22
Vygotsky’s theory of the
development of cultural
tools
Gillian Mooney
Historical background
The social and historical structure in which theorists
are located is a vital component of analysing their
contributions to our understanding of human
development. Theories, or ways of understanding
human development, do not emerge in a vacuum.
Rather, theories and theorists are products and
producers of knowledge in particular social and
historical circumstances. Theorists, or indeed
any reader of psychological writing, are never
completely neutral or objective interpreters of
ideas. We are unable to be totally neutral and
Lev Vygotsky
Vy g o t s k y ’s t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f c u l t u r a l t o o l s 459
local teachers’ training college, and started his doctorate on the psychology
of art (Wertsch, 1987).
In January 1924 Vygotsky, as an unknown theorist from the provinces,
delivered a lecture on the psychology of consciousness in Leningrad. The
audience was so impressed by the clarity and brilliance of his thinking that
he was recommended for a position at the Moscow Institute of Psychology.
In Moscow, Vygotsky worked at a frenetic pace, despite being very ill with
tuberculosis. He completed not only his doctorate, but also rapidly produced
research and worked in neurological clinics. He was both loved and
respected by students, who listened to his lectures through open windows
when venues were too small and who wrote poems in honour of his journeys
to either lecture or conduct research. He worked collaboratively with many
colleagues and students, many of whom actually conducted research in the
satellite states when Vygotsky was too ill to travel (Wertsch, 1987).
In 1934, the year in which his most widely-read work, Thinking and
Speech, was published, Vygotsky succumbed to tuberculosis. The Soviet
government banned his work in 1936 because of Vygotsky’s research on
intelligence testing. The ban reflects one of the many ironies of Stalinist
ideology. The Stalinist state was opposed to the use of intelligence
testing and missed the subtleties of Vygotsky’s argument against the
use of conventional intelligence tests, which he believed were static
and, thus, did not encapsulate potential for development, and were,
therefore, an unhelpful measure of an individual’s ability. Even though
the ban on Vygotsky’s work was only lifted in 1956, his many students
and colleagues kept his ideas alive. Most notable of these is A.R. Luria,
a neuropsychologist, who is well known in the West (Wertsch, 1987).
Vygotsky’s place in history, that is, post-revolutionary Russia during the
infancy of the practice of Communism, resulted in his attempt to create
a scientific approach to the investigation of the development of the mind.
His quest for psychology to be scientific was informed by his allegiance to
the Marxist method of analysis.
(Newman & Holzman, 1993: 13). History is, thus, an ongoing part of a whole
that is a process with a beginning, a present and a future. Not carefully
scrutinising historical process would result in an incomplete understanding
of development; it would be analogous to describing the democratic state in
South Africa without any reference to apartheid, colonisation, or relations of
power between indigenous peoples. Vygotsky, who focused on the cultural
tool of language, analysed language development at the start of formation,
that is, when children learn to verbally interact with other people in their
environments. He believed that analysing adult language skills did not
tell us very much about the development of language because, in order to
understand a historical process, we need to describe it from its origin. This
is known as Vygotsky’s genetic or instrumental method.
As we live our lives from birth to death, or as development occurs, there
is a tension between the ways of thinking that we have already acquired and
the new forms of thinking emerging as social practice, in our interactions
with other individuals. Thus, historical materialism is dialectical because
it concerns the action of opposing forces or interplay between juxtaposed
concepts. These forces are dynamic in the sense that constant change or
activity characterise development. Any social circumstance, or level of
development, may be analysed by determining its constituent parts or units.
These units are then placed alongside, or in opposition to, one another. It is
these units and, more importantly, the relationships between these units that
provide an account of a complex whole. The notion of a unity of concepts or
units is central to this method of analysis.
For Marx, relations of power (between ownership of capital and
workers) and the means of production (for example, technological tools such
as machines or computers) were the two central forces in the development
of societies. Vygotsky provided many analytic units in the development of
the mind. These will be discussed in greater detail below.
tasks. It is the use of tools that represent the higher forms of thinking
that characterise human development.
These tools are ‘cultural’ or ‘ideological’ in the sense that they are used by
distinct groups of people and are related to the exercise of power in society,
or between groups of people. Ideology, in this sense, is a system of ideas
and ideals forming the basis of a political or economic theory and is, more
generally, the set of beliefs or ways of doing things that are characteristic of
particular social groups. In the example of the role of the computer in shaping
our consciousness, those who are computer-literate, or who are able to use a
computer, have access to far more information and ideas than those who
are not. In this sense, computer-literate individuals, those who have appro-
priated the tool, are able to acquire more power in the societal structure.
They have jobs that are described as skilled labour. They are able to perform
a multiplicity of tasks and are thus able to work in many areas of the labour
market. They are more highly rewarded in terms of money for their work,
and, consequently, occupy a more prestigious place in the structure of society,
forming part of the middle, rather than the working class.
CULTURAL TOOL
and do not adequately indicate that the sign has been incorporated into
the individual’s consciousness. The central focus of Vygotsky’s theory is
how you would actually use the tool, how you appropriate the tool, how
cultural forms of thinking are created by, and are active in your thinking,
or the ways in which you engage with the world.
Tools alter the characteristics and course of mental processes. These
instruments of learning re-create and re-organise the entire structure of
our thinking and behaviour (Vygotsky, 1987). It was with the concept
of the tool functioning to re-organise our thinking that Vygotsky separated
himself from the circular forms of logic proposed by the behaviourist school
of thought. Vygotsky (1929) represented this re-organisation of thinking
and interacting in the world in the following manner (see Figure 22.1).
The dotted line between the task and the response to the task represented
the explanation provided by the behaviourists—a simple stimulus-response
bond. For Vygotsky, this dotted line represented an individual’s automatic
response, encompassing the ways of thinking that the individual had already
acquired. Vygotsky was interested in determining how the individual
learnt new ways of thinking (as depicted by the solid lines). These new
ways of thinking incorporated new cultural tools, which altered the way
in which tasks were understood and how problem-solving occurred. What
is of central importance to Vygotsky’s ideas is that the new cultural tool
fundamentally alters the process of responding to tasks. Vygotsky was
attempting to describe how the use of cultural tools becomes automatic in an
individual’s functioning, or how we automatically use the tools of thinking.
The tool used by the individual could not be separated from the response
to the task using that tool because the tool would represent a different way
of thinking. If two individuals used two different tools in order to solve the
same problem, then their responses to the same task would be qualitatively
different (Vygotsky, 1987).
Vygotsky presented evidence for his ideas through the use of analogies
and examples. While this form of evidence does not conform to acceptable
standards of research in psychology today, the example below may help you
to understand the role of tools in changing the ways in which we think:
Private speech
<<
central function of private speech. Berk behaviour. In addition, Berk found
also found that children talked to evidence that private speech develops
themselves more often when working similarly in all children, irrespective of
alone on challenging tasks and also cultural background, and that it
when their teacher was not immediately arises from social experience, as
available to help them, that is, when the Vygotsky maintained.
child needed to take charge of his own Kate Cockcroft
Beyond the upper limit are those tasks that the child is as yet unable to perform, even with assistance,
since they are beyond his or her current cognitive capacity
Upper limit: the highest level of problem solving that the child is capable of,
given the assistance of an able instructor
Lower limit: the level of problem solving that the child is capable of when working independently
the formation of mind, but rather, as an argument for the inclusion of social
factors in the formation of the individual (Wertsch, 1985).
The historical schism in the development of Vygotsky’s ideas is the
result of the manner in which the ‘social’ and the ‘individual’ have been
demarcated and the way in which the relationship between the two
has been conceptualised (Daniels, 2001). There appears to be pervasive
agreement that Vygotsky rejected any rigorous separation of the social
and the individual. In this conception, the social and individual are
‘mutually formative elements in a single interacting system’ (Daniels,
2001: 72). The interacting system may, on the surface, appear to be the
application of Marxist method to the study of development. However,
the neo-Vygotskians have predominantly focused on only one half of
this mutually interacting system, namely, the individual. They do not
explain the dynamic interaction between the social and the individual in
real material circumstances and have primarily focused on ontology as
unrelated to epistemology (Newman & Holzman (1993).
Wertsch (1999) argues that this genetic method dictates that higher mental
functions, therefore, should be examined in terms of processes, rather
than products. This constitutes a distinct shift away from the ‘objects and
outcomes’ of the activity approach. This is congruent with both Marx and
Vygotsky’s conceptualisations of development (albeit in a diluted form in
which revolution is not pivotal). Secondly, Wertsch (1985) believed that the
guiding assumption of the General Genetic Law of Cultural Development
is that mental processes originate from social interaction. It is at this point
that Wertsch violates the central principle of any dialectic, and indeed the
General Genetic Law of Cultural Development, which states that a higher
level of mental processing is the result of the interplay between both the social
and the individual planes. Accordingly, social interaction alone does not
cause the development of mental process (a position that Wertsch proposes).
His cause-and-effect relationship does not meet the requirements of any
juxtapositioning. The third theme that Wertsch believed characterised
Vygotsky’s work is that human action is mediated by tools/signs (Penuel
& Wertsch, 1995). Here Wertsch (1999) places emphasis on the individual
plane and his ontological framework reflects his interest in Linguistics
as the term ‘sign’ is favoured over the term ‘tool’ (Wertsch, 1991; 1993;
1998; 1999). For Wertsch, sign systems are resources in action rather than
representational systems only. Signs transform the purposes of their users
and mediate mental functions, a hierarchical relationship in which signs
themselves dominate those who use them (Penuel & Wertsch, 1995). Wertsch
(1981), with his conceptualisation of social practices in which individuals are
provided with tools and thereby master and transform activity, highlighted
Vygotsky’s idea of mutually formative elements (the individual and social
planes) in an interacting system. What Wertsch does not make clear is that
both tools and activity have no meaning unless they are defined in relation
to one another. Wertsch does not provide a clear account of how Vygotsky’s
natural line and cultural line (with tools) may be distinguished.
Wertsch’s (1987) framework is founded on two fundamental assumptions:
Mediation
The notion of mediation is characterised by a historical schism in the
interpretation of Vygotsky’s work, namely, activity theory (Soviet) and
the sociocultural approach (Western). This historical split between
activity theory and semiotic means of mediation is a result of the different
conceptualisations of the relationship between the social and individual
planes (Daniels, 2001) or the social and the historical. These two distinct
paths of neo-Vygotskian thought are consequently characterised by both
philosophical and sociohistorical difference. What the two approaches
have in common is that neither resorts to a deterministic position. Both
positions recognise that, in terms of individual development, individuals
actively shape the cultural forces that are central in their development
(Kozulin, 1990). This may be conceptualised as a mediational model in
which the social and individual planes are mutually formative.
Activity
Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (1987) contained
a chapter by Davydov and Radzikhovskii (1987) that outlined the activity-
oriented approach. They make their opinion about Wertsch’s interpretation
of Vygotsky’s work quite transparent in their chapter in Wertsch’s book,
by making no reference to him whatsoever in their discussion of activity-
oriented psychology. Davydov and Radzikhovskii’s (1987) activity theory
had joint activity/practice as the unit of analysis and centred on the role
of mediating artefacts in learning (Engestrom, 1996). Davydov and
Radzikhovskii (1987) detailed an instruction strategy as ascending from
the abstract to the concrete, in line with Vygotsky’s scientific concepts/
everyday concepts juxtaposition. The goal of learning at school was to place
knowledge out into the world by making it theoretically powerful and
dynamic when the student is faced with theoretical problems (Engestrom,
1996). Thus, activity theorists locate psychological development and analysis
as grounded in practical cultural activities (Daniels, 2001), seemingly in line
with Vygotsky’s emphasis. Activity theorists define an activity as students
using the tools that they are given for a specific purpose. The effect of objects
in the social plane on the motivation of subjects was viewed as central
(Leontiev, 1981). This conceptualisation was not founded on an account of
any social structures, which may act to organise and confine any act on the
social plane (Penuel & Wertsch, 1991). Davydov (1988) did attempt to address
this concern by adding rules, the community and the division of labour
(that is, lecturers’ control and students’ study) into his conceptualisation.
However, these notions were considered in a narrow way, for example rules
(classroom behavioural codes and assessment standards), and community
(classroom), and did not engage in issues concerning the impact of wider
sociohistorical factors.
PHOTO: KATE COCKCROF T
The Westerners, Lave (1988; 1993), Lave and Wenger (1991; 1996) and
Wenger (1999), in their communities of practice approach, or legitimate
peripheral participation model, attempted to redefine elements of the mo-
del provided by Davydov (1988). This approach conceptualises the student
as a legitimate participant (subject) and defines tools as the technologies
of transparency and simulation stories, and the tools of the established
practice. Davydov’s (1985) ‘community’ was defined as a ‘community of
practice’ within a school setting. Engestrom (1996) attempted to reconcile
the disparate positions of Davydov, Lave and Wenger. His ‘learning by
expanding’ approach was based on studies with adolescents and positioned
the school as a collective instrument (perhaps reflecting an attempt to
incorporate the ideas of Wertsch).
These three interpretations share key ideas. Firstly, their research
focus is on joint activity or practice. Secondly, the theorists clearly position
themselves as followers of the cultural-historical school of Vygotsky,
apparently because of their focus on activity. These theories of activity,
however, differ in their interpretations of previous neo-Vygotskian
work. Davydov (1988) focused on the translation and interpretation by
Leontiev (1981), while Engestrom (1996), Lave (1993) and Wenger (1999)
demonstrate the influence of the Western interpretation of Wertsch
(1985). Thirdly, all three perspectives claim to emphasise the role in
learning of mediating artefacts, which are socially created (Engestrom,
Education prepares 1996). Their triangular conceptualisation places tools at the apex, with the
individuals for participation community (which is narrowly defined) at the centre of the base. This
in social life (or society), triangular conceptualisation does not reflect the explanatory principle of
thus schools (and the dialectic between two analytically distinct constructs in the traditions
consequently, 'school
culture') are key agents
of Marx and Vygotsky. Their subject, learner and group (Davydov, 1988),
of socialisation of the student as a legitimate participant (Lave, 1993; Wenger, 1999), and a
the individual. team of students, lecturers, practitioners, local people (Engestrom, 1996),
PHOTO: ZOË MOOSMANN
Vy g o t s k y ’s t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f c u l t u r a l t o o l s 487
Conclusion
In the introduction to this section of this book, it is mentioned that
cognitive developmental psychologists are interested in how our mental
processes are influenced by our biology and experiences. For Vygotsky,
biology is the natural line of development, maturation as the gradual
unfolding of our genetic blueprints. Our experiences are our interactions
with others in the real world, the social, the external. These experiences,
Vy g o t s k y ’s t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f c u l t u r a l t o o l s 489
our interactions with other people in the world form the basis, or provide
the structure, of our thinking. This means that our thinking does more
than influence our experiences in the real world. Rather, our experiences
and our interactions with other people in the world form the basis or
structure of our thinking.
There are, however, points of agreement between Vygotsky’s notion
of sociohistorical development and subsequent cognitive developmental
theory. The central concepts in cognition relate directly to Vygotsky’s
ideas about human development, in particular to the development of
the human mind. These concepts reveal that cognition is a collective
term that describes a process, that developmental processes include the
notion of change, that individuals are accorded an active role, and that
the intellectual feats of humans are unique. Our thinking, or cognition,
has many parts. Cognition is one theoretical term that represents many
other concepts and theoretical terms. A central concept of cognition is
that it relates directly to the use of knowledge. The important questions
that cognitive psychologists ask include, ‘How do we learn?’, or, ‘How do
we acquire new thinking and new ways of doing things?’. Cognition is a
process, a series of functions, which become increasingly more complex
in the sequence/series. Cognition as the process of appropriating the ways
of thinking inherent in our ‘cultures’, or knowledge is congruent with
Vygotsky’s work on the development of the mind. Our thinking is an
active process. We do not passively internalise information. We are not
the victims either of culture or of the dominant ways in which a society
is structured. When we learn the ways of the thinking of our society, we
actively reconstruct our existing ways of thinking and form new ways of
thinking and interacting with others. Cognition is developmental in the
sense that describes changes in the ways in which we think. The notion
of change also makes the concept of cognition ideological in the sense that
‘advanced’ forms of thinking are regarded as demonstrating a better, or
higher, level of cognitive development. However, on what basis cognition
is considered advanced and what relationships of power are present when
some ways of thinking are subordinated to others are often not questioned
or investigated by cognitive developmental theorists.
Vygotsky’s work may certainly be characterised as ideological. His
notion of what kind of thinking is advanced is discussed in his descriptions
of higher mental functions. One of his central aims was to indoctrinate
Communism in the Stalinist Soviet state. He believed that Marx’s
Communism was the most progressive, or advanced, way of organising
human society, and thus, structuring the thinking of individuals in that
society. Our ways of thinking, our ‘intellect feats’ are unique. Vygotsky
wrote extensively about the unique nature of human consciousness.
Human consciousness is (currently) the highest form of thinking on a
planet called Earth. We have subordinated all other forms of life on Earth
to the development of our species. We believe that other ways of ‘thinking’,
by animals and machines that have ‘artificial intelligence’, are so inferior
to our own that even the people who are relatively new to our society, our
collective of people, are capable of these lower forms of thought. In this
sense, cognition is ideological. We value the ways in which humans think.
490 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
➊ How has being able to read and use a computer changed the ways in which you
think and understand the world?
➋ Discuss how Vygotsky’s ideas about development have resolved the tensions between
the relative roles of nature (genetic inheritance) and nurture (the environment).
Recommended readings
Karpov, Y V (2006). The Neo-Vygotskian Approach to Child Development. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
(Karpov discusses the Soviet interpretation of Vygotsky’s ideas in the 20th century.)
Newman, F & Holzman, L (1993). Lev Vygotsky. Revolutionary scientist. London: Routledge.
(Following the eminent American neo-Vygotskian traditional of both Cole and Wertsch, Newman
and Holzman offers a new interpretation of the role of revolution in Vygotsky’s work.)
Vygotsky, L S (1987). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume One. Problems of General
Psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
(This volume contains Vygotsky’s famous work ‘Thinking and Speech’.)
Vy g o t s k y ’s t h e o r y o f t h e d e v e l o p m e n t o f c u l t u r a l t o o l s 491
Vygotsky, L S (1993). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume Two. The Fundamentals of
Defectology. Abnormal Psychology and Learning Disabilities. New York: Plenum Press.
(Vygotsky discussed physical and mental disabilities with a focus on participation in social life.)
Vygotsky, L S (1997a). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume Three. Problems of the Theory
and History of Psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
(Vygotsky discusses important questions concerning the nature of ontology and epistemology in
psychological knowledge.)
Vygotsky, L S (1997b). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume Four. The History of the
Development of Higher Mental Functions. New York: Plenum Press.
(The nature of sociohistorical development is fully explored in this volume.)
Vygotsky, L S (1998). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume Five. Child Psychology. New York:
Plenum Press.
(The notion of developmental change is discussed with reference to infancy, childhood and adolescence.)
Vygotsky, L S (1999). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume Six. Scientific Legacy. New York:
Plenum Press.
(This volume presents an explication of Vygotsky’s primary ontological concepts.)
SECTION THREE
Psychosocial and socio-political
contexts of development
CHAPTER
23
Developmental psychology:
Critiques and contextual
considerations
Norman Duncan
fact be seen as resulting in the obfuscation of the processes governing Traditional definition
not only human development but also various extant social asymmetries. of development.
Specifically, these theorists argue that developmental psychology and Developmental psychology
is that sub-discipline of
research often aid in the concealment of social asymmetries, such as
psychology that describes,
gender, racial and age-related inequalities through its ‘naturalisation’ or explicates and predicts the
‘normalisation’ of said inequalities. ongoing physical, mental,
For much of the history of developmental psychology, a dominant emotional and behavioural
discourse in the sub-discipline has been that human development is largely development in individuals
a function of intra-individual processes and the individual’s immediate throughout the human
socialising environment. life-span.
Rose (1990: 1-2), however, correctly argues that human development
is not simply an internally propelled process of change or a process
mediated only by the individual’s immediate environment. Instead, it is
also a process that is significantly mediated by broader external public-
political and social processes and conditions. Indeed, as Rose (1990: 1)
argues, human development is an ‘intensively governed’ process that is
continuously subjected to ‘the scope and aspirations of public powers …
at the level of social and political strategies and institutions and techniques
of administration and regulation’ (Rose 1990: 1). The most obvious mani-
festation of this ‘management’ and regulation, Rose (1990: 2) posits, has
traditionally been ‘the complex apparatuses’ targeting the child, such as
the child welfare system, the schooling system, and the surveillance and
control of parents.
For example, Rose (1990) and Burman (2008) illustrate how the
schooling system was harnessed to deal with the anxieties of the domi-
nant classes concerning the growing levels of pauperism and crime in
nineteenth-century England. Specifically, they argue that compulsory
primary schooling was introduced during this period as a means of
occupying ‘disorderly’ groups, such as the pauperised working classes
and keeping them under routine surveillance (Burman, 2008). It has
to be remembered that during this period Europe was experiencing
unprecedented levels of social restiveness and turbulence. Additionally,
rapid industrialisation accompanied by increasing urbanisation led to
the development of sprawling slums, disease and crime. Within this
context, extant dominant groups viewed the working classes with
growing apprehension; hence the implementation of compulsory
primary education and concerns about ‘appropriate’ child-rearing
practices. These concerns were later incorporated into developmental
psychology in significant ways (Burman, 2008).
The role of developmental psychology in what Rose (1990) refers to
as the ‘administration and regulation’ of people and social conditions in
the interest of the powerful was also reflected in the early 20th century
when the political and social agendas of the powerful in Europe (and
later in South Africa) became increasingly preoccupied with finding
scientific justifications for oppression of other-than-European/white
groups (See Chapter 28 by Kasese-Hara). At this point, it was to
the technologies of developmental psychology, such as intelligence
tests and comparable instruments that the ruling elites turned their
attention. It has been argued that, internationally, developmental
496 Developmental Psychology
Developmental psychology is one of the Moreover, given the expertise and social
contemporary disciplines that have authority attributed to developmental
developed a whole range of conceptual psychology in contemporary society, its
systems and ‘truths’ that enable the better power to influence the regulation of the
management and ‘regulation of the human human subject is immense.
subject’ (Rose, 1990: 2).
Popular representation Poster (1988) and Rose (1990) allude to another manner in which
of developmental developmental psychology may have been complicit in social control or
psychology. regulation. During periods when various labour markets internationally
Developmental psychology
experienced a surfeit of workers (as was the case after World War II,
is concerned with the
‘practicalities of child
for example), it was developmental psychology that aided in the produc-
development or ... tion of research and literature indicating the importance of women
human development’ providing full-time home-based care to their children. Developmental
(Burman, 2008: 13). psychologists such as John Bowlby (in Rose, 1990) explicitly admonished
mothers who through routine absences deprived their children of
‘vital’ ongoing maternal care and presence. Such children, according
to Bowlby’s writings, were destined for ‘delinquency’ and a range of
adjustment problems. In this manner, developmental psychologists
reinforced a dominant discourse that had become increasingly evident
in Europe in the nineteenth century, namely a discourse that argued that
mothers were ultimately responsible for the wellbeing of the individual,
and more specifically, for social order (Refer to the discussion in the box
titled Motherhood and social regulation).
It is of course important to note here, as correctly argued by Walkerdine
(in Burman, 2008), that broader social and political motivations cannot be
said to have ‘caused in any simple sense’ the tendencies of developmental
psychology to bolster the interests of the socially and politically
dominant. Rather, it is perhaps more accurate to view these social and
political motivations, on the one hand, and the agendas of developmental
psychology on the other, as being ‘mutually implicated, making and
remaking the other possible’ (Walkerdine, in Burman, 2008: 23).
In view of the attention given to the views of critical developmental
psychologists such as Burman (2008) and Rose (1990) above, it might be
apposite to briefly consider what critical developmental psychology is.
Developmental psychology: Critiques and contextual considerations 497
As indicated in at least two chapters in this providing adequate chances for each
volume, Erik Erikson proposed a individual to attain these values’. Of course,
psychosocial theory of human development. we know that even in societies in which
It is frequently argued that this theory Erikson’s values might have currency, all
advances a more holistic view of human people simply do not have equal
development than its more traditional opportunities to acquire these values.
psychoanalytic predecessors, in that it Lastly, as Poster (1988: 71) correctly
ostensibly pays greater attention to the asserts, through his valorisation of the
impact of social factors on human supposed synthesis between the individual
development. However, herein also resides and social institutions, Erikson effectively
at least three key weaknesses in the theory, repositions the study of human
according to Poster (1988). development away from the ‘conflicts ...
According to Erikson one of the key discontinuities ... [and] antagonisms’ that
consequences of the individual’s successful characterise the human condition in
interaction with her or his society is the contemporary society. Indeed, unlike its
internalisation of a set of values, namely psychoanalytic predecessors, Erikson’s
hope, will, purpose, competence, fidelity, theory constrains us ‘only to see society as
love, care and wisdom. However, as Poster a harmonious unity ... never needing to be
(1988) observes, firstly, this postulate is challenged fundamentally or to be
based on the fallacious assumption that overturned, never really oppressive’ (Poster,
these values have the same import in all 1988: 71). Scrutiny of the conditions
contexts. Secondly, Poster (1988: 70) characterising contemporary society
notes, the manner in which this postulate indicates that it is certainly not
is articulated in Erikson’s theory, results in harmonious, nor is it free of oppression—
‘an affirmation of all social orders as quite the contrary.
Developmental psychology: Critiques and contextual considerations 499
Generative theory and frequently employed in psychology as exemplars, the chapter importantly
knowledge. Theory and also contains a critical examination of the ways in which gender has
knowledge capable of traditionally been defined and investigated in psychology.
generating new insights
In the final chapter of this section and volume, Macleod explores
and social transformation.
the theoretical beliefs and assumptions that have informed the research
and literature in South African developmental psychology in recent
years. Using some of the key theoretical orientations in developmental
psychology as her point of departure, she provides a thorough critical
analysis of the predominant forms of research and literature that have
emerged from the sub-discipline in South Africa. This critical inventory
provides an appropriate conclusion for this volume.
While ostensibly fairly diverse in focus, the chapters in this section
have one important feature in common, and that is their critical
engagement with the field of developmental psychology. Importantly,
this criticality is not an end in itself, but is introduced as a means of
providing pointers for the development of more generative (Cf. Gergen
& Gergen, 1994) and contextually appropriate developmental psycho-
logy theory and knowledge.
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➊ Describe the ways in which developmental psychology theory and research can be seen
to aid in the concealment of inequalities in society.
➋ How has South African developmental psychology in the past, according to you,
provided justification for the racialised inequalities in South African society?
➍ Identify the ways in which any of the theories described earlier in this volume could
be seen to justify existing social inequalities.
Recommended readings
Three key texts are recommended for readers wishing to further explore some of the issues broached in
this chapter:
Burman, E (2008). Deconstructing developmental psychology. London: Routledge.
(This book provides a very accessible overall critique of developmental psychology. Highly recommended.)
Poster, M (1988). Critical theory of the family. New York: Seabury Press.
(This book provides an incisive analysis and critique of extant theories of the family and
human development.)
Rose, N (1991). Governing the soul: The shaping of the private self. London: Routledge.
(While not focused on developmental psychology alone, this book provides a very useful critical
description of the history of the sub-discipline.)
CHAPTER
24
Bronfenbrenner’s ecological
theory of development
Derek Hook
of influences, which impact not only on the child’s relationship with his
mother, but on his relationship within the immediate family more generally,
on his life, on the familial resources available to him and so forth. Thus,
the healthy development of the child is intricately entwined with factors
such as role demands and stresses placed on the parents, the flexibility
of the parents’ job schedules, the adequacy of child-care arrangements,
the presence of family friends and neighbours, the quality of health and
services, neighbourhood safety and so forth (Bronfenbrenner, 1979).
Bronfenbrenner’s conception of environmental influences on
development is not only systemic, it also recognises the fact that
environments are non-static forms of influence that exist in a constant
state of flux. Bronfenbrenner recognises that what can be said of a
given developmental environment today may not necessarily be true
of that environment tomorrow. It is also important to understand that
the developing individual is very possibly an agent of change within a
given environment, including larger environments. In Bronfenbrenner’s
view (1977), this fact is rarely dealt with satisfactorily in psychosocial
Multi-person systems of
developmental studies. In this way Bronfenbrenner (1979) argues that to
interaction. An awareness
understand human development adequately one needs to examine multi- of the complex and
person systems of interaction that are not limited to a single setting, and multifaceted nature of
that take into account aspects of the environment beyond the immediate patterns of interaction
situation that contains the subject. within groups of people.
objective sense, but as how the subject perceives and experiences it. This
subjective approach arises from Bronfenbrenner’s certainty of the:
n osyste m
Ch ro
cr o s y st e m
Ma
s y st e m
Exo
o s y st e
es
M
m
osyste
icr
m
M
The microsystem
The microsystem is the immediate context that directly affects the Microsystem.
developing person (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). It is the realm of face-to-face The microsystem is the
bi-directional relationships where influences flow back and forth so that complex of face-to-face,
bi-directional relationships
a new baby will affect the lives of his parents, just as their attitudes will
between the developing
affect the baby (Papalia, Olds & Feldman, 1998). The microsystem is the person and important
complex of relations between the developing person and important figures figures such as caregivers,
such as caregivers, parents, siblings, friends, classmates and teachers. It is parents, siblings and
important to understand that this complex of relationships includes the friends. It is the smallest
connections across various people within the immediate setting, so that environmental system in
the relationship between the child’s father and the child’s grandfather will Bronfenbrenner’s theory
of development.
also ultimately exert a degree of influence on the child.
Bronfenbrenner’s microsystem is assembled from three basic factors
comprising the pattern of activities, roles and interpersonal relations
experienced by the developing person (1979). By ‘role’ he means the set of
behaviours and expectations associated with a position in society, such as
that of mother, baby, teacher, friend, and so on. He ties the microsystem
firmly to particular concrete settings, such as the home, the day-care
centre, the school playground and suchlike.
To get a sense of the next environmental system of development—
the mesosytem—we need to ‘zoom out’ a little and take a slightly broad-
er overview of the developmental context by looking at interrelations
among microsystems.
506 Developmental Psychology
The mesosystem
Mesosystem. A system
of microsystems, formed
The mesosystem is a system of microsystems, which is formed whenever
whenever the developing the developing person moves into a new setting (Bronfenbrenner, 1979).
person moves into a (Remember that a setting, for Bronfenbrenner, is a place where people
new setting. The sphere readily engage in face-to-face interaction.) The bi-directional interactions
of the mesosystem also of the microsystem are now enlarged to the extent that we are now looking
accommodates linkages at slightly higher-order environments, such as the school as a whole, the
and interconnections
home taken to the level of neighbourhood, the extended family, and social
between the different
facets of microsystems.
relationships on the level of peer groups. The sphere of the mesosytem also
accommodates linkages and interconnections between the different facets
of microsystems. This means that influences across home and school, peer
and family groups, work and recreational settings, are also considered.
A case in point here is how parents and teachers may collaborate in
educational planning for the child (Craig, 1996).
The exosystem
Exosystem. The social The exosystem refers to the social setting or organisation beyond the
setting or organisation individual’s immediate experience that nevertheless affects him or her.
beyond the individual’s Examples may range from formal settings like a parent’s workplace, the
immediate experience that
community, welfare health systems, or the activities of the local school
nevertheless affects him
or her. It is an extension
board, to less formal organisations like the parents’ network of friends or
of the mesosystem, the school class of an older sibling (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Bronfenbrenner
embracing other specific defines the exosystem as:
social structures, both
formal and informal, …an … extension of the mesosystem embracing other specific social
that impinge upon the structures, both formal and informal, that … impinge upon or
immediate settings in
encompass the immediate settings in which that person is found, and
which that person
is found.
thereby influence, delimit, or even determine what goes on there.
These structures include the major institutions of the society, both
deliberately structured and spontaneously evolving, as they operate at
a concrete level. They encompass, among other structures, the world of
work, the neighbourhood, the mass media, agencies of government, the
distribution of goods and services, communication and transportation
facilities and informal social networks (Bronfenbrenner, 1977: 515).
PHO TO : G I LL MO O NEY
popular discourses, values, laws
and customs. Bronfenbrenner
(1977; 1979) warns that macro-
systems should be conceived
and examined not only on
structural terms, but should
be seen also as carriers of
information and discourse that,
both explicitly and implicitly,
endow meaning and moti-
vation to particular agencies,
social networks, roles, activities
and their interrelationships.
In this connection, Craig
(1996) valuably notes as an The sphere of the
example that laws providing for the inclusion of handicapped children mesosystem also
in mainstream school classes are likely to affect profoundly the education accommodates linkages
and social development of both disabled and normal children who are and interconnections
between the different facets
students in these classes. Craig (1996) suggested that the success or failure of microsystems. These
of this ‘mainstreaming’ may encourage or discourage other governmental include influences across
efforts to integrate the two groups. home and school, and
across peer and family
The chronosytem groups.
Bronfenbrenner (1979) also counts the dimension of time—the
chronosystem—as a fundamental influence on the direction of psycho- Chronosystem.
social development. The implication of time applies in two ways within Bronfenbrenner’s term for
the effects of time on other
ecological theory. Time is important as it entails the patterning of
developmental systems.
environmental events and transitions over the life-span. It also refers
to the unique sociohistorical placement of the individual. An example
in the first instance here would be the effects of the divorce of parents,
which, while very severe around the first year of the event, appear,
like the effects of the death of a loved one, to decrease as time passes
(Santrock, 1999).
In the second instance of ecological application, that of sociohistorical
contextualisation, time constitutes a very broad level of ecological influence.
It includes factors as diverse as changes in family size, place of residence,
employment, dominant sociopolitical values (such as the current importance
of democracy as a political system in most ‘first world’ Western countries,
as opposed to 300 or 400 years ago), and larger scale cultural changes
such as those caused by wars or economic cycles. One important example
of a chronosystem change that particularly influences the development
of women is an increasing culture of women’s rights, which means that
women today are less likely to be discouraged from pursuing careers than
they would have been thirty years ago (Santrock, 1999).
such an important place in his theory. In many ways, it is largely through Ecological transition.
such ecological transitions that one can trace the key life events and life An ecological transition
changes of an individual. occurs whenever a person’s
position in the ecological
An ecological transition occurs whenever a person’s position in the
environment is altered as a
ecological environment is altered as a result of a change in either role result of a change in either
or setting, or in both concurrently (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). An ecologi- role or setting, or in both
cal transition can occur throughout the life-span and can occur at any of concurrently. An ecological
the four levels of the ecological environment. Examples of an ecological transition can occur
transition include the arrival of a baby sibling, the beginning of school, the throughout the life-span
establishment of a secure relationship with a significant other, graduating and can occur at any
of the levels of the
from school or an institution of higher learning, getting or losing a job,
ecological environment.
marrying, moving, or even a change in government.
Role changes are nodal points in the developmental history of the in- Ecological environment.
dividual, because they correspond to a change in self-perception and a The ecological environment
change in what behaviours are socially expected from an individual. For is a series of successive
layers, each surrounding
Bronfenbrenner (1979), role changes have an almost magical quality,
a smaller sphere of
which alters how people are treated, how they act, what they do, and even environmental influences.
what they think and feel. The social influence and presence of others are Bronfenbrenner maintains
also paramount in tracing an individual’s developmental progress. Expo- that the ecological
sure to, and active engagement with important and influential individuals context contains five such
can lead to the adoption of certain behaviours and habits on the part of the environmental systems:
individual. A child is ‘more likely to learn to talk in a setting containing the microsystem, the
mesosystem, the exosystem,
roles that obligate adults to talk to children or that encourage … other
the macrosystem and
people to do so’ (Bronfenbrenner 1979: 7). the chronosystem.
Post-apartheid South Africa has found it On the level of the microsystem, one
difficult to assess the full extent of the damage needs to bear in mind that the lives and
of apartheid’s large-scale and governmentally upbringing of many black children were
institutionalised forms of racism on South fundamentally affected by the absence of
African society. In many ways, of course, the parents. Segregated living arrangements,
impact that this form of government has had, created by the old ‘homelands’ system, and
particularly on the lives of black South Africans, by the pass-law system, meant that parents
remains unquantifiable, and it is precisely this frequently had to work in places far removed
fact that has proved such a ‘sticking point’ from where they lived, which, as in the
for the reparations committee of the recent situation of migrant labour, or of a ‘domestic’
Truth and Reconciliation Commission. While worker living in ‘white South Africa’,
it obviously cannot quantify the damage frequently led to the disintegration of the
of apartheid, Bronfenbrenner’s model of family. Furthermore, the division of privilege
developmental influence can be used to trace with regard to public service amenities meant
a series of possible developmental sequelae that it was far more difficult for black families
stemming from the implementation of the to access services such as public health, and
apartheid system. even when they did, the facilities typically
>>
510 Developmental Psychology
<<
were vastly inferior to those available for time enter a township house, forcibly
white South Africans. remove any children and detain them
At perhaps an even more fundamental in prison for indefinite periods, without
level, the fact of racialised poverty—the giving any indication when they would be
effect of, amongst other things, poor released. The security forces also frequently
‘Bantu’ education and white job- added to the punishment of incarceration
reservation—meant that black families by torturing such children in a wide variety
did not have the financial means to of ways (Duncan & Rock, 1997).
afford childcare, which the majority Education was a particularly important
of white families had. Such economic developmental focus of apartheid
disempowerment made for a pervasive repression, as signalled by the Soweto
influence that affected almost all levels of uprisings in 1976. So-called ‘Bantu’
development, even down to questions education was developed to turn black
of basic nutrition and the availability of children into productive and subservient
food. As Chikane (cited in Duncan & Rock menial labourers who could work for
1997: 139) notes: whites. Security forces were also active in
breaking up social gatherings on even the
Being born into apartheid South Africa smallest scale, even when of a religious
meant, for most black children, the nature. Apartheid also worked on a
deprivation and violence associated strong ideological level, and mass-media
with living in the ghettos created for reports were dramatically slanted towards
those not classified as white ... inferior supporting the National Party government,
education ... discriminatory social whereas dissenting voices were quickly
security ... parents [exploited by] local silenced. The educational system for
enterprises [or as] ... migrant labourers whites, in the form of ‘Christian National
... communities constantly destabilized Education’, was ideologically slanted
as a result of forced removals. towards and embedded in strongly
conservative, nationalistic and racist values.
Duncan and Rock (1997) call attention Clearly then, the effects of the
to the fact that, during the apartheid apartheid system were felt in virtually every
state’s various ‘states of emergency’, the conceivable aspect of Bronfenbrenner’s
security forces were given far-ranging model of developmental influence, from
warrants and prerogatives, which meant the basic level of the health of the child,
that they were able to disregard all legisla- through to the levels of the family, the
tion promulgated to protect children. neighbourhood, the church, the school,
No one was exempt from repressive the community, basic public amenities and
measures such as the military occupation resources, recreational and health facilities,
of black residential areas, house arrest and the mass media, commerce, industry,
indiscriminate attacks on black citizens. religion and fundamental social values,
Members of the police force could at any beliefs, discourse and ideology.
Specific tasks
➊ How do you see the ecological model in terms of your own childhood? Draw a
developmental ‘map’ based on Bronfenbrenner’s diagram of the various spheres of
psychosocial influence. Identify at least six salient examples from each of the five
spheres of ecological influence that were foundational to your own development.
In doing so give special thought to who you are, to how you understand yourself,
and to your own sense of identity. Use little icons and diagrams as part of your
developmental ‘map’.
General tasks
➊ The American sociologist Michael Kimmel recounts a revealing anecdote about one
of his first graduate classes in Women’s Studies in which he heard a dispute between
a white and a black woman. The white woman was arguing that the universal
oppression of women by men bound white and black women together in a common
plight. The black woman disagreed and asked, ‘When you wake up in the morning
and look in the mirror, what do you see?’ ‘I see a woman,’ the white woman replied.
‘That’s precisely the problem,’ said the black woman. ‘When I wake up in the
morning and look in the mirror, I see a black woman. My race is visible to me every
day because I am not privileged in this culture. Because you are privileged, your race
is invisible to you’. Kimmel was struck by this exchange because he realised that
when he looked in the mirror he saw neither his whiteness nor his masculinity. All
he saw was a simple human being (Kimmel, in Wetherell & Griffin 1991: 365).
1.1 When you look in the mirror what do you see? How do you account for
the‘invisibility’ of Kimmel’s masculinity and whiteness in the above example,
and what can this tell us about the influence of sociopolitical factors on the
development of our senses of self and identity?
1.2 At what points of the ecological model of development would you place such
forms of influence? And how do you imagine this would impact on childhood
development?
➋ Can Bronfenbrenner’s theory be linked to Erikson’s theory in any way, and if so, in
what ways would these theories usefully complement one another?
Recommended readings
Bronfenbrenner, U (1977). ‘Towards an experimental ecology of human development’,
American Psychologist, 32(7), 513-531.
(The original paper that summarised Bronfenbrenner’s new metatheoretical approach to
developmental psychology.)
25
Violent crime and
human development
in South Africa
Garth Stevens
Introduction
Violent crime is undoubtedly one of the major psychosocial problems
that currently preoccupies South Africans. As an entrenched mode
of interpersonal and social relating, violence has shown a remarkable
resilience and recalcitrance over the entire span of modern South African
history. Not only is this evident from the enduringly high rates of fatal
and non-fatal violence within the specific confines of South African
society today (Matzopoulos, 2004, 2005; SAPS, 2004, 2005, 2006, 2008), but
the obduracy of violent patterns of social relating in the South African
context is also reflected in pre-colonial histories of indigenous conflicts
(Marks & Atmore, 1980), the pernicious impacts of colonial oppression
and dispossession (Milton, 1983; Vail, 1989), the violent perversions of
social engineering associated with apartheid segregation and repression
(Duncan & Rock, 1994), resistance politics and confrontational liberatory
struggles (SATRC, 1998), and presently in the high rates of criminalised
interpersonal violence (Suffla, van Niekerk & Duncan, 2004).
Vi o l e n t c r i m e a n d h u m a n d e v e l o p m e n t i n S o u t h A f r i c a 515
Here it becomes clear that many acts of violence (although not all) may
be deemed criminal, insofar as they infringe upon the rights and liberties
of others. From the above description, the intersection between violence
and crime is primarily at the point at which the violence is expressed as an
interpersonal or group relation, through which the prevailing norms of a
society are flouted and the liberties and rights of others are compromised.
In South Africa, violent crime is probably most common in forms
of interpersonal violence that involve threats, injury or death to others,
either as a primary outcome of a confrontation (in the case of homicide, for
example), or as a secondary outcome of such a confrontational encounter
(as with threat, injury or death as a consequence of a primary crime such
as hijacking). Within the criminal justice system in South Africa, violent
crimes are also often referred to as contact crimes and include categories
of acts such as murder, rape, attempted murder, assault with intent to do
grievous bodily harm, common assault, indecent assault, robbery with
aggravating circumstances and common robbery (SAPS, 2008).
2002/ 2003/ 2004/ 2005/ 2006/ 2007/ Increase/ 2002/ 2003/ 2004/ 2005/ 2006/ 2007/ Increase/
Crime category
2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 decrease 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 decrease
06/07 vs 06/07 vs
07/08 07/08
Contact crimes
Murder
47.4 42.7 40.3 39.5 40.5 38.6 -4.7% 21 533 19 824 18 793 18 528 19 202 18 487 -3.7%
Attempted murder
78.9 64.8 52.6 43.9 42.5 39.3 -7.5% 35 861 30 076 24 516 20 571 20 142 18 975 -6.7%
Common assault
621.6 605.7 575.0 485.3 443.2 413.9 -6.6% 282 526 280 942 267 857 227 553 210 057 198 049 -5.7%
Common robbery
223.4 206.0 195.0 159.4 150.1 135.8 -9.5% 101 537 95 551 90 825 74 723 71 156 64 985 -8.7%
Vi o l e n t c r i m e a n d h u m a n d e v e l o p m e n t i n S o u t h A f r i c a
517
elements within it (Daly & Wilson, 1988; Nell, 2002; Workman & Reader,
2004). Here in particular, this school of thought has argued that increases
in risk-taking behaviours (including violence) may become more evident
in adolescence, as males in particular prepare for adulthood and greater
competition within the gene pool. More recent research has supported this
argument by highlighting gendered differences that are correlated with
differences in behaviours and disease patterns between men and women.
Sexual dimorphism. These findings imply that these differences or sexual dimorphisms may
Refers to the systematic be present at a neurological level within the structures of the brain itself
differences in physical (Dennis 2004; Goldstein, Seidman, Horton, Makris, Kennedy, Caviness,
form and structure
Faraone & Tsuang, 2001), which may have some impact on behavioural
between individuals of
different sexes within
repertoires that either inhibit or facilitate aggression.
the same species. While some evidence for a genetic hypothesis can certainly be found
in the literature, it has often been critiqued for not accounting fully for
how genetic anomalies and transmission become translated into violent
or criminal behaviours. The absence of the mechanism of translation
has therefore undermined the degree to which these studies have been
accepted within the social sciences. In addition, the degree of similarity
between the neurobiology of men and women far outweighs their sexual
dimorphism, suggesting that we are much more similar than dissimilar
across the sexes. However, most geneticists today would acknowledge
the interactional effects of genetics and environmental factors. They
would also acknowledge the argument that genetic anomalies may also be
associated with a range of additional adaptational problems that may also
predispose individuals to aggressive or violent behaviour.
Prenatal factors
A second strand of inquiry has focused on the relationship between
violence and prenatal factors that may impact on congenital physiological
differences. Kandel & Mednick (1991) showed that 80 per cent of youths
arrested for violent crimes had higher rates of delivery complications at
birth, pointing to some congenital effects. Farrington (1997) also found
that youth with a lower resting heart rate showed greater propensities
for risk-taking behaviours that may predispose them to violence and
aggression. In addition, research has also shown that prenatal exposure to
certain hormones such as androgens (due to maternal stress) can sensitise
the foetal brain and contribute to hypervigilance and aggression in later
life (Floody & Pfaff, 1974; Rutter, 1970). Testosterone increases at this stage,
as a result of rapid responses to environmental stimuli from mothers, may
also be associated with postnatal aggressiveness in infants (Lewis, 1992).
Temperament
As a key psychological component that is characterised by our states of
arousal and arousal responses to situational circumstances, and that is also
Vi o l e n t c r i m e a n d h u m a n d e v e l o p m e n t i n S o u t h A f r i c a 521
Intelligence
Low intelligence scores have also consistently been found to be a correlate
and predictor of violence in later life, dating back from studies in the
early 1900s to the present day (Bartollas & Dinitz, 1989; Lipsey & Derzon,
1998; West & Farrington, 1973). However, while the propensity to enact
522 Developmental Psychology
Moral development
Running parallel to cognitive development, and associated with it,
research on moral development in children (Kohlberg, 1981; Piaget,
1932) has also been a focus as a potential correlate of violence. However,
while most contemporary writers acknowledge that moral reasoning
is an important internal psychological process (Garbarino, 1995), it is
also relative to context and therefore not simply a direct determinant
of violence in later life. Illustrative studies within South Africa suggest
that adverse circumstantial influences on moral reasoning do not always
translate into more diffuse forms of truncated moral reasoning. Dawes
(1994b) and Straker (1992) note that exposure to political violence in
South Africa during the apartheid period did not in and of itself reduce
moral reasoning among children and result in greater proclivities towards
violence, and that in instances where such acts did occur, they were not
generalisable to contexts outside of the political terrain.
Psychopathology
With regard to psychopathology, children who have been diagnosed
with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders or Oppositional Defiant
Disorder tend to be at greater risk for asocial behaviours and also for
developing Conduct Disorder if their initial symptoms persist into
adolescence. Both of these disorders include an increased potential for
impulsive and violent ‘acting out’. Conduct Disorders in childhood and
adolescence are also relatively good predictors for later antisocial behaviour
and possible psychopathy, especially if there are comorbid diagnoses such as
substance abuse, alongside poor social resources (Sadock & Sadock, 2007).
Cartwright (2001:14), in his review of research on psychiatric illnesses and
their relationships to rage-type homicides (see for example, Blackburn,
1993; Hollin, 1989), notes that while psychotic disorders such as Paranoid
Schizophrenia are sometimes implicated in violence, these form the
minority of instances. In addition, he notes that studies on depression and
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder tend to ‘endow an individual with a greater
propensity for explosive violence’.
Personality
In terms of research into personality types and disorders Glueck & Glueck
(1968) found that delinquents were more assertive, unafraid, aggressive and
unconventional in their attitudes than non-delinquents, who were more
self-controlled, perceptive and responsive to social cues. Conger & Miller
(1966) also found that delinquents were rated, on average, more highly
as emotionally unstable, suspicious, hostile and unhappy than their non-
delinquent counterparts. Earlier studies using the Minnesota Multiphasic
Vi o l e n t c r i m e a n d h u m a n d e v e l o p m e n t i n S o u t h A f r i c a 523
Personality Inventory (MMPI) found that violent criminals deviated from the
general population on traits of psychopathy, schizophrenia and hypomania
(Hathaway & McKinley, 1951). Many of these studies have provided the basis
for the consistent associations of personality and certain personality disorders
(antisocial personality disorder, for example) with violent acts that include
criminal homicide (APA, 2000; Sadock & Sadock, 2007). Furthermore, while
studies on authoritarianism as a central feature of personality have freq-
uently been directed towards understanding rigid ideological belief systems
(see for example, Perrin, 2005), they have also suggested a correlation to a
propensity for violent enactments (Funke, 2005).
Peer relations
PHO TO : DAV I D LU RI E
Social capital
Mercy et al (2002) note that there is good evidence that community
integration affects the extent and magnitude of violence within
communities. Citing the body of literature on the relationship between
social capital and violence, they argue that lower levels of social capital
are more frequently than not associated with higher levels of youth
Vi o l e n t c r i m e a n d h u m a n d e v e l o p m e n t i n S o u t h A f r i c a 525
Eugene de Kock, also known as ‘Prime Evil’, set of circumstances within his familial
was a commanding officer in the apartheid context. Gobodo-Madikizela (2003) goes
death squads and had his application for on to articulate how this could have given
amnesty denied during the TRC hearings. rise to the psychodynamics of splitting,
identification with the aggressor, as well as
Psychologically, how do we understand a pattern of defensive violence. She argues
de Kock’s violence? that these underlying psychodynamics
Gobodo-Madikizela’s (2003) psycho- found a specific foothold in the social
history of Eugene de Kock attempts context of the apartheid security forces
to unravel how his early childhood in South Africa, where defence against
experiences shaped his psychological the external threat of blackness and
and behavioural responses within the Communism mirrored his defensive
particular social context of apartheid South aggressive impulses at a psychological
Africa. In her interviews with de Kock, level (that were rooted in hostile threats
he disclosed how his early experiences from significant others experienced within
within his family were characterised by an his early childhood years).
authoritarian father who drank too much,
a mother who appeared submissive and Are there any other potential antecedents
emotionally abused, humiliation because to de Kock’s violent behaviour that you
of his stuttering, and a generally violent could identify?
526 Developmental Psychology
Exposure to violence
Other community-level studies have focused on the normativity of exposure
to violence and its relationship to violent enactments. In their community-
based study in Johannesburg, Swart, Seedat, Ricardo & Johnson (1999),
found that high levels of violence within romantic relationships among
the youth coincided with significant exposure to violence within the
community context. A quarter of boys and 32 per cent of girls reported
that they had been physically hurt by an adult family member.
Once again, violence as a normative method of social relating and conflict
resolution within communities may have a significant effect on legitimising
further violence. Here too, social learning theory provides a sound basis for
understanding the transmission of such behaviours after exposure to it. In re-
viewing the extant literature, Mercy et al (2002) also note the effects of exposure
to media violence on immediate aggressive behaviour, but are cautious about
making the linkages to violence in the long-term. The premise of such studies
rests on the idea that various social institutions such as the media may play a
significant role in reproducing and maintaining such normative belief systems
that are embedded within the community fabric of a social formation.
Social transitions
Within the context of socio-structural factors, countries such as South Africa
that are undergoing social transitions to post-conflict and post-authoritarian
nation state formations (Manganyi, 2004) have also been shown to have higher
rates of violence. Shaw (1998) noted that in such contexts, new social structures
require a certain amount of time to develop and to create the necessary levels
of civil obedience in the direction away from violent crime. Kim & Pridemore
(2005) and Pridemore (2006) found that negative socio-economic change in
transitional Russia was associated with higher homicide rates, and this has
been supported by other writers in contexts such as transitional Serbia as well
(Simeunović-Patić, 2003). Co-occurring with such transitions is of course the
process of globalisation, which also influences crime rates in certain regions
due to the penetrating proliferation of associated criminal activities in the
illegal arms industry, the trafficking of drugs, and the trafficking of humans
(Findlay, 1999). Under such circumstances, not only is violence normalised as
a means of social relating, but often becomes reinforced as a central element of
identity that needs to be attained and maintained.
<<
So how does research understand the to studying ‘race’ and homicide. They
linkages between ‘race’ and violent crime? suggest that certain spatial patterns
in homicide rates in black communities in
Mainstream research has focused on particular, can be related to overcrowding,
‘race’ as a proxy measure, most notably insufficient municipal services, and
for economic, wealth, social and political the disruptions of social networks that
disparities. While the extant literature in all exacerbate violent behaviours and
this area is by no means conclusive and result in urban desertification and decay,
entirely consistent (see Ousey, 1999), which further compound the problem
disaggregation studies on ‘race’ and of violence within black communities.
violent crime (see for example Krivo & Parker & McCall (1999) noted that racial
Peterson, 2000) highlight the importance differences in crime rates could also be
of ‘race’ as a proxy for concentrated accounted for by economic deprivation
disadvantage and residential instability, and limited local opportunity structures
especially among black populations. facing blacks in particular. This is
McNulty & Holloway (2000) also noted supported by Ousey’s (1999) contention
that ‘race’ is often also reflective of that deprivation and poverty were the
proximity to public housing (which mainly most significant factors in determining
houses the poor) and that increased black violent crime rates.
proximity to this form of institutional
housing increases the crime rates for What are your ideas about the links
black populations in particular. Wallace between ‘race’ and violent crime in
(1990) and Morenoff & Sampson (1997) South Africa?
also suggest a geographical component
Economic factors
While a significant number of socio-structural studies have also shown
a correlation between economic decline, recessions, downward pressures
on real wages, a lack of economic opportunities and the increase in
crime rates, this relationship is not necessarily as definitive as believed
(Messner, 1982). Schneidman (1996) points out that in periods of
economic crisis, basic social infrastructure is often compromised,
while the WHO (1995) notes that under these conditions, poverty
often becomes concentrated in urban areas. Both of these findings can
partly account for the linkages between poverty and crime. Similarly,
Fajnzylber, Lederman & Loayza (1999) noted a decline in homicide
rates with an increase in the Gross Domestic Product (GDP) across
several countries. In addition, under circumstances of economic crisis,
a decline in access to low-skill, entry-level employment was also
correlated with increased propensities towards violence (Shihadeh &
Ousey, 1998), while the decline in industrialisation within certain sectors
has opened up the possibilities for greater levels of unemployment,
female-headed households and increased rates of juvenile homicide
in many urban cities in the United States of America (Ousey, 2000).
However, Gartner (1990) found that income inequality (that is, relative
poverty and deprivation) was significantly related to rates of violence
in several industrialised countries.
PHO TO : ZO Ë MO O S MAN 528 Developmental Psychology
This pattern remains fairly consistent today, suggesting that the historical
effects of institutionalised racism may still be a significant factor in high
crime rates, even in contemporary South Africa (Matzopoulos, 2004).
Militarisation
A further feature of the macro-political landscape is the increasing levels
of global geopolitical militarisation that reinforces the nexus between
masculinity, violence, weaponry, war and death. Xaba (2001) examined
constructions of masculinity and notes the integral relationship between
violence and masculinity in both the period of liberatory struggle and
in post-apartheid South Africa. In particular, Xaba (2001) argues that
while violence was considered necessary and even noble in the context of
militarisation during the liberatory struggle, the shifting social conditions
have now created a sense of delegitimisation and criminalisation of this
violence. In the context of post-apartheid South Africa, there has been
an inversion of the meanings attached to violence and therefore to mas-
culinity. This has resulted, to some degree, in men who are unable to
attain the new ideal of manhood, and therefore recast their violent ‘skills
and expertise’ into a form of criminalised masculinity.
Cock (2001) also engages with the history of militarisation in South
Africa as she reflects on the relationships between masculinity, violence
and weaponry, especially firearms (see Cock & Nathan, 1989, for a more
detailed discussion of this). She notes that both the apartheid state and
the liberation movements were heavily invested in generating and main-
taining a militarised masculine construction, especially during periods
of intense armed conflict. Weapons became the symbols of defence,
resistance and freedom for many black and white South African males
in the construction of their identities. Similarly, Swart’s (2001) study on
right-wing Afrikaner masculinity notes the relationship between white
men, firearms, masculinity and violence. As with Xaba’s (2001) study,
she examines the historical construction of the white male as dominant,
defender, leader, and associated with symbols of war to mark the historical
hardships and struggles of survival. However, given the marginalisation
of this right-wing identity in contemporary South Africa, there are shifts
towards even more excessive displays of masculinity in which there is the
continued use of symbols and discourses of firearms and militarism in a
context that is yet to be fully demilitarised.
Gender
Given the disproportionate involvement of men as both victims and per-
petrators of violent crime across the globe, research on gender differences in
relation to violent crime has tended to suggest that female victimisation and
male perpetration is directly related to ideological and structural processes
that support male domination. The general findings indicate that women
are more likely to be victims of crime in circumstances where their absolute
status in a social formation is compromised or where their status relative to
men is compromised (Heise & Garcia-Moreno, 2002; Hird, 2000).
Chodorow (1978) has incorporated this broad feminist approach into
a psychoanalytic understanding of gender and violence. She suggests
530 Developmental Psychology
Age
Age too is not merely a demographic variable but also a social
stratification marker across which significant variations in crime rates
can be found. Unlike the previously examined variables which have all
in some manner been directly associated with other variables linked to
inequality and status, age appears to operate relatively independently
from ‘race’, class and gender. However, upon closer inspection, and
moving beyond the developmental, psychological, physiological or
evolutionary sets of processes that may be responsible for this global
trend, the implicated age groups in their late teens and early adulthood
may be more susceptible to the economic strains of an environment,
given that this not only represents the time of entry into the world
of work, but also the period of greatest anticipated economic and
social productivity. The absence of such opportunities may very well
Vi o l e n t c r i m e a n d h u m a n d e v e l o p m e n t i n S o u t h A f r i c a 531
Table 25.2 RSA Contact Crimes Against Children under 18 Years from 2006/2007 to 2007/2008.
Source: SAPS (2008).
While some authors (Macleod, 2002) have argued that a focus on the
negative developmental effects of adverse social contexts pathologises
adolescents unnecessarily, these arguments are perhaps equally problematic
when they overstate adolescents’ abilities to manage such circumstances in
a healthy manner. In addition, they run the risk of negating the very real
material circumstances and impacts that hostile social environments have
on human developmental processes.
Resilience
Nevertheless, it is important to note that several researchers have consistently
argued that adolescents should not only be characterised as victims in such
contexts, and that we should acknowledge that they also display resilience Resilience. Refers to
and fortitude. Even though they may therefore be predisposed to a host the capacity of people to
of negative effects, this is not necessarily a logical outcome for all of them cope with stressors in
a psychologically
(Dawes, 1994a; Levett, 1988; Swartz & Levett, 1989).
healthy manner.
While the negative repercussions of these contexts for adolescent children
should not be underestimated nor negated, Dawes (1994a) notes that there
may be several mediating factors that determine the extent of children’s
subjective experiences of social stressors. Here, several authors have
suggested a number of factors that may reduce these impacts and even help
adolescent children to redefine their negative experiences more positively.
These include the degree of available social support, family integration and
stability, the availability of long-term emotional relationships with others,
the ability of adolescents to elicit support, and the meanings that adolescents
ascribe to their experiences, amongst others (Dawes, 1994a; Richter, 1994;
Straker, 1992). Under these mediating conditions many adolescents may
therefore still be able to acquire a sense of integrated identity, even under Identity integration.
the most difficult social and environmental circumstances. Refers to a presence
What needs to be recognised is that the relationship between intra- of commitment to an
identity, after the presence
psychic factors and social conditions in the formation of identity during
of a struggle for identity,
adolescence is more complex and nuanced than had been conceptualised by and implies that a person
most theorists. It is evident that exposure to violent crime may have severe has arrived at a sense of
negative consequences for identity integration and attainment. However, who he is following a
it is also clear that under certain mediating conditions, the attainment of search of what he can be.
an integrated sense of identity in adolescence remains possible for many
young people in South Africa today.
Conclusion
This chapter has examined the relationship between human development
and violent crime by focusing on both the potential antecedents and
consequences of violent crime. What is apparent is that the interrelation-
ships between intrapsychic processes and sociohistorical conditions are
extremely complex and sophisticated, and that both are integral to human
development. This implies that a holistic and comprehensive approach
to understanding the psychological drivers of violent crime must be
undertaken, if we are to actively promote mental health and adjustment.
Furthermore, an equivalent level of comprehensiveness and integration is
critical if we are to develop successful intervention strategies to reduce and
control the marring psychosocial effects of violent crime.
538 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
➊ Given the extent of research on the antecedents of violent crime, critically discuss the
degree of agency that we as humans have in determining such behaviours (that is, are our
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➌ Much of the research into the relationship between family functioning, family structure
and delinquency in later life appears to be premised on assumptions of the family being
nuclear in orientation. How would you apply these research findings to the South
African context, where a range of family structures and characterisations can be found?
➎ Based on available research on the antecedents of violent crime, what would the key elements
for a comprehensive intervention to combat violent crime entail in the South African context?
Recommended readings
Erikson, E H (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
(This book represents Erikson’s seminal work on the integral relationship between socio-cultural
contexts and psychological development, across many different social formations and social groups.)
Erikson, E H (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
(A classic collection of Erikson’s major essays on the topic of adolescent identity development and
crisis, and the role of cultural influences in shaping new forms of adolescent identity.)
Krug, E, Dahlberg, L, Mercy, J A, Zwi, A B & Lozano, R (2002). World report on violence and health
Geneva: WHO.
(The most comprehensive international report to date on the extent, nature, magnitude, determinants,
consequences and potential intervention strategies to understand and address various forms of violence.)
Marcia, J E (1980). Identity in adolescence, (In Adelson, J. (ed), Handbook of adolescent psychology.
New York: Wiley, 159-187)
(An expansion and elaboration on Erikson’s theory of adolescent identity development, with a focus on the
manner in which the degree of commitment to an identity can determine identity status.)
Stevens, G & Lockhat, R (1997). ‘“Coca-Cola kids” - Reflections on the development of black adolescent
identity in post-apartheid South Africa,’ South African Journal of Psychology, 27(4), 250-255.
(A journal article reflecting on the possible trajectories of black adolescent identity development after the
demise of apartheid in South Africa.)
Suffla, S, van Niekerk, A & Duncan, N (eds), (2004). Crime, violence and injury prevention in South
Africa: Developments and challenges. Tygerberg: MRC-UNISA.
(A collection of papers focusing on the extent, nature, magnitude, determinants, consequences and potential
intervention strategies to understand and address various forms of violence within contemporary South Africa.)
CHAPTER
26
The effects of trauma
on child development:
Children in South Africa
Patrick Connolly and Gillian Eagle
Aftermath of apartheid. conflict and social unrest under apartheid to what it is today, just over a
The aftermath of the decade after the introduction of democracy: a country with considerable
apartheid era is a promise but facing difficult social and economic problems.
generation of children
Many of the problems experienced by contemporary South African
growing up in conditions
of extreme poverty,
children are considered to be a longer-term consequence of the apartheid
despair and regular system, a system entailing almost half a century of systematic racial inequal-
exposure to violence, ity, enforced by a repressive government. Under Apartheid black children
within a context of in particular experienced deprivation and hardships of various kinds that
embattled family life affected psychological development. The following table shows research
and disintegrating findings cited in the Goldstone Commission of Inquiry into the Effects of
communities.
Political Violence on Children and illustrates the difficulties, anxieties and
signs of distress exhibited by a group of children and adolescents as a result
of apartheid-era policies and practices (Duncan & Rock, 1997b).
This research, conducted over a decade ago, reflects the reality of most
black children’s lives under apartheid. State repression in the form of brutal
acts of public violence, detentions, murders, torture and intimidation
affected many children, either directly or indirectly, through their impact
on family members, friends or community. Secondly, so-called ‘political’
violence in the form of counter-violence towards state agencies, as well
as violence between resistance organisations, self-defence units (SDUs)
and vigilante organisations, as well as spontaneous acts of public violence,
were commonplace (Duncan & Rock, 1997a; Emmett, 2003).
Today, over a decade after the 1994 transition to a democratically
elected government, while violence is far less often purely politically
motivated, public acts of violence in the form of violent crime, gang
violence and vigilantism, remain a prominent part of South African life
(Emmett, 2003), affecting different communities in different ways. For
example, Lewis suggests that in some townships exposure to violence is
part of everyday life:
Violence has been a part of township life for decades. For many of
the children who grow up in townships, violence is continuous…
The effects of trauma on child development: Children in South Africa 541
to pass by the body of someone who has been shot on the street is
not unusual … death becomes normal: ‘life becomes cheap. That’s
why they kill someone for earrings’ (Lewis, 1999: 89).
Although they may manifest differently, all these sets of symptoms can
be present in both traumatised adults and children. Some may experience
relatively few of these anxiety-related ‘symptoms’ following such an
event. However, it is normal for some of these reactions to occur for up to
a month following exposure. If sufficient symptoms persist for longer than a
month, a diagnosis of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) can be given,
indicating that some form of assistance is required to process the event and
cope with the symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).
noting that one overarching principle that holds true at just about all ages
and stages of development is that traumatic responses during childhood
typically involve some form of regression, where previously attained
developmental achievements (such as toilet training) and independence
are lost. The child or adolescent may behave as a younger child, showing
more immature emotional regulation and social behaviour than is age
appropriate. For example, two children in their early teenage years
became fearful to sleep in their own beds after an armed robbery in their
home and insisted on sleeping in their parents’ bed as one might expect of
much younger children. As a consequence of losing developmental gains,
later development can be delayed as previous milestones and abilities have
to be reacquired (Eagle & Michelson, 1997; Maddaleno et al, 2006). What
follows is a brief overview of how trauma may be both perceived and
manifest within children at four broad stages of development: birth to
two years (infancy), three to six years (early childhood), seven to 12 years
(middle childhood) and 13 to 18 years (adolescence).
after activity (on the part of the child or others) has appeared fruitless in a
traumatic situation that they have witnessed (Pynoos & Eth, 1985). These
fantasies often involve some person (either the child, or another, such as
a policeman) intervening to prevent the trauma from taking place. Such
fantasies of ‘undoing’ or altering an outcome are common at this age, and
represent an attempt to limit traumatic anxiety and feelings of impotence
(Pynoos & Eth, 1985).
Evan … was present during a robbery and saw his father being
shot. In therapy sessions afterwards, he drew the scene of the killing
but drew himself as the ‘superhero’ with a cape and a mask. In the
story, he saved his father from the ‘bandits’ (Lewis, 1999: 29).
In one such case … a four year old girl whose mother was knifed
to death carefully painted her hands red and acted out a game of
being stabbed with a paint brush (Pynoos & Eth, 1985: 29).
Similar to infants and toddlers, children of this age range may also
demonstrate jumpiness and be easily startled, or may become withdrawn
and quiet (Lewis, 1999). Terr (1985) suggests that there is not much
evidence of emotional numbing in children in this age group, but there
is often a constricted range of play and of affect. They may also have a
limited sense of future (Kerig et al, 2000; Yule, Perrin & Smith, 1999).
Lastly, there may be difficulties in concentration and memory—although
traumatised children are often very alert and attentive to signs or news of
danger. Specific phobias related to an object or situation associated to the
traumatic event occur from this developmental stage onwards and can
persist for years afterwards (Green, 1985; Pynoos & Eth, 1985; Yule, Perrin
& Smith, 1999). For example, the little boy injured in the taxi accident
became phobic about getting into any form of motorised transport.
Regression at this stage may take the form of increased ‘clingy’
behaviour (increased separation anxiety) and reverting to bed-wetting,
thumb-sucking or asking for a bottle (Eagle & Michelson, 1997; Lewis,
1999). Children may become irritable and demonstrate new fears (such as
548 Developmental Psychology
Adolescence
Adolescents tend to have responses to traumatic events that resemble
those of adults, while also retaining many of the facets of traumatic
response demonstrated by younger children. Many adolescents have re-
experiencing and hyperarousal symptoms similar to those of adults. They
may become socially withdrawn, or alternatively, may start socialising far
more than before. Sleeping and eating patterns may be disturbed, and
they may experience difficulty concentrating and poor memory. School
functioning often deteriorates (as with primary-school children), and
irritability and aggressiveness may cause disruption at school. Skipping
classes and running away from home, if only for brief periods, is not
uncommon. Adolescents may have revenge or ‘rescue’ fantasies and may
become fearful of another traumatic event occurring (Kerig et al, 2000;
Lewis, 1999; Maddaleno et al, 2006). All of these responses, should they
persist, can potentially affect future academic and social development,
even in response to a single event.
One important element of adolescent development is the development
of independence. There is a movement to separate from parents and
towards individuation, as well as to integration with the peer group
and the development of a social identity (Erikson, 1950). On the one
hand, there is considerable self-monitoring (or self-consciousness) with
regard to how one is viewed by peers; on the other, there is a need to
reduce dependence on parents and caregivers. As such, the vulnerability
and/or emotional fragility experienced in response to a traumatic
event, together with the increased sense of emotional attachment to, or
dependence on, parents is often experienced as humiliating or shameful
by the adolescent (Eagle & Michelson, 1997; Lewis, 1999; Maddaleno et
al, 2006). Traumas at this stage can have a detrimental effect on further
identity development. Firstly, as a protective measure, adolescents may
foreclose on their identity in the sense of becoming rigid and inflexible Externalising response.
in their identity formation. Secondly, they may lose any real direction or An externalising response
to trauma is one in which
certainty in the identity directions they have explored or taken, resulting
the person ‘acts out’ or
in identity diffusion—a lack of development of any adaptive identity directs his or her distress
formation (Duncan & Rock, 1997c; Eagle & Michelson, 1997; Maddaleno in action (rather than
et al, 2006). an inner experience of
A key issue distinguishes adolescents in terms of their response to distressing thoughts or
traumatic events and also helps make sense of the responses that particular emotions) towards people
younger children or even adults have to emotionally traumatic circum- or objects in the outside
world. This may include
stances. This is the difference between externalising versus internalising
activities such as abusing
responses to trauma (Pepler, Catallo & Moore, 2000). substances, fighting,
An externalising response refers to a ‘turning outward’ of behaviour. driving recklessly or
It has also been referred to as ‘acting out’. This process can be described regular risk-taking.
550 Developmental Psychology
Intellectual factors
A tradition of research has demonstrated a strong association between
intellectual functioning and the ability of a child to successfully overcome
trauma. The manner in which a child’s intellect mediates the effects of a
traumatic event is not entirely clear but it seems most likely that the child’s
ability to cognitively process the overwhelming stimuli related to the event
is implicated in recovery (Peterson, Prout & Schwarz, 1991; Yule, Perrin
& Smith, 1999). As indicated previously, nutrition and adequate home
stimulation are vital to intellectual development. However, educational
opportunities also play a crucial role and the past iniquities of ‘Bantu
education’ continue to hamper the provision of adequate educational
training to all South African children.
Despite the ANC government’s consistent stated commitment to
improving education, schools in poorer communities and townships
continue to offer sub-standard teaching and learning experiences. A lack
of teaching materials, overcrowding, under-qualified and underpaid
teachers, limitations linked to mediums of instruction (English is the
primary language for teaching and yet for the majority of South African
children it is not their first language), and a harsh school environment
continue to limit the effectiveness of teaching in such schools (Emmett,
2003). Further, due to general poverty in the community, many township
children are also under-stimulated outside school, often complaining
that their environments are ‘boring’ and lack recreational outlets. Such
school and community conditions may result in a relative lack of cognitive
development and poor scholastic achievement (Duncan & Rock, 1997b;
Emmett, 2003), depriving children of the intellectual capabilities that may
help them to overcome adversity such as trauma.
Social support
A considerable volume of research has confirmed that one of the strongest
protective factors against trauma impact on children is the presence of
strong social support, in the form of significant caring human relation-
The effects of trauma on child development: Children in South Africa 553
The fact that these black townships or ghettos were usually severely
overcrowded, under-serviced, dreary, poverty-stricken and crime-
ridden created unbearable tensions in family life, often leading to
high levels of anger and aggression, which in turn, frequently led
to the violent abuse of children (Duncan & Rock, 1997c: 74).
The greater and more frequent the personal impact on the child,
the greater the likelihood a traumatic state will occur, and given
enough of these states, the child responds the same way repeatedly
and thus may acquire a trait (Mohr & Fantuzzo, 2000: 75)
and every single event (Green, 1985). Perry et al (1995) suggest that child-
ren under continuous severe stress respond in one of two ways—either
with the kind of hyper-arousal response described above, or, alternatively
with what is called a ‘dissociative’ response (a common defence):
Domestic violence
One might imagine that children’s experience of trauma is more severe
when they are in the position of victim rather than witness. In keeping
with this idea it has been found that children who are direct victims
of violence are more likely to experience dissociative symptoms and to
demonstrate traumatic amnesia or disavowal (inability to remember or
The effects of trauma on child development: Children in South Africa 559
PHO TO : ZO Ë MO O S MANN
accept that the event took place) than those whose involvement
in trauma is at the level of witnessing disturbing events (Pynoos &
Eth, 1985). However, there is strong evidence that children who
witness acts of domestic violence in many instances demonstrate
serious trauma symptoms. Witnessing violence between one’s
parents or other relatives in the home brings with it unique
stressors that can have far-reaching consequences for later
development (Maddaleno et al, 2006; Pepler, Catallo & Moore,
2000; Pynoos & Eth, 1985).
Witnessing domestic violence (in South Africa, this most
often means witnessing a mother being assaulted by a male
partner, often the child’s father) brings with it a particular
experience of passivity and helplessness in the face of the injury,
humiliation and destruction of someone one is attached to
(Leibowitz et al, 1999; Pynoos & Eth, 1985). The child’s limited
ability to prevent the violence may bring with it a distorted sense
of responsibility, as well as a strong need to try to undo the event,
for example through fantasies of ‘inner plans of action’ described
earlier (Pynoos & Eth, 1985). Another fairly common outcome A fairly common outcome of
of witnessing domestic violence during childhood is a later tendency children witnessing domestic
towards suspicion, paranoia and mistrust of people’s intentions (Lewis, violence is a tendency
1999). Occupying the vantage point of observer in witnessing domestic towards suspicion, paranoia
and mistrust later on in life.
violence also means that the child is, in a sense, free to identify with
multiple positions, those of victim, abuser, rescuer, bystander and/or
rescuer, for example. The propensity to identify with the aggressor is
even greater in such situations than it is for direct victims of familial
violence (Dutton, 2000; Pynoos & Eth, 1985). It is not uncommon for
boys to join their fathers in abusing their mothers as they move into
adolescence, for example, indicating their identification and taking on
of both dominating and gendered roles in the family. It has also been
observed that in situations of domestic abuse children may be forced to
demonstrate their loyalty to a particular parent, sometimes prematurely
and inappropriately, creating difficulties for further emotional and
relational development (Dutton, 2000). For example, a boy child who
identifies with his mother and feels hostility and contempt for his father
(which is also not uncommon) may find it difficult to make a positive
masculine identification when attempting to establish a gendered
identity in adolescence.
It is clear that in addition to the trauma of direct exposure to the
infliction of injury and the sounds, sights and sensations associated with
this, domestic abuse places children in conflicted positions with regard
to their role and responsibilities and with regard to their relationship to
both parties. Witnessing familial violence damages the child’s relation-
ship to both the aggressor and the victim. The child may lose faith in the
authority, competence and strength of the victimised parent, including
in his or her ability to protect them from harm, weakening attachment to
the ‘victim’ parent (Dutton, 2000). Exposure to the extreme hostility and
aggressive actions of the perpetrator also damages the child’s attachment
to this parent, as the latter now appears dangerous to the child (Dutton,
560 Developmental Psychology
2000). Further, the horrifying loss of control in the aggressor may make
children afraid of their capacity to control their own impulses, which
may result in generally inhibited and depressed behaviour in the future
(Green, 1985; Lewis, 1999; Pynoos & Eth, 1985). Alternatively, the loss
of control in the aggressor may be replicated by the child in similar
failures in impulse control in the form of uncharacteristic aggression,
recklessness and self-destructiveness (Dutton, 2000; Pynoos & Eth, 1985).
It is apparent then that growing up in a household in which domestic
abuse occurs places children at risk for a number of different kinds of
psychological disturbance and outcomes.
The fact that sexual abuse very often has long-term effects may be due
to a number of factors. First, the child is often confused and frightened
by intense physical feelings that occur together with the abuse, some of
which may be pleasurable but are felt in conjunction with very unpleasant
feelings about the incident. Further, such confused feelings towards an
adult in a caregiving role, and the movement between being a sexual
object and then child again, creates problems in attachment (Goodwin,
1985). Second, the events are often accompanied by high levels of
secrecy, guilt and shame. Perpetrators may often threaten punishment
The effects of trauma on child development: Children in South Africa 561
Conclusion
Having perhaps presented a somewhat bleak picture of the risks for
traumatisation that South Africa children face, it is important to
reiterate that there are various factors that increase resilience in the face Childhood resilience.
of trauma and serve as protective factors. These include both individual Children’s capacity
and social and community resources. For example, as discussed to recover from
environmental insults or
previously, children’s temperament and intellectual ability may assist
attacks to their wellbeing
them to better process the inevitable traumatic events that most people or development.
will experience over their life-span. Children who have been securely
attached and have caring and helpful parents or caregivers will also
experience less adverse effects in the face of traumatisation. Community
cohesion, a strong culture of human rights and access to information
and assistance are also important supports to those attempting to foster
optimal child development. Although levels of traumatisation remain
unacceptably high in a number of respects, South African policy-makers
and interventionists are also trying to tackle some of these problems
more or less successfully. While the material discussed in this chapter
demonstrates that there is a wealth of research, both international and
local, aimed at helping psychologists to understand and respond to
aspects of childhood trauma in an informed way, the need for ongoing
research, particularly local research, remains pressing.
562 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
➊ Etienne Kabimba, the youngest of a migrant family of five had recently been witness to
his home being burnt to the ground and his father being hacked to death by a group of
South Africans in one of the recent incidents of xenophobic violence that have marred
the social landscape in Johannesburg. Identify both the possible short- and long-term
CRITICAL THINKING TASKS
➌ What are three key differences between Type I and Type II trauma? Consider whether
the distinction between Type I and Type II trauma is useful.
➍ Discuss the possible differential responses (in terms of explanations and sense of
culpability) of children in the following stages of development to the trauma of witnessing
a family member being shocked while fixing some faulty wiring in the home:
4.1 Early childhood
4.2 Middle childhood
4.3 Adolescence
Recommended readings
Duncan, N & Rock, B (1997). Children and violence: Quantifying the damage. (In B Rock (ed.)
Spirals of suffering. Pretoria: HSRC Publishers, 43-68.)
(This text reports on the research conducted on behalf of the Goldstone commission of enquiry into the wellbeing
of South African children, focusing on the previously neglected poor and black majority. This focus gives a useful
overview of the particular traumatic pressures facing children in South Africa and developing countries.)
Eagle, G & Michelson, C (1997). Post-traumatic stress in children: presentation and intervention
guidelines. (In B Rock (ed.) Spirals of suffering. Pretoria: HSRC Publishers, 227-254.)
(A useful reading for counsellors, practitioners and clinicians in terms of highlighting principles of
intervention and a model for brief term intervention, illustrated by way of case material.)
Eth, S & Pynoos, R S (eds.) (1985). Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder in Children. Washington: American
Psychiatric Press.
(This important text contains writings of pioneers in child trauma studies. It is essential reading.)
Pynoos, R & Eth, S (1996). Witness to violence: the Child interview. (In M Horowitz (ed.) Essential
Papers on Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. New York: New York University Press, 360-373.)
(Important reading at a more advanced level for the counsellor, practician or clinician working with
traumatised children, highlighting critical information that needs to be obtained during initial assess-
ment and how this can be worked within a single session. It also offers some engaging case material.)
Terr, L (1996). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. (In M Horowitz (ed.) Essential Papers
on Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. New York: New York University Press, 61-81.)
(An overview of research and theory on childhood from one of the pioneering professionals in child trauma stud-
ies. In this paper the idea of two distinct types of trauma is introduced and is illustrated by vivid case examples.)
Yule, W & Williams, R (1990). Post-Traumatic Stress reactions in children. Journal of Traumatic
Stress, 3(2), 279-295.
CHAPTER
27
Critical issues in
developmental psychology
Derek Hook and Norman Duncan
namely Mark Poster and Nicholas Rose are also briefly examined.
Arguably, Poster’s seminal work, Critical Theory of the Family (1988) and
Rose’s text, Governing the Soul: the Shaping the Private Self (1991) have had
an important impact on psychology and have provided important building
blocks for the development of a more critical developmental psychology.
Some of the key ideas on human development that will be examined in
this chapter include the notions of childhood, the innocent child, the conflict-
laden adolescent, the unchanging family and the consistently benevolent family.
This examination will be followed by an exploration of the effects of various
developmental psychology assumptions and theoretical perspectives.
In medieval society the idea of childhood did not exist; this is not
to suggest that children were neglected, forsaken or despised.
The idea of childhood is not to be confused with affection for
children: it corresponds to an awareness of the particular nature
of childhood ... which distinguishes the child from the adult ... In
medieval society, this awareness was lacking … Language did not
give the word ‘child’ the restricted meaning we give it today …
The infant who was too fragile as yet to take part in the life of
adults simply ‘did not count’ (Ariès, 1973: 125).
(1962;1999) and Poster (1988) point out that because children were not
viewed as significantly different from adults in European society prior to
1600, they were expected, when physically able, to work long hours and
to enjoy many of the same pleasures as adults. For this reason, children
at the time were frequently included in adult conversation, jokes and
entertainment (Craig, 1996; Poster, 1988). This is in sharp contrast to
current practices in which every effort is made to ‘protect’ the child from
the ‘realities’ and ‘excesses’ of the adult world.
Importantly too, in early Europe, children were not thought to share
the strong emotional bonds with their parents that they are assumed to in
contemporary society (Poster, 1988). Furthermore, rather than possessing
more rights and privileges than adults—as it seems is overwhelmingly the
case in the modern world—children, prior to the 17th century, were in
certain respects considered significantly less important than adults. To give
an indication of this sometimes inferior status of the child, Ariès (1962,
reprinted in 1999) notes that if children died before an arbitrary age they
would be buried in graves that were not even marked by a tombstone.
By the 17th century, and more so after the 18th century, childhood had
begun to be thought of as a distinct category of humanity. Moreover (and
perhaps partly inspired by the way in which the Christ-child was frequently
depicted during this period), childhood was now increasingly considered to
be a period of innocence. From this point onwards children were less often
seen as anonymous members of a clan or community and more frequently as
an identifiable sub-group located within families. It was not until the 1800s,
however, that this attitude became popularised and that children began to
be treated as persons with a special status of their own (Poster, 1988).
Hendrik (1990) suggests that the idea of children as dependent and
of childhood as a period of innocence and helplessness emerged as a
largely ideological position. He suggests that it may be politically linked to
initiatives for channelling children into the classrooms of mass education
as a means of controlling social groups capable of political insurrection
and rebellion within a given social order. Constructions of the child as
ignorant and in need of education and socialisation furthered these
objectives by keeping children in schools. Here they could be morally
educated and kept away from social activities that could disrupt the social
order (Hendrik, 1990). The British government’s Education Act of 1870,
which made schooling compulsory for all, could be seen as an example of
this kind of political agenda.
Many of Hendrik’s (1990) views might be considered radical, and
some may be countered by the suggestion that universal education, for
example, may well be in the interests of the public good, and therefore
that current constructions are in society’s best interests. Nevertheless, he
does make some important points. For example, the following much-cited
observation regarding the age of conscription gives some force to Hendrik’s
(1990) contention that the predominant constructions of children are used
primarily to control them and regulate their behaviour. In many western
countries, young men of 16 are considered too young to vote or marry
without permission but are considered old enough to kill or be killed for
their country.
566 Developmental Psychology
Changing families
Just as we can question our assumptions of what it means to be an adoles-
cent or an adult, we can interrogate our assumptions about the family.
Mainstream developmental psychology textbooks, especially those
published prior to the 1980s, frequently dealt with families as though they
were timeless ‘units’, similar across all contexts. Indeed, traditionally the
‘nuclear family’ has been represented as the typical and desired family type,
with all deviations from this type seen as atypical and less than ideal.
Critical issues in developmental psychology 567
PHO TO : CHA J O HNS TO N
However, as Poster (1988) observes, the typical nuclear family is The family is not
not timeless. Indeed, in his analysis of the family as one of the key timeless. The family as an
contexts of human development, he identifies at least four dominant institution is not timeless.
Through the centuries it
family types that have developed in Western Europe since the 16th
has undergone various
century, namely: the peasant family type of the 16th and 17th centuries, profound changes.
the aristocratic family type of the 16th and 17th centuries, the working-
class family type of the early industrial period and the bourgeois family
type of the mid-19th century and later. Furthermore, he points out that
each of these family types had distinct advantages and disadvantages
for human development. Very importantly too, he observes that the
dominant family form represented in 20th century and contemporary
developmental psychology texts, namely the nuclear family, is fre-
quently not as conducive to optimal human development as it is typically
held to be. As he puts it, inasmuch as the modern nuclear family is
frequently held to be the healthiest and safest place for the individual
in a hostile world, it is also responsible for ‘oppressing women, abusing
children, spreading neurosis and preventing community’ (Poster,
1988: ix). This, he argues, is largely due to the interaction between
the specific emotional structure of the modern nuclear family and the
society in which it is located. As he puts it:
The changing shape of families and the Along with the high number of adults
changing responsibilities of children are dying from AIDS, and reduced capacity of
brought into dramatic perspective by communities to support and care for
the effects of the HIV/AIDS epidemic. children, is a changing family structure
According to a recent report published and care-giving patterns where the burden
by AIDS Foundation South Africa (2008), of care falls on those who have the least
the burden of HIV/AIDS on South African capacity to provide parenting, support and
society is enormous: care for the affected children, ie the elderly
and the young. Hence the disturbing
South Africa has the sixth scenarios of grandparent-headed
highest prevalence of HIV in the world, households and adolescent/child/sibling-
with 18.8% of the population estimated to headed households (NMCF, 2001: 5).
be infected.
PHOTO: iAFRIKA PHOTOS
<<
AIDS orphans are at greater risk of * begging for food on a daily basis,
malnutrition, illness, abuse and sexual * resorting to prostitution, at ages as
exploitation than children orphaned by young as eight to 10 years (NMCF,
other causes. They grapple with the 2001: 26).
stigma and discrimination so often
associated with AIDS, which can even Stigmatisation is still a huge problem:
deprive them of basic social services
and education (NMCF, 2001: 15). The community often isolates [AIDS
orphans] and they respond by isolating
In addition to the responsibilities of having themselves from the community; parents
to take care of their siblings, the problems discourage their children from playing
faced by orphan heads of households with kids from child-headed families
include: because they are believed to also have
AIDS; the children who go to school tell
* a shortage of income to fulfil basic us that their friends don’t want to play
needs, including food and clothing, with them as they might infect them
* obstacles to their and their siblings’ (NMCF, 2001: 21).
attendance at school, including inability
to pay school fees and to purchase As one child reported:
school materials and uniforms,
stigmatisation and rejection by My teacher said I will also die of AIDS;
community members, neighbours talk bad about us - saying our
* lack of access to health care, and house is the AIDS house (NMCF, 2001: 22).
* lack of moral support (NMCF, 2001: 15).
AIDS orphans have to deal with the
Such children are highly vulnerable to phys- humiliation of begging for food from
ical and sexual abuse by neighbours and neighbours, and the frustration of being
relatives, and to poor health status due to unsuccessful. This is the report of another
the likelihood of being drawn into prostitu- respondent in the study:
tion and criminal acts as a means of
survival. Many AIDS orphans often appear I feel bad because when there is no food
helpless and unable to think of ways of my younger brothers and sisters look at
fending for themselves, tending instead to me and that worries me; at times I travel
look up to the child head to ‘make a plan’, all the way to my aunt to ask for food
especially in terms of providing food. Some and … find nothing - it frustrates me
survival strategies include: even more to think that I still have to tell
the others that I went to my aunt for
* child heads performing casual or ‘piece’ nothing (NMCF, 2001: 22).
work (washing cars, polishing shoes,
selling vegetables) in order to support Many of these children speak about
their siblings, being ridiculed by teachers and other
* running errands for sympathetic children because they do not have school
neighbours, uniforms or because their parents have
* attending church and then asking the died of AIDS. In fact, according to the
pastor for food, NMCF (2001) Report, many claim that
* in the case of teenage girls, attempting school authorities insisted on them paying
to commit themselves to early school fees, even though their situation
marriages in order to survive, and may be well known to the teachers.
570 Developmental Psychology
We have emphasised how historically relative Chapter 15). For example, children tend to
our present-day notions of childhood are, be more egocentric in their thinking, and
and we have considered the effects of are less able to concentrate on more than
‘constructing’ childhood as a qualitatively one dimension of a problem at a time
different concept from adulthood. It is (Piaget, 1926). Likewise, it would seem that
important also to be aware of the opposite children have less difficulty in believing in
tendency, that of assuming too much of a simi- imaginary worlds, friends and figures, which
larity between adults and children. Whereas corroborates the popular notion that
emphasis on the difference between adults children have more active imaginations than
and children seems the far more dominant adults. Children also appear to make moral
tendency in developmental psychological decisions on a different level to that of most
thinking, it is important also not to interpret adults (see Chapter 19 on Kohlberg and
childhood on the basis of adult experience. moral reasoning).
We should be aware that the way we experi- Piaget’s belief in the qualitative
ence the world now may be radically difference between the cognitive abilities of
different from how we experienced the world adults and children should be
as toddlers. For instance, we should be aware understandable, especially since language
of the tendency to project adult categories of use is such a large part of what we typically
experience and understanding onto the consider to be cognition. Children have
phenomenological world of the child. had a far shorter exposure to spoken
Piaget (1926), for example, held firmly to language and hence are far less competent
the belief that in many ways children were language users. This fact, in Vygotsky’s
qualitatively different to adults. For him the view (1962), impacts dramatically on the
difference was principally in the realm ability of children to have, store and make
of cognition (those psychological facets of active use of memories. Similarly, the more
intelligence, memory, language use, and so competent one is in language use, the
on). In fact he provided powerful evidence more one can enquire into what one does
that children think and reason in not understand—one has a better-
qualitatively different ways from adults (see equipped ‘tool-box’ for making sense of >>
Critical issues in developmental psychology 571
<<
the world. Of course the paradox here is world are probably very blurred
that children, for the most part, are so and incoherent).
dramatically superior to adults in the speed In this way the world of the child, like
at which they acquire a new language. its imagination and its various cognitive
The importance of emphasising these abilities, seems to be quite different from
differences is to suggest that certain very that of the adult. It is important to bear
basic concepts that seem so innate and so this in mind, not so that we can accentuate
intrinsic to us need in fact to be learnt. gratuitously the qualitative difference
These are concepts such as the notion of
Influenced by the work of Foucault, Nikolas Rose (1991) has also written
extensively about the important ways in which individual psychology
contributes to the classification and surveillance of individuals. He argues
that, in order to control and regulate potentially unruly, wayward or
unproductive social elements, ‘disciplinary societies’ (Foucault, 1977)
developed a form of monitoring, and psychology helped to provide this by
producing developmental norms.
Just as Eysenck (1953) was critical of the crucial test of practical application by the
regulative functioning of applied psychology American Army during the First World
within society, he also gave critical attention War; their triumphant success there
to the usefulness of intelligence testing: established psychology once and for all as
an indispensable adjunct to all selection
The new scientific discoveries of Binet, procedures. It may be of interest to quote
Spearman, and Stern in the field of the directive setting out the tasks which the
intelligence testing were put to the Army authorities expected the intelligence
>>
574 Developmental Psychology
<<
test to perform. The test was to designate methods to fields where they may not be
and select men whose superior intelligence appropriate (Eysenck, 1953: 8-9).
indicated the desirability of advancement
for special assignment; to select and This quote is interesting in that it reveals
recommend for ‘developmental battalions’ the indispensable use of psychology in the
such men as were so inferior intellectually as making of armies (and, for that matter,
to be unsuited for regular military training; the making of war). It also suggests how
to enable officers to build up organizations of these tests may go wrong and may lead,
uniform mental strength, in accordance with through misapplication and misuse, to the
definitive specifications concerning mental discarding of those people who might have
requirements; to select men for various types been suitable for a given job and to the
of military duty or for special assignments; retention of those who may be unsuitable.
to eliminate men whose intelligence was so This example also suggests how such tests
inferior as to make it impossible to use them may implicitly be used to separate upper
at all ... The very success of the intelligence and lower classes across a division of labour
test in this difficult assignment became the between the more dangerous and more
reason for subsequent disappointment. menial jobs, on the one hand, and those
Thousands of enthusiasts ... eager to cash that are safer and more desirable on the
in on the new fad, invaded the field and other (it would be interesting to question
tried to apply the Army testing procedures whether these tests were not
in industrial and commercial institutions ... at some level implicitly selecting on the
Uncritical enthusiasts, fired by the conviction basis of class rather than on the basis of
of righteousness given them by some ‘pure’ intelligence).
‘system’ or other, are trying to extend these
<<
and the female to be submissive; we consider why one would want to continue using
heterosexual attraction ‘normal’, homosexual the term ‘normality’ to refer to statistical
attraction ‘abnormal’. [One might] hold averages, especially since its various
these views even though it could be shown implications of the ideal, or of the natural,
that in some communities, among the do not necessarily hold in this context.
ancient Greeks, say, homosexuality was The statistical average is not ideal, nor
statistically more frequent than hetero- need it be natural (as far as that term is
sexuality, or that among some nations, say even relevant here). Indeed, we should
the ancient Egyptians, women tended to be also be cautious of using the notion of
more aggressive and males more submissive the statistical average because to a large
(Eysenck, 1953: 178). extent it retains the evaluative implications
of ‘the normal’. Think of the value
Eysenck’s examples are revealing—not judgement implied, for example,
only of the fact that certain politically by being categorised as ‘well below
implicated values (such as chauvinism average intelligence’.
and homophobia) are often embedded Perhaps Eysenck’s most important point
in the designations of what is ‘normal’ or here is that socio-political ideals often
‘natural’—but also of how these values masquerade as ‘norms’; that when we call
change historically. Few academics today something ‘normal’ we are making a politi-
would venture such propositions as in cally loaded statement (one thoroughly
the above paragraph; yet at the time of invested with social values), rather than a
Eysenck’s writing neither seemed to be value-neutral objective statement. Eysenck
particularly controversial, so much so that (1953: 189) ends his commentary by
he thought to use them as examples of calling our attention to the fact that ‘no
how we use ‘normal’ to designate what ... universal norms of behaviour exist’. He
is in fact supposedly natural. Clearly substantiates this fact with reference to
Eysenck’s attempt here is to show us that Kinsey’s massive study of American sexual-
we frequently use the term ‘normal’ to ity. Kinsey reported such great diversity
refer to something that we consider and conflict of opinions (related to topics
to be somehow biologically or essentially such as masturbation, pre- and extramarital
innate, something natural. What his intercourse, the illicitness of certain sexual
example shows us is that what is taken acts, etc) across areas, age groups and
to be normal, just as what is taken to be communities that no overarching norms
natural, changes over time, and therefore could be found. He quotes Kinsey’s findings
supposed descriptions of normality to substantiate his conclusion:
should be seen as neither universal nor
a-historical. It is clear that assertions of The data now available show that patterns of
universal and a-historical verity are implicit sexual behaviour may be strikingly different
in aligning the supposed ‘norm’ with an for the different social levels that exist in
understanding of what is natural. the same city or town, and sometimes
The point is that not only do so- in immediately adjacent sections of the single
called norms possess an evaluative and community. The data show that divergences
denigratory function, they often take on in sexual patterns of such social groups may
the form of dominant social prejudices, be as great as those which anthropologists
which try to present themselves as natural have found between the sexual patterns of
and universally applicable. Given these different... groups in remote parts of the
problems, it is difficult to understand world (Kinsey, cited in Eysenck, 1953: 184).
Critical issues in developmental psychology 577
Thus, Rose (1991) not only calls into question the reliability of
norms, in that norms are largely generated around the study of the
apparently ‘abnormal’, he also argues that they are almost always
developed as part of the agenda of social control, and that they are
inseparably attached to relations of domination and the maintenance
of hegemony within societies.
578 Developmental Psychology
Eysenck examines the dangers of using very select set of social values above and
norms in applied contexts, as in psycho- beyond all others. That is to say, he is
logy. He notes that what essentially occurs wary of how the norms adopted by the
is that select persons (typically advantaged mental health professions may champion
white males): the values only of those in dominant social
positions. Moreover, he is wary of the
… lay down the law, or try to ... help people damage that this perpetuation may cause.
from [historically disadvantaged] ... classes ... If a patient is seeking a psychotherapist’s
In clinical practice ... therapy ... and help in help because of falling short in some way
the sexual sphere are based essentially upon of a social ‘norm’—say, for example, that
concepts of marriage and sexual conduct of ‘normal sexuality’—then any treatment
which agree with the norms obtaining among schedule that, however subtly, reiterates
the [dominant or controlling classes] ... from this ‘abnormality’ will be damaging,
which the practitioner comes, but which may (Consider here that homosexuality used to
be quite inappropriate to the norms of the be considered psychopathological for this
... person to whom [help] ... is being given very reason.) Such treatment will in effect
(Eysenck, 1953: 190). reinforce and perpetuate the very social
problem (in this case homophobia) that
Although Eysenck uses the example had caused the problem in the first place.
here of sexual norms, the value of his In short, notions of ‘normality’, once
comments should not, obviously, be carried into the sphere of something like
restricted to this context alone. He is wary clinical psychology, can in fact be
here of the problem of perpetuating one actively damaging.
is in the home’ and that children suffer if they are not in the full-time
care of their mothers (Burman, 1994; Poster, 1988). Similarly popular
in developmental psychology literature is the notion that children who
have personal and behavioural problems in their later lives have been
inadequately mothered.
Not only does this mean that mothers (women) are held more
‘responsible’ for the healthy upbringing of children than fathers (men),
it also means that they are frequently held responsible for developmental
problems which in many instances stem from contextual problems beyond
their control, such as poverty and the resultant problems of under-nutrition.
As Singer (1992: 99) suggests, while women are frequently excluded
from full participation in the world they are inevitably held accountable
for the impingements emanating from the world beyond the family on
their children. In this regard, Richter & Griesel (1994) observe that while
mothers are frequently blamed for child malnutrition because of the early
termination of breast-feeding or because they purportedly lack a sufficiently
strong attachment relationship with their children, in reality, malnutrition
is over-determined by poverty and associated social conditions.
in the same way. Winnicott (particularly with his concept of the ‘good-
enough mother’) and Bowlby made various bold pronouncements in
respect of women’s economic and social roles. Indeed, both imposed near-
impossible demands on women, arguing that the conscientious mother
needs to be available and attentive to her children, or risk producing
far-reaching ill-effects in her children (Burman, 1994).
Closely linked to these kinds of understanding of motherhood are
very particular definitions of femininity, definitions which characterise
motherhood as women’s ultimate fulfilment, something they should
necessarily pursue at all costs. Idealising motherhood and making the
primary caregiver gendered in this way perpetuate asymmetrical gender
relations and pathologises women who refuse or fail to conform to these
specific standards of mothering.
<<
It is these social and ideological is not purely familial in nature, and that it
factors that intervene between parents is infiltrated and invested by politics and
and their children and which interpellate social values in all its aspects. He uses Louis
children not as individuals, but as subjects. Althusser’s concept of interpellation, which,
In the complex societies of contemporary in short, refers to how we are each socially
capitalism, parents do not experience their situated as subjects of various types of
children in an innocent and immediate power (Althusser, 1971). The example often
way, and it is argued that a factor like provided for this hegemonic mechanism of
ideology and its functioning can contribute interpellation is that of an individual walk-
significantly to explaining why the ing down a road who is hailed by a police
relationship between parents and children officer. In turning around and answering this
is often tense and contradictory. In other hail, the individual is immediately placed in
words, parents experience their children a particular position which is underwritten
for what they are, as they appear, and also with structural power and meaning; thus
as subjects that are constituted by multiple in this very act he or she becomes a subject
determinations. In this sense, ideology of power. The mechanism of interpellation
interpellates individual children as subjects shows how we are not perhaps as free as we
for parents (Hayes, 1989). typically assume; it emphasises how we are
Hayes’s (1989) point here is essentially placed in particular roles within society in
that the bond between parent and child largely unavoidable ways.
Rose (1991) shares Burman’s (1994) Like Poster (1988), Rose observes that
concern about traditional developmental the recent history of childhood has been one
psychology’s neglect to analyse the of multiple anxieties concerning children,
dynamics of power in the study of which have occasioned any number of
childhood. He argues that childhood is programmes to conserve and shape them.
the most intensely governed sector of He observes that the child, both as an idea
personal existence: and as a target, has become inextricably
connected to the aspirations of the
In different ways, at different times, and by authorities. The environment of the growing
many different routes varying from one section child is to be regulated legislatively and
of society to another, the health, welfare, financially, and educational programmes are
and rearing of children have been linked in likewise carefully monitored and developed.
thought and practice to the destiny of the
nation and the responsibilities of the state. Legislative obligations are imposed upon
The modern child has become the focus of parents, requiring them to carry out social
innumerable projects that purport to safeguard duties from the registration of their children
it from physical, sexual, or moral danger, to at birth to ensuring that they receive
ensure its ‘normal’ development, to actively adequate education up to their teens ... Child
promote certain capacities of attributes such as protection legislation has imposed powers
intelligence, educability, and emotional stability and duties upon local authorities, requiring
(Rose, 1991: 121). them to evaluate the standards of care being
>>
Critical issues in developmental psychology 583
<<
provided to children by their parents through Childhood, in short, has become
the agencies of social work, and to intervene intensely regulated by political power.
into the family to rectify shortcomings,
Conclusion
In this chapter we have examined a variety of major criticisms of the
field of developmental psychology. It has been important to consider
some of the questions raised about the nature of childhood, adolescence,
and the family. This is because commonplace and seemingly ‘natural’
assumptions about what a child is, or should be, are powerful forms of
knowledge. These are the kinds of knowledge that affect the way we
treat and understand others, particularly children. Put differently, what
we learn in developmental psychology affects people in powerful ways,
584 Developmental Psychology
Specific tasks
➊ If indeed children were treated and understood differently in past European cultures,
consider what may have been the case traditionally or historically in African societies.
➋ How do you understand your own adolescence? Do you find that the storm-and-
stress view has any relevance in describing your experiences of this process?
of these studied groups, and how legitimate do you think the results would be when
applied to ‘adolescents’ in other sociocultural environments?
➍ What familial factors do you think have a strong influence on the psychological
development of children?
➎ To what extent, and in what ways, do you think children are controlled in modern
Western society? Do you feel that these kinds of control are warranted?
➏ Give some thought to the developmental theories covered in the preceding chapters.
Which theories exhibit certain of the tendencies criticised by Burman? Furthermore,
if you do have a preferred or favourite theory, give particular thought to how it
might be criticised in terms of the arguments Burman presents.
➑ How might one ‘customise’ Burman’s critiques to human development in the South
African context?
➒ Draw up a list of stereotypes around the idea of ‘mothering’. Do the same around
the idea of ‘fathering’ and then contrast and compare them. Reflect on the
gendered nature of these parenting stereotypes, as well as on the different types of
responsibilities and activities implied by each. >>
Critical issues in developmental psychology 585
<<
➓ If it is indeed limiting to maintain a (relatively) exclusive child-focused approached to
development, what other important areas of focus could we use to widen our
understanding of human development?
11 Consider the notion of normalisation: what kind of children/ parents/ families/ homes
would have been the basis for developmental norms in apartheid-era South Africa, and
which would have been routinely ‘problematised’ (considered deviant, problematic,
‘pathological’) in terms of these norms?
Recommended readings
This chapter has been based on four central texts:
28
Race, culture and
psychological theory
Mambwe Kasese-Hara
Introduction
Often students initially experience undergraduate psychology as though it
were an imitation of some form of natural science—as though it were a pure
science born and nurtured in a neutral environment, without the bias
of class, culture or creed. Most psychology classes introduce the discipline
with concepts and phenomena, such as Wilhelm Wundt’s reaction time,
Thorndike’s law of effect and Skinner’s operant conditioning (Gleitman, 1991);
so that from the onset we tend to assume that the discipline of psychology is a
‘science’ of human behaviour, concerned mainly with physiological processes
in interaction with environmental experiences.
Although the environment could be anything and everything that
bears relevance to the individual or group, psychological theory has tra-
ditionally been based mainly on a particular kind of environment, which
is largely European and middle class (Nsamenang, 1992; Liddel, Kvalsig,
Shabalala & Masilela, 1991). As such, when it comes to the social, cultural
and ethnic contexts in which individuals develop, traditional psychological
theory is generally found to be lacking.
Race, culture and psychological theory 587
so that the world our children are expected to thrive in today, whatever
their colour or ethnicity, is still a world that is polarised along socio-
economic, ethnic, and cultural lines. This presents special challenges for
developmental psychology, some of which will become more apparent
later on in this chapter.
Sir Francis Galton (1869), who is noted to psychologists were ardent followers of the
be the first scholar to attempt some form eugenics movement, including Herbert
of psychological testing, was also the first Spencer and Cyril Burt.
to put forward some form of geneticist Herbert Spencer was renowned for
theorisation regarding mental abilities. the publication of his Introduction to
Galton reasoned that quality breeding stock psychology (1886) in which he argued
yielded quality offspring, and consequently that ‘selective breeding’ was necessary
that brilliance and social eminence were to eliminate ‘unfit’ ‘races’ (Duncan et al,
outcomes of good breeding (that is, ‘good’ 2004). Based on Darwin’s and his own
genes). He argued further that the lower evolutionary ideas, Spencer developed a
classes, in contrast to the upper classes who political theory, namely, ‘Social Darwinism’
monopolised constitutional superiority, which argued that natural selection (that
were lacking in these qualities (Howitt is, survival of the fittest) should be allowed
& Owusu-Bempah, 1994). According to to take its course in humans, and that
Galton’s theory, there was a danger that the governments should not try to help the
good hereditary stock would be swamped poor, weak and the helpless by offering
by the bad as a result of the fecundity them resources (Leahey, 2004: 303).
of the lower classes. His solution to this Cyril Burt, another proponent of
problem was to simply give the more eugenics, was descended from Francis
suitable races—or strains of blood— Galton. Burt was a hereditarian and faithful
a better chance of prevailing over the disciple of the eugenics movement. He
less suitable races through various means, is believed to have falsified his data on
including through sterilisation of the mental inheritance in twins to prove his
lower classes. beliefs. Arthur Jensen and others used
Eugenics refers to the ‘scientific’ Burt’s research data to help argue that
movement begun by Galton’s psychology, racial differences in intelligence were
which was committed to a biosocial mainly due to genetic factors (Howitt &
programme of selective breeding. Several Owusu-Bempah, 1994).
racial differences, must give way to approaches and perspectives that are
less prone to bias (Nsamenang, 1992; Seedat, 1997).
Familial context
According to Nsamenang (2000) familial context is an important part of
the child’s social world. Because the family, the child, and the environment
constantly interact, and therefore influence each other, the family context
plays a determinant role in what is normal and what facilitates or hinders
development. Thus children’s search for meanings, competence and the
‘right way’ of the world begins in the family, long before they go to school
(Nsamenang, 2000). In the African context, parental actions and regulatory
behaviours are embedded in a familial ethos that extends beyond the
parents to include siblings, relatives and other mentors, especially peer
mentors of the extended family networks and neighbourhoods.
594 Developmental Psychology
World-views
World-view. A shared World-views are shared frames of reference or psychological outlooks
frame of reference that by which members of a particular culture perceive or make sense of the
informs the members universe and the fate of the people in it. These are integral aspects of
of a particular culture’s
the social representations through which cultures make sense of human
perceptions of the universe
and the fate of people in it.
existence. Indigenous West African world-views are marked by a set
of social realities, cultural traditions and existential imperatives. These
world-views constitute different frames of reference from those that
inform contemporary developmental psychology.
Conceptualisation of development
According to Nsamenang (2006), developmental stages are characterised
by distinctive developmental tasks that are defined within the framework
of cultural realities and developmental agenda. Underlying this is the
conceptualisation of development ‘as the acquisition and growth of
the physical, cognitive, social and emotional competencies required to
engage fully in family and society’ (Nsamenang, 2006: 295). Development
therefore, is seen as a transformation within the individual, which is
brought about by their participation in cultural activities. This means
that child development can be a guided process of gradual and systematic
social integration (Rogoff, 2003).
This characterisation of development as a cumulative process of inte-
gration within the community and clan differs theoretically from the
more individualistic accounts proposed by Freud, Erikson and Piaget,
and resonates much more with the cultural preoccupations expressed
by parents in many African societies (Serpell, 1994). Nsamenang’s social
ontogenic framework also bears much in common with Vygotsky’s
social cultural approach in that both models conceptualise development
as a set of processes embedded within the sociocultural relations (and
events) in the child’s immediate environment (Kasese-Hara, 2004).
Nsamenang (2006) argues that socio-ontogenesis is a universal para-
digm that makes possible the study of human development in the context
of children’s engagement of cognition as they participate in cultural
communities. As such the paradigm, according to Nsamenang, offers
an innovative impetus to conceptualise and generate developmental
knowledge, which not only empowers various cultures but also has the
potential to expand visions and data beyond restrictive Eurocentric grids.
Evaluating Nsamenang
Seedat (1997) observed that there has been a tendency for mainstream
psychology to ignore the value of scholarly ideas from critical theorists
such as Fanon and Bulhan. It is no surprise therefore that Nsamenang’s
work, perhaps the only comprehensive attempt at theorising the cycle
of human development, backed by indigenous research by various
researchers on the continent (see for example Asante, 1990; Babatunde,
1992; Jahoda, 1982; Rogoff, 2003; Serpell, 1993) has received such negligible
attention. Theoretical developments from an African perspective such as
Race, culture and psychological theory 597
Conclusion
The following issues have been raised in this chapter:
Specific tasks
➊ In your own opinion, would the theories of psychological development be any
different if they had evolved from a historical or cultural context other than the
Euro-American one?
➌ Developmental psychology has evolved a great deal since the nineteenth century
when psychology was implicated in societal racism. Given that societies such as South
Africa are still battling with issues of racism, how free is contemporary psychology
from bias based on race, ethnicity or culture, and how objective can researchers and
practitioners be when dealing with individuals or groups from a different racial,
ethnic or cultural background than their own?
Recommended readings
Bulhan, H A (1980). Dynamics of cultural in-betweenity: An empirical study. International Journal
of Psychology, 15, 105-121
(This article is recommended because it provides a unique view of the dynamic nature of identity
development among black intellectuals amidst an alien cultural domination.)
Duncan, N, Stevens, G & Bowman, B (2004). South African psychology and racism. (In D Hook,
N Mkhize, P Kiguwa, A Collins, E Burman & I Parker. Critical psychology. Lansdowne:
UCT Press, 360-388.)
(This chapter analyses ‘racism’ as a concept and gives an extensive and critical discussion of how South
African psychology has been implicated in the racism of the society.)
Howitt, D & Owusu-Bempah, J (1994). The racism of psychology. New York: Harvester Wheatshef.
(This text gives a historical journey of psychology, highlighting the racism inherent in the theorisation
and practice of psychology.)
Race, culture and psychological theory 599
Moosa, R, Moonsamy, G & Fridjon, P (1997). Identification patterns among black students at
a predominantly white university. South African Journal of Psychology, 27(4), 256-260.
(This article has been recommended because it is an empirical report of the application of Bulhan’s
theory of ‘cultural in-betweenity’ among South African students. It further elaborates on the dynamic
role that the subjugated play in forming their own identities in reaction to cultural domination.)
Scribner, S (1985). Vygotsky’s uses of history. (In J V Wertsch (ed.), Communication and cognition:
A Vygotskian perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
(An intricate discussion of the Vygotskian view of cognitive development.)
Serpell, R (1993). The significance of schooling: Life-journeys in an African society. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
(Serpell discusses the significance of schooling in African society, by critically analysing the sociocultural
environment in which African children develop, and how this may differ from the assumptions of Western
developmental psychology. The text draws on empirical research done in rural African communities.)
CHAPTER
29
Gender identity:
Contestations and questions
Gillian Mooney and Peace Kiguwa
display, you might ask them to complete a questionnaire that lists so-
called ‘masculine’ (for example aggressive) and ‘feminine’ (for example
passive) characteristics. However, if you believed that gender is fluid in
nature and multiple forms of gender identity are possible, then you may
examine a specific set of individuals who exist in a particular set of social
and historical conditions. For example, you would interview the same
group of students, asking them questions that allow you to understand
how their gender identities had evolved over the course of their lives.
Thus how we define gender (ontology) determines how we investigate
it (epistemology); in this sense, there is a reciprocal relationship between
ontology and epistemology.
Defining gender
Gender, in many ways, can be described as a practice or something that the
individual does. Gender encompasses the sociocultural and psychological Gender. Social, cultural
differences between men and women. Gender is distinct from sex, which and psychological
describes biological and anatomical differences between men and women. differences between men
and women.
When we behave in certain ways because we are ‘male’ or ‘female’, we are
engaging in social practices that inform us what our gender is. A practice
here may be referred to as a way of behaving. Practices are ideological
in the sense that some ways of being are dominant while others are
marginalised. Gendered practice is ideological when it structures our
choices around our own gendered behaviour. For example, fatherhood
has undergone many ideological changes in practice. Twenty years ago
parenting was largely the prerogative of women; women were given
maternity leave from work in order to care for their children. The work
life of fathers remained uninterrupted. Today however, the practice of
parenting has shifted to include men, who now bear more responsibility
for parenting and are awarded paternity leave from work, although this
is still less than maternity leave. Our choices of gender roles are thus
influenced by our wider social contexts.
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON
Gender is a fundamental guiding principle of
society. The ways we order and organise our world
and ourselves are determined by pre-existing societal
circumstances and relations, and these constrain how
we may understand ourselves and relate to others.
These social relations are material in the sense that
they act upon individuals in society in concrete ways
and affect the kinds of freedom that we may exercise
as men and women. When we employ different mean-
ings to understand certain behaviour as ‘masculine’
or ‘feminine’, we are engaging in a gendered process
that is discursive. Discursive here refers to those social
and cultural meanings that are ascribed to both males
and females. The discursive aspect to understanding gender is useful From the minute we are
born we are assigned
in its focus on the role of ideology and power in the practice of gender.
a gender label which
Gender is a characteristic existing outside of us; it is the way that society determines how society
categorises people. We are born into a world of gendered practices understands us, and how
and we are actively acculturated into these practices. Thus in terms of we understand society.
602 Developmental Psychology
Gender stereotypes
A stereotype is a structured set of expectations and beliefs about the personal
Stereotype. Structured qualities, attributes, and characteristics of a group of individuals (Nelson,
set of expectations and Acker & Manis, 1996). In psychology, stereotypes have traditionally been
beliefs about the personal understood in neutral terms, as one type of categorisation that shares
qualities, attributes and
many of the facets of other cognitive categories (Deux, 1987). The process
characteristics of various
kinds of people.
of gender stereotyping is the portrayal of the ideal or typical characteristics
of males and females. Stereotyping, therefore, is the mechanism through
which the prescriptive process for ideas of ‘normal’ behaviour is
actualised. It is through this process that the concepts of ‘femininity’ and
‘masculinity’ become opposites (Sharpe, 1978). Stereotyping behaviour
may often be linked to broader ideological functioning that is deployed
to reinforce existing power relations between men and women. In this
sense, stereotyping is never only about cognitive processing, as is often
emphasised in traditional social psychology.
Our gender belief system may be our internal site of gender stereotypes.
This system is composed of our beliefs and opinions about males and
females, and the qualities of masculinity and femininity. This belief system
includes attitudes towards prescribed behav-
iours and roles, and attitudes towards those
PHOTO: CHA JOHNSTON
Fuzzy sets
The perspective from which the understanding of masculinity and
femininity is constructed is important. This is because our language, by
which we delimit and define such constructs, changes according to the
usefulness of the constructs within a given society at a particular time.
Often there is no evidence of actual differences between the genders
in terms of constructs such as achievement-orientation, self-esteem,
sociability, nurturance and dominance (Deux, 1987). It is important
to acknowledge that our ideas of what is masculine and feminine are
located as ‘fuzzy sets’. This means that there are no universal definitions
of masculinity and femininity. Definitions of these constructs primarily
606 Developmental Psychology
The scene is a ‘singles’ bar located in a As the music comes to an end, Grey
middle-sized town. Some people are dancing Buttons says, ‘You are a very interesting
in one section of the room. Others are sitting person. I’d like us to talk some more…why
or standing around the bar drinking and don’t we sit over here?’ motioning to a
socialising. An individual with brown shirt small table in the corner.
buttons (Brown Buttons) walks purposefully ?
toward a person with grey shirt buttons They’ve been sitting for quite a while.
(Grey Buttons) and begins a conversation. Brown Buttons orders drinks again,
After a few minutes, Brown Buttons asks Grey and they continue to talk … ‘I’m really
Buttons to dance. Grey Buttons agrees, and fascinated by your life; I’d like to get to
they begin to move to the dancing area. know you better.’
? ‘That’s interesting,’ says Grey Buttons.
As they start to dance, Grey Buttons ‘I find you exciting too, but I’m not
says to Brown Buttons, ‘You are a good sure that I’m able to handle too much
dancer. I don’t come across many people familiarity now. I’m really interested in
who dance this well.’ pursuing my career…’
‘Thank you’ says Brown Buttons with ?
a slightly embarrassed smile. ‘I think you ‘I understand your position,’ says
dance well too.’ Brown Buttons, ‘but I’d really like to
‘What do you do for a living?’ asks see more of you.’
Grey Buttons. ‘I’m going to think about it,’ says
‘I’m a high school teacher’, answers Grey Buttons. ‘Why don’t we stop off at
Brown Buttons. ‘And you?’ my place? Perhaps we’d both get some
‘I’m a research technician,’ says Grey perspective over coffee and…’
Buttons, ‘but I’m thinking of getting
into computers.’ Source: Crawford & Unger, 2004: 70
Gender constancy
When determining whether or not children uphold gender stereotypes,
we need to determine, firstly, whether or not these children have attained
an understanding of gender constancy. For a child to have developed a
sense of gender constancy, he or she has to have a biological understanding
of gender. This involves understanding that gender generally does not
change. This understanding is acquired in three stages. First, the child
will acquire a rudimentary gender identity as either a boy or a girl. Second,
608 Developmental Psychology
the child will learn that the genitalia do not change. This occurs at about
four years. Third, the child will understand that gender remains the same
regardless of changes in clothing, appearance or activities. This occurs at
about five or six years.
In the United States, by the age of three years, children were found to
be able to classify males and females correctly, and were aware of the social
expectations surrounding each gender; for example, girls wear dresses and
boys do not (Papalia & Olds, 1993). Between the ages of four and five years,
children possess knowledge of the stereotypes for adult occupations; for
example, women are nurses and men are doctors. In this sense, culture may
be understood to function ideologically in its favouring of some gendered
practices and not others.
Gender valuing
An important part of the awareness of the social categories of gender is that
children often value the categories to which they assign themselves and
devalue those categories to which they do not belong (Powlishta, Serbin,
Doyle & White, 1994). It is often considered important that children value
their particular categories in order to formulate a gender identity (Turner
& Gervai, 1995). The notion of valuing may be more complex for girls
because the masculine identity is generally more valued in societies such
as South Africa.
Gender stereotyping
Labelling. Attaching a An important part of the acquisition of gender labelling and identity is
meaning to an object or a developing awareness of gender stereotypes. Some children who are
an individual, which helps
as young as two years old have acquired gender stereotypes around toys
individuals to order
their world.
and activities (Mayes, 1986) and adult occupations (Weintraub, Pritchard-
Clemens, Sockloff, Ethridge, Gracely & Myers, 1984). Other important
gender stereotypes include appearance (Edelbrock & Sugwara, 1978),
peers (Beneneson, Apostoleris & Parnass, 1997; Newcomb & Bagwell,
1995), and media figures (Santrock & Yussen, 1987). The examination of
the development of social skills relating to gender has found that mothers
and fathers interact differently with boys and girls (Best, House, Barnard
and Spicker 1994). Boys are generally more stereotyped and show more
negative reactions to female-preferred activities than girls do to male-
preferred activities. This is thought to occur because girls receive more
latitude in terms of constructing themselves around gender stereotypes
(Fagot, 1977). It is in this sense that some feminists have argued that the
category, ‘woman’, is not universal.
With the changes in the political they do ballet... and they wear skirts’
dispensation in South Africa, a new (Haiden, 1998: 56).
pattern of social thought has become
dominant. We now have a Constitution The following dialogue presents a
that guarantees equal rights to all few interesting observations in terms of
citizens. One of these rights is equality in activity and toy stereotypes:
terms of gender.
The Constitution should reflect Question: What do girls do when they play?
the attitudes of the people. We can Answer: They like to do all kinds of stuff.
assume, therefore, that because it is in They like Princess...
the Constitution South Africans believe Question: How do you play Princess?
in gender equality. If this assumption is Answer: Princess ... there’s like the queen
correct, then we should be raising our and the princess, the wicked witch and
children to believe in gender equality. stuff... The wicked witch treats Cinderella
This notion was scrutinised in relation like rubbish ... and like isn’t very, very
to a group of six-year-old girls. These girls nice. The princess, she cleans up the floor
were asked about their beliefs regarding and stuff like the ugly witch tells her to ...
gender and gender stereotypes. All the Question: What makes toys be for girls?
girls could correctly label themselves as Answer: Toys is Barbies and like talking
girls. They valued being a girl because dolls and ... clothes. Barbie clothes and
girls perform better academically (even the most important thing that I like in the
though they were all still in preschool). whole wide world is Barbie. I’ve always
They all would choose to be girls because asked my Mommy and Daddy
girls are not naughty or aggressive. for Christmas.
<<
This was evidenced in the following play with Kens, but not Barbies (Haiden &
girl’s comments: Zietkiewicz, 1999).
Question: Can girls play with guns Thus, even though we have a
and cars? Constitution that guarantees gender
Answer: No ... I don’t play with cars. equality, are children still being raised
Because it isn’t right for [girls] to. with gender stereotypes and rules for
They have to play with Barbies and stuff. gender behaviour?
If they haven’t got anything to play with Considering the evidence presented
then they can, but it’s not right for them. here and your own childhood experiences,
Question: Can boys play with dolls? what do you think is the ideal way to raise
Answer: No ... they just can’t. They can children in terms of gender socialisation?
Biological explanations
Gender identity Gender identity formation has often been explained through the physiology
formation. Development or biology of girls and boys. According to this explanation, the child’s
of the sense of being physiological constitution as either a boy or a girl will automatically and
masculine or feminine.
naturally determine his or her masculine or feminine gender identity
(Craig, 1996). This explanation, however, does not receive much support
and evidence has been produced which indicates that physical sex has little to
do with psychological gender. Money, Erhardt & Masica (1968, in Papalia &
Olds,1993) examined subjects who were chromosomally male (that is, they had
testes and not ovaries), but looked like females, and were raised as girls. These
children were all stereotypically female and played with dolls and other girls’
toys. Money and Tucker (1975, in Sarafino & Armstrong, 1980) reported a
male who was raised as a female. During a circumcision procedure at seven
months, one identical twin’s penis was irreparably damaged. At 17 months,
doctors advised the parents to raise the child as a girl. The child’s genital
structures were modified and she or he underwent hormone treatment. The
parents changed the child’s name, clothing and hair accordingly. At three
years, the child’s gender identity was diverging from her twin brother’s.
At five years, she preferred wearing frilly dresses, and asked for a doll for
Christmas. In short, she was conforming to a feminine stereotype (Money
& Tucker, 1975, in Sarafino & Armstrong, 1980). This evidence would
suggest that the examination of gender identity formation cannot occur
without consideration of social factors. However, Money & Tucker did not
provide an account of the individual’s adolescent development. Subsequent
evidence suggests that the subject in the study in question experienced
anxiety, problems in the formation of identity, struggling with the perception
Gender identity: Contestations and questions 611
of being a man in a woman’s body. This would therefore suggest that biology
may have some role to play in gendered identity.
Cognitive development
Kohlberg (1966) postulated that gender identity development is a function
of cognitive developmental changes. Children achieve gender identity
(classifying themselves as girls or boys) as part of a general tendency
to think in terms of categories. Once this gender identity has been
established, the child will actively seek out behaviour, activities and values
that distinguish boys from girls and subscribe to the gender category that
he or she belongs to. This process is considered to be complete when the
child achieves gender constancy and it is accompanied by Piaget’s notion
of concrete operational thought (Grain, 1992) (see chapter on Piaget’s
theory of cognitive development for more detail).
Block (1973) expanded Kohlberg’s theory in terms of stages of gender
identity development. First, very young children will possess undifferen-
tiated gender role concepts. Second, in the preschool years, the child will
conform to conventional and rigid gender roles. Third, during early
adolescence, this rigidity will be at a maximum. Finally, during adulthood, a
stronger androgyny emerges (Mussen et al, 1990). It should be noted, however,
that not all children place a high value on the roles that have been prescribed
for their gender, and this theory also does not explain individual differences
in the nature and strength of gender typing (Mussen et al, 1990).
schema. If events are in violation of this schema, the child may fail to notice
or remember them. Gender schema theory is an example of the meeting
of cognitive and social theories. This is because it assumes that in societies
that are gender polarised, this internalised lens facilitates the developmental
process of conventionally gendered perceptions. This theory, therefore, rests
on two fundamental tenets: first, that gender lenses are entrenched in societal
discourses and practice, and internalised by children; and second, that once
these lenses have been internalised, the child is predisposed to constructing
his or her identity in a manner that is congruent with this lens (Bem, 1993).
This theory does not, however, explain how the child acquires a gender role
schema and how the schema may differ from child to child.
Gender salience
Gender salience. Where Gender salience may be a further aspect of cognitive development that offers
gender is considered an an explanation for gender identity formation. Gender may be more salient
important organising or important to some children than it is to others. Some children may view
principle for the individual.
and interpret the world through gender-based lenses, while others may
interpret it through other categories (Papalia & Olds, 1993). An example
of the impact of gender salience may be as follows: A group of children are
playing with a ball in a park. Child A, for whom gender is salient, may
ask herself whether or not playing with a ball is an activity in which girls
participate, or whether other girls are playing. Child B, for whom gender is
not salient, may ask herself whether she will look foolish or clumsy if she
participates (Mussen et al, 1990). Although this theory may provide insight
into individual differences in gender identity formation, it does not explain
why for some children gender is salient, while not for others.
An important aspect to remember in the formation of gender identity is
the role of language in this process. The English language contains many
gender biases, for example, man and he have traditionally been used to refer
to all humans, both male and female. Sexist thought and gender stereotyping
may thus be the product of years of exposure to sexist language (Santrock &
Yussen, 1987). This masculinist bias is reflected in many languages.
and more alert. Thus, the expectations of the parents were based on societally-
gendered notions of the different sexes. Lamb (1981) found that, in terms of
parental roles, mothers assumed more responsibility for physical care and
nurturance, while fathers fulfilled the role of providing playful interaction,
and were more demanding and exacting in seeing that the child conformed
to societal norms. McCandless (1967) found that parents engaged in selective
reinforcement of gender-appropriate behaviour. Fathers were rougher in
play with their sons than with their daughters, and generally mothers were
gentler when playing with their sons. In terms of toys, parents bought toys
such as guns, transportation toys and footballs for boys, while girls received
toys such as tea sets, dolls and doll furniture (McCandless, 1967).
Rheingold & Cook (1975, in Sarafino & Armstrong, 1980) examined
the bedrooms of middle-class preschool children and found that the boys’
rooms were decorated with animals and the girls’ rooms were decorated
in florals and frills. In terms of household chores, girls were assigned tasks
such as washing dishes and dusting, while boys had to take out the dustbin
and wash the car (Sarafino & Armstrong, 1980). Thus, the actions and
gender stereotypes of parents themselves that are vital in the formation of
a child’s gender identity. It is through this process that gender stereotypes
are perpetuated from generation to generation.
Parents also tend to socialise boys more intensely than girls, and there is
considerable pressure on the boy to act like a ‘real boy and not like [a] girl’
(Papalia & Olds, 1993: 391). Girls are allowed more freedom in terms of
clothing, games, play and playmates. Fathers appear to be a major influence
in gender stereotyping, appearing more concerned about this than mothers.
Fathers become more easily upset when their sons engage in cross-gender play
(Biller, 1993). In terms of single-parent homes, it is usually the mother who
is the single parent. Children from these homes tend to hold less stereotyped
views because the mother is a more androgynous role model (Kate, 1979). In
these homes, boys show a more feminine patterning of behaviour, but this Children prefer playmates
does depend on the attitude of the mother (Lamb, 1981). of the same gender.
PHOTOS: CH A JOHNSTON
labels are indicative of the varying values and freedom that are attached
to traditional gender roles. Accordingly, ‘sissy’ is a more pejorative term
than ‘tomboy’. Perhaps successful tomboys are the best example of ‘gender
bilinguals’ (Santrock & Yussen, 1987: 492), who are successful in both
same- and cross-gender play, and are not ostracised by their peers. These
girls are able to switch patterns of talk, naming, touch, and space as they
cross the divide between the genders. They have access to the segregated
activity of boys, depending on their verbal and athletic skills, and their
willingness to fight. They maintain access to girls’ activities by claiming
friendship with the most popular girl and guarding the play of girls from
the invasion of boys (Santrock & Yussen, 1987).
Feminist contributions
Since the emergence of the feminist movement there have been significant
contributions to writings on gender development by feminist researchers
that have critiqued traditional models of development as exclusive of
women’s subjective experiences of gender. In addition, these contributions
have also proposed alternative approaches that seek to address such
omissions. The work of Carol Gilligan (1982), for example, represents
a challenge to models of development based on male norms. Gilligan’s
work is not only significant in its critique of how universal standards
Gender identity: Contestations and questions 615
Power relations
The influence of power relations that not only exist within society
as a whole but also between the researcher and the subject has gained
increasing attention in developmental psychological research. Qualitative
research methodologies (see Gergen, 1988) have in many ways promoted
this issue of how power dynamics may play out between psychologists
and their participants. These power dynamics reinforce particular
understandings and interpretations of social phenomena and people.
Furthermore, the role of power in how both women and men may
experience gender within society means that we cannot theorise aspects
of human development without taking into account how particular social
relations are produced and negotiated within different societies (Burman,
1994; Butler, 1990).
Subjective experiences
Through research approaches that emphasise the significance of people’s
subjective experiences and the meanings that they ascribe to these
experiences, a crucial shift in the traditional paradigm of the objective
researcher interested only in experimental and observable behaviour has
been common in some areas of research in developmental psychology (see
Burman, 1994). This tradition has provided significant understandings
of how women experience gender within society, allowing us to
understand the social nature of the gendered process. However, it may
also be redundant in its tendency to focus only on the description of these
experiences without substantive critical interrogation or engagement with
issues of ideology and power.
contexts in the sense that he or she may independently resist these contexts.
This notion has been severely criticised by many feminist theorists who
emphasise the importance of the shared nature of human relations, whereby
the individual only develops identity in relation to others (Shefer, 2004).
Conclusion
The following table presents four different positions related to ontology
and epistemology in understanding gender and how knowledge about
gender is created. These four perspectives may share ways of defining
gender as a social construct (ontology), but have different ways of investi-
gating what the social constructs of gender actually mean (epistemology).
We, as authors, have presented a variety of ways in which gender is
Gender identity: Contestations and questions 617
Specific tasks
➊ What are the three best and three worst things about being the gender you are?
Provide justification for each one.
CRITICAL THINKING TASKS
➋ Do you think that gender roles should exist in society? Provide reasons for your answer.
➌ Examine the ways in which you think your culture impacts on your gender identity.
➍ What do you think is the relationship between gender identity and sexual identity?
➎ ‘Men are perpetrators of crime. Women are victims of crime.’
5.1 Do you agree with this statement? Give reasons for your answer.
5.2 Why do you think that this particular dichotomy exists in societies?
➏ Refer to the Box on Brown Buttons and Grey Buttons in the text. At each question
mark, select the sex of the participants. Do you find that it is easy to determine the
sex of the Grey or Brown Buttons? What criteria of gender stereotyping influence
your choice?
6.1 Do you think it is possible to engage in continuous gender inconsistent behaviour
during social interaction? Can masculinity and femininity be complementary
aspects of gender that reproduce sex?
618 Developmental Psychology
Recommended readings
Bem, S L (1993). The lenses of gender. New Haven: Yale University Press.
(This text is a typical example of the ontological and epistemological positions of the positivist
gender researcher located within the social-cognitive perspective. Bem is also the leading proponent
of “androgyny”.)
Butler, J (1990). Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. New York: Routledge.
(This text is a typical example of the ontological and epistemological positions of a post-positivist
gender researcher. Butler’s seminal text explores the idea of gender identity as performative, that is
the possibilities of forming and choosing individual identity. The text offers a critique of binary forms
of thinking, including feminism, that have promoted a notion of ‘women’ as a group with common
characteristics and agendas.)
Crawford, M & Unger, R (2004). Women and Gender: A Feminist Psychology (4th ed).
Boston: McGraw Hill.
(This text presents feminist analyses of gender in society from social, economic, political contexts,
illustrating the importance of gender as a system of meanings.)
Gergen, K (1988). Feminist Critique of Science and the Challenge of Social Epistemology. In M Gergen
(ed). Chapter 3. Feminist Thought and the Structure of Knowledge. New York: New York University.
(This reading presents a useful and critical exploration of feminism’s position in critical social science,
critiquing the hegemony of gendered knowledge base.)
Reinisch, J M (1987). Masculinity/Femininity: Basic perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press.
(Providing comprehensive summaries of the different positivist psychological explanations of gender
identity and its formation, this text is a comprehensive historical introduction to the study of gender.)
Wood, J T (1994). Gendered Lives: Communication, gender and culture. Belmont: Wordsworth.
(The social nature of gender means that the viewpoints of other disciplines may assist the reader
to understand the notion of gender. This text provides an introduction to the approaches offered
by sociology.)
For a number of articles on the history of adolescence and adolescent sexuality see the journal
The History of the Family, 8, 2003.
CHAPTER
30
Theory and South African
developmental psychology
research and literature
Catriona Macleod
The questions that form the basis for this chapter are:
Theoretical frameworks
It must be made clear from the outset, however, that this chapter is
not intended as a comprehensive review of developmental psychology
research and literature in South Africa, for two reasons. First, putting
boundaries around what counts as developmental psychology, and what
does not, proves to be difficult. For example, collecting research on
children only is not satisfactory, given the life-span developmental theories.
Focusing on work that specifically studies individual development is also
not adequate, given the emphasis on the meso-, exo- and chrono-systems
of Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) approach. Asking the questions ‘Which
studies concerning children, adolescents, adults and the aged are relevant,
and which not?’ and ‘Which studies on the family, the school, race, class,
cultural issues etc. are relevant and which not?’ becomes a tedious, and
perhaps not very useful task. Second, researchers in South Africa, contrary
to popular belief, are relatively prolific. Collating and summarising all the
Mechanistic approach.
A philosophical approach
research in developmental psychology (whatever the boundaries decided
to studying humans upon) exceeds the scope of the current chapter. In this chapter, therefore,
that maintains that all we shall take a broad view of the field in the last 15 years, discussing the
phenomena may be main theoretical trends and illustrating each with examples of research or
understood in terms of theoretical writing.
cause and effect, and that In structuring this chapter I have utilised Overton & Reese’s
basic universal laws may
(1973, cited in Widdershoven, 1997) distinction between mechanistic
be established.
and organismic models of development, as well as Lerner (1986) and
Organismic approach. A Widdershoven’s (1997) extension of this to the contextual and narrative
theoretical approach that models, respectively.
emphasises the need to In 1973, Overton & Reese (in Widdershoven, 1997) identified two
approach people as a total basic metaphors or models that underlay developmental psychology
entity with a multitude of theorising of the time. What they meant by this is that all the theories
inter-related processes.
of human development could broadly be divided into two categories
Contextual approach.
in terms of their underlying philosophical assumptions about the
An approach to the study nature of development and the nature of the developing person. They
of psychology that posits called these two categories mechanistic and organismic (more detail
that behaviour must be concerning what is meant by each of these is supplied in the relevant
studied in relation to the sections below). In response to further developments in the field,
context within which Lerner (1986) introduced a further category, the contextual model.
it occurs; interpreting
At a later stage, Widdershoven (1997) discusses a narrative approach
behaviour outside the
context is misleading.
to developmental psychology. Although Widdershoven (1997)
introduces an important new element to the broad understanding
of the basic models underlying developmental theorising, his use
Narrative approach.
Emphasises the central role of the word ‘narrative’ is less than inclusive. Narrative theory is
of language and meaning- just one approach within many broadly identified with the social
making in the formation constructionist movement in psychology. Thus, for the fourth model
and structuring of self, we shall propose a social constructionist model.
identity and the other. Work in developmental psychology has from its inception been
plagued by a number of controversies. These controversies are usually
Social constructionist
posed in the form of dualisms: nature versus nurture; continuity
approaches. Theoretical
approaches that highlight versus discontinuity; universality versus relativism; activity versus
the role of language in passivity; risk versus resilience. The questions evolving from these
constructing reality. controversies essentially are: To what extent is human development a
T h e o r y a n d S o u t h A f r i c a n d e v e l o p m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y r e s e a rc h a n d l i t e r a t u r e 621
Outcome beliefs
Attitude to the
X
behaviour
Outcome evaluation
Normative beliefs
Intention to perform
X Subjective norm The behaviour
the behaviour
Motivation to comply
Control beliefs
X Perceived
Perceived power of behavioural control
control factor
Figure 30.1 Each box in the diagram represents a discrete and identifiable attribute, or the interaction of
two attributes. The arrows indicate causal (one-way) relationships between these discrete elements.
Source: Modified from Ajzen & Madden (1986 in Morojele, 1997: 223).
Organismic approaches
In organismic approaches the processes of human development are
viewed as an organised whole. In other words, development is seen
T h e o r y a n d S o u t h A f r i c a n d e v e l o p m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y r e s e a rc h a n d l i t e r a t u r e 623
Organismic approaches
assume that people example, recommend in cases of developmental dyslexia (as assessed by
progress through invariant the above-mentioned tests) an intervention programme aimed at the
stages of development. development of orthographic reading skills. Amod, Cockcroft & Soellaart
Environmental factors (2007: 123), in reporting on a study of the use of the Griffiths Mental
(such as school or home) Developmental Scales for infants amongst black South Africans state that,
merely enhance or impede
the progression. ‘the early identification of delay in infancy and early childhood improves
the possibility of intervention’.
One of the potential difficulties with organismic models is that,
because of the emphasis on internal factors, researchers may ignore the
political, social, gendered and cultural context within which development
is taking place. This has certainly been the case in some earlier South
African research (see for example Ackerman, 1990). However, this type
of decontextualisation is not necessarily a feature of research that utilises
organismic frames. For example, Swartz (2007: 361), using psychoanalytic
theory, argues that the Oedipal stage is pivotal in terms of children’s
awareness of ‘racial differences and their effects on class, privilege and
custom’. She emphasises the variability of developmental pathways (recall
the principle of equifinality discussed above) and ‘their construction in
powerful social, economic and political contexts’.
Organismic models that take context into account inevitably bump
up against the universalism versus relativism debate. For example,
Tudin, Straker & Mendolsohn (1994) investigate the relationship between
Kohlberg’s stages of moral development and exposure to political and social
complexity amongst a group of South African university students. They
accept Kohlberg’s assertion that there are universal principles that guide
moral reasoning and that there are basic, invariant developmental stages.
Although their research revolves around the influence of social context on
the development of this moral reasoning. Ferns & Thom (2001: 38), on the
other hand, present their findings in a study on the moral development
of white and black teenagers as ‘evidence against cultural universality in
Kohlberg’s theory’. They argue that the influence of norms and values,
parenting styles and historical and political effects mitigate against a stage-
like progression and identical endpoints of morality.
T h e o r y a n d S o u t h A f r i c a n d e v e l o p m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y r e s e a rc h a n d l i t e r a t u r e 625
Positivist approaches
Much research in South Africa that attempts to understand the relation
between individual children and the environment is premised on
Operationalise. positivist principles. In positivism relevant variables are identified and
To give definition to operationalised, controls are set up to obviate the influence of extraneous
variables and procedures variables, quantitative data is collected, and statistical tests of probability
utilised in research.
are run. In this way it is hoped to gain insight into the influence of the
Extraneous variables.
environment on the child. Importantly, though, the child and the social
Variables that interfere context are treated as two distinct realities, ontologically separate while
with (that is, are affecting each other (Dawes & Donald, 1994).
not central to) the One of the aims of positivism is to build up a cumulative knowledge
main relationships base that is objective and based on universal truths. A key concept here is
being studied. generalisability, which means that results in one situation will hold true
in another. Barbarin & Richter (2001) and Aase, Meyer & Sagvolden (2006)
Probability.
The calculation of
provide examples of studies that investigate generalisable developmental
the likelihood of an processes or properties.
event occurring using Barbarin & Richter (2001) test the cross-national generalisability of
proportional frequency. the relationship between community violence, poverty and psychological
difficulties in children. They find that, as in the United States, community
Ontology. Metaphysical danger in South Africa is linked to a variety of psychological problems,
inquiry concerned with
including anxiety, depression, aggression, opposition and low affability
the question of existence.
in children, but that socio-economic status is not related to behavioural
Generalisability. and emotional adjustment. Aase, Meyer & Sagvolden (2006) argue for the
The ability to judge dynamic developmental theory of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Dis-
whether something is order (ADHD), in which it is postulated that ADHD is a neurobiological
applicable to an entire disorder caused by dysfunctional dopamine systems. They support their
class or category of people, argument through the replication of findings amongst children in Norway
events or phenomena.
with children in the Limpopo province of South Africa. The key difference
Attention Deficit
between these two studies is that Barbarin & Richter (2001) analyse
Hyperactivity Disorder. generalisable relationships between social and individual characteristics
A disorder characterised (that is, a contextual approach) whereas Aase, Meyer & Sagvolden (2006)
by attentional argue for the generalisability of internal, biological characteristics.
problems, impulsivity Positivist research allows for comparative work. This is different to
and hyperactivity. the notion of generalisability discussed above. Researchers conducting
comparative research are not necessarily interested in establishing universal
laws or truths. They may rather want to provide an in-depth description
of two groups identified as different in some ways and as similar in others.
A good example of this is Liddell’s (1996) research on the interpretations
of six pictures by 80 South African and 80 British children in their second
and third year of schooling. Liddell (1996) starts her paper by exploring
how pictures may:
their picture interpretation skills. But then she asks the question whether
the same patterns ‘manifest themselves in children from homes where
literacy skills amongst parents are poorly developed, where picture books
do not exist, where teacher:child ratios mean that one teacher assists
40 to 50 children in the classroom, and where children at school are
exposed to—at most—four illustrated readers in a year’ (Liddell, 1996:
356). This question is important, as it provides the framework within
which the comparative research is located. Here Liddell (1996) invokes
structural-contextual issues, not to explain the differences noted, but to
frame her question. Her results show differences in the way rural South
African and rural British children interpret pictures, as well as different
patterns of change as the two groups progress in school. She discusses
this in the light of the possible different functions of literacy in the
two communities.
One of the most significant achievements of positivist research is
the development of statistical models that allow for prediction. Liddell,
Lycett & Gordon (1997) utilise such a model to predict children’s early
school achievement in rural South African schools. They found that if
children master basic elements of the curriculum and behave in ways that
allow them to survive crowded and under-resourced rural classrooms,
they will do well in Grade Two. Mathews, Aarø, Flisher, Mukoma,
Wubs & Schaalma (2008) use structural equation modelling to predict
transition to first sexual intercourse. Factors include being male, being
older, coming from a lower socio-economic status, intentions to have
sexual intercourse, poor self-efficacy in negotiating delayed sex, inti-
mate partner violence. However, the variables and models used in
making these predictions are not quick or cheap to measure, thereby
putting into question the usefulness of utilising this type of research in
widespread programmes.
One of the potential difficulties with positivist research is that
there is too little critical analysis by researchers as to how terms are
operationalised. For example, Cherian & Malehase (2000) investigate
the relationship between parental control and children’s scholastic
achievement. They state that an ‘objective estimate’ of parental control
was obtained via a questionnaire. The questionnaire items, we are
informed, measured ‘parental order and control of children, parental
supervision of daily activities of children, parental involvement in proper
control and supervision of school tasks, parental time spent on children’s
school work, and parental communication with their children’ (Cherian
& Malehase, 2000: 666). The actual items of the questionnaire are not
provided, so we are not able to judge exactly what questions elicited
responses in these various areas. Nevertheless, there is no indication that
the choice of words such as ‘proper control and supervision’, ‘parental
order’, ‘parental involvement’ is political, and implies the valuing of
particular parental activities over others. Instead, these practices take on
the aspect of naturalness—correct and good parental actions. This point
is taken up by Rose (1989), who points out that scientific and professional
descriptions of good parental practices gain their power by appearing to
be universally valid and natural.
628 Developmental Psychology
Developmental-contextual perspectives
Developmental-contextual approaches take an overtly ‘both/and’
perspective. In other words, researchers attempt to include both individual
and social factors into their theorising, rather than one or the other. For
example, in his discussion on special educational needs, Donald (1994:
151) calls for ‘ecologically sensitive research that clarifies the interactional
relationship between various disabilities and their socially and structurally
determined contexts’.
Stead (1996) utilises a developmental-contextual model to analyse
career development in black adolescents in South Africa. This perspective
emphasises the dynamic interaction between an individual and proximal
(family and peers, for example) and distal (economic and sociocultural, for
example) contexts. He discusses career development in black adolescents in
relation to education, the family, the economy, culture (see later discussion
under cultural perspectives) and identity development. Stead (1996: 272)
states that the developmental-contextual approach ‘overcomes (a) an
inordinate focus on either the individual or the individual’s environment
and (b) an emphasis on a unidirectional relationship between the self and
context’. However, his analysis does not make it clear how this is done. Indeed,
his discussion is limited to an exploration of either the environment (the
family, or education, for example) or the individual (identity development)
without theorising exactly how one relates to the other. Merely saying that
individual development is influenced by context is insufficient. This point
is important as theorising exactly what the inter-relationship is between the
individual and society is one of the crucial aspects of a contextual approach.
It is on this level that we may start differentiating between adequate and
inadequate contextual (and other) analyses of development.
This point is clearly illustrated in two papers about related issues—
child neglect and child abuse and their respective effects in terms of
child development. In the first, Du Preez, Naudé & Pretorius (2004)
research the influence of neglect on language development. They found
that the neglected children in their sample had delays in terms of verbal
development. They postulate that this is owing to a lack of interaction
and communication between the parents or caregivers and children.
However, exactly how this external event leads to the internal one is left
untheorised. What are the cognitive, emotional, social and neurological
processes involved in moving from the one to the other?
This kind of careful theorising is evident in a paper by Panzer &
Viljoen (2004). They argue that child neglect and abuse leaves children
with an experience of ‘fright without solution’ (Panzer & Viljoen, 2004:
11). The child’s efforts to elicit help are met with abuse and, in order to
cope, disassociation of explicit from implicit processing is required. The
detail of their theorising, which draws on psychoneurology, is relatively
complex. Importantly in this context, however, they engage in careful
analysis of how an external event such as abuse may be detrimental to the
neural networks of individuals.
Another important aspect in contextual theorising is the ack-
nowledgement of historical effects. Finchilescu and Dawes’ (1998) paper
on South African teenagers’ socio-political orientations following the
T h e o r y a n d S o u t h A f r i c a n d e v e l o p m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y r e s e a rc h a n d l i t e r a t u r e 629
rapid social change of the early to mid-1990s is an excellent example of the Cohort. A group of
importance of taking cohort effects into account. Their work was partially people that possess
common characteristics,
informed by a generational approach to the study of the influence of political
in particular being born in
context on human development. From a generational perspective, particular a particular year.
age brackets will share a political consciousness, which is shaped by exposure
to particular events occurring during the sensitive developmental period
of youth. Thus, Finchilescu & Dawes (1998) talk of the Resistance cohort,
the Negotiation cohort and the Democracy cohort, referring to people
who entered adolescence prior to 1990, during the time of the political
negotiations, and after the elections of 1994, respectively. Each of these
generational cohorts contains sub-generational units based on the racial
groups defined by apartheid. This approach clearly links contextual issues
to historical effects, effects that are obviously more clearly seen in times
of rapid change, but which should always feature in our thinking about
developmental psychology.
Cultural approaches
That there are diverse cultures in South Africa is probably a truism that few
people would dispute. This is reflected in developmental psychology research
where culture features relatively strongly. However, the way in which
culture is conceptualised and the uses to which it is put differs markedly. In Cross-cultural research.
the first place, there is cross-cultural research in which culture is viewed as An experimental method
in which different cultures
a variable that can be separated from other variables, and that can be used
are evaluated and
in explanations of observed differences or similarities. Secondly, ‘culture’ compared on different
is used as a broadly defining, static and over-arching feature of human cultural dimensions.
existence. Thirdly, there are those studies that locate themselves within
the cultural psychology tradition. Cultural psychology utilises a dynamic
conceptualisation of culture as social practices and traditions that permeate,
transform and regulate human behaviour. Cultural psychology thus studies
the meaning of the cultural worlds we inhabit, their historicity and the
interpersonal maintenance of the practices on which they are premised.
Examples of cross-cultural research are the papers by Akande (1999),
and Meyer, Eilertsen, Sundet & Sagvolden (2004). Akande (1999: 171)
conducted a ‘cross-cultural assessment of self-esteem among youth in the
twenty-first century South Africa’. He hypothesised that perceptions of
the self differs from one cultural context to another. He thus compared
the means obtained on the Self-Description-Questionnaire-1 by South Mean. A measure of
African, Australian, Kenyan, Nigerian, Nepalese and Zimbabwean chil- central tendency that
dren. Meyer et al (2004) investigated whether the Disruptive Behaviour refers to the average,
or the sum of the scores
Disorders rating scale measures the same constructs in South African as in
divided by the number
Western cultures. They administered the scale to South African children of scores.
and compared the results with those found for children in the United
States of America and Europe.
In both of these studies culture is thus seen as a variable. An instrument
to measure some characteristic or trait is administered and the results of the
various ‘cultures’ under scrutiny are compared. The difficulty, even from
a positivist perspective, is to separate out culture as a variable from other
variables such as socio-economic status, household size, location (urban or
rural, for example), and means of subsistence.
630 Developmental Psychology
The second sense in which culture is used (that is, as a static, essentialist
feature of human existence) is evidenced in Stead (1996), who discusses
career development in black adolescents (see earlier discussion). In this
paper, Stead seems to see culture as a possession, something that defines
groups of people in definitive sense. Note the following passage:
Thus, ‘whites’ equal ‘western’ and ‘blacks’ equal ‘traditional’, with all
the attendant characteristics. The use of the qualifying words ‘generally’
and ‘tend’ does not detract from this equation, as there is no indication
that any of these cultural characteristics is dynamic and fluid.
It is this static and essentialist view of culture that cultural psychology
has attempted to counter, while still maintaining the explanatory power
that may be gained by considering cultural issues in developmental
psychology. Gilbert, Van Vlaenderen & Nkwinti (1995: 229), for example,
locate their research within cultural psychology. They study the role
of local knowledge in the process of socialisation in rural families.
They define local knowledge as ‘the presuppositions used to interpret
immediate experience borne out of action in the local environment’. This
conceptualisation illustrates the dynamic nature of a cultural psychology
perspective, in that local knowledge is a product of day-to-day actions and
hence is constantly being constructed and re-constructed while still having
a historicity. Contrast this to the notion of ‘traditional knowledge’, which
has the connotation of stasis and preservation.
This theorising of socialisation as a dynamic process allows for a
dialectical understanding of the active versus passive debate. Utilising
a combination of the Vygotskian zone of proximal development (the space
between what children can do on their own and what they can do with the
help of a more knowledgeable member of the culture) and Geertz’s view
of culture as a set of control mechanisms used for governing behaviour,
Gilbert, Van Vlaenderen & Nkwinti (1995) indicate that socialisation is
both a conservative and a creative process. In other words, it simultaneously
structures children’s lives so that they internalise the available cultural
rules and instructions and allows the space for children to construct their
own meaning.
It is when ‘culture’ is invoked that issues of universality and relativism
tend to be raised. Some authors attempt to tread the middle ground,
indicating the relative importance of both universalism and relativism.
For example, Magwaza (1997), in her discussion of child sexual abuse,
attempts to integrate a cultural relativist perspective (that is, recognising
and theorising about cultural differences in the understanding and
practices of sexuality) while at the same time maintaining moral
T h e o r y a n d S o u t h A f r i c a n d e v e l o p m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y r e s e a rc h a n d l i t e r a t u r e 631
Sociopolitical issues
The status of developmental psychology in Africa and South Africa has
been a disputed matter. Some time ago Liddell & Kvalsig (1990) asked
whether, firstly, developmental psychology can claim to be neutral in the
research that it conducts and, secondly, whether our research has, indeed,
no impact in the real world. They argued that reseach conducted by South
African developmental psychology has been used and, at times, abused
in the real world. Later, Nsamenang & Dawes (1998) argued that Africa
has had a number of alien influences imposed upon it, and that scholars
in developmental psychology should engage in a ‘liberatory project’ in
634 Developmental Psychology
Conclusion
From the above it is clear that a range of theoretical approaches and
models have informed developmental psychology research in South
Africa. Two important questions are: Why is it important to take stock of
our theoretical orientations? Why is it vital that a forum for the discussion
of theory in developmental psychology remains open when there are
clearly pressing issues facing children and adolescents (as well as parents,
adults and the elderly) in South Africa? I hope that the answers to these
T h e o r y a n d S o u t h A f r i c a n d e v e l o p m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y r e s e a rc h a n d l i t e r a t u r e 637
Specific tasks
C R I T I C A L T H I N K I N G TA S K S
➋ Do you think that South African developmental psychologists should take an overtly
political stance in their work, or do you think that there is a place for scientific
neutrality and objectivity? Is there a middle ground? Do you think there is a way of
integrating the two stances, and if so, how?
➌ Do you think that ‘culture’ should be included in our thinking about developmental
psychology? If so, how should it be conceptualised?
Acknowledgements
Thank you to Tracy Morison for invaluable assistance in gathering the literature for this chapter
and for organising the references.
Recommended readings
There are four excellent South African books of relevance to developmental psychology.
The first two provide reviews of research done in a variety of areas relating to developmental psychology,
the third addresses interventions with children, and the fourth takes a public health approach to child
development. The Human Sciences Research Council has a very active Child, Youth, Family and
Social Development Unit. Many useful publications, mostly with free downloads, are available from this
unit. The approach is generally a public health one.
Dawes, A & Donald, D (eds) (1994). Childhood and adversity: psychological perspectives from South
African research, 177-199. Cape Town: David Philip.
De la Rey, C, Duncan, N, Shefer, T & Van Niekerk, A (eds) (1997). Contemporary issues in human
development: a South African focus, 7-24. Johannesburg: International Thomson Publishing.
Donald, D, Dawes, A & Louw, J (eds) (2000). Addressing childhood adversity. Cape Town:
David Philip.
Dawes, A, Bray, R & van der Merwe, A (2007). Monitoring child well-being: A South African
rights-based approach. Cape Town: HSRC Press.
References
A
Aase, H, Meyer, A & Sagvolden, T (2006). Moment-to-moment dynamics of ADHD behaviour in South
African children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2(11). http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/
content/2/1/11 [25 January 2008]
Abraham, K (1927). The influence of oral eroticism on character formation. (In K Abraham (ed), Selected
papers on psychoanalysis. London: Hogarth Press.)
Ackerman, C J (1990). Identiteitsontwikkeling tydens adolessensie: ‘n groep sekondêre skool-leerlinge se
evaluering van die belangrikheid van lewensterreine. South African Journal of Education, 10(4), 283-290.
Addis, D R, Moscovitch, M, Crawley, A P & McAndrews, M P (2004). Recollective qualities modulate
hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus, 14, 752-762.
Adler, G (1967). Methods of treatment in analytic psychology. (In B Wolman (ed), Psychoanalytic
technique. New York: Basic Books.)
Ahmed, R, Seedat, M, van Niekerk, A & Bulbulia, S (2004). Discerning community resilience in
disadvantaged communities in the context of violence and injury prevention. South African Journal of
Psychology, 34(3), 386-408.
AIDS Foundation South Africa (2008). HIV/AIDS in South Africa. Available: http://www.aids.org.za/hiv.
htm. [14 August 2008]
Ainsworth, M D S (1972). Attachment and dependency: A comparison. (In J L Gerwitz (ed), Attachment
and dependency.Washington, DC: V H Winston.)
Ainsworth, M D S, Blehar, M C, Waters, E & Wall, S (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study
of the strange situation. New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Akande, A (1999). Intercultural and cross-cultural assessment of self-esteem among youth in twenty-first
century South Africa. International Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 21(3), 171-187.
Akhtar, N, Jipson, J & Callanan, M A (2001). Learning words through overhearing.
Child Development, 72, 416-430.
Althusser, L (1971). Lenin and philosophy and other essays. London: New Left Books.
Amabile, T (1993). Commentary. (In D Goleman, P Kaufman & M Ray, (eds), The creative spirit. New
York: Plume.)
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
4th ed.Text Revision. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Amod, Z, Cockcroft, K & Soellaart, B (2007). Use of the 1996 Griffiths Mental Development Scales for
infants: a pilot study with a Black, South African sample. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health,
19(2), 123-130.
Angless, T & Shefer, T (1997). Children living with violence in the family. (In C de la Rey, N Duncan,
T Shefer & A van Niekerk, Contemporary issues in human development: A South African focus.
Johannesburg: Thomson.)
Anglin, J M (1995). Classifying the world through language: Functional relevance, cultural significance,
and category name learning. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 19, 161-181.
Appelgryn, A E M & Nieuwoudt, J M (1988). Relative deprivation and the ethnic attitudes of Blacks and
Afrikaans-speaking Whites in South Africa. The Journal of Social Psychology, 128(3), 311-323.
Appelgryn, A E M (1987). Relatiewe deprivasie en militantheid in die Suid-Afrikaanse konteks.
Suid-Afrikaanse Tydskrif vir Sosiologie, 18(3), 95-101.
Appignansi, R & Zarate, O (1992). Freud for beginners. London: Icon.
Archer, J (1980). The significance of sex differences in developmental psychology. (In J Saints (ed),
Developmental psychology and society. London: Macmillan.)
Aries, P (1962/1999). The discovery of childhood. (In J W Santrock (ed), Lifespan development. Boston:
McGraw-Hill.)
640 Developmental Psychology
B
Babatunde, E D (1992). A critical study of Bini and Yoruba value systems of Nigeria in change: Culture
religion and self. Lewiston, NY: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Bacal, H (1985). Optimal responsiveness and the therapeutic process. (In A Goldberg (ed), Progress in
Self Psychology, Volume 1. New York: Guilford Press, 202-227.)
Bacal, H (1995). The essence of Kohut’s work and the progress of Self psychology. Psychoanalytic
Dialogues, 5, 353-366.
Bacal, H & Newman, K (1990). Theories of Object Relations: Bridges to Self Psychology. New York:
Columbia University Press.
Baddeley, A (1982). Your memory: A user’s guide. London: Multimedia Publications.
Baillargeon, R (1987). Object permanence in three-and-a-half and four-and-a-half-month-old infants.
Developmental Psychology, 23 (5), 655-674.
Baillargeon, R & DeVos, J (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child
Development, 62, 1227-1246.
Barbarin, O A & Richter, L (2001). Economic status, community danger and psychological problems
among South African children. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 8(1), 115-133.
Baron-Cohen, S (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baron-Cohen, S (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of autism. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
Psychiatry, 75, 945-948.
Baron-Cohen, S (2008). Autism, hypersystemizing, and truth. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 61, 64-75.
Baron-Cohen, S & Swettenham, J (1997). Theory of mind in autism: its relationship to executive function
and central coherence. (In D Cohen & F Volkmar (eds), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental
disorders. 880-893. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley.)
Baron-Cohen, S, Jollife, T, Mortimore, C & Robertson, M (1997). Another advanced test of theory of
mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism of Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813-822.
Baron-Cohen, S, Knickmeyer, R C & Belmonte, M K (2005). Sex differences in the brain: Implications for
explaining autism. Science, 310, 819-823.
Baron-Cohen, S, Leslie, A & Frith, U (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition,
21, 37-46.
Baron-Cohen, S, O’Riordan, M, Stone, V, Jones, R & Plaisted, K (1999). Recognition of faux pas by
normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407-418.
References 641
Barrett, M D (1979). Semantic development during the single-word stage of language acquisition. Hove:
University of Sussex. Unpublished doctoral thesis.
Bartholomew, K & Horowitz, L M (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-
category model. Journal of personality and social psychology, 61, 226–244.
Bartlett, F (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bartollas, C & Dinitz, S (1989). Introduction to criminology: Order and disorder. New York: Harper & Row.
Bartsch, K & Wellman, H M (1995). Children talk about the mind. London: Oxford University Press.
Basch, M (1983). Empathic understanding: A review of the concept and some theoretical considerations.
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 31, 101-126. Oxford: W.H. Freeman.
Bayley, N (1993). Bayley scales of infant development. 2nd ed. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
Bee, H (1992). The developing child. 6th ed. New York: Harper Collins.
Beebe, B & Lachmann, F (1988). Mother-infant mutual influence and precursors of psychic structure. (In
A Goldberg (ed), Progress in Self Psychology, Vol 3. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press, 3-26.)
Beebe, B & Lachmann, F (1994). Representation and internalization in infancy: Three principles of
salience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11, 127-165.
Beebe, B, Jaffe, J & Lachmann, F (2002). Infant Research and Adult Treatment: Co-Constructing Interactions.
Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Beebe, B, Knoblauch, S, Rustin, J & Sorter, D (2003). Introduction: A Systems View. Psychoanalytic
Dialogues, 13, 743-775.
Bell, S & Ainsworth, M (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171-1190.
Bem, S L (1993). The lenses of gender. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Benedek, E P (1985). Children and psychic trauma: A review of contemporary thinking. (In S Eth &
R S Pynoos, RS (eds), Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder in Children. Washington: American Psychiatric
Press. 1-16.)
Benenson, J F, Apostoleris, N H & Parnass, J (1997). Age and sex differences in dyadic and group
interaction. Developmental Psychology, 33 (3), 538–543.
Benjamin, J (1995). Like Subjects, Love Objects. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Benjamin, J (1998). Shadow of the Other. New York: Routledge.
Benvenuto, B & Kennedy, R (1986). The works of Jacques Lacan: An introduction. London: Free
Association Press.
Berger, K S (1994). The developing person through the lifespan. New York: Worth Publishers.
Bergman, L (1992). Dating violence among high school students. Social Work, 37(1), 21-27.
Berk, L E (1986). Private speech: Learning out aloud. Psychology Today, 20 (5), 34-42.
Berko, J (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150-177.
Bertenthal, B I, Proffitt, D R & Cutting, J E (1984). Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in
biomechanical motions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 213-230.
Best, D L, House, A S, Barnard, A E & Spicker, B S (1994). Parent-child interactions in France, Germany
and Italy: the effects of gender and culture. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 25 (2): 181-193.
Best, S & Kellner, D (1991). Postmodern theory: Critical interrogations. Hong Kong: Macmillan.
Bibby, H & McDonald, S (2005). Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 43, 99-114.
Biesheuvel, S (1987). Psychology, science and politics: Theoretical developments and applications in a
plural society. South African Journal of Psychology, 17, 1-8.
Biller, H B (1993). Fathers and families: Paternal factors in child development.Westport: Auburn House.
Bion, W (1962). Learning from experience. New York: Basic Books.
Bion, W (1967). Second thoughts. New York: Jason Aronson.
Bion, W R (1962). The psycho-analytic study of thinking. International Journal of Psychoanalysis,
306-310.
Bjorklund, D F (1995). Children’s thinking: Developmental function and individual differences. Pacific
Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Bjorklund, D F (2005). Children’s Thinking. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Blackburn, R (1993). The psychology of criminal conduct: Theory, research and practice. New York:
Wiley & Sons.
Blasi, A (1980). Bridging Moral Cognition and Moral Action: A Critical Review of the Literature.
Psychological Bulletin, 88(1), 1-45.
642 Developmental Psychology
Blatt, M M & Kohlberg, L (1975). The Effects of Classroom Moral Discussion upon Children’s
Level of Moral Judgement. Journal Of Moral Education, 4, 129-161.
Block, J H (1973). Conception of sex-roles: Some cross-cultural and longitudinal perspectives.
American Psychologist, 28, 512–516.
Bloom, L (1996). The emotional damage of apartheid: A psycho-analytic view. Psycho-analytic
Psychotherapy in South Africa, 4 (20), 55-71.
Blumstein, A (1995). Youth violence, guns and the illicit drug industry. Journal of Criminal Law
& Criminology, 86, 10-36.
Boas, F (1911). The mind of primitive man. New York: Macmillan.
Bogoiavlenski, DN & Menchinskaia, NA (1959). The Relation between Learning and Mental
Development in Schoolchildren. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds) (1963). Educational Psychology in the
U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 50-67.)
Bogoiavlenski, DN & Menchinskaia, NA (1960). The Psychology of Learning1900-1960. (In B Simon & J Simon
(eds) (1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 101-164.)
Bohbot, V D, Kalina, M, Stepankova, K, Spackova, N, Petrides, M & Nadel, L (1998). Spatial memory
deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex.
Neuropsychologia, 36, 1217-1238.
Bollas, C (1987). The Shadow of the Object. New York: Columbia University Press.
Bonn, M & Webley, P (2000). South African children’s understanding of money and banking. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18 (2), 269-278.
Boris, H N (1986). Bion re-visited. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 22(2), 159-184.
Bornman, E (1998). Group membership as determinant of violence and conflict: The case of South
Africa. (In E Bornman, R van Eeden & M Wentzel (eds), Violence in South Africa: A variety of
perspectives. Pretoria: HSRC. 85-116.)
Bornstein, M H & Sigman, M D (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child
Development, 57, 251-274.
Bourne, L E J & Felipe Russo, N (1998). Psychology: Behaviour in context. New York: Norton.
Bowie, M (1991). Lacan. London: Fontana.
Bowlby, J (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their characters and home life. International Journal of
Psycho-analysis, 25, 19-52 and 107-27.
Bowlby, J (1969). Attachment and loss.Volume 1: Attachment. London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby, J (1973). Attachment and loss Volume 2: Separation: Anxiety and anger. London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby, J (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavistock.
Bowlby, J (1980). Attachment and loss Volume 3: Loss, sadness and depression. London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby, J (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J (1989). The role of attachment in personality development and psychopathology. (In
S Greenspan & G Pollock (eds) The course of life. Volume 1: Infancy. Madison, CT: International
Universities Press.)
Bowman, B & Stevens, G (2004). Injury costing in South Africa: The state of the sector. (In S Suffla,
A van Niekerk & N Duncan (eds), Crime, violence and injury in South Africa: Developments and
challenges. Tygerberg: MRC-Unisa. 170-183.)
Bowman, B, Stevens, G Seedat, M & Snyman, R (forthcoming). Costing injures in South Africa:
Preliminary results and challenges from a pilot study. African Journal of Health Sciences.
Bozalek, V (1997). Representations of the family and South African realities. (In C De la Rey, N Duncan,
T Shefer & A Van Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African focus.
Johannesburg: International Thomson Publishing. 7-24.)
Bradshaw, D & Nannan, N (2004). Health status. (In P Ijumba, C Day & A Ntuli (eds), South African
health review 2003/2004. Durban: Health Systems Trust. 45-58.)
Braine, M D S (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure: The first phase. Language, 39, 1–13.
Brandschaft, B (1985). Resistance and defense: An intersubjective view. (In A Goldberg (ed), Progress in
Self Psychology, Vol 1. New York: Guilford Press, 88-96.)
Bray, R & Dawes, A (2007). Monitoring the well-being of children: historical and conceptual foundations.
(In A Dawes, R Bray & van der Merwe (eds) Monitoring child well-being: A Couth African rights-based
approach. Cape Town: HSRC Press. 5-28.)
References 643
Brazelton, T B, Koslowski, B & Tronick, E (1993). Neonatal behaviour among urban Zambians and
Americans. (In M Gauvain & M Cole (eds), Readings on the development of children. Oxford: W.H. Freeman.)
Bretherton, I (1997). Bowlby’s legacy to developmental psychology. Child psychiatry and human
development, 28 (1), 33-43.
Brody, N (1992). Intelligence. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U (1974). The two worlds of childhood. Middlesex: Penguin Education.
Bronfenbrenner, U (1977). Towards an experimental ecology of human development. American
Psychologist, 32(7), 513-531.
Bronfenbrenner, U (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brooke, R (1990). Jung and phenomenology. London: Routledge.
Broom, Y M & Doctor, E A (1994). Developmental dyslexia: contrasting patterns of performance on a
diagnostic psycholinguistic assessment. South African Journal of Psychology, 24(4), 219-228.
Brothers, D (1998). Exploring the “bi” ways of self-experience: Dissociation, alter-ego selfobject
experience, and gender. (In A Goldberg (ed), Progress in Self Psychology, vol. 14: The World of Self
Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.)
Brown, R (1965). Social psychology. New York: Free Press.
Brown, R (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brüne, M & Brüne-Cohrs, U (2006). Theory of mind—evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and
psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 437–455.
Bruner, J S (1987). Prologue to the English Edition. (In R W Rieber & A S Carton (eds) & N Minick
(translator). The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume One. Problems of General Psychology. New
York: Plenum Press.)
Bruner, J S (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brunner, H G, Nelen, M, Breakefield, X O, Ropers, H H & van Oost, B A (1993). Abnormal behaviour
associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase-A. Science,
262(5133): 578-580.
Buchanan, B G & Shortcliffe, E H (1984). Rule-based expert systems: The MYCIN experiments of the
Stanford Heuristic Programming Project. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Buirski, P & Haglund, P (2001). Making Sense Together: The Intersubjective Approach to Psychotherapy.
Lanham MD: Rowman and Littlefield.
Bukatko, D & Daehler, M (1995). Child Development. 2nd ed. Toronto: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Bulhan, H A (1981). Psychological research in Africa: Genesis and function. Race and class, 23 (1), 25-40.
Bulhan, H A (1980a). Dynamics of cultural in-betweenity: An empirical study. International Journal of
Psychology, 15, 105-121.
Bulhan, H A (1980b). Franz Fanon: the revolutionary psychiatrist. Race and class, 21 (3), 251-271.
Bulhan, H A (1981). Psychological research in Africa: Genesis and function. Race and class, 23(1), 25-40.
Bulhan, H A (1985). Frantz Fanon and the psychology of oppression. New York: Plenum Press.
Bulmer, M & Solomos, J (2000). Racism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bunting, A (2005). Stages of development: marriage of girls and teens as an international human rights
issue. Social and Legal Studies, 14(2), 17-38.
Burman, E (1994). Deconstructing developmental psychology. London: Routledge.
Burman, E (2008). Developments: Child, image, nation. London: Routledge.
Burr, V (1995). An introduction to social constructionism. London: Routledge.
Buss, A H & Plomin, R (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Butchart, A (2000). On the costs and costing of violence and injuries due to violence. ISHS Monograph
Series, 1(4), 130-163.
Butchart, A & Kruger, J (2001). Public health and community psychology: a case study in community-
based injury prevention. (In M Seedat (ed), Community psychology theory, method and practice: South
African and other perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 215-242.)
Butchart, A, Terre Blanche, M, Hamber, B & Seedat, M (2000). Violence and violence prevention in South
Africa: A sociological and historical perspective. (In T Emmett & A Butchart (eds), Behind the mask.
Pretoria: HSRC. 29-54.)
Butterworth, G, & Jarrett, N (1991). What minds have in common is space—spatial mechanisms serving
joint visual-attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55-72.
644 Developmental Psychology
C
Calam, R & Franchi, C (1987). Child abuse and its consequences: Observational approaches. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Campbell, J (1992). “If I can’t have you no one can”: Power and control in homicide. (In J Radford
& D Russell (eds), Femicide: The politics of woman killing. New York: Twayne Publishers.
99-113.)
Carlson, E A & Sroufe, L A (1995). Contribution of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology.
(In D Cicchetti & D J Cohen (eds), Developmental psychopathology. New York: John Wiley.)
Caron, A J, Caron, R F & MacLean, D J (1988). Infant discrimination of naturalistic emotional
expressions: the role of face and voice. Child Development, 59, 604-616.
Caron, A J, Caron, R, Roberts, J & Brooks, R (1997). Infant sensitivity to deviations in dynamic facial–
vocal displays: the role of eye regard. Developmental Psychology, 33, 802-813.
Carpenter, P A, Just, M A & Shell, P (1990). What One Intelligence Test Measures: A Theoretical
Account of the Processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97 (3), 404-431.
Cartwright, D (2001). The role of psychopathology and personality in rage-type homicide: A review.
South African Journal of Psychology, 31(3), 12-19.
Carver, C S & Scheier, M F (1988). Perspectives on personality. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Caspi, A & Silva, P A (1995). Temperamental qualities at age three predict personality traits in young
adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66, 486-498.
Caspi, A, McClay, J, Moffitt, T, Mill, J, Martin, J, Craig, I, Taylor, A & Poulton, R (2002). Role of
genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851-854.
Cassidy, J (1999). The nature of the child’s ties. (In J Cassidy & P R Shaver (eds), Handbook of attachment:
Theory, research and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press.)
Cassidy, J & Kobak, R (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. (In J Belsky &
T Nezworski (eds), Clinical implications of attachment. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Castel, A D, & Craik, F I M (2003). The effects of aging and divided attention on memory for item and
associative information. Psychology and Aging, 18, 873-885.
Castles, S O & Vasta, E (1999). Multi-cultural or multi-racist Australia? (In M Bulmer & J Solomos (eds),
Racism. Oxford University Press.)
Ceci, S J (1990). On intelligence, more or less. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Charman, T, Baron-Cohen, S, Swettenham, J, Baird, G, Cox, A & Drew, A (2000). Testing joint
attention, imitation and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive
Development, 15, 481-498.
Chee, M W L, Goh, J O S, Venkatraman, V, Tan, J C, Gutchess, A, Sutton, B, et al. (2006). Age-related
changes in object processing and contextual binding revealed using fMRI adaptation. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 495-507.
Chen, M J (1994). Chinese and Australian concepts of intelligence. Psychology and Developing Societies,
6 (2), 103-117.
Cherian, V I & Cherian, L (1997). Relationship between parents’ interest, life status and
the academic achievement of Xhosa children in South Africa. Journal of Psychology in Africa,
2, 54–66.
Cherian, V I & Malehase, M C (2000). The relationship between parental control and scholastic
achievement of children from single- and two-parent families. The Journal of Social Psychology, 140(5),
665-667.
Chi, M T H & Koeske, R D (1983). Network representation of a child’s dinosaur knowledge.
Developmental Psychology, 19, 29-39.
ChildrenFirst (2004). South Africa’s child abuse statistics: True or false? [Online]. Available URL: http://
www.childrenfirst.org.za/shownews?mode=content&id=22787&refto=2269. [ 29 April 2008]
Chodorow, N (1978). The reproduction of mothering: Psychoanalysis and the sociology of gender. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Chomsky, N (1959). Review of Verbal behaviour by B F Skinner. Language, 35, 26-58.
References 645
Churchland, P M (1995). The Engine of Reason, the Seat of the Soul: A Philosophical Journey into the
Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Claassen, N, Krynauw, A H, Paterson, H & Mathe, M (2001). A Standardisation of the WAIS-III for
English-speaking South Africans. Pretoria: Human Science Research Council.
Clacherty, G & Donald, D (2007). Child participation in research: reflections on ethical challenges in
the southern African context. African Journal of AIDS Research, 6(2), 147-156.
Cloninger, S C (1996). Theories of personality. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Cluver, L & Gardner, F (2006). The psychological well-being of children orphaned by AIDS in Cape
Town, South Africa. Annals of General Psychiatry, 5, 8-19.
Cock, J & Nathan, L (eds) (1989). War and society. The militarisation of South Africa. Cape Town: David Philip.
Cock, J (2001). Gun violence and masculinity in contemporary South Africa. (In R Morrell (ed), Changing
men in Southern Africa. Pietermaritzburg: University of Natal Press. 43-55.)
Cockcroft, K, Amod, Z & Soellaart, B (2008). Level of maternal education and performance of Black,
South African infants on the 1996 Griffiths Mental Development Scales. African Journal of Psychiatry,
11, 44-50.
Cockcroft, K A S (2004). Ready for Reading: Concurrent and Longitudinal Predictors of Reading in English.
Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand. Unpublished doctoral thesis.
Cocks, G (2002). Heinz Kohut: The Making of a Psychoanalyst. J. American Psychoanaytic Association,
50, 1385-1390.
Cohen, G, Kiss, G & LeVoi, M (1993). Memory: Current Issues. Buckingham: Open University Press, 67.
Cohen, S & Taylor, L (1972). Psychological survival: The experience of long-term imprisonment.
Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Colby, A & Kohlberg, L (1987). The Measurment of Moral Judgment. Volume 1. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Colby, A, Kohlberg, L, Speicer, B, Hewer, A, Candee, D, Gibbs, J & Power, C (1987). The measurement of
moral judgment: Standard issue scoring manual. Volume 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cole, M & Wertsch, J V (1996). Beyond the Individual-Social Antimony in Discussions of Piaget and Vygotsky.
Human Development, Prometheus.org.uk.
Cole, M (1993). Mind as a cultural achievement: Implicationas for IQ testing. (In M Gauvain & M Cole
(eds), Readings on the development of children. Oxford: W.H. Freeman.)
Cole, M (2001). Remembering History in Sociocultural Research. Human Development, 44, 166-169.
Cole, M (2005a). Cross-Cultural and Historical Perspectives on the Developmental Consequences of
Education. Human Development, 48, 195-216.
Cole, M (2005b). Methodological Challenges and Continuing Social Concerns. Human Development,
48, 227-231.
Collins, A M & Loftus, E (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing.
Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.
Conger, J J & Miller, W C (1966). Personality, social class, and delinquency. New York: Wiley.
Connell, R W (1995). Masculinities. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Connell, R W (2000). The men and the boys. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Connell, R W (2002). Gender. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Conway, M A (1996). Autobiographical knowledge and autobiographical memories. (In D C Rubin (ed),
Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.
67-93.)
Conway, M A, Pleydell-Pearce, C W & Whitecross, S E (2001). The neuroanatomy of autobiographical
memory: A slow cortical potential study of autobiographical memory retrieval. Journal of Memory and
Language, 45, 493-524.
Cook, P & Du Toit, L (2005). Overcoming adversity with children affected by HIV/AIDS in the
indigenous South African cultural context. (In M Ungar (ed) Handbook for Working with Children and
Youth: Pathways to Resilience across Cultures and Contexts. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 247-
262.)
Cooper, S (1996). Facts all come with a point of view: Some reflections on fact and formulation from the
75th anniversary edition of the International Journal of Psychoanalysis. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 77, 255-271.
646 Developmental Psychology
Coppock, V, Haydon, D & Richter, I (1995). The illusions of ‘post-feminism’: New women, old myths.
London: Taylor and Francis.
Craig, G (1989). Human development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Craig, G J (1996). Human development. 7th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Crain, W (1992). Theories of Development: Concepts and Applications. 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Crittenden, P M (1985). Social networks, quality of child rearing, and child development. Child
Development, 56, 1299-1313.
Crockenberg, S B (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social support influences on the
security of infant-mother attachment. Child development, 52, 857-865.
Crossley, M L (2000). Introducing narrative psychology: Self, trauma and the construction of meaning.
Buckingham: Open University Press.
Curtiss, S, Fromkin, V, Krashen, S, Rigler, D & Rigler, M (1974). The linguistic development of genie.
Language, 50, 528-554.
D
Dahlberg, L L & Krug, E G (2002). Violence – A global public health problem. (In E G Krug, L L Dahlberg,
J A Mercy, AB Zwi & R Lozano (eds), World report on violence and health. Geneva: WHO. 1-22.)
Dale, M, De’ath, R, Evans, T, Thompson, Q, Georgi, G & Spencer, P (1998). The human body. London:
BBC Films in association with the Learning Channel.
Daly, M & Wilson, M (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine De Gruyter.
Daniels, H (1996). Introduction: Psychology in a social world. (In H Daniels (ed), An Introduction to
Vygotsky. London: Routledge.)
Daniels, H (2001). Vygotsky and Pedagogy. London: Routledge.
Das, J P, Naglieri, J A & Kirby, J P (1994). Assessment of cognitive processes. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Davydov, V V & Radzikhovskii, L A (1985). The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. (In J V Wertsch
(ed), Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.)
Davydov, V V (1988). Problems of developmental teaching: The experience of theoretical and
experimental psychological research. Soviet Education, 30, 8-10.
Dawes, A & Donald, D (eds) (1994). Childhood and adversity. Cape Town: David Philip.
Dawes, A (1986). The notion of relevant psychology with particular reference to Africanist pragmatic
initiatives. Psychology in Society, 15, 55-61.
Dawes, A (1994a). The emotional impact of political violence. (In A Dawes & D Donald (eds), Childhood
and adversity. Cape Town: David Phillip. 177-199.)
Dawes, A (1994b). The effects of political violence on socio-moral reasoning and conduct. (In A Dawes
& D Donald (eds), Childhood and adversity. Cape Town: David Philip. 200-219.)
Dawes, A & Donald, D (1994). Understanding the psychological consequences of adversity. (In A Dawes
& D Donald (eds), Childhood and adversity: psychological perspectives from South African research. Cape
Town: David Philip. 1-27.)
Dawes, A, Bray, R & van der Merwe, A (eds) (2007). Monitoring child well-being: A South African rights-
based approach. Cape Town: HSRC Press.
de Haan, M, Wyatt, J S, Roth, S, Vargha-Khadem, F, Gadian, D & Mishkin, M (2006). Brain and
cognitive-behavioural development after asphyxia at term birth. Developmental Science, 9, 350-358.
De la Rey, C, Duncan, N, Shefer, T & Van Niekerk, A (eds) (1997). Contemporary issues in human
development: A South African focus. Johannesburg: Thomson.
de Waal, F B M (1982). Chimpanzee politics. London: Cape.
Deaux, K (1987). Psychological constructions of masculinity and femininity. (In J M Reinisch,
L A Rosenblum & S A Sanders (eds), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives. New York: Oxford
University Press.)
Deaux, K & Kite, M E (1987). Thinking about gender. (In B B Hess & M M Ferree (eds), Analysing gender:
A handbook of social science research. Newbury Park: Sage.)
DeLoache, J S (2006). Mindful of symbols. Scientific American Mind,Feb/March, 71-75.
Dennet, D (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 568-570.
Dennis, C (2004). The most important sexual organ. Nature, 427(6973), 390-392.
Department of Correctional Services. (2002). Personal communication on departmental statistic, July 2002.
References 647
Duncan, N & Rock, B (1997). The impact of political violence on the lives of children in South Africa.
(In C de la Rey, N Duncan, T Shefer & A van Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development:
A South African focus. Johannesburg: Thomson.)
Duncan, N, Stevens, G & Bowman, B (2004). South African psychology and racism. (In D Hook,
N Mkhize, P Kiguwa, A Collins, E Burman & I Parker (eds), Critical psychology. Lansdowne:
UCT Press. 360-388.)
Dutton, D G (2000). Witnessing parental violence as a traumatic experience shaping the abusive
personality. (In R A Geffner, P G Jaffe & M Sudermann (eds), Children exposed to domestic violence:
Current issues in research, intervention, prevention and policy development. Binghamton: Haworth
Maltreatment & Trauma Press. 59-68.)
E
Eagle, G & Michelson, C (1997). Post-traumatic stress in children: Presentation and intervention guidelines.
(In B Rock (ed), Spirals of suffering: Public violence and children. Pretoria: HSRC Publishing. 227-254.)
Eagle, G & Watts, J (2002). When objects attack in reality: psychodynamic contributions to formulations
of the impact and treatment of traumatic stress incidences. Parts 1 & 2, Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy in
South Africa, 10 (1/2), 1-24 and 1-10.
Edelbrock, C & Sugwara, A I (1978). Acquisition of sex-typed preferences in pre-school aged children’.
Developmental Psychology, 14 (6), 614-623.
Egeland, B, Kalkoske, M, Gottesman, N & Erickson, M F (1990). Preschool behaviour problems: Stability
and factors accounting for change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
31(6), 891-909.
Eimas, P D, Siqueland, E R, Jusczyk, P & Vigorito, J (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171,
303-306.
Ekstrom, A D, Kahana, M, Caplan, J B, Fields, T A, Isham, E A, Newman, E L et al. (2003). Cellular
networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature, 425, 184-187.
Elkind, D & Bowen, R (1979). Imaginary audience behaviour in children and adolescents. Developmental
Psychology, 15, 38-44.
Elkind, D (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 1025-1033.
Elkonin, D B (1959).The Psychology of mastering the Elements of Reading. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds)
(1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 165-179.)
Emmett, T & Butchart, A (eds). (2000). Behind the mask. Pretoria: HSRC.
Emmett, T (2003). Social disorganisation, social capital and violence prevention in South Africa. African
Safety Promotion, 1(2), 4-18.
Emmett, T (2003). Social disorganization, social capital and violence prevention in South Africa. African
Safety Promotion: A Journal of Injury and Violence Prevention, 1(2), 4-18.
Engestrom, Y (1996). Non scolae sed vitae discimus: towards avoiding encapsulation in school learning.
(In H Daniels (ed). An Introduction to Vygotsky. London: Routledge. 151-170.)
Ensink, K, Robertson, B, Zissis, C & Leger, P (1997). Post-traumatic stress disorder in children exposed to
violence. South African Medical Journal, 87(11), 1526-1529.
Epstein, C F (1988). Deceptive distinctions: Sex, gender and the social order. New Haven: Yale University
Press.
Erdelyi, M H & Goldberg, B (1979). Let’s not sweep repression under the rug: Toward a cognitive
psychology of repression. (In J F Kihlstrom & F J Evans (eds), Functional disorders of memory. New
Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Erickson, M F, Sroufe, L A & Egeland, B (1985). The relationship between quality of attachment and
behaviour problems in preschool in a high-risk sample. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 50 (1-2), 147-166.
Erikson, E (1950). Childhood and society. London: Imago Publishing.
Erikson, E (1959/1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Erikson, E H (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H (1964). Insight and responsibility. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H (1968). Identity, youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H (1969). Gandhi’s truth. New York: Norton.
References 649
Eron, L D, Huesmann, L R & Zelli, A (1991). The role of parental variables in the learning of aggression.
(In D J Pepler & K J Rubin (eds), The development and treatment of childhood aggression. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum. 169-188.)
Evans, D (1996). An introductory dictionary of Lacanian psychoanalysis. London: Routledge.
Eysenck, H J (1953). Uses and abuses of psychology. London: Penguin.
Eysenck, H J (1977). Crime and personality. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Eysenck, H J & Gudjonsson, G H (1989). The causes and cures of criminality. New York: Plenum Press.
Eysenck, M W & Keane M T (1990). Cognitive psychology: A student’s handbook. Hove: Erlbaum.
F
Fagot, B (1977). Consequences of moderate cross-gender behaviour in pre-school children. Child
Development, 48, 902-907.
Fajnzylber, P, Lederman, D & Loayza, N (1999). Inequality and violent crime. Washington, DC: World Bank.
Fantz, R L, Fagan, J P, & Miranda, S B (1975). Early visual selectivity. (In L B Cohen
& P Salapatek (eds), Infant perception: From sensation to cognition. New York: Academic Press.)
Farrington, D P (1991). Childhood aggression and adult violence: Early precursors and later-life
outcomes. (In D J Pepler & K H Rubin (eds), The development and treatment of childhood aggression.
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 5-29.)
Farrington, D P (1997). The relationship between low resting heart rate and violence. (In A Raine,
P A Brennan, D P Farrington & S A Mednick (eds), Biosocial bases of violence. New York: Plenum
Press. 89-105.)
Farrington, D P (1998). Predictors, causes, and correlates of male youth violence. (In M Tonry &
M H Moore (eds), Youth violence. Chicago: University of Chicago. 421-474.)
Fausto-Sterling, A (2001) Beyond Difference: Feminism and evolutionary psychology. (In H Rose
& S Rose (2001). Alas poor Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary pscyhology. London: Vintage.)
Feinman, S (1992). Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy. New York: Plenum.
Fenichel, O (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New York: Norton.
Ferns, I & Thom, D (2001). Moral development of black and white South African adolescents: evidence
against cultural universality in Kohlberg’s theory. South African Journal of Psychology, 31(4), 38-47.
Feuerstein, R, Rand, Y & Hoffman, M B (1979) The Dynamic Assessment of Retarded Performers: the Learning
Potential Assessment Device: Theory, Instruments and Techniques. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Fick, M (1929). Intelligence test results of poor White, Native (Zulu), Coloured and Indian school
children and the educational and social implications. South African Journal of Science, 26, 910.
Field, T M, & Walden, T A (1982). Production and perception of facial expressions in infancy and early
childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, 16, 169-211.
Finchilescu, G & Dawes, A (1998). Catapulted into democracy: South African adolescents’ socio-political
orientations following rapid social change. Journal of Social Issues, 54(3), 563-583.
Findlay, M (1999). The globalisation of crime. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fishbein, H D (1984). The psychology of infancy and childhood: Evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives.
New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Fisher, S & Greenberg, R P (1977). The scientific credibility of Freud’s theories and therapy. New York:
Basic Books.
Flavell, J H & Wellman, H M (1977). Metamemory. (In R V Kail & J W Hagen (eds), Perspectives on the
development of memory and cognition. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Flavell, J H, Everett, B H, Croft, K & Flavell, E R (1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual
perception: Further evidence for the level 1-level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 1799-2103.
Flavell, J H, Miller, P H & Miller, S A (1993). Cognitive Development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Fleshner, E A (1958). Psychology of the Mastery and Application by School children of some Concepts in
Physics. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds) (1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul. 198-213.)
Floody, O R & Pfaff, D W (1974). Steroid hormones and aggressive behaviour: Approaches to the study
of hormone-sensitive brain mechanisms for behaviour. Research Publications, Association for Research in
Nervous and Mental Disease, 52, 149-185.
650 Developmental Psychology
Flugel, J C & West, D J (1964). A hundred years of psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Flynn, J R (1998). ‘Intelligence considered’. Scientific American, 9 (4), 7-11.
Follingstad, D R, Bradley, R G, Helff, C M & Laughlin, J E (2007). A model for predicting dating
violence: Anxious attachment, angry temperament, and need for relationship control. Violence &
Victims, 17(1), 35-47.
Fonagy, P & Target, M (1999). Towards understanding violence: The use of the body and the role of the
father. (In R J Perelberg (ed), Psychoanalytic understanding of violence and suicide. London: Routledge.
51-72.)
Fonagy, P (1999). Attachment, the development of the self, and its pathology in personality disorders. (In
J Derksen, C Maffei & H Groen (eds), Treatment of personality disorders. Dordrecht: Kluwer/Plenum.)
Fonagy, P (2000). Points of convergence and divergence between psychoanalytic and attachment theories:
Is psychoanalytic theory truly different? Paper presented at the Michigan Psychoanalytic Institute.
( 2–9 April)
Foster, D (2008). Critical psychology: A historical overview. (In C van Ommen & D Painter (eds),
Interiors. A history of psychology in South Africa, 92-122.)
Foucault, M (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison. London: Allen Lane.
Foucault, M (1980). The history of sexuality. Volume 1: An introduction. London: Vintage.
Frank, K (1997). The role of the analyst’s inadvertent self-revelations. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 7, 281-314.
Freud, A & Burlingham, D T (1943). War and children. London: Medical War Books.
Freud, S (1914). On narcissism: An introduction. Standard Edition, 14, 69-102.
Freud, S (1923). The Ego and the Id. New York: Norton.
Freud, S (1930). Civilization and its Discontents. New York: Norton.
Freud, S (1953). ‘The interpretation of dreams’. (In J Strachey (ed), Standard edition of the complete
worlds of Sigmund Freud. Volumes 4 and 5. London: Hogarth Press.)
Freud, S (1977). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. (In D Richards & A Dickson (eds), The Penguin Freud
Library. Volume 7 on sexuality: Three essays on the theory of sexuality and other works. London: Penguin.)
Freud, S (1978). The interpretation of dreams. London: Penguin.
Freud, S (1982). Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S (1991). The ego and the id. (In D Richards & A Dickson (eds), The Penguin Freud Library.
Volume 11 on metapsychology. London: Penguin.)
Freund, L S (1990). Maternal regulation of children’s problem-solving behaviour and its impact on
children’s performance, Child Development, 61, 113-126.
Frith, U (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. (In K Patterson, M Coltheart & J Marshall
(eds), Surface dyslexia. Hove: Erlbaum.)
Frith, U & Frith, C D (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 459-473.
Fromm, E (1980). Greatness and limitations of Freud’s thought. New York: Harper and Row.
Frosh, S (1994). Sexual difference: Masculinity and psychoanalysis. London: Routledge.
Frotscher, M & Seress, L (2007). Morphological development of the hippocampus. (In P Andersen,
R Morris, D Amaral, T Bliss & J O’Keefe (eds), The hippocampus book. New York: Oxford University
Press. 115-131.)
Fuchs Epstein, C (1988). Deceptive distinctions. Sex, gender and the social order. New Haven: Yale
University Press.
Fugel, J C (1953). A hundred years of psychology. London: Gerald Duckworth and Co Ltd.
Funke, F (2005). The dimensionality of right-wing authoritarianism: Lessons from the dilemma between
theory and measurement. Political Psychology, 26(2), 195-218.
Furrow, D, Nelson, K & Benedict, H (1979). Mother’s speech to children and syntactic development:
Some simple relationships, Journal of Child Language, 6, 423-442.
G
Garbarino, J (1995). Raising children in a socially toxic environment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Gardner, H (1984). On being judged “intelligent”. (In P S Fry (ed), Changing conceptions of intelligence
and intellectual functioning. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Gardner, H (1998). A multiplicity of intelligences. Scientific American, 9 (4), 19-23.
References 651
Garland, C (1999). Thinking about trauma. (In C Garland (ed), Understanding trauma: A psychoanalytic
approach. London: Duckworth, 9-31.)
Gartner, R (1990). The victims of homicide: A temporal and cross national comparison. American
Sociological Review, 55, 92-106.
Gathercole, S E (1998). The development of memory. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 39, 3-27.
Gathercole, S E (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 3, 410-419.
Gay, P (1988). Freud, a life for our times. London: Papermac.
George, C, Kaplan, N & Main, M (1985). Adult attachment interview. Second edition. Berkeley: University
of California. Unpublished manuscript.
Gergen, K & Gergen, M (1984). Historical social psychology. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gerhardt, J & Sweetnam, A (2001). The intersubjective turn in psychoanalysis: A comparison of
contemporary theorists, Part 2. Christopher Bollas. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 11, 43-92.
Gerhardt, J, Sweetnam, A & Borton, L (2000). The intersubjective turn in psychoanalysis: A comparison
of contemporary theorists, Part 1. Jessica Benjamin. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 10, 5-42.
Gibb, B E, Chelminski, I & Zimmerman, M (2007). Childhood emotional, physical, and sexual abuse, and
diagnoses of depressive and anxiety disorders in adult psychiatric outpatients. Depression and Anxiety,
24(4), 256-263.
Gibson, K (1993). The effects of exposure to political violence on children: Does violence beget violence?
South African Journal of Psychology, 23 (4), 167-173.
Giddens, A (1992). Sociology. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Gilbert, A (1989). Things fall apart? Psychological theory in the context of rapid social change. South
African Journal of Psychology, 19, 91-100.
Gilbert, A, Van Vlaenderen, H & Nkwinti, G (1995). Planting pumpkins: socialization and the role of
local knowledge in rural South Africa. South African Journal of Psychology, 25 (4), 229-235.
Gilligan, C (1982). In a Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s development. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Gilligan, C & Wiggins, G (1987). The Origin of Morality in Early Childhood Relationships. (In J Kagan
& S Lamb (eds), The Emergence of Morality in Young Children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.)
Ginsburg, H & Opper, S (1969). Piaget’s theory of intellectual development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Girolametto, L & Weitzman, E (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with
toddlers and preschoolers. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 268-281.
Glasser, M (1985). The weak spot: Some observations on male sexuality. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 66, 405-414.
Glassman, M (1994). All things being equal: The two roads of Piaget and Vygotsky. Developmental
Review, 14, 186-214
Gleitman, H (1991). Psychology. 3rd ed. New York: Norton.
Glick, J (1997). Prologue to The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume 4, The History of the Development
of Higher Mental Functions. New York: Plenum Press.
Glueck, S & Glueck, E T (1968). Delinquents and nondelinquents in perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Gobodo-Madikizela, P (2003). A human being died that night. Cape Town: David Philip.
Gogtay, N, Nugent, T F, Herman, D H, Ordonez, A, Greenstein, D, Hayashi, K M et al (2006). Dynamic
mapping of normal hippocampal development. Hippocampus, 16, 664-672.
Goldberg, A (1988). A Fresh Look at Psychoanalysis: The View from Self Psychology. Hillsdale, New Jersey:
The Analytic Press.
Goldberg, A (1990). The Prisonhouse of Psychoanalysis. Hillsdale, New Jersey: The Analytic Press.
Goldman, D (ed) (1993). In one’s bones: The clinical genius of Winnicott. London: Tavistock.
Goldstein, J M, Seidman, L J, Horton, N J, Makris, N, Kennedy, D N, Caviness, V S, Faraone, S V
& Tsuang, M T (2001). Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human brain assessed by in vivo
magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex, 11(6), 490-497.
Goleman, D (1996a). Studies suggest older minds are stronger than expected. New York Times,
26 February, 1-10.
652 Developmental Psychology
H
Haiden, G A (1998). An exploration of gender stereotypes in pre-school girls: Barbie and beyond.
Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand. Unpublished masters thesis.
Haiden, G A & Zietkiewicz, E (1999). Non-Barbie and beyond: Pre-school girls’ definitions of
themselves in terms of a lack. Paper presented at the Third Annual Qualitative Methods
Conference, Johannesburg.
Haidt, J (2001). The Emotional Dog and its rational Tail: A Social Intuitinist Approach to Moral
Judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814-834.
Hall, G S (1904) Adolescence: Its psychology and its relations to physiology, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime,
religiona and education. London: Sidney Appleton.
Hamilton, C E (1994). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through adolescence.
Dissertation abstracts international, 55 (2), 217.
Hamilton, J (1994). A map of the world. New York: Anchor.
Hanks, W F (1989). Word and image in Mayan culture: Explorations in language, writing and representation.
Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.
Happé, F G (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and
feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 24, 129-154.
Happé, F G (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects
with autism. Child Development, 66, 843-855.
Happé, F G & Frith, U (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119, 1377-1400.
Happé, F G, Winner, E & Brownell, H (1998). The getting of wisdom: Theory of mind in old age.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 358-362.
References 653
Harnishfeger, K K & Bjorklund, D F (1990). Children’s strategies: A brief history. (In D F Bjorklund,
Children’s strategies: Contemporary views of cognitive development. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Harris, M & Coltheart, M (1986). Language processing in children and adults. London: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Harris, P L, De Rosnay, M & Pons, F (2005). Language and children’s understanding of mental states.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 69-73.
Hartley, T, Maguire, E A, Spiers, H J & Burgess, N (2003). The well-worn route and the path less
travelled: distinct neural bases of route following and wayfinding in humans. Neuron, 37, 877-888.
Harwood, R L (1995). Culture and attachment: Perceptions of the child in context. New York: Guilford Press.
Hasher, L & Zacks, R T (1988). Working memory comprehension and ageing: A review and a new
view. Psychology of learning and motivation, 22, 193-225.
Hathaway, S R & McKinley, J C (1951). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. New York:
Psychological Corporation.
Hawkins, J D, Herrenkohl, T, Farrington, D P, Brewer, D, Catalano, R F & Harachi, T W (1998). A
review of predictors of youth violence. (In R Loeber & D P Farrington (eds), Serious and violent juvenile
offenders: Risk factors and successful interventions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 106-146.)
Hayes, G (1989). Psychology and ideology: The case of Althusser. South African Journal of Psychology,
19(2), 84-90.
Hazan, C & Shaver, P R (1987). Romantic love conceptualised as an attachment process. Journal of
Personality andSocial Psychology, 52, 511-524.
Hazan, C & Shaver, P R (1990). Love and work: An attachment-theoretical perspective. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 270-280.
Health24 (2008). How to curb the increase in teen suicides. Available: http://www.health24.com/mind/
Mental_health_in_SA/1284-1301,26685.asp [20 April 2008]
Heaton, J M (2000). Wittgenstein and psychoanalysis. London: Icon.
Hebb, D O (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.
Hedegaard, M (1996). The zone of proximal development as basis for instruction. (In H Daniels (ed),
An Introduction to Vygotsky. London: Routledge. 171-195.)
Heise, L & Garcia-Moreno, C (2002). Violence by intimate partners. (In E G Krug, L L Dahlberg,
J A Mercy, A B Zwi & R Lozano (eds), World report on violence and health. Geneva: WHO. 87-122.)
Helwig, C C (2006). Rights, Civil Liberties, and Democracy Across Cultures. (In M Killen & J Smetana
(eds), Handbook of Moral Development. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.)
Henderson, P C (2006). Examining assumptions around vulnerability from the perspective of rural
children and youth. Childhood, 13(3), 303-327.
Hendrik, H (1990). Constructions and reconstructions of British childhood: An interpretative survey,
1800 to the present. (In A James & A Prout (eds), Constructing and reconstructing childhood:
Contemporary issues in the sociological study of childhood. Basingstoke: Falmer.)
Henry, B, Avsalom, C, Moffitt, T E & Silva, P A (1996). Temperamental and familial predictors of violent
and nonviolent criminal convictions: Age 3 to 18. Developmental Psychology, 32, 614-623.
Herbst, I & Huysamen, G K (2000). The construction and validation of developmental scales for
environmentally disadvantaged preschool children. South African Journal of Psychology, 30(3), 19-24.
Hermann, J (1992). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. Journal
of Traumatic Stress 5, 377-420.
Herrnstein, R & Murray, C (1994). The Bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life. New
York: Free Press.
Hetherington, E & Parke R (1993). Child psychology: A contemporary viewpoint. 4th ed. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Hill, P (1997). Lacan for beginners. London: Writers and Readers.
Hird, M J (2000). An empirical study of adolescent dating aggression. U.K. Journal of Adolescence,
23, 69-78.
Hirsch, J & Kim, K (1997). New views of early language. Nature, 103, 1141-1143.
Hobson, P (2004). The Cradle of Thought: Exploring the Origins of Thinking. London: Pan Books.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E & Shatz, M (1982). Linguistic input and the child’s acquisition of language.
Psychological Bulletin, 92 (1), 3-26.
654 Developmental Psychology
Hoffman, C, Lau, I & Johnson, D R (1986). The linguistic relativity of person cognition: An English
Chinese comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1097-1105.
Hollin, C R (1989). Psychology and crime: An introduction to criminological psychology. London: Routledge.
Holmes, J (1993). John Bowlby and attachment theory. London: Routledge.
Holmes, J (1995). Something there is that doesn’t love a wall: John Bowlby, attachment theory, and
psychoanalysis. (In S Goldberg, R Muir, & J Kerr, (eds), Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and
clinical perspectives. New York: Analytic Press.
Holroyd, S & Baron-Cohen, S (1993). How far can people with autism go in developing a theory of
mind? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 379-385.
Hook, D (2002). Erikson’s psychosocial stages of development. (In D Hook, J Watts & K Cockroft (eds),
Developmental Psychology. Lansdowne: UCT Press. 265-293.)
Hook, D, Mkhize, N, Kiguwa, P, Collins, A, Burman, E & Parker, I (eds) (2004). Critical Psychology.
Lansdowne: UCT Press.
Horn, J L & Cattell, R B (1982). Whimsy and misunderstanding of Gf – Gc theory: A comment on
Guilford. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 623-633.
Horn, P (1991). ‘The way forward towards the emancipation of women’. Agenda, 10, 53-66.
Horney, K (1967). Feminine psychology. New York: Norton.
Howell, J C & Decker, S H (1999). The youth gangs, drugs, and violence connection. Juvenile Justice
Bulletin, January 1999, 1-11.
Howitt, D & Owusu-Bempah, J (1994). The racism of Psychology: Time for change. Hertfordshire:
Harvester/Wheatsheaf.
Huff-Corzine, L, Corzine, J & Moore, D C (1991). Deadly connections: Culture, poverty, and the
direction of lethal violence. Social Forces, 69(3), 715-732.
Huff-Corzine, L, Corzine, J & Moore, D C (1986). Southern exposure: Deciphering the South’s influence
on homicide rates. Social Forces, 64(4), 906-924.
Hulme, C & Tordoff, V (1989). Working memory development: The effects of speech rate,
word length and acoustic similarity on serial recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
47, 72-87.
Humphrey, N K (1976). The social function of intellect. (In P P G Bateson & R A Hinde (eds). Growing
points in ethology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 303-317.)
Hyder, A A (2006). Childhood injuries: Defining a global agenda for research and action. African Safety
Promotion: A Journal of Injury and Violence Prevention 4(1), 87-95.
I
Inhelder, B & Piaget, J (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
International Women’s Health Coalition (2008). Child marriage: Girls 14 and younger at risk. Available:
http://www.iwhc.org/resources/childmarriagefacts. cfm
Isaacs, S (1948). The nature and function of phantasy. (In M Klein (ed), Contributions to psychoanalysis,
1921–1945. London: Hogarth Press.)
Israel, N (2006). Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices Within a South African Context. Johannesburg:
University of the Witwatersrand. Unpublished Research Report submitted for the Degree of Masters
by Coursework and Research Report.
J
Jackson, J F (1993). Multiple caregiving among African Americans and infant attachment: The need for
an emic approach. Human Development, 36, 87-102.
Jackson, S M, Cram, F & Seymour, F W (2000). Violence and sexual coercion in high school students’
dating relationships. Journal of Family Violence, 15(1), 23-36.
Jahoda, G (1982). Psychology and anthropology. London: Academic Press.
Jensen, A (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review,
39, 1-23.
Jewkes, R & Abrahams, N (2002). The epidemiology of rape and sexual coercion in South Africa: An
overview. Social Science & Medicine, 55, 153-166.
References 655
Jolly, A (1966). Lemur social behaviour and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501-506.
Jones, G R (ed) (1988). The nation’s favourite poems. London: BBC.
Jouriles, E N, Murphy, C M & O’Leary, K D (1989). Interspousal aggression, marital discord, and child
problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 453-455.
Jung, C G (1963). Memories, dreams, reflections. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Jung, C G (1986). Analytic psychology: Its theory and practice. London: Ark Paperbacks.
Jung, C G (ed) (1990). Man and his symbols. New York: Aldus Books.
Jung, C G (1983). Jung, selected writings. Introduced by Anthony Storr. London: Fontana.
K
Kail, R (1979). The development of memory in children. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Kalmykova, Z I (1955). Psychological Prerequisites for Increasing the Effectiveness of learning in
Problem Solving in Arithmetic. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds) (1963). Educational Psychology in the
U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 180-191.)
Kaminer, D & Eagle, G (in press). Trauma. Cape Town: S&S Publishers.
Kandel, E & Mednick, S A (1991). Perinatal complications predict violent offending. Criminology, 29,
519-529.
Karphov, Y V & Brandsford, J D (1995). L S Vygotsky and the Doctrine of Empirical and Theoretical
Learning. Educational Psychologist, 30(2), 61-66.
Karphov, Y V (2006). The Neo-Vygotskian Approach to Child Development. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Kasese-Hara, M (2000). Historical contexts of developmental research: The experience of sub-Saharan Africa.
Paper presented at the Fifth Biennial Africa Regional Workshop of the ISSBD. (25-30 September).
Kampala, Uganda.
Kasese-Hara, M (2004). Human development in underdeveloped contexts. (In D Hook, N Mkhize,
P Kiguwa, A Collins, E Burman & I Parker (eds), Critical Psychology. Lansdowne: UCT Press.
540-558.)
Kate, P A (1979). The development of the female identity. (In C B Kopp & M Kirkpatrick (eds),
Becoming female: Perspectives on development. New York: Plenum.)
Kaufman, A S & Kaufman, N C (1983). The Kaufman assessment battery for children. Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance Services.
Keegan, M (2004). Gun violence in South Africa. (In S Suffla, A van Niekerk & N Duncan (eds), Crime,
violence and injury in South Africa: Developments and challenges. Tygerberg: MRC-UNISA. 65-87.)
Kellermann, A L, Rivara, F P, Rushforth, N B, Banton, J G, Reay, D T, Francisco, J T, Locci, A B,
Prodzinski, J, Hackman, B B & Somes, G (1993). Gun ownership as a risk factor for homicide in the
home. New England Journal of Medicine, 329, 1084-1091.
Kendall, F (1993). The SeXY Factor: Gender differences at home and at work. Norwood: Amagi.
Kerig, P K, Fedorowicz, A E, Brown, C A & Warren, M (2000). Assessment and intervention for ptsd in
children exposed to violence. (In R A Geffner, P G Jaffe & M Sudermann (eds), Children exposed to
domestic violence: Current issues in research, intervention, prevention and policy development. Binghamton:
The Haworth Maltreatment & Trauma Press. 161-184.)
Kernberg, O F, Selzer, M A, Koenigsberg, H W, Carr, A C & Appelbaum, A H (1989). Psychodynamic
psychotherapy of borderline patients. New York: Basic Books.
Kiersky, S & Beebe, B (1994). The reconstruction of early nonverbal relatedness in the treatment of
difficult patients: A special form of empathy. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 4, 389-408.
Kim, S & Pridemore, W A (2005). Social support and homicide in transitional Russia. Journal of Criminal
Justice, 33(6), 561-572.
Klein, L & Hickman, D (1994). Baby it’s you. London: Channel 4 Television.
Klein, M (1926). The psychological principles of early analysis. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 8,
25-37.
Klein, M (1932). The psycho-analysis of children. London: Hogarth Press.
Klein, M (1946). Notes on some schizoid mechanisms. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 27, 99-110.
Klein, M (1948). Mourning and its relation to manic-depressive states. (In M Klein (ed), Contributions to
psycho-analysis, 1921–1945. London: Hogarth Press.)
656 Developmental Psychology
Klein, M (1957). Envy and gratitude: A study of unconscious forces. New York: Basic Books.
Klein, M (1975a). Love, guilt and reparation and other works, 1921–1945. London: Hogarth Press.
Klein, M (1975b). Envy and gratitude and other works, 1946–1963. London: Hogarth Press.
Knox, E & Stevens, C B (1993). Introduction to The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume 2: The
Fundamentals of Defectology (Abnormal Psychology and learning Disabilities). New York: Plenum Press.
Kobak, R R (1985). Attitudes towards attachment relations and social competence among first-year college
students. Charlottesville: University of Virginia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Kobak, R R (1986). Attachment as a theory of affect regulation. Denver, Colorado: University of Denver.
Unpublished manuscript.
Kobak, R R & Sceery, A (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation,
and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59, 135-146.
Kobak, R R & Shaver, P (1987). Strategies for maintaining felt security: A theoretical analysis of
continuity and change in styles of social adaptation. Paper presented at the Conference in Honour of
John Bowlby’s Eightieth Birthday, London.
Kohlberg, L (1958). The development of modes of thinking and choice in the years 10 to 16. Chicago, Illinois:
University of Chicago. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Kohlberg, L (1964). Development of Moral Character and Moral Ideology. (In M Hoffman & L Hoffman
(eds), Review of Child Development Research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.)
Kohlberg, L (1966). A cognitive developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes.
(In E E Maccoby (ed), The development of sex differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.)
Kohlberg, L (1968). Moral development. International Encyclopaedia of the Social Sciences. 489-494.
Kohlberg, L (1969). Stage and Sequence: The Cognitive Developmental Approach to Socialization.
(In D A Goslin (ed), Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research: Rand McNally and Company.)
Kohlberg, L (1981). Essays on moral development. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Kohlberg, L (1984). Moral Stages and Moralization: The Cognitive-developmental Approach. (In L
Kohlberg (ed), The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages. San
Francisco: Harper and Row.)
Kohlberg, L (ed) (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice. Volume 1.
Cambridge: Harper and Row.
Kohlberg, L & Candee, D (1984). The relationship of moral judgment to moral action. (In L Kohlberg
(ed), Essays on moral development. Volume 2. New York: Harper and Row.)
Kohlberg, L & Gilligan, C (1971). The adolescent as philosopher: The discovery of the self in a
postconventional world. Daedalus, 1051-1086.
Kohlberg, L & Candee, D (1984). The Relationship of Moral Judgment to Moral Action. (In L Kohlberg
(ed), Essays on Moral Development (Vol. 2). New York: Harper and Row.)
Kohlberg, L & Kauffman, K (1987). Theoretical Introduction to the Measurement of Moral Judgment.
(In A Colby & L Kohlberg (eds), The Measurment of Moral Judgment. (Vol. 1). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.)
Kohlberg, L, Boyd, D & Levine, C (1990). The Return of Stage 6: Its Principle and Moral Point of View.
(In T Wren (ed), The Moral Domain: Essays in the Ongoing Discussion between Philosophy and the Social
Sciences. Cambridge: MIT Press.)
Kohon, G (ed) (1986). The British school of psycho-analysis: The independent tradition. London: Free
Association Books.
Kohut, H & Wolf, E (1978). The disorders of the self and their treatment: An outline. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 59, 413-25.
Kohut, H (1966). Forms and transformations of narcissism. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association, 14, 243-272.
Kohut, H (1971). The Analysis of the Self. New York: International Universities Press.
Kohut, H (1977). The Restoration of the Self. New York: International Universities Press.
Kohut, H (1979). The two analyses of Mr Z. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60, 3-27.
Kohut, H (1982). Introspection, empathy and the semi-circle of mental health. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 63, 395-407.
Kohut, H (1984). How Does Analysis Cure? (In A Goldberg & P Stepansky (eds), Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.)
References 657
Kohut, H (1981). On empathy. (In P Ornstein (ed), The Search for the Self. Vol 4. (1990). Madison:
International Universities Press, 525-535.)
Kostiuk, G S (1956). Some Aspects of the Interrelation between Education and the Development of
Personality. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds), (1963), Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul. 35-49.)
Kottak, C P (1994). Cultural anthropology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Kottler, A (2007). Twinship and “otherness”: A Self psychological, intersubjective approach to
“difference”. (In P Buirski & A Kottler (eds) New Developments in Self Psychological Practice. New
York: Jason Aronson, 207-222.)
Kozulin, A (1990). Vygotsky’s psychology: A biography of ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kozulin, A (1996). The concept of activity in Soviet psychology: Vygotsky, his disciples and critics.
(In H Daniels (ed). An Introduction to Vygotsky. London: Routledge. 99-122.)
Kramer, R (1968). Moral Development in Young Adulthood. Chicago: University of Chicago. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation.
Kritzas, N & Grobler, A A (2005). The relationship between perceived parenting styles and resilience
during adolescence. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 17(1), 1-12.
Krivo, L J & Peterson, R D (2000). The structural context of homicide: Accounting for racial differences
in process. American Sociological Review, 65(4), 547-559.
Krug, E, Dahlberg, L, Mercy, J A, Zwi, A B & Lozano, R (2002). World report on violence and health.
Geneva: WHO.
Krutetski, V A (1961). Some Characteristics of the Thinking of Pupils with Little Capacity for
Mathematics. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds), (1963), Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul. 214-232.)
Kuhl, P K, Conboy, B T, Padden, D, Nelson, T & Pruitt, J (2005). Early speech perception and later
language development: Implications for the “Critical Period”. Language Learning and Development,
1, 237-264.
Kuhn, D (1992). Cognitive development. (In M H Bornstein & M E Lamb (eds), Developmental
psychology: An advanced textbook. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Kumaran, D & Maguire, E A (2005). The human hippocampus: Cognitive maps or relational memory?
The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 7254-7259.
L
Labouvie-Vief, G (1985). Intelligence and cognition. (In J E Birren & K W Schaie (eds), Handbook of the
psychology of ageing. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.)
Labuschagne, I (1999). ‘Personal communication’. Presentation to students, University of the
Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, 16 May 1999.
Labuschagne, I (2000). ‘Criminological evaluation of Kobus Geldenhuys’. Unpublished manuscript, Pretoria.
Lacan, J (1979). The four fundamental concepts of psychoanalysis. London: Penguin.
Lacan, J (1977). Ecrits. New York: Norton.
Lachmann, F & Beebe, B (1992). Representational and selfobject transferences: A developmental perspective.
(In New Therapeutic Visions: Progress in Self Psychology. Vol 8. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press. 3-15.)
Lachmann, F & Beebe, B (1996). Three principles of salience in the organization of the patient-analyst
interaction. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 13, 1-22.
Lachmann, F & Lichtenberg, J (1992). Model scenes: Implications for psychoanalytic treatment. Journal of
the American Psychoanalytic Association, 40, 117-137.
Ladikos, A (1995). A monistic study of murder: Contemporary empirical findings. Acta Criminologica,
8(1), 56-61.
LaFreniere, P, Strayer, F F & Gauthier, R (1984). The emergence of same-sex affiliative preference among
pre-school peers: A developmental/ethological perspective. Child Development, 55, 1958-1965.
Lahey, B B (2001). Psychology: An introduction. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Lamb, M E (1981). Fathers and child development: An integrative overview. (In M E Lamb (ed), The
father’s role in child development. New York: John Wiley.)
Laplanche, J & Pontalis, J B (1973). The language of psycho-analysis. London: Hogarth Press.
Lapsley, D K (1996). Moral Psychology. Boulder: Westview Press.
658 Developmental Psychology
Lapsley, D K (2006). Moral Stage Theory. (In M Killen & J Smetana (eds), Handbook of Moral
Development. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.)
Lashley, K S (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. (In LA Jeffress (ed), Cerebral mechanisms in
behavior. New York: Wiley. 112-131.)
Lau, U (2008). An exploratory study of men’s subjective experiences of their violence towards their intimate
partners. Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand. Unpublished masters thesis.
Laubscher, L & Klinger, J (1997). Story and the making of the self. (In C De la Rey, N Duncan, T Shefer
& A Van Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African focus. Johannesburg:
International Thomson Publishing. 58-79.)
Lave, J (1993). The practice of learning. (In S Chaiklin & J Lave (eds), Understanding practice. Perspectives
on activity and context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.)
Lave, J & Wenger, E (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Lave, J & Wenger, E (1996). Practice, person, social world. (In H Daniels (ed), An Introduction to
Vygotsky. London: Routledge. 143-150.)
Lazarus, J (1993a). Would you have raped and killed me too? Sunday Star: 26 September.
Lazarus, J (1993b). What makes a man become a beast to women? Sunday Star: 26 September.
Lazarus, J & Kruger, L (2004). Small meetings: reflections on the application of psychodynamic thought
in community work with low-income South African children. Parts 1 & 2, Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy
in South Africa, 12 (1/2), 21-51 and 48-73 .
Leader, D & Groves, G (1995). Lacan for beginners. Cambridge: Icon.
Leahey, T H (2004). A history of psychology: Main currents in psychological though.6th ed. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Pearson Education.
Lederman, D, Loayza, N & Menendez, A M (1999). Violent crime: Does social capital matter? Washington,
DC: World Bank, 1999.
Lee, M R (2000). Concentrated poverty, race, and homicide. The Sociological Quarterly, 41(2), 189-206.
Leibowitz, S, Mendelsohn, M & Michelson, C (1999). Child rape: extending the therapeutic intervention
to include the mother-child dyad. South African Journal of Psychology, 29, 103-1-8.
Lenneberg, E (1967). Biological foundation of language. New York: John Wiley.
Lerner, R M (1986). Concepts and theories of human development. New York: Random House.
Levett, A (1994). Problems of cultural imperialism in the study of child sexual abuse. (In A Dawes &
D Donald (eds), Childhood and adversity: Psychological perspectives from South African research. Cape
Town: David Philip.)
Lewis, M (1987). Early sex role behaviour and school age adjustment. (In J M Reinisch, L A Rosenblum,
& S A Sanders (eds), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press.)
Leontiev, A N (1959). Principles of Child Mental Development and the Problem of Intellectual
backwardness. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds), (1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul. 68-82.)
Leontiev, A N (1997). On Vygotsky’s creative development. (In R W Rieber & J Wollock (eds), translated
by R van der Veer. The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 3, Problems of the Theory and History of
Psychology. New York: Plenum Press.)
Lerner, R M (1986). Concepts and theories of human development. New York: Random House.
Lesch, K P & Merschdorf, U (2000). Impulsivity, aggression, and serotonin: A molecular psychobiological
perspective. Behavioral Science & the Law, 18, 581-604.
Leslie, A M (1987). Pretence and representation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychology Review, 94, 412-426.
Levett, A (1988). Psychological trauma: Discourses of childhood sexual abuse. Cape Town: University of
Cape Town.Unpublished doctoral thesis.
Levin, A (2007). CNS sequalae may appear in HIV Infection. Psychiatry Online: Psychiatric News, 42(22), 18.
Levine, M D (1986). Working it out: A community re-creation approach to crime prevention. Journal of
Community Psychology, 14(4), 378-390.
Lewis, D (1992). From abuse to violence: Psychophysiological consequences of maltreatment. Journal of
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 383-391.
Lewis, S (1999). An adult’s guide to childhood trauma: Understanding traumatized children in South Africa.
Cape Town: David Phillip Publishers.
References 659
Lichtenberg, J, Lachmann, F & Fosshage, J (1992). Self and Motivational Systems: Towards a Theory of
Psychoanalytic Technique. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Liddell, C (1996). Every picture tells a story: South African and British children interpreting pictures.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 355-363.
Liddell, C & Kvalsig, J (1990). Science and social accountability: Issues related to South African
developmental psychology. South African Journal of Psychology, 20(1), 1- 9.
Liddle, C, Shabalala, A & Masilela, P (1991). Historical perspectives on South African children.
International Journal of Behavioural Development, 14 (1), 1-19.
Liddell, C, Kvalsig, J, Shabalala, A & Qotyana, P (1994). Defining the cultural context of children’s
everyday experiences in the year before school. (In A Dawes & D Donald (eds), Childhood and adversity:
Psychological perspectives from South African research. Cape Town: David Philip.)
Liddell, C, Lycett, J & Gordon, R (1997). Getting through Grade 2: predicting children’s early school
achievement in rural South African schools. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21(2), 331-348.
Lidz, C S (1991). Practitioner’s guide to dynamic assessment. New York: Guilford.
Lifton, R J (1979). The broken connection. New York: Simon & Shuster.
Lipsey, M W & Derzon, J H (1998). Predictors of serious delinquency in adolescence and early adulthood:
A synthesis of longitudinal research. (In R Loeber & D P Farrington (eds), Serious and violent juvenile
offenders: Risk factors and successful interventions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 86-105.)
Lloyd, C (1993). The Structures of History. Oxford: Blackwell.
Lock, A, Service, V, Brito, A & Chandler, P (1989). The social structuring of infant cognition. (In A Slater
& G Bremner (eds), Infant development. Hove: Erlbaum.)
Lockhat, R & Van Niekerk, A (2000). South African children: A history of adversity, violence and
trauma. Ethnicity and Health, 5 (3), 291-302.
Loftus, E (1994). The myth of repressed memory. New York: St Martin’s Press.
Loftus, E (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, 70-75.September.
Loram, C (1917). The education of the South African native. New York: Longmans & Green.
Louw, D A (1991). Human development. Pretoria: HAUM Tertiary.
Louw, D, Louw, A & Schoeman,W (1995). Developmental psychology. (In D A Louw and J A
Edwards (eds), Psychology: An introduction for students in Southern Africa. Johannesburg: Lexicon.)
Louw, J & Foster, D (1991). Historical perspective: Psychology and group relations in South Africa. (In
D Foster & J Louw-Potgieter (eds), Social psychology in South Africa. Johannesburg: Lexicon. 57-92.)
Luria, A R (1960). The Role of Speech in the Formation of Temporary Connections and the Regulation
of Behaviour in the Normal and Oligophrenic Child. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds), (1963). Educational
Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 83-100.)
Luria, A R (1976). Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press.
Luria, A R (1978). Biographical Note on L S Vygotsky. (In M Cole, V John-Steiner, S Scribner and
E Souberman (eds), Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge,
Massachusetts : Harvard University Press.)
Lyons-Ruth, K, Bruschweiler-Stern, N, Harrison, A, Morgan, A, Nahum, J, Sander, L, Stern, D N &
Tronick, E (1998). Implicit relational knowing: Its role in development and psychoanalytic treatment.
Infant Mental Health J., 19: 282-289.
M
Maccoby, E E (1987). The varied meanings of “masculine” and “feminine”. (In J M Reinisch, L A
Rosenblum & S A Sanders (eds), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives. New York: Oxford
University Press.)
Maccoby, E E (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24 (6), 755-765.
Macfarlane, F (2001). A serial killer – why he kills: An attempt to understand the motives behind the
actions of Jacobus Petrus Geldenhuys. Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand. Unpublished
honours thesis.
MacKinnon-Lewis, C, Volling, B L, Lamb, M E, Dechman, K, Rabiner, D & Curtner, M E (1994). A
cross-cultural analysis of boys’ social competence: From family to school. Developmental Psychology,
30(3): 325-333.
660 Developmental Psychology
Macleod, C (2002). Theory and South African developmental psychology. (In D Hook, J Watts & K
Cockroft (eds), Developmental Psychology. Lansdowne: UCT Press. 379-396.)
Maddaleno, M, Concha-Eastman, A & Marques, S (2006). Youth violence in Latin America:
A framework for action. African Safety Promotion: A Journal of Injury and Violence Prevention, 4(2),
120-136.
Maguire, E A, Burgess, N & O’Keefe, J (1999). Human spatial navigation: Cognitive maps, sexual
dimorphism, and neural substrates. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 171-177.
Maguire, E A, Frackowiak, R S J & Frith, C D (1997). Recalling routes around London: Activation of the
right hippocampus in taxi drivers. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 7103-7110.
Magwaza, A (1997). Sexual abuse: a socio-cultural developmental perspective. (In C De la Rey, N
Duncan, T Shefer & A Van Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African
focus. Johannesburg: International Thomson Publishing. 159-169.)
Maier, H W (1988). Three theories of child development. 3rd ed. New York: University Press of America.
Main, M (1997). Attachment narratives and attachment across the lifespan. Paper presented at the Fall
meeting of the American Psychoanalytic Association, New York.
Main, M & Hesse, E (1990). Lack of mourning in adulthood and its relationship to infant disorganization:
Some speculations regarding causal mechanisms. (In M T Greenberg, D Cicchetti, & E M Cummings (eds),
Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.)
Main, M & Solomon, J (1986). Discovery of a new, insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern.
(In T B Brazelton & M Yogman (eds), Affective development in infancy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.)
Main, M & Solomon, J (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the
Ainsworth Strange Situation. (In M T Greenberg, D Cicchetti, & E M Cummings (eds), Attachment in
the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.)
Malan, D (1979). Individual Psychotherapy and the Science of Psychodynamics. London: Butterworths.
Malinowski, B (1927). Sex and repression in savage society. New York: Humanities Press.
Manganyi, C (2004). Political violence, impunity and the psychology of the transition. (In C Manganyi (ed),
On becoming a democracy. Pretoria: Unisa Press. 40-57.)
Marais, S, Sukhai, A & Donson, H (2004). Monitoring alcohol and other substance use in South Africa.
The alcohol and drug injury nexus. (In S Suffla, A van Niekerk & N Duncan (eds), Crime, violence and
injury in South Africa: Developments and challenges. Tygerberg: MRC-Unisa. 128-144.)
Marcia, J E (1980). Identity in adolescence. (In J Adelson (ed), Handbook of adolescent psychology. New
York: Wiley. 159-187.)
Marini, M (1992). Jacques Lacan: The French context. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Marks, S & Atmore, A (eds), (1980). Economy and society in pre-industrial South Africa. London: Longman.
Maroda, K (2002). Heinz Kohut: The Making of a Psychoanalyst. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 19, 759-766.
Marsh, G, Friedman, M, Welch, V & Desberg, P (1981). A cognitive-developmental theory of reading
acquisition. (In G E MacKinnon & T G Waller (eds), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice.
New York: Academic Press.)
Martin, C L & Parker, S (1995). Folk theories about sex and race differences. The personality and social
psychology bulletin, 21 (1), 45-57.
Massey, D S (1994). Getting away with murder: Segregation and violent crime in urban America.
University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 143, 1203-1232.
Masson, J (1992). Against therapy. London: Harper-Collins
Mathews, C, Aarø, L E, Flisher, A J, Mukoma, W, Wubs, A G & Schaalma, H (2008). Predictors of early
first sexual intercourse among adolescents in Cape Town, South Africa. Health Education. Available: http://
her.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/cym079v1.[25 October 2008]
Matsakis, A (1992). I can’t get over it: a handbook for trauma survivors. Oakland: New Harbinger.
Matzopoulos, R (ed) (2004). A profile of fatal injuries in South Africa. Fifth annual report of the National
Injury Mortality Surveillance System 2003. Tygerberg: MRC-UNISA Crime, Violence and
Injury Lead Programme.
Mayes, P (1986). Sociology in focus: Gender. New York: Longman.
Maylor, E A, Moulson, J M, Muncer, A M & Taylor, L A (2002). Does performance on theory of mind
tasks decline in old age? British Journal of Psychology, 93, 465-485.
Mayseless, O (1991). Adult attachment patterns and courtship violence. Family Relations, 40(1), 21-28.
References 661
McCandless, B R (1967). Children: Behaviour and development. 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
McClelland, J L & Rumelhart, D E (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and
specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 159-188.
McClelland, J L & Rumelhart, D E (1988). Explorations in parallel-distributed-processing: A handbook of
models, programs and exercises. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McClelland, J L, McNaughton, B C & O’Reilly, R C (1995). Why there are complementary learning
systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist
models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419-457.
McClelland, J L, Rumelhart, D E, & Hinton, G E (1986). The appeal of parallel distributed processing.
(In D E Rumelhart, J L McClelland & the PDP Research Group (eds), Parallel distributed processing.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vol. 1, 3-40.)
McClelland, J L, Rumelhart, D E & the PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel distributed processing:
Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Books.
McCord, J (1979). Some child-rearing antecedents of criminal behavior in adult men. Journal of
Personality & Social Psychology, 37, 1477-1486.
McCord, J (1996). Family as crucible for violence: Comment on Gorman-Smith, Tolan & Zelli (1996).
Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 147-152.
McGuire, W & Hull, R C G (eds) (1980). Jung speaking: Interviews and encounters. London: Picador.
McNulty, T J & Holloway, S R (2000). Race, crime, and public housing in Atlanta: Testing a conditional
effect hypothesis. Social Forces, 79(2), 707-729.
Mednick, S A & Christiansen, K O (eds) (1977). Biosocial bases of criminal behaviour. New York:
Gardner Press.
Meltzoff, A N (1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests.
Child Development, 59, 217-225.
Mentis, M, Dunn, M, Durbach, F, Arnott, A, Mentis, M & Skuy, M (1991). Mediated learning experience
working manual. Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand.
Mercy, J A, Butchart, A, Farrington, D P & Cerdá, M (2002). Youth violence. (In E G Krug, L L Dahlberg,
J A Mercy, A B Zwi & R Lozano (eds), World report on violence and health . Geneva: WHO. 23-56.)
Messner, S F & Rosenfeld, R (1997). Political restraint of the market and levels of criminal homicide:
A cross-national application of institutional-anomie theory. Social Forces, 75(4), 1393-1416.
Messner, S F (1982). Poverty, inequality, and the urban homicide rate. Criminology, 20(1), 103-114.
Messner, S F (1983). Regional and racial effects on the urban homicide rate: The subculture of violence
revisited. American Journal of Sociology, 88, 997-1007.
Messner, S F (1989). Economic discrimination and societal homicide rates: Further evidence on the cost
of inequality. American Sociological Review, 54, 597-611.
Meyer, A, Eilertsen, D, Sundet, J M, Tshifularo, J & Sagvolden, T (2004). Cross-cultural similarities in
ADHD-like behaviour amongst South African primary school children. South African Journal of
Psychology, 34(1), 122-138
Meyer, M (2001). The Norwood serial killer: A discourse analytic review. Johannesburg: University of the
Witwatersrand. Unpublished honours thesis.
Mikulincer, M & Florian, V (1998). The relationship between adult attachment styles and emotional and
cognitive reactions to stressful events. (In J A Simpson & W S Rholes (eds), Attachment theory and close
relationships. New York: Guilford.)
Mikulincer, M, Horesh, N, Eilati, I & Kotler, M (1999). The association between adult attachment style and
mental health in extreme life-endangering conditions. Personality and individual differences, 27, 831-842.
Milerian, E A (1960). Psychological Characteristics of the Transfer of Technical Skills in Older
Schoolchildren. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds), (1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul. 233-242.)
Miller, S (1997). Working with traumatized children: A community project. (In B Rock (ed), Spirals of
Suffering: Public Violence and Children. Pretoria: HSRC Publishing. 255-276.)
Milton, J (1983). The edges of war: A history of frontier wars (1702-1878). Cape Town: Juta.
Minde, K (1999). Mediating attachment patterns during a serious medical illness. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 20(1), 105-122.
662 Developmental Psychology
Mingo, C (1999). Perceptions of community violence in an Elsies River primary school in the Western Cape.
Bellville: University of the Western Cape. Unpublished masters thesis.
Mitchell, J (2000) Psychoanalysis and Feminism. London: Penguin.
Mitchell, K J, Johnson, M K, Raye, C L & D’Esposito, M (2000). fMRI evidence of age-related
hippocampal dysfunction in feature binding in working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 197-206.
Mitchell, S & Aron, L (eds) (1999). Relational Psychoanalysis: The Emergence of a Tradition. Hillsdale, NJ:
The Analytic Press.
Miyake, K, Chen, S J & Campos, J J (1985). Infant temperament, mother’s mode of interaction, and
attachment in Japan: An interim report. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 50
(1-2), 276-297.
Mkhize, N (2004). Psychology: An African perspective. (In D Hook, N Mkhize, P Kiguwa,
A Collins, E Burman & I Parker, Critical psychology. Lansdowne: UCT Press. 24-52.)
Mkhize, N & Frizelle, K (2000). Hermeneutic-dialogical approaches to career development: an
exploration. South African Journal of Psychology, 30(2), 1-8.
Mohanty, A K & Perregaux, C (1997). Language acquisition and bilingualism. (In J W Berry, P R Dasen
& T S Saraswathi (eds), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.)
Mohr, W K & Fantuzzo, J W (2000). The neglected variable of physiology in domestic violence.
(In R A Geffner, P G Jaffe & M. Sudermann (eds), Children exposed to domestic violence: Current issues in
research, intervention, prevention and policy development. Binghamton: The Haworth Maltreatment &
Trauma Press. 69-84.)
Moll, L C (1990). Vygotsky and education: Instructional implications and applications of sociohistorical
psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Molon, P (2000). Freud and false memory syndrome. Cambridge: Icon.
Money, J (1965). Psychosexual differentiation. (In J Money (ed), Sex research: New developments. New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.)
Moosa, R, Moonsamy, G, & Fridjon, P (1997). Identification patterns among black students at a
predominantly white university. South African Journal of Psychology, 27(4), 256-260.
Morenoff, J D & Sampson, R J (1997). Violent crime and the spatial dynamics of neighbourhood
transition: Chicago, 1970-1990. Social Forces, 76(1), 31-64.
Morisset, C T, Barnard, K E, Greenberg, M T, Booth, C L & Spieker, S J (1990). Environmental influences
on early language development: The context of social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 127-149.
Morojele, N (1997). Adolescent alcohol misuse. (In C De la Rey, N Duncan, T Shefer & A Van Niekerk
(eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African focus. Johannesburg: International
Thomson Publishing. 207-232.)
Morrell, R (2001). The times of change: Men and masculinity in South Africa. (In R Morrell (ed),
Changing men in Southern Africa. Pietermaritzburg: University of Natal Press. 3-37.)
Morris, C (2000). (Dir.) Criminal minds. Johannesburg: M-Net Television.
Morris, C G & Maisto, A A (1998). Psychology: An introduction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Morris, N (2007). Theories of hippocampal function. (In P Andersen, R Morris, D Amaral, T Bliss&
J O’Keefe (eds), The hippocampus book. New York: Oxford University Press. 581-713.)
Morris, P (1965). Prisoners and their families. London: George Allen & Unwin.
Morris, R G M, Garrud, P, Rawlins, J N & O’Keefe, J (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with
hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297, 681-683.
Morrison, A (1989). Shame: The Underside of Narcissism. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Morrison, A (1994). The breadth and boundaries of a self psychological immersion in shame: A one-and-
a-half person perspective. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 4, 19-35.
Moser, C & Holland, J (1997). Urban poverty and violence in Jamaica. (In World Bank Latin American and
Caribbean studies: Viewpoints. Washington, DC: World Bank. 1-53.)
Moses, L J (2001). Executive accounts of theory of mind development. Child Development, 72, 688-690.
Mosse, G (1999). Eighteenth century foundations. (In M Bulmer & J Solomos (eds), Racism. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.)
Mugny, G & Carugati, F (1989). Social representations of intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Murray, C & Herrnstein, R J (1994). The Bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life.
New York: Free Press.
References 663
Mussen, P H, Conger, J J, Kagan, J, & Huston, A C (1990). A child’s world: Infancy through adolescence. 6th
ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Mussen, P, Conger, J & Kagan, J (1974). Child development and personality. New York: Harper & Row.
Myers, N A & Perlmutter, M (1978). Memory in the years from two to five. (In P Ornstein (ed),
Memory development in children. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
N
Nadig, A S & Sedivy, J C (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children‘s on-line reference
resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329-336.
Naito, M, Komatsu, S & Fuke, T (1994). Normal and autistic children‘s understanding of their own and
others‘ false belief: a study from Japan. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 403-416.
Nash, M (1997). Gift of love. Time, 43-44, March 24.
Natadze, R G (1957). The mastery of Scientific Concepts in School. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds), (1963).
Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R.. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 192-197.)
National Crime Prevention Strategy (NCPS). (1996). National crime prevention strategy: Summary.
Available: http://www.info.gov.za/otherdocs/1996/crime1.htm [13 April 2005]
Neisser, U (1976). Cognitive Psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Neisser, U, Boodoo, G, Bouchard Jr., T J, Wade Boykin, A, Brody, N, Ceci, S J, Halpern, D F, Loehlin,
J C, Perloff, R, Sternberg, R J & Urbina, S (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns. American
Psychologist, 51 (2), 77-101.
Nell, V (2002). Why young men drive dangerously: Implications for injury prevention.Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 11(2), 75-79.
Nelson Mandela Children’s Fund (2001). Report: A study into the situation and special needs of children in
child-headed households. Johannesburg: Nelson Mandela Children’s Fund.
Nelson, C A (1987). The recognition of facial expressions in the first two years of life: mechanisms of
development. Child Development, 58, 889-909.
Nelson, K (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 38, 1-2.
Nelson, T E, Acker, M & Manis, M (1996). Irrepressible stereotypes. Journal of experimental social
psychology, 32, 13-38.
Neuman, W L (1997). Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approaches. Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Newcomb, A F & Bagwell, C L (1995). Children’s friendship relations: A meta-analytic review.
Psychological Bulletin, 117 (2): 306-345.
Newcombe, N S, Huttenlocher, J, Drummey, A B & Wiley, J G (1998). The development of spatial
location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive
Development, 13, 185-200.
Newcombe, N S, Lloyd, M E & Ratliff, K R (in press). Development of episodic and autobiographical
memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. (In R V Kail (ed), Advances in child development and
behaviour. Vol. 35. San Diego, CA: Elsevier.)
Newell, A, Shaw, J C & Simon, H A (1957). Empirical explorations of the logic theory machine: A case
study in heuristics. Proceedings of the Western Joint Computer Conference, 230-240.
Newman, F & Holzman, L (1993). Lev Vygotsky. Revolutionary scientist. London: Routledge.
Nicholson, L J (1990). Feminism/postmodernism. New York: Routledge.
Nippold, M A, Hesketh, L H, Duthie, J K & Mansfield, T C (2005). Conversational versus expository
discourse: A study of syntactic development in children, adolescents and adults. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1048-1064.
Nisan, M & Kohlberg, L (1982). Universality and Variation in Moral Judgment: A Longitudinal and
Cross-Sectional Study in Turkey. Child Development, 52, 865-876.
Nisbett, R E (2003). The geography of thought: Why we think the way we do. New York:
The Free Press.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S (1988). Abnormal psychology. Burr Ridge, IL: McGraw-Hill.
Nsamenang, A B (1992). Human development in cultural context: A third world perspective. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
664 Developmental Psychology
O
O’Keefe, J & Dostrovsky, J (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map: Preliminary evidence from unit
activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research, 34, 171-175.
O’Keefe, J & Nadel, L (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon.
O’Reilly, R, Braver, T S & Cohen, J D (1999). A biologically based computational model of working
memory. (In A Miyake & P Shah (eds), Models of working memory. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press. 375-411.)
Ogden, T (1994). Subjects of Analysis. Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
Okagaki, L & Sternberg, R J (1993). Parental beliefs and children’s school performance. Child
Development, 64, 36-56.
Omi, M & Winant, H (1999). Racial formation in the United States. (In M Bulmer & J Solomos (eds),
Racism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.)
Onishi, K H & Baillargeon, R (2002). 15-month-old infants’ understanding of false belief. Presented at the
International Conference on Infant Studies, Toronto, Canada.
Orange, D, Atwood, G & Stolorow, R (1997). Working Intersubjectively: Contextualism in Psychoanalytic
Practice. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Ornstein, A (1998). The fate of narcissistic rage in psychotherapy. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 18, 55-70.
Ornstein, P & Ornstein, A (1995). Some distinguishing features of Heinz Kohut’s Self psychology.
Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 5, 353-366.
Ornstein, P (1990). Introduction: The unfolding and completion of Heinz Kohut’s paradigm of
psychoanalysis. (In P Ornstein (ed), The Search for the Self: Selected Writings of Heinz Kohut, 1978-1981,
vol.3. Madison, Connecticut: International Universities Press, 1-82.)
Ornstein, P A, Baker-Ward, L & Naus, M J (1978). Rehearsal processes in children’s memory. (In P A
Ornstein (ed), Memory development in children. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Osborne, R (1993). Freud for beginners. London: Writers and Readers.
Ousey, G C (1999). Homicide, structural factors, and the racial invariance assumption. Criminology, 37(2),
405-426.
Ousey, G C (2000). Deindustrialization, female-headed families, and black and white juvenile homicide
rates, 1970-1990. Sociological Inquiry, 7(4), 391-419.
Owen, K (1992). The Suitability of Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices for Various Groups in South
Africa. Personality and Individual Differences, 13 (2), 149-159.
Owen, K & Taljaard, J (eds) (1989). Handbook for the use of psychological and scholastic tests of IPER and the
NIPR. Pretoria: Human Sciences Research Council.
Ozonoff, S & McEvoy, R E (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind
development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415-431.
P
K Kondo-Ikemura (ed), Caregiving: Cultural and cognitive perspectives on secure-base behaviour and
working models: New growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 60 (2–3), Serial no. 244, 133-145.
References 665
Page, M P A & Norris, D G (1998). The Primacy Model: a new model of immediate serial recall.
Psychological Review, 105, 761-781.
Pallone, N J & Hennessy, J J (1999). Blacks and whites as victims and offenders in aggressive crimes in
the US: Myths and realities. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 30(1/2), 1-33.
Pampel, F C & Gartner, R (1995). Age structure, socio-political institutions, and national homicide rates.
European Sociological Review, 11, 243-260.
Panday, S, Reddy, S P, Ruiter, R A C, Bergstrom, E & De Vries, H (2007). Determinants of smoking
among adolescents in the southern Cape-Karoo region, South Africa. Health Promotion International,
22(3), 207-217.
Panzer, A & Viljoen, M (2004). Dissociation: a developmental psychoneurobiological perspective. South
African Psychiatry Review, 7( 3), 11-14.
Papalia, D E & Olds, W S (1993). A child’s world: Infancy through adolescence. 6th ed. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Papalia, D E, Olds, S W & Feldman, R D (1998). Human Development. 7th ed. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Papert, J (1996). Piaget’s history. (In B J Wadsworth (ed), Piaget’s theory of cognitive and affective
development: Foundations of constructivism. White Plains, NY: Longman.)
Parekh, A & Jackson, C (1997). Families of children with a mental handicap. (In C De la Rey, N Duncan,
T Shefer & A Van Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African focus.
Johannesburg: International Thomson Publishing. 38-53.)
Parke, R D (1979). Sex-role development: Introduction. (In R D Parke (ed), Readings in social
development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.)
Parker, K F & McCall, P L (1999). Structural conditions and racial homicide patterns: A look at the
multiple disadvantages in urban areas. Criminology, 37(3), 447-477.
Parker, K F & Pruitt, M V (2000a). Poverty, poverty concentration, and homicide. Social Science Quarterly,
81(2), 555-570.
Parker, K F & Pruitt, M V (2000b). How the west was one: Explaining the similarities in race-specific
homicide rates in the west and south. Social Forces, 78(4), 1483-1508.
Parkes, J (2002). “Children also have rights, but then who wants to listen to our rights?” Children’s
Perspectives on Living with Community Violence in South Africa. Educate, 2(2), 59-71.
Parkin, A (1999). Memory: A guide for professionals. New York: John Wiley.
Parkin, A & Java, R I (1999). Determinants of age-related memory loss. (In T Perfect & E A Maylor
(eds), Theoretical debates in cognitive aging. London: Oxford University Press.)
Pascual-Leone, J (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s developmental stages’ Acta
Psychologica, 32, 301-345.
Paulus, P B (1988). Prison crowding: A psychological perspective. New York: Springer-verlag.
Peden, M & van der Spuy, J (1998). The cost of treating firearm victims. Trauma Review, 6, 4-5.
Peltzer, K (1999). Posttraumatic stress symptoms in a population of rural children in South Africa.
Psychological Reports, 85, 646-650.
Penuel, W R & Wertsch, J V (1995). Vygotsky and identity formation: A Sociocultural Approach.
Educational Psychologist, 30(2), 83-92.
Pepler, D J, Catallo, R & Moore, T E (2000). Consider the children: Research informing interventions for
children exposed to domestic violence. (In R A Geffner, P G Jaffe & M Sudermann (eds), Children
Exposed to Domestic Violence: Current issues in research, intervention, prevention and policy development.
Binghamton: The Haworth Maltreatment & Trauma Press. 37-58.)
Perelberg, R J (ed) (1999). Psychoanalytic understanding of violence and suicide. London: Routledge.
Perrin, A (2005). National threat and political culture: Authoritarianism, anti-authoritarianism, and the
September 11 attacks. Political Psychology 26(2), 167-194.
Perry, B D, Pollard, R A, Blakely, T L, Baker, W L & Vigilante, D (1995). Childhood trauma, the
neurobiology of adaptation, and ‘use-dependent’ development of the brain: How states become traits.
Infant Mental Health Journal, 16(4), 271-289.
Peterson, C, Wellman, H, & Liu, D (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with
deafness or autism. Child Development, 76, 502-517.
Peterson, K, Prout, M & Schwarz, R (1991). Post-traumatic stress disorder: A clinician’s guide. New York:
Plenum Press.
666 Developmental Psychology
Peterson, L M (1980). Why men have pockets in their pants: A feminist insight (or, if Freud had been a
woman). Society for the Advancement of Social Psychology newsletter, 6, 19.
Pettit, G (1997). The developmental course of violence and aggression: Mechanisms of family and peer
influence. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, 283-299.
Pharaoh (ed), A Generation at Risk: HIV/AIDS, Vulnerable Children and Security in Southern Africa (9-32).
Pretoria: Institute for Security Studies, ISS Monograph Series No 109.
Phillips, A T, Wellman, H M & Spelke, E S (2002). Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional
expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85, 53-78.
Phillips, R (1999). The economic cost of homicide to a South African city. Cape Town: University of Cape
Town.Unpublished masters thesis.
Piaget, J (1926). The language and thought of the child. London: Kegan, Paul, Trench and Trubner.
Piaget, J (1932/1965). The Moral Judgement of the Child. (M. Gabain, Trans.). New York: Free Press.
Piaget, J (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.
Piaget, J (1962). Play, dreams and imitation. New York: Norton.
Piaget, J (1972). Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human Development, 15, 1-12.
Piaget, J (1975). The development of thought equilibration of cognitive structures (Translated: A Rosin). New
York: Viking Press.
Pillemer, D B & White, S H (1989). Childhood events recalled by children and adults. Advances in Child
Development and Behavior, 21, 297-340.
Pink Floyd (1979). Mother. The Wall (audio-recording). New York: CBS Studios.
Pinker, S (1997). How the mind works. London: Penguin Books.
Pinnock, D (1982a). Towards an understanding of the structure, function and causes of street gangs in Cape
Town. Cape Town: University of Cape Town Unpublished masters thesis.
Pinnock, D (1982b). The brotherhoods: Street gangs and state control in Cape Town. Cape Town:
David Philip.
Pinnock, D (1997). Gangs, rituals and rites of passage. Cape Town: African Sun Press.
Pistorius, M (1999). Serial killers. (In R Marsh (ed), With criminal intent: The changing face of crime in
South Africa. Kenilworth, Cape Town: Ampersand Press.)
Pistorius, M (2000a). Personal communication. Personal interview conducted on 21 March 2000,
Johannesburg.
Pistorius, M (2000b). Catch me a killer... Serial murders: A profiler’s true story. Natal: Penguin.
Pistorius, M (2002). Strangers on the street: Serial homicide in South Africa. Sandton: Penguin.
Polk, K (1994). When men kill: Scenarios of masculine violence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Popper, K (1960). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.
Posada, G, Gao, Y, Fang, W, Posada, R, Tascon, M, Schoelmerich, A, Sagi, A, Kondo-Ikemura, K,
Ylaland, W & Synnevaag, B (1995). The secure base phenomenon across cultures: Children’s behaviour,
mothers’ preferences and experts’ concepts. (In E Waters, B Vaughn, G Posada, & K Konde-Ikemura
(eds), Caregiving: Cultural and cognitive perspectives on secure-base behaviour and working models: New
growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 60 (2-3), Serial no. 244, 133-145.)
Posada, G, Jacobs, A, Carbonell, O A, Alzate, G, Bustamante, M R & Arenas, A (1999). Maternal care and
attachment security in ordinary and emergency contexts. Developmental Psychology, 35 (6), 1379-1388.
Poster, M (1988). Critical theory of the family. New York: Seabury Press.
Powlitsha, K K, Serbin, L A, Doyle, A B & White, D R (1994). Gender, ethnic and body type biases:
The generality of prejudice in childhood. Developmental Psychology, 30 (4), 526-536.
Premack, D & Woodruff, G (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 1, 515-526.
Pridemore, W A (2006). Criminological transition? Change and stability in homicide characteristics during
rapid social change. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association,
Montreal Convention Centre. (10 August 2006) Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
Pulver, S (1986). Narcissism: The term and the concept. (In A Morrison (ed), Essential Papers on
Narcissism. New York: New York University Press, 91-111.)
Pyle, D W (1979). Intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Pynoos, R S (1994). Post-traumatic stress disorder: A clinical review. Lutherville: Sidran Press.
References 667
Pynoos, R S & Eth, S (1985). Children traumatized by witnessing acts of personal violence. (In Eth, S & Pynoos,
R S (eds), Post-traumatic stress disorder in children. Washington: American Psychiatric Press. 17-44.)
R
Radford, J & Russell, D (eds) (1992). Femicide: The politics of woman killing. New York: Twayne
Publishers.
Raidt, E H (1995). German speakers in South Africa, with special reference to KwaZulu-Natal.
(In R Mesthrie (ed), Language and social history. Cape Town: David Philip.)
Rasool, S, Vermaak, K, Pharoah, R, Louw, A & Stavrou, A (2002). Violence against women: A national
survey. Pretoria: Institute for Security Studies.
Reddy, S P, Panday, S, Swart, D, Jinabhai, C C, Amosun, S L, James, S, Monyeki, K D, Stevens, G,
Morejele, N, Kambaran, N S, Omardien, R G & Van den Borne, H W (2003). The South African youth
risk behaviour survey 2002. Cape Town: Medical Research Council.
Redish, A D (1999). Beyond the cognitive map: From place cells to episodic memory. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Reinisch, J M (1987). Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press.
Reisberg, D (2006). Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind. 3rd ed. New York: WW Norton & Co Inc.
Repacholi, B M & Gopnik, A (1997). Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds.
Developmental Psychology, 33, 12-21.
Republic of South Africa (1996a). The Constitution of the Republic of South Africa, 1996. Act 108 of 1996.
Cape Town: Government Printer.[Laws.]
Republic of South Africa (1996b). Choice on Termination of Pregnancy Act, No. 92 of 1996. Cape Town:
Government Printer. [Laws.]
Rest, J R (1974). Manual for the Defining Issues Test: An Objective Test of Moral Judgment Development.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.
Rest, J R (1983). Morality. (In P Mussen, J H Flavell & E M Markman (eds), Handbook of Child
Psychology. 4th ed. Vol. 3. New York: John Wiley.)
Rest, J R (1986a). Moral Development: Advances in Research and Theory. New York: Praeger.
Rest, J R (1986b). Moral Research Methodology. (In S Modgil & C Modgil (eds), Lawrence Kohlberg:
Consensus and Controversy. Philadelphia: Falmer Press.)
Rest, J R, Narvaez, D, Bebeau, M J & Thoma, S J (1999). Postconventional Moral Thinking: A Neo-
Kohlbergian Approach. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers.
Reyna, V F & Farley, F (2007). Is the teen brain too rational? Scientific American Mind, 59-65 Dec/Jan.
Rich, P B (1990). Race, science, and the legitimization of white supremacy in South Africa 1902-1940.
International Journal of African Historical Studies, 23(4), 665-686.
Richards, A & Dickson, A (eds) (1977).The Penguin Freud Library. Volume 7 on sexuality: Three essays on
the theory of sexuality and other works. London: Penguin.
Richards, K A M, Pillay, Y, Mazodze, O & Govere, A S M (2005). The impact of colonial culture in South
Africa and Zimbabwe on identity development. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 15(1), 41-51.
Richter, L (1994). Economic stress and its influence on the family and caretaking patterns. (In A Dawes &
D Donald (eds), Childhood and adversity. Cape Town: David Phillip. 28-50.)
Richter, L (2002). Strengthening infants and children: South African perspectives. Southern African
Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 14(2), 128-134.
Richter, L & Griesel, R (1994). Malnutrition, low birth weight and related influences on psychological
development. (In A Dawes & D Donald (eds), Childhood and diversity: Psychological perspectives from
South African research. Cape Town: David Philip. 66-91.)
Rieber, R W & Carton, A S (1987). Editors’ Foreword to The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume 1,
Problems of General Psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
Riegel, K (1984). Beyond formal operations. (In M L Commons, F A Richards, & C Armon (eds),
Beyond formal operations: Late adolescence and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger.)
Riegel, K F & Riegel, R M (1972). Development, drop and death. Developmental Psychology, 6, 306-319.
Robertson, J & Bowlby, J (1952). Responses of young children to separation from their mothers. Courr.
Cent. Int. Enf., 2, 131-142.
Robinson, D N (1981). An intellectual history of psychology. New York: Macmilian.
668 Developmental Psychology
S
Sabini, J (1995). Social psychology. 2nd ed. New York: Norton.
Sadock, B J & Sadock, VA (2007). Kaplan & Sadock’s synopsis of psychiatry (10th ed). Philadelphia,
Pennsylvania: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Salthouse, T A (1997). The processing speed theory of adult age differences in cognition.
Psychological Review, 103, 403-428.
Sameroff, A & Emde, R (1989). Relationship disturbances in early childhood. New York: Basic Books.
Sampson, E E (1992). The deconstruction of self. (In J Shotter & K J Gergen (eds), Texts of identity.
London: Routledge.
Sampson, R J (1985). Race and criminal violence: A demographically disaggregated analysis of urban
homicide. Crime & Delinquency, 31(1), 47-82.
Samuels, A (1985). Jung and the post-Jungians. London: Routledge.
References 669
Sharpe, S (1978). Just like a girl. How girls learn to be women. Middlesex: Penguin.
Shatz, M, Wellman, H M, & Silber, S (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs – a systematic investigation
of the 1st reference to mental state. Cognition, 14, 301-321.
Shaw, M (1998). Of crime and country: Reported crime trends in South Africa (1980-1997) and future
scenarios. Nedbank/ISS Crime Index, 2(3). Available: www.iss.co.za/Pubs/
CRIMEINDEX/98VOL2NO3/CrimeAndCountry.html. [10 January 2004]
Shefer, T (1997). The making of the gendered self. (In C De la Rey, N Duncan, T Shefer & A Van
Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African focus. Johannesburg:
International Thomson Publishing. 80-98.)
Shefer, T (2008). Adolescence. (In L Swartz, C De la Rey, N Duncan & L Townsend (eds), Psychology:
An introduction. Cape Town: Oxford University Press. 85-96.)
Shihadeh, E S & Ousey, G C (1998). Industrial restructuring and violence: The link between entry-level
jobs, economic deprivation, and black and white homicide. Social Forces, 77(1), 185-206.
Simeunovi -Pati , B J (2003). Homicides in Serbia within the context of social transition and war. Temida,
6(4), 33-45.
Simon, B & Simon, J (1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Singer, E (1992). Child-care and the psychology of development. London: Routledge.
Sinott, J D (1984). Postformal reasoning: The relativistic stage. (In M L Commons, F A Richards, &
C Armon (eds), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescence and adult cognitive development. New York:
Praeger.)
Sithole, M (1996). Moses Sithole’s Childhood. (In J S Grant (ed) Personal Communication. Boksburg.)
Sithole, M (1999). Personal communication. Personal interviews conducted by James Grant with Moses
Sithole in Pretoria Central Prison.
Skeels, H M & Dye, H B (1939). A study of the effects of differential stimulation on mentally retarded
children. Program of the American Association of Mental Deficiency , 44, 114-136.
Skinner, B F (1957). Verbal behaviour. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
Skinner, D & Swartz, L (1989). The consequences for preschool children of a parent’s detention:
A preliminary South African clinical study of caregivers’ reports. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 30 (2), 243-259.
Skotko, B G, Kensinger, E A, Locascio, J J, Einstein, G, Rubin, D C, Tupler, L A et al. (2004). Puzzling
thoughts for H.M.: Can new semantic information be anchored to old semantic memories?
Neuropsychology, 18, 756-769.
Skuy, M, Lomofsky, L, Fridjohn, P & Green, L (1993). Effectiveness of instrumental enrichment for the
preservice teachers in a disadvantaged South African community. International Journal of Cognitive
Education and Mediated Learning, 2 (2), 92-108.
Skuy, M, Mentis, M, Durbach, F, Cockcroft, M, Fridjohn, P & Mentis, M (1995). Crosscultural
comparisons of effects of instrumental enrichment on children in a South African mining town. School
Psychology International, 16, 265-282.
Slade, A (1999). Attachment theory and research: Implications for the theory and practice of individual
psychotherapy with adults. (In J Cassidy & P R Shaver (eds), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research
and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press.)
Slobin, D I (ed) (1985). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sluzenski, J, Newcombe, N S & Kovacs, S L (2006). Binding, relational memory, and recall of naturalistic
events: A developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 32, 89-100.
Smith, E E & Jonides, J (1998). Neuroimaging analyses of working memory. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 12061-12068.
Smith, M A, Sayre, L M, Monnier, V M & Perry, G (1995). Radical ageing in Alzheimer’s disease. Trends
in Neuroscience, 18, 172-176.
Snarey, J (1985). Cross-Cultural Universality of Social-Moral Development: A Critique Review of
Kohlbergian Research. Psychological Bulletin, 97(2), 202-232.
Snyman, C R (1989). Criminal law. Durban: Butterworths.
Sodian, B, Taylor, C, Harris, P L & Perner, J (1991). Early deception and the child’s theory of mind:
False trails and genuine markers. Child Development, 62, 468-483.
References 671
Solms, M (1995). Is the brain more real than the mind? Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 9, 107-20.
Solms, M & Saling, M (1990). A moment of transition: Two neuroscientific articles by Sigmund Freud.
London: Karnac Books.
Sorensen, P (1995). Thoughts on the containing process from the perspective of infant/mother relations.
Melanie Klein and Object Relations, 13, 1-16.
South African Police Services (SAPS) (2004). 2004 crime statistics. Available: http://www.saps.gov.za/
statistics/reports/crimestats/2004/crime_stats.htm [20 June 2005]
South African Police Services (SAPS) (2005). 2005 crime statistics. Available: http://www.saps.gov.za/
statistics/reports/crimestats/2005/crime_stats.htm [12 May 2006]
South African Police Services (SAPS) (2006). 2006 crime statistics. Available: http://www.saps.gov.za/
statistics/reports/crimestats/2006/crime_stats.htm [14 April 2007]
South African Police Services (SAPS) (2008). 2008 crime statistics. Available: http://www.saps.gov.za/
statistics/reports/crimestats/2008/crime_stats.htm [22 August 2008]
South African Truth and Reconciliation Commission (SATRC) (1998). Truth and Reconciliation
Commission of South Africa report. Cape Town: SATRC.
Sowell, E R, Delis, D, Stiles, J & Jernigan, T L (2001). Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe
maturation between children and adolescence: A structural MRI study. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 7, 312-322.
Spelke, E S, Phillips, A & Woodward, A L (1995). Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human
action. (In D Sperber & D Premack (eds) Causal cognition: a multidisciplinary debate. New York:
Clarendon. 44-78.)
Spencer, H (1886). The principles of psychology. Volume 5. New York: D. Appleton.
Spitz, R (1945). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood.
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1, 53-74.
Sproat, G M (1868). Scenes and studies of savage life. London.
Sroufe, L A (1979). Socioemotional development. (In J Osofsky (ed), Handbook of infant development.
New York: John Wiley.)
Sroufe, L A (1983). Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of adaptation in preschool: The roots of
maladaptation and competence. (In M Perlmutter (ed), Minnesota Symposia in Child Psychology. Volume
16. New Jersey: Erlbaum.)
Sroufe, L A (1990). An organizational perspective on the self. (In D Cicchetti & M Beeghly (eds), The self
in transition: Infancy to childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.)
Sroufe, L A (1996). Emotional development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sroufe, L A & Waters, E (1977). Attachment as an organisational construct. Child Development, 48, 1184-1199.
Sroufe, L A, Schork, E, Frosso, M, Lawroski, N & LaFreniere, P (1984). The role of affect in social
competence. (In C Izard, J Kagan, & R Zajonc (eds), Emotions, cognitions and behavior. New York:
Cambridge University Press.)
State v. Makwanyane 1995 (2) SACR 1 CC.
Stead, G (1996). Career development of black South African adolescents: a developmental-contextual
perspective. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74, 270-275.
Steele, S, Joseph, R & Tager-Flusberg, H (2003). Developmental change in theory of mind abilities in
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33.
Stern, D (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books.
Stern, D, Atwood, G & Brandchaft, B (1994), The Intersubjective Perspective. Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
Stern, D, Brandchaft, B & Atwood, G (1987). Psychoanalytic Treatment: An Intersubjective Approach.
Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Stern, S (1994). Needed relationships and repeated relationships: An integrated relational perspective.
Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 4, 317-345.
Sternberg, R J (1984). Towards a triarchic theory of human intelligence. Behavioural and brain sciences,
7, 269-315.
Sternberg, R J (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R J (1988). Intellectual development: Psychometric and information-processing approaches.
(In M H Bornstein & M E Lamb (eds), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook. New Jersey:
Erlbaum.)
672 Developmental Psychology
T
Tager-Flusberg, H (2007). Evaluating the theory-of-mind hypothesis of autism. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 16, 311-315.
Takahashi, K (1990). Are the key assumptions of the “strange situation” procedure universal? A view
from Japanese research. Human Development, 33, 23-30.
Teasdale, T W & Owen, D R (2005). A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance:
The Flynn Effect in reverse. Personality and Individual Differences, 39 (4), 837-843.
Teicholz, J (1999). Kohut, Loewald, and the Postmoderns: A Comparative Study of Self and Relationship.
Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Teplov, B M (1946). Psychological Aspects of Artistic Education. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds) (1963).
Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 259-280.)
Terr, L C (1983). Chowchilla revisited: The effects of psychic trauma four years after the school-bus
kidnapping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1543-1549.
Terr, L C (1991). Childhood Traumas – An Outline and Overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148,
10-20.
The Center for the Study of Ethical Development (2000). Defining issues test – 2. Available: http://edpsy.
coled.umn.edu/PSYCHF/CSED/.
Thoma, S J (1986). Estimating Gender Differences in the Comprehension and Preferences of Moral
Issues. Developmental Review, 6, 165-180.
Thoma, S J (2006). Research on the Defining Issues Test. (In M Killen & J Smetana (eds), Handbook of
Moral Development. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.)
Thomas, K G F (2003). Cognitive mapping and spatial navigation in patients with anterior temporal
lobectomy. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, 2002). Dissertation Abstracts International,
64, 2409.
Tolman, E C (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Tolman, E C (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and man. Psychological Review, 55, 189-208.
Tolpin, M (1993). The unmirrored self, compensatory structure, and cure: The exemplary case of Anna
O. Annual of Psychoanalysis, 21, 157-178.
Tolpin, P (1985). The primacy of the preservation of the self. (In A Goldberg (ed), Progress in Self
Psychology, Vol 1. New York: Guilford Press, 83-87.)
Tolpin, P (1988). Optimal affective engagement: The analyst’s role in therapy. (In A Goldberg (ed),
Learning from Kohut: Progress in Self Psychology, vol 4. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press, p160-168.)
Trivers, R L (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. (In B Campbell (ed). Sexual selection and the
Descent of Man, 1871-1971. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.)
Tronick, E, Bruschweiler-Stern, N, Harrison, A, Lyons-Ruth, K, Morgan, A, Nahum, J, Sander, L &
Stern, D N (1998), Dyadically expanded states of conciousness and the process of theraputic change.
Infant Mental Health J., 19: 290-299.
Tronik, E (1998). Interventions that effect change in psychotherapy: A model based on infant research.
Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 277-279.
Tudin, P, Straker, G & Mendolsohn, M (1994). Social and political complexity and moral development.
South African Journal of Psychology, 24(3), 163-168.
Tudin, P, Straker, G & Mendolsohn, M (1994). Social and Political Complexity and Moral Development.
South African Journal of Psychology, 24(3), 163-168.
Tulving, E (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. (In E Tulving & W Donaldson (eds), Organization of
memory (381-403). Oxford, England: Academic Press.)
Tulving, E & Donaldson, W (eds) (1972). Organisation of memory. London: Academic Press.
Turiel, E (2003). Resistance and Subversion in Everyday Life. Journal of Moral Education, 32, 115-130.
Turiel, E (2006). Thought, Emotions, and Social Interactional Processes in Moral Development. (In M
Killen & J Smetana (eds), Handbook of Moral Development. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.)
Turing, A M (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59, 433-460.
Turner, P J & Gervai, J (1995). A multidimensional study of gender typing in preschool children and their
parents: Personality, attitudes, preferences, behaviour and cultural differences. Developmental
Psychology, 31 (5), 759-772.
674 Developmental Psychology
U
UNAIDS (1999). Orphans due to AIDS, South Africa 1990–2010. Available: http://www.unaids.org/
publications/graphics/addis/sld016.htm.
UNAIDS (1999). Trends in HIV incidence and prevalence: Natural course of the epidemic or results of
behavioural Change? UNAIDS/99.12 E. Geneva: UNAIDS.
UNAIDS (2000a). Report on the global HIV/AIDS epidemic, UNAIDS, June 2000. Available: http://www.
unaids.org/epidemic_update/report/index.html.
UNAIDS (2000b). Aids epidemic update – December 2000. Available: http://www.unaids.org/epidemic_
update/report_dec00/index_dec.html#full.
UNICEF (n.d.) At a glance: United States of America; Unicef in the USA Statistics. Available: http://www.
unicef.org/infobycountry/usa_statistics.html. [February 25 2007].
V
Vail, L (ed) (1989). The creation of tribalism in Southern Africa. London: James Currey.
Van den Boom, D C (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and
exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers
with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457-1477.
Van der Merwe, A & Dawes, A (2007). Youth violence: A review of risk factors, causal pathways and
effective intervention. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 19(2), 95-113
van der Veer, R (1997). Introduction to The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume 3, Problems of the
Theory and History of Psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
Van Ijzendoorn, M H (1993). Commentary on Jackson (1993), “Multiple caregiving among African
Americans and infant attachment: The need for an emic approach.” Human Development, 36, 103-105.
Van Niekerk, A (1997). Traumatic spinal injury among youth. (In C De la Rey, N Duncan, T Shefer &
A Van Niekerk (eds), Contemporary issues in human development: a South African focus. Johannesburg:
International Thomson Publishing. 233-249.)
Van Rensberg E & Barnard, C (2005). Psychological resilience among sexually-molested girls in the late
middle-childhood: a case study approach. Child Abuse Research in South Africa, 6(1), 1-12.
Vaughn, B E & Bost, K K (1999). Attachment and temperament: Redundant, independent,
or interacting influences on interpersonal adaptation and personality development. (In J Cassidy
& P R Shaver (eds), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications. New York:
Guilford Press.)
Vetten, L (1996). “Man shoots wife”: Intimate femicide in Gauteng, South Africa. Crime & Conflict, 6, 1-4.
Vogelman, L (1990). The sexual face of violence. Johannesburg: Ravan Press.
Vygodskaia, G L (1995). Remembering father. Educational Psychologist, 30 (2), 57-59.
Vygotsky, L S (1956). Selected psychological investigations. Moscow: Izdstel’sto Akademii
Pedagogicheskikh Nauk.
Vygotsky, L S (1962). Thought and language. Harvard, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L S (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Vygotsky, L S (1988). The genesis of higher mental functions. (In K Richardson & S Sheldon (eds), Cognitive
development to adolescence. Hove: Erlbaum.)
Vygotsky, L S (1929). The Problem of the Cultural Development of the Child. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 6, 26-39.
Vygotsky, L S (1934). Learning and Mental Development at School Age. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds),
Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R.. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 23-34.)
Vygotsky, L S (1981). The instrumental method in psychology. (In J V Wertsch (ed), The concept of
activity in Soviet psychology. New York: Sharpe. 17-28.)
Vygotsky, L S (1987). The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume One. Problems of General Psychology.
New York: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L S (1993).The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume Two The Fundamentals of Defectology
(Abnormal Psychology and Learning Disabilities. New York: Plenum Press.)
Vygotsky, L S (1997a). The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume Three. Problems of the Theory and
History of Psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
References 675
Vygotsky, L S (1997b). The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume Four. The History of the Development
of Higher Mental Functions. New York: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L S (1998). The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume Five. Child Psychology. New York:
Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L S (1999). The Collected Works of L S Vygotsky. Volume Six. Scientific Legacy. New York:
Plenum Press.
W
Walker, L J (1984). Sex differences in the development of moral reasoning: A critical review. Child
Development, 55, 677–691.
Walker, L J (2006). Gender and Morality. (In M Killen & J Smetana (eds), Handbook of Moral
Development. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.)
Walker, L J, de Vries, B & Trevethan, S D (1987). Moral Stages and Moral Orientaions in Real Life and
Hypothetical Dilemmas. Child Development, 58, 842-858.
Wallace, R (1990). Urban desertification, public health and public order: ‘Planned shrinkage’, violent
death, substance abuse and AIDS in the Bronx. Social Science & Medicine, 31(7), 801-813.
Ward, C L, Flisher, A J, Zissis, C, Muller, M & Lombard, C (2001). Exposure to violence and its
relationship to psychopathology in adolescents. Injury Prevention, 7, 297-301.
Ward, G (1996). Teach yourself postmodernism. London: Hodder and Stoughton.
Waters, E & Cummings, E M (2000). A secure base from which to explore close relationships. Child
Development, 71 (1), 164-172.
Waters, E, Treboux, D, Crowell, J, Merrick, S & Albersheim, L (1995). From the strange situation to the
adult attachment interview: A 20-year longitudinal study of attachment security in infancy and early
adulthood. Stony Brook, New York : State University of New York. Unpublished manuscript.
Waters, E, Vaughn, B E & Egeland, B (1980). Individual differences in infant-mother attachment
relationships at age one: Antecedents in neonatal behaviour in an economically disadvantaged sample.
Child Development, 51, 208-216.
Weinfeld, N S, Sroufe, L A, Egeland, B & Carlson, E A (1999). The nature of individual differences in
infant-caregiver attachment. (In J Cassidy & P R Shaver (eds), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research
and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press.)
Weintraub, M, Pritchard Clemens, L, Sockloff, A, Ethridge, T, Gracely, E & Myers, B (1984). The
development of sex-role stereotypes in the third year: Relationship to gender labelling, gender identity,
sex-typed toy preference, and family characteristics. Child Development, 55, 1493-1503.
Weisstein, N (1973). Psychology constructs the female. (In P Brown (ed), Radical psychology. New York:
Harper and Row.)
Weiten, W (1991). Psychology: Themes and variations. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Wellman, H M & Estes, D (1986). Early understanding of mental entities—a re-examination of childhood
realism. Child Development, 57, 910-923.
Wellman, H M & Woolley, J D (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: the early development of
everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245-275.
Wenger, E (1999). Communities of practice. Learning, meaning and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Wertheimer, M (1961). Psychomotor co-ordination of auditory-visual space at birth. Science, 134, 1692.
Wertheimer, M (1961). ‘Psychomotor co-ordination of auditory-visual space at birth’. Science, 134, 1692.
Wertsch, J V & Tulviste, P (1996). L S Vygotsky and contemporary developmental psychology. (In H
Daniels (ed), An Introduction to Vygotsky. London: Routledge. 53-74.)
Wertsch, J V (1981). The concept of activity in Soviet Psychology. New York: Sharpe.
Wertsch, J V (1984). The Zone of Proximal Development: Some Conceptual Issues. (In B Rogoff &
J V Wertsch (eds), Children’s Learning in the “Zone of Proximal Development”. California: Jossey Bass.
9-16.)
Wertsch, J V (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard
University Press.
Wertsch, J V (1987). Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
676 Developmental Psychology
Wertsch, J V (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, Mass:
Harvard University Press.
Wertsch, J V (1999). The zone of proximal development: some conceptual issues. (In P Lloyd & C
Fernyhough (eds), Lev Vygotsky. Critical Assessments. London: Routledge. 67-80.)
Wertsch, J V (2005). Cole’s ‘Cross-Cultural and Historical Perspectives on the Developmental
Consequences of Education. Human Development, 48, 223-226.
West, D J & Farrington, D P (1973). Who becomes delinquent? London: Heinemann.
West, M, Livesley, W J, Reiffer, L & Sheldon, A (1986). The place of attachment in the life events model
of stress and illness’. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 202-207.
Westen, D (1996). Psychology: Mind, Brain and Culture. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Western, D (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed
psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124 (3), 333-371.
Western, D (1999). The scientific status of unconscious processes: Is Freud really dead? Journal of the
American Psychoanalytic Association, 47 (4), 1061-1112.
Wetherell, M & Griffin, C (1991). Feminist psychology and the study of men and masculinity: Part 1:
Assumptions and perspectives. Feminism and Psychology, 1, 361-393.
Whiten, A & Byrne, R W (1997). Machiavellian intelligence: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
WHO (2006). Regional report on HIV/AIDS prevalence 2005. Available: http://www.who.int/globalatlas/
includeFiles/generalIncludeFiles/listInstances.asp. [29 April 2008].
WHO (2008) World Health Statistics. Available: http://www.who.int/globalatlas/dataQuery/reportData.
asp?rptType=1. [5 August 2008].
Whorf, B L (1956). Science and linguistics. (In J B Carroll (ed), Language, thought and reality: Selected
writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.)
Widdershoven, G (1997). Models of human development. (In W Van Haaften, M Korthals & T Wren
(eds), Philosophy of development: reconstructing the foundations of human development and education.
Dordrecht: KluwerPublishers. 31-42.)
Widom, C S (1989). Child abuse, neglect, and violent criminal behavior. Criminology, 244, 160-166.
Wilkinson, R G, Kawachi, I & Kennedy, B P (1998). Mortality, the social environment, crime and
violence. Sociology of Health & Illness, 20, 578-597.
Wilson, F & Ramphele, M (1991). Uprooting poverty: The South African challenge. Cape Town:
David Philip.
Wilson, T & Strebel, A (2004). Psychiatric discourses of women abuse. South African Journal of Psychology,
34(3), 421-442.
Winnicott, D W (1965)[1960]. The theory of the parent-infant relationship. (In Maturational
processes and the facilitating environment. New York: International Universities Press.)
Winnicott, D W (1958). Collected papers: Through paediatrics to psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books.
Winnicott, D W (1958)[ 1951]. Transitional objects and transitional, phenomena. (In Collected papers:
Through paediatrics to psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books.)
Winnicott, D W (1958)[1945]. Primitive emotional development. (In Collected papers: Through pae-
diatrics to psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books.)
Winnicott, D W (1958)[1952]. Psychosis and child care. (In Collected papers: Through paediatrics to
psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books.)
Winnicott, D W (1958)[1956]. Primary maternal preoccupation. (In Collected papers: Through paediatrics
to psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books.)
Winnicott, D W (1964). The child, the family and the outside world. London: Penguin.
Winnicott, D W (1965). Maturational processes and the facilitating environment. New York: International
Universities Press.
Winnicott, D W (1965)[1960]. The capacity to be alone. (In Maturational processes and the facilitating
environment. New York: International Universities Press.)
Winnicott, D W (1965)[1960]. Ego distortions in terms of true and false self. (In Maturational
processes and the facilitating environment. New York: International Universities Press.)
Winnicott, D W (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.
Winnicott, D W (1974). Fear of breakdown. International Review of Psychoanalysis, 1, 103-107.
References 677
Wolf, E (1988). Treating the Self: Elements of Clinical Self Psychology. New York: Guilford.
Wolfgang, M E (1958). Patterns in criminal homicide. Philadelphia: University Of Pennsylvania.
Wolheim, R (1977). Freud. Glasgow: Fontana Modern Masters.
Women’s Health and Development (WHO) (1997). Violence against women. A priority health issue. Geneva:
World Health Organisation.
Wood, D J, Bruner, J S & Ross, G (1976). The role of tutoring in problem-solving. Journal of child
psychology, 17, 89-100.
Wood, J T (1994). Gendered lives: Communication, gender and culture. Belmont: Wadsworth.
Wood, K & Jewkes, R (2001a). Dangerous’ love: Reflections on violence among Xhosa township youth.
(In R Morrell (ed), Changing men in Southern Africa. Pietermaritzburg: University of Natal Press.
217-336.)
Wood, K & Jewkes, R (2001b). Violence, rape and sexual coercion: Everyday love in a South African
township. (In S M Whitehead & F J Barrett (eds), The masculinities reader. Cambridge: Polity Press.
133-140.)
Woodward, A L (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1-34.
Workman, L & Reader, W (2004). Evolutionary psychology: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
World Health Organisation (WHO) (1995). A picture of health? A review and annotated bibliography of the
health of young people in developing countries. Geneva: WHO.
Worsley, C L, Recce, M, Spiers, H J, Marley, J, Polkey, C & Morris, R G (2001). Path integration
following temporal lobectomy in humans. Neuropsychologia, 39, 452-464.
Wulfsohn, D (1988). The impact of the South African nanny on the young child. Pretoria: University of
South Africa. Unpublished doctoral thesis.
Wundt, W (1874/1904). Principles of physiological psychology. Leipzig: Englemann.
Wyngaard, G C, Van Niekerk, A, Bulbulia, S, Van der Walt, C, Masuku, S, Stevens, G & Seedat, M
(2000). Learners’ perceptions of safety and feelings of safety in three schools in a peri-urban setting in the
Western Cape. Paper presented at the 9th International Conference on Safe Communities, Dhaka,
Bangladesh.
X
Xaba, T (2001). Masculinity and its malcontents: The confrontation between ‘struggle masculinity’ and
‘post-struggle masculinity’ (1990-1997). (In R Morrell (ed), Changing men in Southern Africa.
Pietermaritzburg: University of Natal Press. 105-124.)
Y
Yerkers, R M (1923). Testing the human mind. Atlantic Monthly, 131, 358-370.
Yule, W (2003). Early intervention strategies with traumatised children, adolescents and families. (In
R Orner & U Schnyder (eds), Reconstructing early interventions after trauma. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.)
Yule, W, Perrin, S & Smith, P (1999). Post-traumatic stress reactions in children and adolescents.
(In W Yule (ed), Post-traumatic stress disorders: Concepts and therapy. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.)
Z
Zankov, L V (1957). Combination of the Verbal and Visual in Teaching. (In B Simon & J Simon (eds)
(1963). Educational Psychology in the U.S.S.R.. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 243-258.)
Zeanah, C H, Mammen, O K & Lieberman, A F (1993). Disorders of attachment. (In C H Zeanah (ed),
Handbook of infant mental health. New York: Guilford.)
Index
Entries are listed in letter-by-letter alphabetical order. Page references in italic indicate where you can find a figure, table or
photograph relating to the index entry term.
Bronfenbrenner, Urie 501 see also rationalism 316-317 constitutional factors and
ecological theory of development stability versus plasticity 320-321 trauma 551-552
Bruner, Jerome 381-382, 479 structuralism 317 constructivist theory 324-326
Buddhism 135 Wundt, Wilhelm 317 see also Piaget, Jean
cognitive mapping theory 445-446 accommodation 327-328
cognitive science adaptation 326, 327-328
C artificial intelligence (AI) 439 animism 332
captation 274 Asperger’s Syndrome 453 assimilation 327-328
castration anxiety 46, 51-52, 53 Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model centration 339
Cattell, Raymond 350-351 of memory 438, 439 circular responses 328-331, 330
child-headed households 568-569 autistic spectrum disorders concrete operational stage 328,
childhood see also (ASD) 453-455 336-337
childhood development autobiographical memory conservation 337
assumptions of 564-567 (AM) 441-448 criticisms of 341-343
constructions of 564-567 Baron-Cohen, Simon 452-453 cultural bias 343
gender identity in 609-610 binding tasks 446-448 deferred imitation 332
imposing adult categories on childhood amnesia 443-448 egocentricity 332
570-571 cognitive mapping theory 445-446 equilibration 327-328
memory development in 371-373 connectionist models 440-441, 440 formal operations stage 328,
psychoanalytic theories 29-30 definition of 441 337-340
sexuality in 566 Dennett, Daniel 448-449 imitation 332
significance of 29-30 episodic memory 442-443 intuitive stage 332-333
view of 564-566 false belief test 448-449 irreversibility 333
violent crime in 534-535, 535 Faux Pas test 452-453 metacognition 337
childhood amnesia 443-448 see also first-order mental state numeracy, principles of 335-336
infantile amnesia inferences 452 object permanence 331
childhood development General Problem Solver (GPS) 439 operations 326
apartheid’s effects on 509-510, Happé, Francesca 452 organisation 327
539-540, 540 hippocampal maturation 444-448 pendulum problem 338-339, 338
domestic violence and 541-542 infantile amnesia 443-448 post-formal operations
power and 582-583 information-processing approach stage 340-341
South African context 539-542 437-438, 439 preconceptual stage 332-333
trauma and 546-549 intentional action 450 preoperational stage 328, 332-336
childhood resilience see resilience nodes 440-441 reversibility 333
chimpanzees 267-268, 383-384 parallel distributed processing risk-taking behaviour 340
Chomsky, Noam 379-380, 380, 426 (PDP) 440-441, 440 schemes 326
circular responses 328-331, 330 psychobiology 436-437 sensorimotor stage 328-332
cognition, definition of 314-315 referential versus representational seriation 337
cognitive development 314-318 understanding 451 stages of cognitive development
analogies 319 second-order mental state 328-340, 341-342
assumptions 318-321 inferences 452 symbolic representation 331,
behaviourism 317 semantic memory 442-443 332-341
constructive process 321 serial processing 439 transductive reasoning 332-333
critique of 321-322 social cognition 449 transitive inference 337
culture and 590-591 theory of mind 448-456 contact crimes see violent crime
definition of 315 Turing test 437 containing 118, 119, 120, 122, 126-128
empiricism 316-317 cognitive-social theory 398 contextual approaches 501-502, 620,
evolutionary psychology and cohesive self 186-187 625-634
430-431 cohort, definition of 14, 629 contextual sub-theory 356-357
experimental research 319-320 collective unconscious 237, 238 continuity versus discontinuity 11-12
Freud, Sigmund and 321 complexes 238-239 conventional morality 401-402, 404
gender identity formation 611 componential sub-theory 355-356 corps morcele 266
intentionality 318 connectionism 382-383, 382 creativity and intelligence 362-363
introspection 316, 317 connectionist models 369-370, crime, definition of 515-516
James, William 317 440-441, 440 see also violent crime
language development and 381 conscience 59-60 critical developmental psychology
mind-brain debate 317-318 consciousness 54, 56, 236, 463-468 497-500, 616
moral development and 398-399 constitutional capacity critical periods 12-13, 602-603
psychoanalytic theories 33-34 see innate capacity crystallised intelligence 350-351
680 Developmental Psychology
telegraphic speech 385 multistore model of memory moral development theory 396-397
theories of language acquisition 367, 367 autonomous moral reasoning 399
378-383 prenatal memory 370 behaviourism 398
thought and 392-394 procedural memory 366 cognitive developmental
visual reading strategy 387 schema theory 367-368 approach 398-399
Vygotsky, Lev 381-382, scripts 368 cognitive-social theory 398
383-384, 394 semantic memory 365-366 conventional/role conformity
Learning Propensity Assessment semantic network models 368 morality 401-402, 404
Device (LPAD) 351 senile dementia 377 criminal conduct and 411
learning theory 379 sensory store 367 critiques of 407-414
libidinal ego 96-97 short-term memory 365 cultural bias 410-412
libido 43, 90, 178 Vygotsky, Lev 374-375 Defining Issues Test (DIT) 407
life-span perspective 6-8 memory strategies 373-376 four component model 413-414
links (Bion’s theory) 123, 126-128 mentalisation 114-115 gender and 408-410
long-term memory 365-366, 366 mental mechanism see defences Gilligan, Carol 408-410
Love (Bion’s theory) 123-125 mesosystems 506 heteronomous moral reasoning 400
LPAD see Learning Propensity metacognition 337, 376 intuitionism 398
Assessment Device (LPAD) microsystems 505 Moral Judgment Interview
militarisation 529 (MJI) 407
mimicry 272 Piaget, Jean 398-399
M mind-brain debate 317-318 postconventional/principled
macro-political factors and mirroring needs 176, 188 morality 402-403, 405
violent crime 528-529 mirror stage theory 30, 261-262, preconventional/premoral morality
macrosystems 506-507 280-281 see also Lacan, Jacques 400, 404
manic defence 84, 84 aggressivity 271, 276-278 psychodynamic theory 398
Marx, Karl 461-462 alienation 278-279 reasoning and action 412-413
mechanistic approaches 620, captation 274 Rest, J R 413-414
621-622, 622 chimpanzees 267-268 stages of 399-407, 404-405
meconnaissance 274-275 corps morcele 266 transitional stages 406-407
media and gender identity 614 critiques of 281 moratorium 300
Mediated Learning Experience ego 262-264 motherese see infant-directed speech
(MLE) 482 enabling function of image 271-272 motherhood 497 see also good-enough
mediating factors affecting trauma fragmented body 276 mothering
551-554 gestalt 269 motivational systems 203-205
mediation 483 hommelette 266 multilingualism 380-381
memory development identification 273-274, 277 multi-person systems of interaction
adults 372-373 image of self 266-271 502-504
Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model of individuation 265 multiple intelligences 357-359
memory 367, 367, 438, 439 instinctual trigger 271-272 multistore model of memory 367, 367
Bartlett, Frederick 367-368 meconnaissance 274-275 mutual regulation 204-205
in childhood 371-372 mimicry 272 myths 240, 270
children, older 372-373 narcissism 263, 270-271
connectionist models 369-370 newborn babies 264-266
declarative memory 366 prematuration 265 N
definition of memory 365-366 reality principle 263-264 narcissism 173-180, 184-186, 263,
dishabituation 370-371 rivalry 277, 278 270-271
elderly people 376-377 separation 265 nascent capacity for thought 119-120
encoding specificity principle 372 statue metaphor 269-270 nativism 8, 379-380, 423
episodic memory 365-366 superego 264 natural selection 418-420
explicit memory 366, 372 transvitism 278 nature-nurture debate 8-10, 417-418
false memory syndrome 369 MI theory see multiple intelligences neglect see trauma
habituation-dishabituation MLE see Mediated Learning neonates 10 see also newborn babies
paradigm 370-371 Experience (MLE) neuroses 30, 62-63, 248-249
implicit memory 366, 372 mnemonics see memory strategies newborn babies 10, 17-19, 217,
infants 370-371 modularity 422-423 264-266, 384
long-term memory 365-366, 366 monotropism 228-229 nodes 368, 440-441
memory strategies 373-376 moral defence 98-99 non-psychotic parts of self 130-131
metacognition 376 moral development and normality 574-578
models 367-370 violent crime 522 norms, definition of 574, 623
684 Developmental Psychology
secure attachment 219-221, 521 social constructionist approaches 620, toddlers see early childhood
secure base 218 634-636 ToM see theory of mind
self disorders 197-201 social control see social regulation tools (Vygotsky’s theory) 463-468, 466,
self-doubt 290 Social Darwinism 423-424 477, 481
selfhood 76 social interaction 468-469 topographical model 54-56, 56
selfobject experiences 187-190 social interactionism 381-382 transductive reasoning 332-333
self psychology 184-186 social ontogenetic framework 595-596 transitional object 146-147
see also Kohut, Heinz social regulation 496-497, 571-574 transitional phenomena 146-147
adversarial transferences 195 social structure and gender 602 transitional space 146-147
aggression 190-191 social support 552-554 transvitism 278
analytic third 207 social transitions 526 trauma
case studies 195-197, 209-212 sociobiology 424 adolescence 549-550
cohesive self 186-187 socio-economic factors and gender constitutional factors 551-552
critique of 208-209 608-609 corporal punishment 554
defence 201-202 sociohistorical development 469-476 definition of 542
development of self 186-191 sociopolitical issues 633-634 dissociation 556
developments in 202-207 South Africa domestic violence 558-560
disorders 197-201 self psychology in 207-208 early childhood 546-548
disruption-restoration cycle 193 theoretical frameworks in 619-637 externalising response 549-550
empathy 191-192 trauma in 539-542, 540 HIV/AIDS and 551-552
fragmenting self 186-187, 198 violent crime in 516-518, 517 infants 545-546
guilt 190-191 Spearman’s two-factor model of intellectual factors 552
idealising transferences 194 intelligence 348-349, 348 internalising response 550
intersubjectivity 205-207 stability versus change debate 6 malnutrition 551
mirror transferences 194 stability versus plasticity 320-321 mediating factors 551-554
motivational systems 203-205 standard social science model 423 middle childhood 548-549
mutual regulation 204-205 Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale 347 physical abuse 557-558
narcissism 184-186 statue metaphor 269-270 physiological factors 551-552
optimal frustration / responsiveness stereotypes of gender 604, 605, 608 Post-Traumatic Stress
193-194 Sternberg, Robert 354-357, 354 Disorder 543
overburdened self 200 Strange Situation 219-221, 220 preschoolers 546-548
overstimulated self 198-200 stress see trauma primary-school children 548-549
pole of ambitions 189 structural model 54, 57-60, 57, 59 ‘pseudo-mature’ behaviour 558
rage 190-191 structure of intellect model recovery from 550-554
regulation 204-205 349-350, 349 regression 545
resistance 201-202 structure of the mind 54-60 resilience 561
selfobject experiences 187-190 sublimation 47, 61 sensitisation of neural paths 555
selfobject transferences 194-197 substance abuse see drugs and sexual abuse 560-561
self structures 197 violent crime social support 552-554
shame 191 superego 57, 57, 59-60, 59, 63, South African context 539-542, 540
in South Africa 207-208 164-165, 264 symptoms of 542
twinship transferences 194-195 symbolic representation 331, 332-341 toddlers 545-548
understimulated self 200-201 symbols 237, 239, 244, 247-248 Type I trauma 542-554
vicarious introspection 191-192 symptom formation, model of 62 Type II trauma 542-543, 554-561
self representation 156-157 systemic forms of influence 502-504 triarchic theory of intelligence 354-357
semantic memory 365-366, 442-443 trieb 39
semantic network models 368 true self 138, 148-149
semantic slanting 393 T truth 123-125
separation 265 temperament 224, 520-521 Tsotsi 157-158
sexual abuse 560-561 see also trauma Terman, Lewis 346, 346 twinship transference 195
sexual aim 40-41 theoretical frameworks in South Africa two-person system see dyadic
sexual identity 428-430, 429 619-637 relationship
sexuality 39-42, 428-430, 566 theory of mind 448-456 Type I trauma 542-554
sexual object 41-42 theory of motivation 90-92 Type II trauma 554-561
shame 191, 290 Theseus 9
signs 466-467, 481 thought 114-119, 392-394
Simon, Theodore 346-347 Thurstone’s primary mental abilities U
Skinner, B F 379, 379 349 unconscious 31-33, 38-40, 54, 56, 56,
social capital and violent crime 524-525 Tinker Bell 172-173 126, 236-237
686 Developmental Psychology