Past Al Kova 2008
Past Al Kova 2008
Past Al Kova 2008
Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online
version of this article at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/321/5894/1322
Information about obtaining reprints of this article or about obtaining permission to reproduce
this article in whole or in part can be found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl
Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright
2008 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title Science is a
registered trademark of AAAS.
RESEARCH ARTICLES
strates are processed in mFAS by individual full 29. I. Molnar et al., Chem. Biol. 7, 97 (2000). 54. K. J. Weissman, ChemBioChem 7, 485 (2006).
sets of active sites, according to the path of ACP 30. D. J. Edwards et al., Chem. Biol. 11, 817 (2004). 55. R. N. Perham, Biochemistry 30, 8501 (1991).
31. A. Witkowski, A. K. Joshi, S. Smith, Biochemistry 36, 56. A. Roujeinikova et al., Structure 10, 825 (2002).
described above. However, these studies have 16338 (1997). 57. A. Roujeinikova et al., J. Mol. Biol. 365, 135 (2007).
also shown that a minority of substrates can be 32. L. Serre, E. C. Verbree, Z. Dauter, A. R. Stuitje, 58. G. A. Zornetzer, B. G. Fox, J. L. Markley, Biochemistry 45,
shuttled between the two sets of active sites, Z. S. Derewenda, J. Biol. Chem. 270, 12961 (1995). 5217 (2006).
either by ACP serving both MAT domains or by 33. C. Oefner, H. Schulz, A. D'Arcy, G. E. Dale, Acta 59. S. Jones, J. M. Thornton, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93,
Crystallogr. D Biol. Crystallogr. 62, 613 (2006). 13 (1996).
direct interaction of ACP with both KS domains 34. V. S. Rangan, S. Smith, J. Biol. Chem. 272, 11975 (1997). 60. S. Smith, A. Witkowski, A. K. Joshi, Prog. Lipid Res. 42,
(6, 60–62). In light of the large 135 Å distance 35. B. Persson, Y. Kallberg, U. Oppermann, H. Jornvall, 289 (2003).
between the ACP anchor point located in one Chem. Biol. Interact. 143-144, 271 (2003). 61. A. K. Joshi, V. S. Rangan, A. Witkowski, S. Smith, Chem.
catalytic cleft and the MAT in the other, the most 36. A. C. Price, Y. M. Zhang, C. O. Rock, S. W. White, Biol. 10, 169 (2003).
Structure 12, 417 (2004).
plausible explanation for the minor mode-of- 37. D. A. Rozwarski, C. Vilcheze, M. Sugantino, R. Bittman,
62. V. S. Rangan, A. K. Joshi, S. Smith, Biochemistry 40,
domain interaction is a large-scale rotation of the 10792 (2001).
J. C. Sacchettini, J. Biol. Chem. 274, 15582 (1999).
63. All data were collected at the Swiss Light Source (SLS,
upper portion of mFAS, relative to the lower 38. T. J. Sullivan et al., Am. Chem. Soc. Chem. Biol. 1, 43
Paul Scherrer Institute, Villigen). We thank
portion (fig. S4). (2006).
C. Schulze-Briese, S. Gutmann, R. Bingel-Erlenmeyer,
39. M. Leesong, B. S. Henderson, J. R. Gillig, J. M. Schwab,
The molecular description of active sites in S. Russo, A. Pauluhn, and T. Tomizaki for their
J. L. Smith, Structure 4, 253 (1996).
mFAS should stimulate the development of outstanding support at the SLS; S. Jenni and M. Sutter for
40. M. S. Kimber et al., J. Biol. Chem. 279, 52593 (2004).
critically reading the manuscript and all members of the
improved inhibitors as anticancer drug candi- 41. A. K. Joshi, S. Smith, J. Biol. Chem. 268, 22508 (1993).
Ban laboratory for suggestions and discussions;
dates. As demonstrated by structural homology, 42. S. Pasta, A. Witkowski, A. K. Joshi, S. Smith, Chem. Biol.
% of ne urons a c tive
fields in the maze are 80 5
similar. (A) Color-coded 4
60
spikes (dots) of simulta-
neously recorded hippo- 3
40
campal CA1 pyramidal 2
neurons. The rat was re- 20
quired to run in the 1
wheel facing to the left 0
Animal's trajectory 1 2 3 4 5
during the delay be-
Firing rate - wheel (Hz)
tween the runs in the
maze. (B) Percent of D E
25 1 1
neurons firing >0.2 Hz 4 17 26
Neurons (sorted)
highest percentage of 0.8
neurons was active when
rats were running in the 0.6
Trials
1
wheel. (C) Relationship 25 18
5 8
between firing rate of 0.4
neurons active in rats
running the wheel and 0.2
the maze (rs = –0.3, P <
0.0001, 681 neurons, 1 30 0
5 10 15 5 10 15 5 10 15 0 5 10 15
three rats, 17 sessions).
(D) Normalized firing Time in wheel (sec) Time in wheel (sec)
rate of six simultane- F G
ously recorded neurons
during wheel running 10
wheel
maze
wheel wheel
(each line shows the 8 1
Time (sec)
N of cells
color-coded activity on
single trials turning to 6 5
the left arm). The epi- 4 2
sode fields occurred at
2
specific segments of the
run. (E) Normalized fir- 0 10 3
0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 5 10 1 2 3
ing rate of 30 simulta- Field width (sec) Time (sec)
neously recorded neurons
1
during wheel running, maze 1
Correlation coeficient
10 wheel maze
ordered by the latency maze
Correl. coef.
8 wheel
of their peak firing rate. 1
0.5
N of cells
Normalized power
0 360 units
in the wheel display 0.8 LFP
theta phase precession
and temporal compres- 180
200 msec
sion. (A) (Top) Unfiltered 0.4
(light gray) and filtered 540 20 F irin g rate (Hz )
(4 to 10 Hz) (dark gray)
traces of LFP and phase 360
Theta phase (deg)
advancement of action 10 6 8 10 12
potentials (dots). (Bottom) 180 Frequency (Hz)
Activity of six example 12
neurons from the same 0 C wheel
Number of cells
12 maze
session. Each dot is an 540 1212
F irin g rate (Hz )
540 540
(Bottom) Unit discharges
F ir in g r a te (Hz )
6 6
(dots) from all trials with-
360 360
in a session as a function 4 4
of theta phase, plotted
180 2 180 2
against time from the be-
ginning of a wheel run.
Red line, smoothed mean 2 4 6 8 10 2 4 6 8 10 2 4 6 8 10 2 4 6 8 10
firing rate. Relatively Time in wheel (sec) Time in wheel (sec)
steady firing rates and a
steady theta phase occur B C
in both control tasks. (B) Memory Control 1 Control 2
1 1
Cross-correlation matrices memory
control
Correl. coef.
in three different tasks 0.8
2.5 Correl. coef.
Time (sec)
unit memory
Number of neurons
10
correlation, P < 0.01).
(C) Population-vector cor- 5
relation coefficient values
283 44 0
in the memory task (n = 6 8 10 12 -4 -2 0 2 4 -4 -2 0 2 4 50 100
17 sessions) and control Frequency (Hz) ∆ frequency (unit-LFP; Hz) % spikes within ACG peak
tasks (n = 8 sessions)
(mean T SD). (D) Power spectrum of spike trains of an episode neuron (unit) (black dots, maxima of the cross-correlograms; white line, sum of all
and simultaneously recorded LFP during wheel running in the memory task neurons). There is a significant frequency shift in the memory task (0.44 T
(30). The frequency of unit firing oscillation is higher than the frequency of 0.6 Hz) and a lack of frequency shift in control tasks (combined control 1 and
LFP. (E) Difference between unit and LFP oscillation frequency in the 2, 0.07 T 0.3 Hz). (F) Ratio of spikes in the center and tail of temporal auto-
memory (left) and control (right) tasks. Each line is a color-coded normalized correlograms (SOM text). High values indicate compact episode fields; low
cross-correlogram between power spectra of a pyramidal neuron and values indicate spikes scattered throughout the time of wheel running
simultaneously recorded LFP. A shift of the maximal correlation values to the (memory task, n = 287 neurons; control tasks, n = 85 neurons; rank sum test,
right indicates that unit theta oscillation is faster than LFP theta oscillation P < 0.0001). Arrows indicate medians.
Right
populations of neurons were active (fig. S5)
(normalized)
Firing rate
(26), arguing for the importance of distant cues
Trials
(2, 20) and against a critical role of idiothetic 0.5
inputs (26). In addition, the temporal organiza-
Left
tion of cell assemblies in control tasks was less
precise, as reflected by much weaker correla-
tions between temporally adjacent populations 0
0 5 10 15 0 5 10 15 0 5 10 15
during the control tasks than during the memory Time in wheel (sec)
task (Fig. 3, B and C), despite the similarity in
firing rates during all tasks (fig. S6). As another
contrast to the memory task, neurons recorded B Left trials Right trials Stem Significance Stem
during the control tasks fired throughout the Rat 1
1 1 right
trial, with spikes locked to a similar phase of the left
theta cycle (Fig. 3A). Consistent with these
observations, neurons in the rats performing the
0.5
memory task oscillated faster than the local field
Neurons
Trials
Right
Err
43
5 10 15 5 10 15 5 10 15 5 10 15
Time in wheel (sec) Time in wheel (sec)
Fig. 5. Cell-assembly activity during the wheel predicts behavioral errors C Single trial (error left) D
Correctly predicted
error trials (err). Correct trials are separated into left- and right-turn trials.
(B) Normalized firing rates of 43 neurons simultaneously recorded during
Neurons
9/13
wheel running, ordered by the latency of peak firing rates during correct
left trials (left). (Right) Firing sequence of the same neurons on correct 50
right trials. (C) Firing sequence of neurons in a single error (left) trial.
firing patterns of neurons during wheel running and, fundamentally, the character and context of 20. J. R. Huxter, T. J. Senior, K. Allen, J. Csicsvari, Nat.
(Fig. 5D). Altogether, these observations demon- the input. The evolving trajectory can be ef- Neurosci. 11, 587 (2008).
21. G. Buzsáki, Neuroscience 31, 551 (1989).
strate that a particular sequence of neurons was fectively perturbed, or updated, by external inputs 22. M. A. Wilson, B. L. McNaughton, Science 265, 676 (1994).
activated in a reliable temporal order from the in every theta cycle (40). Because of this 23. K. Louie, M. A. Wilson, Neuron 29, 145 (2001).
moment the rat entered the wheel to the time it flexibility in the sources of cell-assembly control, 24. S. A. Deadwyler, T. Bunn, R. E. Hampson, J. Neurosci. 16,
reached the reward. we hypothesize that neuronal algorithms, having 354 (1996).
25. H. Eichenbaum, P. Dudchenko, E. Wood, M. Shapiro,
Because running speed, head position, and head evolved for the computation of distances, can also H. Tanila, Neuron 23, 209 (1999).
direction during wheel running before left and right support the episodic recall of events and the 26. A. Czurko, H. Hirase, J. Csicsvari, G. Buzsáki, Eur. J.
choices were apparently indistinguishable (fig. S1), planning of action sequences and goals (19). Neurosci. 11, 344 (1999).
the above findings indicate that trial differences During learning, the temporal order of external 27. H. Hirase, A. Czurko, J. Csicsvari, G. Buzsáki, Eur. J.
Neurosci. 11, 4373 (1999).
in hippocampal assembly configurations cannot events is instrumental in specifying and securing 28. C. Geisler, D. Robbe, M. Zugaro, A. Sirota, G. Buzsáki,
solely arise from instantaneous environmental the appropriate neuronal representations, whereas Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 8149 (2007).
inputs or the integration of motion signals. during recall, imagination (35), or action plan- 29. K. M. Gothard, W. E. Skaggs, B. L. McNaughton, J.
Behavioral function of internally gener- ning, the sequence identity is determined by the Neurosci. 16, 8027 (1996).
30. J. O’Keefe, M. L. Recce, Hippocampus 3, 317 (1993).
ated cell-assembly sequences. These findings intrinsic dynamics of the network.
31. A. P. Maurer, S. L. Cowen, S. N. Burke, C. A. Barnes,
demonstrate that the rat brain can generate con- B. L. McNaughton, J. Neurosci. 26, 13485 (2006).
tinually changing assembly sequences. The pat- References and Notes 32. F. Sargolini et al., Science 312, 758 (2006).
terns of the self-evolving neuronal assembly 1. J. O’Keefe, J. Dostrovsky, Brain Res. 34, 171 (1971). 33. B. L. McNaughton, F. P. Battaglia, O. Jensen, E. I. Moser,
2. J. O’Keefe, L. Nadel, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive M. B. Moser, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 663 (2006).
sequences depend on the initial conditions, and Map (Clarendon, Oxford, UK, 1978). 34. S. Fujisawa, A. Amarasingham, M. T. Harrison,
the particular sequences of cell assemblies are 3. J. Huxter, N. Burgess, J. O’Keefe. Nature 425, 828 (2003). G. Buzsáki, Nat. Neurosci. 11, 823 (2008).
predictive of behavioral outcome. 4. B. L. McNaughton, C. A. Barnes, J. O’Keefe, Exp. Brain 35. X. G. Li, P. Somogyi, A. Ylinen, G. Buzsáki, J. Comp.
Our results offer new insights into the rela- Res. 52, 41 (1983). Neurol. 339, 181 (1994).
5. J. O'Keefe, N. Burgess, Nature 381, 425 (1996). 36. G. Kreiman, C. Koch, I. Fried, Nature 408, 357 (2000).
tionship between hippocampal activity and 6. R. U. Muller, J. L. Kubie, J. B. Ranck Jr., J. Neurosci. 7, 37. J. E. Lisman, Neuron 22, 233 (1999).
navigation (2–7, 14–20, 26–30, 33). Hippocam- 1935 (1987). 38. L. F. Abbott, W. G. Regehr, Nature 431, 796 (2004).
pal firing patterns during maze navigation were 7. B. L. McNaughton et al., J. Exp. Biol. 199, 173 (1996). 39. M. Rabinovich, R. Huerta, G. Laurent, Science 321, 48
similar to those during wheel running in the 8. D. O. Hebb, The Organization of Behavior: (2008).
A Neuropsychological Theory (Wiley, New York, 1949). 40. M. B. Zugaro, L. Monconduit, G. Buzsáki, Nat. Neurosci.
delayed alternation memory task with stationary 9. E. Tulving, Elements of Episodic Memory (Clarendon, 8, 67 (2005).
environmental and body cues. Therefore, we Oxford, UK, 1983). 41. We thank H. Hirase for sharing his data and C. Curto,
suggest that hippocampal networks can produce 10. L. R. Squire, Psychol. Rev. 99, 195 (1992). C. Geisler, S. Ozen, S. Fujisawa, K. Mizuseki, A. Sirota,
sequential firing patterns in two possibly interact- 11. M. Abeles, Corticotronics: Neural Circuits of the Cerebral D. W. Sullivan, and R. L. Wright for comments.
ing ways: under the influence of environmental/ Cortex, (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 1991). Supported by NIH (NS34994 and MH54671), NSF (SBE
12. M. W. Howard, M. S. Fotedar, A. V. Datey, 0542013), the James S. McDonnell Foundation, NSF
idiothetic cues or by self-organized internal mech- M. E. Hasselmo, Psychol. Rev. 112, 75 (2005). (A.A.), the Swartz Foundation (V.I.), and the Robert Leet
anisms. The high-dimensional and largely ran- 13. W. B. Levy, A. B. Hocking, X. Wu, Neural Netw. 18, 1242 and Clara Guthrie Patterson Trust (E.P.).
dom (nontopographical) connectivity of the CA3 (2005).
axonal system (35) and its inputs makes the 14. L. M. Frank, E. N. Brown, M. Wilson, Neuron 27, 169
Supporting Online Material
(2000).
hippocampus an ideal candidate for internal se- www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/321/5894/1322/DC1
15. J. Ferbinteanu, M. L. Shapiro, Neuron 40, 1227 (2003).
quence generation (13, 33, 36, 37). The parame- 16. E. R. Wood, P. A. Dudchenko, R. J. Robitsek,
SOM Text
Figs. S1 to S13
ters of cell-assembly dynamics (including their H. Eichenbaum, Neuron 27, 623 (2000).
Table S1
trajectory and lifetimes) are probably affected by 17. J. A. Ainge, M. A. van der Meer, R. F. Langston,
Movie S1
E. R. Wood, Hippocampus 17, 988 (2007).
a number of factors, including experience- 18. W. E. Skaggs, B. L. McNaughton, M. A. Wilson,
References
dependent and short-term synaptic plasticity C. A. Barnes, Hippocampus 6, 149 (1996). 29 April 2008; accepted 29 July 2008
(34, 38); asymmetric inhibition (39); brain state; 19. G. Dragoi, G. Buzsáki, Neuron 50, 145 (2006). 10.1126/science.1159775
SOM Text
Figs. S1 to S13
Table S1
References
Other Supporting Online Material for this manuscript includes the following: (available at
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/321/5894/1322/DC1)
Movie S1
Supporting Online Material
1
controlled by the experimenter. The running wheel (10 cm wide, 29.5 cm
diameter) was attached to a wall of the delay area (Fig. 1A). Speed and direction
of the wheel rotation was tracked with an optical sensor (0.72o resolution),
attached to the axis of the wheel rotation. Infrared-beam photo-detectors were
fixed in all arms of the maze and were used to track the movement of an animal
in the maze and to control the delivery of water reward (0.05 ml) after correct
choices.
Each session began by placing the rat into the delay area while all three doors
were closed. During the first day of the training the animal was required to run in
the wheel for at least 3 sec, after which the main-stem door was opened. Rats
were not allowed to return to the main stem after making a choice at the T-
junction of arms. After the consumption of the reward (or no reward), rats walked
back to the delay area and could initiate a new trial by entering the wheel. The
required length of a wheel running was gradually increased from the initial 3 sec
up to 10 sec in rat 1 and up to 20 sec in rats 2 and 3. All rats were required to run
steadily in the wheel, always facing the same direction in each trial (West, Fig. 1,
S1). All rats reached high performance of spontaneous alternation (>90% correct
choices) and steady wheel running within a week. A session was terminated after
the animal failed to initiate a new trial within 2 minutes. After entering the wheel,
the velocity of running increased rapidly and reached a steady level within a few
seconds (Fig. S1). This stereotypic initial acceleration was used as a trigger to
mark the beginning of a trial in offline data processing in both the memory task
and in the control tasks (threshold speed: 20cm/sec).
Control task 1: Wheel running for a water reward. Rats 3 and 4 were trained to
run in a running wheel for a water reward. A wheel, identical to the one used in
the maze, was attached to one side of a rectangular box (30x40x35 cm). A water
port was placed on the wall opposite to the wheel. At the beginning of each
session, a rat was introduced into a box and required to run for a fixed period of
time (rat 3, 20sec; rat 4, 10sec). As in the alternation task, rats were trained to
run with steady speed and heading always in the same direction. All trials were
2
rewarded by water (2, 6).
Control task 2: Spontaneous wheel running. A wheel, identical to the one used in
the maze, was attached to the home cage of the rat (30 x 40 x 35 cm). The rats
(rat 2 and 3) had free access to the wheel and could run in either direction. The
trials were defined as at least 10 second-long periods of wheel running with the
running speed maintained above 50 cm/sec and preceded by at least 2 seconds
of exploration in the home cage. Maze and control task recording sessions were
alternated in rats 2 and 3.
Hippocampal inactivation
Five additional male rats (335 - 400 g) were implanted with stainless steel
injection guide cannulae (27 gauges) above dorsal hippocampi bilaterally (7-8)
under Isoflurane anesthesia. After recovery, the rats were water restricted and
trained in both non-delayed (zero) and delayed (15 sec) versions of left/right
alternation task until their performance reached a steady level (5 days). These
rats were not required to run in the wheel but were simply confined in the delay
area for the duration of the delay. Lidocaine (4%, 0.5µl) or saline were slowly (5
min) injected into hippocampi bilaterally through a 33-gauge cannula connected
to a 10-µl Hamilton syringe through tygon tubing. The injection cannula was left
at its position for 1min before and after an injection. Five minutes after the
injection, the rat was tested in either the non-delayed or delayed version of the
alternation task. The order of lidocaine and saline injections and order of delayed
and non-delayed versions of the task varied across animals.
Data processing
Place and episode fields. On each trial each neuron's spike train was convolved
with a Gaussian (SD = 100msec, sampling rate 1250 Hz). Firing rate profile of
each neuron triggered at the beginning of a wheel or a maze run (‘post-stimulus
time histogram’ with the moment of wheel/maze running initiation at time zero)
was calculated from action potentials generated during trails of the same type (e.
3
g., during wheel or maze runs before left or right turn). In order to distinguish
between place fields on a maze and bouts of increased firing generated by
pyramidal neurons during wheel running, we refer to the areas of an increased
firing rate in a wheel as “episode fields”. Place fields on the maze and episode
fields in the wheel were defined by a minimal peak firing rate of 6.0 Hz or 5Hz
and peak firing rate being at least 4.5-times or 3-times SD above a mean firing
rate, respectively. Using these different criteria yielded similar results. The width
of individual fields was determined by the onset and offset of >10 % rate of the
peak firing rate of that field (9-10).
Neuronal sequences. In each session two firing rate profiles for each neuron
were constructed and normalized by their peak firing rate: one from action
potentials generated during wheel runs before correct left arm choice (“left trials”)
and one from action potentials generated during wheel runs before correct right
arm choice (“right trials”) (e. g., Fig. 1D, E). Mean rate histograms of all neurons
within each trial type (left or right) were ordered according to the latencies of
peak firing rates of individual neurons (e.g., Fig. 1E). To establish an estimate of
statistically reliable firing rate differences between left and right trial types we
shuffled left and right single trial firing rate histograms in a randomized manner
(11). Thus, we examined the null hypothesis whether real firing patterns of
neurons during left and right trials differed significantly, and at which portion of
trials these differences occurred. Note that this approach is more rigorous than
simply comparing firing rate differences between left and right trials and may at
least partially explain the low number of trial discriminating neurons in the stem of
the maze, compared to previous findings (12-17).
Population vector correlation. In the spatial domain, “spatial scale factor” of the
hippocampal population is a measure of distance traveled by a rat over which
temporally overlapping neuronal populations become de-correlated (18-20).
Analogously, we calculated a “temporal scale factor”, which estimates an amount
of time it took for population activity to become de-correlated (‘life-time’ of
population activity). Firing rate histograms of all neurons within each trial type
(see above) were binned into 100 msec bins. Thus, activity of the whole neuronal
4
population within each 100 msec time bin was described by a vector that had a
length of the number of active neurons (population vector). Spearman rank
correlation between all pairs of population vectors characterizing all time bins
was calculated. Values of correlation coefficients were color-coded and plotted
(Fig 1G). The ‘life-time’ of population activity was characterized as a width of a
region along the diagonal of a correlation matrix with increased correlation
coefficient values (19-20).
Correlation of population vectors representing the same time bin is equal to 1
(and thus all values along a diagonal of a correlation matrix are equal to 1) unless
different subsets of trials are used to build population vectors representing the
same time bin (19-20, Fig. 1G). In order to exclude a bias of auto-correlation
values along a correlation matrix diagonal, we constructed population vectors
from activity generated during trialsn and trialsn+1. Due to the trial-by-trial
variability of firing rates, the value of correlation between these population
vectors is typically smaller than 1 (Fig. 3B, 5C, only pixels with significant
correlation coefficient values are shown, p < 0.01). In the memory task, the
correlation matrix was calculated from activity generated during trials with the
same future choices (L-trialsn vs. L-trialsn+1 and R-trialsn vs. R-trialsn+1). In the
control tasks, the correlation matrix was calculated from activity generated during
even and odd trials (trialsn vs. trialsn+1; Fig. S9).
Phase precession. Instantaneous theta phase was derived by Hilbert transform
from filtered (4-10 Hz) LFP trace and a phase value was assigned to each action
potential. Spearman rank correlation was used to estimate relationship between
the phase precession slope and place/episode field size (9).
Frequency of single unit oscillation. All action potentials emitted by a neuron
during each pass through a field were convolved with a Gaussian function (SD =
1 msec), convolved with the first four Slepian functions and the dominant
oscillations frequency was estimated with Fast Fourier Transform, as described
in detail earlier (9). The frequency shift between the unit and LFP power spectra
was estimated by cross-correlation. Only units with significant theta modulation
were included in this analysis.
5
Compression index. Correlation of the distance between place/episode fields of
two neurons in a maze or a wheel with the mean temporal offset of action
potentials of the same pair of neurons within a theta cycle is called ‘compression
index’ (10). Only pairs of pyramidal neurons with robust place/episode fields and
significant cross-correlogram peaks were used for this analysis (10). We obtained
similar compression index values (0.75, 0.62, 0.59, p < 0.0001 for all) after
selecting place fields based on three different sets of parameters: minimal peak
firing rate of 6 Hz, 4.5 Hz or 5 Hz and the peak firing rate 4.5, 3.4 or 3 SDabove
the mean firing rate.
Firing ‘fields’ of neurons. Neurons in the control tasks, strictly speaking, did not
have fields. We compared activity of neurons generated during wheel running in
the memory and control tasks in the temporal domain. An autocorrelogram of
each neuron was filtered between 0.2 Hz and 2 Hz and the local minima of the
filtered trace closest to zero were detected. “Intra-field” spikes were defined as
spikes which occurred within 10% boundaries of the peak rate, and “extra-field”
spikes those that occurred outside of this boundary. This index is high for
compact episode/place fields and low for neurons with sustained or random
spiking patterns.
Inferring behavioral choice from neural activity. In addition to identifying neurons
that reliably discriminate between left and right trials in some portion of wheel
running, we also used a probabilistic model to predict future L/R trial type from
total population activity during the preceding wheel run. To infer the L/R trial type
for a given trial, we fit the model on all other trials in the same session and used it
to predict the remaining trial type. The set of trials on which the model was fit is
called the training set; the trial that is predicted from the model is called the test
set. The percentage of correctly inferred trials was computed across all
training/test set trial combinations, with leave-one-out cross-validation. The
significance of this prediction was determined on a session-by-session basis by
comparing the true values with the distribution of shuffled data. Each ‘shuffled’
data set was constructed by randomly shuffling the left/right labels between the
trial spike trains from each cell across all eligible trials. Only behaviorally correct
6
trials of length ≥ 7s for rat 1, and ≥ 17s for rats 2 and 3 were considered for
model-fitting and for correct trial-type inference. For each training set and each k-
th putative pyramidal cell that had an average firing rate of > 0.1Hz we computed
the “predictability index” Ik from the ratio of minimal and maximal average spike-
counts across left and right trials:
L R
min( N k , N k )
Ik = 1 − L R ,
max( N k , N k )
L
(here N k is the average spike count of the k-th neuron across the considered left
R
trials, and N k is the average spike count of the k-th neuron across the
considered right trials.) The higher the value of Ik the more selective the cell on
the training set (Ik = 1 for the most selective, and Ik = 0 for the least selective.)
While this index is not necessarily indicative of statistical significance of left/right
selectivity of a given cell, it was used for ranking cells in order of their tendency
to be predictive.
Putative pyramidal cells were ordered according to Ik in each training set. Ten
cells with the highest predictability index were selected for prediction in each
G
training set. For each trial we computed a binary population vector u = (u1 ,.., u10 )
as follows. For each trial the cell could be in one of two states: uk=“high” if
N k / N k >= 0.25 , and uk=“low” if N k / N k < 0.25 , where N k is the average spike-
count of the considered cell across all trials. We denote by Pqk (u ) the
Note that because there are only two states, Pqk (low) = 1 − Pqk (high) . We assume
that the neurons fire independently from each other. This is of course an
incorrect assumption. However the prediction (based on this assumption) can be
only worse than a prediction that would take correlations into account. Under this
G
assumption the probability of observing a binary population vector u = (u1 ,.., uN )
7
during trial type q can be computed as
G N
Pq (u ) = ∏ Pqk (uk )
k =1
G
Given a new test-set trial, with binary population vector u , the model predicts the
trial type q pred (=L or R) via maximum-likelihood:
G G G G G G
q pred (u ) = L if PR (u ) ≤ PL (u ) and q pred (u ) = R if PR (u ) > PL (u ) .
The same analysis was repeated on 100 shuffled data sets. The prediction for a
given session was considered to be significant if the percentage of trials correctly
inferred from the real data was higher than the percentage of trials correctly
inferred from at least 95 of the shuffled data sets.
Error trials. For each session, only those error trials were selected that were
longer than 7s for rat 1, and 17s for rats 2 and 3 (as in the selection of
behaviorally correct trials). In order to predict error trials, the model was fit on all
behaviorally correct trials in the same session. This model was then used to
predict the L/R type of each error trial in a session, as described above.
8
Supplementary figures
Fig. S1. Motor correlates of wheel running A. Top view of the testing apparatus.
Gray lines: trajectory of the animal. Black and green, position of front and rear
LEDs during wheel running (all wheel runs within one recording session). B.
Head position of the rat shown separately for the wheel running trials with future
left and right choices (trials) in the maze. C. Grand mean head-direction angular
vectors for all analyzed sessions (3 rats combined, red arrows: mean of all trials
from one session followed by left turn choices, blue: mean of all trials from one
session followed by right turn choices). D. Examples of single running wheel trial
mean head-direction angles (black) and session mean (red, left trials, blue, right
9
trials) head direction angles. Data from the same rat as shown in A and B. E. top:
Running speed during single wheel running trials (the same color-scheme as in
the bottom panel, the same recording sessions as in Fig. 5 and Fig. S13).
Bottom: Average running speed during correct left, correct right and error trials
(the same data as in the upper panel). F. The significance level of speed
differences between left and right trials was estimated based by the shuffling of
the left and right speed labels (green lines, p<0.025 each, 11). Running speed
during left and right trials did not differ significantly in any of the analyzed
sessions. G. Mean running speed during L and R trials of all sessions of the
memory tasks and during all trials of the control tasks. Note that head position,
head direction and speed are not significantly different between trials followed by
different behavioral choices. These observations indicate that idiothetic cues
alone cannot be responsible for the choice-specific firing patterns of neurons
(Fig. 4; 5, 12, 14).
Fig. S2. Firing rates of putative interneurons in the wheel and in the maze are
correlated. In contrast to pyramidal neurons (episode cells and place cells, Fig.
1C), the firing rates of putative interneurons were correlated significantly in the
wheel and maze. This may occur because interneurons are typically controlled
10
by multiple pyramidal cell assemblies (19, 9).
Fig. S3. Proportion of active neurons during wheel and maze running is similar at
each moment in time. Percentage of neurons firing at least one spike within 100
msec windows during maze (black) and wheel (blue) running (dashed lines: SD).
Note that similar fraction of neurons is active in the wheel and in the maze at
each moment in time, despite the spatial 'over-representation' of the head
position in the wheel by multiple neurons (Fig. 1B). This can occur because
different episode cells in the wheel are activated in a sequence.
11
Fig. S4. Control task 1. Rats were trained to run for water reward. Rat 4, twelve
simultaneously recorded neurons (each panel shows activity of one neuron,
rows: peak-normalized firing rate profiles of a neuron in 21 successive wheel
running trials). Rat 3, 15 simultaneously recorded neurons in a session with 58
trials. Only pyramidal neurons with mean firing rate higher than 0.35 Hz are
12
shown. Note that most neurons fire in a relatively sustained manner (2, 6) and
that firing rate profiles of almost all neurons vary from trial to trial.
Fig. S5. Control task 2. Rats could freely run in the wheel, attached to the wall of
a home cage. No reward was delivered. Each panel shows activity of one
neuron; all 16 neurons were recorded simultaneously (rat 2; single session).
Rows show peak-normalized firing rate profiles of a neuron in successive trials.
Trials are grouped according to the animal’s head-direction/sense of the wheel
rotation during trials (counter clockwise – CCW, clockwise – CW). Note that most
neurons show differential firing rates depending on the rat’s head direction. This
observation emphasizes the dominance of distant cues in this control task (2, 6).
13
Fig. S6. Distribution of mean firing rates of neurons in the memory and control
tasks. Note similar distribution of firing rates in both tasks. Arrows, medians.
Fig. S7. Performance of rats in the delayed alternation task is impaired after
bilateral inactivation of dorsal hippocampi. Five rats (different from rats 1-4 and
14
not equipped with electrodes) were trained in the alternation task with both 0 and
15 sec delays. Bilateral inactivation of dorsal hippocampi (4% lidocaine, 0.5 µl)
impaired performance of the animals during sessions with 15 sec delay between
trials but did not affect performance of the same animals during sessions with no
waiting period between trials (see also ref. 17 for similar results).
15
Fig. S8. Episode fields in the memory task. Activity of individual neurons during
wheel running predicts future behavior of an animal in the maze. Firing patterns
16
of 32 simultaneously recorded neurons in Rat 2 during wheel running in the
delayed alternation task, separated on the basis of future left and right correct
turns in the maze. Each line corresponds to a single trial. Clu, cluster identity.
FR: Firing rate (Hz). Note that many neurons fire only for a short duration (“life
time”) and consistently at the same time after the beginning of wheel running.
Note also robust firing pattern differences between left and right trials in several
neurons.
Fig. S9. Prediction of behavioral choices in the maze from the population activity
during wheel running. In addition to identifying individual neurons with differential
firing rate patterns before left and right choices (Fig. 4), we also examined how
well population activity can predict correct choices. In this analysis, we used the
best 10 discriminating neurons (based on mean spike count during wheel running
before left and right choices), irrespective of whether they individually showed
significant choice differences. In order to infer the left/right trial type for a given
trial, we fit a model on all other trials (excluding the one to be predicted) and
used the model to predict the remaining trial type (leave-one-out cross-validation
17
procedure). Black bars indicate the percentage of correctly predicted trials during
each session. The significance of the prediction was determined by comparing
the original data set with shuffled data from the same session (box plots; see
Methods). Box plots display median, the first and third quartiles and 95%
confidence intervals for model predictions based on shuffled data sets. Numbers
at the bottom of each bar indicate the fraction of neurons that significantly
differentiated between left and right trials on at least a portion of wheel running
trials (Fig. 4B) compared with the total number of pyramidal neurons within a
session. Rat 3 lost the implant after the last recording session.
18
Fig. S10. Firing rates of some interneurons correlate with specific aspects of the
memory task. Each map shows activity of a putative interneuron. Rows, wheel
running trials separated according to the future choices in the maze (left, right).
Neurons from 3 rats (n = 77 neurons). Several interneurons show increase or
decrease of a firing rate at a specific portion of a wheel run (as visualized by the
19
vertical color-stripes). Interneurons, whose firing patterns were correlated with
future left or right choices are highlighted (black frames). These observations are
in register with recent observations reporting that a fraction of interneurons also
show place or other task-related specificity (19, 21, 9).
20
by opposite choices (third column).
Fig. S12. Behavioral errors in the maze can be predicted from firing patterns
during wheel running. Two representative sessions from 2 rats (each session
with 9 left and 9 right correct trials and 2 error trials). For each rat, 10 best
left/right discriminating neurons (color-coded dots) from a larger population were
chosen. Note that firing patterns of right error trials and right correct trials are
similar. These findings suggest that assembly sequences of neurons reflect
planned travel path of the rat. During most error trials the rat ran smoothly in the
21
wheel and through the center arm as well as the T-junction in one swift trajectory,
and the commission error became detectable only after the wrong behavioral
turn. Deadwyler et al. (23) referred to this kind of error as “miscoding”, implying
that the previous choice is not correctly interpreted and the rat behaves
indistinguishably from correct trials. The second type of error trials was
associated with overt behavioral changes, such as stopping and premature
departure from the wheel and return to the wheel, grooming and immobility in the
wheel or delay area (analogous to the “delay-dependent” errors in ref. 23). The
neuronal patterns associated with such behaviorally abnormal trials were often
not possible to predict reliably from neuronal patterns, mainly because
comparable correct reference assembly patterns were not available.
Nevertheless, our anecdotal observations indicate that even such late errors
could be distinguished by neurons with perfect left vs right discrimination (e.g.,
error trial in Movie 1). In the presented analyses (Fig. 5) all errors are presented,
since the main goal was to differentiate between the effect of past and future
movement paths of the rat.
22
Fig. S13. Supplementary information for Fig. 5. A. Peak rate-normalized firing
23
rates of 43 neurons simultaneously recorded during wheel running, ordered by
the latency of peak firing rates during correct left trials (left) and correct right trials
(right). Note that while in Fig. 5B neuron order 1 to 43 is the same for both
panels, here the ordering of neurons for LEFT and RIGHT trials was done
separately. B. Activity of the same neurons as in A during seven error trials.
Neuronal order in the left column is the same as in the correct-left matrix in A.
Neuronal order in the right column is the same as in the correct-right matrix in A.
C. Correlation between population vectors from the left-correct and left-error
matrices (left column) and correlation between population vectors from the right-
correct and right-error matrices (right column). Mean correlation coefficient along
the correlation matrix diagonal (± 0.5 sec) is given in the upper right corner of
each correlation matrix. Note that left correct vs. left error mean correlation
coefficient is higher than right correct vs. right error correlation coefficient in all 7
left-error trials.
24
Movie 1. Single neurons can reliably predict future left and right turns. Running
wheel on the right, the right and left side arms of the maze are on the top and
bottom of the movie, respectively. Color LEDs indicate head orientation. This
strongly discriminating neuron was virtually silent during wheel runs before right
turns. Note that on the error trial (6th), the erroneous turn to the left is predicted
by the increased firing of the cell at the end of the delay period. On another trial
(last one in movie), the rat headed back toward the wheel. Prior to turning back
toward the T junction the neuron became active followed by the neuron-predicted
behavioral choice to the left. Although this neuron reliably discriminated left and
right turns, it alone cannot be responsible for guiding future motor behavior since
it fired transiently at the beginning of wheel running. This information should be
25
passed onto further neuronal assemblies, which continue to discriminate
between left and right movement trajectories during later parts of running in the
wheel and in the stem area (“prospective” neurons, 12-14).
Supplementary References
1. J. Csicsvari, D. A. Henze, B. Jamieson, K. D. Harris, A. Sirota, P. Barthó, K. D.
Wise, G. Buzsáki, J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1314-23 (2003).
2. H. Hirase, A. Czurko, J. Csicsvari, G. Buzsáki, Eur J Neurosci 11, 4373-4380 (1999).
3. K. D. Harris, D. A. Henze, J. Csicsvari, H. Hirase, G. Buzsáki, J. Neurophysiol.
84, 401–414 (2000).
4. J. Csicsvari, H. Hirase, A. Czurko, G. Buzsáki, Neuron 21, 179–189 (1998).
5. L. Hazan, M. Zugaro, G. Buzsáki, J Neurosci Methods 155, 207–216 (2006).
6. A. Czurko, H. Hirase, J. Csicsvari, G. Buzsáki, Eur J Neurosci 11, 344-352 (1999).
7. D. Klement, E. Pastalkova, A. A. Fenton, Hippocampus, 15, 460–471 (2005).
8. E. Pastalkova, P. Serrano, D. Pinkhasova, E. Wallace, A. A. Fenton, T. C. Sacktor,
Science 313, 1141-1144 (2006).
9. C. Geisler, D. Robbe, M. Zugaro, A. Sirota, G. Buzsáki, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
104, 8149-54 (2007).
10. G. Dragoi, G. Buzsáki, Neuron 50, 145-57 (2006).
11. S. Fujisawa, A. Amarasingham, M. T. Harrison, G. Buzsáki, Nature Neurosci 11,
823 - 833 (2008).
12. L. M. Frank, E. N. Brown, M. Wilson, Neuron 27, 169-178 (2000).
13. J. Ferbinteanu, M. L. Shapiro, Neuron 40, 1227-1239, (2003).
14. E. R. Wood, P. A. Dudchenko, R. J. Robitsek, H. Eichenbaum, Neuron 27,
623-633 (2000).
15. M. R. Bower, D. R. Euston, B. L. McNaughton, J Neurosci 25, 1313-23
(2005).
16. J. A. Ainge, M. Tamosiunaite F. Woergoetter, P. A. Dudchenko, J Neurosci
27, 9769-79 (2007).
17. J. A. Ainge, M. A. van der Meer, R. F. Langston, E. R. Wood, Hippocampus
26
17, 988-1002 (2007).
18. K. M. Gothard, W. E. Skaggs, B. L. McNaughton, J Neurosci 16, 8027-8040 (1996).
19. A. P. Maurer, S. L. Cowen, S. N. Burke, C. A. Barnes, B. L. McNaughton, J.
Neurosci. 26, 13485-92 (2006).
20. A. Terrazas, M. Krause, P. Lipa, K. M. Gothard, C. A. Barnes, B. L.
McNaughton, J Neurosci 25, 8085-96 (2005).
21. W. B. Wilent, D. A. Nitz, J. Neurophysiol. 97, 4152-61 (2007).
22. V. Ego-Stengel, M. A. Wilson, Hippocampus, 17, 161-174 (2007).
23. A. Deadwyler, T. Bunn, R. E. Hampson, J Neurosci 16, 354-72 (1996).
27