0% found this document useful (0 votes)
55 views11 pages

The Sleeping Brain

The document discusses the "Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis" which proposes that sleep serves an important function of renormalizing synaptic strength in the brain. During wakefulness, synapses undergo potentiation through learning but this increases energy demands. Sleep allows the brain to be offline and reactivate neural circuits, weakening synapses to restore homeostasis and optimize memory consolidation by strengthening connections between related memories while weakening unrelated ones. Studies show cognitive deficits from sleep deprivation are reversed by sleep, supporting the idea that sleep provides a special time for brain recovery through synaptic renormalization.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
55 views11 pages

The Sleeping Brain

The document discusses the "Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis" which proposes that sleep serves an important function of renormalizing synaptic strength in the brain. During wakefulness, synapses undergo potentiation through learning but this increases energy demands. Sleep allows the brain to be offline and reactivate neural circuits, weakening synapses to restore homeostasis and optimize memory consolidation by strengthening connections between related memories while weakening unrelated ones. Studies show cognitive deficits from sleep deprivation are reversed by sleep, supporting the idea that sleep provides a special time for brain recovery through synaptic renormalization.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 11

Cerebrum June 2017

The Sleeping Brain


By Chiara Cirelli, M.D., Ph.D., and Giulio Tononi, M.D., Ph.D.

` Source/Shutterstock

Editor’s Note: The role of sleep has long baffled scientists, but the latest research is providing
new indicators about what it does for both the brain and body. While scientists believe that
sleep re-energizes the body's cells, clears waste from the brain, and supports learning and
memory, much still needs to be learned about the part it plays in regulating mood, appetite and
libido.

1
Cerebrum June 2017

Why do we spend a third of our life asleep? The answer seems obvious: To recover from the
“fatigue” of being awake, to be ready for another day of challenges, good or bad. All of us have
experienced the consequences of a sleepless night: everything requires more effort; we lack energy
and motivation, and feel groggy, irritable, and snappish.

But there is strong, objective proof that, far from being just such a “time filler,” sleep serves an
active, essential function.1 We know that all animal species that have been carefully studied sleep,
with no exception.1 If sleep were not essential, one would expect that some would have evolved to
do without it, since time spent asleep reduces time spent foraging, reproducing, or monitoring the
environment. Moreover, being asleep puts an animal in a potentially dangerous situation, because
it reduces the ability to promptly respond to stimuli that signal threat. Thus, sleep makes little
sense, from an evolutionary point of view, unless it provides enough essential benefits to overcome
its inherent risks.

We also have learned that sleep is exquisitely regulated. There are complex mechanisms in our
brain that increase the duration and/or the depth of sleep after sleep deprivation2 to permit the
recovery of some of what was lost (homeostatic regulation). There are also neural mechanisms that
tend to consolidate sleep at a certain phase of the 24-hour cycle (circadian regulation): night for us
and day for many rodents. Sleep loss or chronic sleep disruption also has many negative
consequences, including adverse effects on metabolism and immune function.3,4

The most obvious of these adverse effects are on the brain. Cognitive deficits of many kinds 5 are
apparent in humans, although with substantial inter-individual differences6,7 after just one night of
total sleep deprivation or when sleep is cut short by several hours every night for a week or more.8,9
Attention, working memory, and the ability to learn and remember decline. When we are sleep
deprived, it is more difficult to speak fluently, assess risks, and appreciate humor.8,9 Importantly,
experiments have shown that these cognitive impairments can be reversed by sleep but not by the
same period of quiet wakefulness.10 Similarly, there is evidence that cognitive deficits caused by

2
Cerebrum June 2017

sleep loss at night can be prevented or delayed by naps.11 The open question is: Why does sleep
offer a special time for the brain’s recovery?

Sleep versus Rest


In our search for an answer, we started by identifying the fundamental feature of sleep—what
distinguishes it even from quiet, restful wakefulness: sensory disconnection. During quiet
wakefulness, when we sit on a sofa in a silent and dark room after having exercised, for example,
our muscles can recover from fatigue. Yet, we are still able to react and move promptly if the phone
rings. In other words, we are still connected to the world. On the other hand, when deeply asleep
our capacity to react to a mild stimulus—a noise coming from the next room, or that phone call—is
reduced substantially.

Thus, any hypothesis about the essential function of sleep must take into account that, when
asleep, we are essentially offline: sensory disconnection must be an essential requirement for
whatever function sleep serves. If not, natural selection would likely have found a way to perform
the same function while awake, avoiding the danger of being unable to monitor the environment.

Over the past 20 years, we have developed and tested a comprehensive hypothesis about the core
function of sleep: The Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis (SHY).12-14 Briefly, SHY states that sleep is the
price we pay for brain plasticity. According to our hypothesis, during wakefulness, synapses—the links
that allow neurons to communicate with each other—undergo net strengthening (potentiation) as a
result of learning. Learning is an ongoing process that happens all the time while we are awake,
constantly adapting to an ever-changing environment. The remarkable and pervasive plasticity of
the brain is an established fact and is essential for survival. However, it is a costly process, because
stronger synapses increase the demand for energy and cellular supplies, lead to decreases in signal-
to-noise ratios (because neurons would start responding less selectively to stimuli), and saturate
the ability to learn.

This is where sleep enters the picture. According to SHY, while our brain is offline—disconnected
from the environment—neural circuits can be reactivated, renormalizing synaptic strength. As

3
Cerebrum June 2017

explained below, this renormalization favors memory consolidation and the integration of new with
old memories, and eliminates the synapses that contribute more to the “noise” than to the “signal.”
Just as importantly, synaptic renormalization during sleep restores the homeostasis of energy and
cellular supplies, including many proteins and lipids that are part of the synapses, with beneficial
effects at both the systems and cellular level.

Thanks for the Memories


It is useful to expand a bit on the rationale for SHY. First, the hypothesis emphasizes the obvious fact
that wakefulness is the time when we can learn about our current environment, since only then do
correlations in a neuron’s input reflect the causal structure of that environment. By the same token,
sleep is not a good time for forming new memories, since we would run the risk of remembering
fantasies and dreams, rather than events in the real world.

Second, learning is massive and ubiquitous throughout wakefulness: every waking minute you lay down
innumerable neural traces about facts and events. Try the simple experiment of recalling everything
that you saw, heard, and did today. You will realize that it is possible to remember innumerable facts
and events about your daily activities, even if you did not start the day with the specific goal of
memorizing them.

Third, learning during waking should occur primarily by synaptic potentiation, not depression; i.e. by
strengthening rather than weakening the connections among neurons. Why? Because firing is much
more energetically expensive than silence15,16 by default neurons that tend to fire very little.17,18 And
since any decrease in firing starting from such low levels would be hard to detect, neurons should signal
an important event by increasing their firing.

So how can a neuron, located deep in the brain without direct access to the external world, judge what
events are potentially important? Simply by paying attention to those inputs (or synapses), out of the
few thousands it receives, that are firing strongly and together. The increased firing of several inputs,
more or less synchronously, represents a “suspicious coincidence” and is a good indicator that many of
its input neurons had something important to signal, hence worth relaying further. During a waking

4
Cerebrum June 2017

day, then, one can expect that many synapses will be strengthened throughout the brain, establishing
memory traces.

But now a different issue arises. If the brain undergoes massive synaptic potentiation to establish
memory traces during a waking day, there must be a way to ensure that synaptic strength does not
grow indefinitely. Otherwise, neurons would quickly reach synaptic saturation, obliterating all memory
traces, not to mention that energy and supplies would not be sustainable under these conditions.
Clearly, there must be synaptic renormalization to regulate synaptic strength; a kind of synaptic
homeostasis to prevent the nervous system from descending into chaos.

In this respect, the default assumption among neuroscientists has been that synaptic homeostasis is
maintained during learning itself, when the brain is online. SHY, instead, proposes that synaptic
renormalization should not happen during waking, when we are at the mercy of a particular
environment and slaves of the “here and now,” but during sleep, when the brain is offline. Freed from
the tyranny of its immediate environment, the brain can sample all its memories—old and new—and
renormalize the total amount of synaptic strength in a smart way, preserving and consolidating those
newly formed memory traces that fit best with its overall knowledge basis, while forgetting those that
fit less well. Thus, sleep should be a time for net synaptic depression, leading to optimal “down-
selection” of memory traces.

Testing a Hypotheses
How can the general idea underlying SHY be tested? There is no one comprehensive measure of synaptic
strength, especially in vivo in freely moving animals. Over the years, we have employed many different
methodologies, from continuous electrophysiological recordings in rodents and humans to molecular
and genetic experiments in flies, mice, and rats. For instance, by using electrical stimulation (in rats) or
transcranial magnetic stimulation (in humans, using a magnet above the skull to stimulate the brain non-
invasively), we have found that the neural response triggered by a stimulus of fixed amplitude is bigger
after several hours spent awake compared to several hours spent asleep,19,20 consistent with a net
increase in synaptic strength during wakefulness.

5
Cerebrum June 2017

These results are in line with SHY’s predictions, but one cannot rule out that changes in neuronal
excitability may also play a role. 21 More direct evidence comes from experiments in cortical slices, where
we found that frequency and amplitude of miniature excitatory synaptic potentials increase after waking
and decrease after sleep.22 Measuring the total amount of synaptic proteins in large regions of the fly
and rat brain, we similarly found an increase after waking as compared to after sleep.19,23,24 Furthermore,
in adolescent mice in which neural circuits are still being formed and refined, we found that during
wakefulness the formation of new synapses exceeds their elimination, while the opposite is true during
sleep.25

Perhaps the most direct way to measure synaptic strength is through an ultrastructural approach that
literally measures the size of the synapses. It is well established that stronger synapses are bigger, which
implies that, per SHY, most synapses should grow with wakefulness and shrink with sleep, in a matter of
a few hours. To test this prediction, we used serial block face electron microscopy (SBEM), a new method
that permits the effective and automatic acquisition of high-resolution, tridimensional images of many
synapses. Because the actual measurements of the synapses must be performed manually, it took
several people four years to measure ~7,000 synapses in the mouse cerebral cortex.

At the end of this laborious process, the results were clear: six to eight hours of sleep led, on average, to
an 18 percent decrease in the size of the synapses as compared to six to eight hours of non-forced
wakefulness at night. Similar results were obtained when sleep was compared to six to eight hours of
wakefulness enforced by exposure to novel objects during the day,26 proving that these changes are due
to behavioral state (sleep vs. waking) and not to circadian factors (day vs. night). The sleep-related
decline in synapse size occurred in both areas that were examined—primary motor and primary sensory
cortex.

Across the population of neurons, the decrease in synaptic size with sleep largely followed a scaling
relationship, meaning that the change was proportional to the size of the synapse. Intriguingly, however,
sleep-related downscaling showed some selectivity, occurring in the great majority of synapses (~80
percent) but sparing the largest ones, which may be associated with the most stable memory traces. The
magnitude of these changes is worth considering. There are approximately 100 billion neurons in our

6
Cerebrum June 2017

brain, 16 billion of them in the cerebral cortex alone (14 million in the mouse cortex), and each neuron
receives thousands of synapses. Thus, if what we observed in two cortical areas extends to other brain
regions, every night trillions of synapses in our brain could get slimmer by nearly 20 percent.

The SBEM study was recently published along with research from an independent group, which used
biochemical and molecular methods to confirm SHY’s prediction that synapses undergo a process of
scaling down during sleep.27 This study also showed that one gene, Homer1a, is important for the sleep-
mediated downscaling process.

7
Cerebrum June 2017

Figure 1. Top, Schematic diagram describing the main claim of SHY: net synaptic increase occurs during wake (during the day in humans and
diurnal animals), when many circuits in the brain get potentiated (dark blue lines in the brain schematic), resulting in cellular and systems’
costs, followed by synaptic renormalization during sleep, when most, if not all circuits undergo synaptic down-selection (light blue lines).

Bottom, Parameters used to test SHY. While structural and molecular measures more directly reflect synaptic strength, electrophysiological
measures such as responses evoked by direct stimulation of the cerebral cortex can be strongly affected by other factors that modulate intrinsic
neuronal excitability, including the levels of neuromodulators and the balance between excitation and inhibition, and thus cannot be used
alone to infer synaptic strength. In the upper panel (structural), the red line indicates the axon to spine interface (ASI), and the yellow area
outlines the head of the spine. In the middle panel (molecular), excitatory glutamatergic receptors (AMPARs) are shown as squared boxes.

Avenues to Explore
We still do not know how synaptic renormalization occurs at the individual synapse level, which would
require measuring changes across the sleep/wake cycle and before and after learning. SHY proposes that
total synaptic strength is downscaled during sleep, but this does not mean that all synapses need to be
renormalized every night. In computer simulations, we showed that various synaptic rules enforcing
activity-dependent depression during sleep are compatible with the renormalization process predicted
by SHY.

For example, it could be that stronger synapses are either depressed less than weaker ones28 or are
completely protected from depression.29,30 In the latter implementation of down-selection, when a
neuron detects increased firing in many of its inputs during sleep (and thus fires strongly), the associated
synapses will be protected from depression and maintain their current strength. The result is that, while
the strength of these synapses does not increase in absolute terms, as is the case during wake, it does
increase in relative terms, because the remaining synapses, those that were not protected, end up losing
strength. This competitive down-selection mechanism has the advantage that synapses activated
strongly and consistently during sleep survive mostly unchanged and may actually consolidate, becoming
more resistant to interference and decay.

We believe that through this single process—synaptic renormalization—sleep can provide many of the
benefits that behavioral studies have documented at the systems level: The ability to learn new things
the next day, the consolidation of what has already been learned, and “smart” forgetting (i.e., the
systematic elimination of irrelevant memory traces). However, much remains to be done. We plan to
test whether the ultrastructural changes found in the cerebral cortex also happen elsewhere in the brain,
such as in the hippocampus, another region crucial for brain plasticity and learning.

8
Cerebrum June 2017

It is also essential to establish what happens during early development, when brain circuits are still being
formed and synapses show a very high turnover. Additionally, we are trying to determine what might
happen during sustained sleep loss: Do synapses keep growing as in normal waking, or do they start
shrinking and disappearing, perhaps through a massive “pruning” process that is not as carefully
regulated as during normal sleep? And if so, is abnormal pruning more pronounced during sensitive
periods of development when neural circuits are still forming, perhaps with long-term consequences for
the wiring of the adult brain?31,32

Bios
Chiara Cirelli, M.D., Ph.D., is a professor in the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Wisconsin–
Madison. She received her medical degree and Ph.D. in neuroscience from the University of Pisa, Italy,
where she began her investigation of the molecular correlates of sleep and wakefulness and the role of
the noradrenergic system in sleep regulation. She continued this work at the Neuroscience Institute in
San Diego as a fellow in experimental neuroscience, and subsequently at the University of Wisconsin.
Cirelli’s research is aimed at investigating the fundamental mechanisms of sleep regulation by using a
combination of molecular and genetic approaches.

Giulio Tononi, M.D., Ph.D., is professor of psychiatry, distinguished professor in consciousness science,
the David P. White Chair in Sleep Medicine at the University of Wisconsin–Madison, and the director of
the Wisconsin Institute for Sleep and Consciousness. He received his medical degree from the University
of Pisa, Italy, where he specialized in psychiatry. After serving as a medical officer in the Army, he
obtained a Ph.D. in neuroscience as a fellow of the Scuola Superiore for his work on sleep regulation.
From 1990 to 2000, he was a member of The Neurosciences Institute and, in 2005, received
the National Institutes of Health Director’s Pioneer Award for his work on sleep. His laboratory studies
consciousness and its disorders as well as the mechanisms and functions of sleep.

9
Cerebrum June 2017

References

1. Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Is sleep essential? PLoS Biol 6, e216 (2008).


2. Borbely, A.A., Daan, S., Wirz-Justice, A. & Deboer, T. The two-process model of sleep
regulation: a reappraisal. J Sleep Res 25, 131-143 (2016).
3. Mullington, J.M., Simpson, N.S., Meier-Ewert, H.K. & Haack, M. Sleep loss and inflammation.
Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 24, 775-784 (2010).
4. Arble, D.M., et al. Impact of Sleep and Circadian Disruption on Energy Balance and Diabetes:
A Summary of Workshop Discussions. Sleep 38, 1849-1860 (2015).
5. Lo, J.C., et al. Effects of Partial and Acute Total Sleep Deprivation on Performance across
Cognitive Domains, Individuals and Circadian Phase. PloS one 7, e45987 (2012).
6. Rupp, T.L., Wesensten, N.J. & Balkin, T.J. Trait-like vulnerability to total and partial sleep
loss. Sleep 35, 1163-1172 (2012).
7. Goel, N., Basner, M., Rao, H. & Dinges, D.F. Circadian rhythms, sleep deprivation, and
human performance. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci 119, 155-190 (2013).
8. Killgore, W.D. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Progress in brain research 185, 105-
129 (2010).
9. Banks, S. & Dinges, D.F. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J
Clin Sleep Med 3, 519-528 (2007).
10. Mednick, S.C., et al. The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration. Nature
neuroscience 5, 677-681 (2002).
11. Ficca, G., Axelsson, J., Mollicone, D.J., Muto, V. & Vitiello, M.V. Naps, cognition and
performance. Sleep Med Rev 14, 249-258 (2010).
12. Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain research bulletin
62, 143-150 (2003).
13. Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 10, 49-62
(2006).
14. Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular
homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12-34 (2014).
15. Attwell, D. & Gibb, A. Neuroenergetics and the kinetic design of excitatory synapses. Nat
Rev Neurosci 6, 841-849 (2005).
16. Hallermann, S., de Kock, C.P., Stuart, G.J. & Kole, M.H. State and location dependence of
action potential metabolic cost in cortical pyramidal neurons. Nature neuroscience 15, 1007-
1014 (2012).
17. Buzsaki, G. & Mizuseki, K. The log-dynamic brain: how skewed distributions affect network
operations. Nat Rev Neurosci 15, 264-278 (2014).
18. Suthana, N. & Fried, I. Percepts to recollections: insights from single neuron recordings in
the human brain. Trends in cognitive sciences 16, 427-436 (2012).
19. Vyazovskiy, V.V., Cirelli, C., Pfister-Genskow, M., Faraguna, U. & Tononi, G. Molecular and
electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep.
Nature neuroscience 11, 200-208 (2008).
20. Huber, R., et al. Human cortical excitability increases with time awake. Cerebral cortex 23,
332-338 (2013).
21. Cirelli, C. Sleep, synaptic homeostasis and neuronal firing rates. Curr Opin Neurobiol 44, 72-
79 (2017).

10
Cerebrum June 2017

22. Liu, Z.W., Faraguna, U., Cirelli, C., Tononi, G. & Gao, X.B. Direct evidence for wake-related
increases and sleep-related decreases in synaptic strength in rodent cortex. The Journal of
neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30, 8671-8675 (2010).
23. Gilestro, G.F., Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Widespread changes in synaptic markers as a function
of sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Science 324, 109-112 (2009).
24. Bushey, D., Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: structural evidence in
Drosophila. Science 332, 1576-1581 (2011).
25. Maret, S., Faraguna, U., Nelson, A.B., Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Sleep and waking modulate
spine turnover in the adolescent mouse cortex. Nature neuroscience 14, 1418-1420 (2011).
26. de Vivo, L., et al. Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle.
Science 355, 507-510 (2017).
27. Diering, G.H., et al. Homer1a drives homeostatic scaling-down of excitatory synapses during
sleep. Science 355, 511-515 (2017).
28. Olcese, U., Esser, S.K. & Tononi, G. Sleep and synaptic renormalization: a computational
study. Journal of neurophysiology 104, 3476-3493 (2010).
29. Nere, A., Hashmi, A., Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Sleep Dependent Synaptic Down-Selection (I):
Modeling the Benefits of Sleep on Memory Consolidation and Integration. Frontiers in Sleep
and Chronobiology 4, 143 (2013).
30. Hashmi, A., Nere, A. & Tononi, G. Sleep Dependent Synaptic Down-Selection (II): Single
Neuron Level Benefits for Matching, Selectivity, and Specificity. Frontiers in Sleep and
Chronobiology 4, 144 (2013).
31. Billeh, Y.N., et al. Effects of chronic sleep restriction during early adolescence on the adult
pattern of connectivity of mouse secondary motor cortex. eneuro, ENEURO. 0053-
0016.2016 (2016).
32. Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Cortical development, electroencephalogram rhythms, and the
sleep/wake cycle. Biol Psychiatry 77, 1071-1078 (2015).

11

You might also like