Mameli 2019
Mameli 2019
Mameli 2019
Introduction
There is consensus in the educational psychology literature that to promote worthwhile
learning schools of any level and type should favour student agency and active
participation in everyday classroom practices (e.g., Carpenter & Pease, 2013; Helker &
Wosnitza, 2016; Molinari & Mameli, 2010). Notwithstanding the emphasis on the idea that
students should be actively and accountably engaged in their educational pathways, most
*Correspondence should be addressed to Consuelo Mameli, Department of Educational Sciences, University of Bologna, Via
Filippo Re 6, 40126 Bologna, Italy (email: consuelo.mameli@unibo.it).
DOI:10.1111/bjep.12335
2 Consuelo Mameli et al.
of the studies in the field have focused on basically compliant forms of participation (e.g.,
Reeve, 2012; Renshaw, 2016), that is, students’ actions performed under the implicit
condition that they should fundamentally comply with teachers’ requests and solicita-
tions. Conversely, little research has considered students’ agency (e.g., Rajala, Kumpu-
lainen, Rainio, Hilpp€ o, & Lipponen, 2016), which can sometimes take the form of student
resistance or a challenge to the adult’s authority or decisions.
In this study, we focus on resistant agency perceived in the classroom and referred to
the Self and the Other. In particular, we present an experimental study aimed to assess
whether, and to what extent, resistant agency depends on the perceptions of teacher
behaviour (just vs. unjust) and student achievement (high vs. low).
Student agency
By and large, the concept of agency refers to the individual’s ability to transform the social
and situated practices in which she/he actively participates (Lipponen & Kumpulainen,
2011; Scott, 2008). In school, student agency corresponds to all those actions that
students purposively perform when they intervene and eventually remodel classroom
practices in their everyday life (Clarke et al., 2016; Mameli, Molinari, & Passini, 2019;
Martin, 2016).
Among the various agentic behaviours that students can perform during classes, some
involve actions commonly considered as desirable by teachers, such as ‘asking questions,
expressing opinions, and communicating interests’ (Reeve, 2012, p. 165). These actions
are agentic, as they have the potential to re-direct the unfolding of interactions (Mameli &
Molinari, 2014), and are welcomed as well, as they are usually interpreted by teachers as
indicators of student interest and commitment. Other more controversial forms of agency
correspond to oppositional or challenging stances taken with the aim of contrasting or
changing the course of events that are perceived to be frustrating. The strength of such
forms of agency was recently underlined by Wischmann and Riepe (2019), who claimed
that ‘if there is no resistance at all, there is no agency’ (p. 5), as student agency basically
manifests itself when learners contribute to redefining school practices instead of
adapting to the status quo. Teachers commonly consider these oppositional behaviours
‘as disruptive and counterproductive to student learning and classroom functioning’
(Goodboy, 2011a, p. 298) and thus tend to condemn them by adopting a teacher as
authority perspective (Burroughs, Kearney, & Plax, 1989).
In the current article, we focus in particular on this kind of agentic behaviour, that we
call resistant agency. In schools, these oppositional agentic actions are generally
expressed in forms of complaints, disagreements, or open challenges performed with the
goal of eventually changing a teacher’s decision or a social practice.
In the literature, the issue of student resistance has been addressed in two different
ways (for a review, see Winkler & Rybnikova, 2019). Some authors (Moore, 2007; Seidel &
Tanner, 2013) have pointed out the negative potential of resistance. For example, off-task
behaviours, both active (e.g., disturbing the class) and passive (e.g., avoiding activities), as
well as antisocial communication, such as disruption, revenge, or deception, are
considered to be destructive forms of resistance (Burroughs et al., 1989; Chory-Assad &
Paulsel, 2004) aimed at subverting learning. In our view, these actions of resistance do not
express student agency, as most of the time they launch a symmetric ‘wall against wall’
dynamic that does not bring about changes in the learning environment.
Contrariwise, oppositional actions are constructive and agentic when they challenge
school social structures or power dynamics in ways that potentially re-direct and
Teacher justice and student resistant agency 3
transform the activity (e.g., Goodboy, 2011a; Mameli & Passini, 2019; Rajala et al., 2016;
Rajala & Sannino, 2015). According to Goodboy (2011b), instructional dissent is
constructive when ‘students express their disagreements or complaints about class-
related issues’ (p. 423). Examples of this form of resistance, reported by Burroughs et al.
(1989), include communicating complaints directly, giving advice to the teacher or
challenging the basis of power. By and large, resistance is agentic and constructive when
opposition and challenges are expressed through actions that open to the possibility that
teachers and students eventually share a goal, or form a new one, and cooperate in order to
realize it. In this sense, resistant agency is not merely limited to student–teacher
opposition, but on the contrary, it represents a dialogic attempt to start up negotiations
with the teacher aimed at modifying, possibly with shared goals, some undesirable
practices (Lanas & Corbett, 2011).
therefore, reasonable to assume that this perception will be affected by the level of
students’ academic competence. Several studies (e.g., Gregory, Skiba, & Noguera, 2010;
Miles & Stipek, 2006) have indeed shown that academic achievement is far from being
independent of student behaviour. Under-achieving students are commonly described as
frustrated and disengaged, and this can impact oppositional conducts and vice versa.
Furthermore, teacher perceptions of student task orientation and reactivity behaviours
during classes were found to be associated with teacher grading (Mullola et al., 2014) in
the sense that the more students’ behaviour was seen as resistant and challenging, the
more teachers tended to award low grades. Conversely, we can assume that the good and
competent student is expected to behave compliantly. However, there are no studies, as
far as we know, that have specifically and experimentally addressed this issue.
For this study, we advance four hypotheses. First, in line with the literature (Horan
et al., Goodboy, 2011a; Seidel & Tanner, 2013), we predict a main effect of justice, with
resistant agency being higher in the unjust condition. Second, the achievement level is
expected to moderate the main effect of justice. Although this interaction has never been
tested before, in line with previous studies (Gregory et al., 2010; Mullola et al., 2014) we
assume that low achiever students will be perceived as being more likely to show a higher
level of resistance (Bolkan & Goodboy, 2016) in the unjust condition, while we expect no
differences between high and low achiever students in the just condition. Third, we
hypothesize that the target (Self vs. Other) will moderate the main effect of justice. Based
on the self-enhancement bias found in previous studies (Dufner et al., 2019) and on the
finding that students’ resistant agency is basically perceived as negative in the school
context (Goodboy, 2011a; Seidel & Tanner, 2013), we expect that the Other will be
described as showing a higher level of resistant agency as compared with the one
attributed to the Self and that this difference will be higher in the unjust condition. Finally,
we predict that both the level of achievement and the target, combined, will moderate the
main effect of justice. In line with our previous hypotheses, we anticipate that resistant
agency will be higher in the unjust condition (e.g., Goodboy, 2011a), when the agency of
low achiever students (e.g., Mullola et al., 2014) and the Other is evaluated. No differences
are expected in the just condition.
Method
Participants and procedures
The study was conducted in 2019 on a convenience sample of 620 high school students
(57.1% males, 6.3% non-native Italians) coming from three city-based medium-sized and
mixed-gender secondary schools situated in Northern Italy. Participants’ socio-economic
status (SES) was not assessed directly for this study, though school principals reported that
students attending the three schools came mostly from middle-class families. Participants,
aged 13–17 (Mage = 13.90, SD = 0.56), were all enrolled in the 9th grade.
Prior to data collection, the students’ parents were asked to fill out an informed
consent with twelve families either abstaining or refusing. All the students were then
asked to voluntarily take part in the study and were reassured about the confidentiality and
anonymity of data processing. The researchers administered the self-report instrument
online to students in their classrooms during school laboratory hours. For each class, the
filling out of the questionnaire was preceded by a short illustration of the research and its
general aims. The research was conducted in agreement with the ethical norms set by the
Italian National Psychological Association and obtained the formal approval of the local
Ethical Committee.
scenario were similar to the expected frequencies (v2(3) = 4.90, p = .179) and that
justice conditions (v2(1) = 0.27, p = .600) as well as achievement conditions
(v2(1) = 0.00, p = .990) were equivalent with respect to gender composition.
The questionnaire started with the following statement: ‘Please read the short text in
the box below and then answer the following four questions’. In the condition of justice
and high achievement, the participants were given the following scenario: ‘A student who
does well in history fails a test. Thus she/he asks the teacher, with the consent of the
classmates, to be tested once more in order to keep or increase the final grade. The teacher
denies this chance, but offers him/her the opportunity to engage in a supplementary task
that will require a lot more time and effort’.
In the condition of justice and low achievement, the participants were given the
following scenario: ‘A student who does not do well in history fails a test. Thus she/he asks
the teacher, with the consent of the classmates, to be tested once more in order to keep or
increase the final grade. The teacher denies this chance, but offers him/her the
opportunity to engage in a supplementary task that will require a lot more time and effort’.
In the condition of injustice and high achievement, the participants were given the
following scenario: ‘A student who does well in history fails a test. Thus she/he asks the
teacher, with the consent of the classmates, to be tested once more in order to keep or
increase the final grade. The teacher refuses, without giving an explanation or offering any
alternatives’.
In the condition of injustice and low achievement, the participants were given the
following scenario: ‘A student who does not do well in history fails a test. Thus she/he asks
the teacher, with the consent of the classmates, to be tested once more in order to keep or
increase the final grade. The teacher refuses, without giving an explanation or offering any
alternatives’.
To measure our two dependent variables, participants were asked to think about the
scenario they had just read and answer two items on a scale ranging from 1 (Not at all
likely) to 10 (Highly likely): (1) How likely do you think that the student in this situation
will complain1 about the teacher’s response? (2) If you were in the same situation, how
likely is it you would complain about the teacher’s response?
Afterwards, the participants were asked to answer two additional manipulation check
questions with regard to justice and achievement level: (3) How fair do you think the
teacher’s response is? (10-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = Very unfair to 10 = Very
fair); (4) How good do you think the student is at history? (10-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 = Not good at all to 10 = Very good).
Results
Plan of analysis
In order to take into account variations due to the fact that students came from different
schools, we used a multilevel linear modelling approach to perform all the subsequent
analyses: School was the level 2 variable in which the students were clustered. We
considered random intercepts across schools and used restricted maximum likelihood
(REML) estimates of the parameters.
1
In Italian, the verb ‘lamentarsi’ (‘to complain’) is commonly used as a synonym for ‘to protest’ and indicates the action of
expressing one’s discontent.
Teacher justice and student resistant agency 7
Manipulation check
In order to verify the effectiveness of justice and achievement manipulation, we added the
two manipulation items on hierarchical linear models in which predictors were justice
condition (just vs. unjust) and achievement condition (high vs. low). Participants’ gender
(dummy coded, 0 = male participants) was added as covariates in order to control for its
potential effect (results without covariates were unchanged). For justice manipulation
(ICC = 0.02), the results indicated that participants rated the teacher’s behaviour as fairer
in the just condition (EMM = 5.48, SE = .27) than in the unjust condition (EMM = 4.23,
SE = .27, b = 1.17, SE = .30, t = 3.87, p < .001) and no other effects appeared to affect
the perceived fairness of teacher’s behaviour. For achievement manipulation
(ICC < 0.001), results revealed that male participants (EMM = 6.29, SE = .12) tended
to evaluate target students as being more competent than the female participants did
(EMM = 5.67, SE = .13, b = 0.62, SE = .16, t = 3.76, p < .001). More importantly, the
student depicted in the scenario was considered to be better at history in the high
achievement condition (EMM = 6.99, SE = .12) than in the low achievement condition
(EMM = 4.97, SE = .12, b = 1.78, SE = .24, t = 7.51, p < .001). Moreover, the student
was rated as slightly better at history in the unjust (EMM = 6.17, SE = .12) than in the just
(EMM = 5.78, SE = .12, b = 0.63, SE = .23, t = 2.75, p = .006) condition. No interac-
tion appeared. In sum, it is possible to state that the manipulations were effective.
Preliminary analysis
We detected no outliers (which were defined as 3 median absolute deviation; MAD)
from sample medians (Leys, Ley, Klein, Bernard, & Licata, 2013) on dependent measures.
We also checked whether gender would affect ratings of resistant agency. In line with
previous studies (e.g., Mameli & Passini, 2017), our results indicated that the male
participants reported higher Self resistant agency (M = 5.10, SE = .17) than female
participants (M = 4.27, SE = .19, b = 0.42, SE = .12, t = 3.36, p < .001). On the
contrary, gender did not affect ratings of the Other’s resistant agency (b = 0.17,
SE = .11, t = 1.62, p = .106).
Hypothesis testing
We performed a multilevel analysis 2 (justice: just vs. unjust) 9 2 (achievement: low vs.
high) 9 2 (target: Other vs. Self) with the target as within-participant variable and
resistant agency as dependent variable. Moreover, gender (dummy coded, 0 = male
participants) was added as covariates (ICC = 0.01). The model explained a satisfactory
portion of variance as indicated by the pseudo-R2 of .60 and the Ω20 (Xu, 2003) of 0.74.
An unexpected effect of the target emerged (b = 0.45, SE = .05, b = .15, t = 8.54,
p < .001) indicating that resistant agency was higher for the Other (EMM = 5.66,
SE = .18) than for the Self (EMM = 4.76, SE = .18). In line with hypothesis 1, our results
revealed that resistant agency was higher in the unjust (EMM = 6.16, SE = .20) than in the
just condition (EMM = 4.26, SE = .20, b = 0.95, SE = .10, b = .32, t = 9.93, p < .001).
Importantly, this effect was qualified by an interaction with achievement level (b = .21,
SE = .10, b = .07, t = 2.23, p = .026). In line with our expectations (hypothesis 2),
resistant agency was higher in the unjust and low achievement condition, while, contrary
to our predictions, resistant agency was higher for high achiever students than for low
achiever students in the just condition (see Table 1 and Figure 1). Consistently with
hypothesis 3, we found that the target moderates the effect of justice (b = 0.12, SE = .05,
8 Consuelo Mameli et al.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of student resistant agency ratings according to teacher justice, student
achievement, and target
Just
Other 4.58 (3.92; 5.25) .21 4.64 (4.03; 5.25) .26 4.53 (3.92; 5.13) .26
Self 3.93 (3.26; 4.59) .21 4.33 (3.72; 4.94) .26 3.53 (2.92; 4.13) .26
Total 4.26 (3.51; 5.01) .20 4.49 (3.87; 5.10) .24 4.03 (3.42; 4.63) .24
Unjust
Other 6.73 (6.11; 7.34) .22 6.58 (5.96; 7.19) .28 6.88 (6.29; 7.47) .26
Self 5.58 (4.97; 6.20) .22 5.34 (4.72; 5.95) .28 5.83 (5.24; 6.42) .26
Total 6.16 (5.48; 6.83) .20 5.96 (5.36; 6.56) .25 6.35 (5.77; 6.94) .24
Note. N = 620.
EMM = estimated marginal means.
6.35
5.96
6
Achievement
Agency
Low
5 High
4.49
4.03
4
Unjust Just
Justice
Figure 1. Teacher justice 9 student achievement interaction on ratings of student resistant agency (Self
and Other combined). Note: Bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
7
6.73
6
5.58
Target
Agency
Other
5 Self
4.58
3.93
4
Unj us t Ju s t
Justice
Figure 2. Teacher justice 9 target interaction on ratings of student resistant agency. Note: Bars
represent 95% confidence intervals.
adjustment) and this difference was stronger than in the just condition (EMM = 4.58,
SE = .21, and EMM = 3.93, SE = .21, respectively, d = 0.66, SE = .14, t = 4.54 p < .001,
Tukey’s adjustment, see Table 1 and Figure 2). Finally (hypothesis 4), a three-way
interaction appeared (b = 0.11, SE = .05, b = .04, t = 2.09, p = .037). In order to
understand this effect, we broke down the three-way interaction into two-way
interactions. When participants rated the Other’s resistant agency, no interaction
between justice and achievement conditions appeared (b = 0.41, SE = .44, t = 0.95,
p = .343) so that resistant agency was higher in the unjust condition regardless of the
achievement level of the Other. When instead participants rated Self resistant agency, an
interaction between justice and achievement appeared (b = 1.30, SE = .44, t = 2.96,
p = .003). More precisely, and contrary to hypothesis 4, ratings of Self resistant agency
were stronger in the high achievement condition (EMM = 4.33, SE = .26) than in the low
achievement condition (EMM = 3.53, SE = .26) only when the situation was described as
just (see Table 1 and Figure 3).
Discussion
In this experimental study, we investigated whether, and to what extent, learners’
perceptions of teacher fairness and student achievement affected likelihood estimates of
10 Consuelo Mameli et al.
Figure 3. Student resistant agency rates according to teacher justice and student achievement
conditions, and target. Note: Bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
Self and Other’s resistant agency. The main findings and educational implications are
discussed in the following sections.
to retake a failed test) and moderate costs (there is nothing to lose, for the student
described in the scenario).
In addition, the unexpected results concerning the justice condition are even more
interesting for our discussion. Our analysis indeed showed that in the just condition the
effect of achievement was reversed, with high achiever students rated as being more
agentic than low achiever ones. To comment on these results, we put forward two
considerations. The first concerns low achiever students who – in contrast with the fact
that they are commonly described as enacting challenging behaviours – reveal they are
actually willing to uncritically accept teachers’ instructions (in this study, doing extra
work in order to retake a failed test) if these requests are perceived as meeting their needs
(in this scenario, having a second chance to improve their performance). As far as high
achiever students were concerned – that is, students who are commonly described as
enacting compliant conducts – our study shows that they feel confident enough to resist
agentically when the teacher is perceived as being fair, that is, when they presumably feel
that the risk of negative consequences for their resistant stances is limited. This finding
highlights the importance for teachers to adopt a situational view in classroom
management.
Self one. This seems to indicate that when adolescents identify with a good student, they
feel able to exercise the power to directly oppose and resist the teachers’ instructions
in situations of justice, that is, when they perceive that their dissent does not bring about
negative consequences.
Conflicts of interest
All authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Berti, C., Mameli, C., Speltini, G., & Molinari, L. (2016). Teacher justice and parent support as
predictors of learning motivation and visions of a just world. Issues in Educational Research, 26,
543–560.
Bolkan, S., & Goodboy, A. K. (2016). Rhetorical dissent as an adaptive response to classroom
problems: A test of protection motivation theory. Communication Education, 65(1), 24–43.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03634523.2015.1039557
Burroughs, N. F., Kearney, P., & Plax, T. G. (1989). Compliance-resistance in the college classroom.
Communication Education, 38, 214–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/03634528909378758
Carpenter, J. P., & Pease, J. S. (2013). Preparing students to take responsibility for learning: The role
of non-curricular learning strategies. Journal of Curriculum and Instruction, 7(2), 38–55.
https://doi.org/10.3776/joci.2013.v7n2p38-55
Chory-Assad, R. M., & Goodboy, A. K. (2010). Power, compliance, and resistance in the classroom. In
D. L. Fassett & J. T. Warren (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of communication and instruction (pp.
181–199). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Chory-Assad, R. M., & Paulsel, M. L. (2004). Classroom justice: Student aggression and resistance as
reactions to perceived unfairness. Communication Education, 53, 253–273. https://doi.org/
10.1080/0363452042000265189
Clarke, S. N., Howley, I., Resnick, L., & Rose, C. P. (2016). Student agency to participate in dialogic
science discussions. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 10, 27–39. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.lcsi.2016.01.002
Diamantopoulos, A., Sarstedt, M., Fuchs, C., Wilczynski, P., & Kaiser, S. (2012). Guidelines for
choosing between multi-item and single-item scales for construct measurement: a predictive
validity perspective. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40, 434–449. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11747-011-0300-3
Donat, M., Peter, F., Dalbert, C., & Kamble, S. V. (2016). The meaning of students’ personal belief in a
just world for positive and negative aspects of school-specific well-being. Social Justice
Research, 29(1), 73–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-015-0247-5
Dufner, M., Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Denissen, J. J. (2019). Self-enhancement and
psychological adjustment: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 23(1), 48–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318756467
Fisher, G. G., Matthews, R. A., & Gibbons, A. M. (2016). Developing and investigating the use of
single-item measures in organizational research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
21(1), 3–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039139
Gallagher, S. (2012). Multiple aspects in the sense of agency. New Ideas in Psychology, 30(1), 15–
31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2010.03.003
Goodboy, A. K. (2011a). Instructional dissent in the college classroom. Communication Education,
60(3), 296–313. https://doi.org/10.1080/03634523.2010.537756
Goodboy, A. K. (2011b). The development and validation of the instructional dissent scale.
Communication Education, 60, 422–440. https://doi.org/10.1080/03634523.2011.569894
Gordon, M. E., & Fay, C. H. (2010). The effects of grading and teaching practices on students’
perceptions of grading fairness. College Teaching, 58(3), 93–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/
87567550903418586
14 Consuelo Mameli et al.
Gouveia-Pereira, M., Vala, J., & Correia, I. (2016). Teachers’ legitimacy: Effects of justice perception
and social comparison processes. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(1), 1–15,
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12131
Gregory, A., Skiba, R. J., & Noguera, P. A. (2010). The Achievement gap and the discipline gap.
Educational Researcher, 39(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X09357621
Helker, K., & Wosnitza, M. (2016). The interplay of students’ and parents’ responsibility judgements
in the school context and their associations with student motivation and achievement.
International Journal of Educational Research, 76, 34–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.
2016.01.001
Horan, S. M., Chory, R. M., & Goodboy, A. K. (2010). Understanding students’ classroom justice
experiences and responses. Communication Education, 59(4), 453474. https://doi.org/10.
1080/03634523.2010.487282
Kamble, S. V., & Dalbert, C. (2012). Belief in a just world and wellbeing in Indian schools.
International Journal of Psychology, 47, 269–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2011.
626047
Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., Hays, E. R., & Ivey, M. J. (1991). College teacher misbehaviors: What students
don’t like about what teachers say and do. Communication Quarterly, 39, 309–324. https://doi.
org/10.1080/01463379109369808
Lanas, M., & Corbett, M. (2011). Disaggregating student resistances: Analyzing what students pursue
with challenging agency. Young, 19, 417–434. https://doi.org/10.1177/110330881101900404
Leys, C., Ley, C., Klein, O., Bernard, P., & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard
deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 49, 764–766. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013
Lipponen, L., & Kumpulainen, K. (2011). Acting as accountable authors: Creating interactional
spaces for agency work in teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education, 27, 812–819.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2011.01.001
Mameli, C., Biolcati, R., Passini, S., & Mancini, G. (2018). School context and subjective distress: The
influence of teacher justice and school-specific well-being on adolescents’ psychological health.
School Psychology International, 39, 526–542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034318794226
Mameli, C., & Molinari, L. (2014). Seeking educational quality in the unfolding of classroom
discourse: A focus on microtransitions. Language and Education, 28, 103–119. https://doi.org/
10.1080/09500782.2013.771654
Mameli, C., Molinari, L., & Passini, S. (2019). Agency and responsibility in adolescent students: A
challenge for the societies of tomorrow. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 41–
56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12215
Mameli, C., & Passini, S. (2017). Measuring four-dimensional engagement in school: A validation of
the student engagement scale and of the agentic engagement scale. TPM - Testing,
Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 24, 527–541. https://doi.org/10.4473/
TPM24.4.4
Mameli, C., & Passini, S. (2019). Development and validation of an enlarged version of the student
agentic engagement scale. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 37, 450–463. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0734282918757849
Martin, J. (2016). The grammar of agency: Studying possibilities for student agency in science
classroom discourse. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 10, 40–49. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.lcsi.2016.01.003
Miles, S. B., & Stipek, D. (2006). Contemporaneous and longitudinal associations between social
behavior and literacy achievement in a sample of low-income elementary school children. Child
Development, 77, 103–117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00859.x
Molinari, L., & Mameli, C. (2010). Classroom dialogic discourse: an observational study. Procedia
Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2, 3857–3860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.604
Molinari, L., & Mameli, C. (2018). Basic psychological needs and school engagement: A focus on
justice and agency. Social Psychology of Education, 21(1), 157–172. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11218-017-9410-1
Teacher justice and student resistant agency 15
Moore, H. A. (2007). Student resistance in sociology classrooms: Tools for learning and teaching.
Sociological Viewpoints, 23, 29–44.
Mullola, S., Hintsanen, M., Jokela, M., Lipsanen, J., Alatupa, S., Ravaja, N., & Keltikangas-J€arvinen, L.
(2014). Associations between teacher-rated versus self-rated student temperament and school
achievement. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 58, 147–172. https://doi.org/
10.1080/00313831.2012.725094
Rajala, A., Kumpulainen, K., Rainio, A. P., Hilpp€ o, J., & Lipponen, L. (2016). Dealing with the
contradiction of agency and control during dialogic teaching. Learning, Culture and Social
Interaction, 10, 17–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lcsi.2016.02.005
Rajala, A., & Sannino, A. (2015). Students’ deviations from a learning task: An activity-theoretical
analysis. International Journal of Educational Research, 70, 31–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ijer.2014.11.003
Reeve, J. (2012). A self-determination theory perspective on student engagement. In S. L.
Christenson, A. L. Reschly & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement
(pp. 149–172). New York, NY: Springer.
Renshaw, P. (2016). On the notion of worthwhile agency in reformist pedagogies. Learning,
Culture and Social Interaction, 10, 60–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lcsi.2016.07.002
Resh, N., & Sabbagh, C. (2017). Sense of justice in school and civic behavior. Social Psychology of
Education, 20, 387–409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9375-0
Scott, W. R. (2008). Institutions and organizations: Ideas and interests (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
CA: SAGE.
Seidel, S. B., & Tanner, K. D. (2013). “What if students revolt?”- Considering student resistance:
Origins, options, and opportunities for investigation. CBE Life Sciences Education, 12, 586–595.
https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe-13-09-0190
Winkler, I., & Rybnikova, I. (2019). Student resistance in the classroom—Functional-
instrumentalist, critical-emancipatory and critical-functional conceptualisations. Higher
Education Quarterly, 73(4), 521–538. https://doi.org/10.1111/hequ.12219
Wischmann, A., & Riepe, V. (2019). Resistant agency in German lessons in primary education in
Germany. Ethnography and Education, 14, 136–152. https://doi.org/10.1080/17457823.
2017.1405735
Xu, R. (2003). Measuring explained variation in linear mixed effects models. Statististics in
Medicine, 22, 3527–3541. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1572