Bibliografía Central - Control Aversivo
Bibliografía Central - Control Aversivo
Bibliografía Central - Control Aversivo
Control 6
1. Distinguish between positive and negative punishment.
2. Investigate negative reinforcement as the basis of escape and avoidance.
3. Discover how reduction in shock frequency regulates avoidance.
4. Inquire about learned helplessness induced by inescapable aversive stimuli.
5. Distinguish between respondent and operant aggression.
6. Learn about coercion and its negative side effects in our society.
Aversive stimuli are events or happenings that organisms escape from, evade, or avoid. Insect
stings, physical attacks, foul odors, bright light, and very loud noises are common events that organ-
isms are prepared to evade on the basis of phylogeny. Escaping or avoiding these primary aversive
stimuli was adaptive, presumably because those animals, which acted to remove or prevent contact
with these events, more often survived and reproduced. In other words, organisms do not learn how
to react to aversive stimuli; they are biologically prepared to avoid or escape such events.
Other stimuli acquire aversive properties when associated with primary aversive events during
an animal's lifetime. For people, conditioned aversive stimuli (Save) include verbal threats, public
criticism, a failing grade, a frown, and verbal disapproval. To affect behavior, these events usually
depend on a history of punishment. A 1-week-old infant is not affected by a reprimand such as
"Don't do that!" By the time the child is 2 years old, however, the command may stop the toddler
from tearing pages out of your favorite book. Animals also learn responses to conditioned stimuli
as aversive events. People commonly shout "No!" when pets misbehave, and this auditory stimulus
eventually reduces the probability of the response it follows (e.g., chewing on your new chair).
175
176 Aversive Control of Behavior
At the institutional level, it would seem to be quite possible to eliminate the use of physical punish-
ment. Conceivably, administrative regulations could be altered such that public punishment in the
form of flogging, spankings, or other physical abuse would be excluded. At the level of individual
behavior, it seems somewhat more difficult but still not impossible to eliminate the use of physical
punishment. One type ofpunishment, however, seems to be virtually impossible to eliminate, and that
is the punishing contingencies that are arranged by the physical world. Whenever we interact with
the physical world, there are many punishing contingencies awaiting us. . . . Elimination of punishing
contingencies by the physical world would appear to require elimination of all behavior that involves
interaction with the physical world.
(Azrin & Holz, 1966, p. 438, emphasis added)
At least in the physical world, punishment is a fact of life. With regard to the social world,
Sidman (2001) has documented our excessive reliance on coercion to control human behavior. The
excessive use and advocacy of punishment by some groups is illustrated by the beating of children
as a form of Christian discipline. In 2010, CBS News reported the beating to death of 7-year-old
Lydia Schatz by her adopted parents. The beatings of Lydia and her 11-year-old sister, who recov-
ered from her injuries, ironically took place in Paradise, California. The report stated:
Aversive Control of Behavior 177
CHICO, Calif. (CBS/KOVR) Three years ago, Kevin Schatz and his wife Elizabeth did some-
thing so noble, a local television station featured them; the pair decided to adopt three children from
Liberia. Now, they're accused of killing one of the children because she mispronounced a word. . . .
Prosecutors say that the California couple used quarter-inch plastic tubing to beat their seven-year-
old adopted daughter to death. Apparently, they got the idea from a fundamentalist Christian group,
which promotes this as a way of training children to be obedient. Butte County District Attorney
Mike Ramsey says for several hours the 7-year-old was held down by Elizabeth and beaten dozens of
times by Kevin on the back of her body, which caused massive tissue damage.
(Martinez, 2010; CBS News)
Subsequently, the couple pleaded guilty and Kevin Schatz was sentenced to 22 years in prison for
murder, torture, and misdemeanor cruelty to a child. Elizabeth Schatz received a sentence of 13 years
and 4 months for voluntary manslaughter, infliction of unlawful corporal punishment, and misde-
meanor cruelty to a child (full story on CNN video; Tuckman, 2011). In the USA, for the year 2009
there were more than two million investigations of child maltreatment (mostly neglect and physical
abuse), of which about 25% were substantiated (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,
Administration for Children & Families, 2009). The use of punishment and aversive control in child
rearing is a pervasive practice in American culture (Figure 6.2), and is well documented (see Gershoff,
2002 and Straus, 2001 on the use of corporal punishment in America; also Park, 2002 points to the
difficulty of isolating the effects of parental punishment from a "package" of disciplinary tactics).
perpetrator. A behavior analysis suggests that this erratic shift in behavior from abuse to displays
of affection serves as intermittent reinforcement for responding in the relationship (staying) and
ensures that these staying responses become resistant to extinction—increasing the probability that
a woman usually will choose to stay in the abusive relationship. Staying in the abusive relationship
also may be maintained by negative reinforcement, which arises from a pattern of mounting conflict
and tension followed by a "cooling off' period with reduction in the immediate threat (for more on
negative reinforcement and conditioned reinforcement in the violence cycle, see Miller et al., 2012).
Thus, intermittent positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement operate to establish and main-
tain staying in the abusive relationship by the victims of domestic violence.
Surprisingly, punishment of victim's staying in the relationship by the perpetrator actually may
decrease in effectiveness over time, as the punitive contingencies often violate the principles of
effective punishment (see "How to Make Punishment Most Effective" in this chapter). For example,
punishment is most effective when introduced at high intensity, but survivors of domestic violence
report that the abuse did not happen suddenly overnight, but came on gradually from a man who
was previously appropriate, courteous, and thoughtful. In this scenario, punishment begins with less
severe forms of verbal abuse, slapping, and striking that escalate in severity. One implication is that
punishment arranged by the abuser may not be effective in reducing behavior (responding in the
relationship) upon which it is contingent, but only succeeds in inflicting severe physical injuries and
suffering on the victim.
Although punishment of staying may be relatively ineffective in suppressing this behavior, the
response costs for alternative behavior (responding outside the relationship) operate as punishment to
deter leaving the abusive relationship. Costs of leaving would include all the arrangements and effort
required to obtain food, shelter, and security; the social costs imposed by judgments and behavior
of family and friends; and fear of pursuit and intimidation by the abuser. Overall, domestic violence
involves a complex interplay of behavioral contingencies both within and outside of the abusive rela-
tionship. Behavioral interventions often focus on changing the conditions that influence the victim's
stay/leave behavior by altering contingencies and changing motivational operations (Miller et al., 2012).
Given the acceptance and pervasiveness of punishment in our society, it is notable that today
there is almost no basic research on aversive control and how best to minimize its side effects and
reduce its prevalence (Catania, 2008). Most studies of the basic principles of punishment (and neg-
ative reinforcement) were conducted in the 1960s and 1970s, at which point studies of punishment
almost stopped. A major reason for this was that ethics committees at universities, influenced by
changing cultural values and the animal activist movement, made it almost impossible to conduct
experiments on punishment, believing it to be inhumane to subject animals to punishment proce-
dures. In this chapter, we present the accumulated findings on punishment and other forms of aver-
sive control, assuming that this knowledge about the effects and side effects of aversive control is a
better strategy for improving the human condition than continued ignorance of the facts.
CONTINGENCIES OF PUNISHMENT
When a behavioral contingency results in a decrease in the rate of response, the contingency is
defined as, and called, punishment. Any event or stimulus that decreases the rate of operant behav-
ior is called a punisher. Figure 6.3 makes it clear that it is the relationship between the consequence
and its effects on behavior that defines the contingency (see Sidman, 2006, p. 138, for an alternative
definition of punishment by Skinner that does not include a decrease in behavior). At this point, we
now discuss contingencies of punishment; negative reinforcement is addressed later in this chapter
Aversive Control of Behavior 179
Positive Punishment
Positive punishment occurs when a stimulus is presented following an operant and the operant
decreases in frequency. The contingency of positive punishment is shown in cell 2 of Figure 6.3.
When a parent spanks a child for running into the street and the child stops doing it, this is positive
punishment. Of course technically, spanking is functioning as punishment only if it decreases the
probability of running into the street. This is an important point because in usual language people
talk about punishment without considering its effects on behavior. For example, you may shout
and argue with another person when she expresses a particular political position. Your shouting is
positive punishment only if the other person stops (or decreases) talking about politics. In fact, the
person may increase her rate of political conversation (as often happens in arguments). In this case,
you have actually reinforced rather than punished arguing with you. Thus, positive punishment is
defined as a decrease in operant behavior produced by the presentation of a stimulus that follows it.
By this functional definition, punishment always works.
Although Foxx and Azrin (1972) viewed overcorrection as an "educational" procedure, reviews
of the literature make it clear that both restitution and positive practice involve behavioral effects
similar to punishment imposed by electric shock (Mackenzie-Keating & McDonald, 1990). Note
that overcorrection works to instill compliance with the rules and commands of an authority (par-
ents, teacher, or staff). When the wrongdoer fails to comply, the authority adds other punishers such
as physical restraint or removal from the situation. These backup punishers make overcorrection an
effective punishment contingency. Even so, overcorrection includes additional procedures such as
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior or extinction, which contribute to the effectiveness
of overcorrection as an intervention package. To date, detailed component analysis of the "package"
has not been a primary focus of applied behavioral research. Behavior targeted for punishment,
however, is typically maintained by reinforcement, which decreases when the punished behavior
stops. Thus, the density of reinforcement in a person's life declines and life often gets worse with
the use of punishment in a treatment program. One strategy is to arrange alternative sources of
high-density reinforcement whenever punishment is used as part of a treatment program, including
programs using overcorrection (Cautela, 1984).
Negative Punishment
Negative punishment is portrayed in cell 4 of Figure 6.3. When an ongoing stimulus is removed con-
tingent on a response and this removal results in a decrease in the rate of behavior, the contingency
is called negative punishment (or omission). In other words, if the organism responds, the stimulus
is taken away and behavior decreases. A hungry bird is given continuous access to food, but if it
pecks the key, food is removed. A child is watching TV, but if the she runs around, the television is
turned off. A driver has earned money and is fined for speeding. In these cases, positive reinforce-
ment (i.e., provision of food, TV is turned on, or earned money) is removed contingent on behavior,
and the behavior decreases.
Negative punishment is often confused with extinction. Extinction occurs when a previ-
ously reinforced response no longer produces reinforcement. In this case, a response has pro-
duced reinforcement; extinction for that response
is in effect when the response —> reinforcer contin-
gency is discontinued. A pigeon may peck a key for
food, but when extinction is programmed, pecking
no longer produces food reinforcement. Similarly, a
sr+ child may be allowed to watch a favorite television
show after completing homework assignments. But
R1 T if the TV is broken the contingency is no longer in
R2 effect and doing homework is on extinction.
Relativity of Punishment
In Chapter 4, we discussed the principle of reinforcement and the Premack principle (Premack,
1959, 1962). The principle states that the opportunity to engage in a higher-frequency behavior
will reinforce a lower-frequency response. For Premack, reinforcement is relative, not absolute.
Subsequently, he extended this principle to the relativity of punishment (Premack, 1971). Con-
sider a rat that can run in an activity wheel and drink water from a tube. The wheel apparatus is
modified so that a brake can be activated, locking the wheel and preventing the rat from running In
addition, a motor is installed that permits the wheel to rotate at a set speed, forcing the rat to run. In
this modified apparatus, withholding running while giving free access to water makes running the