Assignment One Human Development

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 6

1

Running head: Attachment and Co-Sleeping

Secure Attachment in Infants and Caregivers That Co-Sleep

73198 Human Development

The Open Polytechnic of New Zealand

Assignment 1

Recommended Word Count Part A: 300-400 words.

Recommended Word Count Part B: 1000-1200 words.

Actual Word Count Part A: 799 words

Actual Word Count Part B: 899 words


2
Running head: Attachment and Co-Sleeping

Which of the infant sleeping arrangements identified by Abel et al.’s (2001)

participants is most likely to result in secure attachment?

Attachment is an emotional closeness that is shared with another person that is

developed by trust over time (Karen, 1994; Santrock, 2015). “Attachment is an instinct that

has evolved in order to keep infants in close proximity to their primary caregivers” (Gillath,

Karantzas, & Fraley, 2016. pp. 4), but there is a significant cultural variation on how infants

are cared for. A study completed in 2001 by Abel, Park, Tipene-Leach, Finau, & Linnan,

2001., outlined and compared the personal theories and care practices for infants under 12

months by conducting an interview of Maori, Tongan, Samoan, Cook Islands, Niuean and

Pakeha (European) caregivers living in New Zealand (Abel, et al., 2001).

Where the infant slept was given priority of place over the position the infants were

slept in, because most caregivers, from all groups interviewed, agreed that sleeping an

infant on their back was the safest of all practices. With very little exceptions, co-sleeping

was seen to be the norm amongst caregivers from the Pacific ethnic groups (Abel et al.,

2001). Co-sleeping is a common intercultural sleeping arrangement for families, with the

arrangements differing across cultures, each arrangement having its own positive and

negative results on the attachment between the infant and primary caregiver. There are

strong beliefs among the cultures that co-sleeping is has many practical, psychological, and

spiritual benefits for the infant (Abel et al., 2001). Especially when forming a secure

attachment to their primary caregivers. John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth both also

conducted research that successfully support the idea that co-sleeping has benefits towards

obtaining secure attachment in infants.


3
Running head: Attachment and Co-Sleeping

John Bowlby (1907-1990) saw how important it was to make the most out of

parenting experiences during the early childhood years, these early experiences have a

great impact on the development of the child (Berzoff, Flanagan, & Hertz, 2011). “Bowlby

proposed that all complex organisms, including humans, have an attachment system, one

that is highly adaptive in that it keeps the young in close proximity to a critical older animal

to whom the young seek to return at times of danger” ((Berzoff, et al., 2011. pp162). It is

also interesting to note that because of Bowlby’s studies on infant/maternal attachment

that due to the needs of children being barely met in orphanages, foster care became the

largely preferred option (Gillath, et al., 2016). Bowlby believed that having a continuous,

warm, and supportive connection with a mother or mother-figure is vital for a child’s mental

health, sense of security, and enables their positive social and emotional development.

(Gillath, et al., 2016).

Mary Ainsworth was well known for her research supporting the importance of

infant and early childhood dependence on an individual caregiver, having enough of an

attachment for ideal development as long as the infant’s changing needs were constantly

met (Berzoff, et al., 2011). Ainsworth found, following a research project that involved

observing the mother and infant behaviors during their first year, that attachment from the

child to the mother or another primary caregiver advances quickly from six to twelve

months following the birth of the child (Berzoff, et al., 2011). It was also observed that the

infant would smile and vocalize more toward the primary caregiver than to other people.

Completion of attachment was seen by the time the infant turned one (Berzoff, et al., 2011).

Ainsworth found that the attachment relationship that is formed between an infant and
4
Running head: Attachment and Co-Sleeping

their caregiver is the foundation for their behavior and attachments in other relationships

throughout the rest of their lives (Ainsworth, 2008).

Both Bowlby and Ainsworth, as seen in their research, both believed that secure

attachment occurs if the child’s needs were constantly and consistently met in the early

stages of childhood. This supports the theory that co-sleeping benefits the development of

secure attachment because the child is close by, warm, comforted, and any needs that are

requested by the child are met quickly whilst remaining safe and secure, always near the

primary caregiver offering easy attendance to the infant’s needs throughout the night (Abel

et al., 2001).

Some Pacific-raised Tongan and Samoan caregivers viewed co-sleeping as a means of

protecting their infant from sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) because the mother

would, being so close by, notice if anything was wrong with the infant (Abel et al., 2001).

Having the infant in a separate room could be seen to be placing the infant at risk where

they cannot be as easily observed (Abel et al., 2001). According to the information gathered

in the interviews caregivers such as older Maori, Pakeha, or those who follow western

beliefs, who choose not to co-sleep with their infants because they believe that if the infant

sleeps in their own cot this will create autonomy and independence. These caregivers may

miss out on secure attachment opportunities that co-sleeping enables (Abel et al., 2004).

In conclusion the infant who co-sleeps and is shown constant warmth, love, care,

and responsiveness will become securely attached to their main caregiver, and in turn the

infant gains the security of being able to explore the world around them and engage

socially, with the confidence that they can return, and their primary caregiver will be there.
5
Running head: Attachment and Co-Sleeping

This security of their place in the world will carry on throughout the child’s lifetime (Karen,

1994 & Gillath, et al., 2016).


6
Running head: Attachment and Co-Sleeping

References.

Abel, S., Park, J., Tipene-Leach, D., Finau, S., & Lennan, M. (2001). Infant care practices in

New Zealand: A cross-cultural qualitative study. Social Sciences and Medicine.

Ainsworth, M. D. D. (2008). Infant-mother attachment. In R. Diessner (Ed.), Classical

Edition Sources: Human Development (3rd ed., pp. 104-109). Dubuque, IA:

McGraw-Hill Contemporary Learning Series.

Berzoff, J., Flanagan, L. M., & Hertz, P. (Eds.). (2011). Inside out and outside in:

psychodynamic clinical theory and psychopathology in contemporary multicultural

contexts. Retrieved from https://ebookcentral.proquest.com

Gillath, O., Karantzas, G. C., & Fraley, R. C. (2016). Adult attachment: A concise

introduction to theory and research. London, UK: Academic Press, an imprint of

Elsevier.

Karen, R. (1994). Becoming attached: First relationships and how they shape our capacity

to love. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Santrock, W. J. (2015). Essentials of life-span development. New York, NY: McGraw Hill

Education.

You might also like