12 Delay of Gratification
12 Delay of Gratification
12 Delay of Gratification
Edelgard Wulfert
Jennifer A. Block
Elizabeth Santa Ana
Monica L. Rodriguez
Melissa Colsman
The University at Albany, State University of New York
Edelgard Wulfert, Jennifer A. Block, Elizabeth Santa Ana, Monica L. Rodriguez and
Melissa Colsman, University at Albany, State University of New York.
Monica L. Rodriguez was supported by a William T. Grant Faculty Scholars Award.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Edelgard Wulfert,
Department of Psychology, The University at Albany, State University of New York.,
SS112, Albany, New York 12222. Electronic mail may be sent to e.wulfert@
albany.edu.
Journal of Personality 70:4, August 2002.
Copyright # 2002 by Blackwell Publishing, 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148,
USA, and 208 Cowley Road, Oxford, OX4 1JF, UK.
534 Wulfert et al.
available in 52 weeks but would rather receive $100 today than $115
in one week (Herrnstein, 1990). Therefore, when money choices are
used to assess adolescents’ self-regulatory abilities, pretesting
different amounts and delays can ensure that no strong preference
exists for the immediate or the delayed reward. Any observed
preferences following an experimental manipulation can then be
attributed to individual differences in the ability to delay gratification.
To test the possibility of using money choices as an indicator of
adolescents’ self-regulation, we invited middle and high school students
to participate in a confidential survey on academic achievement and
substance use. As it is customary to compensate research participants
for their time and effort, we used this ‘‘subject fee’’ to produce an
unobtrusive and uncontrived measure of delay of gratification by
offering the adolescents an immediate and smaller or a delayed and
larger fee. As self-regulatory deficits often manifest themselves in a
cluster of problem behavior (Donovan, Jessor, & Costa, 1988; Jessor,
1987), we predicted, based on previous research, that adolescents who
chose the immediate, smaller reward would show greater involvement
with substances (Brown, Myers, Mott, & Vik, 1994; Clapper, Buka,
Goldfield, Lipsitt, & Tsuang, 1995; Gallucci, 1997; Henderson, Galen,
& DeLuca, 1998), have low self-esteem, and underperform academi-
cally (Dawes, Tarter, & Kirisci, 1997; Donovan et al., 1988).
STUDY 1
METHOD
Research Participants
The research participants (N = 69) were 43 male and 26 female students
from an urban high school (grades 9 through 12). Their mean age was 15.9
years (range 14 to 18); 94% were Caucasian and most students, socio-
economically, came from a middle-class background (the school had 8.1% of
free lunch students). Based on conduct records from school, we designated
36 (23 males and 13 females) as problem students; the remaining 33 were
non-problem students.
Measures
Academic achievement was obtained from school records. English and
Mathematics grades were averaged. This grade point average, GPA, had a
possible range from 0–100.
Delay of Gratification 537
Procedure
The study was approved by the review boards of the university and the
participating high school. Guidance counselors identified problem students
who during the past year had been written up for at least one disciplinary
violation (e.g., truancy, verbal aggression, fighting) and non-problem students
who matched the former in gender and grade level. The school forwarded a
letter from the researchers to the parents, asking them to call the school if they
did not wish the researchers to contact them. No parent called. The researchers
then contacted parents by phone to explain the project and request written
permission for the adolescents to participate. With parental approval, the
538 Wulfert et al.
RESULTS
Initial Analyses
As shown in Table 1 (upper part), exactly one half of the students (21
males and 12 females) chose the $7 whereas the other half (20 males
and 13 females) decided to wait one week for the $10. Of those who
chose the immediate reward, 27 pertained to the problem and 6 to the
non-problem group. Of those who delayed gratification, 7 were
problem and 26 were non-problem students.
Table 1
Comparison of High School Students Choosing the Immediate Versus Delayed Reward (Study 1)
Table 2
Correlation Matrix of Variables Assessed in High School Students
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Impulsiveness 1.00
2. Problem status 0.61 1.00
3. GPA 0.40 0.71 1.00
4. Substance use 0.36 0.67 0.59 1.00
5. Self-esteem 0.33 0.46 0.38 0.49 1.00
6. Gender 0.03 0.01 0.03 0.06 0.13 1.00
Delay of Gratification 541
DISCUSSION
STUDY 2
METHOD
Research Participants
The research participants (N = 48) were 22 boys and 26 girls from an urban
middle school (grades 6 through 8). Exactly one half (11 boys and 13 girls)
were designated problem students because of disciplinary violations at
Delay of Gratification 543
school; the other half were non-problem students. The students’ mean age
was 12.6 years (range 11 to 14); 93.8% were Caucasian, and, socio-
economically, most came from a middle-class background (the school had
9.3% free lunch students).
Procedure
All selection, consent, and testing procedures were identical to those
described in Study 1. Six parents did not grant permission for their children to
participate (four of the children were problem students, and two were non-
problem students). Of 54 potential research participants, parental consent and
adolescent assent was obtained for a total of 48.
In the delay-of-gratification manipulation, one difference to the previous
study was the amount of money offered as an incentive for participating in
the project. Each adolescent was presented individually with a choice of
receiving five dollars immediately or seven dollars one week later.
Analogous to Study 1, the value of the immediate reward was approximately
70% of the value of the delayed reward.
Measures
The same achievement, self-esteem, and behavior measures as described in
Study 1 were administered, with the following exception. Given the younger
age of the participants, it seemed implausible that they would be heavily
involved in substance use. Cigarette smoking, alcohol, and drug use were
assessed with the deviant behavior scale of Jessor et al.’s (1992) Health
Behavior Questionnaire, but a time frame of six months was used. The
adolescents were asked how often they had used cigarettes, alcohol, and
marijuana and how often they had consumed more than five drinks at one
time (which is a widely accepted measure of binge drinking). Their answers
were recorded on 5-point Likert-type scales (0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = twice,
3 = three to four times, 4 = five or more times).
RESULTS
Initial Analyses
As shown in Table 3 (upper part), 13 boys and 11 girls chose the
immediate reward of $5 whereas the 9 boys and 15 girls decided to
wait one week for the delayed $7 reward. When considering problem
status, 20 problem and 4 non-problem students chose the immediate
reward, whereas 20 non-problem and only 4 problem students decided
to wait one week for the larger reward.
Table 3
Comparison of Middle School Students Choosing the Immediate Versus Delayed Reward (Study 2)
Table 4
Correlation Matrix of Variables Assessed in Middle School Students
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Impulsiveness 1.00
2. Problem status 0.68 1.00
3. GPA 0.60 0.84 1.00
4. Substance use 0.52 0.55 0.51 1.00
5. Self-esteem 0.32 0.40 0.26 0.22 1.00
6. Gender 0.17 0.00 0.06 0.03 0.04 1.00
546 Wulfert et al.
DISCUSSION
The findings with high school students (Study 1) proved robust and
were successfully replicated with younger adolescents and different
reward parameters. In both studies, an unwillingness to delay
gratification was related to self-regulatory deficits in other life areas.
Compared to students who delayed gratification, those who chose the
immediate reward were more likely to report lower self-esteem,
transgress against school norms, earn lower grades, and lack self-
restraint in their use of cigarettes, alcohol, and other drugs. One
difference between Studies 1 and 2 was that the middle school students
who chose the immediate reward drank alcohol both more frequently
and in larger quantities than those who delayed, whereas among high
school students the frequency of drinking did not differentiate between
groups. This finding probably reflects the fact that well-adjusted middle
school students do not yet use alcohol, whereas high school students,
regardless of their adjustment, begin to experiment with alcohol, but
only those who are maladjusted use alcohol in large quantities.
One additional point deserves mention. In the middle school sample,
proportionately more girls than boys delayed gratification, although
the gender difference was not statistically significant. In contrast, in
the high school sample there was no indication of a gender difference.
Because of the small sample size, we cannot be sure whether boys and
girls differ in their self-regulatory ability during late childhood. If they
do, the difference apparently vanishes with increasing age. This may
reflect differences in socialization. Perhaps parents inhibit girls’
impulsive tendencies more than boys’, but girls ‘‘catch up’’ as they
become increasingly independent during adolescence. The possibility
Delay of Gratification 547
GENERAL DISCUSSION
REFERENCES
Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, P. K., &
Rodriguez, M. (2000). Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation
for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
79, 776–792.
550 Wulfert et al.
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control: How and
why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Brown, S. A., Myers, M. G., Mott, M. A., & Vik, P. W. (1994). Correlates of success
following treatment for adolescent substance abuse. Applied and Preventive
Psychology, 3, 61–73.
Caetano, R. (1986). Drinking and Hispanic-American family life. Alcohol, Health and
Research World (pp. 26–35).
Chapman, G. B. (1996). Temporal discounting and utility for health and money. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 771–791.
Cherek, D. R., Moeller, F. G., Dougherty, D. M., & Rhoades, H. (1997). Studies of
violent and nonviolent male parolees: II. Laboratory and psychometric measures of
impulsivity. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 523–529.
Clapper, R. L., Buka, S. L., Goldfield, E. C., & Lipsitt, L. P., & Tsuang, M. T. (1995).
Adolescent problem behaviors as predictors of adult alcohol diagnoses. Interna-
tional Journal of the Addictions, 30, 507–523.
Clark, W., & Midanik, I. (1981). Alcohol use and alcohol problems among U.S.
adults: Results of the 1979 National survey. In: Alcohol Consumption and Related
Problems (pp. 4–52). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.
(Alcohol and Health Monograph 1).
Dawes, M. A., Tarter, R. E., & Kirisci, L. (1997). Behavioral self-regulation:
Correlates and 2-year follow-ups for boys at risk for substance abuse. Drug and
Alcohol Dependence, 45, 165–176.
Donovan, J. E., Jessor, R., & Costa, F. M. (1988). Syndrome of problem behavior in
adolescence: A replication. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56,
762–765.
Eysenck, S. B. G., Eastling, G., & Pearson, P. R. (1984). Age norms for
impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in children. Personality and
Individual Differences, 5, 315–321.
Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1977). The place of impulsiveness in a
dimensional system of personality description. British Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 2, 46–55.
Funder, D. C., & Block, J. (1989). The role of ego-control, ego-resiliency, and IQ in
delay of gratification in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 1041–1050.
Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of gratification: Some
longitudinal personality correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
44, 1198–1213.
Gallucci, N. T. (1997). Correlates of MMPI-A substance abuse scales. Assessment, 4,
87–94.
Green, L., & Snyderman, M. (1980). Choice between rewards differing in amount and
delay: toward a choice model of self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 34, 135–147.
Henderson, M. J., Galen, L. W., & DeLuca, J. W. (1998). Temperament style and
substance abuse characteristics. Substance Abuse, 19, 61–70.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1990). Rational choice theory: Necessary but not sufficient.
American Psychologist, 45, 356–367.
Huberty, C. J. (1994). Applied discriminant analysis. New York: Wiley.
Delay of Gratification 551
discounting for single and multiple outcomes in the future. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 122, 3–22.
Stroop, J. P. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D. J., & Stouthamer-
Loeber, M. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its relationship to
delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 192–205.
Wulfert, E., Safren, S., Brown, I., & Wan, C. (1999). Cognitive, behavioral, and
personality correlates of HIV-positive persons’ unsafe sexual behavior. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 29, 223–244.
Wylie, R. C. (1989). Measures of self-concept. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.