M Fine Reinvestigating 2000

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 35

Reinvestigating Remarriage: Another Decade of Progress.

By: Marilyn Coleman, Lawrence Ganong and Mark Fine

Coleman, M., Ganong, L., & Fine, M. (2000). Reinvestigating remarriage: Another decade of
progress. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62(4), 1288-1307.

Made available courtesy of Wiley-Blackwell. The definitive version is available at


www.onlinelibrary.wiley.com.

***Reprinted with permission. No further reproduction is authorized without written


permission from Wiley-Blackwell. This version of the document is not the version of
record. Figures and/or pictures may be missing from this format of the document. ***

Abstract:

The article presents an overview of research and theory on remarriages and stepfamilies
published in the 1990s. Remarriage is a term that encompasses several different types of
relationships--both partners may be in a second marriage or a higher-order marriage. About 75
percent of divorced people remarry and serial remarriages are increasingly common. As people
age, however, the divorce rates of first marriages and remarriages converge. The mean length of
time between divorce and remarriage is less than four years. Men remarry at higher rates than do
women and blacks and Hispanics remarry at lower rates than whites. A substantial proportion of
U.S. births occurs in remarriages. Some first marriages create stepfamilies and stepparent-
stepchild relationships. In 1992, 15 percent of all children in the U.S. lived with a mother and a
stepfather. Although the presence of stepchildren is thought to lower marital quality for
remarried adults, the effects are not always strong. A number of intrapersonal, interpersonal and
societal-level explanations have been proposed for the greater instability of remarriages.
Adolescent stepchildren also generally showed more externalizing behavioral problems than
children living with both parents such as using drugs and alcohol, engaging in sexual intercourse,
nonmarital childbearing and being arrested.

Keywords: parent-child relationships | parenting | family | stepfamilies | remarriage | marriage |


divorce

Article:

The body of stepfamily research published this decade exceeded the entire output of the previous
90 years of the century. The complexity and quality of the scholarly work in this decade
improved as wellwbetter samples were obtained, methods were more sensitive to stepfamily
complexity, longitudinal designs were more frequently employed, and other important
methodological gains were made. Unfortunately, many unknowns regarding remarriages and
stepfamilies remain. We present an overview of trends regarding topics, research methods, and
theories; we critique research methods that have not been productive; and we identify scholarly
advances. Finally, new conceptual, methodological and theoretical directions for future
scholarship on remarriages and stepfamilies are proposed.

Remarriages and stepfamilies have always represented a substantial proportion of marriages and
families in the United States and other Western countries (Phillips, 1997). However, researchers
paid little attention to stepfamilies until the 1970s, when divorce replaced bereavement as the
leading precursor to remarriage (Cherlin, 1992). Postdivorce stepfamilies were hard to ignore
because unlike postbereavement stepfamilies, remarriage no longer reconstituted the nuclear
family, and stepparents often were added "parent figures" rather than substitutes for deceased
parents. The complications and new interaction patterns of postdivorce stepfamilies created an
explosion of scholarly interest in remarriage, stepparenting, and stepfamilies in the 1980s
(Coleman & Ganong, 1990).

Although the 1980s were a productive period for research on remarriage and stepfamilies and
although the quality of investigations improved throughout the decade (Coleman & Ganong,
1990), the scholarship left ample room for conceptual and methodological improvement. For
example, inadequate attention was given to the structural complexity and diversity of
stepfamilies, relationship problems were studied to the near exclusion of positive interactions,
and households and stepfamilies were treated as if they were equivalent. The reactivity of
measures and methods and the difficulty in obtaining representative samples of remarried
families also were problems characteristic of 1980s research. Consequently, in the last decade
review article, Coleman and Ganong made a number of recommendations for research on
stepfamilies, calling for more complexity in designs and in how researchers thought about
remarriages and stepfamilies.

One purpose of this review is to present and summarize research and theory on remarriages and
stepfamilies published in the 1990s. This decade was a period of enormous productivity in the
study of remarriages and stepfamilies. The body of published work was three times larger than
the number of publications on stepfamilies before 1990. We examined more than 850
publications. Given the volume of studies, we decided to emphasize topics that received
relatively greater attention by scholars: (a) demographic trends, (b) remarriage relationships, (c)
the effects on children of living with a stepparent, (d) stepfamily processes, (e) societal views of
stepfamilies, and (f) legal issues. Most (92%) of the decade's research fits into one of these
categories. Many excellent reviews of research were written in this decade; interested readers
should consult them (e.g., Amato, 1994; Cherlin & Furstenberg, 1994; Ganong & Coleman,
1994b; Hetherington & Henderson, 1997; Ihinger-Tallman & Pasley, 1997; Nielsen, 1999). A
second purpose of this review is to identify conceptual and methodological trends in stepfamily
research in the past decade. We critique research methods that have not been productive, and we
identify scholarly advances. A third purpose of this review is to propose conceptual,
methodological, and theoretical directions for future scholarship on remarriages and stepfamilies.
DEMOGRAPHIC TRENDS

Remarriages

Approximately half of the marriages in the United States represent a remarriage for one or both
partners (Bumpass, Sweet, & Castro Martin, 1990). Although remarriage rates are lower than in
the United States, similar trends were reported in Canada (e.g., Wu, 1994) and Europe (e.g.,
Kiernan, 1992). Remarriage is a term that encompasses several different types of relationships--
both partners may be in a second marriage or a higherorder remarriage (e.g., a third or fourth
marriage) or the marriage may be a remarriage for only one of the partners. Historically,
remarriages generally have been treated as a uniform group by researchers, although in this
decade, researchers began to think about remarriages and stepfamilies in more complex ways
(e.g., Bumpass, Raley, & Sweet, 1995). About 75% of divorced people remarry (Furstenberg &
Cherlin, 1991), and serial remarriages are increasingly common (National Center for Health
Statistics, 1993). Remarriages that end in divorce do so more quickly than first marriages. As
people age, however, the divorce rates of first marriages and remarriages converge (Clarke &
Wilson, 1994). Remarriages contracted by older adults (i.e., older than 40) may be more stable
than first marriages (Wu & Penning, 1997).

Remarriages tend to take place rapidly after prior marriages end; the mean length of time
between divorce and remarriage is less than 4 years (Wilson & Clarke, 1992). Thirty percent
remarry within a year after the divorce, and the distribution of time between marriages is
skewed, peaking for both men and women at 1 month after divorce and 13 months after
bereavement. The interval to remarriage lengthens with age for both men and women (Wilson &
Clarke).

Men remarry at higher rates than do women (South, 1991), and Blacks and Hispanics remarry at
lower rates than Whites (South). For divorced women, the probability of remarriage is lower
when they are older (Buckle, Gallup, & Rodd, 1996), have more education, and are employed
(Montalto & Gerner, 1998). For men, education increases the likelihood of remarriage (Montalto
& Gerner). Divorced men who marry divorced women generally marry someone at least a few
years younger than themselves, and it is likely that her children are younger than his (Wilson &
Clarke, 1992). Children lower the likelihood of remarriage for both men and women, but the
impact of children is greater on women's probability of remarriage (Buckle et al.). High
occupational status delays remarriage among women separating at relatively younger ages and
hastens it among women separating at relatively older ages. For older women, those least able to
support themselves are also least likely to remarry. Thus, among older women, patterns of
remarriage tend to increase the concentration of poverty among divorced women.

Fertility in Remarriage
A substantial proportion of all U.S. births occurs in remarriages. For both Blacks and Whites,
about half of women in remarriages give birth to at least one child, most within 24 months of
remarriage (Wineberg, 1990).

Cohabitation

Remarriage rates have been dropping in the United States except among older adults, but this
does not mean that people are recoupling less often. Instead of remarrying after divorce,
increasing numbers of divorced individuals are cohabiting, not as a precursor but as an
alternative to remarriage (Bumpass et al., 1991, 1995). Moreover, most couples cohabit prior to
legally forming a union (Cherlin & Furstenberg, 1994).

Older Adults and New Partners

The marital histories of older Americans are becoming increasingly complex (Cornman &
Kingson, 1996). More divorced older adults, increased longevity, and better health throughout
life are factors related to increases in remarriage among older adults (Holden & Kuo, 1996). An
estimated half million people over the age of 65 in the United States remarry each year (U.S.
Census Bureau, 1995). Over 4% of older people do not remarry but cohabit with a new partner
(Chevan, 1996).

The numbers of later-life cohabiting relationships, remarriages, and stepfamilies are likely to
increase in the next few years as the baby boom generation begins reaching retirement age.
About half of the marriages of this cohort will end in divorce, and about 75% of those who have
divorced will remarry at least once (Furstenberg & Cherlin, 1991). Among this cohort will be
unprecedented numbers of individuals who are stepparents (Comman & Kingson, 1996).

Stepfamilies

Not all remarriages include children from prior relationships, nor do all stepfamilies incorporate
a remarriage. Approximately 25% of the 3.7 million cohabiting couples in the United States are
households in which at least one adult brings children from prior relationships, thereby creating
cohabiting stepfamily households (Bumpass et al., 1991). In fact, cohabiting couples are more
likely (48% versus 37%) to enter a new union with children from previous relationships than are
remarried couples (Wineberg & McCarthy, 1998). Some first marriages create stepfamilies and
stepparentstepchild relationships (i.e., when never-married mothers marry a man who is not the
child's father).

In 1992, 15% of all children in the United States lived with a mother and a stepfather (U.S.
Bureau of the Census, 1995). About one-third of U.S. children will live in a remarried or
cohabiting stepfamily household before they reach adulthood (Bumpass et al., 1995; Seltzer,
1994). In fact, children in stepfamilies may have lived in several types of families before they
reach adulthood, although fewer than 5% of all remarried couples incorporate three sets of
children (i.e., yours, mine, and ours). Complex marital and cohabiting histories over the life
course result in complex family histories for children (O'Connor, Pickering, Dunn, Golding, &
the ALSAC Study Team, 1999; Wojtkiewicz, 1994) and for adults. For example, about 40% of
adult women will likely reside in a remarried or cohabiting stepfamily household as a parent or
stepparent at some time (Bumpass et al.), and 40% of all families include stepgrandparents
(Szinovacz, 1998).

The work of demographers in this decade yielded a more complex understanding of stepfamily
structures. The importance of knowing about relationship histories in order to more fully
comprehend stepfamily dynamics was underscored, and these more elaborated demographic
views influenced how other researchers conceptualized stepfamily structure.

REMARRIAGE RELATIONSHIPS

Remarriage research generally focused on marital dynamics, quality, and stability. Samples were
often small, and many employed qualitative designs.

Marital Dynamics

Remarriage or cohabitation typically occurs within months after beginning the relationship
(Montgomery, Anderson, Hetherington, & Clingempeel, 1992), and we know little about how
decisions to cohabit are made or the effects that cohabiting has on the stepfamily system. A
common courtship pattern is as follows: (a) male partner spends a few nights per week in the
mother's household, followed by (b) a brief period of full-time living together, followed by (c)
remarriage (Montgomery et al.).

Studies of the process of building satisfying remarriages were limited, and the effectiveness of
strategies used was not determined. Evidence exists that decision making in remarriage is
perceived to be equally shared (Crosbie-Bumett & Giles-Sims, 1991; Pyke), primarily because
women seek more power in remarriages than in first marriages (Pyke & Coltrane, 1996).
Reasons offered for why distribution of power in remarriage is more equitable included the
following: (a) personal experiences in prior unions and as divorced single persons cause women
to seek more power (Burgoyne & Morison, 1997; Pyke, 1994), (b) the greater resources women
often bring to remarriage give them more bargaining power (Crosbie-Burnett & Giles-Sims;
Pyke & Coltrane), (c) previously married men and women think differently about marital roles
(Burgoyne & Morison; IshiiKuntz & Coltrane, 1992), (d) women who are reluctant to remarry
gain power (Pyke), and (e) men concede more during marital conflicts than they did in their first
marriages (Hobart, 1991).

Women perceived that they have more power regarding financial decisions in their remarriages
than in their prior marriages (Burgoyne & Morison, 1997; Pasley, Sandras, & Edmondson,
1994), but it is difficult to determine whether financial decision-making is equitable because
finances in remarried families are complex (Burgoyne & Morison; Jacobson, 1993). For
example, remarried couples are more likely than first marriages to maintain some economic
resources under the individual control of each partner, in part because of financial
responsibilities for children from multiple unions, and in part because of individuals' desires to
retain financial independence.

Shared decision making does not mean that household tasks are shared equitably, however.
Household duties appear to be based on traditional gender roles, and most studies find that
remarried women do most of the housework (Demo & Acock, 1993; Pyke & Coltrane, 1996),
although remarried husbands do more housework than husbands in first marriages (Deal,
Stanley-Hagan, & Anderson, 1992; Ishii-Kuntz & Coltrane, 1992).

Remarriage Quality

Findings are mixed regarding differences in marital quality between individuals in first marriages
and in remarriages; some find no differences (Booth & Edwards, 1992; Deal at al., 1992), and
others report lower relationship quality for remarried individuals (Brown & Booth, 1996). It has
been suggested that different processes may be involved in determining the quality of
remarriages and first marriages (Jacobson, 1993; Rogers, 1996b). For example, Kurdek (1991)
found that marital satisfaction declined more rapidly over time in stepfather households than in
first marriages.

Findings based on behavioral observations and self-reports were that remarried spouses more
openly express criticisms, anger, and irritation than do spouses in first marriages (Bray & Kelly,
1998; Hetherington, 1993). Remarried participants also generally report higher levels of tension
and disagreement than their counterparts in first marriages (Hobart, 1991). These disagreements
generally center on issues related to stepchildren, such as discipline, rules for children, and the
distribution of resources to children (Hobart; Pasley, Koch, & Ihinger-Tallman, 1993). Disputes
between adults may also result from arguments between stepparents and stepchildren
(Clingempeel, Colyar, & Hetherington, 1994). Although the presence of stepchildren is thought
to lower marital quality for remarried adults (Brown & Booth, 1996), the effects are not always
strong. In fact, Kurdek (1999) found that children born to first marriages lowered marital quality
more than stepchildren lowered remarital quality. Nonetheless, marital quality is poorer when
both adults have children from prior relationships than when only one adult is a stepparent,
presumably because of added complexity and more opportunities for conflict (Hobart).

Remarriage Stability

Remarriages dissolve at higher rates than first marriages (Bumpass et al., 1990), especially for
remarried couples with stepchildren (Booth & Edwards, 1992). A number of intrapersonal,
interpersonal, and societal-level explanations have been proposed for the greater instability of
remarriages. For example, compared with first marriages, remarriages include more people who
have personality characteristics (e.g., impulsivity, neuroticism) that predispose them to end
relationships more frequently and make them poorer marriage material (Booth & Edwards;
Capaldi & Patterson, 1991). Booth and Edwards concluded that remarriages are more fragile
because (a) they lack social support and clear norms to follow, (b) a larger proportion of people
who remarry than people in first marriages see divorce as a solution to marital problems, and (c)
the smaller pool of partners for remarriages results in unions between people with dissimilar
interests and values.

Some researchers pointed to greater conflict in remarriages as a reason for higher redivorce rates
(Hobart, 1991), but others argued that it is not the amount of conflict that predicts redivorce but
the manner in which remarried couples resolve their disagreements (Pasley et al., 1993). Conflict
is not inherently negative; some women in the powersharing couples that Pyke (1994)
interviewed reported more conflicts than those in husband-dominant remarriages because the
women actively sought power. In such marriages, conflict may represent active problem solving,
which may decrease the probability of redivorce.

Effects of Remarriage on Remarried Adults

A few researchers investigated health status, depression, happiness, and psychological distress of
remarried adults. The findings were mixedwremarriage was associated with higher levels of
depression (e.g., Neff & Schluter, 1993), but other researchers reported less distress for
remarried than for divorced individuals (e.g., Shapiro, 1996) and that remarriage was not related
to well-being (e.g., Richards, Hardy, & Wadsworth, 1997). Moreover, it is not clear whether men
or women benefit more from remarriage (Marks, 1995; Pasley & Ihinger-Tallman, 1990). These
mixed findings suggest that factors other than marital status explain adults' well-being. For
example, differential selection into remarriage may potentially explain differences between
remarried and divorced adults in that people with better mental and physical health are more
likely to find new partners than are those with health problems (Booth & Amato, 1991; Murphy,
Glaser, & Grundy, 1997).

EFFECTS OF STEPFAMILY LIVING ON CHILDREN

Over a third of the studies published--the most on any topic--dealt with the effects on children of
living with a remarried or cohabiting stepparent. Many were national studies (e.g., National
Study of Families and Households [NSFH]) or large representative samples (e.g., Avon
Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children in the U.K.) conducted in North America, Europe,
Asia, Australia, Israel, and New Zealand.

Research on young and adolescent stepchildren focused primarily on academic achievement


(e.g., grades, school completion, achievement test scores), psychological adjustment and well
being, and behavior problems. Typically, stepchildren and children living with one parent were
compared on the outcome measures with children living with both parents. Demographic
characteristics of children and families were usually included, as were various mediating
variables related to such constructs as family relationships and peer characteristics.
Stepchildren and children with single parents did not achieve as well on average as children
living with both parents in grades earned in school (e.g., Astone & McLanahan, 1991;
Bogenscheider, 1997), grades completed (e.g., Teachman, Paasch, & Carver, 1996), and scores
on achievement tests (e.g., Dronkers, 1994; Pong, 1997). However, the largest differences were
in dropout rates, school attendance, and whether the student graduated or received a GED
(regression coefficients of -.36, -.38, and -.33, respectively, for the differences between children
in stepfamilies and first-marriage families) (Astone & McLanahan). The finding pertaining to
dropout rates may be related to the fact that stepchildren tend to leave home to establish
independent households at younger ages than do children living with both parents (e.g.,
Aquilino, 1991b; Kieman, 1992). For example, stepdaughters were more likely to cohabit
(Goldscheider & Goldscheider, 1998) or to marry than were women from first-marriage families
(Aquilino) or from other family forms (Thornton, 1991), which may be related to leaving home
and school early.

Compared with children in first-marriage families, stepchildren on average showed more


internalizing behavior problems, such as depression (e.g., Zill et al., 1993), and were more at risk
for having emotional problems (e.g., Dawson, 1991; Hanson, McLanahan, & Thomson, 1996).
Some studies found that boys have more problems than girls (e.g., Coley, 1998; Dunn et al.,
1998); others found that girls had more problems than boys (Needle, Su, & Doherty, 1990) or
that girls had more adjustment problems than boys only when living with stepfathers (e.g., Lee,
Burkam, Zimiles, & Ladewski, 1994). Still others found more problems for girls living with
stepmothers (e.g., Suh, Schutz, & Johanson, 1996).

Adolescent stepchildren also generally showed more externalizing behavioral problems than
children living with both parents, such as using drags and alcohol (e.g., Hoffman & Johnson,
1998; Needle et al., 1990), engaging in sexual intercourse. (Day, 1992), nonmarital childbearing
(e.g., Astone & Washington, 1994), and being arrested (Coughlin & Vuchinich, 1996). African
American children may benefit from, or are at least not harmed by, the involvement of
stepfathers (e.g., Salem, Zimmerman, & Notaro, 1998; Wojtkiewicz, 1993).

Although the findings ranged widely, most researchers reported that stepchildren were similar to
children living with single mothers on the preponderance of outcome measures and that
stepchildren generally were at greater risk for problems than were children living with both of
their parents. However, most researchers also found that the differences between stepchildren
and children in first-marriage families were small, with effect sizes from Amato's (1994) meta-
analysis ranging from -.07 for academic achievement to -.32 for problems in either or both
conduct or behavior and -.37 for psychological adjustment (according to Cohen's (1969)
commonly used convention, effect sizes of .20, .50, and .80 are considered small, moderate, and
large, respectively). Most stepchildren do well in school (e.g., Pong, 1997) and do not have
emotional or behavioral problems (e.g., Dorius, Heaton, & Steffen, 1993; Lissau & Sorenson,
1994).
Long-Term Effects on Stepchildren

In addition to the plethora of studies focusing on children and adolescents, a substantial number
(n = 39) of investigations examined the long-term effects of having a stepparent. The availability
of several large, longitudinal data sets that extended data collection from birth or early childhood
into adulthood or that followed adolescents into adulthood contributed to the number of studies
on long-term effects.

Although the negative effects of having a stepparent were often reported to be long lasting (e.g.,
Biblarz, Raftery, & Bucur, 1997; Kiernan, 1992), parental remarriage during childhood was
found not to be related to emotional problems during early (Chase-Lansdale, Cherlin, & Kieman,
1995; Rodgers, Power, & Hope, 1997) and middle adulthood (Rodgers, 1994) in a British
longitudinal study. Studies of local samples also reported no relation between parental
remarriage and adjustment in early adulthood (e.g., Lissau & Sorenson, 1994). Long-term effects
may be related to age at parental remarriage (Zill et al., 1993).

Theoretical Explanations for Stepparent Effects on Stepchildren

Although views vary widely in the scholarly community regarding the effects of stepfamily
living (e.g., see Booth & Dunn, 1994), the prevailing perspective was that living with a
stepparent was harmful to children and adolescents. Parental remarriage and cohabitation
generally were viewed as family disruptions that negatively affected children, similarly to
parental divorce or residing with a never-married mother (see Amato, 2000, for a discussion of
the explanatory models used in divorce research). Although many explanations were proposed,
based on systems theory (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; Hetherington, Henderson, &
Reiss, 1999), role theory (Skopin, Newman, & McKenry, 1993; Whitsett & Land, 1992), gender
theory (MacDonald & DeMaris, 1996), exchange theory (Marsiglio, 1992), Bronfenbrenner's
social ecology model (Bogenscheider, 1997; Fine & Kurdek, 1992), and the life course
perspective (Aquilino, 1994), among others, most explanations for stepparent effects on
stepchildren could be categorized as variants of stress models, (step)parent involvement
rationales, (step)parent style models, and selection.

Stress models. Many explanations centered on the idea that parental repartnering was stressful
for children and adults (e.g., Henry & Lovelace, 1995). Parental remarriage and cohabitation
involve many changes for adults and children, such as moving to a new residence, adapting to
new household members, and learning new household routines and activities. These changes are
thought to increase stress for children, which in turn leads to poorer performances in school and
more internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Menaghan, Kowaleski-Jones, & Mott,
1997).

A related stress model, the cumulative effects hypothesis, proposed that the more marital
disruptions experienced by a parent, the more internalizing and externalizing problems children
exhibit as a result of having to cope with these multiple transitions (Capaldi & Patterson, 1991).
Support for this hypothesis was found; children whose parents had several partners over time had
more problems than children who lived with a parent who had repartnered only once (e.g.,
Capaldi & Patterson; Kurdek & Fine, 1993a).

Another stress model proposed that parental competencies are compromised when beginning
stepfamily relationships (e.g., Hoffman & Johnson, 1998). Stepchildren's problems are thus
attributed to the diminished or poor-quality parenting they receive from stressed parents who do
not have the personal resources to monitor children's behavior, participate in school activities, or
interact with their children at the same levels that they did prior to remarriage or cohabiting
(Dawson, 1991). A variation of this explanation proposed that the stepparent, as an added adult,
would reduce familial stress related to economic burdens and the monitoring of children (e.g.,
Bulcroft et al., 1998; Hawkins & Eggebeen, 1991).

Conflicts between divorced parents and within stepparent households also were hypothesized as
stress-related explanations for stepchildren generally fating worse on behavioral and
psychological outcomes than children living with both parents (e.g., Downey, 1995; Hanson,
McLanahan, & Thomson, 1996). Researchers attributed higher rates of early home leaving (e.g.,
Kiernan, 1992) and lower rates of coresidence of adult children in remarried families (e.g.,
Aquilino, 1991 a) to the stressful atmosphere in step-households. Stepchildren may withdraw as
a way to keep peace in the family and to try to maintain their own well-being (Hanson et al.).
Although not all researchers reported more conflict in stepfamilies than in nuclear families
(Barber, 1994; Salem et al., 1998) and studies did not always find that intra- and interhousehold
conflicts were related to stepchildren's outcomes (e.g., Hanson et al.), stepfamily conflict was a
viable explanation for poorer children's outcomes in many investigations (Kurdek & Fine,
1993a).

The amount of conflict in stepfamilies may be related to the ages and sexes of the stepchildren.
Adolescent stepchildren often reported more conflict with stepparents than did adolescents from
first-marriage families (Barber & Lyons, 1994; Kurdek & Fine, 1993b). Adolescents may be
more resistant to accepting authority from a stepparent than younger stepchildren. Sex also may
be relevant in predicting how stepparents relate to stepchildren. For example, Vuchinich,
Hetherington, Vuchinich, and Clingempeel (1991) found that adolescent girls had more difficulty
than boys interacting with stepfathers and had more extended conflicts with and were more likely
to withdraw from stepfathers and treat them like outsiders.

Another explanation for the greater risk of problems for stepchildren was the economic
deprivation hypothesis, which postulated that stepchildren and children living with a single
parent were at a disadvantage compared with children living with both parents because of
economic hardships and the associated deficit conditions that accompany poverty, such as
inadequate schools, dangerous neighborhoods, and adults working long hours (e.g., Pong).
Evidence to support this hypothesis was mixed; when researchers controlled for differences in
household income or socioeconomic status, effects were sometimes attenuated (e.g., Pong, 1997)
but not always (e.g., Hoffman & Johnson, 1998).

The final explanatory model related to stress was the incomplete institutionalization hypothesis.
Over two decades ago, Cherlin (1978) proposed that the absence of societal norms for remarried
families regarding role performances; the dearth of established, socially acceptable methods of
resolving problems; and the lack of institutionalized social support contributed to greater stress
for remarried families. In this view, stepchildren fare worse than children in first-marriage
families because, lacking culturally institutionalized support, stepparents are unsure about how to
relate to stepchildren, and remarried adults lack appropriate solutions to family problems. Critics
have argued that Chedin's views overstate the degree to which remarried families lack
institutional support (Jacobson, 1995), and some researchers have not found support for all of his
contentions (e.g., Coleman, Ganong, & Cable, 1997). However, there is support for the claim
that expectations for stepparents are less clear than expectations for parents (e.g., Bulcroft et al.,
1998; Fine, Coleman, & Ganong, 1998). There needs to be more research on this popular
hypothesis.

(Step)parent involvement models. Many researchers sought to explain stepchildren's behaviors as


the result of reduced involvement in their lives by either parents or stepparents. For example,
stepparent households were hypothesized to lack social capital, defined as time and energy
engaged in positive interactions with children. That is, remarriage disrupts parents' abilities to
competently raise their children because they are investing time and energy in new partners
rather than in childrearing (e.g., Downey, 1995; Pong, 1997). Similarly, stepparents do not invest
as much social capital in stepchildren because they are expending resources on the adult
relationship or on their children from prior unions (e.g., Bogenscheider, 1997; Teachman et al.,
1996). Consequently, children in stepparent households have more problems than other children
do because they are thought to be receiving inadequate parenting and adult support. Researchers
often employed the social capital model to investigate stepparent effects on stepchildren's
academic achievement; generally, stepparents and remarried parents were reported to spend less
time working with stepchildren on schoolwork and being involved with school-related activities
than were parents in nuclear families (e.g., Leung, 1995; Pong). Also, support for the social
capital model was found in studies of behavior problems (e.g., Kim, Hetherington, & Reiss,
1999). However, cooperation between the parent and stepparent in raising children from prior
relationships may be nearly as important as the level of stepparents' involvement with the
stepchildren (Bronstein, Stoll, Clauson, Abrams, & Briones, 1994).

In general, stepparents interacted less with stepchildren than parents did. In addition to the social
capital explanation, a number of other explanations for their lower involvement have been
investigated. For instance, stepfathers may find it hard to break into tightly knit mother-child
systems because of efforts by both mothers and children to keep them at a distance (Hetherington
& Clingempeel, 1992). Social cognitive factors also may play a role--several studies showed that
people generally expect stepparents to be less supportive and less close to stepchildren than
parents (e.g., Ganong & Coleman, 1995). Moreover, the stepparent role has low salience for the
identities of many stepparents, so they may find more satisfaction in work, marriage, or raising
their own children than they do in relating to their stepchildren (Thoits, 1992).

Evolutionary scholars postulated that stepparents invest little of themselves in their stepchildren
because they are not genetically related to them (Daly & Wilson, 1996). This theory proposes
that stepparents who also are parents discriminate in favor of their genetic children and that
stepfathers interact with stepchildren to impress their new partners rather than to foster
stepchildren's growth and well-being. The parental investment-parental discrimination
proposition was supported in some studies (e.g., Flinn, 1999; Mekos, Hetherington, & Reiss,
1996) but not in all (e.g., Bulcroft et al., 1998; Menaghan et al., 1997). For example, stepfathers
who lived with their children spent more time with stepchildren than did stepfathers who just had
stepchildren (Cooksey & Fondell, 1996).

Not only was the evolutionary theory used to explain relative emotional distances and
disengaged parenting practices for stepparents, it was also the theory receiving the most attention
in research on child abuse, although several other theoAes also were proposed to explain child
abuse in stepfamilies (Giles-Sims, 1997). The more than two dozen studies on child abuse in
stepfamilies were evenly divided between investigations of physical and sexual abuse. Most
researchers reported that children in households with nonnatal adults, particularly stepfathers,
mothers' boyfriends, and other nonnatal males, were at much greater Ask for sexual abuse
(Margolin, 1992) and physical abuse (Daly & Wilson, 1996). However, others argued that
stepchildren were not more likely to be abused by stepparents (Gelles & Harrop, 1991; Malkin &
Lamb, 1994). Unfortunately, given how abuse data are recorded, it is sometimes difficult to
determine whether the perpetrator of child abuse is a stepparent or another adult. For example,
mothers' boyfriends and legally remarried stepfathers are often categorized as one group.
Children are more at risk for abuse if they live in a household with an adult who is not their
genetic parent, but the extent to which stepchildren are at greater risk for being abused by a
stepparent continues to be debated (Giles-Sims).

(Step)parental style. Some researchers examined differences between parenting styles of


stepparents and parents that may have placed children at Ask for problems (e.g., Fine & Kurdek,
1992; Salem et al., 1998). Most compared parenting styles in stepparent households with other
types of households, but a few compared stepparents in various types of stepfamilies (e.g.,
Crosbie-Burnett & Giles-Sims, 1994). Unfortunately, most researchers developed their own
measures of stepparenting styles, which makes comparisons across studies difficult.

Consistent with the research on stepparents' involvement with stepchildren, the parenting styles
of stepparents were more disengaged than were those of parents. On average, stepfathers showed
less affection toward stepchildren and engaged in less supervision of them (e.g., Hetherington &
Clingempeel, 1992; Kurdek & Fine, 1995). Similar findings were reported for stepmothers
(Kurdek & Fine, 1993b).
However, not all investigators found differences in parenting style between stepparent
households and first-married-parent households. For example, stepfather households containing
adolescents did not differ from nuclear families in permissiveness and in democratic decision
making (Barber & Lyons, 1994); in support and monitoring of adolescents (Salem et al., 1998);
or in permissive, authoritarian, or authoritative parenting styles (Shucksmith, Hendry, &
Glendinning, 1995). Also, no major differences were found in adolescent independence-giving
(i.e., staying home alone, household rules, and weekend curfews) between nuclear, single-parent,
and stepparent households (Bulcroft et al., 1998).

Researchers generally found that authoritative parenting (high warmth and high control) was
positively related and that authoritarian parenting (low warmth and high control) was negatively
related to adolescent well-being, suggesting that the same family processes that influence
adolescent well-being in first-marriage families are also associated with well-being in
stepfamilies (e.g., Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; Fine & Kurdek, 1992). Stepparent support
was a better predictor of stepchild adjustment than stepparents' monitoring behaviors (Crosbie-
Burnett & GilesSims, 1994).

Although several studies identified sex differences in stepchildren's perceptions of stepparents'


warmth and control, consistent patterns are difficult to discern (e.g., Kurdek & Fine, 1993a,
1993b). There are indications that stepmothers have a harder time raising stepchildren than
stepfathers do (MacDonald & DeMafis, 1996). Additionally, Thomson, McLanahan, and Curtin
(1992) found that parenting was less gendered in father-stepmother families than in mother-
stepfather or first-marriage families, although the differences were considered to be "relatively
small" (p. 376).

Selection. A few researchers examined the selection argument that differences between
stepchildren and children living with both parents were due to factors that predated parental
remarriage or cohabitation (see Amato, 2000). Because correlational data do not allow causal
inferences, some researchers questioned whether differences between stepchildren and other
children were due to pre-existing factors, such as parental psychopathology or poverty, that
influenced both family transitions and child problems (Capaldi & Patterson, 1991). Findings
regarding selection factors have been mixed, with some reporting that children's behavior
problems predated parental remarriage (e.g., Capaldi & Patterson; Cherlin et al., 1991) and
others finding that girls showed negative effects before parental separation but that boys showed
more negative effects after separation (Doherty & Needle, 1991).

STEPFAMILY RELATIONSHIPS AND PROCESSES

Nearly one fourth of the studies in this decade dealt with stepfamily relationships or dynamics.
Stepfathers' and fathers' relationships with children were studied much more often than
stepmothers' and mothers' relationships with children, as was true during the 1980s.
Relationships between siblings and stepsiblings and between children and grandparents or
stepgrandparents were rarely studied.

Development of Stepparent-Stepchild Relationships

Few researchers considered the development of step-relationships, and findings were mixed
among those who did. In three in-depth longitudinal studies of stepfathers, relationships with
stepchildren generally became more negative over time (Bray & Kelly, 1998; Hetherington,
1993; Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992). However, in another study, some relationships
among stepparents and adolescent stepchildren became closer, some grew more distant, and
some changed little over a 5-year period (Ganong & Coleman, 1994a). Research is needed to
determine why some relationships become closer and others grow more hostile or distant.

Some evidence suggests that stepchildren reject stepparents who engage in discipline and control
early in the relationship (Bray & Kelly; Ganong, Coleman, Fine, & Martin, 1999). In contrast,
affection more often characterized stepparentstepchild relationships when stepfathers initially
engaged in supportive behaviors with stepchildren than when no such efforts were made (Bray &
Kelly, 1998; Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992). Moreover, stepparents who intentionally tried
to get their stepchildren to like them and who continued their affinity-seeking and-maintaining
efforts had warmer, closer bonds with their stepchildren than did those who gradually reduced
affinity-seeking efforts (Ganong et al., 1999). The most effective relationship-building strategies
for stepparents were dyadic activities chosen by the stepchild.

However, stepchildren are not passive observers in the developing stepparent-stepchild


relationship (Hetherington, 1993). How they treat stepparents also affects the relationship. For
example, Hetherington and Clingempeel (1992) found that stepfathers withdrew from
stepchildren who ignored their overtures early in the remarriage. We found that when
stepchildren recognized that their stepparents were trying to do things with them that the
stepchildren liked, they generally responded with their own affinity-seeking efforts (Ganong et
al., 1999). Similarly, O'Connor, Hetherington, and Clingempeel (1997) found that the observed
responses of adolescents to (step)parents was as strong a predictor of adolescent adjustment as
was (step)parent-to-adolescent behavior. Nonetheless, even when positive stepparent-stepchild
relationships are established when the child is a preadolescent, conflict may still arise when the
child gets older (Hetherington, 1993). Additionally, adolescent stepchildren tend to see parents
as more accepting of them than stepparents. Using vignettes as stimuli, Russell and Searcy
(1997) found that adolescents responded to parents in a friendlier way than they did to
stepparents in the same situations. This study suggests that adolescent stepchildren are primed to
respond to stepparents in ways that create emotional distance and to attribute motives to
stepparents that discourage warm feelings toward them.

Stepparents' Roles
Stepfamily members often do not agree on what role the stepparent should play (Fine et al.,
1998). Beyond a general consensus that parents are expected to exhibit more warmth toward
children and to more carefully monitor their behavior than are stepparents (Fine & Kurdek,
1994), there is little consistency in perceptions of the content of the stepparent role (Fine et al.).
Stepparents are less certain about their role than are other family members. Some stepfathers
deal with the issue of role confusion and stepparent identity by assuming a parent role (Berger,
1995; Erera-Weatherly, 1996), a role that contributes to their family life satisfaction (Marsiglio,
1992), although mothers and stepchildren may have different reactions. Stepmothers are much
less likely to assume a parental role (Church, 1999). Instead, most stepmothers, and many
stepfathers, especially nonresidential ones, see themselves as a friend to their stepchildren
(Church) or in some role between a friend and a parent (Erera-Weatherly). A number of studies
showed that role clarity and role agreement are related to adjustment in stepfamilies (e.g., Fine,
Ganong, & Coleman, 1997; Fine et al.). For example, stepfathers' and adolescents' perceptions of
stepfather-stepchild relationship quality were predicted best by the degree to which stepfathers
and mothers agreed on how adolescents should be raised (Skopin et al., 1993).

Mothers and Children

Generally, mother-child relationships in stepfamilies received little attention from researchers. In


a longitudinal study, Hetherington and Clingempeel (1992) found that mother-preadolescent
child relationships deteriorated after remarriage but that after 2 years, they were similar to
mother-child relationships in first-marriage families. In fact, children in all family types showed
increasingly negative behavior as they entered adolescence, but they eventually behaved more
positively toward their mothers as they were granted greater autonomy.

Fathers and Children

Most studies of fathers in stepfamilies were of nonresidential fathers. Results were mixed
regarding whether remarriage affects nonresidential parental involvement. For example, the
remarriage of either parent was not related to the frequency of contacts with nonresidential
parents (Stephen, Freedman, & Hess, 1993) or the amount of time children spent in
nonresidential fathers' households (Stephen et al.). Other researchers, however, found that
contact diminished after either parent began residing with a new partner (McKenry, McKelvey,
Leigh, & Wark, 1996).

Older Parents and Adult Children

In examinations of coresidence, emotional closeness, and intergenerational exchanges of


resources and support, researchers found that adult children were less likely to live in a remarried
parent's home than with a parent in a first marriage (Aquilino, 1991a). Adult children who grew
up with a remarried father also were less likely to take older parents into their homes than were
adults from first-marriage families, although adults with remarried mothers were as likely as
adults from first-marriage families to share their residences (Szinovacz, 1998). Findings have
been mixed on whether adult stepchildren help their stepparents and parents as much as adult
children in nuclear families--some researchers found no differences (Aquilino, 1994; Spitze &
Logan, 1992), and some found that stepchildren provide less support to parents (White, 1992)
and stepparents (Amato, Rezac, & Booth, 1995). However, when help given to or received from
the households of both parents was combined for adult children of divorced and remarried
parents, there were no significant family structure differences in exchanges between parents and
adult children (Amato et al., 1995).

Remarried parents (White, 1992) and stepparents (Spitze & Logan, 1992) in general provided
less support to adult (step)children than did parents in first marriages, but remarried mothers
gave some types of support as much as first-married mothers did (Marks, 1995; Spitze & Logan).
Differences between remarried adults and those in first marriages in attitudes about their
financial obligations to assist children (Marks), normative beliefs about intergenerational
responsibilities after remarriage (Ganong & Coleman, 1999), and differences in family solidarity
were offered to explain these findings (White).

Stepfamily Processes

There has been relatively little work examining family-level processes beyond parenting styles in
stepfamilies. However, some investigators assessed the extent to which stepfamily processes are
similar to those of first-marriage families.

Several investigators reported similar processes among stepfamilies and first-marriage families
(Bogenscheider, 1997; O'Connor, Hetherington, & Reiss, 1998; Waldren, Bell, Peek, & Sorell,
1990). In fact, a substantial proportion of stable, longterm stepfamilies appear to function
similarly to nuclear families (O'Connor et al.; Vuchinich et al., 1991). For example, stepfathers'
involvement in supporting and monitoring stepchildren is related to better outcomes and more
satisfaction with family life for stepchildren (Henry & Lovelace, 1995), a finding that suggests
that the more a stepfather acts like a parent, the better it is for children. It may be that
stepfamilies come to function more similarly to first-marriage families over time. For example,
over a 2-year period, Vuchinich, Vuchinich, and Wood (1993) found that mother-stepfather-
preadolescent stepson triads became similar to first-marriage family triads in problem-solving
effectiveness. In both types of families, parental agreement facilitated problem solving, but
parental coalitions did not. Marital conflict also was not related to problem-solving effectiveness
in either type of family.

A few researchers reported differences in perceived family functioning between stepfamilies and
first-marriage families, with remarried individuals viewing their families as more stressful and as
less cohesive and adaptable and reporting less positive affect toward children than individuals in
first marriages (e.g., Waldren et al., 1990). Moreover, remarried couples used different coping
styles and were more likely to seek counseling than those in first marriages, and parents
generally were perceived to be closer to children than were stepparents (Brown, Green, &
Druckman, 1990; Waldren et al., 1990). Additionally, O'Connor et al. (1997) found that the
patterns of relations among newly formed stepfamilies were less "coherent'' than the pattern of
relationships in firstmarriage families. For example, mothers' marital positivity and positivity
toward their adolescent children were closely related in first-marriage families but were
unrelated in stepfamilies. Similarly, Fine and Kurdek (1995) found that the association between
stepparents' marital quality and the quality of the stepparent-stepchild relationship was stronger
than the relation between parents' marital quality and parent-child relationship quality,
suggesting that the boundary between marital and stepparent-stepchild dyads is more permeable
than the boundary between marital and parent-child dyads. These findings suggest that perhaps
unlike the case in first-marriage families, boundaries among subsystems in stepfamilies may be
differentially permeable for different stepfamily members. In contrast, Rogers (1996a) found that
marriage was less influential on mothers' parenting and children's outcomes in remarried families
than in first-marriage families. Clearly, more research is needed on how stepfamily processes
differ from those of first-marriage families.

Some researchers examined variations within stepfamilies. For example, legally remarried
stepfather families did not differ from first-marriage families in cognitive stimulation for
children, maternal warmth and responsiveness, and active structuring of the child's environment
(e.g., monitoring behaviors, parental involvement in school activities), but cohabiting stepfather
households differed from first-marriage families in these areas (Menaghan et al., 1997).
Relationship problems of mothers and their partners were related to children's behavior
problems, particularly when the partner and mother were not married.

In a comparison of family dynamics among stepfamilies and between different family structures,
Banker and Gaertner (1998) found that college students who were members of first-marriage
families or stepfamilies did not differ in perceived family harmony or in perceptions of being
treated equally to other children in the family by adults, although stepchildren rated their families
as less cooperative. The degree to which stepchildren perceived their stepfamily as one inclusive
group was related to stepfamily harmony. Factors that facilitated perceiving stepfamilies as one
unit instead of two included perceived cooperation, fair treatment of children by adults, and
positive relationships among stepkin.

Differences in processes generally were found between clinical and nonclinical samples of
stepfamilies. For example, compared with nonclinical stepfamilies, clinical stepfamilies were
characterized by less involvement between stepfathers and their stepchildren, stronger tendencies
toward relationship coalitions, more conflict, less emotional expressiveness, poorer problem
solving, less spousal individuation, lower marital quality, and more negative and less positive
parent-child interactions (Bray & Kelly, 1998). These characteristics were related to more
behavior problems and less prosocial behavior for stepchildren. In another study, stepfamilies in
therapy did not differ from other stepfamilies in the amount of stepparents' authority-related or
nurturing behaviors and couples' ability to communicate and resolve conflicts. However, they
differed in that stepchildren in nonclinical stepfamilies reciprocated appropriately rather than not
reciprocating to stepparents' behaviors, as in clinical stepfamilies (Brown et al., 1990). Also,
clinical stepfamilies reported more conflict and less expressiveness, and adults were less satisfied
with stepparents' role performances than in nonclinical stepfamilies.

Stepfamilies develop as family units in several different ways (Baxter, Braithwaite, & Nicholson,
1999). They also configure themselves in disparate ways--as reconstituted nuclear families, as
integrated but distinct cultures, as binuclear systems, and as separate units (Berger, 1995;
EreraWeatherly, 1996).

SOCIETAL VIEWS OF REMARRIAGE AND STEPFAMILIES

During the past decade, a body of work has emerged on societal views about stepfamilies. The
prevailing perspective on stepfamilies, shared cross-culturally, is that they are deviant and
harmful environments for children and adolescents (e.g., Ganong & Coleman, 1997; Levin,
1993). Stepfamilies are stigmatized through labels (Ganong, Coleman, & Kennedy, 1990),
stereotypes (e.g., Ganong & Coleman, 1995) and cultural myths (Coleman & Ganong, 1995;
Dainton, 1993). Also, the behaviors and attitudes of helping professionals are affected by
negative stereotypes about stepparents (Ganong & Coleman, 1997).

When stepfamilies are not stigmatized, they are often invisible to social systems--the policies and
practices of schools (Crosbie-Burnett, 1994) and youth organizations (Ganong, 1993) create
barriers to participation by stepfamily members because they are based on models of first-
marriage families. There is less consensus on the responsibilities that stepkin have to each other
than there is about family obligations between genetic kin (e.g., Ganong & Coleman, 1999),
indicating fewer norms about step-relationships. Given these findings, it is not surprising that
outsiders have more favorable impressions of stepfamilies who present themselves to others as
first-marriage families (Ganong et al., 1990), which could reduce stress for stepfamily members
(Dainton, 1993). It has been hypothesized that stepfamilies are adversely affected by being
viewed as inherently less functional than nuclear families (Ganong & Coleman, 1997), but there
have been few investigations of the effects of negative societal views on stepfamilies and
stepfamily members.

Legal Issues

Both social scientists and attorneys wrote prodigiously about the absence of legal relationships
between stepparents and stepchildren (e.g., Fine & Fine, 1992; Mason & Mauldon, 1996).
Because the legal system in the United States has not codified stepfamily members' rights and
responsibilities to each other, various solutions have been proposed, such as allowing a
stepparent or a nonmarital partner who has lived with a child and the custodial parent for at least
3 years to obtain a residence order that gives him or her almost the same authority as the parent
(Fine, 1997; Mason, 1998). The granting of a residence order to a stepparent does not remove
parents' rights or responsibilities; rather, it means that children have legal relations with three
adults. Whether such a law would work is unclear; changes in laws related to marital dissolution
and reconstitution are seldom informed by research on the effects of such changes.

An Overview of Conceptual and Methodological Trends

There has been an exponential increase over the course of the 20th century in stepfamily
research. The empirical knowledge base is broader, researchers from more disciplines and more
countries are making contributions, investigations are more theoretically grounded and
methodologically sophisticated, and more is known about stepfamilies apart from how they
compare to other types of families.

In several large research projects, remarriages and stepfamilies are either the primary or among
the main foci (e.g., the NEAD project, Kurdek's longitudinal project on marriages and
remarriages, the Virginia Longitudinal Study of Divorce and Remarriage, Hetherington and
Clingempeel's longitudinal study of stepfather households, Booth's longitudinal study of
marriages, the ALSPAC study). Other longitudinal projects (e.g., the Oregon Social Learning
Center study of at-risk youths) and national panel studies (e.g., NSFH) have allowed researchers
to look at changes in stepfamilies and in stepfamily members over time. Both the large-scale
national and regional data sets and the small-scale, intensive studies of whole households or
families have enabled researchers to examine more variables and to use more sophisticated
analytic strategies. In many of these studies, data have been gathered from multiple family
members. There were a few more observational studies (e.g., Vuchinich et al., 1991) this decade,
multiple methods of data collection were used more frequently than before, and there were more
qualitative studies (e.g., Erera-Weatherly, 1996; Pyke, 1994) using a variety of approaches (e.g.,
ethnography, dialectics, grounded theory). The research on stepfamilies also was characterized
by more frequent use of grand and midrange theories to explain phenomena and to test
theoretical propositions (e.g., Banker & Gaertner, 1998; O'Connor et al., 1997). These
developments in research and theory were facilitated by the increased number of researchers
interested in remarriage and stepfamilies and by the maturing of several programs of research.

In contrast to earlier decades, more researchers attempted to reflect the complexity of


stepfamilies in their work. Sex of the stepparent and stepchild, whether the household
configuration is simple (one stepparent) or complex (both adults are stepparents), years residing
together, socioeconomic status, and race were often included as control variables. Also, a few
researchers distinguished between remarried stepparents and cohabiting, de facto stepparents
(e.g., Bulcroft et al., 1998; Menaghan et al., 1997). However, despite the heightened sensitivity
to the structural diversity of stepfamilies, researchers continued to be challenged to adequately
describe the complicated dynamics and complex configurations of remarriages and stepfamilies.
For instance, only a few researchers took note of the multiple pathways to creating stepfamilies,
and samples did not always adequately reflect the diversity of step-relationships, either because
some types of stepfamilies were excluded or because potentially important distinctions between
types of stepfamilies were ignored. In addition, the interactions of stepfamily household
members with nonresidential parents or former spouses and other nonresidential family members
were usually ignored. Treating stepfamily households as if they were unaffected by family
members not sharing a residence full time leads to distorted conclusions about stepfamily
dynamics. Moreover, factors related to the larger social environments in which families reside
were not included. This was true for small-scale, local samples and for many large-scale
secondary data sets.

Some of the reasons why researchers disregarded stepfamily complexity are pragmatic. It is
prohibitively expensive to recruit as many stepfamilies as are needed to examine or control for
all relevant structural variables. Certain types of stepfamilies are hard to identify (i.e., members
of father-stepmother households may share a last name; some stepfamilies are reluctant to
identify themselves to researchers), so they end up being underinvestigated. Consequently,
stepfather households were often the only stepfamilies studied.

The extraordinary emphasis (i.e., more than 200 studies) placed on the effects on children of
living with a stepparent is probably only partially due to the importance of the issue. These
studies were possible because of the availability of large data sets and the ease with which family
structure and a variety of outcomes can be measured. Since at least the middle of the decade, it
has been safe to conclude that stepchildren are at somewhat greater risk for educational
difficulties and internalizing and for externalizing behavior problems than children living with
two parents, although the vast majority of stepchildren do not exhibit these problems.
Nonetheless, researchers continue to spend enormous efforts studying a similar set of child
outcomes using between-group designs that compare stepchildren to children in other family
structures. A typical approach is to examine the distribution of selected outcome variables by
family structure, control for various demographic characteristics, and sometimes family process
variables and then to see whether the relations between family structure and the outcome
variables persist. Although a useful way to determine group differences, this design enlightens
our understanding of family processes only incrementally. If data sets contain variables that fit
with investigators' explanatory models, it can potentially be determined why one group of
children differs from another based on certain predictor variables, but too often researchers are
left trying to infer causal relations from correlational data. Even longitudinal data sets, which can
help researchers make inferences about causality, may fall short because measures do not
adequately assess constructs in researchers' explanatory models.

The emphasis of too many studies has been on identifying problems within stepfamilies;
researchers focused heavily on only part of the findings (e.g., stepchildren were more likely than
those living with both parents to be depressed), while barely mentioning the rest (e.g., three
fourths of stepchildren were not clinically depressed). When small, but statistically significant,
effects are treated by many researchers as if they were large and generalizable to all stepfamily
members (Amato, 1994) or when extremist positions are taken (Cherlin, 1999), scholarship is not
well served. We want to note that many researchers were careful in their reporting and
interpreting of complicated findings, but to paraphrase Kuller's (1999) critique of circular
epidemiology, family journals are filled with well-conducted studies that primarily repeat known
findings or are variations on a theme. It is time for research on stepchildren to move on in ways
we suggest in the final section of this paper.

The use of the deficit comparison approach (Coleman & Ganong, 1990) continued into this
decade. Additionally, cultural values that depict the first-marriage family as the best or only
acceptable family structure in which to raise children no doubt influenced social scientists
(Clingempeel, Flescher, & Brand, 1987). Epistemic values that shape stepfamily research are as
heavily influenced by cultural and personal beliefs as they are by the guidelines and rules for
sound social science. For example, a between-group bias and deficit comparison approach taken
by many researchers has been popular in part because it is congruent with a socially approved
way of thinking about nonnuclear families as deficient and harmful to children. The functionalist
framework is not dead; it just has gone underground. There are many examples--Popenoe (1994)
recommends that the United States should discourage remarriage because stepchildren are at
risk, researchers compare stepfamilies to "intact" families, and sociobiologists devote an issue of
a major journal (Flinn, 1999) on studies of why stepfathers invest less in their stepchildren than
parents invest in children (adoptive fathers apparently escape such scrutiny, despite lacking
genetic connections to children).

Many areas of research suggested in the previous decade review remain relatively unexplored.
For example, too few studies focused on stepfamily processes, we continue to be limited in our
understanding of variables that contribute to positive stepfamily functioning, and we know little
about factors that facilitate the formation of positive stepparent-stepchild bonds, stepsibling
bonds, or remarried couple relationships. Knowledge of African American, Latino, and other
ethnic stepfamilies remains woefully inadequate as well. Perhaps even more surprising is the
small number of studies investigating mothers in stepfamilies. We know more about
nonresidential fathers than we do about residential mothers in stepfamily households.

On a more encouraging note is the increased attention to the legal aspects of stepfamily living,
although many questions about this have yet to be examined. Also encouraging is the increase in
information being derived from well-designed longitudinal studies, the interest shown by
researchers from communication (e.g., Baxter et al., 1999), and the application of theories and
methods not previously associated with stepfamily research (e.g., Bogenscheider, 1997;
O'Connor et al., 1997; Pyke, 1994).

CONCLUSIONS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR RESEARCH IN THE NEXT MILLENNIUM

In this final section, we offer suggestions to build upon the advances made in the past decade.
First, the clinical, empirical, and conceptual literatures on remarriage and stepfamilies have all
suggested that change over time is both critical and constant in the life course of stepfamilies.
Thus, we believe that longitudinal work is needed that assesses how stepfamily members'
experiences, perceptions, interactions, and adjustment change over time, both long term (years)
and shortterm (i.e., daily, weekly).

Second, we suggest that new insights on stepfamily dynamics will be gained by focusing more
attention on nontraditional stepfamilies, particularly cohabiting couples and gay or lesbian
couples with children. Because remarriage is the traditional gateway into a stepfamily,
cohabiting couples with children generally have been overlooked in studies on stepfamilies.
However, cohabiting couples with children are likely to have both similarities to (e.g., the
presence of a nongenetically related adult) and differences from (e.g., marriage has legal rights
and responsibilities that cohabiting arrangements do not) married couples. By investigating
similarities and differences, we can add greatly to our understanding of stepfamily dynamics.

Third, although progress was made in this domain, there remains a need for within-group
designs. We continue to need more studies of factors that contribute to healthy and adaptive
functioning in stepfamilies. To design more effective interventions and educational programs, we
need greater insights into the characteristics of well-functioning stepfamilies. Therefore, we
encourage researchers to use a variety of quantitative and qualitative methods and to more
frequently study a variety of processes in stepfamilies (e.g., parenting practices, marital conflict)
and how they relate to the adjustment of individuals.

Fourth, research would be enhanced by studying bidirectional effects (i.e., how stepchildren are
affected by their parents and stepparents and how parents and stepparents are affected by
stepchildren) (e.g., see Hetherington et al., 1999). Researchers generally have narrowly
conceptualized stepchildren as passive recipients of effects from parents and stepparents, but
there is evidence that stepchildren's responses to stepparents may be critical in developing
stepfamily relationships.

Fifth, because families are too often conceptualized as households, we call for more studies that
include nonresidential parents and other nonresidential family members. Researchers cannot
continue to assume that where a child lives is static rather than changing; there is ample evidence
that many children who do not share a household with both parents nonetheless live at various
times with both parents.

Sixth, we recommend more studies of how stepfamilies interface with other societal institutions.
Stepfamily members are clearly affected by social policies and by cultural attitudes, and they
affect change in social policies and cultural attitudes as well. Stepfamily members adopt roles
from cultural norms, and they also create their own roles that influence cultural attitudes and,
eventually, social policy.

Seventh, we strongly support theory-based research. Research increasingly has been grounded in
conceptual frameworks, but there is room for improvement. Advances in knowledge will occur
more rapidly if researchers both use and explicitly identify the theoretical considerations
underlying their work. Theoretical grounding yields numerous benefits, including clearer
definitions of key constructs, the potential for greater consensus in which measures might be
used to assess particular constructs, and greater acknowledgment of how different methods can
yield complimentary contributions to our understanding.

Finally, we encourage more qualitative work that examines the experiences, perceptions, and
reflections of stepfamily members. Quantitative studies have yielded tremendous benefits, but
we also need research that explores the mechanisms underlying some of the trends, patterns, and
relations found in quantitative work. We need studies of the meanings of experiences for people.
We could gain considerable insight from such qualitative approaches as in-depth interviewing
and grounded theory approaches to data analysis. In addition, the triangulation of methods
(qualitative and quantitative) can be used to better capture stepfamily processes. Future work
should combine biological, psychological, interpersonal, and cultural influences on individuals
and families. The increase in the volume of studies in the past decade has been phenomenal, the
increase in quality has been steady, and yet there is much to know to enhance our understanding
of stepfamilies.

REFERENCES

Amato, P. R. (1994). The implications of research findings on children in stepfamilies. In A.


Booth & J. Dunn (Eds.), Stepfamilies: Who benefits? Who does not? (pp. 81-87). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 62.

Amato, P. R., Rezac, S., & Booth, A. (1995). Helping between parents and young adult
offspring: The role of parental marital quality, divorce, and remarriage. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 57, 363-374.

Aquilino, W. S. (1991 a). Predicting parents' experiences with coresident adult children. Journal
of Family Issues, 12, 323-342.

Aquilino, W. S. (199 lb). Family structure and homeleaving: A further specification of the
relationship. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 999-1010.

Aquilino, W. S. (1994). Impact of childhood family disruption on young adults' relationships


with parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 295-313.

Astone, N. M., & McLanahan, S. S. (1991). Family structure, parental practices and high school
completion. American Sociological Review, 56, 309-320.

Astone, N. M., & Washington, M. L. (1994). The association between grandparental coresidence
and adolescent childbearing. Journal of Family Issues, 15, 574-589.
Banker, B. S., & Gaertner, S. L. (1998). Achieving stepfamily harmony: An intergroup-relations
approach. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 310-325.

Barber, B. (1994). Cultural, family, and personal contexts of parent-adolescent conflict. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 56, 375-386.

Barber, B. L., & Lyons, J. M. (1994). Family processes and adolescent adjustment in intact and
remarried families. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 23, 421436.

Baxter, L., Braithwaite, D., & Nicholson, J. (1999). Turning points in the development of
blended families. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16, 291-313.

Berger, R. (1995). Three types of stepfamilies. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 24, 35-50.

Biblarz, T. J., Raftery, A. E., & Bucur, A. (1997). Family structure and social mobility. Social
Forces, 75, 1319-1339.

Bogenscheider, K. (1997). Parental involvement in adolescent schooling: A proximal process


with transcontextual validity. Journal of Marriage and the Farnily 59, 718-733.

Booth, A., & Amato, P. R. (1991). Divorce and psychological stress. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 32, 396-407.

Booth, A., & Dunn, J. (Eds.). (1994). Stepfamilies: Who benefits ? Who does not? Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Booth, A., & Edwards, J. N. (1992). Starting over: Why remarriages are more unstable. Journal
of Family Issues, 13, 179-194.

Bray, J., & Kelly, J. (1998). Stepfamilies. New York: Broadway.

Bronstein, P., Stoll, M., Clauson, J., Abrams, C. L., & Briones, M. (1994). Fathering after
separation or divorce: Factors predicting children's adjustment. Family Relations, 43, 469-479.

Brown, S. L., & Booth, A. (1996). Cohabitation versus marriage: A comparison of relationship
quality. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 668-678.

Brown, A. C., Green, R.-J., & Druckman, J. (1990). A comparison of stepfamilies with and
without childfocused problems. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 556-566.

Buckle, L., Gallup, G. G., & Rodd, Z. A. (1996). Marriage as a reproductive contract: Patterns of
marriage, divorce, and remarriage. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 363-377.

Bulcroft, R., Carmody, D., & Bulcroft, K. (1998). Family structure and patterns of independence
giving to adolescents. Journal of Family Issues, 19, 404-435. Bumpass, L., Raley, R. K., &
Sweet, J. (1995). The changing character of stepfamilies: Implications of cohabitation and
nonmarital childbearing. Demography, 32, 425-436.

Bumpass, L., Sweet, J., & Castro Martin, T. (1990). Changing patterns of remarriage. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 52, 747-756.

Bumpass, L. L., Sweet, J. A., & Cherlin, A. (1991). The role of cohabitation in declining rates of
marriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 913-927.

Burgoyne, C. B., & Morison, V. (1997). Money in remarriage: Keeping things simple-and
separate. Sociological Review, 45, 363-395.

Capaldi, D. M., & Patterson, G. R. (1991). Relation of parental transitions to boys' adjustment
problems: I. A linear hypotheses. II. Mothers at risk for transitions and unskilled parenting.
Developmental Psychology, 27, 489-504.

Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Cherlin, A. J., & Kiernan, K. E. (1995). The long-term effects of parental
divorce on the mental health of young adults: A developmental perspective. Child Development,
66, 1614-1634.

Cherlin, A. (1978). Remarriage as an incomplete insti tution. American Journal of Sociology, 84,
634-650.

Cherlin, A. J. (1992). Marriage, divorce, remarriage. Social trends in the United States.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Cherlin, A. J., & Furstenberg, F. F. (1994). Stepfamilies in the United States: A reconsideration.
Annual Review of Sociology, 20, 359-381.

Cherlin, A. J. (1999). Going to extremes: Family structure, children's well-being, and social
science. Demography, 36, 421-428.

Cherlin, A. J., & Furstenberg, F. E, Chase-Lansdale, P., Kiernan, K., Robins, P. K., Morrison, D.
R., & Teitler, J. O. (1991). Longitudinal studies of the effects of divorce on children in Great
Britain and the United States. Science, 252, 1386-1389.

Chevan, A. (1996). As cheaply as one: Cohabitation in the older population. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 58, 656-667.

Church, E. (1999). Who are the people in your family? Stepmothers' diverse notions of kinship.
Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 31, 83-105.

Clarke, S. C., & Wilson, B. F. (1994). The relative stability of remarriages: A cohort approach
using vital statistics. Family Relations, 43, 305-310.
Clingempeel, G., Colyar, J., & Hetherington, E. M. 1994). Toward a cognitive dissonance
conceptualization of stepchildren and biological children loyalty conflicts: A construct validity
study. In K. Pasley & M. Ihinger-Tallman (Ed.), Stepparenting: Issues in theory, research, and
practice (pp. 151-174). Westport, CT: Greenwood.

Clingempeel, G., Flescher, M., & Brand, E. (1987). Research on stepfamilies: Paradigmatic
constraints and alternative proposals. In J. P. Vincent (Ed.), Advances in family intervention:
Assessment and theory (pp. 229-251). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic
Press. Coleman, M., & Ganong, L. (1990). Remarriage and stepfamily research in the 80s: New
interest in an old family form. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 925-940.

Coleman, M., & Ganong, L. (1995). Insiders' and outsiders' beliefs about stepfamilies:
Assessments and implications for practice. In D. Huntley (Ed.), Assessment of stepfamilies (pp.
101-112). American Counseling Association Press.

Coleman, M., Ganong, L., & Cable, S. (1997). Perceptions of stepparents: An examination of the
incomplete institutionalization and social stigma hypotheses. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage,
26, 25-48.

Coley, R. L. (1998). Children's socialization experiences and functioning in single-mother


households: The importance of fathers and other men. Child Development, 69, 219-230.

Cooksey, E. C., & Fondell, M. M. (1996). Spending time with his kids: Effects of family
structure on fathers' and children's' lives. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 693-707.

Cornman, J. M., & Kingson, E. R. (1996). Trends, issues, perspectives, and values for the aging
of the baby boom cohorts. Gerontologist, 36, 15-26. Coughlin, C., & Vuchinich, S. (1996).
Family experience in preadolescent and the development of male delinquency. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 58, 491-501.

Crosbie-Burnett, M. (1994). The interface between stepparent families and schools: Research,
theory, policy, and practice. In K. Pasley & M. Ihinger-Tallman (Eds.), Stepparenting: Issues in
theory, research, and practice (pp. 199-216). Westport, CT: Greenwood.

Crosbie-Bumett, M., & Giles-Sims, J. (1991). Marital power in stepfather families: A test of
normative-resource theory. Journal of Family Psychology, 4, 484496.

Crosbie-Burnett, M., & Giles-Sims, J. (1994). Adolescent adjustment and stepparenting styles.
Family Relations, 43, 394-399.

Dainton, M. (1993). The myths and misconceptions of the stepmother identity: Descriptions and
prescriptions for identity management. Family Relations, 42, 93-98.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. I. (1996). Violence against stepchildren. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 5, 77-80.

Day, R. D. (1992). The transition to first intercourse among racially and culturally diverse youth.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 749-762.

Dawson, D. A. (1991). Family structure and children's health and well-being: Data from the
1988 National Health Interview Survey on Child Health. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53,
573-584.

Deal, J. E., Stanley-Hagan, M., & Anderson, J. C. (1992). The marital relationships in remarried
families. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (2-3), Serial No.
227.

Demo, D. H., & Acock, A. C. (1993). Family diversity and the division of domestic labor: How
much have things really changed? Family Relations, 42, 323331.

Doherty, W., & Needle, R. (1991). Psychological adjustment and substance use among
adolescents before and after a parental divorce. Child Development, 62, 328-337.

Dorius, G. L., Heaton, T B., & Steffen, P. (1993). Adolescent life events and their association
with the onset of sexual intercourse. Youth and Society, 25, 323.

Downey, D. B. (1995). Understanding academic achievement among children in stephouseholds:


The role of parental resources, sex of stepparent, and sex of child. Social Forces, 73, 875-894.

Dronkers, J. (1994). The changing effects of lone parent families on the educational attainment
of their children in a European welfare state. Sociology, 28, 171191.

Dunn, J., Deater-Deckard, K., Pickering, K., O'Connor, T G., Golding, J., & the ALSPAC Study
Team. (1998). Children's adjustment and prosocial behaviour in step-, single-parent, and non-
stepfamily settings: Findings from a community study. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 39, 1083-1095.

Erera-Weatherly, P. (1996). On becoming a stepparent: Factors associated with the adoption of


alternative stepparenting styles. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 25, 155-174.

Fine, M. A. (1997). Stepfamilies from a policy perspective: Guidance from the empirical
literature. Marriage and Family Review, 26, 249-264.

Fine, M. A., Coleman, M., & Ganong, L. (1998). Consistency in perceptions of the stepparent
role among stepparents, parents, and stepchildren. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
15, 810-828.
Fine, M. A., & Fine, D. R. (1992). Recent changes in laws affecting stepfamilies: Suggestions
for legal reform. Family Relations, 41, 334-340.

Fine, M., Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (1997). The relation between role constructions and
adjustment among stepparents. Journal of Family Issues, 18, 503-525.

Fine, M., & Kurdek, L. (1992). The adjustment of adolescents in stepfather and stepmother
families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 725-736.

Fine, M., & Kurdek, L. (1994). A multidimensional cognitive-developmental of stepfamily


adjustment. In K. Pasley~ M. Ihinger-Tallman (Eds.), Stepparenting: Issues in theory, research,
and practice (pp. 1532). Westport, CT: Greenwood.

Fine, M. A., & Kurdek, L. A. (1995). Relation between marital quality and (step)parent-child
relationships quality for parents and stepparents in stepfamilies. Journal of Family Psychology,
9, 216-223.

Flinn, M. (1999). Growth and fluctuating asymmetry of stepchildren. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 20, 465-479.

Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., & Cherlin, A. J. (1991). Divided families: What happens to children when
parents part. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ganong, L. (1993). Family diversity in a youth organization: Involvement of single-parents


families and stepfamilies in 4-H. Family Relations, 42, 286-292.

Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (1994a). Adolescent stepchild-stepparent relationships: Changes


over time. In K. Pasley & M. Ihinger-Tallman (Eds.), Stepparenting: Issues in theory, research,
and practice (pp. 87106). New York: Greenwood.

Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (1994b). Remarried family relationships. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (1995). The content of mother stereotypes. Sex Roles, 32, 495-512.

Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (1997). How society views stepfamilies. Marriage and Family
Review, 26, 85106.

Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (1999). New families, new responsibilities: Intergenerational
obligations following divorce and remarriage. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Ganong, L., Coleman, M., Fine, M., & Martin, P. 1999). Stepparents' affinity-seeking and
affinitymaintaining strategies with stepchildren. Journal of Family Issues, 20, 299-327.

Ganong, L., Coleman, M., & Kennedy, G. (1990). The effects of using alternate labels in
denoting stepparent or .stepfamily status. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 453-463.
Gelles, R. J., & Harrop, J. W. (1991). The risk of abusive violence among children with
nongenetic caretakers. Family Relations, 40, 78-83.

Giles-Sims, J. (1997). Current knowledge about child abuse in stepfamilies. In I. Levin & M.
Sussman (Eds.), Stepfamilies: History, research, and policy (pp. 215-230). New York: Haworth.

Goldscheider, F., & Goldscheider, C. (1998). The effects of childhood family structure on
leaving and returning home. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 745-756.

Hanson, T L., McLanahan, S. S., & Thomson, E. (1996). Double jeopardy; Parental conflict and
stepfamily outcomes for children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 141-154.

Hawkins, A. J., & Eggebeen, D. J. (1991). Are fathers fungible? Patterns of coresident adult men
in maritally disrupted families and young children's well-being. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 53, 95872.

Henry, C. S., & Lovelace, S. G. (1995). Family resources and adolescent family life satisfaction
in remarried family households. Journal of Family Issues, 16, 765-786.

Hetherington, M. (1993). An overview of the Virginia longitudinal study of divorce and


remarriage with a focus on early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 39-56.

Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1992). Coping with marital transitions: A family
systems perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (2-
3,Serial No. 227).

Hetherington, E. M., & Henderson, S. H. (1997). Fa-thers in stepfamilies. In M. Lamb (Ed.), The
role of fathers in child development (3ra ed.) (Pp. 212-226). Erlbaum.

Hetherington, E. M., Henderson, S., & Reiss, D., in collaboration with Anderson, E., Bridges,
M., Chan, R., Insabella, G., Jodl, K., Kim, J., Mitchell, A., O'Connor, T, Skaggs, M., & Taylor,
L. (1999). Adolescent siblings in stepfamilies: Family functioning and adolescent adjustment.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64 (4,Serial No. 259). Malden,
MA: Blackwell.

Hobart, C. (1991). Conflict in remarriages. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 15, 69-86.

Hoffmann, J. P., & Johnson, R. A. (1998). A national portrait of family structure and adolescent
drug use. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 633-645.

Holden, K. C., & Kuo, H.-H. D. (1996). Complex marital histories and economic well-being:
The continuing legacy of divorce and widowhood as the HRS cohort approaches retirement.
Gerontologist, 36, 383-390.
Ihinger-Tallman, M., & Pasley, K. (1997). Stepfamilies in 1984 and today--a scholarly
perspective. In I. Levin & M. Sussman (Eds.), Stepfamilies: History, research, and policy (pp.
19-40). New York: Haworth.

Ishii-Kuntz, M., & Coltrane, S. (1992). Remarriage, stepparenting, and household labor. Journal
of Family Issues, 13, 215-233.

Jacobson, D. (1993). What's fair: Concepts of financial management in stepfamily households.


Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 19, 221-238.

Jacobson, D. (1995). Incomplete institution or culture shock: Institutional and processual models
of stepfamily instability. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 24, 3-18.

Kiernan, K. E. (1992). The impact of family disruption in childhood on transitions made in


young adult life. Population Studies, 46, 213-234.

Kim, J. E., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D. (1999). Associations among family relationships,
antisocial peers, and adolescents' externalizing behaviors: Gender and family type differences.
Child Development, 70, 1209-1230.

Kuller, L. (1999). Invited commentary: Circular epidemiology. American Journal of


Epidemiology, 150, 897-903.

Kurdek, L. A. (1991). Predictors of increases in marital distress in newlywed couples: A 3-year


prospective longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 27, 627-636.

Kurdek, L. (1999). The nature and predictors of the trajectory of change of marital quality for
husbands and wives over the first 10 years of marriage. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1283-
1296.

Kurdek, L., & Fine, M. (1993a). The relation between family structure and young adolescents'
appraisals of family climate and parenting behavior. Journal of Family Issues, 14, 279-290.

Kurdek, L., & Fine, M. (1993b). Parent and nonparent residential family members as providers
of warmth and supervision to young adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 245-249.

Kurdek, L. A., & Fine, M. A. (1995). Mothers, fathers, stepfathers, and siblings as providers of
supervision, acceptance, and autonomy to young adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 9,
95-99.

Lee, V., Burkam, D., Zimiles, H., & Ladewski, B. (1994). Family structure and its effect on
behavioral and emotional problems in young adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
4, 405-437.
Leung, J. J. (1995). Family configurations and students' perceptions of parental support for
schoolwork. Sociological Imagination, 32, 185-196.

Levin, I. (1993). Family as mapped realities. Journal of Family Issues, 14, 82-91.

Lissau, I., & Sorensen, T I. A. (1994). Parental neglect during childhood and increased risk of
obesity in young adulthood. Lancet, 343, 324-327.

MacDonald, W., & DeMaris, A. (1996). Parenting stepchildren and biological children: The
effects of stepparent's gender and new biological children. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 5-25.

Malkin, C. M., & Lamb, M. E. (1994). Child maltreatment: A test of sociobiological theory.
Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 25, 121-133.

Margolin, L. (1992). Child abuse by mothers' boyfriends: Why the overrepresentation? Child
Abuse and Neglect, 16, 541-551.

Marks, N. F. (1995). Midlife marital status differences in social support relationships with adult
children and psychological well-being. Journal of Family Issues 16, 5-28.

Marsiglio, W. (1992). Stepfathers with minor children living at home: Parenting perceptions and
relationship quality. Journal of Family Issues, 13, 195-214.

Mason, M. A. (1998). The modern American stepfamily: Problems and possibilities. In M.


Mason & A. Skolnick (Eds.), All our families: New policies for a new century (pp. 95-116). New
York: Oxford University Press.

Mason, M. A., & Mauldon, J. (1996). The new stepfamily requires a new public policy. Journal
of Social Issues, 52, 11-27.

McKenry, P., McKelvey, M., Leigh, D., & Wark, L. (1996). Nonresidential father involvement:
A comparison of divorced, separated, never married, and remarried fathers. Journal of Divorce
and Remarriage, 25, 1-13.

Mekos, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D. (1996). Sibling differences in problem behavior and
parental treatment in nondivorced and remarried families. Child Development, 67, 2148-2165.

Menaghan, E. G., Kowaleski-Jones, L., & Mott, F. L. (1997). The intergenerational costs of
parental social stressors: Academic and social difficulties in early adolescence for children of
young mothers. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 72-86.

Montalto, C. P., & Gerner, J. L. (1998). The effect of expected changes in marital status on labor
supply decisions of women and men. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 28, 25-51.
Montgomery, M. J., Anderson, E. R., Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1992).
Patterns of courtship for remarriage: Implications for child adjustment and parent-child
relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 686-698.

Murphy, M., Glaser, K., & Grundy, E. (1997). Marital status and long-term illness in Great
Britain. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, 156-164.

National Center for Health Statistics. (1993). 1988 marriages: Number of the marriage by of
bride by groom [Computer program]. Washington, DC: NCHS Computer Center.

Needle, R., Su, S., & Doherty, W. (1990). Divorce, remarriage, and adolescent substance use: A
prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 157-169.

Neff, J. A., & Schluter, T D. (1993). Marital status and depressive symptoms: The role of
race/ethnicity and sex. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 20, 137160.

Nielsen, L. (1999). Stepmothers: Why so much stress? A review of the research. Journal of
Divorce and Remarriage, 30, 115-148.

O'Connor, T G., Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1997). Systems and bidirectional
influences in families. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 491-504.

O'Connor, T G., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D. (1998). Family systems and adolescent
development: Shared and nonshared risk and protective factors in nondivorced and remarried
families. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 353-375.

O'Connor, T G., Picketing, K., Dunn, J., Golding, J., & the ALSPAC Study Team. (1999).
Frequency and predictors of relationship dissolution in a community sample in England. Journal
of Family Psychology, 13, 436-449.

Pasley, K., & Ihinger-Tallman, M. (1990). Remarriage in later adulthood: Correlates of


perceptions of family adjustment. Family Perspectives, 24, 263-274. Pasley, K., Koch, M., &
Ihinger-Tallman, M. (1993).

Problems in remarriage: An exploratory study of intact and terminated remarriages. Journal of


Divorce and Remarriage, 20, 63-83.

Pasley, K., Sandras, E., & Edmondson, M. (1994). The effects of financial management
strategies on the quality of family life in remarriage. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 15,
53-70.

Phillips, R. (1997). Stepfamilies from a historical perspective. In I. Levin & M. Sussman (Eds.),
Stepfamilies: History, research, and policy (pp. 5-18). New York: Haworth.
Pong, S.-L. (1997). Family structure, school context, and eighth grade math and reading
achievement. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, 734-746.

Popenoe, D. (1994). The evolution of marriage and the problem of stepfamilies. In A. Booth & J.
Dunn (Eds.), Stepfamilies: Who benefits? Who does not? (pp. 3-27). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Pyke, K. D. (1994). Women's employment as a gift or burden? Marital power across marriage,
divorce, and remarriage. Gender and Society, 8, 73-91.

Pyke, K., & Coltrane, S. (1996). Entitlement, obligation, and gratitude in family work. Journal of
Family Issues, 17, 60-82.

Richards, M., Hardy, R., & Wadsworth, M. (1997). The effects of divorce and separation on
mental health in a national UK birth cohort. Psychological Medicine, 27, 1121-1128.

Rodgers, B. (1994). Pathways between parental divorce and adult depression. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1289-1994.

Rodgers, B., Power, C., & Hope, S. (1997). Parental divorce and adult psychological distress:
Evidence from a national birth cohort: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 38, 867-872.

Rogers, S. (1996a). Marital quality, mothers' parenting and children's outcomes: A comparison
of mother/ father and mother/stepfather families. Sociological Focus, 29, 325-340.

Rogers, S. J. (1996b). Mothers' work hours and marital quality: Variations by family structure
and family size. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 606617.

Russell, A., & Searcy, E. (1997). The contribution of affective reactions and relationship
qualities to adolescents' reported responses to parents. Journal of Social & Personal
Relationships, 14, 539-548.

Salem, D., Zimmerman, M., & Notaro, P. (1998). Effects of family structure, family process, and
father involvement on psychosocial outcomes among African American adolescents. Family
Relations, 47, 331-341.

Seltzer, J. A. (1994). Intergenerational ties in adulthood and childhood experience. In A. Booth


& J. Dunn (Eds.), Stepfamilies: Who benefits? Who does not? (pp. 153-163). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Shapiro, A. D. (1996). Explaining psychological distress in a sample of remarried and divorced


persons: The influence of economic distress. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 186-203.
Shucksmith, J., Hendry, L. B., & Glendinning, A. (1995). Models of parenting: Implications for
adolescent well being within different types of family contexts. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 253-
270.

Skopin, A. R., Newman, B. M., & McKenry, P. (1993). Influences on the quality of stepfather-
adolescent relationships: View of both family members. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 19,
181-196.

South, S. J. (1991). Sociodemographic differentials in mate selection preferences. Journal of


Marriage and the Family, 53, 928-940.

Spitze, G., & Logan, J. (1992). Helping as a component of parent-adult child relations. Research
on Aging, 14, 291-312.

Stephen, E. H., Freedman, V. A., & Hess, J. (1993). Near and far: Contact of children with their
non-residential fathers. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 20, 171-191.

Suh, T, Schutz, C. G., & Johanson, C. E. (1996). Family structure and initiating non-medical
drug use among adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 5, 21-36.

Szinovacz, M. E. (1998). Grandparents today: A demographic profile. Gerontologist, 38, 37-52.

Teachman, J. D., Paasch, K., & Carver, K. (1996). Social capital and dropping out of school
early. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 773-783.

Thoits, P. A. (1992). Identity structures and psychological well-being: Gender and marital status
comparisons. Social Psychology Quarterly, 55, 236-256.

Thomson, E., McLanahan, S. S., & Curtin, R. B. (1992). Family structure, gender, and parental
socialization. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 368-378. Thornton, A. (1991). Influence
of the marital history of parents on the marital & cohabitational experiences of children.
American Journal of Sociology, 64, 868894.

U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1995). Statistical abstract of the United States: 1995. (ll5th ed.).
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Vuchinich, S., Hetherington, E. M., Vuchinich, R., & Clingempeel, G. (1991). Parent-child
interaction and gender differences in early adolescents' adaptation to stepfamilies.
Developmental Psychology, 27, 618626.

Vuchinich, S., Vuchinich, R., & Wood, B. (1993). The interparental relationship and family
problem-solving with preadolescent males. Child Development, 64, 1389-1400.
Waldren, T, Bell, N., Peek, C., & Sorell, G. (1990). Cohesion and adaptability in post-divorce
remarried and first-married families: Relationships with family stress and coping styles. Journal
of Divorce and Remarriage, 14, 13-28.

White, L. K. (1992). The effect of parental divorce and remarriage on parental support for adult
children. Journal of Family Issues, 13, 234-250.

Whitsett, D., & Land, H. (1992). Role strain, coping, and marital satisfaction of stepparents.
Families in Society, 73, 79-91.

Wilson, B., & Clarke, S. (1992). Remarriages: A demographic profile. Journal of Family Issues,
13, 123141.

Wineberg, H. (1990). Childbearing after remarriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 31-
38.

Wineberg, H., & McCarthy, J. (1998). Living arrangements after divorce: Cohabitation versus
remarriage. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 29, 131-146.

Wojtkewicz, R. A. (1993). Duration in parental structures and high school graduation.


Sociological Perspectives, 36, 393-414.

Wojtkiewicz, R. A. (1994). Parental structure experiences of children: Exposure, transitions, and


type at birth. Population Research & Policy Review, 13, 141-159.

Wu, Z. (1994). Remarriage in Canada: A social exchange perspective. Journal of Divorce and
Remarriage, 21, 191-224.

Wu, Z., & Penning, M. (1997). Marital instability after midlife. Journal of Family Issues, 18,
459-478.

Zill, N., Morrison, D. R., & Cioro, M. J. (1993). Longterm effects of parental divorce on parent-
child relationships, adjustment, and achievement in young adulthood. Journal of Family
Psychology, 7, 91-103.

You might also like