Self Affirmation On Anxiety and Stress
Self Affirmation On Anxiety and Stress
Self Affirmation On Anxiety and Stress
Morgan, JI and Harris, PR (2015) Evidence that brief self-affirming implementation intentions can
reduce work-related anxiety in downsize survivors. Anxiety, stress, and coping, 28 (5). 563 - 575.
ISSN 1061-5806 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2015.1004665
Document Version:
Article (Accepted Version)
The aim of the Leeds Beckett Repository is to provide open access to our research, as required by
funder policies and permitted by publishers and copyright law.
The Leeds Beckett repository holds a wide range of publications, each of which has been
checked for copyright and the relevant embargo period has been applied by the Research Services
team.
We operate on a standard take-down policy. If you are the author or publisher of an output
and you would like it removed from the repository, please contact us and we will investigate on a
case-by-case basis.
Each thesis in the repository has been cleared where necessary by the author for third party
copyright. If you would like a thesis to be removed from the repository or believe there is an issue
with copyright, please contact us on openaccess@leedsbeckett.ac.uk and we will investigate on a
case-by-case basis.
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 1
Abstract
Background and Objectives: Workers were recruited from a UK further education college during
a period of organisational downsizing. The study assessed the effects of a brief health psychology
employees were randomly allocated to one of two conditions: one in which they were asked to
anxiety and depression were measured before and after the intervention or control task, and three
weeks later. Job satisfaction, self-efficacy, and self-esteem were also measured. Results: There
were statistically significant differences between the WS-AII condition and the control. Workers
reduction in anxiety. This reduction was also observed in their appraisal of job-related anxiety
three weeks later. There were no significant effects of WS-AII’s on depression, job satisfaction
or self-esteem. There was, however a significant effect on self-efficacy with workers in the WS-
AII condition reporting greater self-efficacy. Conclusions: The present findings suggest that the
integration of brief health psychology interventions, such as the WS-AII, into existing
downsize survivors
“The recent financial crisis has affected most countries. With cutbacks in business, trade
and government spending, millions world-wide have lost their jobs…” (Greenglass et al., 2014,
p. 10). In the UK alone, according to Labour Market Statistics, between the 2nd quarter of 2008
(the start of economic recession) and the 4th quarter of 2011, 2.68 million workers were laid off
or made redundant (Philpott, 2012). This active reduction in staff is a “downsizing” strategy
almost universally adopted by companies in difficult financial times, in order to save costs and
remain competitive. Whilst the implications of downsizing for the newly unemployed are
explicitly apparent, there are also underlying consequences for those who remain in post
downsize survivors can include the heightened perception of job threat, the burden of taking on
the responsibilities of their departed colleagues, and an overall increase in reported stress and
anxiety (e.g., Armstrong-Stassen, 1994; Brocker & Wiesenfeld, 1993; Cascio, 1993; Wiesenfeld
et al., 2001). In order to improve employee mental health at work, there is a need to develop
interventions that can reduce or buffer these negative outcomes. With British businesses alone
estimated to lose £26 billion per year as a result of work-related stress (CIPD/MIND, 2011), the
companies, especially those trying to tighten their financial belts. It appears that failing to
effectively manage the negative effects of downsizing may incur more cost and thus render the
improve employee mental health, UK businesses could save up to £8 billion per year
(CIPD/MIND, 2011).
One potentially useful approach is to draw on the extensive health psychology research
literature in order to develop and test interventions that can be easily applied in occupational
settings, i.e., those that will not place any additional strain on employees or their employers. The
present study applies Steele’s (1988) concept of self-affirmation to reduce stress levels in
downsize survivors over both the short- and longer-term. The principal aim of the study was to
test the effectiveness of a brief self-affirmation intervention adapted for use in a work setting.
positive, moral and adaptive self-image and to thereby maintain “self-integrity”. Thus, threats to
the self elicit defensive information processing. According to Steele’s (1988) self-affirmation
theory, however, because people are motivated to defend their global sense of self-worth, self-
affirmation in one domain (e.g., by recalling past acts of kindness) should reduce the need to be
defensive when threatened in another domain (e.g., by job uncertainty). In other words, if a
person’s self-image can be ‘affirmed’ in a domain that is important to them, this should act as a
buffer against threats to the self, and therefore reduce the impact on both physiological and
Evidence suggests that drawing on self-resources can reduce the physiological and
psychological impact of both laboratory-induced (see Creswell et al., 2005; Taylor, Lerner,
Sherman, Sage, & McDowell, 2003), and naturally occurring (see Sherman, Bunyan, Creswell, &
Jaremka, 2009) stressors. For example, one study (Taylor et al., 2003) reported a significant
laboratory stressor, while another study (Creswell et al., 2005) found that self-affirmation
attenuated cortisol response to the Trier Social Stress Task (Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer,
1993). In addition to physiological response, Creswell et al. (2005) found that participants with
higher dispositional self-resources (e.g. trait self-esteem and optimism), who affirmed their core
values, reported the least psychological stress. To our knowledge only one study (Sherman et al.,
responses to an everyday stressor (academic stress prior to an exam). While there was a
cumulative increase in epinephrine (an indicator of sympathetic nervous system activation) in the
urine samples of undergraduates in the control condition in the run up to an exam, there was no
Multiple means of affirming the self exist in the research literature, principally because any
Harris, & Arden, 2011; Armitage & Rowe, 2011; Harris & Epton, 2009). Self-affirming
techniques used to reduce stress and anxiety in lab-based studies have typically added to the
demand placed upon participants, limiting their potential application as useful interventions in
real-world settings such as the workplace. For example, Creswell et al. (2005) used the Values
Questionnaire (Allport, Vernon, & Lindzey, 1960) in which participants are required to identify
five personal values across the domains of religion, social issues, politics, theory, and aesthetics,
rank these in terms of their personal importance, and then answer a series of questions about the
highest ranked value. In studies assessing the effects of self-affirmation on the acceptance of
health risk information and/or health behavior change, methods generally involve participants
being asked to reflect on their most cherished values, either by writing essays or elaborating on
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 5
prompts within questionnaires (e.g. see Reed & Aspinwall’s, 1998, “kindness questionnaire”).
While such methods have been shown to increase participant receptiveness to threatening
messages (e.g., Armitage et al., 2011; Armitage, Harris, Hepton, & Napper, 2008; Epton &
Harris, 2008; Reed & Aspinwall, 1998), and improve health behaviour (see Harris & Epton,
2009, for a review), they have not escaped criticism. Firstly, Napper, Harris, and Epton (2009)
argue that due to their length and complexity, researchers have found it consistently difficult to
studies. Secondly, existing self-affimation methods are generally time-consuming, and require
participants to be verbally fluent (Armitage et al., 2011). To address these latter concerns,
Armitage et al. (2011) developed and tested a briefer, standardized self-affirmation manipulation
Implementation intentions are specific kinds of if-then plans that work by encouraging
people to link in memory-critical situations with appropriate behavioral responses, and which
have been used with some success to change health behaviors (Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006). The
principal idea behind implementation intentions is that the salience of critical situations is
enhanced when they are encountered in the environment and that appropriate behavioral
responses are triggered automatically (Gollwitzer, 1993). Research has shown that forming
implementation intentions can have a significant impact on future behaviour over the longer-term
(Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006). Based on the work of Harris, Napper, Griffin, Schuez, and Stride
(2011), Armitage et al. (2011) sought to develop a brief manipulation in which participants are
asked to form an implementation intention (an if-then plan) to self-affirm. For ‘the self-affirming
implementation intention’ participants were presented with the stem, adapted from Harris et al.
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 6
(2011), “If I feel threatened or anxious, then I will . . .” where “feeling threatened or anxious” is
the critical situation, and a choice of appropriate self-affirming responses include: “thinking
about the things I value about myself” and “remembering things that I have succeeded in”
(Harris, et al., 2011). To test the efficacy of their new self-affirming implementation intention,
Armitage et al. (2011) compared its influence on the acceptance of messages about alcohol risk,
and subsequent alcohol intake, with the effects of an existing self-affirming questionnaire, and an
active control. While overall self-affirmation significantly increased message acceptance and
decreased alcohol consumption, there was an additional small public health advantage associated
with the briefer self-affirming implementation intention and compared to the self-affirming
the authors suggest that this should encourage its future use.
The rationale for the present research is as follows. First, there is a need for workplace
interventions that combat employee stress. While self-affirmation has been shown to reduce
anxiety in student populations and in the laboratory, less is known about the extent to which the
effects persist beyond the experimental session, and whether this can be extrapolated to a
workplace undergoing a stress inducing procedure, such as downsizing. Second, two potential
methodological difficulties with self-affirmation research can be addressed by (a) testing a brief
means of affirming the self that has practical potential for use in the workplace, e.g., that does not
require the respondent to write in length about cherished values, and (b) using a non-self-
affirming implementation intention task as an active control, in order to achieve greater “control
In line with previous research (Creswell et al., 2005; Sherman et al., 2009; Taylor et al.,
implementation intention will reduce the stress response (state anxiety) of workers immediately
after the manipulation. In addition, the strength of the new self-affirmation technique will be
explored by evaluating its impact on job-related stress (anxiety and depression), and job
Although the mechanics of self-affirmation remain unclear (Harris and Epton, 2009), a
number of studies suggest that positive effects can be mediated by increases in self-efficacy (see
Epton and Harris, 2008; Milne, Sheeran, and Orbell, 2000) and/or self-esteem (Armitage, et al.,
2011). To explore this further we will also examine the effect of self-affirmation on the appraisal
of these constructs.
Method
Participants
initially agreed to take part (reduced to n = 28 at 3-week follow-up, see Figure 1). The initial
sample consisted of 22 men and 44 women, aged between 25 and 60 years (M = 45.18 years, SD
= 8.33 years). Participants were asked to indicate their job type, and 36.4% (n = 24) classified
themselves as teaching staff, 16.7% (n = 11) as managers, 33.3% (n = 22) as support staff, and
13.6% (n = 9) as premises maintenance staff. Tenure ranged from 0.5 years to 26 years (M = 8.43
Design
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 8
The research design was mixed. The between-participants variable was condition:
variable was the time interval between baseline and post-manipulation follow-up (immediately
after or 3 weeks later). There were six outcome variables. Job-related wellbeing (anxiety and
depression), and job satisfaction were measured at baseline and at 3-weeks postbaseline. State
anxiety was measured at baseline and immediately following the manipulation, and self-esteem
Procedure
The study was approved by a University ethics committee. Prior to data collection the
research was advertised to all workers via a staff bulletin. The bulletin asked if employees would
be willing to participate in a study about ‘work and well-being’ and provided a date on which
questionnaires would be posted in all staff pigeon holes. The staff bulletin also informed
potential participants that the study would involve filling out a second questionnaire, 3 weeks
later. The lead author numbered all questionnaires before sorting them into a random order (using
random number tables) and placing them in an unmarked folder. The front sheets of all
On the date specified in the staff bulletin, the questionnaires were removed from the folder
and one was placed in each of the staff pigeon holes, by an individual who was unaware of the
conditions. A secure post box was positioned in the staff corridor in order for participants to
deposit their questionnaires after completion. A similar procedure was used at follow-up, with
the addition of a written thank you sent to all staff with a study debrief (in which all workers
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 9
The first page of the questionnaire provided participants with details of their ethical rights
including a request for their consent, as well as instructions for completing the measures.
Demographic measures and measures of job-related wellbeing, job satisfaction and feelings of
state anxiety followed. The self-affirmation manipulation or control task appeared on the next
checks. State anxiety was then measured once more. Participants in the experimental and control
conditions received exactly the same questionnaire content with the exception of the self-
Materials
self- affirming implementation intention was an adapted version of the brief self-affirming
implementation intention developed by Armitage et al. (2011) in which participants are provided
with an implementation intention prompt in the form of a sentence stem, "If I feel threatened or
anxious, then I will…". This is followed by four options: "…think about the things I value about
myself", "…remember things that I have succeeded in", "…think about what I stand for" and
"…think about things that are important to me". Participants are asked to write out the stem and
their chosen option on three blank lines, with "If…" at the start of the first blank line. To reflect
the organisational focus in the present study, the stem was adapted to read "If I feel threatened or
The control implementation intention (CII). In response to a call for greater “control
control implementation intention task for use in the present study. Participants in the control
condition were given the same sentence stem as those in the experimental group, "If I feel
threatened or anxious about work, then I will…", however the four options that followed were
designed to ensure that there was no opportunity for participants to self-affirm. To do this the
options were adapted from existing self-affirmation control tasks. The first option, "…think about
the shops and buildings I pass on a journey I travel regularly”, was taken from the journey
control conceived by Napper et al. (2009). The second option, "…remember the food I have eaten
in the last 48 hours”, was adapted from Cohen’s (2000) food control, and the third and fourth
options, "…think about the most satisfying season of the year”, and "…think about the best
flavour for ice-cream”, were from the personal opinion survey (Reed & Aspinwall, 1998). Akin
intention, participants were asked to rewrite the sentence stem followed by their chosen option on
Measures
Premanipulation. Warr's (1990) job-related wellbeing 12-item scale was used to measure
the axes of anxious-comfort and depressed-enthusiastic. The respondent was asked to evaluate
how frequently his/her job has caused him/her to feel certain feelings during the past 2 weeks.
The anxious-comfort axis is represented by six adjectives, tense, uneasy, worried, calm,
contented, and relaxed. The depressed–enthusiastic axis is comprised of the adjectives depressed,
gloomy, miserable, cheerful, enthusiastic, and optimistic. Respondents indicated the frequency
of their feelings on 6-point response scales, ranging from never (1), to all of the time (6). Positive
items were reverse-scored so that high scores on either axis represents lower levels of work-
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 11
related wellbeing (anxiety or depression). Cronbach’s alpha for each sub-scale indicated good
Job satisfaction was measured using the 16-item job satisfaction scale (Warr, Cook, &
Wall, 1979). Respondents were asked to indicate how satisfied or dissatisfied they are with
different aspects of their jobs such as rate of pay, and the amount of responsibility they are given.
The measure uses 7-point response scales ranging from extremely dissatisfied (1) to extremely
State anxiety was measured using the state version of Marteau and Bekker’s (1992) short
form of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, 1983). Respondents were
asked to indicate the extent to which they were experiencing six affective states at that present
moment in time on 4-point scales ranging from not at all (1) to very much (4). The six states are
"I feel calm", "I am tense", "I feel upset", "I am relaxed", "I feel content", and "I am worried".
After reverse scoring the responses to the positively worded statements, high scores represent
anxiousness, state anxiety was measured again, using the measure described above. Again
Robins, Hendin, and Trzesniewski’s (2001) single-item self-esteem scale was used to
measure levels of self-esteem. Respondents were asked to rate the statement "I have high self-
esteem" on a 5-point scale ranging from not very true of me (1), to very true of me (5).
Schwarzer and Jerusalem’s (1995) general self-efficacy scale was used to measure levels of
self-efficacy. Participants were asked to rate their degree of agreement with 10 statements about
their performance, such as "If I am in trouble, I can usually think of a solution" and "I am
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 12
confident that I could deal efficiently with unexpected events". Responses were made on 4-point
scales ranging from not true at all (1) to exactly true (4). A higher score indicated a higher level
Follow-up. Job-related wellbeing and job satisfaction were measured 3-weeks postbaseline
Results
Randomization Check
variable was condition with two levels: work-related self-affirming implementation intention and
control. The dependent variables were baseline age, gender, tenure, job type, job-related
wellbeing (anxiety and depression), job satisfaction, and state anxiety. The multivariate test, F(8,
57) = 0.84, p = .57, p2 = .11, and all univariate tests, were nonsignificant, Fsunivariate(1, 64) =
0.04 to 3.17, ps > .08, p2s < .05, which suggests that randomization to condition was successful.
The immediate effects of the manipulation were tested using MANCOVA (see Table 1),
anxiety entered as the dependent variables, and premanipulation state anxiety (WS-AII M = 2.85,
SD = 1.00; CII M = 2.79, SD = 0.98) entered as a covariate. The multivariate test, F(3, 61) =
6.66, p < .001, p2 = .25 was significant, as were two of the three univariate tests. There were
significant differences between groups in state anxiety scores, F(1, 63) = 4.48, p = .04, p2 = .07,
and self-efficacy scores, F(1, 63) = 13.71, p < .001, p2 = .18, but not self-esteem scores, F(1, 63)
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 13
= 1.79, p = .19, p2 = .03. Self-affirmation was associated with lower postmanipulation state
missing cases for follow-up measures of job-related wellbeing (anxiety and depression) and job
satisfaction. The chi-square statistic for testing the assumption that the missing data was ‘missing
completely at random’ (MCAR) is referred to as ‘Little’s MCAR test’ (Hill, 1997). If this
assumption is met, “[both] complete cases… [and] EM… methods [of missing data analysis and
imputation] give consistent and unbiased estimates of correlations and covariances” (Hill, 1997,
p. 42). The Little's MCAR test obtained for the present study’s data resulted in χ² (df = 9) =
12.17, p = .21 for the control group, and χ² (df = 9) = 15.34, p = .08 for the experimental group,
which indicates that the data was indeed missing at random (i.e., there was no identifiable pattern
of missing data).
While data was missing at random within each group, there was a proportionally higher
attrition rate in the control condition (65.52%) compared with the intervention condition
(51.35%). A 2 x 2 chi-square analysis was conducted to test whether there was an association
between attrition (completion vs. non-completion) and condition (experimental vs. control).
missing cases replacement, carry forward of last observed response, was utilized. For those
participants with missing data at follow-up, their baseline job-related wellbeing and job
satisfaction scores were inputted. Reported inferential statistics and descriptive statistics (means,
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 14
standard deviations) are from analyses conducted with imputed data. The same analyses
conducted with raw data, excluding imputed missing values are reported as footnotes.
The effects of the manipulation on these outcomes were tested using ANCOVAs (see Table
1). Firstly, Condition (WS-AII vs. CII) was entered as the between-participants factor, work-
related anxiety at follow-up as the dependent variable, and baseline work-related anxiety as the
covariate. The test revealed that participants in the control group were experiencing significantly
more job-related anxiety at follow-up, controlling for baseline job-related anxiety, compared with
the work-related self-affirming implementation intention group, F(1, 63) = 8.14, p = .01, p2 =
.111. Two further ANCOVA’s revealed no significant differences, at follow-up, between control
and self-affirming participants job-related depression, F(1, 63) = 0.42, p = .52, p2 = .012 or job
satisfaction, F(1, 63) = 0.01, p = .92, p2 = .003, when controlling for baseline scores.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to assess the effect of a brief work-based intervention on
stress in downsize survivors at a further education college. The main findings were that,
consistent with previous research, self-affirmation was associated with an immediate reduction in
state anxiety (see Creswell et al., 2005; Sherman et al., 2009; Taylor et al., 2003). Additionally,
for the first time, the present research showed that the effects were sustained longer than the
period immediately following the manipulation, exhibited as a reduction in the appraisal of job-
1
F(1, 25) = 8.76, p = .01, p2 = .26
2
F(1, 25) = 0.20, p = .66, p2 = .01
3
F(1, 25) = 0.16, p = .69, p2 = .01
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 15
related anxiety 3-weeks postbaseline. The remainder of the discussion considers the theoretical
Practical Implications
The present study is the first to test the effects of a brief means of affirming the self in an
occupational setting, compared with the effects of an equivalent control. The findings that
anxiety can be decreased in both the short and longer term suggest that, for the benefit of their
employees, organisations should consider the use of self-affirmation techniques in order to buffer
the effect of stressors, such as ongoing job loss threat, particularly during periods of recession,
organisational change, and more specifically, during downsizing. A novel aspect of the present
research was the evaluation of an intervention that was intended to be simple to complete and
could easily be applied in work settings without placing additional strain on employees or
employers. Full participant adherence to the manipulation instructions to form either a control, or
Despite this cautious optimism, additional work is required to test the validity, reliability,
avenues for this further research. First, the use, and assessment of the present technique may be
extended to other anxiety-provoking work domains (e.g. for health and social care workers,
safety-critical workers, etc.). Second, the present procedure could be refined in order to examine
with broader occupational health initiatives also shown to improve worker well-being and
organizational behavior (e.g. psychosocial safety climate, see Hall, Dollard, Winefield, Dormann,
and Bakker, 2013). Third, considering the positive effect of the WS-AII on work-related anxiety
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 16
and self-efficacy, in line with existing health psychology research on the benefits of self-
affirmation for the processing of health risk information, and health promotion (see Harris &
Epton, 2009), there is the potential to examine the utility of the intervention for work-related
Theoretical Implications
The current findings show that self-affirmation can reduce anxiety outside of the laboratory,
beyond the undergraduate student population, and that the effects can last into the longer-term,
thus increasing the potential generalizability of earlier studies (cf. Sherman et al., 2009; Taylor et
al., 2003). In addition we provide evidence to support the assertion that self-affirmation may
The results concerning job satisfaction were less encouraging. Although our study is the
first to explore the effect of self-affirmation on job satisfaction, we anticipated that any reduction
in feelings of stress might lead to a more positive appraisal of job satisfaction. The present
findings did not support our tentative predictions. One possible explanation centers around the
specificity of our manipulation in comparison with our measures: Because our aim was to assess
a brief means of affirming the self at work, we chose ‘work in general’ as the context for our
implementation intention stem. In contrast, we chose to use a facet job satisfaction scale with a
focus on the appraisal of specific work characteristics (Warr et al., 1979). In future, it might be
valuable to consider measures with a similar level of focus on work in general, or to refine the
current manipulation instructions by asking participants to think about a specific aspect of their
Limitations
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 17
in the study at follow-up, even though they were made aware of the opportunity and had ample
time to participate. A second potential limitation concerns the fact that we only followed
workers over a relatively short period. In order to minimize disruption to the workforce during a
period of considerable pressure, and to comply with senior management wishes, we made the
pragmatic decision to use a 3-week follow-up instead of reassessing participants over a number
of months. While recognizing the potential for similar design constraints, in future studies it
would be useful to see whether the significant effects on anxiety can persist over a longer period.
Conclusions
The present study provides evidence that brief self-affirming implementation intentions can
reduce work-related stress in downsize survivors. Further education college worker anxiety was
reduced in both the short- and longer-term, extending the utility of self-affirmation interventions
beyond student populations and the laboratory. The suggestion is that the integration of brief
health psychology interventions, such as the WS-AII, into existing organisational practices may
be of benefit to the well-being of both further education staff and their employers. Further work
is required to establish whether these benefits can be replicated in other work populations.
STRESS REDUCTION IN DOWNSIZE SURVIVORS 18
REFERENCES
Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1960). Study of values (3rd ed.). Boston:
Houghton-Mifflen.
Armitage, C.J., Harris, P.R., and Arden, M.A. (2011). Evidence that self-affirmation reduces
alcohol consumption: Randomized exploratory trial with a new, brief means of self-
Armitage, C. J., Harris, P. R., Hepton, G., & Napper, L. (2008). Self- affirmation increases
164X.22.1.88
Armitage, C.J., & Rowe, R. (2011). Testing multiple means of self-affirmation, British Journal
Brockner,J. & Wiesenfeld, B.M. (1993). Living on the edge (of social and organizational
psychology): The effects of job layoffs on those who remain. In J.K. Mumighan (Ed.),
Cascio, W.F. (1993). Downsizing: What do we know? What have we learned? Academy of
CIPD/MIND (2011). Managing and supporting mental health at work: disclosure tools for
managers, http://www.cipd.co.uk/binaries/managing-and-supporting-mental-health-at-
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2000). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing
biased evaluation by affirming the self, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9),
Creswell, J. D., Welch, W., Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Gruenewald, T., & Mann, T. (2005).
Epton, T., & Harris, P. R. (2008). Self-affirmation promotes health behavior change, Health
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions, European Review of Social
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A
69–119. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1
Greenglass, E., Antonides, G., Christandl, F., Foster, G., Katter, J.K.Q., Kaufman, B.E., & Lea,
S.E.G. (2014). The financial crisis and its effects: Perspectives from economics and
doi:10.1016/j.socec.2014.01.004
Hall, G.B., Dollard, M.F., Winefield, A.H., Dormann, C., & Bakker, A.B. (2013). Psychosocial
safety climate buffers effects of job demands on depression and positive organizational
Harris, P. R., & Epton, T. (2009). The impact of self-affirmation on health cognition, health
behaviour and other health-related responses: A narrative review, Social and Personality
Harris, P. R., Napper, L., Griffin, D. W., Schuez, B., & Stride, C. (2014). Developing a measure
Hill, M.A. (1997). SPSS Missing Value Analysis 7.5 [Computer program manual].
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K., & Hellhammer, D.H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’-a tool for
Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale
Milne, S., Sheeran, P., and Orbell, S. (2000). Prediction and intervention in health-related
Napper, L., Harris, P. R., and Epton, T. (2009). Developing and Testing a Self-affirmation
Philpott, J. (2012). Counting the cost of the jobs recession, CIPD Work Audit Issue 42,
http://www.cipd.co.uk/binaries/counting-the-cost-of-the-jobs-recession_2012.pdf
Reed, M. B., and Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-
Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global self-esteem:
Wright & M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio, pp. 35–
Sherman, D. K., Bunyan, D. P., Creswell, J. D., & Jaremka, L. M. (2009). Psychological
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014663
Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory.
In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 38, pp. 183–242.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R., & McDowell, N. (2003). Are self-enhancing
Warr, P. B. (1990). The measurement of well-being and other aspects of mental health. Journal
Warr, P. B., Cook, J. D., & Wall, T. D. (1979). Scales for the measurement of some work
Wiesenfeld, B. M., Brockner, J., Petzall, B., Wolf, R., and Bailey, J. (2001). Stress and coping
Table 1
Follow-Up
Control Experimental Fa
n = 29 n = 37
Dependent Variables M SD M SD
*p < .01.
Figure 1
Available sample
N = 120
Randomized to Declined to
condition participate
n = 66 n = 54