Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma
To cite this article: Komal Sharma-Patel , Elissa J. Brown & William F. Chaplin (2012) Emotional and
Cognitive Processing in Sexual Assault Survivors' Narratives, Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment &
Trauma, 21:2, 149-170, DOI: 10.1080/10926771.2012.639053
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 21:149–170, 2012
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1092-6771 print/1545-083X online
DOI: 10.1080/10926771.2012.639053
More than two decades of research has established the serious mental health
outcomes of sexual assault, with high rates of lifetime posttraumatic stress
disorder (PTSD), current PTSD, and depressive disorders (Koss, Figueredo, &
Prince, 2002; Ozer, Best, Lipsey, & Weiss, 2003). Additionally, a number of
studies have documented alcohol use disorders as common sequelae of sex-
ual assault survivors (e.g., Kilpatrick, Acierno, Resnick, Saunders, & Best,
149
150 K. Sharma-Patel et al.
1997; Ullman, Filipas, Townsend, & Starzynski, 2005). Given the widespread
use of alcohol among college-aged students (e.g., O’Hare, 2005) and subse-
quent risky sexual behaviors (e.g., Kilpatrick et al., 1997; O’Hare, 2005), it is
important to determine whether established trauma-based interventions are
effective in reducing risky alcohol use behaviors (Foa & Williams, 2010).
Likewise, an extensive body of research (Ehlers et al., 2010) has pro-
vided support for the efficacy and effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral
treatments for rape-related PTSD and depression, including prolonged expo-
sure (PE; Foa et al., 1999; Foa, Rothbaum, Riggs, & Murdock, 1991) and
cognitive processing therapy (CPT; Nishith, Resick, & Griffin, 2002; Resick &
Schnicke, 1992). Furthermore, treatment outcome studies with rape survivors
comparing PE to CPT have found unequivocal improvements in maladaptive
cognitions, PTSD, and depression (e.g., Nishith et al., 2002).
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
about safety, trust, esteem, intimacy, self, and others was related to post-
traumatic stress symptoms, providing support to the recent explication of
the cognitive processing theory that incorporated McCann, Sakheim, and
Abrahamson’s (1988; cf. Cason et al., 2002) themes. Ehlers and colleagues
also have presented findings on their cognitive theory with a recent empha-
sis on rumination as a “powerful predictor” of symptoms (e.g., Michael
et al., 2007; Steil & Ehlers, 2000). Collectively, these studies underscore
the need to extend the scope of investigations to both emotional activa-
tion and dysfunctional beliefs when examining mechanisms of change in
interventions.
1992). Klein and Boals (2001) conducted two studies of the written narra-
tive task and found that those undergraduates who wrote about negative
events (vs. positive and trivial) evinced reductions in intrusive and avoid-
ance symptoms at two-month follow-up that were linked to increased use
of causal words. Park and Blumberg (2002) reported reductions in perceived
threat (cognitive variable) and PTSD symptoms in the trauma-writing group
(vs. control condition) at four-month follow-up. In another study (Sloan,
Marx, Epstein, & Dobbs, 2008), rumination (cognitive processing variable)
moderated the effects of writing condition on depressive symptoms, with
improvements in symptoms persisting 6 months postwriting task for expres-
sive writing group (vs. control). Relatedly, Alvarez-Conrad, Zoellner, and
Foa (2001) transcribed trauma narratives reported during exposure therapy
by female sexual and physical assault survivors and found that linguistic vari-
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
ables measuring cognitive processes (e.g., causal and insight words) were
related to posttreatment change in PTSD, anxiety, and depressive symp-
toms. These studies offer a basis for this study’s methodology (i.e., linguistic
measure of cognitive processes) and aims.
Recent investigations (e.g., Hunt, Schloss, Moonat, Poulos, & Weiland,
2007) have reported preliminary results in the examination of emotional and
cognitive mechanisms. Epstein et al. (2005) examined changes in physiolog-
ical reactivity and linguistic indexes of cognition and emotion in participants
writing about an upsetting (stressful) or neutral event and found that those
in the former group (vs. control) exhibited significantly elevated heart rate
activity during the first session. This elevated reactivity was associated
with significant reductions in depressive symptoms at one-month follow-
up. Additionally, those writing about a stressful event used more negative
emotion words, more positive emotion words, and more insight or causal-
ity words relative to the control group. Limits included no measurement of
PTSD symptoms or trauma-specific cognitions. Finally, Hunt et al. (2007)
examined the effects of cognitive restructuring, emotional processing, and
their combination on depressive symptoms after the illness or death of a
pet. The combined condition evidenced greatest improvements in depres-
sive symptoms. Limitations of the study included a single assessment method
(linguistic) of emotional arousal and cognitive variables and the specificity
of the sample (pet owners).
THIS STUDY
Emerging research on the utility of the written narrative paradigm has shifted
from investigating whether the written narrative task is beneficial to under-
standing who benefits from it (ethnicity, age, individual differences) and
under what conditions (e.g., number of sessions, duration, and time between
sessions; Epstein et al., 2005; Sloan & Epstein, 2005; Sloan & Marx, 2004b).
To better understand the possible underlying mechanisms of change of the
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 155
METHOD
Participants
This investigation draws from a broader study that examined experiences of
sexual assault and perpetration, coping behaviors, and benefits of writing
about sexual assault (Sharma-Patel, 2010). The sexual assault screener was
completed by 461 undergraduate women attending a large urban university.
Attempted or completed sexual assault was endorsed by 118 (26%) partic-
ipants, of whom 38 (32%) consented to complete the narratives, 24 (63%)
attended the first session, and 19 (16%) completed all four sessions. A series
of analyses were conducted to examine whether there were significant dif-
ferences in age, sexual assault severity, and 15 coping tendencies between
those who were eligible in the second phase but refused and those who
consented; none of the analyses were significant (Sharma-Patel, 2010). Both
groups (consented to study and completers) were primarily of Caucasian
background. Demographic information on the 19 who completed the study
is presented in Table 1.
Measures
SEXUAL EXPERIENCES SURVEY
The Sexual Experiences Survey (SES; Koss & Gidycz, 1985) is a self-report
instrument designed to assess degree of sexual victimization of women. The
10 questions are administered in yes–no format and include victimization
156 K. Sharma-Patel et al.
Characteristic n %
Race/ethnicity
Caucasian/White 9 47.4
African American/Black 2 10.5
Latin American/Hispanic 2 10.5
Asian East 3 15.8
Asian South 1 5.3
Biracial 1 5.3
Other (multiracial) 1 5.3
Year in school (average age:
19.5 years, SD = 3.06)
First year 8 42.1
Sophomore 5 26.3
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
Junior 4 21.1
Senior 2 10.5
Mode of assessment
Classroom 10 52.6
Online 9 47.4
was used. LIWC–2 uses a word count strategy whereby it searches for more
than 2,300 words or word stems that previously have been categorized
by independent judges into 22 linguistic dimensions, including psycholog-
ical processes (e.g., positive and negative emotion, cognitive processes).
Pennebaker, Mehl, and Niederhoffer (2003) reviewed studies and reported
good internal consistency. In this study, the following category items were
included in the analysis: Positive Emotion Words, Negative Emotion Words,
Insight, and Undoing/Discrepancy (discrepancies reflected in use of words
like should, would, could).
Procedure
Participants were recruited from large undergraduate classrooms and the
online participant pool of the undergraduate psychology department.
At Time 1, researchers administered the consent form and SES. Women
who disclosed attempted or completed sexual assault were contacted by
the investigators and asked to participate in the writing task. Those who
agreed to participate in the narrative were administered a second consent
form at Time 2, informing them that they would be completing measures
of feelings, thoughts, and behaviors about a “traumatic or stressful event”
four times over four weeks, and writing about the event three times over
three weeks. Participants were informed that they could withdraw from the
study at any time. Following informed consent, participants completed the
battery of questionnaires (BDI, AUDIT, PSS–SR, PBRS, Trauma Characteristic
Survey). Participants then completed the narrative task as described next.
The Time 3 and 4 assessments consisted of the PBRS and narrative task. The
Time 5 assessment included the BDI, AUDIT, PSS–SR, and PBRS. Participants
received reminder calls for subsequent sessions. On completion of the
study, participants received a gift card in the amount of $20. In addition,
participants were provided with a debriefing and referral to psychotherapy
services if necessary.
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 159
WRITING TASK
Participants received the following instructions, adapted from Pennebaker
(1997) and Smyth et al. (2001):
we will allow you as much time as you want when you have finished
writing to compose yourself. It is important for you to know that your
name will not be connected in any way with your essay. As you write,
the timer will go off periodically, at which point you should rate your
feelings of anxiety on a scale of 1 to 10 on the form provided.
Participants completed the task in a private office and were asked to put
their completed essay in a marked envelope.
RESULTS
At baseline, 7 (37%) participants met criteria for PTSD and 12 (63%) did not.
Ten (52.6%) participants presented with BDI scores indicative of no depres-
sive symptoms, 2 (10.5%) with mild to moderate, 5 (26.3%) with moderate
to severe, and 2 (10.5%) with severe depressive symptoms. Six (32%) par-
ticipants presented with AUDIT scores indicative of alcohol dependence, 5
(26%) indicative of harmful or hazardous behaviors, and 8 (42%) with low
risk. Chi-square tests revealed no differences between those who dropped
out and completers on PSS–SR, Pearson’s χ 2 (1, N = 24) = .505, p > .05;
BDI, Pearson’s χ 2 (1, N = 24) = 20.62, p > .05; or AUDIT, Pearson’s χ 2 (1,
N = 24) = 14.25, p > .05. Most participants reported knowing the per-
petrator (82%; i.e., friend, boyfriend, acquaintance), 1 reported weapon
use (4.3%), and 4 (17.4%) reported physical injury. Time since assault
ranged from 0.5 to 6 years. In regard to alcohol use, the participants varied
greatly in the number of drinks they endorsed for themselves (M = 2.71,
SD = 3.62) and their assailants (M = 3.63, SD = 3.92). Nine participants
(47%) reported no alcohol use and 6 (32%) indicated that their assailant
had no alcohol at the time of the assault. No participant reported seeking
psychotherapy for the assault.
160 K. Sharma-Patel et al.
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Linguistic variables
Positive emotion words 3.51 1.22 3.79 1.11 3.62 1.16 —
Negative emotion words 2.70 1.45 2.88 1.13 3.08 1.44 —
Causal words 1.92 0.76 2.20 1.15 1.96 0.77 —
Insight words 2.95 1.04 3.21 0.80 3.39 1.07 —
Undoing/discrepancy 1.97 0.86 2.13 0.98 2.07 0.84 —
words
Symptom severity variables
PSS–SR re-experiencing 1.11 1.29 — — 2.00 1.94
scores
PSS–SR avoidance scores 3.79 3.43 — — 4.42 3.55
PSS–SR arousal scores 4.89 3.84 — — 3.89 3.59
PSS–SR total scores 9.79 7.64 — — 10.32 7.58
BDI total scores 12.63 10.83 — — 9.89 9.21
AUDIT total scores 9.21 6.49 — — 7.21 5.42
PBRS variables
Safety 5.31 0.95 — — 5.32 1.17
Trust 5.13 1.03 — — 5.29 1.23
Power 4.67 0.88 — — 5.07 0.89
Esteem 5.42 0.84 — — 5.53 1.00
Intimacy 5.09 0.82 — — 4.95 1.01
Note. N = 19. Linguistic variables derived from narrative task. PSS–SR = PTSD Symptoms Scale-
Self Report; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test;
PBRS = Personal Beliefs and Reactions Scale.
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 161
Participant 1 Participant 3
10 10
Time 2 (M = 3.8) Time 2 (M = 4.8)
9 9
Time 3 (M = 7.4) Time 3 (M = 0.8)
8 Time 4 (M = 1) 8 Time 4 (M = 0)
7 7
SUDS Rating
SUDS Rating
6 6
5 5
4 4
3 3
2 2
1 1
0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Writing Time Intervals Writing Time Intervals
Participant 10 Participant 13
10 10
Time 2 (M = 1.0) Time 2 (M = 6.4)
9 9
Time 3 (M = 0.8) Time 3 (M = 3.0)
8 Time 4 (M = 1.2) 8 Time 4 (M = 1.8)
7 7
SUDS Rating
SUDS Rating
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
6 6
5 5
4 4
3 3
2 2
1 1
0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Writing Time Intervals Writing Time Intervals
Participant 8 Participant 17
10 10
Time 2 (M = 4.6)
9 9
Time 3 (M = 3.2)
8 8 Time 4 (M = 2.4)
7 7
SUDS Rating
SUDS Rating
6 6
5 5
4 4
3 3
Time 2 (M = 8.2)
2 2
Time 3 (M = 6.8)
1 Time 4 (M = 4.4)
1
0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Writing Time Intervals Writing Time Intervals
FIGURE 1 Selected participants SUDS ratings across times and within writing time intervals
(color figure available online).
selected individual SUDS rating across times of assessment and within writ-
ing time intervals for 19 participants who completed the study and Figure 2
for an aggregate of all participants across time.
LINEAR MODEL
The first model (Model 2) assessed the effects of time of assessment, within
writing task time intervals, and Time × Within Writing Task interaction (fixed
factors) on SUDS ratings. The baseline intercept-only model (Model 1) was
significant, F(1, 20) = 53.28, p < .001. For Model 2, the average slope
for the time of assessment was significant when all other predictors were
taken into account. On average, participants reported initial SUDS of 3.02,
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 163
Aggregate of 19 Participants
10
Time 2 (M = 3.0)
9
Time 3 (M = 1.0)
8 Time 4 (M = 2.0)
SUDS Rating
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
1 2 3 4 5
Writing Time Intervals
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
F(1, 23) = 33.84, p < .001, and, at each subsequent time of assessment,
the initial reported SUDS decreased by .60, F(1, 38) = 7.19, p < .05. The
linear effect of within writing task intervals also was significant, indicating
that participants reported a significant increase in SUDS as they wrote about
their sexual assault experiences, F(1, 30) = 9.29, p < .01. The interaction
effect of writing task interval by time of assessment was not significant, F(1,
235) = 1.80, p > .05. Model 2 was a significantly better fit than the baseline
intercept only (Model 1), χ 2 (5, N = 285) = 125.29, p < .001.
CURVILINEAR MODEL
To test the hypothesis that participants’ reported SUDS ratings would
decrease within session after the initial increase (i.e., quadratic effect), time-
within-writing task was recoded in two variables: (a) the midpoint of the
writing task intervals was centered as the mean, and (b) a product term
of the centered midpoint squared. Model 3 was conducted with centered
time within writing task (Level 1), time of assessment (Level 2), and prod-
uct term (Level 3) as fixed variables as predictors of SUDS ratings as the
dependent variable. The average slope and time of assessment remained
significant, F(1, 23) = 77.56, p < .001, and F(1, 18) = 19.27, p < .001.
The effect of the writing task intervals (Level 1) also remained significant,
indicating an increase in SUDS ratings at the midpoint of the writing ses-
sions, F(1, 251) = 34.04, p < .001. The quadratic term was significant, F(1,
251) = 8.47, p < .01. The residual also was statistically significant, indicating
that the model can be improved. Model 3 was a better fit than the baseline
model, χ 2 (4, N = 285) = 80.942, p < .001. Model 2 (linear) was a bet-
ter fit than Model 3 with the smaller –2 Log Likelhood value (1127.553 vs.
1171.900).
164 K. Sharma-Patel et al.
DISCUSSION
The first goal of this study was to examine whether writing about sexual
assault at repeated time points would result in reductions in PTSD, depresso-
genic symptoms, and risky alcohol use behaviors. Consistent with previous
studies (e.g., Sloan & Marx, 2004a), participants who wrote about traumatic
experiences evidenced statistically significant reductions in symptoms of
depression. Not previously examined, statistically significant reductions in
alcohol use also were evidenced. No previous study has included alcohol
use as an outcome in examinations of the written narrative tasks. Contrary to
previous studies (e.g., Sloan et al., 2005), improvements in PTSD symptoms
were not observed, most likely due to low levels of PTSD symptomatology
at initial assessment or insufficient power. In explaining their similar findings
with participants diagnosed with PTSD, Smyth et al. (2008) suggested that
high-risk groups might require additional components (e.g., skills training)
to benefit from the writing task.
This study also examined patterns of emotional processing over the
course of writing about sexual assault, predicting that SUDS anxiety would
decrease across time subsequent to an initial increase “peak” during the first
assessment. This hypothesis was partially supported, with the mixed linear
model regressions revealing several important findings. One, participants’
first SUDS rating significantly decreased at each subsequent assessment.
This finding suggests that participants perceived significantly less stress as
they repeatedly wrote about their sexual assault experiences. Two, partici-
pants reported an increase in SUDS anxiety as they engaged in the narrative
task irrespective of the time of assessment (e.g., first vs. second vs. third
session). Although this finding was not initially predicted, it provides addi-
tional evidence for Foa’s emotional processing theory (Foa, Steketee, &
Rothbaum, 1989) as it reflects the activation of the affect associated with
the traumatic content, a critical processing component. Three, across writing
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 165
nitive distortions (Alvarez-Conrad et al., 2001; Klein & Boals, 2001). Most
likely, the sample size might have been too small to detect significant but
modest changes in cognitions. Alternatively, the writing task might not have
provided adequate time to facilitate the correction of dysfunctional cogni-
tions. In cognitive therapies, clients are taught to identify, self-monitor, and
challenge their assumptions through Socratic questioning and more adaptive
beliefs are generated and practiced. The writing task might be adequate only
for monitoring; the instructions might need to be tailored to elicit challenging
of the distortions (e.g., Kallay, Vaida, Borla & Opre, 2008).
There were several limitations of this study. Methodologically, despite
remuneration, recruitment was a significant barrier, with lower than antic-
ipated participation as compared to previous comparable studies (e.g.,
Sloan & Marx, 2004a). Although preliminary analyses on coping and sex-
ual assault severity did not yield significant differences between those who
did and did not participate in the second phase (Sharma-Patel, 2010), there
might have been significant differences on unmeasured constructs. Second,
the majority of analyses were conducted with only 19 participants; thus, all
quantitative analyses should be viewed with caution. Third, the study took
place over a one-month time span. The findings observed could represent
acute trends that dissipate over a longer period. Fourth, given the small
volunteer sample, the participants recruited for this study might have been
more open to exploring their sexual assault experiences. Also, despite the
supporting evidence from efficacy trials, the inclusion of a control condition
could have elucidated the effects of writing about sexual assault compared
to neutral events or other traumatic experiences.
In spite of limitations, there are a number of research and clinical
implications. This study is the first short-term longitudinal investigation of
the written narrative task on sexual assault utilizing multiple assessment
methods of the assault experience (subjective writing task, standard mea-
sure of assault-related cognitions) and inclusion of emotional and cognitive
processing measures (linguistic, self-report within and across times). The
166 K. Sharma-Patel et al.
REFERENCES
Alvarez-Conrad, J., Zoellner, L. A., & Foa, E. B. (2001). Linguistic predictors of trauma
pathology and physical health. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 159–170.
Babor, T. F., Biddle-Higgins, J. C., Saunders, J. B., & Monteiro, M. G. (2001). AUDIT:
The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for use in primary
health care (2nd ed.). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., Saunders, J., & Grant, M. (1989). AUDIT: The alco-
hol use disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary health care.
Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
Beck, A. T. (2005). The current state of cognitive therapy: A 40-year retrospective.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 953–959.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of
depression. New York: Guilford.
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 167
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck
Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology
Review, 1, 77–100.
Brewin, C. R., & Holmes, E. A. (2003). Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress
disorder. Clinical Psychological Review, 23, 339–376.
Brillon, P., Marchand, A., & Stephenson, R. (1999). Influence of cognitive factors
on sexual assault recovery: Descriptive review and methodological concerns.
Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 28(3), 119–137.
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Neill, W. T. (2000). Information processing and
PTSD: A review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 20,
1041–1065.
Cason, D. R., Resick, P. A., & Weaver, T. L. (2002). Schematic integration of traumatic
events. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 131–153.
Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2001). A prospective investigation of the
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
834–847.
Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). Expressive writing can increase working memory
capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 520–533.
Kloss, J. D., & Lisman, S. A. (2002). An exposure-based examination of the effects
of written emotional disclosure. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 31–46.
Koss, M., Figueredo, A. J., & Prince, R. J. (2002). Cognitive mediation of rape’s
mental, physical and social health impact: Tests of four mediation analyses in
cross-sectional data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 926–941.
Koss, M. P., & Gidycz, C. A. (1985). Sexual Experiences Survey: Reliability and
validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 422–423.
Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Ohman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and
human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137–159.
Lange, A., Rietdijk, D., Hudcovicova, M., van de Ven, J., Schrieken, B., &
Emmelkamp, P. (2003). Interapy: A controlled randomized trial of the stan-
dardized treatment of posttraumatic stress through the Internet. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 901–909.
Lange, A., van de Ven, J., Schrieken, B., & Emmelkamp, P. (2001). Interapy:
Treatment of posttraumatic stress through the Internet: A controlled trial.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 32, 73–90.
McCann, I. L., Sakheim, D. K., & Abrahamson, D. J. (1988). Trauma and victim-
ization: A model of psychological adaptation. The Counseling Psychologist, 16,
531–594.
Mechanic, M., & Resick, P. (2002). The Personal Beliefs and Reactions Scale.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Missouri, St. Louis, MO.
Michael, T., Halligan, S., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2007). Rumination in
posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 24, 307–317.
Nishith, P., Resick, P. A., & Griffin, M. G. (2002). Pattern of change in prolonged
exposure and cognitive-processing therapy for female rape victims with post-
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70,
880–886.
O’Hare, T. (2005). Risky sex and drinking contexts in freshman first offenders.
Addictive Behaviors, 30, 585–588.
Owens, G. P., & Chard, K. M. (2001). Cognitive distortions among women reporting
child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 178–191.
Emotional and Cognitive Processing 169
Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic
stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin,
129, 52–73.
Park, C. L., & Blumberg, C. J. (2002). Disclosing trauma through writing: Testing the
meaning-making hypothesis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 597–616.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic
process. Psychological Science, 8, 162–166.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an
understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95,
274–281.
Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2007). Linguistic Inquiry and Word
Count (LIWC–2): LIWC 2007. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects
of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology,
Downloaded by [University of Delaware] at 14:50 06 October 2014
54, 547–577.
Resick, P. A., & Schnicke, M. K. (1992). Cognitive processing therapy for sexual
assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(5), 748–756.
Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M.
(1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Screening Test (AUDIT):
WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol
consumption. II. Addiction, 88, 791–804.
Sharma-Patel, K. (2010). An examination of the narrative writing task: Emotional
and cognitive mechanisms of change in sexual assault survivors. Dissertation
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 70(11).
Sloan, D. M., & Epstein, E. M. (2005). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia predicts written
disclosure outcome. Psychophysiology, 42, 611–615.
Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2004a). A closer examination of the structured writ-
ten disclosure procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72,
165–175.
Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2004b). Taking pen to hand: Evaluating theories underly-
ing the written disclosure paradigm. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,
11, 121–137.
Sloan, D. M, Marx, B. P., & Epstein, E. M. (2005). Further examination of the expo-
sure model underlying the efficacy of written emotional disclosure. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 549–554.
Sloan, D. M., Marx, B. P., Epstein, E. M., & Dobbs, J. L. (2008). Expressive writing
buffers against maladaptive rumination. Emotion, 8, 302–306.
Smyth, J. M., Hockemeyer, J. R., & Tulloch, H. (2008). Expressive writing and post-
traumatic stress disorder: Effects on trauma symptoms, mood states, and cortisol
reactivity. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 85–93.
Smyth, J. M., True, N., & Souto, J. (2001). Effects of writing about traumatic expe-
riences: The necessity for narrative structuring. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 20, 161–172.
Soliday, E., Garofalo, J. P., & Rogers, D. (2004). Expressive writing intervention
for adolescents’ somatic symptoms and mood. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 33, 792–801.
170 K. Sharma-Patel et al.