The Sciences of Science Communication
The Sciences of Science Communication
The Sciences of Science Communication
Baruch Fischhoff1
Department of Engineering and Public Policy, and Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213-3890
Edited by Dietram A. Scheufele, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, and accepted by the Editorial Board May 23, 2013 (received for review September
19, 2012)
The May 2012 Sackler Colloquium on “The Science of Science Com- The same mixed emotions may accompany phenomena that
munication” brought together scientists with research to communi- arise from the confluence of technology and society. How is it
cate and scientists whose research could facilitate that communication. that human actions can both discover antibiotics and encourage
The latter include decision scientists who can identify the scientific microbial resistance to them, create “smart” electricity grids and
results that an audience needs to know, from among all of the threaten them with cyberattacks, and produce a food supply
scientific results that it would be nice to know; behavioral scientists whose sheer abundance undermines the health of some people,
who can design ways to convey those results and then evaluate the while others still go hungry?
success of those attempts; and social scientists who can create the Without some grasp of the relevant science, it is hard to make
channels needed for trustworthy communications. This overview informed decisions about these issues. Those include private
offers an introduction to these communication sciences and their decisions, such as whether to choose fuel-efficient vehicles, robot-
roles in science-based communication programs. guided surgery, or dairy products labeled as “produced by animals
not treated with bovine growth hormone—not known to cause
decision making | environment | judgment | risk | uncertainty health effects.” And they include public decisions, such as whether
to support politicians who favor fuel efficiency, lobby for disclosing
Burger’s The Parrot Who Owns Me (1) and getting some insight been offered fair compensation for having a hazardous facility
into how our white-crested cockatoo, Big Bird, appeared to have sited in its midst. Reasonable people may also disagree about what
such human emotions, despite our species having diverged so long time frame to consider when making a decision (the next year? the
ago (even if he was born on Pittsburgh’s South Side). A third was next decade? the next century?), about whose welfare matters
following a proposal from our son Ilya, an evolutionary biologist, (their own? their family’s? their country’s? all humanity’s?), or
to take out our lawn and let our yard go feral with native Penn- about whether to consider the decision-making process as well as
sylvania flora—after which he taught us how to observe the sea- its outcomes (are they being treated respectfully? are they ceding
sons. Now, I can wonder why the first grackles arrived 10 days early future rights?).
this year, even though the main flock took over the yard on March The goal of science communication is not agreement, but fewer,
7th as usual. Was it a sign of climate change—or of my own im- better disagreements. If that communication affords people a
proved powers of observation? shared understanding of the facts, then they can focus on value
Science provides a sense of wonder not just from revealing the issues, such as how much weight to give to nature, the poor, future
world, but also from showing that the world can be revealed. For generations, or procedural issues in a specific decision. To realize
that reason, I tell my advisees, in our Decision Science major, that that potential, however, people need a venue for discussing value
they should join a research laboratory, any research laboratory, issues. Otherwise, those issues will percolate into discussions of
just to see how science is done. They may discover that they love science (2). For example, if health effects have legal standing in
the life, including the camaraderie of the graduate students and a facility-siting decision, but compensation does not, then local
postdocs who are the engines of much science. Or, they may find residents may challenge health effect studies, when their real
the constant fussing over details to be “mind-numbingly dull” (to concern is money. Science communication cannot succeed when
use the phrase that Calvin, of Calvin and Hobbes, applied to ar- people feel that attacking its message is the only way to get redress
chaeology) and then choose to get their science from places like for other concerns (3).
Nova, Frontline, the New York Times, and Wired. Either way, they Because science communication seeks to inform decision
will have seen the pursuit of uncertainty that distinguishes science making, it must begin by listening to its audience, to identify the
from other forms of knowing and the scientific turn of mind: trying decisions that its members face—and, therefore, the information
to get to the bottom of things, knowing that one never will.
Decisions That Require Understanding Science
This paper results from the Arthur M. Sackler Colloquium of the National Academy of Scien-
Although people can choose not to do science, they cannot choose ces, “The Science of Science Communication,” held May 21–22, 2012, at the National Academy
to ignore it. The products of science permeate their lives. Nuclear of Sciences in Washington, DC. The complete program and audio files of most presentations
power, genetically modified organisms, nanotechnology, geo- are available on the NAS Web site at www.nasonline.org/science-communication.
engineering, and xenotransplantation are just a few of today’s Author contributions: B.F. wrote the paper.
realities that would have been impossible, even inconceivable, The author declares no conflict of interest.
without scientific advances. Their very existence should be a This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. D.A.S. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial
source of wonder, even for people in whom they evoke a sense Board.
of terror (“we can do that?!”). 1
E-mail: baruch@cmu.edu.
site (www.nasonline.org/programs/sackler-colloquia/completed_
comes important to many of them, such as relationships and
colloquia/science-communication.html).
reputations (18).
Task 1: Identify the Science Relevant to Decision Making As elsewhere, such applied basic science can reveal basic applied
science opportunities. For example, we found the need for addi-
Although advances in one science (e.g., molecular biology) may
tional research regarding the effectiveness of self-defense strategies
provide the impetus for a decision (e.g., whether to use a new drug), in reducing the risk of sexual assault, studies that are rare com-
informed choices typically require knowledge from many sciences. pared with the many studies on how victims can be treated and
For example, predicting a drug’s risks and benefits for any patient stigmatized. More generally, the methodological challenges of
requires behavioral science research, extrapolating from the con- asking people what matters to them, when forced to choose among
trolled world of clinical trials to the real world in which people risky options, have changed scientific thinking about the nature of
sometimes forget to take their meds and overlook the signs of side human values. Rather than assuming that people immediately
effects (6). Indeed, every science-related decision has a human know what they want, when faced with any possible choice, decision
element, arising from the people who design, manufacture, inspect, scientists now recognize that people must sometimes construct
deploy, monitor, maintain, and finance the technologies involved. their preferences, to determine the relevance of their basic values
As a result, predicting a technology’s costs, risks, and benefits for specific choices (19, 20). As a result, preferences may evolve,
requires social and behavioral science knowledge, just as it might as people come to understand the implications of novel choices
require knowledge from seismology, meteorology, metrology, (such as those posed by new technologies). When that happens,
physics, mechanical engineering, or computer science. the content of science communications must evolve, too, in order
No layperson could understand all of the relevant sciences to any to remain relevant.
depth. Indeed, neither could any scientist. Nor need they have such
vast knowledge. Rather, people need to know the facts that are Task 2: Determine What People Already Know
“material” to their choices (to use the legal term) (7). That Having identified the scientific facts that are worth knowing,
knowledge might include just summary estimates of expected communication researchers can then identify the subset of those
outcomes (e.g., monetary costs, health risks). Or, it might require facts that are worth communicating—namely, those facts that
enough knowledge about the underlying science to understand why people do not know already. Unless communications avoid re-
the experts make those estimates (8). Knowing the gist of that peating facts that go without saying, they can test their audience’s
science could not only increase trust in those claims, but also allow patience and lose its trust (“Why do they keep saying that? Don’t
members of the public to follow future developments, see why they think that I know anything?”). That can happen, for ex-
experts disagree, and have a warranted feeling of self-efficacy, from ample, when the same communication tries to meet both the
learning—and being trusted to learn—about the topic (9, 10). public’s right to know, which requires saying everything, and its
Thus, the first science of communication is analysis: identifying need to know, which requires saying what matters. Full disclo-
those few scientific results that people need to know among the sure is sometimes used in hopes of hiding inconvenient facts in
myriad scientific facts that it would be nice to know (11, 12). The plain sight, by burying them in irrelevant ones (6, 7, 10).
results of that analysis depend on the decisions that science Identifying existing beliefs begins with formative research, using
communications seek to inform. The scientific facts critical to one open-ended, one-on-one interviews, allowing people to reveal
of views among public health experts and then sided with the
more worried ones. More likely, though, they had heard the great publication standard, even if their topics lack the theoretical in-
concern among the experts and then assumed that the probability terest needed for actual publication. That standard includes test-
was high (“otherwise, why would they be so alarmed?”). Given that ing the validity of the elicitation procedures. One class of tests
the public health experts anticipated a 7% case-fatality rate (their examines the consistency of beliefs elicited in different ways.
median response to another question), a 15% chance of efficient For example, the question in Fig. 1 opened a survey whose final
transmission provides ample reason for great concern. However, question reversed the conditionality of the judgment. Instead of
such numeric judgments were nowhere to be found in the volu- asking about the probability of efficient transmission in 3 years, it
minous coverage of the threat. Even though the ambiguity of asked for the time until the probability reached several values
verbal quantifiers (e.g., “likely”) has long been known, experts are (10%, 50%, 90%). Finding consistent responses to the two for-
mulations suggested that respondents had succeeded in trans-
often reluctant to express themselves in clear numeric terms (14–
lating their beliefs into these numeric terms. A second class of tests
16, 25–27).
examines construct validity, asking whether responses to different
When decision makers are forced to read between the lines of
measures are correlated in predicted ways. For example, in a study
experts’ statements, they cannot make full use of scientific knowl-
of teens’ predictions for significant life events, we found that they
gave high probabilities of getting work, pregnant, and arrested if
they also reported higher rates of educational participation, sexual
activity, and gang activity, respectively—in response to questions
in other parts of the survey. That study also found evidence of
predictive validity, such that the events were more likely for teens
who assigned them higher probabilities. Whether these probabil-
ities were over- or underestimated seemed to depend on teens’
beliefs about the events, rather than on their ability to express
themselves numerically (34).
People are good at tracking what they see, but not at detecting People consider the return on their investment in making decisions.
sample bias. People dislike uncertainty, but can live with it.
People have limited ability to evaluate the extent of their People are insensitive to opportunity costs.
own knowledge. People are prisoners to sunk costs, hating to recognize losses.
People have difficulty imagining themselves in other visceral states. People may not know what they want, especially with novel questions.
People have difficulty projecting nonlinear trends.
People confuse ignorance and stupidity.
an event is disproportionately visible (e.g., because of media re- Even within a domain, knowledge may vary widely by topic. People
porting practices) and they cannot correct for the extent to which forced to learn about cancer may know little about diabetes.
appearances are deceiving. For example, people may think that People adept at reading corporate balance sheets may need help
(widely reported) homicides are more common than (seldom with their own tax forms. As a result, knowing individuals’ general
reported) suicides, even though these two causes of death occur level of knowledge (sometimes called their “literacy”) in a domain
about as often (38). The first choice principle (in Table 1) is that provides little guidance for communicating with them about
people consider the return on investing in decision-making pro- specific issues. For that, one needs to know their mental model for
cesses (39). Applying that principle could lead them to disregard that issue, reflecting what they have heard or seen on that topic
communications that seem irrelevant, incomprehensible, or un- and inferred from seemingly relevant general knowledge.
trustworthy (40). The study of mental models has a long history in cognitive
Each of these principles was a discovery in its time. Each is sup- psychology, in research that views people as active, if imperfect,
ported by theories, explicating its underlying processes. Nonethe- problem solvers (46–48). Studies conducted with communication
less, each is also inconclusive. Applying a principle requires in mind often find that people have generally adequate mental
additional (auxiliary) assumptions regarding its expression in models, undermined by a few critical gaps or misconceptions (8,
specific circumstances. For example, applying the first judgment 21). For example, in a study of first-time mothers’ views of child-
principle to predicting how people estimate an event’s frequency hood immunization (49), we found that most knew enough for
requires assumptions about what they observe, how they encode future communications to focus on a few missing links, such as the
those observations, how hard they work to retrieve relevant importance of herd immunity to people who cannot be immunized
memories, whether the threat of biased exposure occurs to them, and the effectiveness of postlicensing surveillance for vaccine side
and how they adjust for such bias. Similarly, applying the first effects. A study of beliefs about hypertension (50) found that many
Downloaded from https://www.pnas.org by 187.189.101.55 on March 21, 2023 from IP address 187.189.101.55.
choice principle to predicting how people estimate the expected people know that high blood pressure is bad, but expect it to have
return on their investment in reading a science communication perceptible symptoms, suggesting messages about a “silent killer.”
requires assumptions about how much they think that they know A classic study found that many people interpret “once in 100
already, how trustworthy the communication seems, and how ur- years” as a recurrence period, rather than as an annual probability,
gent such learning appears to be. showing the importance of expressing hydrologic forecasts in lay
Moreover, behavioral principles, like physical or biological terms (51).
ones, act in combination. Thus, although behavior may follow At other times, though, the science is so unintuitive that people
simple principles, there are many of them (as suggested by the have difficulty creating the needed mental models. In such cases,
incomplete lists in Table 1), interacting in complex ways. For ex- communications must provide answers, rather than asking people
ample, dual-process theories detail the complex interplay between to infer them. For example, risks that seem negligible in a single
automatic and deliberate responses to situations, as when people exposure (e.g., bike riding, car driving, protected sex) can become
integrate their feelings and knowledge about a risk (9, 10, 13, 35, major concerns through repeated exposure (52, 53). Invoking
38). Getting full value from behavioral science requires studying a long-term perspective might help some (“one of these days, not
not only how individual principles express themselves in specific wearing a helmet will catch up with you”) (54). However, when
settings, but also how they act in consort (41). Otherwise, com- people need more precise estimates of their cumulative risk, they
municators risk grasping at the straws that are offered by simplistic cannot be expected to do the mental arithmetic, any more than
single-principle, “magic bullet” solutions, framed as “All we need they can be expected to project exponential processes, such as the
to do is X, and then the public will understand”—where X might proliferation of invasive species (55), or interdependent nonlinear
be “appeal to their emotions,” “tell them stories,” “speak with ones, such as those involved in climate change (56). If people need
confidence,” “acknowledge uncertainties,” or “emphasize op- those estimates, then someone needs to run the numbers for
portunities, rather than threats” (42). Indeed, a comprehensive them. Similarly, people cannot be expected to understand the
approach to communication would consider not only principles of uncertainty in a field, unless scientists summarize it for them (6,
judgment and choice, but also behavioral principles identified in 11, 16, 27, 57, 58).
studies of emotion, which find that feelings can both aid com-
munication, by orienting recipients toward message content, and Task 4: Evaluate their adequacy and repeat as necessary
undermine it, as when anger increases optimism—and diminishes Poor communications cause immediate damage if they keep
the perceived value of additional learning (43, 44). A compre- people from using available science knowledge. They cause lasting
hensive approach would also consider the influences of social damage if they erode trust between scientists and the public. That
processes and culture on which information sources people trust happens when lay people see scientists as insensitive to their needs
and consult (45). and scientists see lay people as incapable of grasping seemingly
Whereas the principles governing how people think may be basic facts (2, 9, 24, 50). Such mistrust is fed by the natural ten-
quite general (albeit complex in their expression and interaction), dency to attribute others’ behavior to their essential properties
what people know naturally varies by domain, depending on their rather than to their circumstances (59). Thus, it may be more
desire and opportunity to learn. People who know a lot about natural for laypeople to think, “Scientists don’t care about the
health may know little about physics or finance, and vice versa. public” than “Scientists have little chance to get to know the public.”
Conclusion
Communications are adequate if they reach people with the in-
formation that they need in a form that they can use. Meeting that
goal requires collaboration between scientists with subject matter
knowledge to communicate and scientists with expertise in com-
munication processes—along with practitioners able to manage
the process. The Sackler Colloquium on the “Science of Science
Communication” brought together experts in these fields, to in-
teract with one another and make their work accessible to the
general scientific community. Such collaboration affords the sci-
ences the best chance to tell their stories.
It also allows diagnosing what went wrong when communica-
tions fail. Did they get the science wrong, and lose credibility?
Did they get the wrong science, and prove irrelevant? Did they
lack clarity and comprehensibility, frustrating their audiences?
Did they travel through noisy channels, and not reach their
audiences? Did they seem begrudging, rather than forthcoming?
Did they fail to listen, as well as to speak? Did they try to per-
suade audiences that wanted to be informed, or vice versa?
Fig. 2. A process for integrating communication and decision making. Correct diagnoses provide opportunities to learn. Misdiagnoses
Reproduced with permission from ref. 24.
can compound problems if they lead to blaming communication
partners, rather than communication processes, for failures (46).
Downloaded from https://www.pnas.org by 187.189.101.55 on March 21, 2023 from IP address 187.189.101.55.
listen to local residents’ concerns before saying anything”; “we’ll For example, scientists may lose patience with a public that cannot
get you estimates on the economic and social impacts of that seem to understand basic facts, not seeing what little chance the
problem”; “you might be able to explain ocean acidification by public has had to learn them. Conversely, laypeople may lose faith
building on your audience’s mental models of acids, although we in scientists who seem distant and uncaring, not appreciating the
don’t have direct evidence”; or “that’s a new topic for us; we’ll challenges that scientists face in reaching diverse audiences. When
collect some data and tell you what we learn.” By helping individual a science communication is successful, members of its audience
scientists, such a center would serve the scientific community as should agree on what the science says relevant to their decisions.
a whole, by protecting the commons of public goodwill that each They need not agree about what decisions to make if they value
communication either strengthens or weakens. different things. They may not be reachable if issues are so po-
Realizing the value of science communication requires in- larized that the facts matter less than loyalty to a cause that
tegrating it with the decision-making processes that it is meant to interprets them in a specific way (45). However, in such cases, it is
inform. Fig. 2 (76) shows one version of the advice coming from social, rather than communication, processes that have failed.
many consultative bodies (2, 77, 78). In the center is a conven- Thus, by increasing the chances of success and aiding the di-
tional management process, going from initiation to imple- agnosis of failure, the sciences of communication can protect
mentation and monitoring. At the left is a commitment to two-way scientists from costly mistakes, such as assuming that the public
communication at each stage. Thus, a process begins by reporting can’t handle the truth, and then denying it needed information, or
preliminary plans and hearing the public’s thoughts early enough becoming advocates, and then losing the trust that is naturally
to incorporate its input. Sustained contact, throughout the pro- theirs (9, 24, 72). To these ends, we need the full range of social,
cess, reduces the chance of any party blindsiding the other with behavioral, and decision sciences presented at this Colloquium,
unheard views or unexpected actions. In this view, the process coupled with the best available subject matter expertise. When
itself represents a communication act, expressing a belief in the science communication succeeds, science will give society the
public’s right to know and ability to understand. greatest practical return on its investment—along with the sense of
The specifics of such consultations will depend on the decisions wonder that exists in its own right (84).
involved. Siting a factory, wind farm, or halfway house may require
direct conversation with those affected by it. Informing a national ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. Preparation of this paper was supported by the
debate may need social scientists to act as intermediaries between National Science Foundation Grant SES-0949710.
1. Burger J (2002) The Parrot Who Owns Me (Random House, New York). 5. Baddeley AD (1978) Applied cognitive and cognitive applied research. Perspectives on
2. Dietz T (2013) Bringing values and deliberation to science communication. Proc Natl Memory Research, ed Nilsson LG (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ).
Acad Sci USA 110:14081–14087. 6. Schwartz LM, Woloshin S (2013) The Drug Facts Box: Improving the communication of
3. Löfstedt R, Fischhoff B, Fischhoff I (2002) Precautionary principles: General definitions prescription drug information. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:14069–14074.
and specific applications to genetically modified organisms (GMOs). J Policy Anal 7. Merz J, Fischhoff B, Mazur DJ, Fischbeck PS (1993) Decision-analytic approach to de-
Manage 21(3):381–407. veloping standards of disclosure for medical informed consent. J Toxics Liability 15(1):
4. Klahr D (2013) What do we mean? On the importance of not abandoning sci- 191–215.
entific rigor when talking about science education. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110: 8. Bruine de Bruine W, Bostrom A (2013) Assessing what to address in science commu-
14075–14080. nication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:14062–14068.