Women Over 50 - Psychological Perspectives
Women Over 50 - Psychological Perspectives
Women Over 50 - Psychological Perspectives
Psychological Perspectives
Women Over 50
Psychological Perspectives
Edited by
Varda Muhlbauer
Netanya Academic College
Netanya, Israel
and
Joan C. Chrisler
Connecticut College
New London, Connecticut, USA
Varda Muhlbauer Joan C. Chrisler
Netanya Academic College Department of Psychology
Kiryat Yitzhak Rabin Connecticut College
1 University Street 270 Mohegan Avenue
Netanya, 42365, Israel New London, CT 06320
Email: vardam@netvision.net.il USA
Email: jcchr@conncoll.edu
All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written
permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC., 233 Spring Street, New York,
NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use
in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer
software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden.
The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks and similar terms, even if they
are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are
subject to proprietary rights.
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
springer.com
Foreword
It is truly remarkable that so little has happened in feminist studies in the last 20
years to rectify the dearth of psychological literature on older women. So it is with
great pleasure that I note the publication of this book, which addresses so many
of the needs that have gone begging for so long. But before I go forward to laud
this volume, I’d like to go back in time to my own first reckonings that there was
a great gap in the developmental story related to women’s lives.
As I celebrated my 40th birthday and finished my graduate studies in the late
1970s, I became especially sensitive to the issue of women and aging. I was
embarking on my dissertation research, which involved studying people between
the ages of 65 and 100, most of whom were women, who were living either in
their own homes, in retirement villages, or in nursing homes. As I did my literature
review, I was struck by the paucity of research on women once they had reached a
certain age. Like old soldiers, older women just seemed to fade away. From the time
they had left puberty and found a life mate, the developmental psychology story had
fewer and fewer episodes in each succeeding decade. What little was written tended
to focus on loss—appearance, reproductive capacities, meaningful activity, sexual
interest, mental stability, marital relationships, and, finally, cognitive capacities.
Women were said to endure the pain of the “empty nest,” menopause, loneliness,
widowhood, and depression. It seemed that there was nothing nice to say; so why
say much of anything at all?
In 1990 I wrote my article “Finished at 40” as a way of headlining the lack of
presence of maturing women in the psychology literature. The title characterized
the absence of both research and theory related to women’s lives and also the
perception that women’s growth and development had already stopped by age 40.
They were simply “finished,” that is “used up,” in the vernacular meaning of the
word. Although I knew that there was some hyperbole in my framing of the title,
I wanted the thesis to be provocative and to incite action. I hoped that feminist
psychologists (and others) would pay more attention to older women’s lives, and,
most especially, I wanted more favorable, creative, and productive images cast for
them.
As I myself was entering into this supposedly bleak and endless tunnel of loss,
I recognized strong and contrary feelings of energy and potential within me. I
v
vi Foreword
wasn’t going downhill; I was going up. Indeed I was about to enter a wonderful
new phase of my professional life. And I was not alone. Around me were my
friends and colleagues who were, for the most part, also vigorous, creative, and
enthusiastic about their lives. Also reinforcing this alternative vision for what
women might be were the data from my dissertation research. When asked what
their most wonderful time of life was, the overwhelming choice for my women
respondents was age 55. They loved that time, they said, because of their freedom
from worry and the need to care for children and husbands; they described their lives
at 55 as full of wealth, health, and activities galore. Some of them had interesting
jobs; many had leadership roles in religious, civic, and social organizations. They
traveled, learned new skills, and seemed to have a great deal of fun. Many relished
the fact that worries about getting pregnant were behind them, and they spoke
enthusiastically about their happy marriages, their grown-up children, and, most
especially, their grandchildren. Most of the women in my study continued to have
rewarding, active, and satisfying lives, although there were more challenges as they
reached their 70s, 80s, and 90s. I recall, however, that two of the most interesting
and intellectually lively participants were 90 and 100 years old, respectively.
Another research project involving women in their late 40s substantiated my
view that life was getting better, not worse. As part of a study of the de-
medicalization of menopause among the “patients-in-waiting,” I asked women
about their mood states at ages 20, 35, and at the present. For all of the partici-
pants, the least satisfying time of their lives was age 35 (Gergen, 1989). Apparently
being a “soccer mom” was not the wonderful period of life that some might imag-
ine. It helped to have a station wagon, but it didn’t take away the longing to be
free of some of the daily obligations that conspired against one’s sense of hav-
ing a personal identity or a personal life. Most were happy to have their children
growing up, although they did anticipate the future with some trepidation; this was
often brought about by their exposure to medical information about menopause
and aging.
Today, I think if I could change the title of my article, it would be “Finished
at 50,” in that menopause is now the more common marker for the disappearance
act. I might even be more optimistic and delete “finished” altogether. Since that
article was published, the public, developmental psychologists, and gerontologists
alike have concurred that 50 is the new 40; 60 is the new 50, etc. Middle age
has been extended as well, and nobody wants to claim they are old anymore.
There are many reasons for this recalibration, including better health care, “the
pill,” cosmetic surgery, women’s liberation from certain social expectations (thanks
to the feminist movement to a great extent), and a greater public acceptance of
gender equality, especially in the workplace. There is an expansion of women’s
roles to include a professional life as well as a family one. The “social clock” of
which Neugarten (1979) spoke has lost its Greenwich standard time. Parenting has
become a multigenerational activity, as have attaining educational diplomas and
changing career choices. Even sexual orientation is open for reconsideration in
later years. One’s social credit, at least for middle and upper middle class people,
is, I think, enhanced by the denunciation of “traditional roles.” Recently a friend of
Foreword vii
mine got remarried, took her husband on an adventure tour to Egypt to celebrate his
75th birthday, and became the head of her condominium board, all while pursuing
her career as a painter; another has used her legal skills to adopt a child from
Europe at age 53; a third, a new grandmother, has quit her job as a doctor to spend
some time training horses and improving her dressage skills with her daughter. In
all of these cases, middle age has only given these women the courage to engage
in life to the fullest.
An exciting aspect of Women Over 50: Psychological Perspectives is that editors
Varda Muhlbauer and Joan Chrisler, are advancing similar viewpoints, sharing with
readers how blurred the age-group boundaries are and how women are defying
limiting role expectations. Another important virtue of this volume is that, in
itself, it creates a new climate for considering the meaning of aging. As people
are exposed to this message, they become more open to re-evaluating their own
lives and those of others with whom they live and work. Every promotion of
this more positive vision of aging is critical in shifting a formerly devastating
stereotype of the aging woman to one that is much more optimistic and powerful.
In my own case, I am editing an electronic newsletter called the Positive Aging
Newsletter (www.positiveaging.net) with Kenneth J. Gergen; in it we have set
about reconstructing the stereotype of aging so that it is a time of generativity
and strength. Instead of a downward spiral from a good life in youth and young
adulthood, through middle age, to old age, and finally to death, we redefine aging as
having an upward trajectory. To bolster our vision, we include material gathered
from the social and biological sciences, as well as from news articles, Web sites,
and books that find benefits and potentials in aging. Let others advertise the deficit
discourses; that’s not for us.
For me, “Finished at 40” was a clarion call for what I hoped would be a whole
new world of research and theory making within the psychological community. At
last, 16 years later, there seems to be a response. Women Over 50: Psychological
Perspectives is elaborating the benefits, as well as the challenges, to women as
they age. The authors advance the idea that this segment of the population should
be taken seriously in terms of their political and social power. They are not simply
patients waiting for the worst to befall them. Although it is extremely difficult to
shake the prevailing problem-centered approach to aging, I commend the authors
on their efforts, and wish them great success in this venture, now and in future
editions. I also thank the editors for inviting me to write this foreword, which has
encouraged me to reflect on this crucial period in my own development as a scholar
and as a woman.
Mary Gergen
References
Gergen, M. (1989). Talking about menopause: A dialogic analysis. In L. E. Thomas (Ed.),
Research on adulthood and aging: The human sciences approach (pp. 65–87). Albany,
NY: State University of New York Press.
viii Foreword
Gergen, M. (1990). Finished at 40: Women’s development within the patriarchy. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 14, 471–493.
Neugarten, B. (1979). Time, age, and the life cycle. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136,
887–894.
Contents
Contributors ........................................................................ xi
Introduction ........................................................................ 1
Varda Muhlbauer and Joan C. Chrisler
ix
x Contents
xi
xii Contributors
women’s issues from a feminist perspective. She was among the initiators of a
pioneering center in Israel that conducted programs designed to advance women
in the workplace and to aid women in distress. The center was based on principles
of feminist social psychology and utilized cognitive–behavioral interventions.
Suzanna Rose is Professor of Psychology and Director of Women’s Studies at
Florida International University, where she teaches courses on gender and social
psychology. She has published dozens of articles and book chapters, and she is best
known for her work on friendship, love scripts, gay and lesbian issues, and the ways
that gender, sexual orientation, and race affect interpersonal relationships. The
advice contained in her book Career Guide for Women Scholars has contributed
to the success of many women faculty.
Judith A. Sugar is Associate Professor of Public Health at the University of
Nevada—Reno, where she teaches courses on aging. As a life span developmental
psychologist, she has published dozens of articles and book chapters on aging,
cognition, and health, and she is best known for her work on quality of life in
frail elders, aging and memory, faculty retirement, and gerontology education in
universities and colleges. She has held many leadership roles in academia and
professional organizations.
Cheryl Brown Travis is Professor of Psychology and Chair of the Women’s Stud-
ies Program at the University of Tennessee—Knoxville, where she teaches courses
on the psychology of gender and social psychology. She has published extensively
on women’s health issues, particularly medical practice patterns, equity, and qual-
ity of health care delivery. She authored a pioneering two-volume text on Women
and Health Psychology; her most recent books are Evolution, Gender, and Rape
and Sexuality, Society, and Feminism.
Introduction
Varda Muhlbauer and Joan C. Chrisler
The shift in the cultural representations and real life experiences of women at
midlife is much more than a passing trend. Rather, it is the product of a substantial
transformation in the sociocultural construction of collective gendered-age iden-
tity, which has given rise to a blurring and diversification of both age and gender
roles. By moving away from the traditional sociocultural construct of middle-age,
the newly evolving gendered-age representations of women over 50 have gener-
ated positive meanings and more liberal behavior codes. This is a welcome move
toward a better-balanced representation of this age group of women.
As committed feminist researchers, we hold to the long-established claim that the
existing political constellation (i.e., male supremacy), which is clearly indicative
of an uneven balance of power, is at the core of the sociocultural representations
of both gender and age. Indeed the connection cannot be overstated. Our thesis
endorses Sampson’s (1993) assertions that representations, as evidenced in the
current discourse on power, constitute the foundations of what people have come
to accept as reality. Thus, the reconstruction of gendered-age representations of
women over 50, which defines a better and more equitable division of power and
greater personal and collective freedom, ultimately may lead to a fundamentally
changed reality.
The present cohort of women over 50 has benefited from the accomplishments
achieved in the wake of the massive societal changes of the last four decades,
including the feminist movement, the gay rights movement, and the sexual revo-
lution of the 1960s and 1970s and the more recent fight against ageism (including
the baby boomers’ refusal to see themselves as aging). The feminist movement
made major strides in the reconstruction of gender roles on a more egalitarian ba-
sis. This influential development facilitated the empowerment of women mainly
through educational and professional gains (Barnett & Hyde, 2001). Currently, as
part of the antiageist revolution, the empowered cohort of women in midlife is
rewriting its own collective identity (Ashmore et al., 2004) and thus expanding its
boundaries and experiences through discursive processes.
The current shift in the representations and lifestyles of middle-aged women
is marked, and already has spawned what might be called “a new middle-aged
collective identity.” A quick Internet search revealed the ways in which women in
1
2 Varda Muhlbauer and Joan C. Chrisler
midlife (a term with flexible boundaries) have differentiated themselves from “old
women.” Key words such as “elderly women” bring up different Web sites (often
medically oriented) than do key words such as “middle-aged women” or “women
over 50.” It was a pleasant surprise to discover how many sites intended for women
over 50 are concerned with romance, sex, friendship, and general joie de vivre.
Some of the nonacademic sites relate to an active sexuality, which is almost de
rigueur for this age group of women. The Internet thus provides a fine illustration
of what Mitchell and Helson (1990) referred to as the prime of life.
Transformative sociocultural discourse also has been successful in shifting the
focus away from menopause to other age-related issues. Moreover, menopause
itself has been given alternative interpretations that tend to reflect more closely
the authentic attitudes of women in this age group. Many women enjoy the new
freedom and possibilities that are open to them with the termination of the re-
productive phase of their lives. The fact that this is in sharp contrast to traditional
perceptions of menopausal women is another product of the overall transformation
of women’s place in society.
Therefore, it comes as no surprise that women currently in midlife (however
they define it) differ considerably from midlife women of previous generations.
Personal recollections of our own mothers and other women in their generation
readily confirm this change. When middle-aged women today look at photographs
of their mothers, their response often is: “I can’t be the same age now that she
was in that picture . . . she looks so much older.” Of course, women of today are in
better physical health and wear more youthful fashions than women of yesteryear,
but the difference between us and our mothers is much more than meets the eye.
We have experienced a psychological makeover facilitated by the sociocultural
changes in gender equity.
It goes without saying that there is great variability within the generation that has
been privileged to undergo the cultural transformation that has bettered the quality
of our lives. Many women cannot claim achievements in power domains, either
institutional or personal, and, consequently, in well-being. However, on the whole,
aspects of the transformation seem to be inclusive enough to trigger the need to
take the pulse of this generation and raise questions related to all pertinent spheres
of women’s lives. We must remain mindful of the fact that women in midlife—
both collectively and individually—are being thrust into novel gendered-age roles
for which they have little training and about which they have scant information.
Therefore, we have invited feminist scholars to share their knowledge about midlife
women and to help us to think through the sum of what we know, and what we
need to learn, about the group at the center of this remarkable transformation.
The word “aging” typically brings to mind changes in the body—both superficial
(e.g., gray hair, wrinkles) and organic (e.g., chronic illness, infirmity). Thus, we
begin with several chapters that concern aspects of midlife women’s embodiment.
Joan Chrisler examines the available research on body image issues of women
over 50. She finds that the research that exists is generally not specific to this age
group and that how well or poorly women seem to adjust to age-related changes
in their bodies may depend on how the data are acquired. There is a lot yet to be
Introduction 3
done to understand how satisfied midlife women are with their bodies and how to
assist those who are dissatisfied to learn to care for and about their bodies more
positively.
Maureen McHugh exposes myths and stereotypes about sex and romance at
midlife. She shows readers how to consider women’s sexuality in context and
explains why it should not be measured by androcentric standards. Sexual urges
and the need to give and receive affection do not disappear with age, and the
evidence shows that all most women need to enjoy active sex lives is a willing and
caring partner and their own ability to define for themselves what form lovemaking
will take.
Midlife is a time when many chronic illnesses are first diagnosed, and some of
those illnesses are more likely to be experienced by women than by men. Susan
Lonborg and Cheryl Travis review some of the recent research on women’s health
at midlife and put that work in the context of medicine’s historic neglect of women’s
health conditions. They offer a number of suggestions about the use of self-care
to prevent the development (or worsening) of chronic illnesses.
Self-care includes fun as well as work. Ruth Hall encourages us all to remember
that in her chapter on exercise and leisure activities. Exercise is an important
form of physical illness prevention, and it also helps to keep us mentally alert
and psychologically healthy. When today’s midlife women were children it was
considered unusual for girls and women to take up sports, and therefore we may
have to struggle at first before we get to the point where we enjoy our daily exercise
routines. If that sounds familiar, let Coach Hall give you a pep talk!
Exercise, sexual activity, and physical health all contribute to women’s well-
being and quality of life. Varda Muhlbauer shows us how expanded gender roles,
empowerment, self-confidence, positive media images and messages, and the en-
hanced freedom of self-definition that more liberal lifestyles provide can impact
the quality of life of middle-aged women. Women over 50 today have a greater
opportunity than previous generations could imagine to determine the course of
their lives and pursue their own aims. And we are seizing those opportunities!
Women’s lives at any age include varied roles and relational contexts. Suzanna
Rose examines the role of friendships in middle-aged women’s lives. The end of
childrearing often means more time to spend with friends, and, as we age, our
friends often become more dear to us. Yet, until recently, women’s friendships
were a neglected topic of research, and, even now, we know less about friendship
at midlife than we do about the role of friends in other age groups. Perhaps the
existence of the Red Hat Society will spark an interest in this topic. Wouldn’t that
be an interesting group to study?
Grandmotherhood is a role many women eagerly await, and it is often experi-
enced for the first time in midlife. Liat Kulik reviews the research on grandpar-
enthood, often referred to as a “roleless role” because its rights and obligations
are more open to interpretation than is true of many other roles. Some grand-
mothers rarely see their grandchildren and communicate with them infrequently
via telephone or e-mail. Others live nearby and are an important part of their
grandchildren’s lives, especially at holidays or when last-minute babysitters are
4 Varda Muhlbauer and Joan C. Chrisler
needed. Still others end up raising their grandchildren in their own homes. Little
is known about how contemporary grandmothers might differ from grandmothers
of previous generations in the way they create this role for themselves.
Midlife is often referred to as “the sandwich generation” because middle-aged
women often have caregiving responsibilities for children still at home (or for
grandchildren whose parents need help raising them) and for their own parents,
who maybe ill or disabled. Rosalie Ackerman and Martha Banks provide poignant
examples of busy midlife women whose lives were disrupted by the call to care
for a friend or family member who needed their assistance. The acts of giving and
receiving care are both beneficial and stressful. This chapter will help us to think
through ways to prepare for the caregiving that most of us will have to give and
receive as we age.
Midlife is usually the peak earning point of people’s careers and the time when
they accomplish the most. It is also the time when people begin to plan seriously for
retirement. Judith Sugar explains why so little is known about women’s retirement
planning and adjustment—after all, we can never really “retire” from our traditional
work as homemakers. She critiques the androcentric perspective of most of the
available research and exposes the sexism in retirement policies. This chapter is
a must read for any woman who hopes to retire, and it provides a blueprint for
activism to promote greater equity for all working women.
The life experience and self-confidence that women have earned by the time
they reach midlife are often experienced as empowering, and this empowerment
can be used effectively to make positive changes in one’s own life, in the lives of
others around us, and in the larger society. Florence Denmark and Maria Klara
review the feminist research on empowerment, explain how women can increase
their empowerment by working collectively, and suggest ways that midlife women
can use their empowerment for the public good. They provide inspiring examples
of what some empowered women over 50 have accomplished.
We are delighted that Mary Gergen accepted our invitation to provide a preface,
in which she reminisces about her own midlife empowerment and how it led to her
work on positive aging. Her article “Finished at 40” (which she says she would call
“Finished at 50” if she were writing it today!) is a feminist classic, and this book
would probably not exist if she had not led the way. We thank all of the authors
for their thought-provoking contributions to this volume; we have all learned a lot
from each other’s work. We hope that our readers will be inspired to expand the
literature by filling in the blanks we have identified and discovering the answers
to the questions we have raised. We look forward to reading the work to come and
to finding out how future generations of women over 50 will live out their lives.
References
Ashmore, R.D., Deaux, K., and McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework
for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. American
Psychologist, 130, 80–114.
Introduction 5
Barnett, R.C., and Hyde, J.S. (2001). Women, men, work, and family. American Psycholo-
gist, 56, 781–796.
Gergen, M. (1990). Finished at 40: Women’s development within the patriarchy. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 14, 471–493.
Mitchell, V., and Helson, R. (1990). Women’s prime of life: Is it the 50s? Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 14, 451–470.
Sampson, E.E. (1993). Identity politics: Challenges to psychology’s understanding. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 48, 1219–1230.
1
Body Image Issues of Women Over 50
Joan C. Chrisler
In her poem I Met a Woman Who Wasn’t There, Marge Piercy (2006) described a
common sensation experienced by midlife women: the transition from visibility to
invisibility. In cultures in which notions of beauty and femininity are closely tied
to youth, there comes a point when women, no matter how healthy, well groomed,
and nicely attired they are, can pass by without attracting the attention of men or
younger women. The point at which this happens no doubt differs for different
women, but anecdotal evidence suggests that it is around age 50 when women,
particularly women who had previously been praised as beautiful, suddenly realize
that no one is looking at them anymore. This realization is a shock, but then what
happens? Some women seem to react with relief—there is no longer any need to
dress up and make up in order to impress; they can relax and simply be themselves.
Other women panic—those who can afford it seek out cosmetic surgeons, personal
trainers, and others who earn a living that derives in large part from the fear of
aging. Is there any way to predict which women will react which way? How do
women feel about the changes that accompany aging? How well or poorly do they
adjust to those changes? These are some of the topics this chapter will address.
Youth-oriented Cultures
It can be a challenge to feel comfortable about aging in cultures where older
women are rarely seen, and those who are seen are celebrated primarily for their
“youthful” good looks (Chrisler & Ghiz, 1993). Although we are told that we are
only as old as we feel, the dearth of images of women over 50 in the media drive
home the message that women should either grow old “gracefully” by hiding the
signs of aging (Chrisler & Ghiz, 1993) or stay out of sight. Wolf (1991) interviewed
6
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 7
editors of North American women’s magazines who admitted that signs of aging
are routinely “airbrushed” from photographs through computer imaging, so that
60-year-old women are made to look 45. Lear’s, a U.S. magazine aimed at midlife
women (it’s slogan was “the magazine for the woman who wasn’t born yesterday”;
it ceased publication after only a few years), rarely published photographs of gray-
haired women (Gerike, 1990), and a content analysis (Nett, 1991) of Chatelaine, a
Canadian magazine for midlife women, showed that midlife women were absent
from the covers and the fashion and beauty sections and underrepresented in the
advertisements. The editorial decisions made by the magazines’ staff suggest that
even midlife women do not want to see images of midlife women. Perhaps the
editors are correct, but, if so, it is because the media shape women’s preferences.
Midlife women told McFarland (1999) that they are well aware that the media
create the beauty standards women espouse; even though midlife women have the
wisdom to realize that the images represent fantasy rather than reality, many of
them still wish that they could match those standards.
Most women in Hollywood films are in their 20s and 30s (Lauzen & Dozier,
2005). It is not uncommon to see older men paired romantically on screen with
women several decades younger than they are; for example, Clint Eastwood, Sean
Connery, and Jack Nicholson have continued to play romantic lead roles well
into their 70s. But as women approach midlife they begin to disappear from the
Hollywood scene. Some who have had cosmetic surgery can hang on to their
careers into their 40s, but eventually they find that there are few roles for them
unless they start production companies and develop film projects for themselves.
In the 2003 film Something’s Gotta Give Diane Keaton played a woman in her
50s who stole her daughter’s lover played by Jack Nicholson. It was both a shock
and a delight to see Keaton on the screen—beautiful, yet clearly showing signs
of age that had not been surgically altered. In a content analysis of the top 100
grossing Hollywood films of 2002, Lauzen and Dozier (2005) found that midlife
and older women were seen on screen significantly less often than their male peers.
As female characters aged, they were less likely to have goals or a purpose to their
lives; as male characters aged, they were more likely to have power.
The same invisibility of midlife and older women is found on U.S. broadcast
television. Over the years a number of content analyses (Gerbner et al., 1980;
Glascock, 2001; Vernon et al., 1991) of prime time television programming have
shown that the majority of female characters are 35 years old or younger, whereas
male characters are more evenly distributed across the age range—at least up to
the mid-50s. Davis (1990) found little gender difference in the number of female
(12.1%) and male (14.8%) characters over age 50, but Vernon et al. (1991) pointed
out that older men tend to be portrayed more positively than older women. The
invisibility of older women is as common in news and public affairs programming
as it is in entertainment programming. Former Secretary of State Madeline Albright
is the only older woman I regularly see on these programs, and it is not unusual
to find her on a panel with a number of men her age and older, usually being
interviewed by male journalists over age 50. Barbara Walters has managed to
continue her career well beyond the age when most women on television fade
8 Joan C. Chrisler
away, but she has had cosmetic surgeries over the years so that she does not look
her age. Mike Wallace, Jim Lehrer, and other older men are able to continue their
news careers as long as they like without altering their signs of aging, which are
interpreted by viewers as indications of their wisdom and experience.
Women over 50 also are neglected in both popular and scholarly literature. Most
books about midlife women are focused on menopause. A 1990 special issue of
Psychology of Women Quarterly (PWQ) titled “Women at Midlife and Beyond”
(edited by Violet Franks and Iris Fodor) was intended to encourage research on
the many aspects of midlife and older women’s lives. It is certainly true that more
research is available now than when the issue was published, but much of what is
available concerns women over 65. At the end of her 5 years as editor of PWQ,
Jackie White (2005) looked back over the articles she had published and found that
only 20% of those articles contained any data from women over 50. She concluded
that women over 30 are an under-researched population. As I prepared to write this
chapter I read a number of articles in which the body image of college students
was compared to that of a convenience sample of older women. “Older women”
was often broadly defined, for example, as women ages 30 to 84. It seems obvious
that concerns of women in their 30s will differ (at least to some extent) from those
of women in their 40s, 50s, 60s, 70s, and 80s, but how are we to tell what those
differences are if researchers see any woman who is over 30 as “old”?
So what are women over 50 to think, and how are they to feel, about themselves
if they cannot see other women their age in magazines and newspapers, in films
and on television, or find their lives reflected in the contents of bookstore shelves
or the pages of scholarly journals? The focus on menopause as the paramount
issue of women in their 50s drives home the message that what is important about
midlife is the end of youth and fertility. Once women have passed reproductive
age, the culture seems to say, they are no longer interesting. No one cares about
them. No one wants to see them or hear what they have to say. As Piercy (2006,
p. 2) put it, “but to your prophecies only your cats will listen.”
Weight Consciousness
The body’s basal metabolic rate slows down with age, and is accompanied by a
decrease in lean body tissue and an increase in fat (Rodin et al., 1984). A study
(Young et al., 1963) of a large sample of midlife and older women showed an
increase in body fat composition after age 50. The mean percentage of body fat
in women in their 40s was 23%; it was 46% in women in their 50s, and 55% in
women in their 60s. Women tend to gain weight at each of the major reproductive
milestones (menarche, pregnancy, and menopause; Rodin et al., 1984), and the
average age of menopause in North America is 51 years. Furthermore, weight may
become redistributed during perimenopause, which results in larger breasts and
waist and increased fat on the upper back (Voda et al., 1991). To put it simply,
women should expect to gain weight and change shape as they get older, media
ideals notwithstanding.
A number of researchers have reported weight-related concerns among midlife
women. Wilcox (1996) surveyed women and men ages 20 to 80 about physical
health status and attitudes toward their bodies. Older women with greater body
mass index (BMI) reported more negative attitudes than did younger women or
age peers with lower BMI. Donaldson (1994) surveyed 180 women ages 40 to 59,
and found that weight status was the largest predictor of body image at midlife.
Disparaging oneself and “feeling fat” were particularly related to the salience of
body weight and shape, especially weight gain in the lower body. Lesbians were
less concerned than straight women were about lower body fatness. McFarland
(1999) conducted in-depth interviews with 10 midlife women about their body
image at various points in their lives. The women frequently brought up weight
10 Joan C. Chrisler
gain and loss, which they seemed to see as evidence of their competence and their
success or failure. Weight-related comments were made about both appearance
and health concerns.
Markey et al. (2004) surveyed 172 midlife married couples in the northeastern
United States for a study of family health. They reported that the wives were
much more dissatisfied with their own bodies than the husbands were with their
wives’ bodies. The wives estimated that their husbands were much less satisfied
than they actually were with their wives’ appearance, and the wives selected a
smaller body from the Figure Rating Scale (FRS) than their husbands did as the
ideal for women their age. Wives’ BMI was not strongly related to the husbands’
satisfaction with their wives’ bodies. This study is interesting in that it supports the
results of previous studies (Fallon & Rozin, 1985; Miller, 2001; Rozin & Fallon,
1988) that have shown a discrepancy between what body size and shape men think
is most attractive and what size and shape women think that men think is most
attractive. Men generally choose a larger body size for women than women chose
for themselves. If women allowed themselves to believe their partners when they
say “You look fine to me,” then women might feel more comfortable with body
changes that occur at midlife.
Although eating disorders are usually associated with adolescent and young
adult women, evidence of disordered eating has also been documented in midlife
women. As the studies reviewed are all cross-sectional, it is impossible to tell
whether the women in question had had eating disorders earlier in life or whether
their disordered attitudes and behaviors emerged in midlife in reaction to age-
related weight gain. Lewis and Cachelin (2001) reported that midlife women (ages
50 to 65) had higher scores on the Eating Disorders Inventory than did older
women (ages 66 and older). They found positive correlations between fears of
aging and attitudes and behaviors associated with disordered eating. Gupta and
Schork (1993) also reported a direct connection between aging-related concerns
and drive for thinness in a sample of 200 women (ages 30s to 50s) who were
surveyed at a shopping mall.
However, not all midlife women hold themselves to unrealistic weight standards.
Deeks and McCabe (2001) surveyed 304 women (ages 35 to 65) drawn from the
community in and around Melbourne, Australia. The found that the premenopausal
women selected smaller figures from the FRS than peri- and postmenopausal
women did in response to a question about how society expected them to look.
In a comparison of college students to a community sample of adults over age 39
from the southeastern United States, Lamb et al. (1993) found that women in both
age groups would like to be thinner, but the young women chose a much thinner
figure from the FRS as their ideal. These studies suggest that midlife women do
not think that they are expected to measure up to the ideal weight standards to
which younger women aspire. Furthermore, in a survey of 180 women (ages 18
to 60) Tiggemann and Stevens (1999) found that stronger feminist attitudes were
correlated with lower weight concern, and Stevens et al. (1994) reported that the
older Black women they surveyed were less likely than the older White women
to consider themselves overweight. The White women also chose a smaller ideal
body size on the FRS than the Black women did.
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 11
women can be dissatisfied with their weight, yet not ashamed of it. They can
feel good about themselves without experiencing the need to keep their physical
appearance constantly in mind. McKinley (in press) surveyed the same women 10
years later and found that the mothers’ (ages 48 to 68) body-esteem was relatively
stable, even though their BMI had increased and they were exercising and dieting
more. These data are consistent with some of the comments made by McFarland’s
midlife participants who said that they had learned to take care of their bodies
and to judge them less harshly; they drew their self-esteem primarily from their
accomplishments (McFarland, 1999).
McKinley (2004) also studied body esteem in a sample of 128 women ages
21 to 63, who were recruited from a list of subscribers to Radiance, a magazine
aimed at heavy weight women. Among the questions she asked them were items
about prejudice and discrimination against fat people and the need to seek social
justice remedies. Those who endorsed the need for social change in attitudes
toward fat people had higher body esteem and self-acceptance than did those who
only endorsed the need for individuals to learn to accept themselves regardless of
body size. In her in-depth interviews with 11 midlife Canadian women (ages 40
to 53) Banister (1999) heard comments from some women who actively resisted
cultural beauty standards by labeling them as oppressive. The results of these two
studies, combined with the findings of Tiggemann and Stevens (1999), suggest
that midlife women’s body esteem and self-esteem can be protected, at least to
some extent, if they have a high consciousness of social justice issues. If cultural
messages and societal strictures can be labeled as examples of sexism, ageism, or
sizism, they can be more easily rejected. Perhaps midlife women would benefit
from a revival of the consciousness-raising groups that were popular in the 1970s,
which would provide an opportunity to discuss oppressive constraints on positive
aging. Participants in such groups would no doubt come to the conclusion that the
personal is still political.
Appearance Dissatisfaction
Research on the psychology of appearance has shown that qualities of the face
are the most important determiners of attractiveness and that injury or illness that
results in scarring or mutilation of the face and neck is the most difficult for people
to accept (Bernstein, 1990). The Western beauty ideal demands a smooth, soft,
and blemish-free face, but skin changes that occur with aging make this more and
more difficult to achieve as the skin of the face and neck becomes drier and start
to flake, loosen, and crease. Wrinkles or warts may appear on the face and “age”
spot on the hands. In addition the hair may become thinner and grayer. The extent
of these changes varies among individuals due to both genetic and environmental
effects (Bernstein, 1990). Although the body image literature suggests that gradual
changes (such as those caused by aging) are easier to adapt to than sudden changes
(such as those caused by injuries) (Pruzinsky & Cash, 1990), such adaptation may
be easier said than done for women, especially for those who have been closest to
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 13
the beauty ideal, than it is for men due to the prevalent double standard of aging
(Bernard, 1981; Sontag, 1979).
In a study of 268 adults (ages 18 to 80) from the southwestern United States,
Harris (1994) found that signs of aging in both women and men were considered
unattractive, but especially so in women. The participants expected women, more
so than men, to take steps to conceal signs of aging as they appeared, and the women
who participated in the study were significantly more likely than the men were
to use (or to expect to use in the future) age concealment products or techniques.
Harris’ study is particularly interesting because, in addition to the attitudes survey,
she asked participants to read scenarios in which midlife characters either did or
did not make use of age concealment techniques. The participants judged those
characters who tried to conceal their age more harshly than those who did not (i.e.,
they gave them higher ratings on such adjectives as “conceited,” “foolish,” “vain,”
“pathetic”), and this was true even for those participants who themselves practiced
(or expected to practice) age concealment. McFarland’s participants mentioned
gray hair, wrinkles, double chins, and facial hair as signs of aging that they disliked
(McFarland, 1999). Although some of the women were resigned to or philosophical
about the changes, others worried that signs of aging could threaten their economic
security by making it more difficult for them to get or keep a job, and they may be
right about that given Harris’ findings.
McLaren and Kuh (2004) reported high rates of body dissatisfaction among
their sample of Canadian women in their 50s. When the researchers controlled
for BMI, they found that body dissatisfaction was higher in women with higher
socioeconomic status. Women with high socioeconomic status are likely to have
the resources (e.g., time to exercise, money for cosmetics and surgery) necessary
to allow them to approach the cultural beauty standard. Thus, we should not be
surprised that they would worry more than other women about deviating further
from the standard as they age. Yet, body dissatisfaction at midlife has been doc-
umented even in groups that are relatively protected from mainstream cultural
beauty standards. Platte et al. (2000) surveyed a sample of Old Order Amish in
rural Pennsylvania and found that older people (ages 45 to 66) were significantly
more likely than younger people (ages 14 to 22) to report body dissatisfaction.
Older women also tended to overestimate their body size.
However, not all studies show dissatisfaction with appearance at midlife. Deeks
and McCabe (2001) reported that the older women in their sample (ages 35 to
65) were not more dissatisfied with their bodies overall than were the younger
women, and Ross et al. (1989) reported that healthy older people (ages 62 to 79),
although more conscious of their physical appearance than younger people (ages
17 to 28), actually evaluated their bodies more positively than the younger people
did. In a survey of 678 women (ages 16 to 70) in the United Kingdom about their
grooming rituals, Toerien et al. (2005) found that women over 50 were much less
likely than younger women to remove their body hair, which suggests a willingness
to deviate from the beauty standard. Davidson and McCabe (2005) reported that
women in their 30s and 40s obtained higher body dissatisfaction scores and made
more attempts at body concealment than did the younger and older women in their
14 Joan C. Chrisler
sample. The older women were less likely than the younger ones to engage in
appearance comparisons, which suggests greater contentment with (or, at least,
greater acceptance of) their bodies. The older women who were very concerned
about other people’s evaluations of their appearance were more depressed and
anxious than the other women their age. In her interviews with 32 women ages 28 to
63, Giesen (1989) learned that single women were more likely than married women
to believe that they were becoming more attractive and sexually appealing with age.
Donaldson (1994) reported that the participants in her sample (ages 40 to 59)
had neutral to positive body image. They generally agreed that they were attractive
to themselves and to others. Foerster (2001) conducted in-depth interviews with
seven healthy women (ages 60 to 67), most of whom said that they were more
accepting of themselves on a variety of dimensions than they had been earlier
in life. The women referred to “making peace with” and “learning to like” their
bodies (p. 42). Five of the women said that they were generally satisfied with
their bodies, and three noted that their body image was better now than it had
been when they were younger. When Foerster asked specifically what the women
liked and disliked about their bodies, they mentioned disliking wrinkles, sagging
skin, changes in their hair, knee problems, and extra weight around the abdomen;
however, the majority said that they did not think about their looks very much. All
said that they got positive appraisals of their appearance from others, especially
their partners and friends. The women (ages 40 to 53) in Banister’s study expressed
similar mixed messages (Banister, 1999). Some spoke of being surprised to see
reflections in mirrors or shop windows that look older than the women themselves
feel. One recalled saying to herself: “My God, I look like my mother!” (p. 530).
Another said: “If I look at myself, especially early in the morning [and see] all
those wrinkles and saggy places, then I think, ‘Gosh, I guess I am getting older!’”
(p. 530). Yet another said: “. . . when I compare women my own age to women
who are really young the young women’s faces look so bland in a sense. . . . Life
hasn’t written its story on their faces” (p. 527).
Functional Dissatisfaction
Body image is as much about how the body functions and feels as it is about how
the body appears to others, yet psychological researchers have focused much more
on the body’s ornamentality than its instrumentality. Although there are multidi-
mensional measures of body image, in the research reviewed for this chapter the
most common measures were the FRS, the Body Esteem Scale, and the Objectified
Body Consciousness Scale, all of which are appearance-focused. Yet, we know
that aging-related changes in functionality begin to be noticed in midlife, and thus
may affect midlife women’s body image in various ways. Changes that accompany
aging or the onset and course of chronic illness may require the use of devices
such as hearing aids, eyeglasses, pacemakers, or canes. These affect both one’s
appearance and bodily experience, as do surgical scars, limps or stiffness that re-
sult from injury or arthritis, hot flashes, imbalance, or side effects of medications
(Hyman, 1987).
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 15
demands that they pursue the beauty ideal, and, before surgery, they had a better
chance than midlife or older women of approaching it.
Menopause
The very fact of menopause requires an alteration in body image. Whether the
cessation of the menstrual cycle is greeted with sadness, indifference, or relief it
changes the way we think about our bodies (Chrisler & Ghiz, 1993). In addition,
the physical signs (e.g., vaginal dryness, dry skin, thinning hair) that typically
accompany menopause can affect body image. Vasomotor instability (e.g., hot
flashes, night sweats) might make a woman feel that her once reliable body is out
of control (Chrisler & Ghiz, 1993). How easily women adjust their body image in
response to perimenopausal changes remains unclear, as most of the research on
body image at midlife concerns weight and other aspects of appearance and body
esteem.
Perimenopausal women in the United States have been depicted by the media
as “diseased, hormone deficient, sexless, irritable, and depressed . . . and as passive
victims of their changing hormones” (Golub, 1992, p. 215). This view has been
encouraged by physicians and the pharmaceutical industry since the development
in the 1960s of hormone replacement therapy (HRT; in earlier years—estrogen
replacement therapy or ERT). Most books and magazine articles about menopause
have examined the topic from a biomedical perspective, and, over the years, have
suggested that HRT would keep women youthful, attractive, and both physically
and mentally healthy (Chrisler et al., 1989; Gannon & Stevens, 1998). Although
the promises of youth and beauty were debunked early on, beauty and health
have since been conflated in our contemporary consumer culture, and television
advertisements in the 1990s that featured supermodel Lauren Hutton (and other
less known but attractive models) urging viewers to ask their doctors if HRT is
right for them have no doubt perpetuated these old notions without naming them
specifically. When the results of the Women’s Health Initiative trials demonstrated
that HRT is less beneficial (and for some women even harmful) than had been
widely thought, many peri- and postmenopausal women stopped (or decided not to
start) using HRT. However, it had been used primarily by well-educated, upper and
upper-middle class women, those women who were, perhaps, closest to the cultural
beauty standard and most interested in approaching it. In Deeks and McCabe’s
2001 study, for example, only 18.4% of the peri- and postmenopausal participants
reported current HRT use; another 3.9% had tried it but discontinued use.
Studies (Dillaway, 2005; Elson, 2002; Foerster, 2001; Maoz et al., 1970;
Neugarten et al., 1968; Theisen et al., 1991) of women’s attitudes toward
menopause generally indicate ambivalence, or a mix of positive and negative atti-
tudes. The realization that the fertility years are over is a stark reminder of aging
for some women, and attitudes toward menopause are tied up with attitudes toward
aging in general. The negative aspects that women have reported include the onset
of aging, loss of fertility, loss of femininity, worries about emotional disturbance,
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 17
worries about physical health, concerns about age-related changes rendering them
unattractive or invisible, and a general sense that menopause has come too soon.
Positive aspects of menopause that women have reported include no further need
for contraception, no more menstrual periods, and a general sense of freedom
from reproductive-related cares. It is interesting that younger women tend to have
more negative attitudes toward menopause than older women do (Foerster, 2001;
Neugarten et al., 1968). Foerster’s participants (in their 60s) said that they had had
negative expectations of the menopausal transition, but found that it was not so
bad in retrospect. Some reported few, if any, physical symptoms; others reported
“some symptoms or changes in appearance but were not bothered by them” (p. 42).
The older women in Neugarten et al.’s classic study provide support for Margaret
Mead’s concept of “postmenopausal zest.” They described postmenopausal women
as “feeling better, more confident, calmer, and freer than before” (Golub, 1992,
p. 216). In fact, most researchers report that women say that the worst thing about
menopause is not knowing what to expect. It seems reasonable to predict that
women with more positive (on balance) attitudes toward menopause would have
an easier adjustment to menopause-related changes in body image, but there are,
as yet, no data available to support or refute this hypothesis.
question social norms and cultural expectations, to put their own needs first, to
put time and effort into taking care of their bodies, and to reflect on being a part
of nature. These comments are similar to those made by Arnold’s and Burns and
Leonard’s participants, and, again, they indicate a blend of bodily acceptance and
self-redefinition that suggests that the women are adjusting well to changes in their
body image.
Authenticity
The comments made by the participants in the qualitative studies reviewed above
suggest a midlife striving for authenticity. Some reasons why women value break-
ing out, redefinition of self, and the freedom “to be” are that the growth in wisdom
and self-confidence, the loosening of role demands (e.g., grown children, retire-
ment), and, yes, even the invisibility associated with midlife allow for greater
choice and flexibility in how one spends one’s time and energy. The realization
that life is finite, which accompanies midlife and is especially associated with
menopause and diagnoses of chronic illness, has the benefit of focusing the mind
on a reorganization of priorities that allows women to put their own needs first and
decide how to live their own lives. A conscious reorganization provides the perfect
opportunity for midlife women to decide to take care of their bodies (e.g., by feeding
them properly, allowing adequate time for rest, scheduling a massage) rather than
to torment their bodies (e.g., by chronic dieting, compulsive exercise, scheduling
a facelift) in an increasingly hopeless attempt to approach the beauty ideal.
Beauty rituals are time-consuming activities. Jokes about how long women take
to get ready to go out are based on the many tasks that women do (and men do
not) when they are getting dressed, and with age these rituals are more demanding
(Saltzberg & Chrisler, 1995). It takes time to pluck eyebrows, shave legs, mani-
cure nails, apply makeup, and arrange hair. Women’s clothing is more complicated
than men’s (especially at midlife when body shape changes make “foundation gar-
ments” necessary if women wish to wear fashionable clothes). Although all women
know that the “transformation from female to feminine is artificial” (Chapkis, 1986,
p. 5), we conspire to hide the amount of time and effort it takes, perhaps out of fear
that other women do not need as much time as we do to appear beautiful (Saltzberg
& Chrisler, 1995). To be artificial, of course, is to be inauthentic. To choose to be
authentic is to gain time (and money!) for more important or pleasurable pursuits.
Yet to turn one’s back on the beauty ideal after years of pursuing it is easier
said than done. Cultural messages that to “age gracefully” is to “age successfully”
(Calasanti et al., 2006) merge with messages that promote the importance of beauty
and thinness for women of all ages to encourage midlife women to “pass” as
young for as long as they can (Ostenson, 2004). Passing, whether it refers to
light-skinned African Americans “allowing” others to assume they are “White” or
lesbians wearing mainstream attire and keeping quiet about their personal lives so
that others will assume they are heterosexual, can involve one-time, temporary acts
(e.g., lying about one’s age on a job application, getting a botox injection) or an act
20 Joan C. Chrisler
or series of acts with long-term implications (e.g., regularly coloring one’s gray
hair, getting a facelift). Regardless of whether passing is applauded (“you look
so young for your age!”) or denigrated (e.g., people who conceal signs of aging
are pathetic; Harris, 1994), it is an inauthentic act, a denial of a person’s identity,
experience, and maturity (Ostenson, 2004). A preoccupation with passing will not
help women to experience the striving for authenticity reported by the midlife
women in the qualitative studies discussed above; in fact, it might hinder their
ability to relax into living their own lives.
Cultural messages about beauty and femininity overlap, as the former is gen-
erally considered to be a prerequisite for the latter. Therefore, feminine women
may place more emphasis on beauty (Gillen & Lefkowitz, in press) and may have
a harder time adjusting to bodily changes at midlife. Pliner et al. (1990) reported
a correlation between high femininity scores and greater appearance orientation
in girls and women ages 10 to 79. Pecor (2004) found that women with higher
femininity scores reported more distress and poorer psychosocial adjustment after
breast cancer surgery than did women who were classified as masculine or androg-
ynous, and Mahalik et al. (2005) included subscales on “thinness” and “investment
in appearance” in their new Conformity to Feminine Norms Inventory. Cultural
messages about femininity also encourage inauthenticity in interpersonal relation-
ships (e.g., women should fake orgasms and express interest in whatever topics
or hobbies interest their partners), and Gillen and Lefkowitz (in press) found that
women college students who were less instrumental and more inauthentic in their
relationships also had more negative attitudes toward their bodies.
It would be interesting to know whether high femininity predicts more body
image concerns at midlife, and there is some evidence to support this notion. For
example, lesbians (Donaldson, 1994) and feminists (Tiggemann & Stevens, 1999)
express less weight concern that other women do, and they are also probably more
likely than heterosexual nonfeminists to question traditional gender roles and gen-
dered expectations. Perhaps Black women are less likely to consider themselves
overweight (Stevens et al., 1994) and less distressed after breast cancer treatment
(Pecor, 2004) because Black women are more likely than White women to behave
androgynously. Latinas, on the other hand, are generally thought to be more tradi-
tional in their gender role attitudes, and that might account for part of the reason
why they have been shown to be more distressed than Black and White women after
breast cancer treatment (Spencer et al., 1999). In addition, participants in the qual-
itative studies (Arnold, 2005; Burns & Leonard, 2005) mentioned that as they aged
they increasingly questioned (and resisted) the traditional feminine gender role.
Conclusion
Much remains to be learned about body image issues and adjustments of women
over 50. What little we know is concentrated on weight and appearance; much
less work has been done on functional changes and on the menopausal transition.
There are hints that women who are in better health, who are in stable long-term
relationships and/or are sexually active, and who are more nontraditional in their
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 21
gender role orientation will have a more positive body image at midlife. However,
these relations need to be tested directly.
Furthermore, the research on midlife and older women rarely includes longi-
tudinal data or samples large enough to contrast cohorts with each other. In most
cases there is no way to know if women in their 50s differ from women in their 40s,
60s, and 70s. It is also impossible at this point to tell whether data about women
in their 50s today would differ from data about women who were in their 50s 20
years ago or those who will be in their 50s 20 years from now. Women who are
currently in their 50s and 60s were impacted strongly by the Women’s Liberation
Movement, and, therefore, one might expect them to be less traditional in their
attitudes, more comfortable with themselves, and more willing to resist cultural
messages that they should take steps to conceal signs of aging and pass as younger
than they are. One can only wonder about how women in their 20s and 30s today,
the so-called postfeminist generation, will cope with aging. They came of age in
a time of hyperconsumerism, where there is a “cure” for almost everything and
where women are expected to be both beautiful and high achieving. Only time will
tell how they will confront signs of aging.
It is interesting that the quantitative studies of body esteem and appearance
concerns yield different information than do qualitative studies that address midlife
and aging more generally. Perhaps what we need are mixed method studies in which
both standardized scales and interviews or focus groups are used. This might help
us to understand how focused women are on bodily changes and concerns and, if
negative body image is an important issue for them, to provide clues about how to
design appropriate interventions.
Like most things in life, aging is neither all good nor all bad. Experience, mat-
uration, development, lifecycle transitions, and even invisibility are both benefits
and challenges. Midlife, with its focus on finding balance and reorganizing pri-
orities, can be the perfect time to stop fighting our bodies and learn to appreciate
them. Our stretch marks, scars, gray hairs, and extra pounds are proof of who we
are and what we’ve been through to get where we are. Let us embrace the bodily
changes we like as well as those we do not, resist the impulse to alter ourselves
in inauthentic ways, and take good and gentle care of ourselves so that our bodies
will last long enough for us to gain even more wisdom and experience. Let us not
waste the precious time we have left in trying to be what we are not.
References
Alderson, B.W. (1991, March). An Overview of Emotional Issues Faced by Women Over 50.
Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Women in Psychology, Hartford,
CT.
Allen, K.E., and Wellard, S.J. (2001). Older women’s experiences with sternotomy. Inter-
national Journal of Nursing Practice, 7, 274–279.
Arnold, E. (2005). A voice of their own: Women moving into their fifties. Health Care for
Women International, 26, 630–651.
Banister, E.M. (1999). Women’s midlife experiences of their changing bodies. Qualitative
Health Research, 9, 520–537.
22 Joan C. Chrisler
Gerike, A.E. (1990). On gray hair and oppressed brains. Journal of Women & Aging, 1(1/2/3),
35–46.
Giesen, C.B. (1989). Aging and attractiveness: Marriage makes a difference. International
Journal of Aging and Human Development, 29, 83–94.
Gillen, M.M., and Lefkowitz, E.S. (in press). Gender role development and body mage
among male and female first year college students. Sex Roles.
Glascock, J. (2001). Gender roles on prime-time network television: Demographics and
behaviors. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 45, 656–669.
Golub, S. (1992). Periods: From Menarche to Menopause. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Greenspan, M. (1983). A New Approach to Women and Therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gupta, M.A., and Schork, N.J. (1993). Aging-related concerns and body image: Possible
future implications for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14,
481–486.
Harris, M.B. (1994). Growing old gracefully: Age concealment and gender. Journal of
Gerontology, 49, P149–P158.
Hays, K.F. (1999). Working it Out: Using Exercise in Psychotherapy. Washington, DC:
American psychological Association.
Hyman, J. (1987). Who needs cosmetic surgery? Reassessing our looks and our lives. In
Boston Women’s Health Book Collective (Eds.), Our Bodies, Ourselves: Growing Older
(pp. 37–45). New York: Simon & Schuster.
Kasper, A.S. (1995). The social construction of breast loss and reconstruction. Women’s
Health, 1, 197–219.
Kriss, R.T., and Kramer, H.C. (1986). Efficacy of group therapy for problems with post-
mastectomy self-perception, body image, and sexuality. Journal of Sex Research, 22,
438–451.
Lamb, C.S., Jackson, L.A., Cassiday, P.B., and Priest, D.J. (1993). Body figure preferences
of men and women: A comparison of two generations. Sex Roles, 28, 345–358.
Latteier, C. (1998). Breasts: The Women’s Perspective on an American Obsession. New
York: Harrington Park Press.
Lauzen, M.M., and Dozier, D.M. (2995). Maintaining the double standard: Portrayals of
age and gender in popular films. Sex Roles, 52, 437–446.
Lewis, D.M., and Cachelin, F.M. (2001). Body image, body dissatisfaction, and eating
attitudes in midlife and elderly women. Eating Disorders, 9, 29–39.
MacRitchie, K.J. (1980). Prenatal nutrition outside the hospital: Psychosocial styles of
adaptation. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 308–313.
Mahalik, J.R., Morray, E.B., Coonerty-Femiano, A., Ludlow, L.H., Slattery, S.M., and
Smiler, A. (2005). Development of the Conformity to Feminine Norms Inventory. Sex
Roles, 52, 417–435.
Markey, C.N., Markey, P.M., and Birch, L.L. (2004). Understanding women’s body satis-
faction: The role of husbands. Sex Roles, 51, 209–216.
Maoz, B., Dowty, N., Antonovsky, A., and Wijsenbeek, H. (1970). Female attitudes to
menopause. Social Psychiatry, 5, 35–40.
McFarland, M.B. (1999). A Descriptive Study of Body Dysphoria and Body Image in Midlife
Women. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Dakota.
McKinley, N.M. (1999). Women and objectified body consciousness: Mothers’ and daugh-
ters’ body experience in cultural, developmental, and familial context. Developmental
Psychology, 35, 760–769.
McKinley, N.M. (2004). Resisting body dissatisfaction: Fat women who endorse fat accep-
tance. Body Image, 1, 213–219.
24 Joan C. Chrisler
McKinley, N.M. (2006). The developmental and cultural contexts of objectified body con-
sciousness: A longitudinal analysis of two cohorts of women. Developmental Psychology,
42, 679–687.
McLaren, L., and Kuh, D. (2004). Body dissatisfaction in midlife women. Journal of Women
& Aging, 16(1/2), 35–54.
Meyerowitz, B.E., and Hart, S. (1995). Women and cancer: Have assumptions about women
limited our research agenda? In A.L. Stanton and S.J. Gallant (Eds.), Psychology of
Women’s Health: Progress and Challenges in Research and Applications (pp. 51–84).
Washington, DC: American psychological Association.
Miller, D.J. (2001). Weight satisfaction among Black and White couples: The role of per-
ceptions. Eating Disorders, 9, 41–47.
Nett, E.M. (1991). Is there life after fifty? Images of middle age for women in Chatelaine
magazine. Journal of Women & Aging, 3(1), 93–115.
Neugarten, B.L., Wood, V., Kraines, R.J., and Loomis, B. (1968). Women’s attitudes toward
menopause. In B.L. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle age and aging (pp. 195–200). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
O’Brien, J. (1980, April 24). Mirror, mirror: Why me? Nursing Mirror, pp. 36–37.
Ostenson, R. (2004). Who’s in and who’s out: The results of oppression. In J.C. Chrisler, C.
Golden, and P.D. Rozee (Eds.), Lectures on the Psychology of Women (3rd ed., pp. 16–26).
Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Pecor, M.J. (2004). The Impact of Breast Cancer on Body Image in Ethnically Diverse
Women. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Auburn University.
Pettus, M. (2001). Kudos for me: Self-esteem. In J.J. Robert-McComb (Ed.), Eating Disor-
ders in Women and Children: Prevention, Stress Management, and Treatment (pp. 283–
290). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Piercy, M. (2006). I met a woman who wasn’t there. NWSA Journal, 18, 1–2.
Platte, P., Zelton, J.F., and Stunkard, A.J. (2000). Body image in the Old Order Amish: A
people separate from “the world.” International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 408–414.
Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., and Flett, G.L. (1990). Gender differences in concern with body
weight and physical appearance over the lifespan. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 16, 263–273.
Pruzinsky, T., and Cash, T.F. (1990). Integrative themes in body image development, de-
viance, and change. In T.F. Cash and T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body Images: Development,
Deviance, and Change (pp. 337–349). New York: Guilford.
Rackley, J.V., Warren, S.A., & Bird, G.W. (1988). Determinants of body image in women
at midlife. Psychological Reports, 62, 9–10.
Roberto, K.A. (1990). Adjusting to chronic disease: The osteoporotic woman. Journal of
Women & Aging, 2(1), 33–47.
Roberto, K.A., and McGraw, S. (1991). Self-perceptions of older women with osteoporosis.
Journal of Women & Aging, 3(1), 59–70.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., and Striegel-Moore, R. (1984). Women and weight: A normative
discontent. In T. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 267–304).
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Ross, M.J., Tait, R.C., Grossberg, G.T., Hamdal, P.J., Brandeberry, L., and Nakra, R. (1989).
Age differences in body consciousness. Journal of Gerontology, 44, P23–P24.
Rozin, P., and Fallon, A. (1988). Body image, attitudes toward weight, and misperceptions
of figure preferences of the opposite sex: A comparison of men and women in two
generations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 342–345.
1. Body Image Issues of Women Over 50 25
Saltzberg, E.A., and Chrisler, J.C. (1995). Beauty is the beast: Psychological effects of the
pursuit of the perfect female body. In J. Freeman (Ed.), Women: A Feminist Perspective
(5th ed., pp. 306–315). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Schumacher, D. (1990). Hidden death: The sexual effects of hysterectomy. Journal of
Women & Aging, 2(2), 49–66.
Segar, M., Spruijt-Metz, D., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2006). Go figure? Body shape mo-
tives are associated with decreased physical activity participation among midlife women.
Sex Roles, 54, 175–187.
Shaw, J.M., Ebbeck, V., and Snow, C.M. (2000). Body composition and physical self-
concept in older women. Journal of Women & Aging, 12(3/4), 59–75.
Sontag, S. (1979). The double standard of aging. In J.H. Williams (Ed.), Psychology of
Women: Selected Readings (pp. 462–478). New York: W.W. Norton.
Spencer, S.M., Lehman, J.M., Wynings, C., Arena, P., Carver, C.S., Antoni, M.H., Der-
hagopian, R.P., Ironson, G., and Love, N. (1999). Concerns about breast cancer and rela-
tions to psychosocial well-being in a multiethnic sample of early-stage patients. Health
Psychology, 18, 159–168.
Stevens, J., Kumanyika, S.K., and Keil, J.E. (1994). Attitudes toward body size and diet-
ing: Differences between elderly Black and White women. American Journal of Public
Health, 84, 1322–1325.
Stokes, R., and Frederick-Recascino, C. (2003). Women’s perceived body image: Relations
with personal happiness. Journal of Women & Aging, 15(1), 17–29.
Theisen, C.X., Mansfield, P.K., Voda, A.M., and Seery, B. (1991, June). Predictors of
Attitudes Toward Menopause among Midlife Women. Paper presented at the meeting of
the Society for Menstrual Cycle Research, Seattle.
Tiggemann, M., and Stevens, C. (1999). Weight concern across the life-span: Relationship to
self-esteem and feminist identity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 103–106.
Toerien, M., Wilkinson, S., and Choi, P.Y.L. (2005). Body hair removal: The ‘mundane’
production of normative femininity. Sex Roles, 52, 399–406.
Vernon, J.A., Williams, J.A. Jr., Phillips, T., and Wilson, J. (1991). Media stereotyping: A
comparison of the way elderly women and men are portrayed on prime-time television.
Journal of Women & Aging, 2(4), 55–68.
Voda, A.M., Christy, N.S., and Morgan, J.M. (1991). Body composition changes in
menopausal women. Women & Therapy, 11(2), 71–96.
White, J.W. (2005). Psychology of Women Quarterly, 2000–2004. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 29, 107–109.
Wilcox, S.J. (1996). Sex and Age Differences in Body Attitudes Across the Adult Life-Span.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Washington University.
Wilkinson, S., and Kitzinger, C. (1993). Whose breast is it anyway? A feminist consideration
of advice and treatment for breast cancer. Women’s Studies International Forum, 16, 229–
238.
Wolf, N. (1991). The Beauty Myth: How Images of Beauty are Used Against Women. New
York: Morrow.
Young, C.M., Blondin, J., Tensuan, R., and Fryer, J.H. (1963). Body composition studies
of older women, 30–70 years of age. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 110,
589–607.
2
Women and Sex at Midlife: Desire,
Dysfunction, and Diversity
Maureen C. McHugh
Are women likely to experience more sexual and relationship satisfaction in midlife
than in earlier periods of their lives? Which women are, and why? Does a woman
experience heterosexual partner sex more positively when she is no longer respon-
sible for birth control or parenting? In what ways, if any, do the sexual relations of
aging lesbians improve over time? Are women who are autoerotic more likely to be
in touch with their own sexual desire than women who are dependent on partners
for sexual arousal and activity? Is sexual authenticity an important component in
the positive sexual response of older women? These are examples of possible re-
search (and personal) questions that cannot be answered at this point. Researchers
have not typically investigated the possible positive sexual feelings and experi-
ences of women at midlife. Rather, the literature has focused almost exclusively
on the possible declines in sexual activity and desire allegedly experienced by
women as a result of declining hormones. The research on women and sexuality
at midlife both reflects and reifies the negative cultural views and disease-oriented
medical perspectives on aging that are present today in many societies. The current
research is reviewed acknowledging the degree to which limited perspectives on
women’s sexuality has limited our understanding of the sexuality of women at
midlife and beyond.
The idea that women’s sexuality declines at midlife, adversely affected by
menopause, is widely held by the general public and by many professionals. Mid-
dle age is a developmental stage marked by changes at multiple levels, and many
women report changes in their intimate relationships, as well as in sexual interest,
responsiveness, and behavior. Menopause, the cessation of the menstrual cycle
and the declining levels of estrogen, is only one aspect of the changes that occur
in middle-aged women’s lives. Menopause occurs during the 40s and 50s when
other aging processes also occur, when the woman’s roles and family structures
are often changing, when her partner, if she has one, is also aging, and when she is
viewed differently than younger women in her culture and society. Thus, there are
many potential influences on women’s intimate relationships and sexual responses
at midlife. A substantial amount of the research on women’s sexuality in midlife
and beyond has focused on the role of menopause and has been conducted from
within the medical or biological position. Research conducted from the biomedical
26
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 27
thinner, penetration may result in bleeding. Vaginal dryness and hot flashes, two
conditions that occur along with declining estrogen, have been linked to declining
sexual responsiveness (McCoy et al., 1985).
Both mental and physical health can impact sexual functioning. As women
age, various health conditions may impact their sexual response. Menopause-
related problems, such as heavy bleeding or urinary leaking, can adversely impact
women’s sexual interest and experience. Additional physical problems that can
impact sexual interactions were outlined by Diokno et al. (1990) based on research
with individuals 60 years of age or older: decreased mobility, incontinence, use of
sedatives, and a history of heart attacks. Other health problems experienced as a
result of aging, such as diabetes or high blood pressure, can also affect lovemaking
or sexual interest. Women may feel less sexual as a result of chronic or acute
illnesses due to pain or fatigue. Arthritic disease may hamper sexual activity. In
both research and clinical practice it is important to consider the impact of specific
health problems rather than simply attributing women’s problems or symptoms to
aging or menopause (Boston Women’s Health Book Collective, 2005).
for the Study of Aging at Duke University (Pfeiffer et al., 1972) declines in sex-
ual responsiveness were reported by both men and women. In that large study of
biologically and socially advantaged individuals born before 1900, respondents
indicated a substantial decline in sexual interest and activity; 6% of the men and
33% of the women reported not being interested in sex, and 12% of the men and
40% of the women reported no longer engaging in sex.
with sexual performance increased significantly, and women’s positive feelings for
the partner declined significantly. Frequency of sexual activity and sexual desire de-
creased most in the menopausal phase. The longitudinal study confirms the earlier
structural equation model finding that both age and menopausal status contribute
to declines in women’s sexual functioning. Although both analyses confirmed the
effects of aging and menopause, the authors stressed that women’s relationships
with their partners had a particularly powerful effect on women’s sexual desire, a
fact that is often lost in discussions of sexuality and aging.
To challenge the biomedical model, Mansfield et al. (2000) asked a sample of
505 White, married, middle-aged women about their sexual responses and their
own understanding of any changes. The women were recruited from the Tremin
Trust Research Program and from the 1963 graduating class of Douglass College.
Postmenopausal hormone users were not included. Forty percent of the respon-
dents indicated some change in their sexual response. The changes were primarily
decrements in desire, arousal, enjoyment, and/or decreases in the frequency of
sex and orgasm. The majority of those who reported changes reported: desiring
sex less (64%), having sex less often (57%), desiring more nongenital touching
(55%), and finding arousal more difficult (53%). The women attributed changes
involving a decline in their response to physical and emotional changes as related
to menopause, whereas increases in responsiveness were attributed to changing
life circumstances.
Note that the researchers who reported declining levels of sexual activity and/or
desire in midlife did not report that this was the case for all of the women in their
samples. Variability, including the fact that some women experience increases in
desire, can be found in the literature (Dennerstein et al., 2003; Hallstrom, 1977;
Hallstrom & Samuelson, 1990; Mansfield et al., 1998; McCoy & Davidson, 1985),
and not all researchers have reported a significant decline in women’s sexual
response during midlife. Increases in sexual response or a lack of substantive
changes have been reported (Bachmann et al., 1989; Dennerstein et al., 1997).
For example, no significant changes in pre- or postmenopausal sexual responses
of middle aged women were reported by the women in Cutler and colleagues’
study (Cutler et al., 1987). Women aged 33 to 56 were recruited from the Bay
area for a study of menopause and responded to a 31 item questionnaire that
assessed their sexual desire, sexual response, and sexual satisfaction in relation to
steroid levels in the perimenopausal period (Cutler et al., 1987). Questions about
frequency of masturbation, intercourse, sexual thoughts, and arousal, and items that
concerned satisfaction, pleasure, orgasm, distress, dysfunction, and dyspareunia
were examined in relation to changes related to the menstrual cycle. Women did
not report a decline in arousability; most reported that sexual arousal occurred
in every sexual episode. The overwhelming majority (86%) did not experience
deficits in lubrication, and dyspareunia was rarely encountered. No recent change
was perceived by the respondents in frequency of fantasies. However, women with
low estradiol levels did report a decline in frequency of intercourse.
Similarly, a longitudinal study of married women living in a Swedish city showed
evidence for stability of sexual desire through the premenopausal period (Hallstrom
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 31
& Samuelson, 1990). The research involved data collected from a representative
sample of 800 middle-aged, married women (from cohorts born in 1914, 1918,
1922, and 1930) who were interviewed twice, 6 years apart. Hallstrom and Samuel-
son (1990) reported on the changes in sexual desire for a subsample of 497 re-
spondents who lived with their partner at the time of both interviews. Present level
of sexual desire was assessed using a single item: Respondents characterized their
desire as absent, weak, moderate, or strong. Nearly two-thirds of the respondents
indicated the same level of sexual interest at both interviews, and very few women
indicated an absence of sexual desire. After the age of 50, no respondents reported
strong sexual interest, and increasing proportions of women reported little or no
sexual desire. By the age of 60, 39% reported no sexual desire. Over the 6-year pe-
riod, a change of sexual desire was reported by 37% of the women, 27% reported
a reduction in sexual interest, and 10% reported an increase. The data indicate
some regression to the mean for respondents who had indicated either strong or
absent sexual desire at Phase 1 of the study. Respondents who reported strong
sexual interest in the first interview later indicated a decline in interest; at the same
time, one-half of those who indicated a lack of interest at Phase 1 had regained
a weak to moderate sexual desire. A small number of women who indicated a
strong, moderate, or weak sexual desire at Phase 1 reported an absence of sexual
desire at Phase 2. The results of this study again suggest that for these cohorts of
married women, middle-age is characterized by stability of sexual interest rather
than by decline, although there was a trend toward decreasing sexual desire for the
older cohorts. The declines that were observed were associated with relationship
or mental health issues. Similar findings of stability of sexual desire were reported
for a comprehensive longitudinal study of sexual behavior in healthy older men
and women by the Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development at
Duke University (George & Weiler, 1981). Inconsistencies in the literature may
be the result of sample differences, including cultural and sociohistorical context,
age or health effects, and socioeconomic status, or may result from differences in
how researchers define or measure sexual desire and response.
In the recently published text, Kliger and Nedelman (2005) reported on their
multiple approaches to research on women’s sexual desire and self-esteem. They
collected survey data from 408 women in the 50 to 95 age range across the United
States, facilitated 10 focus groups with 100+ middle aged and senior women,
and conducted in depth interviews with 55 women in this age group. They were
interested in how older women age sexually, how women define and perceive
sexual desire, and their experiences of sexual desire. Kliger and Nedelman (2006)
found that desire tends to diminish with age; more than one-half of their sample
experienced a drop in desire, but 40% said that their desire was the same as ever
or was even greater. A very small number of respondents did not know what their
current level of desire was. The results of this research, although they indicated
a loss of desire for many women, may also be interpreted as favoring stability in
level of sexual desire.
In an exhaustive review of research on this topic Myers (1995) reported that,
although hormones do appear to be important for some aspects of postmenopausal
32 Maureen C. McHugh
sexuality, hormonal factors only account for a small portion of the variance in
the published studies. Myers argued that, based on her meta-analysis, the research
from 1972 to 1992 does not support the thesis that women’s hormones explain
much of the variance in women’s sexual response at midlife. Myers commented
on the lack of methodological quality in the literature, including the inadequacy
of measures, failure to include control groups, and confounding of variables, and
she encouraged researchers to expand their definitions of sexuality, to be more
complete in their descriptions of methods and results, and to be more attentive to
context.
Desire is sometimes seen as synonymous with arousal, but this may be more
valid for men than for women. In physically healthy men, genital engorgement is
assessed accurately by the individual and this sensation is enjoyed. In women, how-
ever, accurate awareness of genital engorgement is lacking. Recent research has in-
dicated that women generally do not separate “desire” from “arousal,” and women
care more about subjective arousal than they do about physiological arousal. Jill
Wood, who studies the sexual responses of women at midlife, found that, although
women could distinguish between desire (an interest in sexual activity) and arousal
(physical and emotional changes that indicate a readiness for sex), they often used
these terms interchangeably. She concluded that women do not conceptualize de-
sire in the same way that researchers have and that women do not distinguish
between arousal and desire unless specifically asked to do so (Wood, personal
communication, September 30, 2004).
The pharmaceutical industry has recently reported similar findings. Referring
to the failure of Viagra to impact women the same way as it impacts men, the New
York Times (Harris, 2004) reported that Pfizer had recently found that men and
women have a fundamentally different relationship between arousal and desire.
Women’s sexuality was found to be more cognitively complex and less genitally
focused than men’s. Although Pfizer’s researchers viewed this as a disconnect in
women between genital changes and mental changes (Harris, 2004), one might
also say that the fact that the production of physiological changes in the genitals is
enough to impact men’s sexual response suggests a simple mechanical response
in men.
Tolman (2001) wrote that desire is part of women’s embodied and relational
self, and she argued for its importance in the understanding of women’s sexuality.
Desire connects us to our bodies and, at the same time, connects us to another
(Tolman, 2001). Yet, women’s desire may not be acknowledged, and it is often
seen as dangerous in an androcentric culture that emphasizes men’s desire and
women’s responsiveness. These issues can be seen as contributors to women’s
inability or reluctance to admit to their own sexual desire: “When one is treated
as the object of the desires of others, and treats oneself as such, the ability even to
know one’s own needs and desires is undercut” (Tolman, 2001, p. 199).
Koch (1995, 1997) argued that sexual responding is a complex physiological,
psychological, and sociological process. Many social, cultural, religious, and eco-
nomic factors can affect sexual desire in women. Women may have different and
multiple motives for being sexual rather than simply the biologically based de-
sire that many models of sexuality theorize. Women have reported being sexual to
enhance emotional closeness and commitment, to express attraction and attractive-
ness, and to share physical pleasure (Basson, 2002). Women’s experience of low
levels of desire might result from intimacy problems, the nature of sexual stimuli
and stimulation, environmental triggers, and psychological factors that allow or
do not allow arousal. A sexual response cycle that incorporates these factors was
suggested as an alternative by Basson (2001, 2002), who identified a number of
affective, cognitive, and genital feedback loops in women that can contribute to or
interfere with sexual “arousability.”
34 Maureen C. McHugh
What Is Dysfunctional?
The biomedical approach is based on the disease concept, and it labels dissatisfac-
tion and deviation from the norm as dysfunction. Women who do not engage in
normative quantities of sexual activity are labeled pathological and dysfunctional
(Ogden, 1999; Tiefer, 2001b). The standards established to distinguish between
functional and dysfunctional behavior, like definitions of desire, are often based on
youthful, heterosexual men’s expression as the central criterion of “good” sexual
response (Mansfield et al., 1998). Conceptions of function and dysfunction are
grounded in biological perspectives of sexuality, which do not consider women’s
unique sociocultural position with regard to sexual desire and expression (Richgels,
1992). For example, criteria for the diagnosis of hypoactive sexual desire fail to
consider the ways in which women’s sexuality is repressed and criticized. Further,
Richgels argued that the norms for women’s sexual response have varied greatly
through history, and have generally not corresponded to women’s actual experi-
ences. Norms for healthy or satisfying sexuality for older women based on the
experiences of older women have not been established. Feminist sexologists (e.g.,
Irvine, 1990; Tiefer, 1995) have urged researchers to use measures that focus on
women’s pleasure. Others have suggested that menopause actually represents, for
some women, the opportunity to define their sexuality based on their own desires
and needs rather than on soicocultural expectations of reproduction and the sat-
isfaction of marital duties (Barbach, 1975; Conway-Turner, 1992; Laws, 1980).
Bancroft et al. (2003) have questioned whether we should label older women as
dysfunctional when they have less sexual interest than they had when they were
younger. They argued that the label of sexual dysfunction should rely heavily
on the woman’s own construction of her sexuality, rather than on the researchers’
standardized criteria for what constitutes functional and dysfunctional sex. Midlife
women may be sexually satisfied despite any sexual changes they experience (Avis
et al., 1995; Bancroft et al., 2003). Across four studies reviewed by Bancroft et al.,
only one-third to one-half of the women who were defined as having a problem
by the research criteria regarded themselves as having a problem. Lessened sexual
response did not concern most of the older women (Koch et al., 1995; Bancroft
et al., 2003). As long as couples still are sexually active, and it is not unpleasant,
the pairs believe that they are meeting a requirement of marriage (Schwartz &
Rutter, 2000). In research conducted by Osborn et al. (1988), English women did
report having sexual problems. However, their conceptions of the sexual problems
differed from the operational definition of the researchers. For example, lack of
emotional well-being and emotional feelings during sexual interaction with a part-
ner were more important determinants of sexual distress from the women’s point
of view. These discrepancies between the respondents’ and the researchers’ con-
ceptions of problems emphasizes the question: Who gets to define or determine
who has a sexual problem? Who sets the criteria for having enough or too much
sexual activity or desire?
Is absence or reduction in sexual desire or sexual activity a dysfunction? The
question “When is it appropriate to call a pattern of behavior a sexual problem or a
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 35
dysfunction?” has been raised repeatedly (Bancroft et al., 2003; McHugh, in press;
Tiefer, 2001b). Bancroft et al. (2003) found no significant relationship between
age and self-defined “problems.” In their research, it was younger women who
were more likely to report nonpleasurable sex, sexual anxiety, and pain during sex.
The authors suggested that, in at least some cases, inhibition of sexual desire is
an adaptive mechanism (Bancroft et al., 2003). This may be because the greater
experience and confidence that comes with aging mean that women are more likely
to refuse nonpleasurable sex and less likely to be sexually anxious.
According to Koch et al. (2005) midlife women tend to be sexually satisfied
regardless of the sexual changes they report. The effects of aging on the levels of
sexual interest of women in their study were not a cause of concern. Sexual thoughts
were reported less frequently by older women, but that was not necessarily seen as
problematic. Older women seemed to have more sexual problems (as defined by the
researchers) but less distress over them. The women who were not having partner
sex were older and masturbated more frequently; they reported more distress about
their relationship or lack thereof, but not about their own sexuality. This finding is
consistent with Conway-Turner’s conclusion that quality of intimacy, more than
frequency of sexual intercourse, is important to African American senior women
and positively related to their sexual self-esteem (Conway-Turner, 1992).
Laumann et al. (1999) reported that 43% of American women suffer from a
sexual problem, yet the problems encountered were most often associated with
mental health, relationship problems, and various aspects of the quality of life
than with physiologically based desire and arousal. The women’s levels of distress
about their relationship and their own sexuality were related both to their physical
and mental health. Their own physical health was a direct determinant of women’s
feelings about their own sexuality; older women with lubrication problems reported
marked distress about their own sexuality.
The research reviewed above reiterates the importance of conceptualizing the
sexual problems of women differently than those of men. The findings also sug-
gest the importance of the contextual factors. Women’s well-being, physical health,
level of education, and relationship to their partner predicted whether they were
experiencing distress. Factors about the relationship including communication,
intimacy, and respect are good predictors of women’s sexual satisfaction (Koch
et al., 2005). “While it is good to encourage older couples to maintain and foster
their sexual intimacy, should we be encouraging older women to regard them-
selves as dysfunctional because they have less sexual interest than when they were
younger?” (Bancroft et al., 2003, p. 502).
Foucault (1978) viewed desire as socially constructed rather than merely per-
ceived; in his theory desires are not biological entities, but are produced within
social practices and cultural discourses. From this perspective sexual desire is a
cultural product, and it is produced or constructed differently in different societies
and historical periods. Richgels (1992) reviewed the history of cultural perspectives
on women’s sexual desire or lack thereof from the Victorians’ denial of women’s
sexual feelings to contemporary prescriptions of “sex experts.” She argued that
modern views in which women’s lack of desire is seen as dysfunctional are as
36 Maureen C. McHugh
orientation. Women over 50 are a diverse group whose sexuality and intimate
relations are multifaceted and dynamic.
(1994) critiqued the belief that the problems we are confronting today are radi-
cally new ones, unknown in previous generations. There are both continuities and
differences in the experiences of generations.
Even within a specific historical era women’s experiences are diverse. Women’s
sexuality is impacted by a number of factors, including geographic region, class,
ethnicity, sexual orientation, and history of violence. Older women, like younger
women, vary enormously in their sexual desire, arousal, and experience of or-
gasm (Leiblum, 1990). The context of her life also influences what a woman
knows/thinks about menopause and aging.
Relationship Context
Research has demonstrated that marital status is an important predictor of de-
creased sexual activity (Mansfield et al., 1995). Kinsey et al. (1953) attributed the
decrease in coital frequency of a couple to the male partner’s declining interest
in sociosexual activities. Because older women often have even older partners, it
is not safe to assume that decreased sexual activity is related to women’s desire,
much less related to women’s hormone levels. Thus, Bachman (1990) concluded
that the sexual activity of a heterosexual woman is dictated by the availability,
functioning, and desire of her male partner.
In the Midlife Survey (Mansfield et al.), marital status, not menopausal status
predicted desire and enjoyment; the longer the marital relationship, the less desire
and enjoyment was reported. Furthermore, being married rather than single was
associated with a decline of sexual responsiveness, but menopausal status was not.
In other research, women’s sexual activity also was strongly related to marital
status (Diokno et al., 1990), and women indicated that their husbands were the
reason they were no longer engaging in sexual activities (Pfeiffer et al., 1972).
Decreases in sexual desire were associated with a poor relationship with one’s
spouse, with life stress events, and with mental disorder, major depression, and
use of psychotropic medicine (Hallstrom & Samuelson, 1990). Similarly, Bancroft
et al. (2003) reported that, in their large study of heterosexual women aged 20 to 64,
age had only a modest effect in predicting sexual distress regarding the relationship
or one’s own sexuality. Lack of emotional well-being and emotional feelings during
sexual interaction with the partner were more important determinants of sexual
distress. In a number of well-designed studies factors associated with the quality of
life and aspects of the relationship have been shown to predict sexual satisfaction
for women (Koch et al., 2005).
Other researchers have reported a strong positive correlation between sexual
satisfaction and relationship satisfaction for the middle-aged and older women in
their samples; sexual satisfaction has also been positively correlated with passion-
ate love (Traupmann et al., 1982). This research suggests that when relationship
satisfaction is low, so is sexual satisfaction. As previously cited, Mansfield et al.
(1995, 1998) concluded that marriage, rather than menopausal status, was associ-
ated with sexual response declines. Youthful men’s sexual experience may continue
40 Maureen C. McHugh
to serve as the sexual norm that both women and men use to judge the quality of
their sexual response.
For many women desire is experienced in the context of a relationship. Their own
desire is experienced in relation to their attractiveness to a partner. Thus, their desire
mirrors their partner’s desire. Schwartz and Rutter (2000) suggested that women
are trained to value commitment, and they may rely less on erotic stimulation, and
more on relationship satisfaction, as the basis for sexual arousal. Some widows
have said when their spouse died, their desire faded; yet other women have found
desire in a new relationship. Older women’s descriptions of their needs and desires
in a qualitative study by Wood et al. (in press) suggested that some women had
internalized a cultural ideology that privileged their male partner’s needs over
their own. Some women were disappointed with their partner’s technique and had
experienced sex as unsatisfying and work-like, but the women felt an obligation
to attend to their partner’s needs. Other women reported that they avoided sex to
protect their partner from the embarrassment of erectile dysfunction. The women
generally did not have the ability to express their own sexual desires and needs. The
quality of the women’s relationship with their husbands was the most frequently
cited influence on their own experience of sexual desire.
Marital relationships, as well as the individuals in them, go through devel-
opmental changes over time. Relationships need to grow and develop as do the
individuals involved. There may be an ebb and flow to marital sexual relations
that is not considered in culturally prescriptive norms. According to Schwartz and
Rutter (2000), declining sexual activity among older adults may be more related
to the length of the relationship and habituation than to aging. “Couples evolve
into partners rather than lovers” (Schwartz & Rutter, 2000, p. 132). The sex lives
of 50- and 60-year-old newlyweds resemble the sex lives of younger couples more
than they do the sex lives of long-married couples of the same age, and their ac-
tive sex lives follow the same pattern of eventual decline (Blumstein & Schwartz,
1983). In most marriages, sex becomes less frequent, but not less pleasant, over
time (Schwartz & Rutter, 2000).
Relationship conflicts have been noted as a common source of sexual problems
(Leiblum, 1990). Women have reported various relationship problems, including a
lack of spontaneity, initiative, or romance. Complaints about the personal hygiene
and appearance of the partner are not uncommon (Leiblum, 1990), and couples
with well established sexual routines may find that they no longer elicit much
excitement or interest. Butler and Lewis (1976) reported that relationship issues,
including boredom, too much togetherness, illness, or problems with hygiene, may
be expressed as pain during intercourse, perhaps because pain is a more acceptable
“excuse” for limiting sexual activities.
In their study of women’s sexual response at midlife, Mansfield et al. (1998)
found that the 40% of women who reported changes in their sexual response re-
ported less sexual interest. Women indicated that they wanted more fulfilling sex-
ual relationships. The women wanted to become more passionate, more interested
in sex, more romantic, more affectionate, more communicative, more sexually
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 41
responsive, more desirous of sex, more initiating, more fun, more creative, less
boring, more loving, and less inhibited. They wanted their husbands to be more
communicative, more romantic, more affectionate, more fun, more passionate,
more loving, more creative, and less boring. The biomedical approach, with its
focus on changes in physiological responsiveness, has ignored women’s stated
desire for more communication and affection in their relationships.
Women’s sexual functioning has been shown to be impacted by partner factors,
such as presence of a partner, feelings toward the partner, and partner’s health
(Dennerstein et al., 2001). The research by Dennerstein et al. (1999, 2001) con-
firmed the effects of aging and menopause, yet the authors stressed that women’s
relationships with their partners had particularly powerful effects on women’s
sexual desire. In research by Ellison and Zilbergeld (as cited in Ellison, 2001) the
top three items associated with satisfying sex for women were: feeling close to
a partner before sex, emotional closeness after sexual activity, and feeling loved.
Similarly, Byers (2001) presented empirical research that indicates that relation-
ship satisfaction is the most important contributor to women’s sexual satisfaction,
more important than the types of sexual exchanges such as oral sex, the consistency
of orgasm, and the expression of affection, which are all important determinants of
women’s sexual satisfaction. An earlier study of 100 married couples (Frank et al.,
1978) showed that, although 80% of the couples labeled their marriages and sexual
relationships as satisfying, 43% of the men and 63% of the women reported arousal
or orgasmic difficulties. High levels of additional concerns were also indicated,
such as lack of interest or inability to relax (Frank et al., 1978). These and other
descriptions of women’s sexuality confirm the perspective that women’s sexual
satisfaction is connected to the relationship context. Ellison stated that her survey
results support the following conclusion: that women associate sexual satisfaction
in relationships with closeness, love, acceptance, and safety, and that the sexual
problems and concerns of women often center on intimacy and relationship issues.
(in press), the New Zealand women reported “putting up” with sex and engaging in
coital sex when they did not desire it. Both sets of authors commented on the prob-
lems women had negotiating with their partners concerning when to have sex and
in which type of sexual and intimate activities to engage. Although some women
have had difficulty negotiating with their partners regarding the use of Viagra, other
women have used Viagra as the occasion to speak out about women’s sexuality
and to offer critical comments on the medical industry (Loe, 2004).
Lesbian Couples
The heterosexual bias in existing research on older women’s sexual functioning
has often been criticized. The research that suggests that length of relationship
predicts the frequency of sexual relations and that satisfaction with the relationship
predicts sexual satisfaction is all based on heterosexual couples. In a unique study
of menopausal lesbians, Cole and Rothblum (1991) concluded that lesbians are
more positive about sex than their heterosexual counterparts are. The sample
of 41 sexually active lesbians willing to talk about sex may not represent the
population of menopausal lesbians, but the results suggest that sexual orientation
is an important consideration in women’s sexuality at midlife. For example, the
respondents in the Cole and Rothblum’s study did not report declines in sexual
satisfaction and activity.
Winterich (2003) used a qualitative approach to examine the social aspects of
women’s sexuality after menopause in a sample that included both heterosexual
and lesbian women. She found that, in some cases, both lesbian and heterosexual
women had active and fulfilling sex lives. For her participants, although changes
in desire or orgasm or vaginal dryness might have occurred, their sexual relations
and satisfaction were not negatively affected. Open communication with their
partners and a flexibility regarding sexual repertoire was present in the sexually
satisfied couples, regardless of orientation. Among the women who reported sexual
problems, heterosexual women described issues that were related to the cultural
constructions of menopause, gender roles, and heterosexual sex. For example,
some of the heterosexual women reported faked orgasms, partners’ complaints
about their vaginal dryness, and an inability to talk to their partners about what
they wanted. Although some of the lesbians did report sexual problems, they
also reported the ability to discuss their needs and issues with their partners.
Several lesbian and heterosexual women were not having sex for a variety of
reasons, including not being in a relationship, healing from past trauma, and current
relationship problems.
In addition to including nonheterosexual options for sexual pleasure, Winterich’s
(2003) research emphasizes the need to consider how women view menopause
rather than just to focus on menopause as a problem, or as the cause of sexual
dissatisfaction. In women’s own accounts of sex after menopause, the status and
quality of relationships and their own and their partner’s health were important
factors for both lesbian and heterosexual women.
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 43
behaviors. Marriage mediated the degree to which older women viewed them-
selves as attractive and sexually appealing.
Single women, but not married women, viewed themselves as having grown
more attractive, feminine, and sexually appealing as they aged. Single women saw
themselves as having grown more attractive and more sexually appealing over the
past 10 years, and they saw the peak years of women’s attractiveness as occurring in
the early 30s to early 50s. Older married women saw women’s peak attractiveness
as occurring in their early 20s.
penile–vaginal intercourse, childbearing and a loss of sexuality in old age” (p. xi).
Others too have commented on the cultural emphasis on being pleasing and acces-
sible to men. Daniluk (1998) presented exercises and other techniques for “helping
women to extricate themselves from the oppressive shoulds and should nots . . . of
our dominant sexual scripts” (p. 231). She argued that our culture does not have
ways of constructing sexual expression that “affirm women’s sexual agency and
the diversity of their sexual feelings and desire” (p. 221).
Researchers’ conceptions of sexual desire and response may or may not cor-
respond with or include women’s own construction of their sexual desire and
response. For example, the responses that women gave to open-ended questions
about sexuality caused some researchers to begin to include questions about non-
genital as well as genital sexual expression (Mansfield et al., 2000). Research that
uses interviews, narratives, or other approaches to study women’s experience of
sexuality from their own points of view does not produce accounts that correspond
to Masters and Johnson’s sexual response cycle (Masters & Johnson, 1966). The
response cycle of Masters and Johnson does not address the relationship context
of sexual experiences; yet women’s experience of sexuality is more relationship-
oriented and less genitally focused that men’s (Conway-Turner, 1992; Peplau &
Gordon, 1985). Women appear to desire greater intimacy than men do (Tiefer,
1995), and they value physical affection and nongenital intimacy more than men
do (Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983; Mansfield et al., 1998). Consistent with this per-
spective, the New View campaign has called for research and services “driven not
by commercial interests, but by women’s own needs and sexual realities” (Working
Group on Women’s Sexual Problems, n.d.).
Hetero-normative Prescriptions
The universalizing and homogenizing aspects of the biomedical approach may
have detrimental effects on women and on the sexuality research agenda. Al-
though women want to be informed about what to expect, the presentation of
certain outcomes as likely can become prescriptive or self-fulfilling. As Irvine
(1990) pointed out, sex is a social product; it is negotiated and constructed through
discursive practices, including educational texts as well as advertising and self-help
manuals. Sexuality is organized through regulation and definition. The conception
that hormones are responsible for a decline in sexual desire and activity may re-
lieve some women, but can become an expectation in others that is negative and
self-fulfilling. On the other hand, in attempting to overcome the cultural belief that
older women are not sexual, psychologists’ and others’ encouragement to remain
or become passionate (Sheehy, 2006) may be experienced as pressure to engage
in unwanted sexual activities. To document and publicize norms regarding older
women’s sexual response is sometimes to provide a cultural prescription. Women
may be criticized when their sexual desire exceeds cultural expectations (Ogden,
1999), and they may be labeled dysfunctional when their interest in sex falls below
the (androcentric and youthful) norms. It can feel like a no-win situation.
46 Maureen C. McHugh
Sexual Authenticity
Miller (1976) argued that sexual authenticity is a key feature of women’s psy-
chological health. She identified sexual authenticity as the ability to bring one’s
own real feelings of sexual desire and sexual pleasure into intimate relationships.
Authenticity distinguishes women who feel disoriented in their aging bodies from
women who are confident and sexually secure. Alternatively, Tolman (2002) refers
to a similar concept, sexual subjectivity, which is to experience oneself as a sexual
being, to be in touch with one’s own sexual desire. Several authors (e.g., Martin,
1996; Richgels, 1992; Tolman, 2002) ) have noted that it is a challenge in an
androcentric society for women to exercise sexual agency, that is, the ability to
recognize and act on one’s own desire and to experience sexual pleasure. Women’s
ability to recognize their own sexual desire and to negotiate on their own behalf
was also emphasized in recent approaches (e.g., Wood et al., in press; Potts et al.,
2003; Kliger & Nedelman, 2006). Women’s ability to act on their own behalf (also
called negotiated sexual agency) was identified as a core variable in women’s ex-
perience of sexual desire in qualitative research conducted with postmenopausal
women (Wood et al., in press). Thus, the application of homogenized prescrip-
tions and androcentric standards to women’s sexual desire and response works
against the goal of helping women to be in touch with themselves and to be com-
fortable with their own levels of desire and activity. Researchers may contribute
to women’s experience of sexual authenticity at midlife by examining women’s
own conceptions and experiences of desire and by emphasizing the variability and
potential of women’s sexual experiences rather than assuming a physiologically
based, homogenized, and declining sexual desire.
Variability
Kinsey et al. (1953) emphasized the variability in the human sexual response and
noted that diversity is the single most identifiable aspect of sexuality. Leiblum
(1990) observed that women, at all ages, vary enormously in sexual desire, sexual
satisfaction, orgasmic experience, and arousability. Similarly, Kliger and Nedel-
man (2005) argued that there is not a single best or right way for women at any age
to express their sexuality. The women they surveyed reported multiple and diverse
sexual responses. Many older women have a fulfilling, exciting, and creative life
without any sexual desire or sexual activity, whereas others report increased appre-
ciation for sensual experiences as they age. In each of the studies reviewed above
there was at least a small percentage of women for whom desire increased in the
senior years, and, for others, desire remained as a steady state. Kliger and Nedel-
man (2006) concluded that sexual desire waxes and wanes over time. The changes
women experience in sexual desire and activity may relate to other aspects of their
lives or may have no clear trigger or influence (e.g., stress levels, ages of children).
At age 50, as at all other ages, women may express their sexuality in varied ways.
Variability is experienced by both married and single, heterosexual and lesbian
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 47
women (Kliger & Nedelman, 2006). A similar conclusion can be reached by read-
ing the personal accounts collected by Taylor and Sumrall (1993). They present
stories, personal accounts, and poems written by women over 40 about women’s
sexuality at midlife and beyond. Their text attests to the diversity of women’s expe-
riences. The new edition of Our Bodies, Ourselves (Boston Women’s Health Book
Collective, 2005) also enumerates some of the experiences of women as they age
including increased sexual desire, changes in sexual preference, feeling removed
from sexual practices and urges, finding alternatives to traditional relationships,
appreciating a non sexual sensuality, and recognizing an awakening of old feelings.
Ellison (2001) similarly concluded that women can experience sexual pleasure in
a variety of ways. Each woman has the capacity to respond sexually in a variety
of ways, and she is likely to experiences changes during her lifetime in how she
experiences her body, her relationships, and her sexual desire. Women’s sexual-
ity at all ages is multifaceted, complex, and dynamic. To evaluate the adequacy
of women’s sexuality against a single standard or on a single dimension seems
senseless.
Several recent texts concern the variability and the dynamic nature of women’s
sexuality over the lifespan and encourage women to respect their own experience,
rather than to subscribe to normative and pharmaceutical prescriptions. Kliger and
Nedelman (2006) and Sheehy (2006) emphasized women’s passion and diverse
sexual options after 50, explicitly counteracting the homogenized view of older
women as lacking in desire and desirability. Some women at midlife are more
self-actualized and more willing to explore, and perhaps redefine, what feels right
sexually rather than conforming to a coitally focused model of sexuality (Mansfield
et al., 2000; Tiefer, 1995). Cole (1988) encouraged therapists to provide women
with permission to have different, more mature kinds of sex lives and to help women
to develop an expanded view of sexuality and sensuality. The Boston Women’s
Health Book Collective (2005) encouraged women to recognize that lovemaking
may become more enjoyable when they no longer are concerned with pregnancy
and have more privacy at home. Some women enjoy lovemaking more as a result
of years of a committed relationship. Some women, such as Betty Dodson, have
relationships with younger men. Others have sexual relationships with women for
the first time.
Viewing women’s sexuality from androcentric, heterosexist, and biomedical
perspectives has limited the types of research questions we have asked, and has
impacted the methodologies employed in the conduct of research. Challenges to
existing research paradigms have suggested new research questions and strategies.
Future research on the sexual experiences and feelings of women at midlife might
entertain multiple perspectives and allow for positive changes as well as declines
or loss of function. Increasingly theorists are recognizing the need to conduct gy-
nocentric research on women’s experience and to recognize the variability among
women. Researchers, like the women they are studying, might increasingly exam-
ine the sexual possibilities that arise from developmental transitions in women’s
lives.
48 Maureen C. McHugh
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Anderson, B.L., and LeGrand, J. (1991). Body image for women: Conceptualization, as-
sessment, and a test of its importance to sexual dysfunction and medical illness. Journal
of Sex Research, 28, 457–477.
Avis, N.E., Stellato, R., Crawford, S., and Johannes, C. (1995). How does menopause impact
sexual activity? Menopause, 2, 245.
Bachmann, G., Leiblum, S., and Grill, J. (1989). Brief sexual inquiry in gynecologic practice.
Obstetrics & Gynecology, 73, 425–427.
Bancroft, J., Loftus, J., and Long, J.S. (2003). Distress about sex: A national survey of
women in heterosexual relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 193–209.
Banister, E.M. (1999). Women’s midlife experience of their changing bodies. Qualitative
Health Research, 9, 520–537.
Barbach, L. (1975). For Yourself: The Fulfillment of Female Sexuality. New York: Anchor
Books.
Basson, R. (2001). Human sex response cycles. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 27,
33–43.
Basson, R. (2002). Women’s sexual desire: Disordered or misunderstood. Journal of Sex
and Marital Therapy, 28(Suppl), 17–28.
Bernard, J. (1981). The Female World. New York: Free Press.
Blumstein, P., and Schwartz, P. (1983). American Couples. New York: Morrow.
Boston Women’s Health Book Collective. (1976). Our Bodies, Ourselves. Boston: Touch-
stone.
Boston Women’s Health Book Collective. (2005). Our Bodies, Ourselves. Boston: Touch-
stone.
Butler, R.N., and Lewis, M.I. (1976). Sex After Sixty. New York: Harper & Row.
Byers, E.S. (2001). Evidence for the importance of relationship satisfaction for women’s
sexual functioning. In E. Kaschak and L. Tiefer (Eds.), A New View of Women’s Sexual
Problems (pp. 23–26). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Candib, L.M. (2001). “A new view of women’s sexual problems”: A family physician’s
response. In E. Kaschak and L. Tiefer (Eds.), A New View of Women’s Sexual Problems
(pp. 9–15). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Cole, E. (1988). Sex at menopause: Each in her own way. Women & Therapy, 7(2/3),
159–168.
Cole, E. and Rothblum, E.D. (1991). Lesbian sex at menopause: As good as or better than
ever. In B. Sang, J. Warshaw, and A. Smith (Eds.), Lesbians at Midlife: The Creative
Transition (pp. 184–193). San Francisco, CA: Spinsters Ink.
Conway-Turner, K. (1992). Sex, intimacy, and self-esteem: The case of the African Amer-
ican older woman. Women & Aging, 4, 91–104.
Cutler, W.B., Garcia, C.R., and McCoy, N. (1987). Perimenopausal sexuality. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 16, 225–234.
Daniluk, J.C. (1998). Women’s Sexuality Across the Lifespan: Challenging Myths, Creating
Meanings. New York: Guilford.
Dennerstein, L., Alexander, J.L., and Kotz, K. (2003). The menopause and sexual func-
tioning: A review of the population-based studies. Annual Review of Sex Research, 14,
64–82.
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 49
Dennerstein, L., Dudley, E., and Burger, H. (2001). Are changes in sexual functioning
during midlife due to aging or menopause? Fertility & Sterility, 76, 456–460.
Dennerstein, L. Dudley, E. Hopper, J., and Burger, H. (1997). Sexuality, hormones and the
menopausal transition. Maturitas, 26, 83–93.
Dennerstein, L., Lehert, P., Burger, H., and Dudley, E. (1999). Factors affecting sexual
functioning of women in the midlife years. Climateric, 2, 254–262.
Diokno, A.C., Brown, M.B., and Rugula-Herzog, A. (1990). Sexual function in the elderly.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 150, 197–200.
Ellis, B.J., and Symons, D. (1990). Sex differences in sexual fantasy: An evolutionary
psychological approach. Journal of Sex Research, 27, 527–555.
Ellison, C.R. (2001). A research inquiry into some American women’s sexual concerns and
problems. In E. Kaschak and L. Tiefer (Eds.), A New View of Women’s Sexual Problems
(pp. 147–160). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Faith, M.S., and Schare, M.L. (1993). The role of body image in sexually avoidant behavior.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22, 345–356.
Fonatana, B. (1994). Plastic sex and the sociologist: A comment on “The transformation of
intimacy” by Anthony Giddens. Economy & Society, 73, 374–384.
Fooken, L. (1994). Sexuality in later years: The impact of health and body image in a sample
of older women. Patient Education and Counseling, 23, 227–233.
Foucault, M. (1978). The History of Sexuality (Part 1): An Introduction. New York: Random
House.
Frank, E., Anderson, C., and Rubinstein, D. (1978). Frequency of sexual dysfunction in
“normal” couples. New England Journal of Medicine, 229, 111–115.
Geison, C.B. (1989). Aging and attractiveness: Marriage makes a difference. International
Journal of Aging and Human Development, 29(2), 83–94.
George, L.K., and Weiler, S.J. (1981). Sexuality in middle and late life. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 38, 919–923.
Hallstrom, T. (1977). Sexuality in the climacteric. Clinical Obstetrical Gynecology, 4, 227–
239.
Hallstrom, T., and Samuelson, S. (1990). Changes in women’s sexual desire in middle
life: The longitudinal study of women in Gotenborg. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19,
259–268.
Harris, G. (2004, February 28). Pfizer gives up trying to prove Viagra works for women.
New York Times, pp. B1, B2.
Irvine, J. (1990). Disorders of Desire: Sex and Gender in Modern American Sexology.
Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Kaschak, E., and Tiefer, L. (Eds.). (2001). A New View of Women’s Sexual Problems.
Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.B., and Martin, C.E. (1953). Sexual Behavior in the Human
Female. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kliger, L., and Nedelman, D. (2005, July). Redefining “Sexy”: Sexual Desire and Sexual
Self-esteem in Women Beyond 50. Paper presented at the New View Conference, Montreal,
Canada.
Kliger, L., and Nedelman, D. (2006). Still Sexy after all these Years? The 9 Unspoken Truths
about Women’s Desire. New York: Berkeley.
Koch, P.B. (1995). Exploring our Sexuality: An Interactive Text. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Koch, P.B. (1997). Feminism and sexuality in the United States. In R.T. Francoeur (Ed.), The
International Encyclopedia of Sexuality (Vol. 3, pp. 1375–1382). New York: Continuum.
50 Maureen C. McHugh
Koch, P.B., Mansfield, P.K., Thurau, D., and Carey, M. (2005). “Feeling frumpy”: The
relationship between body image and sexual response changes in midlife women. Journal
of Sex Research, 42, 215–222.
Koch, P.B., Voda, A., and Mansfield, P.K. (1995). Predictors of sexual response changes in
heterosexual midlife women. Health Values, 19(1), 10–21.
Laumann, E.O., Paik, A., and Rosen, R.C. (1999). Sexual dysfunction in the United States:
Prevalence and predictors. Journal of the American Medical Association, 28, 537–
544.
Laws, J.L. (1980). Female sexuality through the life span. In P.B. Baltes and O.G. Brim, Jr.
(Eds.), Life Span Development and Behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 207–252). New York: Academic
Press.
Leiblum, S.R. (1990). Sexuality and the midlife woman. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
14, 495–508.
Leiblum, S.R., and Perrin, L. (Eds.). (1980). Principles and Practice of Sex Therapy. New
York: Guilford.
Loe, M. (2004). Sex and the senior woman: Pleasure and danger in the Viagra era. Sexualities,
7, 303–326.
Mansfield, P.K., Koch, P.B., and Voda, A.M. (1998). Qualities midlife women desire in their
sexual relationships and their changing sexual response. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
22, 285–303.
Mansfield, P.K., Koch, P.B., and Voda, A.M. (2000). Midlife women’s attributions
for their sexual response changes. Health Care for Women International, 31, 543–
559.
Mansfield, P.K., Voda, A.M., and Koch, P.B. (1995). Predictors of sexual response changes
in heterosexual midlife women. Health Values, 19(1), 10–20.
Marrow, J. (1997). Changing Positions: Women Speak Out on Sex and Desire. Holbrook,
MA: Adams Media.
Martin, K.A. (1996). Puberty, Sexuality, and the Self: Girls and Boys at Adolescence. New
York: Routledge.
Masters, W.H., and Johnson, V.E. (1966). Human Sexual Response. Boston: Little, Brown.
McCoy, N., Cutler, W.B., and Davidson, J.M. (1985). Relationships among sexual behavior,
hot flashes, and hormone levels in peri-menopausal women. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
14, 381–390.
McCoy, N.L., and Davidson, J.M. (1985). A longitudinal study of the effects of menopause
on sexuality. Maturitas, 7, 203–210.
McHugh, M.C. (in press). What do women want? A New View of women’s sexual problems.
Sex Roles.
Miller, J.B. (1976). Toward a New Psychology of Women. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Morokoff, P.J. (1988). Sexuality in peri-menopausal and post-menopausal women. Psy-
chology of Women Quarterly, 12, 489–511.
Moynihan, R. (2001). The making of a disease: Female sexual dysfunction. British Medical
Journal, 326, 45–57.
Moynihan, R. (2004). Fix for low sex drive puts reporters in a bad patch. British Medical
Journal, 329, 1294.
Myers, L.S. (1995). Methodological review and meta-analysis of sexuality and menopause
research. Neuroscience and Bio-behavioral Reviews, 19, 331–314.
Ogden, G. (1999). Women Who Love Sex: An Inquiry into the Expanding Spirit of Women’s
Erotic Experience (rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: Womanspirit Press.
2. Women and Sex at Midlife 51
Osborn, M., Hawton, K., and Gath, D. (1988). Sexual dysfunction among middle-aged
women in the community. British Medical Journal, 296, 959–962.
Peplau, L.A., and Gordon, S.L. (1985). Women and men in love: Gender differences in
close heterosexual relationships. In V.E. O’Leary, R.K. Unger, and B.S. Walston (Eds.),
Women, Gender, and Social Psychology (pp. 257–291). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pfeiffer, E., Verwoerdt, A., and Davis, G. (1972). Sexual behavior in middle life. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 1262–1267.
Potts, A., Gavey, N., Grace, V.M., and Vares, T. (2003) The downside of Viagra: Women’s
experiences and concerns. Sociology of Health and Illness, 25, 697–719.
Richgels, P.B. (1992). Hypoactive sexual desire in heterosexual women: A feminist analysis.
Women & Therapy, 12(1/2), 123–135.
Riley, A., and Riley, E. (2000). Behavioral and clinical findings in couples where the man
presents with erectile disorder: A retrospective study. International Journal of Clinical
Practice, 54, 220–224.
Shah, S. (2001). The orgasm industry. Progressive, 65, 29.
Sheehy, G. (2006). Sex and the Seasoned Woman: Pursuing the Passionate Life. New York:
Random House.
Schwartz, P., and Rutter, V. (2000). The Gender of Sexuality. New York: Rowan & Littlefield.
Taylor, D., and Sumrall, A.C. (1993). The Time of Our Lives: Women Write on Sex after 40.
Freedom, CA: Crossing Press.
Tiefer, L. (2001a). A new view of women’s sexual problems: Why new? Why now? Journal
of Sexual Research, 38, 89–96.
Tiefer, L. (2001b). Arriving at a “new view” of women’s sexual problems: Background,
theory, and activism. In E. Kaschak and L. Tiefer (Eds.), A New View of Women’s Sexual
Problems (pp. 63–90). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Tiefer, L. (1995). Sex Is not a Natural Act and Other Essays. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Tiggerman, M., and Lynch, J.E. (2002). Body image across the life span in adult women:
The role of objectification. Developmental Psychology, 37, 243–253.
Tolman, D.L. (2001). Echoes of sexual objectification: Listening for one girl’s erotic voice.
In D.L. Tolman and M. Brydon-Miller (Eds.), From Subjects to Subjectivities: A Hand-
book of Interpretive and Participatory Methods (pp. 130–144). New York: New York
University Press.
Tolman, D.L. (2002). Female adolescent sexuality: An argument for a developmental per-
spective on the new view of sexual problems. In E. Kaschak and L. Tiefer (Eds.), A New
View of Women’s Sexual Problems (pp. 195–210). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Trafford, A. (1995, May 16). What the pill did. Washington Post. p. 26.
Traupmann, J., Eckles, E., and Hatfield, E. (1982). Intimacy in older women’s lives. Geron-
tologist, 22, 493–498.
Vance, C.S. (1984). Pleasure and danger: Toward a politics of sexuality. In C.S. Vance
(Ed.), Pleasure and Danger: Exploring Female Sexuality (pp. 1–27). Boston: Routledge
& Kegan Paul.
Voda, A.M., and George, T. (1986). Menopause. In H.H. Wesley, J.J. Fitzpatrick, and R.L.
Taunton (Eds.), Annual Review of Nursing Research (pp. 55–75). New York: Springer.
Williams, S.P. (2001). Reaching the hard to reach: Implications of the new view of women’s
sexual problems. In E. Kaschak and L. Tiefer (Eds.), A New View of Women’s Sexual
Problems (pp. 39–42). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Winterich, J.A. (2003). Sex, menopause, and culture: Sexual orientation and the meaning
of menopause for women’s sex lives. Gender & Society, 17, 627–642.
52 Maureen C. McHugh
Wood, J.M., Mansfield, P.K., and Koch, P. (in press). Negotiating sexual agency: Post-
menopausal women’s meaning and experiences of sexual desire. Qualitative Health Re-
search.
Working Group on Women’s Sexual Problems. (n.d.). The “New View” Manifesto. Re-
treived May 10, 2004 from the Female Sexual Dysfucntion-Alert website: http//www.fsd-
alert.org/manifesto.html.
3
Living Longer, Healthier Lives
Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
Contrary to popular images that portray a bleak future, important life tasks and
accomplishments characterize the lives of older women. Midlife women are more
likely than not to work full-time, and some will earn the highest salaries of their
careers. A high percentage of women in public service or elected office are in their
50s and 60s. Bernadine Healy was in her late 40s when she became the first woman
to serve as Director of the National Institutes of Health and later became Director of
the American Red Cross. Diane Feinstein was in her late 50s when she was the first
woman elected to the U.S. Senate from California; she has subsequently served
three terms. Women beyond menopause energize the planning of community parks
and greenways projects and serve on school boards. Increasingly older women
are likely to run a small business and to provide jobs and paychecks for others.
Older women support daughters and sons in transition to adult identities, and, not
infrequently, rear their own grandchildren when their adult children suffer illness
or economic reversals. Older women have developmental tasks involving their
own generativity, values, and significant relationships. They write textbooks, learn
new languages, and become articulate advocates for causes of all sorts. These
examples of active, vital women run counter to common stereotypes of older
women. Therefore, the purposes of this chapter are to examine major findings
concerning the health status of women in their 50s and 60s and to explore the
health challenges and opportunities this population may experience. Given societal
notions about menopausal women, we begin our discussion with the menopausal
transition.
53
54 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
women are understood to be “naturally” more closely tied to their biology. Such
a cultural understanding leads readily to the idea that women, their experience,
emotions, and behaviors, are best understood in terms of biology, especially repro-
ductive biology. As women age, it is women’s flagging biology that is perceived
as the problem, and this perception provides a cultural context that supports hor-
mone replacement and estrogen supplements. Popular books and magazine articles
promised women they could be “feminine forever,” if only they retained their es-
sential biological profile. Thus, a ready “cure” for the negative stereotypic aspects
of menopause has traditionally involved replacement of reproductive hormones,
particularly estrogens (Chrisler et al., 1991).
Early reports about the health benefits derived from estrogen replacement of-
ten were based on self-selected women who sought out treatment and on women
who were initially healthier than women who did not seek hormones as preventive
medical care. Because participants in these studies differed initially on important
health status indicators, later findings that they continued to experience different
health outcomes cannot be attributed to hormone replacement. One large-scale
study (Luoto et al., 2000) showed no differences in the incidence of acute myocar-
dial infarction (i.e., heart attack) between women using estrogen and those who
had never used estrogen supplements. The same study revealed that hopes that
estrogen might protect aging women against brain atrophy were unfounded.
estrogen plus progestin replacement; it increases the risk of heart attack and blood
clots. (Summaries of these reports are available on the web from the National Heart
Lung and Blood Institute of NIH at http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/resources/docs/w-
health.htm.)
Heart Disease
Most people believe the cultural stereotype that heart disease is a man’s disease.
However, heart disease is the leading cause of death for women as well as for men in
Western societies. For example, in 1995, over 300,000 women were hospitalized
specifically for acute myocardial infarction, and over 1.5 million women were
hospitalized for chronic ischemic heart disease. In fact, for well over a decade the
total number of women hospitalized with some type of cardiovascular condition
has been consistently greater than the number of men. This gender pattern has been
documented annually from 1990 through 2000 in the National Hospital Discharge
Survey conducted by the National Center for Health Statistics (Travis, 2005).
Women in general do not worry as much about heart disease as they do about
breast cancer, though heart disease kills many more women. Furthermore, many
women have been guided by physicians’ beliefs and assumptions that whatever
risk they have of heart disease could be handled by estrogen replacement therapy
after menopause.
56 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
particularly evident for women in relatively young age groups (40s and 50s), when
angina was a primary diagnosis, and in the presence of any chronic health condi-
tion (e.g., diabetes or hypertension) (Travis, 2005). Thus, gender stereotypes and
sex discrimination continue to affect decisions about the treatment of midlife and
older women with cardiovascular conditions.
Breast Cancer
Cancer is overall the second leading cause of death among women of all ages,
and it accounts for nearly one-quarter of deaths from all causes. However, among
women 45 to 75 years, it is the leading cause of death (American Cancer Society,
2005). Although it was once rare among women, lung cancer is now one of the
more common forms of cancer among women of all ages. Roughly 80,000 U.S.
women are diagnosed with lung cancer each year, and nearly an equal number of
women die of lung cancer each year (American Cancer Society, 2005). Most of
these cases are caused by cigarette smoking, as about 20% of American women
smoke (American Cancer Society, 2005; Wolberg, 2004). Notably, rates of cancer
vary among White women and Women of Color. Rates of death for all types of
cancer are consistently and dramatically higher among Black women than among
other race—sex groups (American Cancer Society, 2005).
Although deaths from lung cancer are the most common form of cancer among
women, the most dreaded form surely is breast cancer. Approximately 200,000
women are diagnosed with breast cancer each year (American Cancer Society,
2005). Fortunately, about 60% of these cases are detected early, when the tumors
are fairly small and localized or in situ (i.e., no involvement of other types of
tissues, organs, or lymph nodes). When breast cancer is detected and treated at this
early point, 10-year survival rates are very high; estimates range from 72 to 92%
(Fletcher et al., 2005; Romero et al., 2004; Solin et al., 2005). As with most major
diseases, age is the biggest risk factor for cancer, thus annual mammograms after
age 50 are one of the best forms of prevention. Another factor is family history.
However, inherited genetic risk accounts for only about 3 to 5% of all breast cancers
(Eccles & Pichert, 2005). Short of a genetic screening test, the best indicator of an
inherited gene problem is early age of onset. Heritable breast cancers often emerge
before women reach menopause, whereas onset at an older age, by itself, conveys
little definite information about genetic factors. Thus, having a family member
who developed breast cancer at age 75 is not a strong indicator of an inherited
genetic problem.
Beyond risks associated with the general processes of aging, a major risk factor
for breast cancer is the lifetime exposure to estrogens; the more estrogen, the more
breast cancer. It has long been known, for example, that women are at greater
risk for breast cancer if they have experienced an early start to menstruation (first
menses before age 13) or if they have late menopause (after age 50) (McPherson,
2004). In addition, direct prospective evidence exists for a positive link between
58 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
levels of estrone and estradiol (forms of estrogen) (Toniolo et al., 1995). Replace-
ment estrogens during menopause are an example of this, and have already been
discussed. The potential cancer risk from exogenous estrogens during the years
of normal menstrual cycling (ages 13 to 50) is difficult to assess, as virtually all
women are also producing their own estrogens at the time.
Osteoporosis
Eighty percent of individuals in the United States diagnosed with osteoporosis are
women; currently at least 10 million Americans have this disease, and another
34 million are at risk for developing it by virtue of their low bone mass (Office of
the Surgeon General, 2004). The fact that osteoporosis is the source of the greatest
number of fractures in the elderly is perhaps the most health-impairing aspect of
this disease. Unfortunately, osteoporosis is both underdiagnosed and undertreated,
even in those women who have already suffered at least one fracture. According
to the Surgeon General, about 1.5 million people a year suffer an osteoporosis-
related fracture, most often in the hip, spine, or wrist; of those with hip fractures,
20% will likely die within a year, and another 20% will end up confined to a
nursing home (Office of the Surgeon General, 2004). Clearly, women in their
50s and 60s may benefit not only from early screening, diagnosis, and treatment,
but also from health promotion behaviors initiated earlier in their lives. Women
are encouraged to ensure that they routinely obtain the recommended amounts
of calcium in their diets, engage in a regular program of weight-bearing activity,
and request osteoporosis screening (i.e., bone density tests) following any bone
fracture. Unfortunately, recent research suggests that strikingly few—18%—of
female Medicare patients over age 67 receive either bone density screening or
medication following diagnosis of a fracture, despite the fact that treatment of
osteoporosis significantly reduces the risk of subsequent fractures (Office of the
Surgeon General, 2004). Though it is possible that many older men with fragility
fractures do not receive postfracture screening for osteoporosis (Kiebzak et al.,
2002), we cannot help but wonder whether health care practitioners would fail to
60 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
provide the gold standard of care for fractures in older adults (Gardner et al., 2002;
Hajcsar et al., 2000) if osteoporosis were a condition diagnosed more often in men
rather than in women.
Bone density can be measured, but testing with the most reliable equipment is
expensive. Health fairs often offer ultrasound imaging of the heel that produces a
quick score, but it is not particularly accurate. Dual X-ray absorptometry (DXA)
is the most accurate test. DXA involves a large machine that uses two X-ray beams
that are passed through the bone, and typically focuses on the spine and/or hip.
Calcium intake and physical activity throughout life are recommended to prevent
osteoporosis. The National Institute of Health Consensus Panel on Calcium con-
cluded that a daily intake of 1500 mg of calcium is recommended for older women,
but this intake is seldom met (National Consensus Development Panel on Optimal
Calcium Intake, 1994). Natural food sources high in calcium include most dairy
products that often are restricted in efforts to control weight or cholesterol. Dark
green vegetables typically contain notable levels of calcium; less commonly rec-
ognized sources include fruits such as cantaloupes, dried fruits, and many berries.
Nutritional supplements of calcium are widely available, but the plethora of for-
mulations can be puzzling. For example, although calcium in the form of carbonate
and citrate is absorbed at about the same rate, calcium citrate may be better for
people with reduced stomach acid (Office of Dietary Supplements, 2006). Prescrip-
tion supplements have gained increasing recognition in recent years. Some of the
commonly prescribed bisphosphonates are risedronate (Actonel), r alendronate
(Fosamax), r and etidronate (Didrocal). r Most of these block the action of osteo-
clasts that breakdown bone for resorption. Alternative pharmaceutical approaches
may use SERMs (special estrogen receptor modulators); side effects associated
with SERMs seem to mimic those of menopause, such as hot flashes. Low bone
density is associated with more fractures, but the extent to which fracture risk is
causally reduced by these pharmaceutical interventions remains to be determined.
For example, surveys might indicate a greatly reduced risk of fracture among those
taking the medication, even though the medication raised actual bone density only
slightly. Whether these studies suffer from the same flaws as the early estrogen
replacement studies is, of course, a concern. Women who seek care and follow
prescription regimens simply may have better overall health, diet, and lifestyle
than those who do not. Thus, the beneficial outcomes of lower fracture may be due
as much to lifestyle health as to prescription medications.
Arthritis
Joint diseases constitute the leading cause of disability among American adults
and, unfortunately, arthritis rates also increase with age. The rate of arthritis among
women age 75 and older is more than five times the rate in women 18 to 44 years of
age (National Center for Health Statistics, 2003). Among the most common forms
of arthritis are osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, systemic lupus erythematosus,
gout, and fibromyalgia (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2005b). Of
these joint diseases, only gout is diagnosed less frequently in women than in men.
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 61
Arthritis represents a significant health issue for women in that it is the leading
reason for activity limitations in this population (Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, 2005b). Osteoarthritis, characterized by pain and restricted movement
occurs most frequently in the knees, hips, spine, and hand. About one-half of all
women over the age of 65 experience some pain and some activity limitations asso-
ciated with this condition (National Center for Health Statistics, 2003). In contrast
to the localized nature of osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis is a systemic inflam-
matory joint disease most likely caused by a faulty immune response. In addition
to eroding bone and cartilage, which causes pain, swelling, and redness in the
joints, this inflammatory process can also affect other organs. Patients coping with
arthritis conditions have a variety of medical and behavioral treatment strategies
available to them. Numerous prescription and nonprescription medicines may be
used to treat the pain and inflammation associated with these conditions; however,
it is important to note that pharmacotherapy is not without side effects or risks. Pa-
tients with advanced osteoarthritis may wish to consider joint replacement surgery.
Although arthritis-associated pain is often a barrier to exercise, women often find
low-impact activities (e.g., water aerobics, swimming) to be both physically tol-
erable and health enhancing methods of coping with a chronic musculoskeletal
condition. Along with exercise, weight management is also essential for reducing
stress on weight-bearing joints.
status (SES), negative emotions, and physical health, Gallo and Matthews (2003)
proposed that low-SES environments are often stressful, thereby reducing indi-
viduals’ “reserve capacity to manage stress” and increasing their vulnerability to
negative cognitions (e.g., pessimism, hopelessness) and emotions (e.g., anxiety,
depression, worry). In summary, constructive emotion-focused coping strategies
should ideally provide opportunities to identify and experience positive emotions
(e.g., determination, hope) and also help women to identify and practice methods
of coping with those negative emotions that may occur in the context of chronic
illness or pain. Positive emotions are generally an indicator of hope and the reso-
lution of doubt. However, one would not want to demand that women have only
positive emotions; so-called negative emotions such as anger can also energize
positive actions.
In contrast to emotion-focused coping, problem-focused coping strategies are
directed at understanding and solving health-related problems. Typically, these
methods of coping involve information gathering and planning, strategies that
require some degree of activity on the part of the patient. Problem-focused coping
methods, particularly information seeking, have been associated with more positive
health outcomes (i.e., slower disease progression, fewer symptoms) and improved
health-related quality of life (Ransom et al., 2005). Unfortunately, as women age,
their risk for other health-compromising conditions (e.g., heart, respiratory, and
joint diseases) may increase; in turn, these chronic health problems can potentially
limit their ability to engage in some of the more active forms of problem-focused
coping (e.g., exercise, relaxation procedures).
Patients are often encouraged to use social support as a source of coping with
illness, however, it is important to note that the relationship between social support
and health outcomes is somewhat complicated (Aldwin & Yancura, 2004; DiMat-
teo, 2004). Several studies suggest that social support from family and friends
may boost patients’ adherence to treatment via a number of different mechanisms,
most notably, by encouraging optimism (Brissette et al., 2002; Symister & Friend,
2003), reducing depression and isolation (Brown et al., 1989; Goodenow et al.,
1990; Symister & Friend, 2003), and providing practical (or tangible) assistance
(DiMatteo, 2004; Wallston et al., 1983). The results of a recent meta-analysis of
the social support and treatment adherence literature indicate that “[f]unctional so-
cial support has stronger effects on adherence than does structural social support,
suggesting that the mere presence of other people does not matter as much as the
quality of relationships with them” (DiMatteo, 2004, p. 212). It is not surprising,
then, that health professionals have become increasingly interested in relationship
(Badr & Acitelli, 2005; Manne et al., 2006) and family-based coping interventions
(Martire et al., 2004). The goals of these system-based interventions are ideally to
improve patient health outcomes, reduce illness symptoms, enhance psychological
well-being, and provide empathy and support (Martire et al., 2004), yet, to date,
there are relatively few empirical investigations of the outcomes of such interven-
tions in comparison to individual patient-oriented approaches. In a review of just
12 family-based intervention studies, Martire et al. (2004) found that five of 12
investigations demonstrated clear advantages associated with social support, most
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 65
female survivors of hemorrhagic stroke showed that these women often expressed
concerns about how they might be perceived by others. More specifically, women
in the study were clearly aware that stroke is typically viewed as a disease of
old age and that “disabilities worth taking seriously are readily visible” (Stone,
2005, p. 293). Despite the possibility of such misperceptions, women who find
themselves in need of disability accommodations should be encouraged to pur-
sue whatever mechanisms are available to improve both their quality of life and
personal functioning. Such accommodations might include, but are not limited to,
work station adjustments (i.e., ergonomic improvements) and other adaptive equip-
ment, improved accessibility, and schedule modifications that will allow for time
and access to required health care. Because women’s work lives often contribute to
their sense of psychological well-being, as well as their financial stability, women
should not be pressured to take early retirement or disability until all avenues of
workplace accommodation have been explored.
Informational Support
As mentioned previously, problem-focused coping strategies have been frequently
linked to improved health outcomes in women. Information seeking represents one
such problem-focused approach to coping. Women who have been diagnosed with
chronic diseases or conditions often benefit from access to current information
about their conditions and available treatment alternatives. Such information is
often found from one of three resources: the woman’s own health care provider(s),
support groups, and print or electronic media. Women with chronic illness ideally
will have the opportunity to develop collaborative working relationships with their
treatment providers, an issue to which we will return later in this chapter. Disease
or condition-specific support groups often provide another important source of
information and social support for patients. Although many communities have
health information libraries associated with either their local universities or medical
centers, the Internet is rapidly becoming a primary source of health information
for patients (Bansil et al., 2006) who are dealing with a wide range of chronic
diseases.
Community Resources
Whether women are focused primarily on health promotion and disease preven-
tion or are already having chronic health problems, there are a growing number of
community resources available for information, education, and support. In some
communities, women may avail themselves of the resources of a local commu-
nity health library; in others, health information may be obtained from county
health departments or health care facilities. Those women who have experience
with and access to the Internet can find a plethora of information related to health
promotion, disease prevention, and the treatment of acute and chronic illness. For
example, both the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) and the National Institutes
of Health (NIH) maintain Web sites with links to important health and illness infor-
mation for consumers as well as for their health care providers. Many organizations
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 67
concerned with education about and prevention of specific diseases (e.g., arthritis,
heart disease) have established websites that provide comprehensive information
for newly diagnosed patients and their families. Finally, the Internet is home to
many electronic support groups that are often established to provide informational
and social support for those diagnosed with specific diseases (e.g., inflammatory
bowel disease, breast cancer). Women who enter the name of a specific disease or
health topic in an Internet search engine (e.g., Google) are likely to find dozens, if
not hundreds, of links to websites with information related to their topic of inter-
est. As information becomes more readily available online, women may encounter
conflicting information and recommendations, which requires some structure or
strategy for evaluating the accuracy and usefulness of the health care recommenda-
tions available to them via this medium. One such strategy might involve gathering
information from multiple Internet sources, compiling a list of recommendations,
and then discussing this information with health care providers to determine the
best course of treatment for the individual woman.
As mentioned previously, informational support is often an important compo-
nent of problem-focused coping. Women who possess up-to-date, accurate infor-
mation about their illnesses not only may find themselves more empowered to
manage their health or illness, but also to advocate for more collaborative and
satisfactory relationships with their health care providers.
Patient–Provider Relationships
Beginning in adolescence, women are more likely than men to utilize the health
care system (National Center for Health Statistics, 2003). Women ages 45 to 64
average more than eight physician contacts per year compared to about six per
year for men. It is not surprising then that a number of researchers have examined
women’s experiences with health care and health care providers. One nationwide
study conducted by the Commonwealth Fund’s Commission on Women’s Health
(2003) indicated that women were twice as likely as men to report negative feelings
about the patient–provider relationship. More specifically, 25% of women believed
that their physicians “talked down to them,” and 17% reported that their physicians
68 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
suggested that their health problems were “all in their heads.” According to a special
task force report of the American Medical Association, women are more likely
than men to have their symptoms attributed to “overanxiousness”—a psychological
problem—even when medical test results suggest the presence of an organic (i.e.,
physical) problem (Task Force, Ethical and Judicial Affairs, American Medical
Association, 1991). Physicians may also tend to underestimate women’s (and
patients of lower socioeconomic status)1 understanding of medical terminology
as well as their interest in receiving technical information, which results in a
gender disparity in the technical complexity and specificity of information given
to patients (Sprague-Zones, 1995). It is not surprising, then, that women are more
likely than men to change physicians, perhaps in large part due to problems in the
patient–provider relationship.
Women over 65 participating in focus groups reported that, although their phys-
ical health needs were generally met, a number of areas of the patient–provider
relationship might otherwise be improved (Tannenbaum et al., 2003). Among these
were women’s desire to feel validated as active participants in their health care,
the importance of recognizing women’s aging-related concerns and anxieties, and
the need for greater information sharing and education in the patient–provider re-
lationship. The women who participated in that study were particularly interested
in learning more about diet, exercise, and other health maintenance strategies as-
sociated with successful aging, yet they believed that time, accessibility, and con-
tinuity of care represented significant barriers to obtaining effective health care.
More specifically, women suggested that their providers did not always have suffi-
cient time to listen to and address their health care concerns or to provide detailed
information about the normal aging process. Although patient satisfaction and pa-
tient adherence to medical regimens have been found to be improved by a good
patient–provider relationship (Carlson & Skochelak, 1998),2 the fundamental goal
always should be to improve the quality of care patients receive. Positive nonver-
bal behavior, optimistic talk, and social conversation may contribute to an overall
good feeling about the encounter, but they should not substitute for technical com-
petence or for thorough understanding of the patients’ symptoms and concerns.
Similarly, general sociable conversation should not be used to avoid recognition
of or resolution of possible disagreements. Fortunately, effective patient–provider
communication has been shown to be associated with improved health outcomes
in breast cancer, diabetes, hypertension, and peptic ulcer disease (Kaplan et al.,
1989).
1
The similarity of patterns for women in general and for anyone in a lower socioeconomic
status supports the point that gender is a status or class variable and not simply a personal
attribute.
2
Research has suggested that female physicians may engage in more interactive question
asking and dialogue (Bertakis et al., 1995; Roter et al., 1991). However, this is sometimes
confounded with the area of specialization, for example, pediatrics.
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 69
Consent to Treatment
Women with acute or chronic disease often face a number of important decisions
concerning their treatment. Although advances in technology afford women more
opportunity than ever to collect independent information about available treatment
alternatives, treatment decisions ultimately develop in the context of an individ-
ual patient’s relationship with her health care provider. Consequently, informed
consent to treatment becomes an issue of paramount importance.
Advance Directives
Given the ethical and legal climate in health care today, women are strongly en-
couraged to anticipate and plan for the possibility that they, at some point in the
future, may not be capable of providing informed consent for medical treatment. A
number of legal documents have been designed to assist patients in communicat-
ing their wishes should such circumstances arise. More specifically, women of all
ages—but particularly those in midlife—are advised to prepare advance directives
for health care. Generally, three types of advance directives are available: a living
will, a durable power of attorney for health care, and a do-not-resuscitate (DNR)
order. Such documents are intended to provide patients’ families and health care
providers with specific instructions about future treatment and to identify those
who have been legally authorized to make treatment decisions on behalf of the
patient.
One type of written document, the living will, describes for family members and
health care providers the types of medical care the patient wants to receive in the
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 71
goals of nurturing and caring for others. This orientation has been valorized as a
moral framework that is predominate among, if not unique to, women. In fact, a
systematic review of research on gender differences in a care perspective of moral-
ity indicates that differences are small and that most women and men rely on a
combination of attention to principled rules and to relational caring (Jaffe & Hyde,
2000). Although many women may feel guilty about being assertive or advocat-
ing for themselves, such agentic behavior may foster better problem solving and
greater satisfaction with outcomes. As noted earlier in this chapter, women benefit
from a number of health-promoting behaviors (e.g., nutrition, physical activity,
stress management), particularly when established earlier in their lives, however,
given the multiple role demands often experienced by adult women, it may be
difficult to find time for these health-promoting activities. Consequently, we must
encourage women to view self-care as a priority in their lives and to implement
healthy behaviors in support of that goal. For many women, this is easier said than
done, but important nonetheless.
In thinking about ways to support women’s health at midlife, we consider both
educational and action strategies at four levels: (a) the individual woman; (b)
the relationship and family system; (c) the professional health care provider; and
(d) society and public policy. For the individual woman, we cannot overstate the
importance of education about health-promoting behaviors, disease prevention,
and both emotion- and problem-focused approaches to managing chronic health
conditions. It is also clear that women often benefit from increased knowledge of
their own bodies and health status, as well as from greater personal advocacy when
dealing with health care providers and systems.
At a relational or family system level, it is important once again to acknowledge
the social context of women’s lives. A woman’s health may be affected by the cir-
cumstances in which she lives; in turn, when women are living with chronic health
conditions or pain there are likely to be effects on the family system. For example,
research with sheltered homeless and low-income women suggests that physi-
cal violence and childhood physical or sexual abuse are associated with avoidant
coping strategies and increased depression (Rayburn et al., 2005). In contrast, func-
tional social support from family and friends is often an important contributor to a
woman’s ability to cope with challenging health conditions. Encouraging family
members to become educated about the symptoms, course, and treatment of a dis-
ease may improve their understanding of women’s experience; similarly, improv-
ing communication between women and their intimate partners may help to ame-
liorate the negative effects of illness and pain on such interpersonal relationships.
Health care providers who work with women at midlife should commit
themselves to a number of supportive practices; for example, taking time to provide
information to patients, engaging women as collaborators in the health care rela-
tionship, and encouraging and supporting informed, autonomous decision-making
about the prevention and management of disease. We expect that practitioners
would also avail themselves of the current medical and behavioral scientific liter-
ature that addresses the health status and health care needs of women at midlife
and beyond.
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 73
References
Aldwin, C.M., and Revenson, T.A. (1987). Does coping help? A reexamination of the
relation between coping and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
53, 337–348.
Aldwin, C.M., and Yancura, L.A. (2004). Coping and health: A comparison of the stress and
trauma literature. In P.P. Schnurr and B.L. Green (Eds.), Physical Health Consequences
of Exposure to Extreme Stress (pp. 99–125). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
American Cancer Society. (2005). Estimated new cancer cases and deaths by sex for all
sites. Cancer Facts and Figures. Retrieved 9-19-05, from http://www.cancer.org/ doc-
root/STT/stt 0.asp:
American Medical Association, Council on Ethical and Judicial Affairs. (2004). Code of
Medical Ethics, Current Opinions with Annotations 2004–2004. Chicago: American
Medical Association.
Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990. (1990). Retrieved 1-12-06, from http://www.
usdoj.gov/crt/ada/pubs/ada.txt: U.S. Department of Justice
Arthritis Foundation. (2005). The Facts about Arthritis. Retrieved 11/6/05, from http://www.
arthritis.org/resources/gettingstarted/default.asp.
Ayanian, J.Z., and Epstein, A.M. (1991). Differences in the use of procedures between
women and men hospitalized for coronary heart disease. New England Journal of
Medicine, 325, 221–225.
74 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
Badr, H., and Acitelli, L.K. (2005). Dyadic adjustment in chronic illness: Does relationship
talk matter? Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 465–469.
Bansil, P., Keenan, N.L., Zlot, A.I., and Gilliland, J.C. (2006). Health-related information
on the Web: Results from the Health Styles Survey, 2002–2003. Preventing Chronic
Disease, 3(2), A36.
Barnefski, N., Legerstee, J., Kraaij, V., van-den Kommer, T., and Teerds, J. (2002). Cogni-
tive coping strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety: A comparison between
adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 603–611.
Bertakis, K.D., Helms, L.J., Callahan, E.J., Azari, R., and Robbins, J.A. (1995). The influ-
ence of gender on physician practice style. Medical Care, 33, 407–416.
Brissette, I., Scheier, M.F., and Carver, C.S. (2002). The role of optimism in social network
development, coping, and psychological adjustment during a life transition. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 102–111.
Broadwell, A.W., Boisaubin, E.V., Dunn, L.K., and Engelhardt, H.T. (1993). Advance
directives on hospital admission: A survey of patient attitudes. Southern Medical Journal,
86(2), 165–168.
Brown, G.K., Wallston, K.A., and Nicassio, P.M. (1989). Social support and depression in
rheumatoid arthritis: A one-year prospective study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
19, 1164–1181.
Carlson, K.J., and Skochelak, S.E. (1998). What do women want in a doctor? Communica-
tion issues between women and physicians. In L.A. Wallis (Ed.), Textbook of Women’s
Health (pp. 33–38). Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005a). Bone Health. Retrieved 11/1/05,
from http://www.cdc.gov/needphp/dnpa/bonehealth/: Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005b). Arthritis. Retrieved 11/1/05, from
http://www.cdc.gov/arthritis/index.htm: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
Chlebowski, R.T., Hendrix, S.L., Langer, R.D., Stefanick, M.L., Gass, M., Lane, D., Rod-
abough, R.J., Gilligan, M.A., Cyr, M.G., Thomson, C.A., Khandekar, J., Petrovitch, H.,
McTiernan, A., and Women’s Health Initiative Investigators. (2003). Influence of es-
trogen plus progestin on breast cancer and mammography in healthy postmenopausal
women: The Women’s Health Initiative randomized trial. Journal of the American Med-
ical Association, 289, 3243–3253.
Chrisler, J.C., Torrey, J.W., and Matthes, M.M. (1991). Brittle bones and sagging breasts,
loss of femininity and loss of sanity: The media describe the menopause. In A.M. Voda
and R. Conover (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th conference of the Society for Menstrual
Cycle Research (pp. 23–35). Salt Lake City: Society for Menstrual Cycle Research.
Colcombe, S.J., Kramer, A.F., Erickson, K.I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N.J., Webb,
A., Jerome, G.J., Marquez, D.X., and Elavsky, S. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical
plasticity, and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 3316–3321.
Commission on Women’s Health. (2003). The Commonwealth Fund Survey of Women’s
Health. New York: Commonwealth Fund.
delCarmen, M.G., and Joffe, S. (2005). Informed consent for medical treatment and research:
A review. Oncologist, 10, 636–641.
DiMatteo, M.R. (2004). Social support and patient adherence to medical treatment: A meta-
analysis. Health Psychology, 23, 207–218.
Eccles, D.M., and Pichert, G. (2005). Familial non-BRCA1/BRCA2-associated breast can-
cer. Lancet Oncology, 6, 705–711.
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 75
Edwards, K.A. (1998). Ethics in Medicine: Informed Consent. Retrieved 11/5/05, from
http://depts.washington.edu/bioethx/topics/consent.html.
Fletcher, A.S., Erbas, B., Kavanaugh, A.M., Hart, S., Rodger, A., and Gertig, D.M. (2005).
Use of hormone replacement therapy (HRT) and survival following breast cancer diag-
nosis. Obstetrical and Gyncological Survey, 60, 650–651.
Gallo, L.C., and Matthews, K.A. (2003). Understanding the association between socioe-
conomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychological
Bulletin, 129, 10–51.
Gardner, M.J., Flik, K.R., Mooar, P., and Lane, J.M. (2002). Improvement in the under-
treatment of osteoporosis following hip fracture. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, 84,
1342–1348.
Geller, B. (n.d.). Planning for Medical Care in the Event of Loss of Decision-making Abil-
ity. Retrieved 11/6/05, from http://www.michbar.org/elderlaw/adpamphlet.cfm: Michi-
gan Bar Association.
Goodenow, C., Reisine, S.T., and Grady, K.E. (1990). Quality of social support and asso-
ciated social and psychological functioning in women with rheumatoid arthritis. Health
Psychology, 9, 266–284.
Gylys, K.H., Fein, J.A., Yang, F., Wiley, D.J., Miller, C.A., and Cole, G.M. (2004). Synaptic
changes in Alzheimer’s disease: Increased amyloid and gliosis in surviving terminals is
accompanied by decreased PSD-95 fluorescence. American Journal of Pathology, 165,
1809–1817.
Hajcsar, E.E., Hawker, G., and Bogoch, E.R. (2000). Investigation and treatment of osteo-
porosis in patients with fractures. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 163, 819–822.
Hare-Mustin, R.T., and Marecek, J. (1988). The meaning of difference: Gender theory,
postmodernism, and psychology. American Psychologist, 43, 455–464.
Healy, B. (1991). The Yentil syndrome [Editorial]. New England Journal of Medicine, 325,
274–276.
Hebert, L.E., Scherr, P.A., Bienias, J.L., Bennett, D.A., and Evans, D.A. (2003). Alzheimer’s
Disease in the US population: Prevalence estimates using the 2000 census. Archives of
Neurology, 60, 1119–1122.
Holley, J.L., Stackiewicz, L., Dacko, C., and Rault, R. (1997). Factors influencing dialysis
patients’ completion of advance directives. American Journal of Kidney Disease, 30,
356–360.
Jaffe, S., and Hyde, J.S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726.
Jahng, K.H., Martin, L.R., Golin, C.E., and DiMatteo, M.R. (2005). Preferences for medical
collaboration: Patient–physician congruence and patient outcomes. Patient Education
and Counseling, 57, 308–314.
Jick, H., Walker, A.M., Watkins, R.N., D’Ewart, D.C., Hunter, J.R., Danford, A., Madsen,
S., Dinan, B.J., and Rothman, K.J. (1980). Replacement estrogens and breast cancer.
American Journal of Epidemiology, 112, 586–594.
Jick, H., Zornberg, G.L., Jick, S.S., Seshadri, S., and Drachman, D.A. (2000). Statins and
the risk of dementia. Lancet, 357, 1627–1631.
Kaplan, S.H., Greenfield, S., and Ware, J.E. (1989). Assessing the effects of physician–
patient interactions on the outcomes of chronic disease. Medical Care, 27, S110–S127.
Kiebzak, G.M., Beinart, G.A., Perser, K., Ambrose, G.C., Siff, S.J., and Heggeness, M.H.
(2002). Undertreatment of osteoporosis in men with hip fracture. Archives of Internal
Medicine, 162, 2217–2222.
76 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
King, J. (2005). Informed consent: Does practice match conviction? Journal of the American
College of Dentists, 72, 27–31.
Kozora, E., Ellison, M.C., Waxmonsky, J.A., Wamboldt, F.S., and Patterson, T.L. (2005).
Major life stress, coping styles, and social support in relation to psychological distress
in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus. Lupus, 14, 363–372.
Kramer, A.F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N.J., Banich, M.T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C.R., Chason,
J., Vakil, E., Bardell, L., Boileau, R.A., and Colcombe, A. (1999). Ageing, fitness, and
neurocognitive function. Nature, 400, 418–419.
Lepore, S.J., Ragan, J.D., and Jones, S. (2000). Talking facilitates cognitive-emotional
processes of adaptation to an acute stressor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
78, 499–508.
Luoto, R., Manolio, T., Meilahn, E., Bhadelia, R., Furberg, C., and Cooper, L. (2000).
Estrogen replacement therapy and MRI-demonstrated cerebral infarcts, white matter
changes, and brain atrophy in older women: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Journal
of the American Geriatrics Society, 48, 467–472.
Mack, T.N., Pike, M.C., Henderson, B.E., Pfeffer, R.I., Gerkings, V.R., Arthur, M., and
Brown, S.E. (1976). Estrogens and endometrial cancer in a retirement community. New
England Journal of Medicine, 294, 1262–1267.
Manne, S.L., Ostroff, J.S., Norton, T.R., Fox, K., Goldstein, L., and Grana, G. (2006).
Cancer-related relationship communication in couples coping with early stage breast
cancer. Psychooncology 15, 234–247.
Manne, S.L., Ostroff, J.S., Winkel, G., Fox, K., Grana, G., Miller, E., Ross, S., and Frazier,
T. (2005). Couple-focused group intervention for women with early stage breast cancer.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 634–646.
Martire, L.M., Lustig, A.P., Schulz, R., Miller, G.E., and Helgeson, V.S. (2004). Is it ben-
eficial to involve a family member? A meta-analysis of psychosocial interventions for
chronic illness. Health Psychology, 23, 599–611.
McPherson, K. (2004). Where are we now with hormone replacement therapy? British
Medical Journal, 328, 357–358.
Memmi, A. (1965). The Colonizer and the Colonized. Boston: Beacon Press.
Million Women Study Collaborators. (2003). Breast cancer and hormone-replacement ther-
apy in the Million Women Study. Lancet, 362, 419–427.
Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A., Bienias, J.L., Tangney, C.C., Bennett, D.A., Aggarwal, N.,
Schneider, J., and Wilson, R.S. (2003). Dietary fats and risk of incidents of Alzheimer’s
Disease. Archives of Neurology, 60, 194–200.
Munir, F., Jones, D., Leka, S., and Griffiths, A. (2005). Work limitations and employer
adjustments for employees with chronic illness. International Journal of Rehabilitation,
28, 111–117.
National Center for Health Statistics. (2003). National Health Interview Survey. Atlanta:
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
National Consensus Development Panel on Optimal Calcium Intake. (1994). NIH consensus
conference. Optimal calcium intake. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272,
1942–1948.
Office of Dietary Supplements, National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences. (2006).
Dietary Supplement Fact Sheet: Calcium. In. Retrieved November 21, 2005, from
http://ods.od.nih.gov/factsheets/calcium.asp#h10.
Office of the Surgeon General. (2004). Bone Health and Osteoporosis: A Report
of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services
3. Living Longer, Healthier Lives 77
Ransom, S., Jacobsen, P.B., Schmidt, J.E., and Andrykowski, M.A. (2005). Relationship of
problem-focused coping strategies to changes in quality of life following treatment for
early stage breast cancer. Journal of Pain Symptom Management, 30, 243–253.
Rayburn, N.R., Wenzel, S.L., Elliott, M.N., Hambarsoomians, K., Marshall, G.N., and
Tucker, J.S. (2005). Trauma, depression, coping, and mental health service seeking among
impoverished women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 667–677.
Reidenberg, M.M. (2005). Informed consent or acknowledgment of disclosure [Letter to
the editor]. Clinical Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 78, 439–440.
Richman, L.S., Kubzansky, L., Maselko, J., Kawachi, I., Choo, P., and Bauer, M. (2005).
Positive emotion and health: Going beyond the negative. Health Psychology, 24, 422–429.
Riemsma, R.P., Taal, E., and Rasker, J.J. (2003). Group education for patients with rheuma-
toid arthritis and their partners. Arthritis Care and Research, 49, 556–566.
Romero, L., Klein, L., Ye, W., Holmes, D., Soni, R., Silberman, H., Lagios, M.D., and Sil-
verstein, M.J. (2004). Outcome after invasive recurrence in patients with ductal carcinoma
in situ of the breast. American Journal of Surgery, 188, 371–376.
Rossouw, J.E. (1999). Hormone replacement therapy and cardiovascular disease. Current
Opinion in Lipidology, 10, 429–434.
Rostosky, S.C., and Travis, C.B. (1996). Menopause: An examination of the medical and
psychological literature 1984–1994. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 291–318.
Rostosky, S.S., and Travis, C.B. (2000). Menopause and sexuality: Ageism and sexism unite.
In C.B. Travis and J.W. White (Eds.), Sexuality, Society, and Feminism (pp. 181–210).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Roter, D., Lipkin, M., and Korsgaard, A. (1991). Sex differences in patients’ and physicians’
communication during primary care medical visits. Medical Care, 29, 1083–1093.
Salmond, S.W., and David, E. (2005). Attitudes toward advance directives and advance
directive completion rates. Orthopedic Nursing, 24, 117–127.
Sayre, L.M., Moreira, P.I., Smith, M.A., and Perry, G. (2005). Metal ions and oxidative
protein modification in neurological disease. Annals of the Instituto Superiore di Sanità,
41(2), 143–164.
Simon, C.M., and Kodish, E.D. (2005). “Step into my zapatos, doc”: Understanding and
reducing communication disparities in the multicultural informed consent setting. Per-
spectives in Biology and Medicine, 48(1), S123–S138.
Solin, L.J., Fourquet, A., Vicini, F.A., Taylor, M., Olivotto, I.A., Haffty, B., Strom, E.A.,
Pierce, L.J., Marks, L.B., Bartelink, H., McNeese, M.D., Jhingran, A., Wai, E., Bijker,
N., Campana, F., and Hwang, W.T. (2005). Long-term outcome after breast-conservation
treatment with radiation for mammographically detected ductal carcinoma in situ of the
breast. Cancer, 103, 1137–1146.
Sprague-Zones, J. (1995). Gender effects in physician/patient interaction. In M. Lipkin, S.
Putnam, and A. Lazare (Eds.), The Medical Interview: Clinical Care, Education, and
Research (pp. 163–171). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Steingart, R.M., Forman, S., Coglianese, M., Bittner, V., Mueller, H., Frishman, W., Hand-
berg, E., Gambino, A., Knatterud, G., and Conti, C.R. (1996). Factors limiting the en-
rollment of women in a randomized coronary artery disease trial. Clinical Cardiology,
19, 614–618.
Stone, S.D. (2005). Reactions to invisible disability: The experiences of young women
survivors of hemorrhagic stroke. Disability Rehabilitation, 27, 293–304.
Symister, P., and Friend, R. (2003). The influence of social support and problematic support
on optimism and pessimism in chronic illness: A prospective study evaluating self-esteem
as a mediator. Health Psychology, 22, 123–129.
78 Susan D. Lonborg and Cheryl B. Travis
Tannenbaum, C.B., Nasmith, L., and Mayo, N. (2003). Understanding older women’s health
care concerns: A qualitative study. Journal of Women and Aging, 15(4), 103–116.
Task Force, Ethical and Judicial Affairs, American Medical Association. (1991). Gender
disparities in clinical decision-making. Journal of the American Medical Association,
266, 559–562.
Tobin, J.N., Wassertheil-Smoller, S., Wexler, J.P., Steingard, R.M., Budner, N., Lense, L.,
and Wachspress, J. (1987). Sex bias in considering coronary bypass surgery. Annals of
Internal Medicine, 107, 19–25.
Toniolo, P.G., Levitz, M., Zeleniuch-Jacquotte, A., Banerjee, S., Koenig, K.L., Shore, R.E.,
Strax, P., and Pasternack, B.S. (1995). A prospective study of endogenous estrogens and
breast cancer in postmenopausal women. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 87(3),
190–197.
Travis, C.B. (1993, January). Psychological Aspects of Menopause. Invited address at the
conference on Women’s Voices/Women’s Bodies: Psychological Aspects of Women’s
Health, Albuquerque, NM.
Travis, C.B. (2005). Heart disease and gender inequity. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
29, 15–23.
Travis, C.B. (2006, March). Methods to Quantify Gender and Race Disparities. Paper
presented at the meeting of the Southeastern Psychological Association, Atlanta, GA.
Travis, C.B., Gressley, D.L., and Phillippi, R.H. (1993). Medical decision making, gender,
and coronary heart disease. Journal of Women’s Health, 2, 269–279.
van Praag, H., Christie, B.M., Sejnowski, T.J., and Gage, F.H. (1999). Running enhances
neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 96, 13427–13431.
Vinia, J., Lloret, A., Orti, R., and Alonso, D. (2004). Molecular bases of the treatment of
Alzheimer’s disease with antioxidants. Molecular Aspects of Medicine, 25, 117–123.
Wallston, B.S., Alagna, S.W., DeVellis, B.M., and DeVellis, R.F. (1983). Social support and
physical health. Health Psychology, 2, 367–391.
Weitzman, P.F., and Weitzman, E.A. (2000). Interpersonal negotiation strategies in a sample
of older women. Journal of Clinical Geropsychology, 6, 269–276.
Wenger, N.K. (1985). Coronary disease in women. Annual Review of Medicine, 36, 285–
294.
Wenger, N.K. (1987). Coronary heart disease in women: Clinical syndromes, prognosis and
diagnostic testing. In E.D. Eaker, B. Packard, and N.K. Wenger (Eds.), Coronary Heart
Disease in Women (pp. 173–186). New York: Haymarket Doyma.
Wolberg, W.H. (2004). Benign Breast Disease and Breast Cancer Tutorial. Retrieved
September 18, 2005, from http://www.wisc.edu/wolberg/breast.html#endogenous%20
estrogen.
Writing Group for the Women’s Health Initiative Investigators. (2002). Risks and benefits
of estrogen and progestin in healthy postmenopausal women: Principal results from the
women’s health initiative randomized trial. Journal of the American Medical Association,
288, 321–333.
Yamalik, N. (2005). Dentist–patient relationship and quality care: Professional information
and informed consent. International Dentistry Journal, 55, 342–344.
Zautra, A.J., Johnson, L., and Davis, M.C. (2005). Positive affect as a source of resilience
for women in chronic pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 212–220.
4
On the Move: Exercise, Leisure
Activities, and Midlife Women
Ruth L. Hall
A few weeks before my 50th birthday, I received a letter from the American
Association for Retired People (AARP) informing me that I was now eligible
to join AARP. I had arrived. But what did that mean? I’m postmenopausal and
beginning to gray more quickly. My physician told me that I had “age-related
osteoarthritis,” as if that makes the pain any better. Other symptoms of aging
continue to emerge. Middle age is a time when our bodies remind us of the process
of aging, a time when we say to ourselves, “I don’t remember this [body part]
giving me trouble when I did [blank] in the past.” It is a time to become more
active in developing and sustaining a lifestyle that will help us to remain mentally
alert, spiritually fulfilled, and physically sound as we grow older.
As a baby boomer, I came of age in the ’60s—a time of activism for many
oppressed populations throughout the United States and worldwide. The Civil
Rights Movement morphed into many other strong movements: the Black Power
Movement, the Women’s Movement, and the Gay Rights Movement. In some
instances, the momentum of the ’60s created policies and laws that were concrete
signs of progress—Affirmative Action and Roe v. Wade altered the landscape for
many people. Another legal coup for women in the United States was Title IX,
which required federally funded high schools, colleges, and universities to provide
equity for girls and women with their male counterparts in sport. All of these legal
interventions and progressive changes are now in jeopardy. We can only hope that
the strides made for women and the rights of others who are covered by these laws
will sustain the backlash that confronts us.
One of the repercussions of the Third Wave of feminism was greater access to
sport for girls and women, which has benefited us physically and psychologically.
As middle-aged women we did not directly benefit from Title IX in high school
or college, but we did gain some benefits from the groundswell of interest in,
availability of, and “permission” to participate in physical activity. Around the
time when Title IX became the law, feminists encouraged more flexible gender
roles and urged all girls and women to take ownership of their bodies and to see
themselves as more than sexual beings. More recently, messages about body image
in the media have expanded to include the goal of fitness and the importance of a
toned and healthy body.
79
80 Ruth L. Hall
However, gender stereotypes die hard(ly), and the culture still has a strangle-
hold on what women should look like. In the ’60s Twiggy catapulted women
into thinking that the androgynous look was the best. No longer was Marilyn
Monroe, or women with contoured figures, the ideal. Our preoccupation with the
perfect woman’s body continues to prevail and influence who we are and our
self-esteem. Body consciousness remains a volatile commodity, as most women
have an unhealthy focus on appearance, a perspective reinforced by the media,
commercialism, and a male-dominated economy.
The lives of women over 50 today are very different from the lives of our
mothers and grandmothers. Modern technology has diminished the amount of
physical labor required in many jobs and household chores, and the amount of
discretionary time we have has increased. Baby boomers are living longer and
healthier lives due to improvements in medicine and a greater knowledge of healthy
lifestyles. Baby boomers reaped the benefits of the twentieth century’s progress,
and many of us have made a conscious effort to build physical activity into our
schedules. Of course, there are many women who have physically demanding jobs
(e.g., farming, domestic work, construction work). However, that work does not
necessarily translate into better health. AARP (2002) reported that women who
had physically demanding jobs were not necessarily healthier than those that did
not.
More women are on the move. Trends in physical activity choices today include
aerobics, Pilates, yoga, cycling, hiking, walking, team sports (e.g., field hockey),
and master’s level sporting and recreational events. The increased interest in sport
and physical activity has been encouraged by the marketing of fitness: A greater
variety of exercise gear is now made for women, including clothing and women’s
shoes and bicycles. Furthermore, there is an increased awareness and availability of
healthy choices at the supermarket—foods with less salt and sugar, lower in fat, and
with fewer carbohydrates make access to a healthy lifestyle easier to obtain. This
being the case, why is the campaign for better fitness not reaching all women? Why
is the body’s need for regular physical activity not met by so many women over 50?
What prevents women, especially middle-aged women, from becoming active in
ways that increase our chances of aging that is guided by good health and fitness?
Even though Title IX eluded us, middle-aged women certainly have opportunities
their foremothers did not have, and we have the awareness that physical activity is
essential to good health and a smart way to use our leisure time.
The purpose of this chapter is to address physical activity and its central role in
women’s pleasure in midlife. Whether physical activity involves a sport, exercise,
or leisure activities, using our bodies is key to a healthy lifestyle. Integrating phys-
ical activity into daily living enhances the quality of life for women in a variety of
ways. First I will define middle age and discuss how middle age affects women’s
bodies including the roles of health care and nutrition. Then I will provide an
overview of physical activity and the results of the AARP study on physical activity
for men and women over 50 and focus on women’s midlife sport and leisure activ-
ities. A discussion of the effects of physical aging on the body and the importance
of the duration and frequency of exercising will follow. Next I will discuss at-risk
4. On the Move 81
purposive in the sense that improvement and maintenance of one or more com-
ponents of physical fitness is the objective” (Caspersen, Powell, & Christensen,
1985, as cited in U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996, p. 20);
Physical fitness: “. . . the ability to carry out daily tasks with vigor and alertness,
without undue fatigue, and with ample energy to enjoy leisure time pursuits and
to meet unforeseen emergencies. . . . Health-related fitness includes cardiorespi-
ratory fitness, muscular strength and endurance, body composition and flexibil-
ity” (p. 20);
Health: Bouchard, Shephard, Stephens, Sutton, and McPherson (1990, as cited
in U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996) stated that health
is “. . . a human condition with physical, social and psychological dimensions,
each characterized on a continuum with positive and negative poles. Positive
health is associated with a capacity to enjoy life and to withstand challenges; it is
not merely the absence of disease. Negative health is associated with morbidity
and, in the extreme, with premature mortality” (p. 22).
Thus, good health, the ultimate goal, is contingent upon using our bodies in a
manner that enhances our fitness. All middle-aged women must make a conscious
effort to incorporate physical activity into their daily routines. Clearly, exercise is
a factor in quality of life, for without physical fitness, we cannot continue to excel
in the workplace, play with our grandchildren, or enjoy hobbies and other leisure
pursuits, such as gardening and travel activities, that bring pleasure to the lives of
so many midlife women.
Types of Exercisers
In May 2002, AARP conducted a survey on the exercise attitudes and behaviors
of adults age 50 to 79. Two thousand men and women participated in a telephone
interview of 15 to 20 min and responded to questions about their exercise habits.
AARP (2002) identified six broad categories of types that describe why middle-
aged and older adults exercise: socializers, matintainers, mind and body, the infirm,
unmotivateds, and hectics. Women tended to be the following types: mind and
body (exercise for fitness and the psychological benefits), unmotivated (tend not
to exercise), the infirm (have health problems that compromise their ability to
exercise), the hectics (no time), and the maintainers (exercise for health reasons).
Only the socializer group was dominated by men (to have fun, to socialize, and to
have a better sex life). Sixty-one percent of American women realize that exercise
is important to good health yet they do not exercise (AARP, 2002). As one woman
told me, “I don’t get anything out of it.”
Muscle Strength
Muscles not only help to stabilize bones and joints but they also aid in increas-
ing the metabolism and burning calories (Weil, 2005). Aging shrinks the body’s
muscle mass by one-third to one-fifth, but this loss can be delayed by physical
activity (Fiatarone et al., 1990, as cited in Bassey et al., 2002). In summary, all
three factors—muscle strength, flexible joints, and bone density—are critical to
healthy living, and all three are affected by aging. Healthy muscles, joints, and
bones enhance both flexibility and balance. In turn, flexibility and balance aid
4. On the Move 85
with short sessions (5 to 10 min) of physical activity and gradually build up to the
desired level of activity (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996).
Adults with chronic health conditions, such as heart disease, diabetes, or obesity,
or who are at high risk for these conditions, should consult a physician before
beginning a new program of physical activity. Men over age 40 and women over
age 50 who plan to begin a new program of vigorous activity should consult a
physician to be sure they do not have heart disease or other health problems.
At-Risk Populations
According to the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1996), “Physical
inactivity is more prevalent among women than men, among Blacks and Hispanics
than Whites, among older than younger adults, and among the less affluent than
the more affluent” (p. 8). As there are more women and more Women of Color
occupying low-income status in the United States, it is no surprise that class, race,
and gender are intimate bedfellows (Hall, 1998). For example, using education
as a measure of socioeconomic status, Grzywacz et al. (2004) found that people
who are less educated report more severe stressors in their lives, which reinforces
the notion that physical health, mental health, and stress are interconnected. The
relationship between discretionary income, discretionary time, and safe environ-
ments contribute to low levels of exercise in communities of Color. Krause (2000)
pointed out that there are a disproportionate number of women, especially Women
of Color, who are poor. Concerns about access to health care may draw attention
away from a focus on and an appreciation of physical activity as a preventive
health measure. Socioeconomic status affects accessibility of services in heath
and in health prevention, as well as accessibility of safe and convenient places to
exercise (e.g., safe green spaces in the city), which, in turn, influence the desire to
participate in physical activities.
Floyd et al. (1994) used the marginality and ethnicity hypotheses in their ex-
amination of leisure activity preferences (not participation) of Black and White
women and men. The marginality hypothesis focuses on how socioeconomic sta-
tus (i.e., limited time and money) affects leisure activity preferences, whereas the
ethnicity hypothesis addresses the role of cultural norms in leisure activity prefer-
ences. Floyd and his associates found race differences between low income Blacks
and Whites in leisure activity preferences, mainly among women, but not among
middle income Black and White women. Thus, for middle class Black women,
class supersedes race in preferred leisure activities. However, Blacks, regardless
of class, rank exercise and socializing (e.g., parties; visiting friends and family;
church, club, and voluntary activities) as preferred activities more than do Whites,
who rank outdoor activities (e.g., bicycling, swimming) higher than Blacks do.
Floyd et al. concluded that it was not culture or class that distinguished low in-
come Black women, but it is the interaction of race and class that accounted for
their dissimilar leisure activity preferences. They attributed this phenomenon to
the discrimination that low income Black women face as Blacks, as poor, and as
4. On the Move 87
women. Their data are important to consider when we suggest exercise preferences
with the race and socioeconomic status of the women in mind.
According to the 1991 National Health Interview Survey (U.S. Department
of Health and Human Services, 2004), women, especially Blacks and Latinas,
participate less than men do in strengthening activities, and their participation in
strengthening activities declines with age. However, women do participate more
in stretching activities than men do, and this remains relatively consistent until age
75. Again, education and income are also related to physical activity, as poorer and
less educated adults tend to be less involved than more affluent and better educated
adults 55 and older in all types of physical activity (Schoenborn et al., 2006).
Barriers to Exercise
Although many women over 50 know that they should exercise, their reasons for
not exercising loom large and are roadblocks to following an exercise program.
AARP’s survey showed that the biggest barrier is lack of time, followed by fa-
tigue, and health problems (AARP, 2002). The health problems most commonly
referenced were arthritis, chronic pain, injury, physical disabilities, and heart prob-
lems. In addition, lack of desire, inconvenient locations, safety concerns, and the
lack of an exercise history prevent middle-aged women from exercising. AARP
also found that safety, low-cost facilities, and having an exercise companion are
important precursors to regular exercise. Some middle-aged women prefer to take
classes, especially weight lifting, strength training, and aerobics. One hypothesis
for the success of classes is that they are structured and provide instruction, which
women may find appealing, and this is especially so for middle-aged women who
are self-conscious because of their body image, unfamiliar with exercise equip-
ment, or inexperienced in workout routines. For example, Curves is a women only
facility, and many women like it because they prefer to avoid the “meat market”
atmosphere associated with coed gyms. However, for the most part, it seems that
middle-aged women do not see exercise and physical activity as a priority.
88 Ruth L. Hall
Not only does exercise prevent disease, but it also improves one’s quality of life
(U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996). Physical exercise helps
with sleep problems (Flora, 2003), stress, and depression (Berger et al., 2002).
Clearly an overall sense of well-being is a common byproduct of physical activity,
as are feelings of competence (Berger et al., 2002). Exercise actually gives us more
energy, and the fatigue that accompanies vigorous workouts generates feelings of
satisfaction and accomplishment. As a psychotherapist and sport psychologist, I
regularly recommend the benefits of exercise to my sedentary clients.
they might enjoy and one that they are willing to make part of their lifestyle. The
key is to make physical activity as important as other necessities in life such as
sleeping and eating. If people “fall off the wagon” and hit periods of inactivity, they
should simply begin again. Life is full of stops and starts, and exercise programs
are no exception. Remember that the benefits of physical activity diminish after
2 weeks, and if physical activity is stopped for 2 to 8 months, the cost benefit of
previous efforts is erased (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996).
Keeping up an exercise program is easier for most women if they find a friend, who
can act as a motivator and as someone to whom they are accountable. Exercising
with a friend makes it more enjoyable for most women.
Gender, race, age, social class, and health status all affect the physical activity of
middle-aged women. With middle age, gravity is making itself known. Wrinkles
are here to stay. Many of us have lost and found the same pounds over and over
again. We’re on our nth gym membership. However, when we hit 50 we don’t
care as much about small things and, simultaneously, we realize the importance of
our own needs and priorities. I’m reminded of Towanda, the alter ego of Evelyn
Couch (played by Kathy Bates), in the film Fried Green Tomatoes, who said: “I’m
too old to be young and too young to be old.” It is unfortunate that many of us
have not made exercise a priority. Motivation is a major factor that needs to be
addressed. However, in sport, like the rest of life, nothing is as simple as it appears.
Sometimes life and exercise are most aptly reflected in the Nike slogan “Just Do
It.” We do not have to process completing our exercise for the day; we just need to
do it. Some days are harder than others, but, in the long run, making exercise part
of daily life is worthwhile. I hope that, with more role models, greater access to
facilities, and continued media visibility about the importance of exercise, changes
will continue to be made. We are the generation that worked to ensure that girls have
the same rights as boys to sport and physical activities. We saw the manufacture
and marketing of sporting goods and clothing that was specific to women’s bodies
so that women no longer had to use men’s hand-me-downs. Middle age is a time
when many women begin to explore what is out there in the world for them—
what we missed and what we want to do. It is a time to get reacquainted with our
playfulness. Yes, physical activity is self-care, but it is also fun. And we deserve
it.
References
American Association of Retired People (AARP). (2002). Exercise Attitudes and Behaviors:
A Survey of Adults Age 50–79. Washington, DC: AARP.
Bassey, J., Sayer, A.A., and Cooper C. (2002). A life course approach to musculoskeletal
aging: Muscle strength, osteoporosis, and osteoarthritis. In D. Kuh and R. Hardy (Eds.),
A Life Course Approach to Women’s Health (pp. 141–160). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Berger, B., Pargman, D., and Weinberg, R.S. (2002). Foundations of Exercise Psychology.
Morgantown, VA: Fitness Information Technology, Inc.
4. On the Move 93
Cooper, D. (1994). Osteoarthritis epidemiology. In J.H. Klippel and P.A. Dieppe (Eds.),
Rheumatology (pp. 731–734). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Clay, R.A. (2003, April). Researchers replace midlife myths with facts: Job changes and
stress management can positively affect midlife health. APA Monitor, pp. 34, 36.
Flora, C. (2003, November). Exercise helps sleep in menopause. Psychology Today, p. 25.
Floyd, M.F., Shinew, K.J., McGuire, F.A., and Noe, F.P. (1994). Race, class, and leisure
activity preferences: Marginality and ethnicity revisited. Journal of Leisure Research,
26, 158–173.
Grzywacz, J.G., Almeida, D.M. Neupert, S.D., and Ettner, S.L. (2004). Socioeconomic
status and health: A micro-level analysis of exposure and vulnerability to daily stressors.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45, 1–16.
Hall, R.L. (1998). Softly strong: African American women’s use of exercise in therapy.
Psychotherapy Patient, 10(3/4), 81–100.
Health Education Associates. [retrieved March 5, 2006]. Aerobic Versus Anaerobic Exercise.
http://www.well-net.com/cardiov/aerex1.html).
Jokl, P., Sethi, P.M., and Cooper, A.J. (2004). Master’s performance in the New York City
Marathon. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 408–412.
Krause, M. (2000). Are we really entering a new era of aging? In K.W. Schaie and J.
Hendricks (Eds.), The Evolution of the Aging Self: The Societal Impact on the Aging
Process (pp. 307–318). New York: Springer.
Levinson, D.J. (1996). The Seasons of a Woman’s Life. New York: Knopf.
Nordon, K. (2003, May 7). Hitting their stride. Christian Science Monitor. http://www.
csmonitor.com/atcsmonitor/specials/women/sports/sports050703.html.
MayoClinic.com. (retrieved April 12, 2006). Menopause and Weight Gain: Reverse Middle
Aged Spread. http://www.mayhoclinic.com/health/menopause-weight-gain/HQ01076.
Mutschler, P.H., and Alcon, A. (1997). Health concerns in midlife and older women: Im-
proving the quality of women’s live as they age [Electronic Version]. National Council
of Jewish Women Journal, 20, 22–26.
National Center for Health Statistics. (1993). Advance report of final mortality statistics,
1991. Monthly Vital Statistics Report, 42(2). Hyattsville, MD: Author.
OB/GYN Reference Desk. (retrieved April 13, 2006). Menopause and Midlife Health for
Women. http://www.umanitoba.ca/womens health/meno5.htm.
President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports (retrieved February 26, 2006). Fitness
fundamentals. [http://www.hoptechno.com/book11.htm].
Schoenborn, C.A., Vickerie, J.L., and Powell-Griner, E. (2006, April 11). Health character-
istics of adults 55 years of age Andover: United States, 2000–2003. Advance Data from
Vital and Health Statistics, 370. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/ad/ad370.pdf.
Schultz, S. (1999, November 8). Why we’re fat. U.S. News & World Report, pp. 82–85.
The Food Trust. (2006, April 19). The Next Decade: Building Strong Communities Through
Healthy Food. http://www.thefoodtrust.org/catalog/download.php?product id=69.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Center for Disease Control and Prevention.
(2004). 1991 National Health Interview Survey. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for
Health Statistics. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/elec prods/subject/nhis.htm
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1991). Healthy People 2000: National
Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Objectives—Full Report with Commentary.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996). Physical Activity and Health: A
Report of the Surgeon General. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
94 Ruth L. Hall
WebMD Medical Reference in collaboration with the Cleveland Clinic. (2006, February
13). Menopause Basics. http//www.webmd.com/content/article/51/40624.htm.
Weil, A. (2005). Healthy Aging. New York: Knopf.
Woods, N.F., and Mitchell, E.S. (1997). Preventive health issues: The perimenopausal to
mature years (45–64). In K.M. Allen and J.M. Phillips (Eds.), Women’s Health across
the Lifespan: A Comprehensive Perspective (pp. 72–89). Philadelphia: Lippincott.
5
The Well-being and Quality of Life
of Women Over 50: A Gendered-Age
Perspective
Varda Muhlbauer
In recent decades renewed attention has been given to the notion of middle age
and particularly to women in this age group. Many of us think that this is a wel-
come development. These women (popularly referred to as “baby boomers”) have
capitalized on the major sociocultural changes and transformed meanings and be-
havioral codes traditionally attached to this age group. In numerous interviews,
in both academic and popular media, the dominant sentiment voiced by middle-
aged women is of overall satisfaction, to the effect that “the puzzle [life issues]
straightens out . . . there is a greater sense of confidence in relations and overall
a greater sense of authenticity . . . now it is totally me.” The discussion becomes
complicated, however, when issues related to body image and, particularly, sex
appeal are raised. Statements such as “I feel invisible . . . I don’t attract men” can
be heard. It is as though there has been a trade-off: a sense of greater self-assertion
in return for vulnerability vis-à-vis desirability and romance. Therefore, issues
related to well-being and the quality of life of women in this age group cannot be
easily measured. In this chapter I will discuss changes in gendered-age roles of
women in this age group and the fragmented structure of well-being that results.
It is clear that any debate concerning the well-being of women in midlife in-
volves both personal and social issues. To a large extent, well-being is the outcome
of an ongoing interchange between sociocultural influences and human agencies
(Blaikie, 1999). In this process, women proactively construct their personal ex-
periences within conceptual and normative boundaries delineated by powerful
sociocultural factors. This view is supported by the social construction perspective
advocated by such diverse disciplines as cultural anthropology, social gerontol-
ogy, social psychology, sociology, and feminist studies (Gergen, 1985; Gergen,
1990; Kaufman, 1986). One of the basic tenets of social construction is that social
constructs are not the direct and inevitable outcome of forces of nature (a point
of view often termed “essentialist” and indicative of a biological bias driven by
political and cultural interests). They are, rather, “social artifacts, products of his-
torically situated interchanges among people” (Gergen, 1985, p. 267). Thus, the
major organizing principles of psychosocial experience and well-being, among
them gender and age, have attracted considerable theoretical interest, and political
attention, and in some contexts are even viewed as sensitive issues.
95
96 Varda Muhlbauer
Feminist researchers and theorists have pioneered studies of the ways in which
the dominant culture attaches gendered meanings to a large variety of psychosocial
experiences, thereby influencing every aspect of women’s lives. These culturally
constructed meanings are shaped into gender roles that appear to be so basic
to people’s lives that it requires a conscious effort to focus on how they have
been produced (Lorber, 1994). The construction and maintenance of gender roles
and a gendered hierarchy, and the psychosocial problems associated with them,
are among the leading issues that have been investigated (Collins, 1998). The
similarities discovered among women (e.g., domains of power and powerlessness),
despite individual differences, reveal that common cultural notions of gender play
an important role in establishing well-being and expectations regarding possible
life styles. Nonetheless, gender is basically a flexible social category subject to
constantly evolving cultural meanings and shifts in power structures. Consequently,
even a constructed consensus regarding gendered meanings does not exclude the
existence of an ongoing process of alteration and modification (whether gradual
and subversive or swift and overt), either on a personal or an institutional level.
Age is an equally important core socio-cultural construct, which is currently
attracting unprecedented attention similar to the earlier interest in gender. Al-
though age is undoubtedly linked to demographic, social, and cultural changes,
the constructionist orientation again stresses the interdependence of aging and so-
ciocultural changes, as each can transform the other. Frequent references are made
to the need to redefine the borderlines between age groups and the growing salience
of middle age (Neugarten & Neugarten, 1996). New terms, such as “young-old,”
are being coined to adapt to a changing reality. Thus, age is as subject to changes
in perception as is gender, because age, too, is dependent upon social and cultural
developments and adjustments in power structures (Blaikie, 1999).
The major cultural shifts of the last four decades are clearly reflected in re-
vised gendered-age roles. These changes are particularly pronounced for women
in midlife (Friedman, 1996). Given the assumption that self-definition and lifestyle
options are realigned over time as social circumstances change (Gergen, 1985), the
alteration in gendered-age roles is of considerable importance for the well-being of
women at midlife. I will attempt here to demonstrate the link between the new so-
ciocultural meanings of gendered-age roles and well-being that have resulted from
societal shifts in the gendered-age balance of power. Power, a complicated con-
struct in itself, not only enhances people’s self-esteem and self-confidence, rather,
it “increases the boundaries of what is achievable” (Mosedale, 2005, p. 250). It
is, therefore, assumed that power roles (particularly accessible to privileged mem-
bers of this group) constitute an important feature of the requisite repertoire of
behaviors for positive functioning and overall well-being. Consequently, the cur-
rent process of blurring and diversifying age and gender roles allows for greater
access to power structures and has, therefore, a positive impact on the psycholog-
ical well-being of women over 50.
Although the modern emphasis on the “new” identities of middle-aged women
is intended to celebrate this life stage, several constraints remain intact. These con-
straints are particularly conspicuous in contemporary sexualized societies (Arber
5. The Well-being and Quality of Life of Women Over 50 97
et al., 2003). To a large extent, the common conceptions of the sexual attractiveness
of women over 50 are still unchallenged, and continue to be seen through a man’s
perspective. Powell and Moores (2001) stated that this tendency is indicative of
the pressure on women to comply with cultural standards of desirability and the
degree of male domination in society. In addition, patterns of intimate relationships
and partnerships are still negatively affected by the gendered-age role of women
over 50. This is especially worrisome from the perspective of well-being, as the
need for intimate relations does not change fundamentally through the life course,
and such relationships are associated with sustained self-esteem and self-identity
(Davidson & Fennell, 2002).
The popular media (e.g., television, the Internet, advertisements, newspapers,
films) provide abundant illustrations of the fragmented shift in the perception of
women over 50. Indeed, the connection between media representations and socio-
cultural constructs of gender and age cannot be overstated, so that documenting
them is likely to enhance our understanding. With this in mind, Lauzen and Dozier
(2005) analyzed the 100 top-grossing American films of 2002, and found evi-
dence of a lingering double standard for aging women. They reported that female
characters are, for the most part, kept frozen forever in their 20s and 30s. There
are, however, a number of more innovative and daring trends that seem to incor-
porate a fresher approach in which gendered-age traditionalism is losing ground.
The popular media therefore reflect the disjointed changes in the perception of
gendered-age roles, particularly those of women aged 50 and over.
and Baruch (1978) held that relatively little was known about the middle years and
“particularly in respect to women, theoretical work is in its infancy, and empirical
findings tend to be scattered and non-cumulative” (p. 187). They also drew attention
to underlying biological assumptions in certain studies of women in midlife that
exaggerate the importance of the reproductive role, the menopause, and the “empty
nest,” and they pointed to conflicting findings regarding the meanings that women
attach to those events.
An iconoclastic approach to women in midlife was slow to come. When it did,
however, the transition from concerns about the prevalence of depression among
middle-aged women (Bart, 1971) to a view of middle age as the “prime of life”
(Mitchell & Helson, 1990) was nothing less than revolutionary in nature. Mitchell
and Helson described the early 50s as a junction between enhanced personal re-
sources and a freer lifestyle made possible by the departure of grown children.
They referred to this period as “an androgynous time of good health combined
with autonomy and relational security” (p. 451). Burns and Leonard (2005) re-
ported that women’s perception of gain (in terms of satisfaction with life and
stress relief) continues into the mid-60s and that the gains are related in part to
role change and the passage of time. Additional studies (Helson & Moane, 1987;
Helson & Wink, 1992) also contributed to a new interpretation of the period fol-
lowing menopause and the diminishing role of mothering. In these studies women
in their 50s were found to be self-confident, secure, and better adjusted in their
relationships.
The association between disengagement from traditional gender roles and cu-
mulative personal resources on the one hand, and the quality of life in middle age
on the other hand, is in sharp contrast to previous perceptions of this development
in a woman’s life. Obviously, the implications for the subjective well-being and
lifestyle opportunities of women over 50 are enormous. Women today often wel-
come the cessation of menstruation, and they see new options available to them
when their children leave home. In retrospect, this transformation can be attributed
to a powerful blend of societal and conceptual changes, which have resulted from
partial shifts in the balance of power structures, most particularly a greater diver-
sity of gender and age roles and the widespread acceptance of a contextualized
approach to gendered-age identities (Denmark, 1994; Ryff, 1987).
The reshaping of the quality of life patterns of many women is abundantly clear,
and it helps women to confront serious issues in the realm of power and self-
support. According to Helson (1997), whose findings were based on both self-
descriptions and observers’ ratings, women’s competence increased significantly
between the ages of 30 and 60. Stewart and Ostrove (1998) also referred to the
development of a more effective instrumental personality in middle age.
Nonetheless, certain key concerns are still being largely ignored. These include
the issues of body image, sex appeal, sexual relations, and romantic attachments.
Although the literature on the institutional and personal power of women in their
50s (especially socially privileged women) explains their greater overall sense of
well-being, it often describes them as though they were asexual and devoid of
5. The Well-being and Quality of Life of Women Over 50 99
sexual needs. The mainstream media reinforce this image. Wolf (1998) claimed
that, although women have more money, power, and recognition than ever before,
they are worse off in terms of the way they feel about themselves physically. She
has written that the cults of weight and age feed the terror of aging. In addition,
the cultural construct of female sexuality still holds women to more restrictive
standards than those for men (Peach, 1998). This sexist double-standard is quite
conspicuous in middle age, when men are encouraged to be sexually active, often
with much younger partners, whereas women are considered past the acceptable
age for sexual activity. Wolf contended that this double-standard amounts to a
counter-offensive against women.
grow and expand their psychological horizons: “Traditional sex roles, if taken for
granted in early adulthood, may change or demand redefinition . . . Women who or-
ganized their identities around mothering may now be motivated to find new roles
and sources of satisfaction” (Livson, 1981, p. 196). Thus, from a developmental
perspective, for women the significance of the period following the intensively de-
manding parental years is related to a reshuffling of gender roles in ways that allow
for more flexibility and greater acceptance of parts of themselves conventionally
associated with the masculine gender role.
It goes without saying, however, that changes in gender identity and role expecta-
tions are not the same in all generations (Parker & Aldwin, 1997; Stewart & Healy,
1989) or social contexts. Midlife personality development and its implications for
well-being must therefore be considered from a much broader sociocultural per-
spective. Although the links between the developmental processes (particularly
changes related to life-course stage and role occupancy) and the larger sociocul-
tural context (cohort and period effects) are far from simple, they can enhance our
understanding of issues that pertain to the well-being and quality of life of women
in their 50s.
The growing repertoire of diversified gender roles available to women who made
their transition to adulthood in the 1960s is especially meaningful when viewed
from the perspective of identity theory. Thoits (2003) argued that “we accept
our positions and roles as identities [i.e., ‘role identities’]” (p. 179). Identities, in
turn, have important mental health implications because they affect the ways that
individuals think and feel about themselves and others. Consequently, the extensive
selection of gender roles made available to women influenced the opportunities they
had for role-identity acquisition and accumulation which, eventually, engendered
resources for their well-being. The accumulation of role-identities enabled women
to expand their capabilities, interests, and goals and to build up institutional and
personal resources, often while retaining traditional responsibilities. As a result,
women who have recently entered midlife have also experienced the labor market
in a different way than previous generations.
fluidity of lifestyles across age group boundaries was addressed by Tessa Jowell,
the British Secretary for Cultural Affairs, in an interview published in the electronic
form of the Guardian (Hinsliff, 2004). Jowell claimed that modern older women
were no longer easily pigeonholed: “Some of them are themselves bringing up
relatively young children, some are already deep into grandmotherhood. Some are
still working while others have taken early retirement,” and she added that the
British Labor Party has not engaged enough with the particular interests of these
women because it’s so hard to pin them down. She admitted that the party was not
really ready for the change that has taken place among women in this age group
and that an understanding of the trends and issues is necessary in order to appeal
to midlife women’s needs and inclinations. The need to update the categorization
of age groups was also central to a discussion led by Kaufman (2000) at Cornell
University, where it was concluded that what is considered old age has changed
dramatically over the last few decades as a function of culture and context.
The importance of age as a regulator of sequentially related life events that mark
differences between age groups has thus waned. In other words, the phenomenon
defined by developmental theorist Neugarten (1968) as the “social clock” (i.e.,
socially constructed expectations for organizing one’s major events in life [such as
marriage, parenthood, retirement] according to accepted age norms) is, to a large
extent, obsolete. Consequently, the exclusively vertical stratification of society may
soon be replaced, at least in some respects, by a horizontal stratification, thereby
doing away with the segmental nature of age groups. Indeed, a more complex
combination of physical, social, and psychological parameters would probably be
a better and more accurate indicator of a person’s lifestyle and well-being.
in all their “sagging glory,” seeking to show that there is beauty in the female
form, no matter what age or shape it is. The taboos surrounding nudity in older
women have also been broken by commercial firms such as Benetton. A Benetton
advertisement in October 2001 in honor of the UN Year of the Volunteer featured
a retired German photographer living in California at her local nudist colony, and
a high quality version of the advertisement was uploaded onto Benetton’s website.
In the same year, Ikea used photographs of naked elderly women in magazine ads
and billboards in select cities. Popular culture thus appears to be playing up the
desirability and attractiveness of women over 50, a trend that is likely to have a
positive affect on this generation’s self-image.
Another issue being tackled in the mass media, mainly in films, is the persistence
of the libido or sexual desires of older women. The British film The Mother tells
the story of a widow who flirts with her daughter’s lover; the death of her husband
had awakened her long-suppressed craving for a meaningful and sexually fulfilling
relationship. Research on sex and the elderly (Demeter, n.d.) indicates that women
are expected to become asexual earlier than men are. Women’s real-life difficulties,
therefore, largely involve cultural myths and unfavorable public attitudes, rather
than a diminished sex drive or functioning.
Liberal lifestyles are already emerging in some retirement communities in
Denmark, where the elderly residents can choose to watch pornographic videos in
the communal television area or to receive regular visits from prostitutes. As one
residence manager explained: “Like all other homes we have a council of residents.
If they decide that they want to watch porn films in the main living room once a
week, we will do it . . . We do not reject any suggestion” (www.ananova.com). In a
similar vein, an article in the Washington Post (Nicolosi, 2001) about the sex lives
of older single heterosexuals was accompanied by an extensive list of safe sex tips
and resources.
Still, for many older heterosexual women, feeling sexy and having the know-
how and capability to engage in a sexual relationship in later life has not necessarily
opened up the option of an active sex-life. The greatest barrier was—and remains—
the lack of available male partners. The statistics are commonplace: Older men
have much more opportunity to engage in intimate relationships, and their partners
tend to be considerably younger than they are. However, popular culture, and the
film industry in particular, is beginning to question the common beliefs regarding
desirable heterosexual matches between mid-life adults. A romantic comedy en-
titled Something’s Gotta Give, which featured Diane Keaton and Jack Nicholson,
tells the story of the romance and physical attraction between two middle-aged
adults. The heroine, a divorced playwright, outstrips the younger competition and
enjoys a romantic encounter in Paris. Applauding the preference for same age
sexual partners in midlife and later might ultimately create opportunities for older
women to practice their sexuality actively.
Newspapers and Internet forums contain references to an additional change
in the attitude and behaviors of women in midlife. Ashton and Asthana (2004)
reported that a growing number of women are choosing to disregard the stigma of
becoming a mother later in life: Pregnancy rates for women over 40 rose by 41%
106 Varda Muhlbauer
in the 1990s. They noted that the trend is particularly apparent among celebrities
(the “trendsetters”), and quoted their interviewees as saying that the decision to
become a parent has to be made in light of many factors, of which age is only one.
It is surprising that so little has been written in the academic literature on the
current deconstructionist trends in media representations of women over 50 and
their possible implications for enhancing the quality of life of this age group. As
previously noted, the media possess enormous power in enmeshing what is real
and unreal (Berger & Luckmann, 1991). Thus, the recent cultural images that
indicate a gradual shift in cohort-based norms, accentuating new options, partic-
ularly in the realm of sexuality and intimate relations, could possibly undermine
the exclusivity of traditional conventions related to sex appeal and romance. This
development has special significance for women whose perceptions of their own
sexuality were established by the liberal messages of the sexual revolution. In this
respect, the media representations of role models defying traditional conventions
that link women’s attractiveness to their youthfulness, might open the way to real
life experiences for women over 50. It is worth noting that many of the screen
plays of sexual encounters and romantic attachments are portrayed together with
experiences and gestures of mastery, personal power, and the willingness to make
daring lifestyle choices, all of which are connected with various dimensions of
well-being.
may be one explanation for the resentment and fear experienced by older women
in the face of sexual constraints and losses.
The concept of “possible selves” introduced by Markus and Nurius (1986) is also
helpful in addressing the difficulties of midlife women with respect to sexuality.
Possible selves are defined as the “cognitive components of hopes, fears, goals, and
threats” (p. 954). Women who were exposed to liberal sexual messages in young
adulthood have incorporated them into their structure of accessible and desirable
selves. Frustration is only to be expected when they are rather suddenly confronted
with cultural barriers that exclude the sexual self from the range of possible selves.
The fact that, at present, the feelings and expectations of women in midlife are not
supported by the dominant social norms and cultural representations is indicative of
what Davidson and Fennel (2002, p. 4) referred to as a “cultural lag” (i.e., cultural
conceptions have not caught up with the changes in the way women experience
their sexuality in reality and their expectation of acting accordingly). However, as
indicated above, at least some of the popular media allow open, sometimes even
subversive, discourse on sexual ethics and fresh cultural representations of the
body, sex appeal, and sexual activity of middle-aged women. Future studies are
needed to examine the interaction between what remain the separate expansive
and contractive domains of well-being and the quality of life of women over 50.
Conclusions
I have focused here on the well-being of women in their 50s from a sociocultural
perspective, highlighting the favorable effect of cultural shifts in social constructs,
such as gender and age. Middle-aged women today tend to score similarly to
younger women on well-being assessment scales, partly because they tend to retain
the liberal values and empowered behavioral codes acquired in young adulthood.
In this sense, they differ not only from older women, but also from middle-aged
women in previous generations. The latter difference can be explained by the social
constructionist orientation: It is connected to changes in structural opportunities,
mainly in education and employment, as well as to shifts in the normative ex-
pectations concerning gender and age roles (Parker & Aldwin, 1997). In other
words, the expansion and growth in well-being that have been found in recent
years for women in this age group are related to an increase in the available per-
sonal resources (and/or to the decrease in the personal constraints) that can be
expected at this stage in life (according to the developmental perspective), along
with sometimes dramatic advances in cultural representations and opportunities.
It stands to reason that the major shifts in the balance of power in society in the
last four decades, often reflected in gender and age crossovers or in a blurring of
demarcation lines between groups, have facilitated this change. Women in their 50s
have more power today than ever before, and consequently display better positive
functioning (as defined by current theories, e.g., Keyes et al., 2002; Ryff & Keyes,
1995). This leads us to question the connection between structures of well-being
and power constructs, both on the personal and the institutional levels. Indeed,
108 Varda Muhlbauer
References
Arber, S., and Ginn, J. (Eds.). (2002). Connecting gender and ageing: A sociological ap-
proach. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
Arber, S., Davidson, K., and Ginn, J. (2003). Changing approaches to gender and later life.
In S. Arber, K. Davidson, and J. Ginn (Eds.), Gender and ageing (pp. 1–15) Maidenhead,
UK: Open University Press.
Ashton, L., and Asthana, A. ( 2004, February 15). Nappies at fifty. Retrieved July13, 2005
from http://www.observer.guardian.co.uk.
Barnett, R.C., and Baruch, G.K. (1978). Women in the middle years: A critique of research
and theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 3, 187–198.
Bart, B.P. (1971). Depression in middle-aged women. In J.M. Bardwick (Ed.), Readings on
the psychology of women (pp. 134–143). New York: Harper & Row.
Baumeister, R.F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive as
socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 347–374.
Benetton advertisement. (2001, October). Retrieved May, 12, 2004, from http://www.
benetton.com.
Berger, P., and Luckmann, T. (1991). The social construction of reality: A treatise in the
sociology of knowledge. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. [Original work published in
1966.]
Blaikie, A. (1999). Ageing and popular culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Borell, K., and Karlsson, S.G. (2003). Re-conceptualizing intimacy and ageing: Living apart
together. In S. Arber, K. Davidson and J. Ginn (Eds.), Gender and ageing (pp. 1–15)
Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press.
5. The Well-being and Quality of Life of Women Over 50 109
Broverman, I.K., Broverman, D.M., Clarkson, F.E., Rosenkrantz, P.S., and Vogel, S.R.
(1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgments of mental health. Journal of Con-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 1–7.
Burns, A., and Leonard, R. (2005). Chapters of our lives: Life narratives of midlife and
older Australian women. Sex Roles, 52, 269–277.
Cheng, S.T. (2004). Age and subjective well-being revisited: A discrepancy perspective.
Psychology and Aging, 19, 409–415.
Collins, L.H. (1998). Illustrating feminist theory: Power and psychopathology. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 22, 97–112.
Davidson, K., and Fennell, G. (2002). New intimate relationship in later life. Ageing Inter-
national, 27, 3–10.
Demeter, D. (n.d.). Sex and the elderly. Retrieved May 5, 2004, from University of Missouri-
Kansas City, The human sexuality web site: http://www. umkc.edu\sites\hsw\age.
Denmark, F.L. (1994). Engendering psychology. American Psychologist, 49, 329–324.
Ehrenreich, B., Hess, E., and Jacobs, G. (1986). Re-making love: The feminization of sex.
Garden City, NY: Doubleday Anchor.
Friedman, A. (1996). Annie Oakley won twice: Intimacy and power in female identity.
Tel-Aviv: Hakibbutz Hameuchad.
Gergen, K.J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American
Psychologist, 40, 266–275.
Gergen, M.M. (1990). Finished at 40: Women’s development within patriarchy. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 14, 471–493.
Ginn, J., and Arber, S. (2002). Only connect: Gender relations and ageing. In S. Arber
and J. Ginn (Eds.), Connecting gender and ageing: A sociological approach (pp. 1–14).
Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
Gott, M., and Hinchliff, S. (2003). Sex and ageing: A gendered issue. In S. Arber, K.
Davidson., and J. Ginn (Eds.), Gender and ageing (pp. 63–79). Maidenhead, UK: Open
University Press.
Gutmann, D. (1975). Parenthood: A key to the comparative study of the life cycle. In N.
Datan and L. Ginsberg (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Normative life crises
(pp. 98–119). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Hawkes, G. (1996). A sociology of sex and sexuality. Buckingham, UK: Open University
Press.
Helgeson, V.S. (1994). Relation of agency and communion to well-being: Evidence and
potential explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 412–428.
Helson, R. (1997). The self in middle age. In M.E. Lachman and J.B. James (Eds.), Multiple
paths of middle development (pp. 21–43). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Helson, R., and Moane, G. (1987). Personality change in women from college to midlife.
Journal of Social Psychology, 53, 176–186.
Helson, R., and Picano, J. (1990). Is the traditional role bad for women? Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 59, 311–320.
Helson, R., and Wink, P. (1992). Personality change in women from the early 40s to the
early 50s. Psychology of Aging, 7, 46–55.
Hinsliff, G. (2004, April 25). The calendar girl generation. Retrieved May 8, 2004, from
http://www.guardian.co.uk.
Hirshbein, L.D. (2001). Popular views of old age in America, 1900–1950. Journal of Amer-
ican Geriatrics Society, 4, 1555–1560.
Ikea. (2001, October). Retrieved May 5, 2004, from http://www.philly.com. Press.
110 Varda Muhlbauer
Kaufman, D. (2000). Sex and the elderly woman. Retrieved May 6, 2004, from www.
globalaging.org\health\us.
Kaufman, S.R. (1986). The ageless self. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.
Keyes, C.L.M., Shmotkin, D., and Ryff, C.D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical
encounter of two traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 1007–1022.
Laumann, E.O., Gagnon, J.H., Michael, R.T., and Michaels, S. (1994). The social organiza-
tion of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Lauzen, M.M., and Dozier, D.M. (2005). Maintaining the double standard: Portrayals of
age and gender in popular films. Sex Roles, 52, 437–446.
Livson, B.F. (1976). Patterns of personality development in middle-aged women: A longi-
tudinal study. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 7, 107–115.
Livson, B. F (1981). Paths to psychological health in middle years: Sex differences. In D.H.
Eichorn, J.A. Clausen, M.P. Honzik, and P. Mussen (Eds.), Present and past in midlife
(pp. 195–221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Lorber, J. (1994). Paradoxes of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Markus, H., and Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.
Matzkin, A. (2003). The naked body. Retrieved May 10, 2004, from www.
matzkinstudio.com.
Mitchell, V., and Helson, R. (1990). Women’s prime of life—Is it the 50s?. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 14, 451–470.
Mosedale, S. (2005). Assessing women’s empowerment: Toward a conceptual framework.
Journal of International Development, 17, 243–257.
Muhlbauer, V., and Zemach, M. (1991). Onesies twosies. Tel Aviv: Am Oved.
Neugarten, B.L. (1968). Adult personality: Toward a psychology of the life cycle. In B.L.
Neugarten (Ed.), Middle age and aging: A reader in social psychology (pp. 137–147).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Neugarten, B.L., and Gutmann, D. (1968). Age sex-roles and personality in middle age: A
thematic apperception study. In B L. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle age and aging: A reader
in social psychology (pp. 77–84). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Neugarten, B.L., and Neugarten, D.A. (1996). The changing meanings of age. In D.A.
Neugarten (Ed.), The meaning of age: Selected papers of Bernice L. Neugarten
(pp. 72–77). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Nicolosi, M. (July, 2001). Sex lives of older single heterosexuals. Retrieved May, 8 2004,
from http://www.washingtonpost.com.
Parker, R.A., and Aldwin, C.J. (1997). Do aspects of gender identity change from early
to middle adulthood? Disentangling age, cohort, and period effects. In M.E. Lachman
and J.B. James (Eds.), Multiple paths of middle development (pp. 67–109). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Peach, L.J. (1998). Sex, sexism, sexual harassment, and sexual abuse. In L .J. Peach (Ed.),
Women in culture: A women’s studies anthology (pp. 283–301). Oxford: Blackwell.
Pearlman, S.F. (1993). Late mid-life astonishment: Disruptions to identity and self-esteem.
In N.D. Davis., E. Cole., and E.D. Rothblum (Eds.), Faces of women and aging (pp. 1–13).
New York: Harrington Park Press.
Pietromonaco, P.R., Manis, J., and Frohardt-Lane, K. (1986). Psychological consequences
of multiple social roles. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 10, 373–382.
Powell, J.L., and Moores, J. (2001). Theorising social gerontology: The case of so-
cial philosophies of age. E-Journal of Culture Studies. Retrieved 8 June, 2005, from
http://www.fuentes.csh.udg.mx./cuchs/sincronia.
5. The Well-being and Quality of Life of Women Over 50 111
Rosenkrantz, P., Vogel, S., Bee, H., Broverman, I., and Broverman, D. (1968). Sex role
stereotypes and self-concept in college students. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 32, 287–295.
Rossi, A.S. (2004). The menopausal transition and aging processes. In O.G. Brim, C.D.
Ryff, and R.C. Kessler (Eds.), How healthy are we? A national study of well-being at
midlife (pp. 153–201). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Ryff, C.D. (1987). The place of personality and social structure research in social psychol-
ogy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1192–1202.
Ryff, C.D. (1991). Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of shifting horizons.
Psychology and Aging, 6, 286–295.
Ryff, C.D., and Keyes, C.L.M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 719–727.
Ryff, C.D., and Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychological
Inquiry, 9, 1–28.
Schmuck, P., and Sheldon, K.M. (2001). Life goals and well-being: To the frontiers of
life goal research. In P. Schmuck and K.M. Sheldon (Eds.), Life goals and well-being
(pp. 1–17). Seattle: Hogrefe and Huber.
Schmutte, P.S., and Ryff, C.D. (1997). Personality and well-being: Reexamining methods
and meanings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 549–559.
Stewart, A., and Healy, J.M. (1989). Linking individual development and social changes.
American Psychologist, 44, 30–42.
Stewart, A.J., and Ostrove, J.M. (1998). Women’s personality in middle age: Gender, history,
and midcourse corrections. American Psychologist, 53, 1185–1194.
Strinati, D. (1995). An introduction to theories of popular culture. London: Routledge.
Thoits, P.A. (1983). Multiple theories and psychological well-being: A reformulation and
test of the social isolation hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 48, 174–187.
Thoits, P.A. (2003). Personal agency in the accumulation of multiple role-identities. In P.J.
Burke, T.J. Owens, R.T. Serpe, and P.A. Thoits (Eds.), Advances in identity theory and
research (pp. 179–194). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Vandewater, E.A., Ostrove, J.M., and Stewart, A.J. (1997). Predicting women’s well-being
in midlife: The importance of personality development and social role involvements.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1147–1160.
Welsh, W.M., and Stewart, A.J. (1995). Relationship between women and their parents:
Implications for midlife well-being. Psychology and Aging, 10, 181–190.
Wolf, N. (1998). The beauty myth. In L.J. Peach (Ed.), Women in culture (pp. 179–187).
Oxford: Blackwell.
Woodhill, B.M., and Samuels, C.A. (2003). Positive and negative androgyny and their
relationship with psychological health and well-being. Sex Roles, 48, 555–567.
6
Enjoying the Returns: Women’s
Friendships After 50
Suzanna M. Rose
Women after 50 show a new vigor in their friendships. The second half of life
elicits review and contemplation concerning where one has been and also, some-
times, decision and change concerning the personal priorities that will guide the
remaining decades. New perspectives of the self and intimacy emerge. As time
becomes more valuable, choices about how and with whom to spend it become
more pressing. As women assess their lives, they also take stock of their friend-
ships, often making deliberate and clear-eyed decisions about where to increase
and reduce their emotional investment. Old friendships may be recalibrated or
new ones sought to match fresh views of the self and relationships. What does not
change is the immense importance women attach to their friendships. Commitment
to the role of friend is even more predictive than income or marital status in the
determination of older women’s life satisfaction (Trotman & Brody, 2002).
In this chapter, the expectations, functions, and development of women’s friend-
ships after age 50 will be hypothesized and explored. This is relatively new terrain
from the standpoint of psychological research, which has focused on friendship
among the young or old but given little attention to the middle years. Thus, specu-
lation will be required. Factors that typically are known to affect friendship will be
considered as well, including historical forces, gender, sexual orientation, race and
ethnicity, individual differences, and culture. Negative aspects of friendship such
as conflict and false friends also will be discussed. Fruitful research directions will
be proposed to begin a systematic exploration of women’s friendships after age 50.
112
6. Enjoying the Returns 113
In contrast, the dominant view from the time of Aristotle until the 1970s was that
women were incapable of true friendship. One argument was that women were
not genetically programmed to bond with one another; others asserted that sexual
jealousy and the desire for men’s approval inevitably resulted in hostility between
women that prevented friendship. Women’s friendships often were trivialized as
being “two-faced,” “gossipy,” or “juvenile” (O’Connor, 1992).
With the growth of women’s studies and also the “science of relationships”
from the 1970s to the mid-1980s, more positive attention was focused on women’s
friendship. Contextual factors that affect women’s friendships were emphasized.
For instance, the primacy of marriage as a social institution, particularly for women,
was recognized as lessening opportunities for friendship (O’Connor, 1992). Fem-
inist historians also challenged the negative view of women’s friendships by un-
covering numerous cases of romantic friendship between women friends during
the 1800s (e.g., Faderman, 1981). However, the research that resulted from those
analyses was aimed primarily at identifying gender differences rather than un-
derstanding the nature of women’s friendship in its own right. Thus, women’s
friendships continued to be regarded as secondary attachments relative to the mar-
ital bond, a view that reflects implicit heterosexist and patriarchal views of the
importance of women’s friendship (Rose, 2000).
A more recent trend has emerged that questions the secondary status of women’s
friendship. For instance, the emotional strength of women’s friendships was found
to be no different from their relationships with husbands or lovers (Goodenow &
Gaier, 1990). Women’s longer lifespan in the developed countries and the like-
lihood that they will outlive their husbands has resulted in reconsiderations of
the importance of friendship, particularly later in life (Allan, 2001). This new
interest in women’s friendship highlights the fact that very little is known about
this important relationship, particularly during midlife. Much more remains to
be explored concerning the convergence of life stage and friendship for women
after 50.
Expectations of Friends
Expectations of friends in Western, industrialized cultures tend to be idealized.
Friendships typically are expected to involve intimacy, enjoyment, dependability,
acceptance, and caring. Furthermore, women’s expectations for friendship tend to
be very high. Gouldner and Strong (1987) reported that the middle-aged women in
their study expressed “a great longing for friendship” and described their idealized
friendships in terms that were similar to those used for romantic relationships:
The perfect friend was thought of as possessing, above all, the traits of trustworthiness
and unswerving loyalty and the ability to keep confidences. She was a person, who was, at
the same time, a good listener, an entertaining companion, someone with whom she could
gossip and air serious problems. Ideally she would provide sympathy and opportunities for
catharsis and self-insight along with distraction and fun. (p. 105)
114 Suzanna M. Rose
Friendship Functions
Adults of all ages endorse six functions of friendships (Argyle & Henderson,
1984). At a minimum, friends are supposed to stand up for each other (even in
a friend’s absence), share news of success, provide emotional support, trust and
confide in each other, volunteer help when needed, and try to make the friend happy
when together. In addition, close friends are supposed to repay debts and favors,
be tolerant of the friend’s other friends, avoid criticizing the friend in public, keep
confidences, avoid jealousy and criticism of other relationships, avoid nagging, and
respect privacy. Violation of these functions is likely to jeopardize the friendship.
Friendships play a unique and crucial role in adults’ lives. Friends’ similarity
in terms of personal and lifestyle characteristics makes them well suited to affirm
each other’s identity, reminisce, give advice, provide socialization, share leisure
activities, and help with nontechnical tasks. Friends are expected to provide the
companionship and emotional support required to meet the losses and transitions
of growing older.
Equality is a distinguishing feature of friendship. Friends are happiest when the
friendship is perceived as being equal in terms of the “give-and-take” in the rela-
tionship (Roberto, 2001). Inequalities in material resources or interpersonal power
must be leveled between friends or the friendship may not survive. Reciprocity
also is important, particularly in the early stages. Established friendships are ex-
pected to be “communal” rather than “exchange” relationships (Clark & Mills,
1993). Communal relations do not require that a specific debt be returned with a
comparable benefit, as would be expected in exchange relationships. Equality of
affect rather than equality of exchange governs communal relations.
6. Enjoying the Returns 115
Long-term friends provide a sense of continuity with the past, and over time they
may be regarded as family, which enhances a sense of connectedness (Lewittes,
1989). Women (and men) report being happier spending time with friends than
with anyone else (Larson & Bradney, 1988). Fortunately, friendships among older
women contribute to psychological growth, as well as to physical and mental health
(Patrick et al., 2001).
Gender Roles
Friendships are strongly affected by gender roles. Women are more likely to provide
solace, sympathy, and sophisticated types of emotional support to their friends than
men are (e.g., Basow & Rubenfeld, 2003). These behaviors are learned early in life.
Girls are encouraged much more than boys to behave in a nurturing way toward
others, to talk about their emotions, and to express sympathy. By adulthood, women
are not only better at giving nurturance and support, they are expected to be ready
and willing to provide it.
Women place a high premium on giving and receiving comfort from their friends,
which suggests that women who are unable to provide it will be negatively per-
ceived or rejected (Holmstrom et al., 2005). Although gender roles are robust across
the lifespan, countervailing forces at midlife might temper stereotypic expectations
for unconditional support from women friends. Anecdotal evidence suggests that
women in midlife come to value truth-telling more than they did when they were
young. Levine (2005) characterized this as a “new intimacy”: “We are much more
forthcoming about our failings and failures, more willing to seek and accept advice,
less know-it-all about dispensing advice, and a lot less concerned with eliciting
sympathy for its own sake” (p. 145). The expectation for a greater level of honesty
may free women to have a greater range of response to friends’ predicaments. Less
sympathy and more direct advice, such as “don’t be upset about that” or “you’d
better get your act together,” might be given and appreciated.
Making Friends
The formation of friendship is constrained by at least three factors that affect the
opportunities adults have to meet potential friends and promote friendship. First,
demographic variables tend to limit friendship. At all ages, women tend to come
into contact with women who are similar in terms of age, race, social class, sexual
orientation, and who live and work in similar areas. Women with young children
often become friends with mothers of their children’s friends. Roberto (2001)
reported that older women tend to live close to their close friends, to be about the
same age and social class status, and to share social and ethnic backgrounds.
Second, the norm of equality in friendship also tends to restrain the development
of friendships across socioeconomic status or identity categories that cause social
distance. For instance, a friendship between a woman and her household worker
must surmount both economic barriers and social ones caused by inequality of
rank in the social order. Similarly, friendships between Black and White women
or lesbians and heterosexual women may have to meet additional criteria in order
to bridge the social distance and to put the friendship on an equal footing (e.g.,
Hall & Rose, 1996).
Patriarchal definitions of women’s place as secondary and subservient to men
also shape women’s friendships. Among heterosexual couples, men’s preferences
and friendships often dictate couples’ social lives. Women more often than men
report that dating or marriage precipitates the loss of a same-sex friendship
(Gullestad, 1984; Rose, 1984). Wives may seek to maintain friendships with their
women friends independent of their husbands, but arranging to see women friends
separately from couple activities requires extra effort and makes the friendships
more difficult to maintain. The negative effect of marriage on women’s friendships
continues after divorce or widowhood. Both divorced and widowed women report
having to rebuild their friendship networks (e.g., Armstrong & Goldsteen, 1990).
Once the husband is no longer present, his friends may drop the wife as a friend.
Situational factors that arise from male dominance and gender roles also place
major constraints on women’s friendships. As Enright and Rawlinson (1992)
pointed out: “The bosom of the family is not a rich breeding ground for friend-
ships” (p. 96). Women continue to shoulder about 70% of the household and child
care responsibilities, and most have little free time for their own leisure pursuits
(e.g., Green et al., 1990). Women earn less than men on average and have fewer
resources to use in establishing and maintaining friendships. Women also have less
access to and less control of public space than men do, including parks, bars, social
clubs, athletic courts, and arcades. Fear of violence from men limits women’s for-
ays alone outside the home. Thus, demographic, social, and physical constraints
limit friendship choice and interactions.
By age 50, some of the limitations on women’s friendships described above
may be reduced. Working women in their 50s are more likely to have their own
financial resources and also to have more leisure time due to a decline in family
and household responsibilities as children grow up and leave home. In fact, the
50s might amount to a “golden age” of women’s friendships, given that women
6. Enjoying the Returns 117
are likely to be at their height of confident power (i.e., Neugarten, 1968), financial
stability, and still in relatively good health. It may be the era when women fully
learn to “treat a friendship like the gift that it is” (Paul, 2004, p. 164).
Maintaining Friendships
Studies of friendship in the middle years have not asked about the strategies used
to maintain friendships. Friends interact both at home and in community activities.
Getting together to talk is the most common social activity, but friends also help
each other with transportation, shopping, and running errands (Adams, 1997).
Communication strategies are important in friendship maintenance throughout
life. Aspects of communication that create tension are the interplay between inde-
pendence and dependence in friendship and between friendship’s protective and
expressive functions. Friendships provide room for women to pursue individual
goals and interests, but also in times of need require interdependence. These two
privileges of friendship require ongoing negotiations to keep the friendship in
balance. Likewise, friends must balance expressiveness with protectiveness. Hon-
esty, candor, and self-disclosure have to be managed carefully to avoid harming
the friend.
Ending Friendships
Friendships may end for a number of reasons, but this has been examined empiri-
cally in only a few studies of either younger or older adults. Causes of endings cited
by women and men in their 20s included lack of social skills or reciprocity, in-
appropriate self-disclosures, inability to express feelings, and learning something
distasteful about the friend (e.g., Rose, 1984). Women are more likely than men to
lose a friendship because their romantic relationship competed with the friendship
for time. Adults in their mid-20s to mid-30s attributed friendship endings to a
lack of respect for privacy and too much demand for personal advice (Argyle &
Henderson, 1984). In old age, active termination of friendship is rarely reported
other than due to the death of a friend. Friendships either decline or end by “fading
away” due to a change in lifestyle or pathway over the years, a move, or major
breaches of friendship norms (Bleiszner & Adams, 1992).
At present, no research exists that addresses how and when midlife women
end friendships. However, developmental research concerning women at midlife
suggests that they may act to terminate even long-term friendships if they are
chronically unsatisfying. For instance, Gersick and Kram (2002) studied high
achieving professional women at midlife and reported that the age 50 transition
was characterized by the task of coming into one’s own. This task deals with gaining
confidence in one’s abilities, knowing what one wants, and being able to go after
it. The women’s stories suggested that a wellspring of energy to pursue one’s own
life was released during the age 50 transition. The reassessment of relationships
118 Suzanna M. Rose
during this period, combined with increased confidence to ask for what one wants,
hypothetically might trigger decisions to end unsatisfying friendships.
For example, one woman approaching 50 who had recently ended a 25-year-
old friendship described it this way: “The older you get, the wiser you get about
your own personality and what you’re willing to compromise. You become more
selective. You’re not going to waste time on people who don’t share common goals
in friendship. If you have a friend who just takes, forget about it! If you’re in a
friendship where you are just giving, giving, giving, you’re going to burn out. In
a long distance friendship, it may take years for the animosity to build. That’s
what happened with Lois” (personal communication, 2006). The dissatisfaction
in this friendship stemmed from a lack of reciprocity in visiting each other and in
showing interest in the friend’s life and relationships.
At midlife, the extent to which gender differences occur in response to troubled
or lost friendships is unknown. Women have a better history of making and main-
taining friendships throughout life than do men. As women age, they may be less
upset than men if friendships end because they are more likely to establish new
ones. Or, women’s self-concept in later life may be less entangled with the need
to perform well at relationships. It remains to be determined if gender differences
in response to friendship problems or endings may disappear or even reverse after
age 50.
may have relatively few people whom they would name as friends (Allan, 1998).
In contrast, middle class women interact with friends more often, provide more
assistance to their friends, and are more satisfied with the support they receive in
turn (Krause & Borawski-Clark, 1995). In fact, friendships play a larger role in the
lives of middle class individuals than kin do (Allan, 1998). The greater mobility of
middle class women may result in having fewer kin nearby, which causes greater
reliance on friends.
Friendships across social classes are often difficult to establish and maintain. The
difference described above between working class and middle class norms is one
barrier. Social class also impinges on equality in a friendship. Friends are expected
to regard and treat each other as social equals. Difference can be tolerated if it does
not undermine the sense that each person has equal social worth. Reciprocity also
is expected in friendship, although exchanges do not have to be in-kind and do not
have to be returned immediately. However, if two women have unequal or limited
resources, the balance of equal social worth and reciprocity is upset (Allan, 1998).
For instance, working class women may not have the resources to entertain at home
or to dine at restaurants as middle class women may be able to do.
For women at midlife, social class norms are likely to be entrenched. This
suggests that working class women after 50 would be more embedded in kin
networks than would middle class women. In contrast, middle class women may
be freer to cultivate friendships at midlife than ever before. As a result, midlife
friendships across social class may be unlikely.
Individual Differences
Any discussion of friendship must take individual differences into account. Women
have different personalities and may not approach friendship in the same way.
Three elements of adult personality development that might affect friendships are
identity, generativity, and confident power (Stewart et al., 2001). Research indicates
that identity development is positively related to self-esteem and life satisfaction
in midlife women and that the early attainment of a well-articulated identity also is
related to women’s well-being at midlife (Vandeventer et al., 1997). Generativity
refers to the second stage of adulthood posited by Erikson (1968). In generativity,
“a man and a woman must have defined for themselves what and whom they have
come to care for, what they care to do well, and how they plan to take care of
what they have started and created” (Erikson, 1968, p. 395). If this stage is not
mastered, the negative outcome is stagnation. A third element of personality at
midlife is confident power. Neugarten (1968) proposed that executive processes
such as mastery and competence make up the core of the middle-aged personality.
Research confirms that middle-aged women feel more in command of their world
and themselves than do adults of other ages (e.g., Cartwright & Wink, 1994).
Specifically, women report increases in confidence, dominance, and coping skills
from early adulthood to middle age (e.g., Stewart et al., 2001).
The connection between personality and friendship remains to be explored. A
next step in terms of research in this area would be to investigate how identity,
generativity, and confident power affects women’s friendships at midlife. One
prediction might be that women’s established friendships will gain in importance
and clarity at midlife for those women who become self-actualized. It may also be
the case that establishing friendships may become more difficult or less eagerly
122 Suzanna M. Rose
approached at midlife. Paul (2004) explained that, by midlife, one is more aware
of what effort is involved in starting a friendship:
The chocolate cake in the cooking magazine immediately snared me. The triple layers of
dark devil’s food—my favorite—were glazed with a rich, fudgy ganache. I’d been craving
chocolate cake. I could almost taste it. Maybe I’d share it with my family, maybe not.
Then I noticed the length list of ingredients, including an artisan chocolate that had to be
specially ordered from a catalog. The recipe was complicated – carefully melting chocolate
in a double boiler and whipping egg whites. Way too much trouble. Even though I knew it
would be delicious, I turned the page. New friendships can feel like that. (p. 90)
Cross-cultural Perspectives
People in numerous cultures characterize the friendship process as one of increas-
ing levels of intimacy. Despite the universality of the process, the character of
friendship varies cross-culturally. Some rules of friendship appear to cut across
culture; other rules suggest that differences in cultural values might affect friend-
ship. Argyle and Henderson (1984) reported that adults from Britain, Hong Kong,
Japan, and Italy (ages 18 to 25 and 30 to 60) endorsed four common rules of
friendship: respect the other’s privacy, trust and confide in one another, volunteer
help in time of need, and avoid jealousy or criticism of the friend’s relationships.
However, Japanese adults gave more weight than the other three groups did to the
friend fulfilling ritual obligations, providing help if requested, and offering infor-
mation and regard, and they gave less weight than the other groups did to verbal
intimacy and supportiveness.
The dimension of individualism–collectivism has been used to explain many dif-
ferences in interpersonal behaviors, including friendship. The difference between
the Japanese and the other cultures above may be attributable to the individualism–
collectivism dimension; perhaps intimacy is more highly regarded in individualis-
tic (Western) societies and fulfillment of formal obligations is more important in
collectivist (Eastern) societies.
If we extrapolate from that study, we might hypothesize that women in collec-
tivist societies would have more close friends than women in individualist societies.
An alternative hypothesis is that women in collectivist cultures would have fewer
but closer friends than do women in individualist cultures. The latter hypothesis
was partially borne out by a study of adults from West Africa (Ghana) and North
America (Adams & Plaut, 2003), although gender effects were not reported sepa-
rately. West Africans had fewer friends than North Americans, and the friendship
expectations of the two groups also differed. Significantly more West Africans
expected material and practical support and guidance, correction, and warning,
whereas more North Americans expected emotional support, trust, and respect.
Thus, a limited amount of cross-cultural research suggests that gender and culture
both are significant factors in determining friendship patterns between women
from other cultures.
6. Enjoying the Returns 123
Responses to Conflict
Responses to conflict may determine friendship outcomes. Four types of responses
to conflict have been identified: exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect (Rusbult et al.,
1982). Exit responses refer to threatening to end the friendship, discussing end-
ing it, driving the friend away, or actually ending the friendship. Voice includes
responses such as talking about the problem, recommending solutions, compro-
mising, or trying to change the friend or the self. Loyalty responses refer to waiting
to see if things improve or continuing to have faith in the friendship or the self.
Last, neglect responses refer to ignoring the friend, spending less time together,
refusing to talk about the problem, or complaining without suggesting solutions.
Typically, friends tend to adopt passive responses to conflict in friendship, such
as loyally continuing the friendships despite dissatisfaction or by neglecting the
friend (e.g., Fehr & Harasymchuk, 2005). Because passive responses are the norm,
active responses to conflict, such as threatening to end the friendship or voicing
the issue, might be regarded as disruptions in the usual way of dealing with things.
People expect friendships to be “self-maintaining” and thus, a friend who responds
to conflict more actively may be a surprise. On the other hand, a friendship could
be strengthened by a friend’s attentive response to an expression of dissatisfaction.
The research on conflict raises at least two questions concerning mature women’s
friendships that have yet to be addressed. First, are older women quicker to react
to conflict in friendship or more active in resolving it than young women are?
Predictions vary. It could be that older women would be more patient in waiting
for conflicts to resolve spontaneously. In contrast, if the friendship is not rewarding,
they may be more willing than young women to end it. Second, are older women
better at predicting a friend’s response or selecting the most successful way to
124 Suzanna M. Rose
handle the conflict with that particular friend? It might be expected that older
women, who have sustained long-term friendships, have developed ways to cope
with problems successfully. A stable interpersonal script based on long intimacy
may govern friendships; a woman may know what friends respond to neglect and
what friends appreciate an active approach to problems. In other words, older
women’s life experience and in-depth knowledge of friends might broaden their
repertoire concerning how to handle conflicts.
women who have positive experiences with caring in early and middle adulthood
will be more likely to have caring and intimate friendships in the later years of
life. Current sociohistorical influences also are expected to play a role in women’s
friendships. For example, members of the baby boom cohort are likely to view
friendships differently in old age than the previous generation did. The prediction
is that baby boomers will have mainly age-homogeneous friendships because of
to the strong cohort identity they forged in their youth due to sharing powerful
historical events together such as the Vietnam War (Bleiszner, 2006).
The beginning of the twenty-first century has been marked by tremendous
changes that will continue to affect friendships (Allan, 2001). The introduction
of mass-scale, worldwide, electronic communication and continuing globaliza-
tion has had an impact on women’s everyday lives and relationships. More women
work outside the home, and in industrialized nations women may expect to live
for nearly a century. Marriage has become less normative, and divorce quite soon
may be the majority pattern. Overall, women are having fewer children and their
dependency on men is declining. As a result, “the domestic, sexual, and familial
arrangements that adults construct are now perceived far more as a personal mat-
ter for those involved and not as issues over which others have strong rights to
influence” (Allan, 2001, p. 329).
These changes have large implications for informal relationships like friend-
ship. Friendship is likely to become an even more important part of social identity
as women become freer to develop nondomestic aspects of their lives. Friend-
ship may also play an increasingly important role in confirming new decisions or
changes that are made. Lesbian identities provide a good example. Friendships
are highly significant for many lesbians for whom friends represent their “chosen
family” (Weeks et al., 2001). These are the friends who are trusted and relied
upon. Widowed and divorced heterosexual women as they age potentially might
begin to adopt a similar model of establishing a “chosen family” comprised of
friends.
Signs that women after 50 will forge new directions for friendship already have
begun to appear. The Red Hat Society, which has chapters in dozens of cities, is
an example of women using friendship ties to promote a more positive cultural
view of women after age 50. Red Hat Society members wear red hats and purple
clothing to symbolize that after age 50 women will make their own choices about
what is fashionable. As founder Sue Ellen Cooper explained:
“The Red Hat Society began as a result of a few women deciding to greet middle age with
verve, humor and elan. We believe silliness is the comedy relief of life, and since we are
all in it together, we might as well join red-gloved hands and go for the gusto together.
Underneath the frivolity, we share a bond of affection, forged by common life experiences
and a genuine enthusiasm for wherever life takes us next.” (http://www.redhatsociety.com)
Washington (Rabin & Slater, 2005). The Florida location was founded in 1995 and
currently has 330 residents on 278 landscaped home sites within 50 acres of gated,
fenced land (Rabin & Slater, 2005). The model of the lesbian residential/retirement
community might become a viable type of community structure for heterosexual
women friends in the future, as well.
The feminist potential of women’s friendships in the future remains to be seen.
Irigaray (1985) argued that interaction between women enables them to define a
self that transcends the limits imposed on women by patriarchal language and cul-
ture. Interactions between women may not always be harmonious, but they allow
women a new and different way of being that cannot appear when women are de-
fined by the needs, desires, and fantasies of men. If women’s friendships become
more significant as they age, they have the potential to challenge the centrality of
men in women’s lives. This could lead women to recognize and use their power
in the public arena. The functions of women’s friendships could broaden beyond
intimacy and ego support to include instrumental ones, such as access to economic
and political resources, much as men’s friendships do (O’Connor, 1998). Thus,
women’s friendships after 50 potentially have important implications for feminist
and political activism. Guttentag and Secord (1983) theorized that feminist move-
ments tend to develop when women greatly outnumber men. If so, in the future,
women over 50 may become an important base of political influence and social
change.
In conclusion, it remains to be seen if women’s friendships will continue to
reinforce women’s place in the private sphere, as defined by patriarchy, or will
become a catalyst for their entry into the public arena. However, current trends
suggest that friendship in the future will become increasingly important across the
lifespan, particularly so among women after age 50 who will have the greatest
opportunity to construct their own place in the world.
References
Adams, R.G. (1997). Friendship patterns among older women. In J.M. Coyle (Ed.), Hand-
book on Women and Aging (pp. 400–417). Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Adams, G., and Plaut, V.C. (2003). The cultural grounding of personal relationship: Friend-
ships in North American and West African worlds. Personal Relationships, 10, 333–347.
Allan, G. (1998). Friendships and the private sphere. In R.G. Adams and G. Allan (Eds.),
Placing Friendship in Context (pp. 92–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Allan, G. (2001). Personal relationships in late modernity. Personal Relationships, 8, 325–
339.
Antonucci, T., and Akiyama, H. (1997). Concern with others at midlife: Care, comfort,
or compromise? In M.E. Lachman and J.B. James (Eds.), Multiple Paths of Midlife
Development (pp. 147–169). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Argyle, M., and Furnham, A. (1983). Sources of satisfaction and conflict in long-term
relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 481–493.
Argyle, M., and Henderson, M. (1984). The rules of friendship. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 1, 211–238.
128 Suzanna M. Rose
Armstrong, M.J., and Goldsteen, K.S. (1990). Friendship support patterns of older American
women. Journal of Aging Studies, 4, 391–404.
Bank, B.J. (1995). Friendships in Australia and the United States: From feminization to a
more heroic image. Gender & Society, 9, 79–98.
Bank, B.J., and Hansford, S.L. (2003). Gender and friendship: Why are men’s best same-sex
friendships less intimate and supportive? Personal Relationships, 7, 63–78.
Basow, S.A., and Rubenfeld, K. (2003). “Troubles talk”: Effects of gender and gender
typing. Sex Roles, 48, 183–187.
Bell, S., and Coleman, S. (Eds.). (1999). The Anthropology of Friendship. Oxford: Berg.
Bleiszner, R. (2006). A lifetime of caring: Dimensions and dynamics in late-life close
relationships. Personal Relationships, 13, 1–18.
Bleiszner, R., and Adams, R.G. (1992). Adult Friendship. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bleiszner, R., and Roberto, K.A. (2004). Friendship across the life span: Reciprocity in indi-
vidual and relationship development. In F.R. Lang and K.L. Fingerman (Eds.), Growing
Together: Personal Relationships Across the Lifespan (pp. 159–182). Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Bradford, J., and Ryan, C. (1991). Who we are: Health concerns of middle-aged lesbians. In
B. Sang, J. Warshow, and A. Smith (Eds.), Lesbians at Midlife: The Creative Transition
(pp.147–163). San Francisco: Spinsters Ink.
Cartwright, L.K., and Wink, P. (1994). Personality change in women physicians from med-
ical student years to mid-40s. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 291–308.
Clark, M.S. (1981). Noncomparability of benefits given and received: A cue to the existence
of friendship. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 375–381.
Clark, M.S., and Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange re-
lationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
684–691.
Enright, D.J., and Rawlinson, D. (1992). The Oxford Book of Friendship. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Erikson, E.H. (1968). Identity: Youth and Crisis. New York: Norton.
Faderman, L. (1981). Surpassing the Love of Men. New York: Morrow.
Fehr, B. (1996). Friendship Processes. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fehr, B., and Harasymchuk, C. (2005). The experience of emotion in close relationships:
Toward an integration of the emotion-in-relationships and interpersonal script models.
Personal Relationships, 12, 181–196.
Gersick, C.J., and Kram, K.E. (2002). High achieving women at midlife: An exploratory
study. Journal of Management Inquiry, 11, 104–127.
Goodenow, C., and Gaier, E.L. (1990, August). Best Friends: The Close Reciprocal Friend-
ships of Married and Unmarried Women. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Gouldner, H., and Strong, M.S. (1987). Speaking of Friendship: Middle-class Women and
Their Friends. Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Green, E., Hebron, S., and Woodward, D. (1990). Women’s Leisure: What Leisure? Bas-
ingstoke, UK: Macmillan.
Greenberg, S., and Motenko, A.K. (1994). Women growing older: Partnerships for change.
In M.P. Mirkin (Ed.), Women in Context: Toward a Feminist Reconstruction of Psy-
chotherapy (pp. 96–117). New York: Guilford.
Gullestad, M. (1984). Kitchen Table Society. Oslo: Universities Forlaget.
Guttentag, M., and Secord, P. (1983). Too Many Women? The Sex Ratio Question. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
6. Enjoying the Returns 129
Hall, R., and Rose, S. (1996). Friendships between African-American and White lesbians.
In J.S. Weinstock and E.D. Rothblum (Eds.), Lesbian Friendships: For Ourselves and
Each Other (pp. 165–191). New York: New York University Press.
Holmstrom, A.J., Burleson, B.R., and Jones, S.M. (2005). Some consequences for helpers
who deliver “cold comfort”: Why it’s worse for women than men to be inept when
providing emotional support. Sex Roles, 53, 153–172.
Irigaray, L. (1985). This Sex Which Is Not One. New York: Cornell University Press.
Keefe, S.E., Padilla, A.M., and Carlos, M.L. (1979). The Mexican-American extended
family as an emotional support system. Human Organization, 38, 144–152.
Kimmel, D.C., and Sang, B.E. (1995). Lesbians and gay men in midlife. In A.R. D’Augelli
and C.J. Patterson (Eds.), Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities Over the Lifespan: Psy-
chological Perspectives (pp. 190–214). New York: Oxford University Press.
Kitzinger, C. (1996). Toward a politics of lesbian friendship. In J.S. Weinstock and E.D.
Rothblum (Eds.), Lesbian friendships: For ourselves and each other (pp. 295–299). New
York: New York University Press.
Knapp, M.R., and Miller, G.R. (1994). Handbook of Interpersonal Communication (2nd
ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Krause, N., and Borawski-Clark, E. (Eds.) (1995). Social class differences in social support
among older adults. Gerontologist, 35, 498–508.
Larson, R.W., and Bradney, N. (1988). Precious moments with family members and friends.
In R.W. Larson, N. Bradney, and R.M. Milardo (Eds.), Families and Social Networks.
New Perspectives on Family (pp. 107–126). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Levine, S.B. (2005). Inventing the Rest of Our Lives: Women in Second Adulthood. London:
Viking Press.
Lewittes, H.J. (1989). Just being friendly means a lot–Women, friendship, and aging. In
L. Grace and I. Susser (Eds.), Women in the Later Years: Health, Social, and Cultural
Perspectives (pp. 138–159). New York: Harrington Park Press.
Masheter, C. (1997). Healthy and unhealthy friendship and hostility between ex-spouses.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, 207–222.
Nardi, P.M., and Sherrod, D. (1994). Friendships in the lives of gay men and lesbians.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 185–199.
Neugarten, B.L. (1968). The awareness of middle age. In B.L. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle Age
and Aging (pp. 93–98). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
O’Connor, P. (1992). Friendships between Women. New York: Guilford.
O’Connor, P. (1998). Women’s friendships in a post-modern world. In R.G. Adams and
G. Allan (Eds.), Placing friendship in context (pp. 117–135). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Patrick, J.H., Cottrell, L.E., and Barnes, K.A. (2001). Gender, emotional support, and well-
being among the rural elderly. Sex Roles, 45, 15–29.
Paul, M. (2004). The Friendship Crisis: Finding, Making, and Keeping Friends When You’re
Not a Kid Anymore. Emmaus, PA: Rodale.
Pressman, S.D., Cohen, S., Miller, G.E., Barkin, A., Rabin, B.S., and Treanor, J.J. (2005).
Loneliness, social network size, and immune response to influenza vaccination in college
freshmen. Health Psychology, 24, 297–306.
Rabin, J., and Slater, B.R. (2005). Lesbian communities across the United States: Pockets
of resistance and resilience. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 9, 169–182.
Rodriguez, N., Mira, C.B., Myers, H.F., Morris, J.K., and Cardoza, D. (2003). Family
or friends: Who plays a greater supportive role for Latino college students? Cultural
Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9, 236–250.
130 Suzanna M. Rose
Roberto, K.A. (2001). Older women’s relationships: Weaving lives together. In J.D. Garner
and S.O. Mercer (Eds.), Women as They Age (2nd ed., pp. 115–129). New York: Haworth.
Rose, S.M. (1984). How friendships end. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1,
267–278.
Rose, S. (2000). Heterosexism and the study of women’s romantic and friend relationships.
Journal of Social Issues, 56, 315–328.
Rothblum, E.D., Mintz, B., Cowan, D.B., and Haller, C. (1995). Lesbian baby boomers at
midlife. In K. Jay (Ed.), Dyke Life: From Growing Up to Growing Old-A celebration of
the Lesbian Experience (pp. 61–76). New York: Basic Books.
Rusbult, C.E., Zembrodt, I.M., and Gunn, L.K. (1982). Exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect:
Responses to dissatisfaction in romantic involvements. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 43, 1230–1242.
Samter, W., Whaley, B.B., Mortenson, S.T., and Burleson, B.R. (1997). Ethnicity and
emotional support in same-sex friendship: A comparison of Asian-Americans, African-
Americans, and Euro-Americans. Personal Relationships, 4, 413–430.
Sang, B. (1991). Moving toward balance and integration. In B. Sang, J. Warshow, and A.
Smith (Eds.), Lesbians at Midlife: The Creative Transition (pp. 206–214). San Francisco:
Spinsters Ink.
Stanley, J.L. (1996). The lesbian’s experience of friendship. In J.S. Weinstock and E.D.
Rothblum (Eds.), Lesbian Friendships: For Ourselves and Each Other (pp. 39–59). New
York: New York University Press.
Stewart, A.J., Ostrove, J.M., and Helson, R. (2001). Middle aging in women: Patterns of
personality change from the 30s to 50s. Journal of Adult Development, 8, 23–27.
Tangri, S.S., Thomas, V.G., Mednick, M.T., and Lee, K.S. (2003). Predictors of satisfaction
among college-educated Black women in midlife. Journal of Adult Development, 10,
113–135.
Taylor, S.E., Klein, L.C., Lewis, B.P., Gruenewald, T.L., Gurung, R.A., and Updegraff, R.,
(2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-
flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411–429.
Trotman, F.K., and Brody, C.M. (Eds.) (2002). Psychotherapy and Counseling with Older
Women. New York: Springer.
Tully, C.T. (1989). Domestic violence: The ultimate betrayal of human rights. Journal of
Lesbian and Gay Social Services, 13, 83–98.
Vandeventer, E.A., Ostrove, J.M., and Stewart, A.J. (1997). Predicting women’s well-being
in midlife: The importance of personality development and social role involvement.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1147–1160.
Weeks, J., Heaphy, B., and Donovan, C. (2001). Same Sex Intimacies: Families of Choice
and Other Life Experiments. New York: Routledge.
Weinstock, J.S. (2000). Lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender friendships in adulthood:
Review and analysis. In C.J. Patterson and A.R. D’Augelli (Eds.), Lesbian, Gay, and
Bisexual Identities in Families: Psychological Perspectives (pp. 122–153). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Weinstock, J.S. (2004). Lesbian flexibility: Friend and/or family connections among lesbian
ex-lovers. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 8, 193–238.
Weston, K. (1997). Families we choose: Lesbians, gays, kinship (rev. ed.). New York:
Columbia University Press. [Originally published 1991.]
Yager, J. (2002). When Friendship Hurts: How to Deal with Friends Who Betray, Abandon,
or Wound You. New York: Fireside.
7
Contemporary Midlife
Grandparenthood
Liat Kulik
Grandparenthood has always been considered a basic human experience, and usu-
ally it is a positive one. The grandparent role is salient for most older individuals
(Cherlin & Furstenberg, 1985), and some scholars have argued that its impact on
the individual and family will continue to grow (Uhlenberg & Kirby, 1998). To-
day, grandparenting can span several decades, from the 30s in cases of teenage
pregnancy, to over 100 years of age in cases of extreme longevity (Hagestad,
1985). Although it is difficult to determine whether the grandparent role is more
significant than the roles of spouse and parent, there is no doubt that becoming a
grandparent is a milestone in the life cycle, and, as such, it is of considerable rele-
vance to self-identity. From a developmental perspective, because the transition to
grandparenthood symbolizes a new stage of life, it is an especially important com-
ponent of age identity (Bastida, 1987; Giarrusso et al., 1996). From a psychosocial
perspective, the experience of grandparenting is influenced by synchronicity with
other events in the older person’s life (Troll, 1985). Accordingly, a person’s be-
havior in the grandparent role and the significance attributed to grandparenting are
influenced by other major life cycle events, such as changes in marital and work
status.
The grandparent role has been defined as a “roleless role,” or a role that is not
governed by rights and obligations to the same extent as the parent role (Clavan,
1978). Based on a combination of perspectives presented in the literature, Kahana
and Kahana (1971) proposed a complex conceptualization of grandparenthood.
According to this conceptualization, grandparenthood is a social role that involves
ascribed status and expectations for role performance vis-à-vis the family and the
society. Grandparenthood also can be viewed as an emotional state or an intrapsy-
chic experience, which is part of individual development (Kornhaber & Woodward,
1981). In that context, grandparenthood is a transaction between the grandchild
and grandparent, which involves reciprocity (Werner, 1991). Grandparenthood
can also be viewed as part a group process within the family, which involves rela-
tionships and interdependencies among three generations (Cohler & Gruenbaum,
1981). Finally, grandparenthood can be considered a symbol. It is a reflection of
continuity, potency, and usefulness to society (Troll, 1983; Werner, 1991). From
131
132 Liat Kulik
any point of view, it is clear that far-reaching changes in modern society are also
reflected in the grandparent role.
In line with the developmental and psychosocial approaches described above,
the significance attributed to the grandparent role and styles of grandparenting are
usually affected by the sociocultural environment and by the stage of life in which
the event occurs. In this chapter, I will attempt to shed light on the significance of
grandparenthood in the contemporary period, with emphasis on an aspect that has
been neglected in existing research, namely grandmothers in their 50s.
as a hamis. Not only can one individual perform several family roles at the same
time, but each role is characterized by diverse characteristics and patterns. Several
decades ago, for example, the age range of children in a family tended to be
relatively homogeneous. Today, at the same point in time one person can be a
parent to married offspring, and still have adolescents or even infants living at
home. This creates complex situations, in which one person needs to use a variety
of parenting skills in order to satisfy the needs of children who are at different
stages of life. There are situations, albeit not common ones, in which a person
can have grandchildren and offspring of the same age, or even offspring who are
younger than the grandchildren. There are several reasons for the prolonged period
of parenthood and diverse age range of children. First, in light of increased rates
of divorce and remarriage, the remarried couple may wish to have children of their
own, in addition to the children that each spouse has from a previous marriage.
Furthermore, especially with the advancements in medical technology, there is
an increasing tendency for women to give birth in middle age and prolong the
fertility period. Thus, when a woman reaches an age where she is about to become
a grandmother, she may conceive one last child of her own out of a desire to
prove her femininity, to cement a new marriage, or to prolong motherhood to the
maximum. In Israel, the tendency to give birth in middle age (and even at the age of
grandparenthood) is particularly significant among ultra-Orthodox Jewish women,
who view procreation as a divine commandment and seek to have as many children
as possible during the period of fertility. Furthermore, in light of persistent military
tension in Israeli society, there are couples who lose a son or daughter in war or in
a terrorist attack. In these cases, the couple might conceive a child at a relatively
late stage of life in order to fill the vacuum and in an attempt to rehabilitate the
family.
Diversity in the nature of family roles may also derive from changes in fam-
ily structure as a result of divorce and separation, remarriage, and blending of
families. Thus, at one and the same time, a grandparent can have biological and
step-grandchildren. For example, in cases of midlife remarriage, a person can
act as a step-parent to the new spouse’s children and as a step-grandparent to
that spouse’s grandchildren. Moreover, when divorced offspring remarry, a grand-
parent can acquire step-children (the new spouse of their offspring) as well as
step-grandchildren. These multiple and diverse family roles can be a source of
pleasure to some individuals in the modern world (including grandparents), but
can also generate stress and confusion of self-identity.
earning patterns and division of labor differ from the traditional ones known in
the past. For example, there are dual-career families, in which both spouses fo-
cus on developing their careers, but one spouse bears the main responsibility for
maintaining the household. In some cases, both spouses earn about the same in-
come (“modern” families), and in other cases the wife is the main provider and the
husband sometimes cares for the household (“innovators”) (Izraeli, 1994). Even
though these configurations include families that are not distressed or at risk, and
even families that are relatively well-off, there are families that experience a certain
degree of instability due to the excessive burden on one or both parents and due to
ambiguous gender roles or parental roles. In these situations, grandparents are of-
ten called on to lend a hand and to help maintain stability in the family (Kornhaber,
1996).
and send them home when they act bratty’” (p. 553). Some have described grand-
parenthood as bliss: “Being Daphna’s grandmother is the ultimate bliss, the crème
de la crème. When little Daphna opens the door shouts ‘grandma, grandma,’ I
am filled to the brim with happiness, and there’s no other feeling like it” (Kulik,
2005).
Today, it seems that these feelings are accompanied by other voices, which
shatter the prevailing image of grandparenthood as the ultimate happiness. These
voices reflect changes in the women’s perceptions of self-fulfillment, as well as
changing aspirations, and portray a more balanced view of being a grandmother.
According to Johnson (1983), some women described a conflict between their role
as a grandmother, and emphasized other interests and aspirations in life: “I am
just not the grandmother type—I travel, take courses, have my own interests . . . I
have my own life to lead, and so do my grandchildren” (p. 554). In a similar vein,
Goodfellow (2003) cited the following response: “My friend had trouble with her
young grandchild, who would not want to go home because he had been with her
12 hours and more a day” (p. 5). Another response to the experience of grandmoth-
erhood is the fear of growing old, as reflected in the following statements: “I’m
too young to be a grandmother” (Johnson, 1983, p. 554); “If I had enough guts,
I would tell my children to wait a while before they start a family. Grandchildren
will bring me into another stage of life, and I feel like I’m still a child myself”
(Kulik, 2005). Some grandmothers complain about feeling bored when they are
with their grandchildren: “I get bored sitting and playing with her [the grandchild]
and doing things like drawing and playing with playdough” (Goodfellow, 2003,
p. 6). Sometimes the transition to grandmotherhood in midlife forces women to
confront the fact that they will not bear any more children and that the childbearing
function will be passed on to the next generation. As one grandmother expressed
it: “When my first grandchild was born, I felt that a biological stage of my life had
ended, the stage of childbearing . . . but in a strange way I returned to my own expe-
rience of childbearing and child rearing. I was filled with memories from that time,
nostalgia . . . It was the most beautiful period of my life—and now, with the birth
of my grandchild, I realize how I can’t turn the wheels back . . . ” (Kulik, 2005).
The transition to grandmotherhood can also generate inner conflict, for example,
when the new grandmother thinks that her children are taking advantage of her, as
described by Goodfellow (2003): “I used to have my grandson for breakfast, give
him his meals, bathe him at night, and get him ready for bed . . . so what does his
mother do?” (p. 6).
Goodfellow (2003) also described strain caused by the overload that ensues
when the grandmother feels drained by the responsibility of taking care of grand-
children: “I sometimes feel tied down. It’s really hard to look after your grandchild,
particularly when the child’s parents want to go out (socially) or I have something I
want to do” (p. 8). Another source of stress may be the grandmother’s desire to free
herself of the constraints of being a grandmother: “If it’s illness or something like
that, I’d have to [help], I would [help] . . . But I don’t think I’d do it by choice, just
to suit them, because I like freedom as well” (Cotterill, 1992, p. 613). Finally, with
regard to the feelings of stress that undermine the consensus of happy grandmoth-
erhood, Robertson (1977) revealed that the role of grandmother is not significantly
7. Contemporary Midlife Grandparenthood 137
related to life satisfaction. In fact, the findings of Robertson’s (1977) study were
consistent with Blau (1973), who argued that “when in need, one good friend
is more important in maintaining morale than a dozen grandchildren” (Johnson,
1983, p. 549). Evidently, there is no longer a consensus that grandmotherhood is
all good. The increasing emphasis on freedom of expression, openness, and break-
ing myths has enabled women to develop a more balanced, complex, and diverse
perspective of grandmotherhood.
Personal Context
Regarding the personal context that may affect the transition to grandparenthood,
researchers (Kivett, 1998) have mentioned the grandmother’s health as a major
factor. Grandparents who are in relatively good health adapt better to the transition
than do those who are in poor health. Another personal factor that may affect the
138 Liat Kulik
Interpersonal Context
Relationships with other family members and with grandchildren can also affect
the transition to grandparenthood. In this connection, researchers have found that
the characteristics of the grandchild affect the grandparenting experience. For ex-
ample, chronically ill grandchildren may intensify the involvement of grandparents
in caregiving and provision of other resources (Dilworth-Anderson, 1994). Fur-
thermore, certain social problems experienced by adult children today may affect
the grandparenting role. Unexpected roles that grandparents assume as a result
of social and economic crises, such as cases of alcoholism among adult children,
can be a source of considerable stress (Bahr, 1994). In addition, the grandparents
often intensify their involvement in the grandparenting role when their children
experience economic problems, such as marginal employment or unemployment
(Dressel & Barnhill, 1994).
Conversely, the experience of grandparenthood can also be affected by the way
the offspring treat their grandparents. A study by Cotterill (1992) revealed that
most women consider the child’s grandparents to be the best source of childcare,
although they would rather receive such assistance from their own mother than
from their mother-in-law. Evidently, when it comes to childcare, young women
share more in common with their mothers than with their mothers-in-law.
Social Context
Social context affects the transition to grandmotherhood, as well as the experience
and expectations of being a grandmother. One of the main characteristics of the
7. Contemporary Midlife Grandparenthood 139
(Kornhaber, 1996). In addition, they expect more of themselves, and their quality
of life is usually better. Today, grandmotherhood is no longer a single commit-
ment, and many grandmothers in their 50s are full-time workers and career women.
Thus, grandmothers in their 50s fulfill more social roles than in the past—not only
compared with women their age in previous generations, but also compared with
the roles they fulfilled at previous stages in their own lives. Indeed, Hall’s classic
study on the number of roles that women perform during the life cycle revealed
that in middle age women fulfill more roles than at any other stage of life (Hall,
1975).
In light of the rapid changes in the modern world, and following the general
increase in longevity in the Western world, as described above, care of aging parents
continues for a long period of the woman’s life. In Israel, especially among the
religious community and among Jews of Asian-African origin, responsibility for
care of elderly members of the extended family (e.g., the father-in-law or mother-
in-law) often falls squarely on the woman’s shoulders (Izraeli, 1994). Thus, the
traditional caregiving role that women assume in midlife is broader than at any
time in the past, or at any other stage of the life cycle. In the contemporary era,
it is possible that at age 50 a woman will have to care for her aging parents—a
responsibility which is physically and emotionally taxing in itself—in addition to
caring for children who are living at home (and sometimes are still very young),
and assisting children who have left the home. Furthermore, in Israel many women
at about age 50 have children serving in the army—a role that is stressful in itself.
In the best of cases, being a parent to a soldier involves a considerable amount
of domestic work, such as doing laundry, cooking, and ironing when the son or
daughter spends weekends on leave (Izraeli, 1999). Moreover, the perpetual state
of military tension in Israel is an especially significant stressor for parents of
young men serving in combat units. All of this tension is compounded when the
same woman is a grandmother, and is asked to care for her grandchildren so that
the “younger generation” will be able to advance in their own careers. A study
(Kornhaber, 1996) conducted in the United States revealed that over 3.2 million
children had grandparents who were surrogate (full-time) parents. Although the
situation in which grandparents replace the parents is an extreme one, grandparents
often find themselves “on standby” in case they are called on to care for their
grandchildren and relieve the parents of that burden. Thus, that burden often falls
on the shoulders of the grandmother.
In addition to their multiple and simultaneous family roles, women who are
grandmothers in their 50s also have a wide range of social roles. In many cases,
at this stage of life women are at the peak of their careers after years of intensive
efforts and hard work. Often, because of the changes in their lives and in their
perspectives (e.g., the “empty nest” effect, changes in time perspective), women
in their 50s may decide to give their careers an extra push. Midlife changes and
evaluations can also provide an incentive for new activities such as going back to
school, taking courses, developing a hobby they always wanted to pursue (even to
the point of learning a new profession), or making a career of volunteer activities
(Lieber, 1990).
7. Contemporary Midlife Grandparenthood 141
Researchers (e.g., Friedman, 1987) have argued that the woman’s power in-
creases in midlife as marital power relations begin to shift. There are various ex-
planations for the reported shift in power with age. Neugarten (1968) argued that
it reflects a reversal of gender roles with age, as older men become more feminine
and older women become more masculine. Expanding on this argument, Gutmann
(1987) claimed that biologically determined reproductive capacities cause differ-
ent aspects of the personality to be expressed or suppressed by each sex. Although
both men and women have the potential to be nurturing and powerful, Gutmann
(1987) referred to the “parental emergency,” which forces women to develop char-
acteristics of empathy, compassion, and nurturing. At the end of the “parental
emergency” women can express aspects of themselves that they had suppressed.
In a similar vein, Jung (1971) suggested that women become more masculine
in middle age out of a desire to express latent personality traits. Alternatively,
Rossi (1980) claimed that the increase in women’s power is a result of biological
changes, and other researchers have emphasized the role of psychosocial factors
as reflected in changing marital, family, and social dynamics (for a review, see
Friedman & Todd, 1994). In that connection, it can be argued that middle-aged
grandmothers should take advantage of this shift in power because they have to be
strong and develop appropriate skills for coping with simultaneous and multiple
roles in contemporary society.
Thus, grandmothers in their 50s are like acrobats who juggle many roles, such as
mother, wife, mother-in-law, grandmother, community volunteer, career woman,
student. Often they change hats at different times of the day, depending on which
role they are playing. However, there are times when they have to wear all of their
hats at one time. Juggling roles without letting any of the hats drop is an art in
itself. The effort and courage required to perform all of these roles successfully is
truly a heroic feat.
Conclusion
This chapter provides an overview of grandparenthood in the contemporary era,
with emphasis on middle-aged grandmothers in their 50s. The characteristics of
grandmothers in that age group derive partly from developments in the modern
era, and partly from the psychosocial processes that typify midlife. As shown here,
some of the salient characteristics of grandmothers in their 50s include a variety of
grandmothering styles, which differ from the image of the traditional grandmother.
The diverse range of grandmothering styles derives from the relationship between
the grandmothering role and the other roles that many women fulfill in midlife. In
contrast to the traditional grandmothering style that prevailed in the past, which fo-
cuses on the relationship with grandchildren, the modern grandmothering style is a
product of the relationship between the grandmother role (which is one dimension)
and other roles such as career, leisure activities, and volunteer work.
Today, middle-aged grandmotherhood among women in their 50s can be por-
trayed through a gallery of images, where grandmothers have more freedom and
142 Liat Kulik
legitimacy to choose the image or style that is appropriate for them. Because
middle-aged grandmothers have many years of life ahead of them, they attempt
to give those years meaning and content in various ways. Some women adhere to
the traditional grandmother image and devote themselves to their grandchildren.
However, of the diverse range of styles that they can adopt, they may choose alter-
native patterns that are more appropriate for them. In this connection, Goodfellow
(2003) adopted Hakin’s “preference theory” to explain grandmothers’ choices of
styles (Hakin, 2000). Hakin (2000), who considered the lifestyle choices of work-
ing women, proposed that women today are in a position to make a real choice
between work and family when they decide to adopt a lifestyle. Preference the-
ory focuses on women’s choices in relation to work, family, and the nature of
decisions about lifestyle. Consistent with preference theory, it can be argued that
today middle-aged grandparents are freer than they have ever been in the past to
choose the style of grandparenting that they prefer. Even women with no children
(or grandchildren) who aspire to this role can choose to “adopt” neighborhood
children with no grandparents (or none nearby), or, in the United States, to join
an organization such as “Foster Grandparents,” which matches children with older
people.
In sum, one of the major conclusions of this review is that grandmothers in
their 50s no longer have to conform to a homogeneous image. The diversity that
characterizes numerous roles and phenomena in the contemporary era also applies
to grandmotherhood. Notably, the grandmother role is integrated with other roles
that women fulfill in midlife, and the possible combinations are infinite. Thus, the
weight that women allocate to the grandmother role within the role set can vary,
and every woman is free to create the grandmothering style that is appropriate for
her.
References
Bahr, K.S. (1994). The strengths of Apache grandmothers: Observations on commitment
culture and caretaking. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 25, 233–248.
Barranti, C.C.R. (1985). The grandparent/grandchild relationship: Family resources in an
era of voluntary bonds. Family Relations, 34, 343–351.
Bastida, E. (1987). Sex-typed age norms among older Hispanics. Gerontologist, 27, 29–60.
Blau, Z.S. (1973). Old Age in a Changing Society. New York: New Viewpoints.
Boyd, R. (1967). The emerging social roles of the four-generation family. In G. Dockes
(Ed.), Our Elderly Americans: Challenge and Response (pp. 11–21). Spartenburg, SC:
Converse College Press.
Burton, L.M. (1992). Black grandparents rearing children of drug-addicted parents: Stres-
sors, outcomes, and social service needs. Gerontologist, 32, 744–751.
Burton, L.M., and Bengston, V.L. (1985). Black grandmothers: Issues of timing and conti-
nuity of roles. In V.L. Bengston and J.F. Robertson (Eds.), Grandparenthood (pp. 61–77).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Burton, L.M., Dilworth-Anderson, P., and Merriwether-deVries, C. (1995). Context and
surrogate parenting among contemporary grandparents. Marriage and Family Review,
20, 349–366.
Cherlin, A.J., and Furstenberg, F.F. (1985). Styles and strategies of grandparenting. In V.L.
Bengtson and J.F. Robertson (Eds.), Grandparenthood (pp. 97–116). Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage.
Clavan, S. (1978). The impact of social class and social trends on the role of the grandparent.
Family Coordinator, 27, 351–357.
Cohler, B.J., and Gruenbaum, H.V. (1981). Mothers, Grandmothers, and Daughters: Per-
sonality and Child Care in Three Generations. New York: Wiley.
Cotterill, P. (1992). But for freedom, you see, not to be a babyminder: Women’s attitudes
toward grandmother care. Sociology, 26, 603–618.
Creighton, L. (1991, December 16). The silent saviors. US News & World Report, pp. 80–89.
144 Liat Kulik
Denham, T.E., and Smith, C.W. (1989). The influence of grandparents on grandchildren: A
review of the literature and resources. Family Relations, 38, 345–350.
Deutsch, H. (1945). The Psychology of Women. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Dilworth-Anderson, P. (1994). The importance of grandparents in extended kin caregiv-
ing to Black children with sickle cell disease. Journal of Health and Social Policy, 5,
185–202.
Dressel, P.L., and Barnhill, S.K. (1994). Reframing gerontological thought and practice:
The case of grandmothers with daughters in prison. Gerontologist, 34, 685–691.
Elder, G.H. (1994). Time, human aging, and social change: Perspective on the life course.
Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 4–15.
Friedman, A. (1987). Getting stronger with age: Changes in women over the life cycle.
Israel Social Science Research, 5, 76–86.
Friedman, A., and Todd, J. (1994). Kenyan women tell a story: Interpersonal power of
women in three subcultures in Kenya. Sex Roles, 31, 503–525.
George, L.K. (1980). Role Transitions in Later Life. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Giarrusso, R., Silverstein, M., and Bengston, V.L. (1996). Family complexity and the grand-
parent role. Generations, 20, 17–23.
Goodfellow, J. (2003, May). Grandparents’ Caring for Their Children’s Chil-
dren. Paper presented at the Conference on Our Children: The Future, Sydney,
Australia.
Gutmann, D.L. (1975). Parenthood: A key to the comparative study of life cycle. In N. Dotan
and L.H. Ginzberg (Eds.), Life Development Psychology: Normative Life Crises (pp. 167–
184). New York: Academic Press.
Gutmann, D.L. (1987). Reclaimed Power: Toward a New Psychology of Men and Women
in Later Life. New York: Basic Books.
Hagestad, G.O. (1985). Continuity and connectedness. In V.L. Bengston and J.F. Robertson
(Eds.), Grandparenthood (pp. 31–48). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hakin, C. (2000). Work-lifestyle Choices in the 21st Century: Preference Theory. London:
Oxford University Press.
Hall, T. (1975). Pressures from work, self, and home in the life stages of married women.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 6, 127–135.
Israel Central Bureau of Statistics. (2005). Household expenditure survey 2000–2002:
Households of immigrants from the USSR (former). [Special Publication No. 1231].
Jerusalem: Author.
Izraeli, D.N. (1994). Money matters: Spousal income and family/work relations among
physician couples in Israel. Sociological Quarterly, 35, 69–84.
Izraeli, D.N. (1999). Hamigdur beolam ha’avoda [Genderizing in the work place]. In D.N.
Izraeli, A. Friedman, H. Dahan-Kalev, S. Fogiel-Bijaoui, H. Herzog, N. Hasan, and H.
Naveh (Eds.), Sex, Gender, Politics: Women in Israel (pp. 167–216). Tel Aviv: Hakibbutz
Hameuchad (in Hebrew).
Jannelli, L.M. (1988). Depictions of grandparents in children’s literature. Educational
Gerontology, 14, 193–202.
Johnson, C.L. (1983). A cultural analysis of the grandmother. Research on Aging, 5, 547–
567.
Jung, C.V. (1971). The stage of life. In J. Campbell (Ed.), The Portable Jung (pp. 3–22).
New York: Viking Press.
Kahana, B., and Kahana, E. (1971). Theoretical and research perspective on grandparent-
hood. Aging and Human Development, 2, 61–68.
7. Contemporary Midlife Grandparenthood 145
Kennedy, G.E. (1990). College students’ expectations of grandparent and grandchild be-
havior. Gerontologist, 30, 43–48.
Kivett, V.R. (1998). Transitions in grandparents’ lives: Effects on the grandparent role.
In M.E. Szinovacz (Ed.), Handbook of Grandparenting (pp. 131–143). Westport, CT:
Greenwood Press.
Kornhaber, A. (1996). Contemporary Grandparenting. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kornhaber, A., and Woodward, K. (1981). Grandparents, Grandchildren: The Vital Con-
nection. Garden City, NY: Doubleday/Anchor Press.
Kulik, L. (2005). Being a Grandmother in 2000. Unpublished manuscript.
Lajewski, H.C. (1959). Working mothers and their arrangements for the care of their chil-
dren. Social Security Bulletin, 22, 8–13.
Laqueur, T.W. (1992). The facts of fatherhood. In B. Throne and M. Yalom (Eds.), Rethinking
the Family: Some Feminist Questions (pp. 155–175). Boston: Northeastern University
Press.
Lieber, M. (1990). Shinui kariera be’emtza hahyim [Changing careers in mid-life]. Unpub-
lished Master’s thesis, Tel Aviv University (in Hebrew).
Marsiglio, W. (1993). Contemporary scholarship on fatherhood: Culture, identity, and con-
duct. Journal of Family Issues, 14, 484–509.
Matthews, S., and Sprey, J. (1984). The impact of divorce on grandparenting: An exploratory
study. Gerontologist, 24, 41–47.
Minkler, M., and Roe, K.M. (1993). Grandmothers as Caregivers. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Neugarten, B. (1968). Middle Age and Aging: A Reader in Social Psychology. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Neugarten, B., and Weinstein, K. (1964). The changing American grandparent. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 26, 199–204.
Orbach, Z. (2001). Savta bejeans [Grandma in jeans]. Tel Aviv: Orbach Press (in Hebrew).
Robertson, J.F. (1976). Significance of grandparents’ perceptions of young adult grandchil-
dren. Gerontologist, 16, 137–140.
Robertson, J.F. (1977). Grandmotherhood: A study of role conceptions. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 39, 165–174.
Rossi, A. (1980). Life-span theories and women’s life. Signs, 6, 4–32.
Schlesinger, R., and Schlesinger, B. (1998). Grandparenthood: Multiculural perspectives.
Journal of Psychology and Judaism, 22, 247–264.
Smith, P.K. (1995). Grandparenthood. In M.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting
(pp. 89–111). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Storm, R., and Storm, S.K. (1993). Grandparents raising grandchildren: Goals and support
groups. Educational Gerontology, 19, 705–715.
Sussman, M.B. (1953). The help pattern in the middle class family. American Sociological
Review, 18, 22–28.
Sussman, M.B. (1962). Kin family network: Unheralded structural current conceptualiza-
tions of family functioning. Marriage and Family Living, 24, 231–240.
Timberlake, E.M. (1986). The value of grandchildren to grandmothers. Journal of Geron-
tological Social Work, 2, 63–67.
Troll, L.E. (1983). Grandparents: The party watchdogs. In T. Brubaker (Ed.), Family Rela-
tionships in Later Life (pp. 67–74). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Troll, L.E. (1985). The contingencies of grandparenting. In V.L. Bengston and J.F. Robertson
(Eds.), Grandparenthood (pp. 135–149). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
146 Liat Kulik
Uhlenberg, P., and Kirby, J.B. (1998). Grandparenthood over time: Historical and demo-
graphic trends. In M.E. Szinovacz (Ed.), Handbook on Grandparenthood (pp. 23–39).
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Werner, E.E. (1991). Grandparent–grandchild relationships among US ethnic groups. In
P.K. Smith (Ed.), The Psychology of Grandparenthood: An International Perspective
(pp. 68–82). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Young, M. (1959). The role of the extend family in disaster. Human Relations, 7,
189–204.
8
Women Over 50: Caregiving Issues
Rosalie J. Ackerman and Martha E. Banks
1
Pseudonym
147
148 Rosalie J. Ackerman and Martha E. Banks
at home for about 2 or 3 weeks before my grandmother’s final hospitalization. During the
previous 2 years, my mother’s father and then her mother had died. My mother was unable
to spend time with her father before he died, and she saw her mother only briefly before life
support was removed. Her sister, single, in her mid 40s, and living with my grandparents,
had provided care for my grandparents at home in Saint Vincent, West Indies where she
had had the assistance of servants and nurses. When my mother called my aunt, then also
in her mid 60s, to help with my father, my aunt was a relative expert at physical caregiving
for ill adults.
Was I prepared for caregiving for an adult? Although I was in clinical practice in a
geriatric rehabilitation facility, I was not at all prepared to care for my father. The personal
assistance I observed 5 days a week was provided by a trained multidisciplinary staff. In
my parents’ home, three untrained women were not appropriately prepared to take care of
my father.
In my family, on both sides, despite the differences in national backgrounds, eldest daugh-
ters were historically expected to be the family caregivers. As a child, I had observed some
of my paternal grandmother’s elderly cousins providing care for other elderly relatives. I
assumed they learned from each other as part of a large extended family living in close prox-
imity to each other. I grew up in a nuclear family that included my paternal grandmother;
there were no extended family members in the same state. At the time of my father’s illness,
I was a single eldest daughter with a career. I was pressured by some distant relatives to
give up the career and devote my full efforts to my father’s care.
My best contribution to my father’s care was my ability to talk with the healthcare
professionals and to facilitate their direct communication with my father. In addition, I was
able to assist in his pain management. He tried to follow a schedule for his pain medication
in order to avoid addiction. As a result, he had cycles of terrible pain. I explained that he
needed to take the medicine more often in order to control the pain; again, I interceded
with the oncologist for better explanation that “prn (as needed)” superceded the schedule
listed on the prescription.
Sometimes, caregiving ends with a telephone call. Mine did.
Tina’s Sister (mid 20s): Daddy just passed away.
The level of my mother’s grief when my father died was reflective, in part, of a disbelief
that such a thing could happen. She was as unprepared for the end of caregiving as she was
for the personal assistance during caregiving.
During my early 50s, I served as a caregiver for a friend following emergency surgery.
Although I had anticipated that I would be providing mere living space for 1 or 2 weeks, I
was a caregiver for about 6 months. My home was on one level, and my friend was unable
to manage the stairs in her house. When I made the offer of my home, I did not realize
how ill my friend would be. I had assumed that she would be able to take care of herself
when she was released from the hospital. Instead, she needed assistance with meals and
transportation to medical appointments. I also had to assist in some medical decisions.
Because I had not planned to provide that level of care, and found that I had to juggle my
job and social life with her care, it created a strain on our friendship. I was angry, but had
no suitable target for my anger.
2
Pseudonym
8. Caregiving Issues 149
Family caregivers wrestle not only with tasks, but with fundamental questions of life, their
own mortality, as well as with relationships with people important to them, who may be
suffering and dying (Ziemba & Lynch-Sauer, 2005, p. 111).
What Is Caregiving?
Caregiving involves direct personal assistance [assistance with activities of daily
living (ADLs3 ) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs4 )], as well as
primary responsibility for the health and welfare of people receiving informal care
3
“Activities of daily living are activities related to personal care and include bathing or
showering, dressing, getting in or out of bed or a chair, using the toilet, and eating” (US
Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
National Center for Health Statistics, 2004a).
4
“Instrumental activities of daily living are activities related to independent living and
include preparing meals, managing money, shopping for groceries or personal items, per-
forming light or heavy housework, and using a telephone” (US Department of Health and
Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health
Statistics, 2004b).
150 Rosalie J. Ackerman and Martha E. Banks
in the community or formal care in institutions. Many women over the age of 50
provide caregiving. This issue is of concern to women because they dispropor-
tionately care for others (Adams et al., 2002; Banks & Ackerman, 2006; Brow-
der, 2002; Farran, 1997; Fuller-Thomson, 2005; Hunt, 2003; Kiecolt-Glaser &
Newton, 2001; Minkler, 2005; Minkler & Fuller-Thomson, 2005; Sleath et al.,
2005; US Census Bureau, 2004a; 2004b). In addition, there is some evidence
that the caregiving process negatively affects women’s health more severely and
for longer periods than it does for men (Vahtera et al., 2006). Some of the is-
sues faced by women caring for parents, adult children, grandchildren, signifi-
cant others, and friends include ways in which they find themselves in the role
of caregiver, preparation for caregiving, variety of family roles, support and
self-care, harm in the caregiving relationship, and loss during and at the end
of caregiving.
The term “caregiving” has been perceived by some to indicate passive provision
and receipt of care (Mona, 2003), however, caregiving involves an active relation-
ship. For this chapter, the term “personal care assistance” is limited to situations in
which the person providing the assistance does not have primary responsibility for
that care. By connotation, “caregiving” excludes parenting of infants and young
children. “Parenting” generally tends to be celebrated, whereas “caregiving” is
often regarded as negative and burdensome. It is important, however, to consider
that both “caregiving” and “parenting” involve provision of personal assistance
and responsibility for the welfare of another person.
There has been considerable research on caregiving. Much of the research has
focused on the negative impact of caregiving on caregivers. In this chapter, we will
provide an overview of the positive aspects of caregiving while acknowledging the
realistic difficulties engendered in the caregiving process. The research provides
models of caregiving and recommendations for efficient and mutually beneficial
caregiving relationships.
involves the unrealistic medical model that doggedly focuses on cure rather than
acknowledging the reality that some disease processes are not curable and that
inevitable death should not be construed as failure. An additional conflict occurs
as new medications and treatments become available, leaving caregivers with deci-
sions about the introduction of experimental treatments that might alleviate some
symptoms without being actual “cures.”
Grandparenting
Recent attention has been given to the increasing number of grandparents, usually
grandmothers, who are raising their grandchildren. Consistent with the myth of the
nuclear family is the myth of grandparents as couples sharing a pleasant retirement
without the caregiving responsibilities that dominated their younger years. The re-
ality is that nearly 6 million grandparents live in households with their grandchil-
dren, and approximately 2.5 million grandparents are raising their grandchildren
due to the unavailability of the children’s parents (Minkler & Fuller-Thomson,
2005). More than one-half (62.8%) of those childrearing grandparents are women.
Many of these grandparents complain of psychological burden, role overload, and
lack of support. They are dealing with their own health issues, including visual and
hearing problems, as well as functional limitations in their own activities of daily
living. Grandmothers, who have physical problems and/or are living in poverty,
have more difficulty providing caregiving for their grandchildren and experience
more depression than do those in better health and with more resources (Li, 2005).
For more information about grandparenting, readers are referred to the chapter by
Liat Kulik (this volume).
A stress/coping paradigm most commonly asks “What does it mean to provide care to an
impaired family member in terms of caregiving tasks (stress appraisal), what resources are
available to caregivers, and what effect does this experience have on caregivers (outcomes)?”
In an existential paradigm, one asks “How can I discover or create meaning?” (Farran, 1997,
p. 254)
8. Caregiving Issues 155
According to Farran (1997), several investigators have suggested that there are
recurring themes that describe the caregiving process. Adaptation defines the entire
process that occurs in response to the stress of caring for a person with dementia. In
addition, resource variables such as coping skills, personal control, self-efficacy,
knowledge, and hardiness; primary and secondary appraisal; and emotional and
physical health outcomes are important. Tina found caregiving to be a means to
repay her parents for raising her. Becky, in retrospect, felt a closer bond with her
friend as she and her friend shared an experience that most friends do not have.
They had mutual greater appreciation for each other’s strengths and vulnerabilities.
In an overview of the caregiving literature, Hunt (2003) found that the predomi-
nant foci were on negative (caregiver burden, caregiver strain, or caregiver stress),
positive (caregiver esteem, uplifts of caregiving, satisfaction, finding or making
meaning through caregiving, or gain in the caregiving experience), and neutral
(caregiver appraisal).
Farran’s existential approach to understanding caregiving provides a positive
framework, but much of the research concerns buffers or modifiers that make it
possible for people to manage caregiving, although the focus is on the burden-
some aspects of caregiving. For example, Lyons et al. (2002) noted that “[t]here
is broad consensus in the caregiving literature that caring for an elderly rela-
tive places the caregiver at risk for compromised physical and mental health”
(p. P195), and Gilley et al. (2005) indicated that “[t]he emotional cost of providing
care to a family member with a dementia syndrome can be substantial” (p. 173).
Sherwood et al. (2005) focused on the depression that can arise with the burden of
caregiving.
Lyons et al. (2002) examined caregiving dyads with positive and negative at-
tributions of the interactive context within individual relationships and in a group
sample. Important comparisons were the perceptions of depression, subjective so-
cial burden, medical negative health conditions across the time span, restricted
social-activity burden in both roles of caregiving, and dyadic psychological well-
being. The dyadic relationship predicted negative outcomes of caregiver strain.
Within the dyad, both caregivers and care recipients generally provided equiva-
lent evaluations of the daily living activities and needs of care recipients. Dyads in
which caregivers who reported more caregiving difficulties than care recipients did
had poorer relationship quality. Coping training or other therapeutic interventions
might be used to reduce the situational stress of the caregivers.
Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton (2001) developed a pathway model for aspects of
caregiving in marital relationships that impacts upon physical and psychological
health. They noted “that negative aspects of social relationships are often indepen-
dent of positive aspects . . . and are important independent predictors of psycho-
logical and physical functioning” (p. 474). Li and Seltzer (2005) found a similar
independence between positive (daughters’ feelings of closeness to parents) and
negative (daughters’ sense of strain in interactions with parents) aspects of parent–
daughter relationships as they impacted on daughters’ self-esteem. The results of
both studies suggest that reduction of the strain in relationships between caregivers
and care recipients is critical to the success of the caregiving relationship.
156 Rosalie J. Ackerman and Martha E. Banks
During the process of caregiving, changes in the care recipient’s health can lead
to the need for changes in the living situation. For people with progressive disor-
ders, “family caregivers are likely to be increasingly aware that institutionalization
may eventually become necessary. This awareness is likely to engender some cog-
nitive dissonance, particularly when the caregiver has negative attitudes related to
institutionalization” (Gilley et al., 2005, p. 185). Tina’s mother, who had been a
geriatric social worker dealing with nursing home placement, assisted in her care-
giving by developing a living will that stated her preference to be institutionalized
as soon as she was unable to live independently. However, Tina’s mother had not
wanted to place her own husband in a nursing home, even when it was clear that
adequate care could no longer be provided in her home.
One of the burdens of caregiving is that a family member who provides di-
rect personal assistance has a limited opportunity to be gainfully employed. This
results in increased financial pressures during a period when the care recipient,
and therefore the family, is likely to be faced with high healthcare costs (Hirst,
2005). The United States has been slow to consider or implement universal health
care. In one country with universal health care, Väänänen et al. (2005) found that
Finnish women who perceived themselves as providing support for intimate oth-
ers had low rates of work absence and better health than those who did not hold
such self-perceptions. The state of Arkansas instituted a program that provided
cash allowances for the purchase of personal assistance for Medicaid recipients
(Simon-Rusinowitz et al., 2005). The allowances made it possible for families to
hire nonspousal family members to provide assistance with ADLs and IADLs.
This allowed caregivers to work fewer hours or to leave their jobs in order to
provide more care than would otherwise have been possible. Simon-Rusinowitz
et al. (2005) found that families often hired relatives as caregivers in the belief
that they would provide better quality assistance than strangers would. There were
high levels of satisfaction among caregivers and care recipients. It is important
to note that family members would have served as caregivers without the finan-
cial incentive and did not feel coerced into service; this might be a reflection of
the fact that 40% of the family members in the sample were African Americans,
whose culture includes caring for relatives as a highly valued activity. Feld et al.
(2005) found that African American spousal caregiving dyads were 63% more
likely than European American spousal caregiving dyads to include other people
in their caregiving networks.
treatment. Such decisions are never easy, but can be assisted, as in Tina’s situation
with her mother, by preplanning when the care recipient is able to participate in
the decision making. Hansen et al. (2004) described the process for caregivers as
involving strain or relative ease in making decisions. They noted that the sense of
strain or “ease” can change prior to, during, and after the actual decision making,
which can occur over an extended period of time or quickly during a sudden
change in physical status. Such decision making can be further complicated by
media coverage of extreme situations regarding life extension (“Terry Schiavo,”
2005).
Bonanno et al. (2005) described the importance of resilience in the management
of grief during and after caregiving. In some societies, the taboo about the dis-
cussion of death and an unrealistic focus on a (miracle) cure make it particularly
difficult for caregivers to grieve and to share their grief with the care recipient.
Tina and her mother both experienced such difficulty with openly addressing loss
and handling grieving during the caregiving process.
r get regular exercise and try to find something to do that will relax you. Taking
care of yourself helps to keep your mind and body primed to deal with situations
that require strength;
r use meditation or other spiritual practices to build connections and restore
hope.
References
Ackerman, R.J., and Banks, M.E. (2003). Assessment, treatment, and rehabilitation for
interpersonal violence victims: Women sustaining head injuries. In M.E. Banks and
E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with Visible and Invisible Disabilities: Multiple Intersections,
Multiple Issues, Multiple Therapies (pp. 343–363). New York: Haworth Press.
Ackerman, R.J., and Banks, M.E. (2006). Manual for Administration and Interpretation for
the Ackerman-Banks Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Battery C
(A-BNRB, 2006 ed.).
Akron, OH: ABackans Diversified Computer Processing.
Adams, B., Aranda, M.P., Kemp, B., and Takagi, K. (2002). Ethnic and gender differences
in distress among Anglo American, African American, Japanese American, and Mexican
American spousal caregivers of persons with dementia. Journal of Clinical Geropsychol-
ogy, 8, 279–301.
American Psychological Association. (2004). The Road to Resilience. Retrieved October
17, 2005 from http://www.apahelpcenter.org/featuredtopics/feature.php?id=6
Banks, M.E. (2003). Disability in the family: A life-span perspective. Cultural Diversity
and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9, 367–384.
Banks, M.E., and Ackerman, R.J. (1992). Family psychotherapy for brain-injured patients.
In J.C. Chrisler and D. Howard (Eds.), New Directions in Feminist Psychology: Practice,
Theory, and Research (pp. 66–84). New York: Springer.
Banks, M.E., and Ackerman, R.J. (2002). Head and brain injuries experienced by African
American women victims of intimate partner violence. Women & Therapy, 25(3/4),
133–143.
Banks, M.E., and Ackerman, R.J. (2006). Health disparities: Focus on disability. In
K. Hagglund and A. Heinemann (Eds.), Advances in Disability and Rehabilitation Re-
search (pp. 45–70). New York: Springer.
Banks, M.E., Ackerman, R.J., and Corbett, C.A. (1995). Feminist neuropsychology: Issues
for physically challenged women. In J. Chrisler and A. Hemstreet (Eds.), Variations
on a Theme: Diversity and the Psychology of Women (pp. 29–49). Albany, NY: State
University of New York Press.
Banks, M.E., Ackerman, R.J., Yee, B., and West, C. (2005). Stress and trauma in the lives
of women of color. In K. Kendall-Tackett (Ed.), The Handbook of Women, Stress, and
Trauma (pp. 207–228). New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Beach, S.R., Schultz, R., Williamson, G.M., Millar, L., Weiner, M.F., and Lance, C.E.
(2005.) Risk factors for potentially harmful informal caregiver behavior. Journal of the
American Geriatrics Society, 53, 255–261.
Bergeron, L.R. (2005). Abuse of elderly women in family relationships: Another form of
violence against women In K. Kendall-Tackett (Ed.), The Handbook of Women, Stress,
and Trauma (pp. 141–157). New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Bonanno, G.A., Moskowitz, J.T., Papa, A., and Folkman, S. (2005). Resilience to loss in
bereaved spouses, bereaved parents, and bereaved gay men. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 88, 827–843.
160 Rosalie J. Ackerman and Martha E. Banks
Boyd-Franklin, N., and Franklin, A.J. (2000). Boys into Men: Raising our African American
Teenage Sons. New York: Plume.
Brodaty, H., Thomson, C., Thompson, C., and Fine, M. (2005). Why caregivers of people
with dementia and memory loss don’t use services. International Journal of Geriatric
Psychiatry, 20, 537–546.
Browder, S. (2002). “My mind’s made up”: Assumptions and decision-making in accounts
of caregiving women. Journal of Women & Aging, 14(3/4), 77–97.
Bruce, D.G., Paley, G.A., Nichols, P., Roberts, D., Underwood, P.J., and Schaper, F. (2005).
Physical disability contributes to caregiver stress in dementia caregivers. Journals of
Gerontology: Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 60A, 345–349.
Cagney, K.A., and Agree, E.M (2005). Racial differences in formal long-term care: Does
the timing of parenthood play a role? Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological
Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B, S137–S145.
Davis, C.G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Larson, J. (1998). Making sense of loss and benefiting
from the experience: Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 561–574.
Eisdorfer, C., Czaja, S.J., Loewenstein, D.A., Rubert, M.P., Arguelles, S., Mitrani, V.B., and
Szapocznik, J. (2003). The effect of a family therapy and technology-based intervention
on caregiver depression. Gerontologist, 43, 521–531.
Farran, C.J. (1997). Theoretical perspectives concerning positive aspects of caring for el-
derly persons with dementia: Stress/adaptation and existentialism. Gerontologist, 37,
250–258.
Feld, S., Dunkle, R.E., and Schroepfer, T. (2005). When do couples expand their ADL
caregiver network beyond the marital dyad? Marriage and Family Review, 37, 27–44.
Fredriksen, K.I. (1999). Family caregiving responsibilities among lesbians and gay men.
Social Work, 44, 142–155.
Fuller-Thomson, E. (2005). Canadian First Nations grandparents raising grandchildren:
A portrait in resilience. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 60,
331–342.
Fultz, N.H., Jenkins, K.R., Ostbye, T., Taylor, D.H., Kabeto, M.U., and Langa, K.M. (2005).
The impact of own and spouse’s urinary incontinence on depressive symptoms. Social
Science and Medicine, 60, 2537–2548.
Garand, L., Dew, M.A., Eazor, L.R., DeKosky, S.T., and Reynolds, C.F. (2005). Caregiv-
ing burden and psychiatric morbidity in spouses of with mild cognitive impairment.
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20, 512–522.
Gilley, D.W., McCann, J.J., Bienias, J.L., and Evans, D.A. (2005). Caregiver psychological
adjustment and institutionalization of persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Aging
and Health, 17, 172–189.
Hansen, L, Archbold, P.G., and Stewart, B.J. (2004). Role strain and ease in decision-making
to withdraw or withhold life support for elderly relatives. Journal of Nursing Scholarship,
36, 233–238.
Harland, P., and Cuskelly, M. (2000). The responsibilities of adult siblings of adults with dual
sensory impairments. International Journal of Disability, Development, and Education,
47, 293–307.
Hayles, V.R., Jr., Bell, S.R., Evans, W., Floyd, L.J., Monteiro, N., Daniels, I.N., and
Harrell, C.J.P. (2004). African American strengths: A selective contemporary review.
In R.L. Jones (Ed.), Black Psychology (4th ed., pp. 405–425). Hampton, VA: Cobb &
Henry.
8. Caregiving Issues 161
Health Compass: Navigating Research Information on Aging and Health. (2005). Retrieved
October 17, 2005 from http://www.healthcompass.org/
Hirst, M. (2005). Carer distress: A prospective, population-based study. Social Science and
Medicine, 61, 697–708.
Hunt, C.K. (2003). Concepts in caregiver research. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 35,
27–34.
Jones, C., and Shorter-Gooden, K. (2003). Shifting: The Double Lives of Black Women in
America. New York: Harper Collins.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J., and Newton, T. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological
Bulletin, 127, 472–503.
Kwong, M.J., Bartholomew, K., Henderson A.J.Z., and Trinke, S.J. (2003). The intergen-
erational transmission of relationship violence. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 288–
301.
Li, L.W. (2005). From caregiving to bereavement: Trajectories of depressive symptoms
among wife and daughter caregivers. Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological
Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B, P190–P198.
Li, L.W., and Seltzer, M.M. (2005). Relationship quality with parent, daughter role salience,
and self-esteem of daughter caregivers. Marriage and Family Review, 37, 63–82.
Loos, C., and Bowd, A. (1997). Caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease: Some
neglected implications of the experience of personal loss and grief. Death Studies, 21,
501–514.
Lyons, K.S., Zarit, S.H., Sayer, A.G., and Whitlatch, C.J. (2002). Caregiving as a dyadic
process: Perspectives from caregiver and receiver. Journals of Gerontology: Series B:
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, P195–P204.
Mace, N.L., and Rabins, P.V. (2001). The 36-hour day: A Family Guide to Caring for
Persons with Alzheimer’s Disease, Related Dementing Illnesses, and Memory Loss in
Later Life (rev. ed.). New York: Warner Books.
Melberg, K. (2005). Family farm transactions in Norway: Unpaid care across three farm
generations. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 36, 419–441.
Minkler, M. (2005). American Indian/Alaskan Native grandparents raising grandchildren:
Findings from the Census 2000 supplementary survey. Social Work, 50, 131–139.
Minkler, M., and Fuller-Thomson, E. (2005). African American grandparents raising grand-
children: A national study using the census 2000 American community survey. Journals
of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B(2), S82–
S92.
Mona, L.R. (2003). Sexual options for people with disabilities: Using personal assistance
services for sexual expression. In M.E. Banks and E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with Visible
and Invisible Disabilities: Multiple Intersections, Multiple Issues, Multiple Therapies
(pp. 211–221). New York: Haworth.
Nabors, N.A., and Pettee, M.F. (2003). Womanist therapy with African American women
with disabilities. In M.E. Banks and E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with Visible and Invisible
Disabilities: Multiple Intersections, Multiple Issues, Multiple Therapies (pp. 331–341).
New York: Haworth Press.
Riggle, E.D.B., Rostosky, S.S., Prather, R.A., and Hamrin, R. (2005). The execution of
legal documents by sexual minority individuals. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11,
138–163.
Riley, L.D., and Bowen, C. (2005). The sandwich generation: Challenges and coping strate-
gies of multigenerational families. Family Journal, 13, 52–58.
162 Rosalie J. Ackerman and Martha E. Banks
Williamson, G.M., Martin-Cook, K., Weiner, M.F., Svetlik, D.A., Saine, K., Hynan, L.S.,
Dooley, W.K., and Schulz, R. (2005). Caregiver resentment: Explaining why care recip-
ients exhibit problem behavior. Rehabilitation Psychology, 50, 215–223.
Winston, C.A. (2003). African American grandmothers parenting AIDS orphans: Concomi-
tant grief and loss. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 73, 91–100.
Ziemba, R.A., and Lynch-Sauer, J.M. (2005). Preparedness for taking care of elderly parents:
“First, you get ready to cry.” Journal of Women & Aging, 17(1/2), 99–113.
9
Work and Retirement: Challenges and
Opportunities for Women Over 50
Judith A. Sugar
Age puzzles me. I thought it was a quiet time. My seventies were interesting and
fairly serene, but my eighties are passionate. I grow more intense as I age.
Florida Scott-Maxwell (1968), a psychologist, writer, playwright,
and suffragist, who began a career in analytical psychology at age 50.
For most people the word work conjures up an image of someone employed in a
job for which pay is received. Yet, much of the work that women do is unpaid,
and, consequently, it is neither recognized nor defined as work. Ignoring women’s
unpaid labor has detrimental effects on their financial well-being while they are in
the labor force and later for their retirement prospects. The psychological, social,
and emotional toll of ignoring women’s unpaid work is largely unexamined. Fur-
thermore, women face cumulative discrimination in the workplace that begins at
their entry into the labor force, continues in their wages and promotions through-
out their employment, and then affects their financial resources and benefits in
retirement.
Retirement is also an interesting concept for women, because much of the un-
paid work that women do continues after they exit from the paid labor force. In
fact, the concept of retirement for women has been described as problematic for
several reasons, including the fact that women’s paid and unpaid work are often
interconnected (Onyx & Benton, 1996). In addition, many cannot afford to exit
the labor force. Nevertheless, the definition of retirement that would fit with most
Americans’ conceptions early in the new millennium is a period of leisure that fol-
lows cessation of a full-time job. Here, too, there is a dearth of studies of women’s
experiences.
Women live longer than men. It would seem sensible, then, that women should be
a major focus of research within the field of aging. As Crose (1997) has pointed out,
studying older women’s lives has the potential to benefit everyone. Nonetheless,
whereas some have argued that research on older women’s issues is prolific (e.g.,
Adams, 1994), others have said that researchers have mostly ignored older women
(e.g., Hooyman & Rubinstein, 1997). In the first study to assess those competing
claims my colleagues and I (Sugar et al., 2002) surveyed 25 gerontology teaching
164
9. Work and Retirement 165
texts and reference sources published between 1995 and 2001 and more than
1200 journal articles published between 1996 and 2000 in five major gerontology
journals. For text and reference sources, the data collected included the number
of chapters in which a female-related word was present in the chapter title and the
number of pages, according to the index, on which any content on women’s issues
appeared. For journal articles, the data collected included the number of articles
with a female-related word in the title.
The results of the survey were revealing. Most sources had very little content
on women’s issues at all. Only two gerontology textbooks (of 10 surveyed) and
only one gerontology handbook (of 13 surveyed) devoted a chapter to women’s
issues, and these books also had significantly more pages devoted to those issues
than did the textbooks and handbooks with no separate chapters (13.3% versus
4.5%). Thus, it cannot be argued that the latter sources integrated content on
women throughout their pages rather than concentrating it in particular chapters.
Overall, journals had more content on women, although these numbers were also
low; only 241 of 1207 articles (7.5%) focused on women’s issues. Among the five
journals surveyed, articles on older women’s issues were most likely to be pub-
lished in the Journals of Gerontology: Medical Sciences (14.3% of articles) and
least likely to be published in the Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences
(2.5% of articles). There are, of course, excellent sources for scholarship on aging
women, including specialty publications such as the Journal of Women & Aging
and the Handbook on Women and Aging (Coyle, 2001). Our focus, however, was
on general gerontology textbooks and major reference sources, such as handbooks,
because they define the field of gerontology and its central variables (Katz, 2000;
McAuley, 2000), and thus, it is critical that their content reflect subject matter that
is relevant to women, who are the majority of aging adults. Similarly, journals
publish the latest research in a field, and, again, researchers and editors must rec-
ognize the importance of studying the aging process of women. Although content
on women in middle age was not the focus of the study, there is work on midlife
published in gerontology texts and journals. Clearly, the results of the Sugar et al.
survey of recent gerontological literature indicate that Hooyman and Rubinstein
(1997) were correct that researchers have mostly ignored aging women. Thus,
women’s aging remains a fertile and important area for future research and pub-
lication, which can provide a basis for improved policies and practices that result
from it.
Table 9.1. Number of women in the United States age 55 years and over by age and
race/ethnicity.
Years of age
On economic indicators, data are simply not reported separately for 50- to 54-year-
olds. Thus, to provide comparable demographic, employment, and retirement data,
I have selected the age categories that most closely correspond to our population
of interest for this book, namely women 55 years of age or older.
According to the most recent census, there are more than 35 million women
55 years of age or older in the United States (U.S. Census Bureau, 2004). Table 9.1
shows the number of these women by age group and race/ethnicity. Note that there
are an additional nine million women in the United States between the ages of
50 and 54.
Career Issues
Cohort differences play a major role in women’s career experiences. Since the
early years of the women’s movement, the number of women in the workforce has
increased significantly. Although single women and Black women have always
had high rates of participation in the paid workforce, the growth in the numbers of
married women, and especially married women with children, has been dramatic.
In 1978, 44% of married women with children under age 6 were in the workforce,
and by 1998 that percentage had grown to 65% (Costello & Stone, 2001). These
cohort differences in the participation of women in the workforce will continue to
affect the patterns of employment of female baby boomers and future generations
as they age. By their sheer numbers, female workers now have the power to be the
architects of a different, and better, workplace environment.
In 2005, almost 11 million women aged 55 and over were employed in the
workforce (see Table 9.2), and another 380,000 women were unemployed but
looking for work. One pattern of employment evident in Table 9.2 is related to
age; specifically, the older women are, the less likely they are to be employed. In
fact, consistent across race and ethnicity, most women age 55 or more, who were in
the workforce in 2005, were between the ages of 55 and 64. This pattern is due to
several factors. One factor is the cohort of today’s older women, a majority of whom
did not have a history of participating in the paid workforce in their middle-age
9. Work and Retirement 167
Table 9.2. Number of women over 55 employed in the workforce in the United States in
2005 by age and race/ethnicity.
Years of age
years. Thus, the fact that a large proportion of these women are not employed
as they grow older might not be surprising. Other factors that can influence the
employment status of older women include their desire to be employed (or not),
their health, employers’ penchant for hiring them (or not), and the availability of
suitable employment where they live. Even though in 2005 most of the employed
women in our population of interest were younger than 65, we should note that
there were also more than two million women aged 65 or older employed at that
same time.
Paid work has several well-recognized meanings, as a source of: income, per-
sonal identity (especially in the United States), community status, a sense of accom-
plishment, and social interaction (Friedmann & Havighurst, 1954). These mean-
ings, as well as much of the research that has followed from them, have been based
primarily on the work experiences of White men. The importance of paid work in
women’s lives has significantly increased over the last half-century, so that many of
these meanings are now as relevant to women’s lives as they have been to men for
some time. On the other hand, women’s lives, both in and out of the workforce, are
different from men’s and consequently lead to different or other meanings of paid
work. In addition to being a source of income, financial independence from men
has been demonstrated to be an important meaning of labor force participation for
many women. This need for independence is sometimes accompanied by concerns
about the risks of financial dependence on men or the need for insurance against
abandonment or abuse (Altschuler, 2004). The historical lack of opportunities for
women to pursue careers in the past and pressures to marry and be a stay-at-home
parent have resulted in another meaning of paid work for women—lost dreams
and regrets about what might have been. This meaning is often played out by
mid- and late-life women going back to school and encouraging and supporting
their children, especially their daughters, to pursue an education that will lead to a
meaningful career (Altschuler, 2004). With the dramatic shifts in the employment
landscape that have already taken place, and many more to come, the issue of the
meaning of work will continue to be of interest from research, practical, and policy
perspectives.
168 Judith A. Sugar
Table 9.3. Sex differences in annual income of full-time workers in the United States:
1970–2004a .
1970 1980 1990 2004
Women $22,918 $25,167 $28,857 $32,101
Men $38,691 $41,630 $40,612 $41,667
Difference $15,773 $16,463 $11,755 $9,566
Percentage of difference 41 40 29 23
Table 9.4. Number and percentage of full-time workers in the United States by
occupational type and sex in 2004.
Percentage of workers
$60,000 $57,096
$50,000 Women
Men
Difference
$40,560
$40,000
Median annual salary
$34,788
$32,552
$30,000 $26,624
$24,752
$23,556 $23,140
$21,112
$19,448
$20,000 $16,536
$8996
$10,000 $8164
$5304
$2028
$0
Management, Resources, construction Sales, office service Production,
professional transportation
Occupational category
Figure 9.1. Sex differences in median annual salaries of American workers in 2004 by
occupational categories.
transportation jobs to well over $16,000 per year for management and professional
jobs. The two occupational categories where sex differences are the smallest are
the two categories with the lowest salaries.
Thus, although some progress has been made in improving women’s salaries
relative to men’s, women are still at a disadvantage in their paychecks. Partly to
blame is the lack of mentoring women receive when it comes to negotiating their
starting salaries and salary increases. Fortunately, authors are beginning to address
this issue by making explicit both the process of salary negotiations and the price
of being “nice” (e.g., Babcock & Laschever, 2003; Rose & Danner, 1998), which
should help ease this problem for the future.
Over the previous 40 years, women’s perseverance in pursuing education to
fulfill their career interests has led to greater job opportunities for them. Women
now earn more than one-half the bachelor’s and master’s degrees conferred in the
United States (Hussar, 2005). Although they remain the primary holders of tradi-
tional women’s jobs—women are still most of the registered nurses (93%), dental
assistants (98%), elementary school teachers (84%), and librarians (84%)—they
have made substantial gains in some professional occupations, at least doubling
and even quadrupling their numbers as architects, dentists, physicians, financial
managers, and lawyers (Costello & Stone, 2001). They have even made inroads in
some well-paid blue-collar occupations traditionally held by men, such as aircraft
engine mechanics, truck drivers, police and detectives, and industrial truck and trac-
tor equipment operators (Costello & Stone, 2001). Progress in many occupational
arenas, however, has tended to be in entry-level positions rather than senior ones.
170 Judith A. Sugar
Academia is one of those arenas, where women now comprise 45% of Assistant
Professors but are still only 24% of full Professors (Chronicle of Higher Education,
2006).
50,000 49,964
45,000
Women
40,000 36,113 Men
Number of workers (in thousands)
35,000
30,000
25,000
20,000
15,000
9846
8890
10,000 7123
5566
4135 3169
5,000 2343 2504 2011
1680
0
Full-time Part-time Full-time Part-time Full-time Part-time
20–24 yrs 25–54 yrs 55+ yrs
Figure 9.2. Number of full-time and part-time American workers in 2004 by age and sex.
likely to be part-time workers than are men. In fact, during the critical years between
25 and 54 years of age, when employees are in the growth years of their careers for
earnings, promotions, and accumulating retirement savings, only 80% of employed
women are working full-time compared to 95% of men (see Figure 9.2). Of course,
part-time employees are also less likely to have the employment income to invest
and to save for retirement, and they are less likely to be promoted because part-time
jobs seldom lead to promotions. Working part-time, as opposed to full-time, leads
to at least two additional problems for women as they age: The amount they will
contribute toward social security and other retirement vehicles will be less than if
they were full-time workers, and they are less likely than they would be as full-
time workers to receive benefits, especially health care and employers’ pension
contributions.
who took leaves of 3 years or more lost an average of 37% in their salaries. They
were also significantly less likely to receive promotions after their return to work.
The Hewlett and Luce study also debunked the claim by recent media reports
that a new generation of young women is giving up their careers to return to the
traditional role of housewife for the long haul. For many women, of course, taking
a hiatus in the midst of their working lives to devote time to their family is not a
realistic option. Even for professional women who take that hiatus, however, most
(93%) were clear that they wanted to, or planned to, return to their careers.
Retirement Issues
Most of the available demographic data on American retirees can be found through
reports based on social security benefits. These numbers are reasonably good
approximations for male retirees because the vast majority of male employees
in the United States are eligible for Social Security benefits at age 62. However,
these numbers are poor approximations for the current cohort of female retirees
over age 50 because many of those women do not qualify for their own social
security benefits, and the available data do not distinguish between women’s own
benefits from their employment and the benefits to which they are entitled as
widows. Currently, accurate data on the numbers of female retirees over age 50 are
impossible to come by. The best demographic data available, which are on women
not in the workforce, are presented in Table 9.5.
According to the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2005a), more than 24 million
women age 55 or older were not in the workforce in 2005. Similar to the work-
force data reported in Table 9.2, more women were out of the workforce after
age 64 than at younger ages, except for the category of “other” (which includes
Hispanics/Latinas, American Indians, Native Alaskans, Pacific Islanders, “other”
race, and two or more races). A major reason that the numbers do not continue to
increase for Women of Color is that their lifespans are currently shorter than those
of White women.
Table 9.5. Number of women over 55 not in the workforce in the United States in 2005
by age and race/ethnicity.
Years of age
employment in 1975 and wanted to retire in 2015. I used the average sex differ-
ences in salary in 1970, 1980, 1990, and 2004 to interpolate and sum the annual
salary shortfalls for Sarah during her 40 years of full-time paid work. The resulting
total shortfall was a whopping $480,000. This amount does not include the interest
or potential additional income that would accrue with the investment of the “ex-
tra” annual salary that Sarah would have earned if she had been a male employee.
Furthermore, both her social security earnings and private pension (if she has one)
are also affected by her lower income because they are both based on employ-
ment earnings. And, because employers often match employees’ contributions to
private pension plans, Sarah’s employer would also contribute significantly fewer
dollars to her pension savings than would be the case had her salary been higher.
Conservatively, we can estimate that the net effect of Sarah’s lower income would
be that she would end up with at least $1,000,000 less at retirement than her male
counterpart would have. That amount for every retired woman would go a long
way toward ending poverty among older women.
65 and 69 years of age, were participating in the workforce (Purcell, 2000). How-
ever, evidence is accumulating that retirement will be quite different for the baby
boomer cohort. Based on a nationwide survey of 2001 employees between 50 and
70 years of age, AARP (2003a) reported that 63% of preretirees planned to work
at least part-time after the age of 65, and another 5% said that they planned never
to retire. These preretirees’ personal definitions of retirement in part reflect the
new financial realities for this and future cohorts of retirees. Their definitions also
reflect different attitudes toward work. Table 9.6 lists the proportion of survey
respondents who indicated that specific aspects of retirement were “very much” or
“somewhat” part of their personal definition of retirement. Many of their choices
are related to postretirement work, including “slowing down and working fewer
hours,” “having to do some kind of work to help pay bills,” and “a chance to
leave your main career to try a different type of work.” Nevertheless, middle-aged
Americans are looking forward to many aspects of retirement enjoyed by current
retirees, including spending more time with family and friends, relaxing, having
more fun, and “doing things they have never had time for.”
Similarly, the reasons given by Americans age 50 and over for working in
retirement are diverse. When they are forced to choose only one, financial concerns
are paramount, but most Americans have other reasons, too, which include a desire
to be mentally and physically active, generativity, pleasure, and social interaction
(AARP, 2005a; see Table 9.7).
A vast array of volunteer and recreational choices already exist, and many more
can be expected as the marketplace more fully recognizes the potential for re-
tirees to be engaged in their communities and to seek new experiences. In 2002
Americans age 55 and older contributed more than $160 billion to society through
their volunteer and caregiving activities (Johnson & Schaner, 2005). Furthermore,
9. Work and Retirement 177
most women have not been employed full-time for long enough to be said to retire
or to qualify for their own social security benefits. Furthermore, researchers have
not wanted to deal with the complexity of women’s multiple entries and exits from
the workforce and their attendant implications for retirement.
In addition to focusing on men, retirement research has also focused heavily on
the financial issues of leaving the full-time workforce—financial planning for re-
tirement, methods of accumulating wealth, and managing assets after retirement—
and has largely ignored the social, emotional, and psychological issues. Employ-
ers, too, when they offer seminars or workshops on retirement for their employees,
typically devote the time to the financial issues.
There is a substantial body of literature on women’s careers and work experi-
ences, but there are still too few data in this arena, and with the rapidly changing
work environment much of what we know needs to be reconceptualized or com-
pletely overhauled (Moen & Roehling, 2005). We also have much to learn about
women’s retirement, notwithstanding more recent publications (e.g., Hall, 2002;
Mellor & Rehr, 2005; Riggs, 2004). More research is needed on issues related to
retirement for women. Attention should be paid to the diversity among women,
including race/ethnicity, sexuality, ability status, and partner and parental status.
Even basic demographic data are needed. Topics that are underexplored, or not
explored at all, include decision-making about retirement, activities and work after
retirement; and psychological effects of work, work past the “traditional” age of re-
tirement, and retirement, including how they impact personal identity, self-esteem,
and social well-being.
Women who pioneered working full-time are now pioneering the stage of life
beyond full-time employment. There are few role models. Many women are dis-
appointed and frustrated that, after so many years of fighting for equal rights in the
workplace, there could still be such large gaps in wages and benefits, as well as em-
ployment and retirement policies that still adversely affect women. The wage gaps
are bad enough in and of themselves, but they also have long-term and significant
consequences for women’s quality of life as they age.
Policy issues to be addressed include current and proposed provisions of so-
cial security and amendments to the Social Security Act to overcome its inherent
discrimination against women. Changes to social security should include elimi-
nating the possibility that partners can opt out of survivor coverage and allowing
adjustments for interruptions in work histories that are a consequence of childcare
and parentcare. With lower salaries throughout their careers, women are signifi-
cantly more likely than are men to become impoverished as they age, so improving
salaries for women should also be a priority. Such changes, of course, will have
positive impacts not just for women, but for everyone.
one in which education, work, and leisure are intermingled. Thus, for example, a
period of education could be followed by work, then more education, then some
leisure, then more work, then leisure again, etc.
Linear life pattern
This idea, of course, is not new to American women, and, in fact, has been
quite common for many of them. What is new, though, is to use the cyclic pattern
as a model for education, work, and leisure across the lifespan and to develop
policies and practices that would support it. In addition, it would advantageous to
expand and create new opportunities for work other than full-time “permanent”
jobs. Limited time, part-time, and project-oriented jobs that are meaningful and
fulfilling would be desirable to many people. Women of all ages would benefit
from these new choices if they were supported in the public and private spheres
and if they improved the quality of life in work and retirement.
We need to rethink the work environment and develop new policies and practices
to make room for the millions of middle-aged women and men who do not want
to exit completely and permanently from the workforce when they reach the age
where they could begin to collect social security, private pensions, or both. To
provide the opportunities most future older adults desire will require reshaping
jobs as well as the workplace to accommodate part-time employees of all kinds—
those who would like to work full-time for a few days a week, those who would like
to work for a few hours every weekday, perhaps sharing a job, those who would
like to work on time-limited projects perhaps full-time for a few months each
year, and so forth. With unprecedented numbers of women poised for retirement,
the future for “midcourse” women (Moen, 2003) with unprecedented numbers of
healthy years of life before them is wide open.
Aging is not ‘lost youth’ but a new stage of opportunity and strength.
Betty Friedan (1994)
References
AARP. (2002). Staying Ahead of the Curve: The AARP Work and Career Study. Washington,
DC: Author.
AARP. (2003a). Staying Ahead of the Curve 2003: The AARP Working in Retirement Study.
Washington, DC: Author.
180 Judith A. Sugar
AARP. (2003b). Boomers at Midlife. The AARP Life Stage Study, Wave 2. Washington, DC:
Author.
AARP. (2005a, March). Attitudes of Individuals 50 and Older Toward Phased Retirement.
Online poll of 2,167 workers over 50. Retrieved March, 2006 from http://www.aarp.org.
AARP. (2005b, May 23). Monster and AARP Announce Alliance to Help 50+ Workers.
Retrieved February, 2006 from http://www.aarp.org.
Adams, R.G. (1994). Older men’s friendship patterns. In E.H. Thompson, Jr. (Ed.), Older
Men’s Lives (pp. 159–177). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Altschuler, J. (2004). Beyond money and survival: The meaning of paid work among older
women. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 58, 223–239.
American Benefits Council. (2004). Pensions at the Precipice: The Multiple Threats Facing
Our Nation’s Defined Benefit Pension System. Washington, DC: Author. Available at
http://www.americanbenefitscouncil.org.
Babcock, L., and Laschever, S. (2003). Women Don’t Ask: Negotiation and the Gender
Divide. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Brontë, L. (1995). The Longevity Factor: The New Reality of Long Careers and how it Can
Lead to Richer Lives. New York: HarperCollins.
Chronicle of Higher Education. (2006, August 26). Almanac issue 2005–6.
Costello, C.B., and Stone, A.J. (Eds.). (2001). The American Woman 2001–2002: Getting
to the Top. New York: Norton.
Coyle, J.M. (Ed.). (2001). Handbook on Women and Aging. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Crose, R. (1997). Why Women Live Longer than Men . . . and What Men Can Learn from
Them. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Dychtwald, K. (1999). Age Power: How the 21st Century Will Be Ruled by the New Old.
New York: Penguin Putnam.
Friedan, B. (1994, March 20). Quoted in Parade Magazine. Retrieved May 2006 from
http://womenshistory.about.com/od/quotes/a/betty friedan.htm.
Friedmann, E., and Havighurst, R.I. (1954). The Meaning of Work and Retirement. Chicago,
IL: University of Chicago Press.
Hall, P. (2002). The Bonus Years: Women and Retirement. New York: Miranda Press.
Hewlett, S.A., and Luce, C.B. (2005, March). Off-ramps and on-ramps. Keeping talented
women on the road to success. Harvard Business Review, pp. 43–54.
Hooyman, N.R., and Rubinstein, R.L. (1997). Is aging more problematic for women than
men? In A.E. Scharlach and L.W. Kaye (Eds.), Controversial Issues in Aging (pp. 125–
135). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Hussar, W.J. (2005). Projections of Education Statistics to 2014 (NCES 2005-074). U.S.
Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC:
U.S. Government Printing Office. Available through http://nces.ed.gov.
Johnson, R.W., and Schaner, S.G. (2005, September). Value of unpaid activities by older
Americans tops $160 billion per year. Perspectives on Productive Aging, Number 4.
Washington, DC: Urban Institute. Available at http://www.urban.org.
Katz, S. (2000). Reflections on the gerontological handbook. In T.R. Cole, R. Kastenbaum,
and R.E. Ray (Eds.), Handbook for the Humanities and Aging (2nd ed., pp. 405–418).
New York: Springer.
McAuley, W.J. (2000). Defining the field of gerontology through introductory texts. Geron-
tologist, 40, 242–246.
Mellor, M.J., and Rehr, H. (Eds.). (2005). Baby Boomers. Can My Eighties Be Like my
Fifties? New York: Springer.
9. Work and Retirement 181
Moen, P. (2003). Midcourse. Reconfiguring careers and community service for a new life
stage. Contemporary Gerontology, 9, 87–94.
Moen, P., and Roehling, P. (2005). The Career Mystique: Cracks in the American Dream.
Lanham, MD: Rowan & Littlefield.
Onyx, J., and Benton, P. (1996). Retirement: A problematic concept for older women.
Journal of Women and Aging, 8, 19–35.
Purcell, P.J. (2000, October). Older workers: Employment and retirement trends. Monthly
Labor Review, pp. 19–30.
Riggs, K.E. (2004). Granny@work: Aging and New Technology on the Job in America.
New York: Routledge.
Robertson, A. (2000). “I saw the handwriting on the wall”: Shades of meaning in reasons
for early retirement. Journal of Aging Studies, 14, 63–79.
Roosevelt, F.D. (1935, August 14). Presidential Statement on Signing the Social Security
Act. Available at www.ssa.gov/history/fdrstmts.html.
Rose, S., and Danner, M.J. (1998). Money matters: The art of negotiation for women faculty.
In L.H. Collins, J.C. Chrisler, and K. Quina (Eds.), Arming Athena: Career Strategies
for Women in Academe (pp. 157–186). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Scott-Maxwell, F. (1968). The Measure of My Days. New York: Knopf.
Stone, R., Cafferata, G.L., and Sangl, J. (1987). Caregivers of the frail elderly: A national
profile. Gerontologist, 27, 616–626.
Sugar, J.A., Anstee, J.L.K., Desrochers, S., and Jambor, E.E. (2002). Gender biases in geron-
tological education: The status of older women. Gerontology & Geriatrics Education,
22, 43–53.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2005a). Employment Status of the Civilian Noninsti-
tutional Population by Age, Sex, and Race. [Household data Table 3]. Available at
http://www.bls.gov/cps.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2005b). Women in the Labor Force: A Databook. [Table
18. Median usual weekly earnings of full-time wage and salary workers by detailed
occupation and sex, 2004 annual averages.] Available at http://www.bls.gov/cps/wlf-
databook2005.htm.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). Current Population Survey. Annual Social and Economic
Supplement. Available at http://factfinder.census.gov.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2004). Current Population Survey. Annual Social and Economic
Supplement. Available at http://factfinder.census.gov.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2005). Annual Estimates of the Population by Age and Sex for the
United States. Available at http://factfinder.census.gov.
10
Empowerment: A Prime Time for
Women Over 50
Florence L. Denmark and Maria D. Klara
In preparing this chapter, the first author noted: “As someone who achieved a
variety of offices and received many awards after age 50, I felt very much in
control of my own life, but in no way felt that I was able to or wanted to control
others.” Thus, although it is important to understand that empowerment and power
are tightly intertwined, these two concepts are different.
Power is often related to our ability to make others do what we want, regardless
of their own wishes or interests. For most people, the word “power” typically
brings to mind thoughts concerning control and domination (Page & Czuba, 1999).
Traditional social science emphasizes power as influence and control, often treating
power as a commodity or structure separate from human action. In this way, power
can be viewed as unchanging or unchangeable, and available only to a select few
(Page & Czuba, 1999).
Alternatively, empowerment refers to individuals gaining command over their
own destinies (American Heritage Dictionary, 2000) as well as helping others
to attain this control as well. Something that is empowering makes individuals
feel more confident that they are in control of their own lives. Empowerment
involves learning to redefine who we are and what we can do, to speak in our own
voice, and to change the way we perceive our relationships to institutionalized
power (Chamberlin, 1997). In general, an individual or even a group moves from a
state of relative powerlessness to power through the empowerment process (Pillai,
1995). This process encompasses attitudes, values, and beliefs about the self,
especially beliefs about the ability to exert control over one’s destiny (Chadiha
et al., 2004).
Empowering others is the process of supporting people to construct new mean-
ings and use their freedom to choose new ways to respond to the world, often to
the benefit of others. Empowering others involves providing individuals with the
appropriate tools and resources to enhance their self-confidence and self-esteem,
to develop leadership skills, and to strive for personal and professional success.
Empowering others entails making a systematic and sustained effort to provide
others with more information, knowledge, support, and opportunities to use their
power for mutual benefit.
182
10. Empowerment 183
and men might not achieve their identities at the same point in development.
Rather, Erikson proposed that women might not achieve a sense of identity until
later in life, in the context of intimate relationships. Identity development seems to
have important implications for well-being in midlife. For instance, research has
indicated that identity development is positively related to midlife self-esteem and
life satisfaction in women (Stewart et al., 2001).
Erikson’s theory also posits that midlife adults face the conflict of “gener-
ativity versus stagnation” and acquire the ego strength (i.e., virtue) of care in
its resolution. Erikson suggested that women, like men, experience a midlife
generativity crisis, which results to some extent from age-related social pressures
to make a contribution to the next generation. This crisis results in a capacity and
commitment to care for ideas, cultural products, institutions, values, and other
people (Stewart et al., 2001).
Generativity has only recently been widely discussed. It refers not only to bear-
ing and nurturing children but also includes creativity (i.e., the production of new
works and things) and the generation of new ideas. Thus, generativity includes
both creating and caring (De St. Aubin et al., 2004). Conventional gender pat-
terns of generativity have changed due to substantial changes in gender roles over
time, including the increased vocational aspirations of women and the increased
domestic responsibilities of men. Feminists have challenged traditional definitions
of generativity as a result of these new conceptions of adult development (De St.
Aubin et al., 2004).
Researchers have found a number of differences between middle-aged women
and men. The most profound difference in attitude between men and women at
middle age is that women are twice as likely as men to be hopeful about the future.
More and more women see midlife not as a crisis, but rather as a challenge, or,
on a more positive note, as an opportunity to better themselves. They also have a
more powerful urge to help others or to make a contribution to some larger good.
Women are typically more willing than men at midlife to consider trying something
completely new in a search for greater flexibility, challenge, or satisfaction. They
are more likely to be optimistic, despite obstacles due to aging. Optimism in
middle-aged women takes many forms. Women at midlife believe that they will
stay healthy longer than women of previous generations. They are joining gyms
at twice the rate of their male peers (Gibbs, 2005). In addition, full-time college
enrollment by older women rose to 31% in the past decade (Gibbs, 2005). The
National Center on Women & Aging at Brandeis University (as cited in Gibbs,
2005) found that women age 50 and older said that they feel happier about getting
older than they had anticipated. Thus, women at midlife feel more confident about
their coping skills, which enhances their sense of mastery of life.
that are present in the culture about this time in women’s lives. A recent article in
Time magazine (Gibbs, 2005) focused on how midlife women in this generation are
taking advantage of this critical life period in order to reinvent themselves. Rather
than falling prey to the obstacles of midlife, women are figuring out how to turn
challenges into opportunities. As a result of higher incomes, better education, and
considerable experience at managing multiple roles, women may actually realize
that there has never been a better time than now to have a “midlife crisis.” Because
such a large cluster of women are experiencing middle age (i.e., there are roughly
43 million American women ages 40 to 60), some rules may have to be rewritten
and boundaries shifted to accommodate them. The word “crisis” may not apply to
midlife for women in this generation because they are creating a new model for
what midlife might look like.
Gibbs (2005) found that women, more specifically, middle and upper middle
class and professional women, from their 50s on, often experience the most fruitful
and satisfying period of life. Many women, at this time of their lives, realize skills
and strengths that were never before tapped or exercised. Others experience this
period as one of tremendous growth, and they frequently redefine their personali-
ties. In some cases, occupational and social pursuits are picked up where they had
been left off for years due to marriage and familial responsibilities. Women have
a great deal to look forward to during this exciting period in their lives such as
travel, work, sociability, and study. The midlife years are a time to try new things,
to go to places never visited before, and to do things not done earlier. Books and
magazine articles urge women at about age 50 to take the time to think seriously
about what is most important to them and what they want to accomplish with the
rest of their lives.
Staying involved in professional organizations and volunteering are ways for
older women to have an influence and a voice. Of course, one probably has the most
impact with continuing professional involvement. The vast majority of women who
are chairs of academic departments, high level executives, and owners of small
businesses are also in this age bracket.
This is also a wonderful time of opportunity and freedom, and many women
exert their influence politically. Currently, the number of midlife women in the
population is larger than ever, which means that there is an increasingly important
role for older women in public life. Many older women activists consider feminism
broadly in this context and perceive their activities as a means of nurturing others.
Women in their 50s and beyond are in perfect positions to make great impacts on
local, national, and even worldwide levels. For example, nearly all of the women
in the United States Congress are at least 50 years old (Congressional Research
Service, 2004).
Times are changing for women in general, and “the daughters of today’s women”
are more likely than their mothers to be well-educated, to have explored several
personal options, and to have had long years of employment experiences. There-
fore, they are more likely, when grey hair and wrinkles appear, to resist being
pushed aside by younger people (Lott, 1987). Currently, older active women in
the United States and worldwide can and do have an impact on the world around
them, at least in part by virtue of their age. Women gain personal power, prestige,
188 Florence L. Denmark and Maria D. Klara
and influence as they grow older. The perceived balance of interpersonal power in
the latter half of women’s lives increases in favor of older women. This increase is
also apparent in terms of equality between men and women during the later stages
of a woman’s development. It is interesting that it is the social status of a woman
that impacts on her power, and it is those women who have achieved a higher social
status who can expect to have a greater amount of perceived power in their middle
and later years (Todd et al., 1990). Conditions for gifted women during their 50s
and beyond are particularly positive.
Some women say that life “begins” at 50, and it is not uncommon for women
at midlife and even later to discover new talents and interests and employ them
successfully (Allington & Troll, 1984). One study (Mitchell & Helson, 1990)
indicated that women in their early 50s gave higher satisfaction ratings to their
lives than did either older or younger women. An increasing number of women
are beginning or returning to college or work in middle or older adulthood after
years of devoting their lives exclusively to their families. These women are referred
to as “reentry” women. Despite the obstacles these women face, reentry women
tend to do very well in their pursuits. In college, they work hard and participate
actively in their education. Research (De Groot, 1980) shows that women who
attend college at an older age are more assertive and, as such, expect and receive
more spousal support, which increases the likelihood of their success. Middle-
aged women also function effectively in the workplace and experience great job
satisfaction. This is particularly true for those in “careers” as opposed to those with
“jobs.” In one study (Coleman & Antonucci, 1983) older women in the workforce
reported experiencing greater psychological well-being, self-esteem, and health
than did their homemaker counterparts. These findings attest to the possibilities
and opportunities that arise during midlife for women. The population of reentry
women is likely to increase as life spans grow longer and good health care continues
longer in life.
All of these factors combine to make midlife and beyond a fruitful and pro-
ductive time in women’s lives, one that breeds empowerment. Women perceive
themselves as intelligent, assertive, and determined in middle age, and they are
serious about the task of empowering themselves and others (Babladelis, 1999).
Women in midlife are geared through family, economic, and social factors to be
able to focus on their own needs, and they often have the means to ascertain them
successfully.
The 50s onward appears to be almost a golden age for certain women. Stud-
ies (e.g., Mitchell & Helson, 1990) have shown that women feel secure, enjoy
good health, and experience a fairly autonomous and androgynous period of time.
College educated women in their early 50s who were polled as to their quality
of life and current life satisfaction rated their lives as “first-rate” (Mitchell &
Helson, 1990). Good health and increased income during these years also con-
tribute to feelings of security, greater self-confidence, involvement, and breadth of
personality by women who are older. Considering the 50s onward as the empow-
ering prime of life for women is an appropriate classification (Mitchell & Helson,
1990).
10. Empowerment 189
self-definition and self-determination as the power to name and decide one’s own
destiny. Community, spirituality, and speaking out against oppression have also
been used as tools and methods through which to attain empowerment.
Especially in working contexts, African American women must “negotiate and
reconcile the contradictions separating internally defined images of self as African
American women whose identities are (re)produced through patriarchal systems
of domination and subordination” (Parker, 2003, p. 262). It is this process of
negotiating identities that has given African American women unique ways of
empowering themselves. They actively strive to use the strategies mentioned above
to work against the stereotypes that patriarchal society has developed.
The empowerment model is a model of social change and therapeutic interven-
tion that focuses on promoting assets, strengths, and resilience in people (Querimit
& Conner, 2003). This has important implications for Women of Color in midlife
and beyond. Quermit and Conner (2003) studied this model in youth of Color but
many comparisons can be made to older Women of Color as well. Women over
50 still must overcome social inequality, but now they have achieved more skills
and assets on which to rely. They can depend upon these developed strengths in
order to empower themselves, despite the social inequalities and oppression in
their lives. In this way, age becomes a mediating factor and an aid in empowering
older Women of Color.
The following is a case example of an African woman who used many of the
strategies noted above to empower herself. Ellen Johnson-Sirleaf, President of
Liberia, is a prime example of a woman over 50 who promotes empowerment
for women. Elected by a stunning 60% of the vote in November 2005 to become
the first female elected leader in Africa’s history, Johnson-Sirleaf now has the
responsibility of mending her broken country. A Harvard graduate in economics,
Johnson-Sirleaf is currently tackling the economic and social problems in her
country. Born to two Liberian parents who were adopted by American Liberians
living in the United States, she was instilled with a sense of duty and honesty.
On the way, however, she has not forgotten issues important to the women of
her country. She is working to raise public consciousness about issues of wife
abuse, rape, and women’s inheritance of property. A rape victim herself, she,
along with a group of lawyers, has worked to enact laws that ensure that rapists
will be penalized with stricter sentences. According to Hammer (2006), Johnson-
Sirleaf is one of the growing number of women in Africa who are climbing to
power and working toward women’s empowerment initiatives. African women are
“breaking the male stranglehold on national legislation, cabinets, courts, and other
government institutions. They’re making laws, changing attitudes, inspiring other
women to follow them” (Hammer, 2006, p. 32). In other words, Johnson-Sirleaf
embodies the concept of the empowerment of women.
Women immigrants, and women of all ethnicities born into families below the
poverty line, report that monetary disadvantage has led to part of the disempower-
ment that they experience (Darlington & Mulvaney, 2003). Darlington and Mul-
vaney (2003) distributed a questionnaire to 25 women who identified themselves
as Hispanic, Cuban American, Spanish, White Spanish, White Cuban American,
and “multi.” Questions were aimed at determining these women’s view of power
and empowerment in the United States. Some women’s statements make clear the
connection between class and empowerment. As a response to what they thought
power meant in the United States today, one women stated that power in American
society is “to be White and to have money” (all quotes p. 131). Other responses
included that power is “wealth, it seems only rich people are powerful,” “I think
power is related to wealth; it’s also related to how much you’ve got in terms of
what you drive and what you wear,” and “Power is viewed as a tool for control.”
In terms of power definitions in Latin American countries, these women said that
power was also related to control. One woman said that power is “To be of high
class, have studied in good private schools, and to come from a wealthy family.”
Andrews et al. (2003) studied the conceptualization of empowerment by women
in poverty and lower socioeconomic classes, specifically from the southern United
States. These women had to endure geographic isolation, unemployment, low
educational attainment, and limited access to services. Participants’ comments
about a fictitious woman named Angela and her life story were audiotaped for
analysis to capture the women’s beliefs about what methods are possible for Angela
to use to break free from the difficult situation in which she finds herself (Angela
is poor with few resources). The women were asked to comment about Angela’s
life and about her strengths and limitations. Empowerment was woven into their
answers, and most women thought that Angela needed to take control of her life,
despite the challenges with which she is confronted due to her class status. Their
conceptions of empowerment were in strong relation to notions of class and the
need to break free from the trouble that low socioeconomic status creates (i.e., to
empower oneself). There were themes in their responses of optimism, persistence,
ability to let go, ability to seek and accept help, and spirituality. Their responses
indicated that empowering attributes were closely related to interpersonal and
environmental factors and were not in isolation to what was experienced day to
day.
The results of that study make it clear that class is a large consideration in
women’s conceptions of who is empowered and who is not. In both the United
States and in immigrants’ countries of origin, the upper classes have more power,
and they are “entitled” to greater advantages that the society has to offer. In the
United States and elsewhere, class is, therefore, also related to empowerment,
in that lower classes must strive harder to empower themselves in a society that
is structurally skewed against them. Becker et al. (2004) also sought to opera-
tionalize individual notions of empowerment in a sample of advocates working
with low-income mothers. Overall, these authors used the term empowerment out-
right as “setting goals, gathering information, defining needs, and making and
implementing decisions” (p. 332). All of these were ways in which the women
192 Florence L. Denmark and Maria D. Klara
could empower themselves and, in doing this, help to alleviate the pressures put
on them by their class status.
that preserve peace, heal the wounded environment, and respect the rights of all
individuals to share in determining policy. Furthermore, they seek to unite young,
old, women, and men of all ethnic, racial, and economic backgrounds for the study
and promotion of social justice. They reason that governments exist in order to
facilitate the achievement of social justice for all (Gray Panthers, 2006).
sex and gender; (3) combating the oppression of Women of Color; (4) develop-
ing a feminist model of psychotherapy; (5) achieving equality for women within
the profession of psychology and allied disciplines; (6) promoting unity among
women of all races, ages, social classes, sexual orientations, physical abilities,
and religions; (7) sensitizing the public and the profession to the psychological,
social, political, and economic problems of women; (8) helping women to create
individual sexual identities; and (9) encouraging research on issues of concern to
Women of Color.
lasting world peace. The UN and its agencies, including the World Bank and the
UN Development Programme, are the premier vehicles for furthering development
in poorer countries.
The millennium development goals (MDGs,), issued by the UN Secretary Gen-
eral Kofi Annan in 2001, are a “roadmap” for implementing the Millennium Dec-
laration, which was presented at the September 2000 UN Millennium Summit. The
Millennium Declaration reflects widespread international recognition that the em-
powerment of women and the achievement of gender equality are issues of human
rights and social justice. Equality and women’s empowerment are fundamental
to the achievement of all of the MDGs, whether it is the eradication of poverty,
protection of the environment, or access to healthcare. Attempts to meet the MDGs
without integrating gender equality would both raise the cost and diminish the suc-
cess. Because the MDGs are mutually reinforcing, success in attaining the goals
will have positive effects on gender equality, just as advancement toward gender
equality in any one domain will help to promote each of the other goals (Women’s
Environment & Development Organization, 2004).
By the year 2015, all 191 UN Member States have pledged to meet the
MDG goals. The eight MDGs include: (1) eradicate extreme poverty and hunger;
(2) achieve universal primary education; (3) promote gender equality and em-
power women; (4) reduce child mortality; (5) improve maternal healthcare; (6)
combat HIV/AIDS, malaria, and other diseases; (7) ensure environmental sustain-
ability; and (8) develop a global partnership development (Millennium Project,
2006).
Goal #3 makes clear the importance of activating a comprehensive, rather than
a piecemeal, program to advance gender equality. Accordingly, Goal #3 encom-
passes gender equality in all aspects of women’s lives—gender-based violence,
cultural stereotypes, trafficking and prostitution, armed conflict, political life, laws
and legal status, government structures, the media, education, employment, health
care, family planning, poverty, the environment, rural life, and marriage and family
relations. The full range of measures that must be taken to achieve gender equality
and women’s empowerment have already been comprehensively mapped out in
the Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Women
(CEDAW) and the Beijing Platform for Action, as well as in major provisions of
other international instruments and conference documents (UNIFEM, 2004).
In addition to the UN’s international policy programs and organizations, the
NGOs within the UN promote social awareness and activism of various issues.
Two specific NGOs, the committee on the status of women and the committee on
ageing, are active in promoting the empowerment of women. The committee on
the status of women, specifically the sub-committee of older women (SCOW), is
a group of women, primarily over 50, who promote issues and education of issues
that are relevant to women in this age group. The NGO committee on the status of
women (NGO CSW) was founded after the UN general assembly proclaimed 1975
as the International Women’s Year and recommended that international action be
intensified to: promote equality between men and women; ensure full integration of
women in the total development effort; and recognize the importance of women’s
198 Florence L. Denmark and Maria D. Klara
understanding others (Colwill & Townsend, 1999). Women are more exploratory
and less instrumental in their communication than men are. They are more likely
to communicate issues that are judged unnecessary by men, but, in doing so, they
can impart a broader understanding to others (Colwill & Townsend, 1999).
Merely being recognized as a leader does not make one either powerful or em-
powering. For example, Queen Elizabeth II of the United Kingdom and Northern
Ireland, Queen Margrethe II of Denmark, and Queen Beatrix Wilhelmina Armgard
of the Netherlands are primarily figureheads. Other leaders are powerful or have
been powerful but not empowering, such as Margaret Thatcher, Indira Gandhi, and
Condoleezza Rice. Women leaders who have worked to empower other women
include Susan B. Anthony, Betty Freidan, Gloria Steinem, Hillary Rodham Clin-
ton, Oprah Winfrey, and Donna Shalala, President of the University of Miami and
former Secretary of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Although
Hillary Clinton became known as the First Lady of the United States, it was not
until she was into her 50s that she was elected a U.S. senator in her own right.
Empowerment is demonstrated not only in the political sphere but in others as
well. Take the American painter, Grandma Moses, as she is commonly called.
Anna May Robertson Moses was the third of 10 children, and she was encouraged
as a child by her father to paint and draw. She worked on a neighboring farm from
the age of 12 until her marriage to Thomas Salmon Moses in 1887. While living on
the farm that the couple owned and worked together, Grandma Moses decorated
certain objects in her home with painted scenes, but it was only in her 70s, with no
prior artistic training or formal classes that she started to paint in oils. Her paintings
are on display in museums worldwide and have brought her to the forefront as an
example of what can be accomplished by women over 50 (ArtCyclopedia, 2005).
According to Pillai (1995), empowerment is an active multidimensional process
that enables women to realize their full identity and power in all spheres of life.
Empowerment for women involves having a say and being listened to as well
as being able to create from a woman’s perspective. Empowerment insists that
women be appreciated and acknowledged for who they are and what they do.
Once recognized, they are more effective in their future endeavors. They develop
a capacity to face the social facts of their actual situation boldly. They are able to
come to a better understanding of themselves and their circumstances once they
examine the truth of their lives. An empowered woman becomes free of social,
cultural, and, perhaps most important, psychological barriers (Pillai, 1995).
Just as many individuals are not entirely self-actualized according to Maslow’s
theory, many individuals also are not fully empowered (Maslow, 1943). There are
various steps that women can take in order to become more empowered. Women
can
Conclusions
Empowerment is an issue that is relevant to women of all socioeconomic classes,
races, and cultures. It is particularly pertinent in any discussion of women at
midlife, considering that middle class women of this age group frequently have
the necessary capabilities needed to make positive changes in their lives and to turn
the balance of power in their favor. There are various steps to empowerment that
these women have used. Many have been successful at gaining control of their lives,
enhancing their confidence and self-esteem, gaining awareness of their situation,
and developing an assertive belief system. However, some women, particularly
those of lower socioeconomic status or ethnic minorities who are discriminated
against, find themselves in a particularly challenging situation when it comes
to empowerment. Societal structures and discrimination make it exponentially
difficult, but not necessarily impossible, to follow these steps to empowerment for
these women.
One critical component to empowerment is moving beyond ourselves toward
the empowerment of others. Empowerment must be viewed not only as personal
empowerment, but also as the empowerment of other women. Women must work
toward the empowerment of all women, and one way to support this is to cooperate
with other women toward a common good. One way that this has been accom-
plished is through the establishment of groups and organizations that have been
constructed to help women empower themselves, such as NOW, AWP, and the
Gray Panthers.
In addition, in the political sphere, world policy is finally beginning to sup-
port the empowerment of women by encouraging policies that empower women
and give them the much needed resources to help fight poverty, discrimination,
10. Empowerment 201
and inequality. Women in midlife can be examples of the productive, able, and
resourceful qualities that most women possess. With increased support from orga-
nizations and government, as well as with the increased cooperative participation
of women of all ages, women’s empowerment will continue to increase.
Overall, women over 50 are in a prime period in their lives where they can
exemplify and expand empowerment for themselves and other women. Now that
the children have grown up and left the house, women have more time to dedicate
to efforts to enhance their lives and their societies. They also have more financial
resources at their disposal and the means to set goals and attain them. This includes
an increased sense of confidence, which comes with age and experience. More life
experience gives women key assets to learn new strategies and the personal ability
to put these strategies into effect. Women over 50 have many key elements in place
to continue to promote the empowerment of themselves as well as that of other
women of all ages.
Acknowledgment. We thank Lani Sherman for her assistance with the question-
naire distribution.
References
Allington, D.E., and Troll, L.E. (1984). Social change and equality: The roles of women and
economics. In G. Baruch and J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Women in Midlife (pp. 181–202).
New York: Plenum.
American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language (4th ed.). (2000) Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
American Psychological Association. (2005). Division 35—Society for the Psychology of
Women. Retrieved October 4, 2005, from http://www.apa.org/about/ division/div35 .html.
Andrews, A.B., Guadalupe, J.L., and Bolden, E. (2003). Faith, hope and mutual support:
Paths to empowerment as perceived by women in poverty. Journal of Social Work Re-
search and Evaluation, 4, 5–18.
ArtCycylopedia (2005). Grandma Moses. Retrieved from http://www.artcyclopedia.com/
artists/moses grandma.html.
Association for Women in Psychology. (2006). The Association for Women in Psychology.
Retrieved from www.awpsych.org.
Babladelis, G. (1999). Autonomy in the middle years. In M.B. Nadien and F.L. Denmark
(Eds.), Females and Autonomy—A Lifespan Perspective (pp. 101–129). Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.
Becker, J., Kovach, A.C., and Gronseth, D.L. (2004). Individual empowerment: How com-
munity health workers operationalize self-determination, self-sufficiency, and decision-
making abilities of low income mothers. Journal of Community Psychology, 32, 327–342.
Browne, C.V. (1995). Empowerment in social work practice with older women. Social
Work, 40, 358–364.
Chadiha, L.A., Adams, P., Biegel, D.E., Auslander, W., and Gutierrez, L. (2004). Empow-
ering African American women informal caregivers: A literature synthesis and practice
strategies. Social Work, 49(1), 97–109.
Chamberlin, J. (1997). A working definition of empowerment. Psychiatric Rehabilitation
Journal, 20, 43–46.
202 Florence L. Denmark and Maria D. Klara
Coleman, L.M., and Antonucci, T.C. (1983). Impact of work on women at midlife. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 19, 290–294.
Colwill, J. and Townsend, J. (1999). Women, leadership and information technology.
Journal of Management Development, 18, 207–213.
Congressional Research Service. (2004). Membership of the 109th Congress: A Pro-
file. Retrieved April 5, 2006 from www.er.doe.gov/bes/109th Congress 1st Session
CRS 20DEC04.pdf.
De St. Aubin, E., McAdams, D.P., and Kim, T. (2004). The Generative Society: Caring for
Future Generations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Darlington, P.S., and Mulvaney, B.M. (2003). Women, Power, and Ethnicity: Working To-
ward Reciprocal Empowerment. New York: Haworth Press.
Denmark, F.L., Rabinowitz, V.C., and Sechzer, J.A. (2005). Engendering Psychology:
Women and Gender Revisited (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
De Groot, S.C. (1980). Female and male returnees: Glimpses of two distinct populations.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5, 358–361.
Garland, A.W. (1988). Women Activists: Challenging the Abuse of Power. New York: Fem-
inist Press.
Gibbs, N. (2005, May 16). Midlife crisis? Bring it on! Time, pp. 52–66, 63.
Gray Panthers. (2006). Age and Youth in Action. Retrieved from www.graypanthers.org.
Hammer, J. (2006, April 3). Healing powers. Newsweek, pp. 30–35, 38–39.
Hyde, J.S. (2004). Half the Human Experience: The Psychology of Women (4th ed.). Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
Lips, H.M. (2005). Sex and Gender: An Introduction. Boston: McGraw Hill.
Lott, B. (1987). Women’s Lives: Themes and Variations in Gender Learning. Monterey,
CA: Brooks/Cole.
Maslow, A.H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370–396.
Millennium Project. (2006). 2006: The Year of Action. Retrieved from http://www. unmil-
lenniumproject.org.
Mitchell, V., and Helson, R. (1990). Women’s prime of life: Is it the 50s? Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 14, 451–470.
National Organization for Women (2006). About NOW. Retrieved from http://www.now.
org/organization/info.html.
Page, N. and Czuba, C.E. (1999). Empowerment: What is it? Journal of Extension, 37(5).
Retrieved October 3, 2005 from www.joe.org/joe/1999october/comm1.html.
Parker, P.S. (2003). Resistance and empowerment in raced, gendered, and classed work
contexts: The case of African American women. Communication Yearbook, 27, 257–
291.
Pillai, J.K. (1995). Women and Empowerment. New Delhi: Gyan.
Querimit, D.S., and Conner, L.C. (2003). Empowerment psychotherapy with adolescent
females of Color. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59, 1215–1224.
Reedy, M.N., Birren, J.E., and Schaie, K.W. (1981). Age and sex differences in satisfying
relationships across the life span. Human Development, 24, 52–66.
Rollins, B.C. (1989). Marital quality at midlife. In S. Hunter and M. Sundel (Eds.), Midlife
Myths: Issues, Findings, and Practice Implications (pp. 184–194). Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Stewart, A.J., Ostrove, J.M., and Helson, R. (2001). Middle aging in women: Patterns of
personality change from the 30s to the 50s. Journal of Adult Development, 8(1), 23–37.
Todd, J., Friedman, A., and Kariuki, P.W. (1990). Women growing stronger with age: The
effect of status in the United States and Kenya. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14,
567–577.
10. Empowerment 203
204
Index 205
Safety issues, 41, 87, 173, 175 Vaginal dryness, 16, 27–29, 42
Same-sex friendships, 112 Viagra, 33, 36, 41–42
Sedentary, 82, 85, 89 Vulnerability
Self-concept, 8, 11, 97, 118 body image, 64, 71
Self-confidence, 19, 96, 182, 188 romance, 64
Self-efficacy, 11, 65, 100, 155 sex appeal, 64
Self-esteem, 11–12, 15
Self-worth, 11, 193 Well-being
Sexual desire, 26–27, 29–31 and socio-cultural changes, 95–96, 106
Sexual dysfunction, 34, 36 androgyny, 100
Sexual response cycle, 27, 33, 45 autonomy, 98, 100, 114, 189, 192
Sexual responsivity, 29 confidence, 99
Sexual revolution fragmented structure of, 95
gendered-age norms, 102 gendered balance, 100
life-style options, 102 gendered meaning, 96
and well-being, 102 in middle life, 98
Sexual satisfaction, 30, 35, 39, 41–43, 46 Widowhood, 116, 125
Sexual self esteem, 35 Women
Sexualized society education and, 198
cultural standards of, 97 leadership and, 198
male domination, 97 Women of color, 57, 81, 86, 118
Social care, 185 Asian/Asian American/Asian-African
Social class, 37, 92, 115–116, 119 women, 118
Social construction, 95, 103, 106, 196 Black women/African American women,
Social norms/cultural norms 189–190
appearance comparisons, 19, 107 Hispanic women/Latinas, 15, 20, 87, 119, 172
Social support, 58, 63–64, 66–67, 72 Women’s Health Initiative (WHI), 16, 54–55