This book review summarizes the key points of the book "A Study of Thinking" by Jerome Bruner and his colleagues. The book presents the results of 5 years of research on concept formation and categorization. It describes the process of concept formation through a series of experiments using different materials and conditions. The experiments analyzed subjects' strategies for categorization and how these strategies were affected by limitations, probabilistic cues, and different materials like geometric shapes versus thematic scenes. The review provides detailed descriptions of the experimental procedures and "ideal strategies" used as a benchmark for analyzing subject performance. It concludes that the book makes an important contribution by focusing on describing the process of concept formation rather than interpreting results through existing theories.
This book review summarizes the key points of the book "A Study of Thinking" by Jerome Bruner and his colleagues. The book presents the results of 5 years of research on concept formation and categorization. It describes the process of concept formation through a series of experiments using different materials and conditions. The experiments analyzed subjects' strategies for categorization and how these strategies were affected by limitations, probabilistic cues, and different materials like geometric shapes versus thematic scenes. The review provides detailed descriptions of the experimental procedures and "ideal strategies" used as a benchmark for analyzing subject performance. It concludes that the book makes an important contribution by focusing on describing the process of concept formation rather than interpreting results through existing theories.
This book review summarizes the key points of the book "A Study of Thinking" by Jerome Bruner and his colleagues. The book presents the results of 5 years of research on concept formation and categorization. It describes the process of concept formation through a series of experiments using different materials and conditions. The experiments analyzed subjects' strategies for categorization and how these strategies were affected by limitations, probabilistic cues, and different materials like geometric shapes versus thematic scenes. The review provides detailed descriptions of the experimental procedures and "ideal strategies" used as a benchmark for analyzing subject performance. It concludes that the book makes an important contribution by focusing on describing the process of concept formation rather than interpreting results through existing theories.
This book review summarizes the key points of the book "A Study of Thinking" by Jerome Bruner and his colleagues. The book presents the results of 5 years of research on concept formation and categorization. It describes the process of concept formation through a series of experiments using different materials and conditions. The experiments analyzed subjects' strategies for categorization and how these strategies were affected by limitations, probabilistic cues, and different materials like geometric shapes versus thematic scenes. The review provides detailed descriptions of the experimental procedures and "ideal strategies" used as a benchmark for analyzing subject performance. It concludes that the book makes an important contribution by focusing on describing the process of concept formation rather than interpreting results through existing theories.
tive, but merely to indicate where supplementation from an instructor
or additional reading is needed. JAMES C. REED Wayne State University A Study of Thinking, by Jerome S. Bruner, Jacqueline J. Goodnow, and George A. Austin. New York: Wiley, I956. Pp. xi + 330. IN 4 Study of Thinking Jerome Bruner and his associates of the Harvard Laboratory of Social Relations present the outcome of a five year project on concept attainment or categorizing. Concept formation has been the subject of numerous investigations, and one might wonder why these Harvard researches merit publication in book form. In a review of concept formation experiments (6), W. E. Viancke opined that concept formation is poorly understood despite the considerable amount of research time devoted to the topic, and he suggests that the methods of investigation have been better adapted to showing the results of categorizing than to revealing its nature. The studies of Bruner and his coworkers have been directed toward this neglected aspect: the process of concept formation. Their approach has been chiefly descriptive. One does not find discussions in terms of systematic positions such as those of learning or personal- ity theories. The general nature of categorizing is considered in the first three chapters. Some points of this discussion follow. Understanding of categorizing illuminates a wide range of problems in psychology, not only in the areas of concept formation and perception; judgment, memory, problem-solving, inventive thinking, and esthetics involve categorizing. We base our categorizing on attributes which vary in their preferential value and validity, but preference need not be based on validity. For example, under pressure of time we rely upon the most readily available cues, and these may not be the most valid. Categories defined by the joint presence of attributes (conjunctive) i.e. large red triangle, seem the most &dquo;natural&dquo; type of category, but classes may be defined on an either-or basis (disjunctive), i.e. either large figure or triangle. A review of the experimental literature is not presented. This decision was &dquo;made much easier by the recent appear- ance of reviews&dquo; by Viancke, Johnson, Leeper, and Humphrey. The remaining chapters present the concept attainment experi- ments. All subjects were students at Harvard or nearby colleges. With the exception of studies on categories based on probabilistic cues, investigations followed the procedure described below. The nature of the task was carefully explained: to attain one concept of a certain kind (conjunctive or disjunctive). The subject was shown which attributes should be considered, and he was given examples of possible concepts. These procedures were guided by the aim of show- I58 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT
ing concept attainment at its best. In some cases the experimenter
presented a predetermined series of instances, while in other studies the subject selected instances to test. The investigators considered it important that the decisions of the subject during concept attain- ment be externalized. Sometimes the subject was required to guess the concept after each instance, otherwise the subject was told to &dquo;feel free&dquo; to state an hypothesis. The authors argue that the classical rote learning method of Hull (4) obscures the process of concept attainment; their method shows concept formation with the per- ceptual-abstraction phase by-passed. E announced after each instance whether it was positive or negative. The behavior of subjects during concept attainment is described in reference to ideal strategies. An ideal strategy is basically an analytic device used as a yardstick against which to compare the performance of human operators in the situations we set them. It is our conception of the &dquo;pure form&dquo; of the kind of behavior we have been observing: the way we would set a computer to do what the subject appears to be doing (i, p. 241). The ideal strategies are sets of rules for acquiring, retaining and utilizing information in a series of instances. These strategies meet three objectives with varying degrees of success. One objective is to gain from each instance the maximum amount of information con- cerning the concept. In concept attainment tasks the mental book- keeping may be considerable, so an objective of a strategy is to manage cognitive strain. The third aim is the regulation of risk; some strategies involve the slow working out of the concept with sure attainment after a certain number of trials, while others permit one to gamble, so with luck the category is attained very quickly. For an example of an ideal strategy, consider the case where the experimenter presents a predetermined series of instances. The rules of one of the strategies-the wholist strategy-are: (a) begin with a positive instance in toto as the hypothesis; (b) when a positive instance is encountered which the hypothesis did not correctly categorize, take as your new hypothesis what the old hypothesis and the instance have in common; (c) ignore all other instances. Of the subjects who gave an initial hypothesis according to the rules of the wholist strategy, half followed the wholist rules perfectly on subse- quent choices; the rest appeared to use the wholist approach despite occasional deviations. The central problem of the research was to describe how various conditions affected the use of strategies. In one study, S had reference to a display of all possible instances, positive and negative. S was required to arrive at the concept by selecting instances. After two problems had been solved with the display, S was required to solve a third concept without it. The purpose of this study was to see how different strategies fared under increased cognitive strain. In another BOOK REVIEWS I59 investigation, subjects were drastically limited in the number of instances they could test, and an increased use of risk taking strategies was observed. When thematic material (parent and child scenes) was substituted for geometric designs, a considerable change in strategies was noted. With the thematic material certain hypotheses were
especially salient. The preference for testing &dquo;reasonable&dquo; hypothesis
with thematic material resulted in utilization of less effective strategies than for abstract materials. Disjunctive concepts proved relatively difficult for subject to handle, despite the fact that the nature of either-or concepts was carefully explained. Effective strategies required that S lean heavily on information from negative instances- what the category is not. S’s frequently proved either unwilling or unable to utilize this indirect information. Many persisted in employ- ing inappropriate strategies based on positive instances. Several studies are reported on categorizing with probabilistic cues. R. E. Goodnow required subjects to assign aircraft silhouettes to one of two classes. Examples of each type were demonstrated and the relevant cues were pointed out. E determined that some cues would have only partial validity, e.g., E placed silhouettes with straight tail 67 per cent of the trials in one category and the remaining 33 per cent in the other. That preference for cues differs from validity of cues was apparent from the data, e.g. a number of subjects treated the completely valid cue as if it were less than certain, and the cue with 67 per cent validity was actually used to predict the category 80 per cent of the trials. For one group, after a number of trials E discontinued validation (i.e. stopped reporting which category the silhouette belonged to). Without the validation S tended to use partially valid cues on an all-or-none basis. A number of previously published experiments on categorizing with event frequency as the only cue are discussed, e.g. Jarvik’s experiment (5) where S predicted whether E would say &dquo;check&dquo; or &dquo;plus,&dquo; or J. J. Goodnow’s study (3) in which S decided whether to bet on the right or the left key of a gambling device. In some of these two-choice problems S shows &dquo;event matching&dquo;: the proportion he predicts one of the two alternatives will occur is the same as the pro- portion of times the event occurs. The explanation offered for event matching is that S is using the over-all relative frequency as a general guide, and that S has a problem solving orientation. Conspicuous by its absence is the fact that a mathematical learning model with certain trial-by-trial effects will produce event matching (2): the simple model requires no assumption that S is perceiving over-all relative frequency. An appendix on language and categories has been written by Roger Brown for the psychologist unacquainted with psycholinguistics. The chapter contains a discussion of units of linguistic analysis such as phoneme and morpheme, the problem of linguistic meaning, and I60 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT
the relation between language and culture. Some five experiments on
psycholinguistics are discussed, one previously unpublished. The fact that the authors present a small number of tables adds to the readibility, but we would have appreciated some additional tables, perhaps in an appendix. An appendix might also have included the raw data from one or two of the studies. We would have liked to try our hand at classifying subjects according to strategy to determine whether the description is as objective as the authors maintain. Some readers will find the lack of discussion of the experimental results in terms of theoretical positions to be an important omission. Others will look in vain for discussion of non-human categorizing. In this reviewer’s opinion, A Study of Thinking rates high in read- ability and stimulation. The material is well organized, with frequent overviews and summaries; the general discussions are enlivened by well-chosen examples. For those interested in experimenting on concept formation, the volume offers many leads; the model of ideal strategies should prove a useful descriptive tool, and much food for experimental thought is provided in the general discussions. THURLOW R. WILSON University of New Mexico REFERENCES i. Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, Jacqueline J., and Austin, G. A. A Study of Thinking. New York: John Wiley & Sons, I956. 2. Bush, R. R. and Mosteller, F. Stochastic Models for Learning. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, I955.
3. Goodnow, Jacqueline J. "Determinants of Choice-Distribution in Two-Choice Situations." American Journal of Psychology, LXVIII (I955), I06-II6. 4. Hull, C. L. "Quantitative Aspect of the Evolution of Concepts." Psychological Monographs, XXVIII (I920), No. I (Whole Number I23). 5. Jarvik, M. E. "Probability Learning and a Negative Recency Effect in the Serial Anticipation of Alternative Symbols." Journal of Experimental Psychology, XLI (I955), 29I-297. 6. Viancke, W. E. "The Investigation of Concept Formation." Psychological Bulletin, XLVIII (I95I), I-3I.
The Object Relations Technique by Herbert Phillipson. Glencoe, Illi-
nois, The Free Press, I955. Pp. x+ 224, $6.00. THIs book introduces an impressive new projective method of personality assessment by a clinical psychologist at the Tavistock Clinic, London. The rationale of the approach is expressed in terms of unconscious object relations, constructs used by British psychoanalysts in psycho- therapy. According to this approach &dquo;the personality seeks to explain its main features in terms of the individual’s need to impose on the persons and things of his external world those unconscious relation- ships between himself and figures of his internal world which have resulted from his early experience of the objects upon which he depended for the satisfaction of his primitive needs.&dquo;