The Development of Color Perception and Cognition: Annual Review of Psychology
The Development of Color Perception and Cognition: Annual Review of Psychology
The Development of Color Perception and Cognition: Annual Review of Psychology
87
PS74CH04_Franklin ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 17:0
Contents
1. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88
1.1. Color Perception and Its Relevance to Psychology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88
1.2. Why Investigate the Development of Color Perception and Cognition? . . . . . . 90
2. COLOR VISION AND DISCRIMINATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
2.1. Development of Trichromatic Color Vision in Infancy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
2.2. Color Discrimination Throughout the Life Span . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
2.3. Plasticity, Sensitive Periods, and Early Experience . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
2.4. Tuning In to Chromatic Scene Statistics? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3. USING COLOR AS A CUE FOR OBJECT PERCEPTION
AND COGNITION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3.1. The Development of Color Constancy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
1. INTRODUCTION
1.1. Color Perception and Its Relevance to Psychology
Color is a ubiquitous feature of our psychological experience. The human visual system constructs
a perceptual experience of color from wavelengths of light reflected or emitted from the objects
and surfaces around us (see Figure 1). Color provides a key signal for basic vision. For example, it
is a useful cue for object perception and cognition: It enables us to distinguish between objects of
similar shape and aids the visual segmentation of objects from their backgrounds and the recog-
nition of visual scenes (e.g., Gegenfurtner & Rieger 2000). Color holds useful information about
the properties of objects and scenes (e.g., Osorio & Vorobyev 1996). For example, we use color to
know when fruit is ripe to eat and when meat is cooked. The color of the sky tells us about the time
of day and the weather, and the color of the trees tells us about the season that we are in. Color
provides a signal about people’s internal states—the blush of someone’s cheeks tells us if they
are embarrassed or aroused, whereas the pallor of someone’s skin can indicate poor health (e.g.,
Stephen et al. 2009). Color terms enable us to be descriptive and communicate efficiently (e.g.,
Conway et al. 2020). Color is also strongly associative, with different colors having reliable associa-
tions with emotions (e.g., joyful yellow; Jonauskaite et al. 2019), and abstract concepts (e.g., Tham
et al. 2020). This enables color to be used in symbols and signage (e.g., red for stop, green for go) as
well as in marketing and design to communicate abstract concepts such as romance and environ-
mentalism (e.g., Schloss et al. 2018b). Color also contributes to aesthetics and our appreciation of
art (e.g., Nascimento et al. 2021), and humans have reliable preferences for some colors (e.g., blue)
over others (e.g., chartreuse; e.g., Palmer & Schloss 2010). Finally, color informs the other senses,
with the color of food contributing to how it tastes (e.g., Spence 2015) and the color of a room’s
illumination contributing to its perceived temperature (e.g., Huebner et al. 2016). Given the
importance of color for so many aspects of human mind and behavior, understanding how humans
encode, perceive, talk about, respond to, and use color has been a major interdisciplinary research
effort.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
perience, learning, culture, and environment to perception and cognition in their mature form.
For example, research on infant color categorization has contributed to debate about the extent to
which color categories are arbitrarily constructed by language (e.g., Bornstein et al. 1976). Second,
a developmental approach can provide insight into the processes that underpin perceptual and
cognitive development such as discrimination, constancy, and statistical processing (e.g., Skelton
et al. 2022b). Color can be used as a testing ground for understanding these processes, and com-
parisons with other domains can establish how domain general these processes are. Third, un-
derstanding how infants and children see color has the potential to provide insight into broader
aspects of their minds and behaviors—for example, object reasoning (e.g., Káldy & Blaser 2009)
and language acquisition (e.g., Wagner et al. 2013). Finally, the development of color percep-
tion has relevance to clinical, educational, and industrial contexts. For example, color perception
is atypical in children with neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., Franklin et al. 2010), congenital
color vision deficiency (CVD, or color blindness) in children can present barriers in education
(e.g., Torrents et al. 2011), and children’s color perception has implications for design (e.g., Luo
2006).
summarize the main findings. We also examine the role of experience and consider the hypothesis
that early experience in infancy shapes how color vision develops, determining color vision in its
mature form (e.g., Laeng et al. 2007).
matic gratings (see Figure 1) with various spatial properties (the contrast sensitivity function)
(e.g., Peterzell et al. 1995; see Teller 1998 for a review). Visual evoked potentials (VEPs), which
measure electrophysiological changes over the occipital cortex in response to visual stimuli, have
also identified the development of the chromatic and luminance neural pathways in infancy. For
example, Crognale (2002) charts developmental changes in the VEPs to achromatic, red-green,
and blue-yellow gratings, presenting data for individuals ranging from 1 week to 90 years of age.
VEPs appear for red-green gratings around 4 weeks and for blue-yellow around 6–8 weeks. How-
ever, infants’ VEP waveform is different from that of adults: There are rapid and complex changes
in the waveform shape and latency of components over the first year of life, and by 12 months in-
fants’ chromatic VEPs have a positive negative complex rather than the negative positive complex
of the adult waveform. The shape of the chromatic VEP waveform continues to change through-
out childhood and is not adult-like until 12–14 years of age. Crognale (2002) considers that there
is likely a cortical reason for these changes, as suggested by source localization of child evoked
potentials in another study (Ossenblok et al. 1992). VEPs to achromatic gratings appear in their
mature form at 12–15 weeks of age, pointing to more rapid maturation of luminance than chro-
matic neural pathways.
age until adolescence and thresholds increasing (i.e., sensitivity worsening) thereafter. There is
also a hint that blue-yellow (S-cone) color discrimination may initially develop at a slower rate
than red-green (L- or M-cone discrimination), which is supported by a study that finds that it is
not until 10 years of age that red-green and blue-yellow color discrimination develop at a similar
rate (e.g., Ling & Dain 2018). Other studies suggest a later age of maturation than proposed by
Knoblauch and colleagues, with the age varying from 18 to 30 years across studies (e.g., Paramei
& Oakley 2014). The decline in color discrimination post-maturation has also been investigated:
During this ageing phase, thresholds increase at a rate of around 1% per year for red-green and
1.6% for blue-yellow discrimination over the rest of the life span (Barbur & Rodriguez-Carmona
2015).
These studies that have charted the development and ageing of color discrimination are valu-
able as they have established norms for color discrimination that can guide the diagnosis of color
vision defects at various ages (see Section 5.1.1). The research also further contributes to our un-
derstanding of neural and visual development (and ageing) more broadly. It is currently unclear
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
whether the relatively poor color discrimination of young children hampers their ability to use
color as a cue for identifying objects and their properties, and what kind of real-world color cues
(such as the blushing of skin) might be missed.
long term. Evidence consistent with that hypothesis comes from a psychophysical study that finds
that at 4–6 months of age infant color vision is aligned with the distribution of chromaticities in
the environment (Skelton et al. 2022a). Both adults and 4- to 6-month-old infants have poorest
saturation sensitivity for the chromaticities that have the greatest variation in saturation in natural
scenes (blue and yellow-orange hues), and an efficient encoding account for this effect has been
proposed (Bosten et al. 2015, Skelton et al. 2022a) (see Simoncelli & Olshausen 2001 for more
on efficient encoding; see Section 4.2.1 for more on cultural effects on color perception). These
effects at just 4–6 months of age either point to evolutionary tuning or suggest that infant color
vision tunes in to the chromatic and illumination scene statistics of environments in the first few
months of life. Comparing the color vision of infants born in environments with different chro-
matic and illumination scene statistics (e.g., above versus below the Arctic Circle, or lush versus
arid environments) would clarify whether the effect is due to early tuning.
constancy (e.g., Yang et al. 2013) but that color constancy continues to develop and mature
throughout childhood (e.g., Wedge-Roberts et al. 2022). The evidence for color constancy in
infancy comes from four studies that use the habituation/novelty preference method (Chien et al.
2006, Dannemiller 1989, Dannemiller & Hanko 1987, Yang et al. 2013). These studies each at-
tempted to measure infants’ sensitivity to illumination changes using either physical surfaces and
real illuminations (Chien et al. 2006, Dannemiller & Hanko 1987) or simulated illumination
changes using computer-rendered stimuli (Dannemiller 1989, Yang et al. 2013). If infants are color
constant, their visual systems should discount the effect of illumination changes on surfaces, and
so they would show familiarity (i.e., no novelty preference) to the same surface despite a change in
illumination but a novelty preference when the color of the surface is changed. These studies have
shown that by 4–5 months of age, infants appear to already have some color constancy (Danne-
miller 1989, Dannemiller & Hanko 1987, Yang et al. 2013) as well as lightness constancy (Chien
et al. 2006), whereas color constancy is not evident at 9 weeks (Dannemiller 1989).
There are many questions about how infant constancy compares to constancy in its mature
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
form and what perceptual cues infants draw on. The novelty preference method is a blunt instru-
ment for asking these questions, since infants sometimes prefer to look at the familiar stimulus
during a test depending on the amount of familiarization, and a lack of novelty preference does not
indicate that the familiar stimulus is perceived as visually identical to the novel one (e.g., Houston-
Price & Nakai 2004). Studies that simulate illumination change by changing the chromaticity of
a computer-rendered background stimulus are also clouded by lack of clarity on whether infants
actually perceive this as an illumination change. In adult color constancy studies, the adult can be
instructed that the illumination is changing in the task instructions, and adults can be directed to
find the stimulus that is cut from the same card as the original one; however, such instructions can-
not be given to infants [see Witzel & Gegenfurtner (2018), who argue that even adults may fail to
perceive the change in background as a change in illumination]. These methodological challenges
make further probing of the mechanisms underlying infant color constancy difficult.
The methods that are used to measure color constancy in adults can be gamified to make them
suitable to use with young children from around 2 years of age. Rogers et al. (2020) devised a color
matching game to measure color constancy in which children were required to match printed col-
ored stimuli (cut in the shape of sweaters) with two cardboard bears under different illuminations.
Rogers and colleagues found that at 2–4 years of age some children had color constancy approxi-
mating adults’ performance on the task, but other children had very poor color constancy. Another
study asked 3- to 5-year-old children to assign a set of colored stimuli (cut from the Munsell card)
to color groups based on color terms, and children repeated the task under different room illumi-
nations (Witzel et al. 2021). The way in which children grouped the colors varied only to a small
extent under different illuminations. Both of these studies found a relationship between children’s
color term knowledge and their degree of color constancy, potentially flagging the importance of
color constancy for cognitive development or vice versa (see Section 4.1.2 on color naming for
further discussion).
Color constancy has also been measured in older children, at 6–11 years of age, in a series
of carefully controlled experiments using an object selection task that involved helping a dragon
find his favorite colored sweet under different illuminations (Wedge-Roberts et al. 2022). In two
experiments that used 2D and 3D computer-rendered stimuli, color constancy surprisingly weak-
ened between the ages of 6 and 11, and children had better color constancy than adults. However,
in a third experiment with a small sample of children (N = 15), color constancy was better in
adults than in children when tested with rendered matte and glossy objects under daylight and
non-daylight illuminations (Wedge-Roberts 2021). There was no consistent evidence for a day-
light prior in color constancy that would get stronger with development, and there was tentative
evidence that children’s color constancy is not helped by specular highlights (bright spots of light
on glossy objects) as it is in adults. However, it is unclear whether the developmental differences
are due to children’s use of different cues for color constancy than adults or to differences between
children and adults in interpreting the computer-rendered stimuli, illuminations, and tasks.
Another question is the extent to which the development of color constancy depends on com-
mon perceptual systems that enable other types of perceptual constancy to develop. Color con-
stancy appears to have a similar developmental trajectory to other types of perceptual constancy,
such as size and shape constancy. For example, there is converging evidence that young infants
have rudimentary size constancy (e.g., Granrud 2006), and at large viewing distances this still ap-
pears to be developing at 9 years (e.g., Granrud & Schmechel 2006). For the case of size constancy,
evidence has pointed to the need for children to develop reasoning abilities so that they can use a
deliberate strategy to help their perceptual estimates (Granrud 2009). The roles of reasoning and
deliberate strategies and of object knowledge in color constancy in children remain to be probed.
Direct comparisons of the development of perceptual constancy across domains (e.g., size, shape,
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
again potentially suggesting that color is less prominent than other object properties. There is
limited research on the age at which object color knowledge emerges. Color is diagnostic of certain
objects: Objects such as food, natural objects (e.g., leaves), or faces have a typical (canonical) color.
One study finds that 6-month-olds look longer at typically than at atypically colored faces and fruit,
suggesting that object color knowledge may start to develop in early infancy (Kimura et al. 2010).
However, an unpublished study failed to find a preference (in 5- and 8-month-olds) for naturally
colored faces over digitally manipulated green, purple, and blue faces equated in saturation, and
infant hue preferences were the same when colored stimuli were faces or scrambled faces (Clifford
et al. 2014).
In the real world, changes in the color of objects can also be indicative of the properties of the
objects: Meat changes color when it is cooked, leaves change color with the seasons, fruit changes
color when it ripens, and skin changes color when someone is aroused or ill. In fact, it has been
proposed that the need to be able to register changes in the color of skin or the ripeness of fruit
provided the basis for trichromatic color vision to evolve (e.g., Changizi et al. 2006, Osorio &
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Vorobyev 1996). The development of the ability to use object color changes to infer the prop-
erties of objects is a topic for further research. In the real world, the color of objects also has
statistical regularities. For example, an analysis of the color of a database of real-world images
found that objects were more frequently warm colored (e.g., red) and the backgrounds were more
frequently cool colored (e.g., blue), potentially making warm colors more communicable than cool
ones (Gibson et al. 2017). Research considering when such statistical regularities in the color of
objects feed into the development of object perception and cognition could be fruitful.
Color is important not just for the perception of objects; in adults, color appears to be also
important at both the encoding and retrieval stages when recognizing scenes (Gegenfurtner &
Rieger 2000, Gegenfurtner et al. 1998), and scene category identification is fastest when scenes
are presented in their natural colors (Oliva & Schyns 2000, Wichmann et al. 2002). There has
been little developmental investigation of this topic. Skelton et al.’s (2022b) investigation of in-
fants’ saturation thresholds, outlined in Section 2.4, potentially suggests that infants tune into the
chromatic properties of scenes. Another unpublished study conducted by Skelton and colleagues
further suggests that young infants are sensitive at least to the chromatic properties of scenes,
finding that 6-month-olds’ looking preference for urban compared to rural scenes can be pre-
dicted by a number of low-level spatial and chromatic features of the scene (Skelton et al. 2021).
However, the age at which infants are able to draw on the color information in a scene to encode
and recognize scenes is currently an open question.
in how languages talk about color: Some languages have only a few basic color terms, whereas oth-
ers have 11 or more (e.g., Kay et al. 2009). However, despite this variation, commonalities across
color lexicons have also been identified, and it is now commonly accepted that color categorization
is not an arbitrary process (see Lindsey & Brown 2021 for a review). Here we outline develop-
mental research that has asked questions about how color categories develop and how children
learn the words for color categories. We illustrate that this research contributes to the debate about
the origin and nature of color categories and to our understanding of how infants and children
categorize information and learn terms for categories more generally.
4.1.1. Infant color categorization. One hotly debated question about the development of
color categorization has been whether or not infants can categorize color before they have learnt
the words for color. Bornstein and colleagues’ classic studies were the first to claim that infants
categorize color (e.g., Bornstein et al. 1976). These studies presented infants repeatedly with a
monochromatic light composed of one wavelength so that they habituated to it (i.e., their looking
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
time decreased) and then recorded how long the infants looked at light of a novel wavelength.
Infants only dishabituated to the novel light (i.e., increased their looking) when it was from a dif-
ferent lexical category than the habituated light (e.g., green and blue) but not when it was from
the same lexical category (e.g., both green). Bornstein and colleagues interpreted these findings
as evidence that infants categorize the continuum of wavelength into blue, red, green, and yellow
categories.
Since Bornstein and colleagues’ work, there has been much debate about the existence of in-
fant color categories. Stimulus limitations in those classic studies were identified (e.g., Davies &
Franklin 2002), and a number of subsequent studies have attempted to replicate and extend the
work using stimuli and color spaces that address those limitations. These studies have also used
a range of methods, such as eye-movement search tasks (e.g., Franklin et al. 2005), event-related
potentials (Clifford et al. 2009), and functional near infrared spectroscopy (f-NIRS) (Yang et al.
2016), to attempt to probe the nature of infants’ categorical responses. We refer the reader to
Maule & Franklin (2019) for a detailed overview of these studies, their limitations, and a nuanced
debate about whether infant color categories affect infant color perception or memory. The overall
message of Maule & Franklin’s review is that there is substantial evidence that infants’ recognition
memory responds to color in a categorical manner.
Skelton et al. (2017) further investigated the nature of infant color categories with a large-scale
infant study that mapped infant color categories onto the stimulus array used in a survey of the
world’s color lexicons (World Color Survey; Kay et al. 2009). Using a method similar to that of
Bornstein’s original studies, Skelton and colleagues found that infants’ response parsed the con-
tinuum of hue into five categories: red, green, blue, yellow, and purple. Skelton and colleagues
then used this infant color category map to establish the extent to which it followed the common
pattern of categorization across the world’s color lexicons. The centers of lexical color categories
(category centroids) across the world’s color lexicons tend to cluster around particular points in
color space (Regier et al. 2005), and Skelton and colleagues’ quantitative analyses found that in-
fant color categories were also organized around these hues. This similarity was used to argue
that infant color categorization and the commonality across color lexicons could be partly deter-
mined by similar processes. Further analyses of infants’ responses suggested that the categorical
distinctions infants make can be accounted for by early color representation in retinogeniculate
pathways, providing support for a biological account of infant color categorization. That account
has been challenged based on the claim that early color representation cannot neatly account for
lexical color categories in adults (e.g., Siuda-Krzywicka et al. 2019); yet it is plausible for infant
color categorization to be biologically determined, whereas color lexicons are determined by a
mix of biological, cultural, and linguistic forces. The link between infant color categories and
color lexicons deserves further investigation.
4.1.2. Learning color terms. Although infants appear to coarsely categorize color, children
have the challenge of learning the words for the color lexicon of their own language and culture. It
has been commonly argued that children find this process of color term learning difficult and that
color terms are harder for children to learn than other abstract terms (e.g., Kowalski & Zimiles
2006, Sandhofer & Smith 1999). The age of color term acquisition has come down over time,
with children now typically being able to point to and name the best (focal) examples of the basic
color terms by around 3 years of age (Pitchford & Mullen 2002). However, despite this reduction
in age, a body of work has considered color term acquisition to be atypically effortful and has
investigated the reasons for this (e.g., Rice 1980). One common proposal is that children find it
difficult to abstract the property of color and to know that color is the object property that is
being labeled (e.g., Kowalski & Zimiles 2006). However, Wagner et al.’s (2013) study of color
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
naming has strongly challenged this proposal. Wagner and colleagues carefully considered the
nature of children’s errors before they were able to point to and name a term correctly. They
found that even before children succeeded in accurate comprehension and production of a color
term, their pattern of errors was systematic and indicated some understanding of the terms and the
categorical structure of color. For example, children’s errors at this stage were commonly directed
at similar colors (e.g., red was named as pink). Contrary to the hypothesis that children who do
not know color terms have a problem with abstracting color, Wagner and colleagues argued that
these children can abstract color and respond to it in a meaningful way; they are just working
out the color category boundaries of their own language. Further support for the hypothesis that
children have an understanding of color terms before they pass on to formal comprehension and
production tasks comes from eye tracking and parental reports (Forbes & Plunkett 2020, Wagner
et al. 2018). Wagner et al. (2018) found that children as young as 23 months of age were showing
some color term comprehension with these measures. Forbes and colleagues also found that even
infants as young as 19 months of age could reliably look at the correctly colored object following
an instruction to look at the object with a given color term (Forbes & Plunkett 2019).
These recent studies on color term acquisition have also considered the factors that determine
the speed at which color terms are learnt. Re-analysis of Wagner et al.’s (2013) color naming
data found that three factors could account for whether or not terms were applied consistently
(across objects of the same color) and precisely (not to other colors). The frequency with which
children hear a given term in child-directed speech, the size of a term’s color category, and the
perceptual salience of the best example of a given term could account for the majority of the
variance in Wagner et al.’s (2013) data (Yurovsky et al. 2015). Forbes & Plunkett’s (2020) analysis
of infant language surveys in 12 languages also identifies that the frequency of the color term
in child-directed speech and the syllabic complexity (number of syllables) of a color term are
predictors of the ease with which a term is learnt (Forbes & Plunkett 2020). These findings are
important because these factors also affect the learning of concrete nouns, providing support for
the hypothesis that color terms are not a special case: Color term acquisition is driven by the same
mechanisms as the acquisition of other kinds of words.
Wagner et al.’s (2013) proposal that color term acquisition involves a “gradual inductive pro-
cess” of learning the category boundaries of a given term points to the importance of investigating
color term learning with stimulus sets that include both focal examples (e.g., category centroids)
and poor examples (e.g., colors at the category boundaries) of color terms. Studies that have asked
children to sort stimulus sets similar to those used in adult color naming research (e.g., World
Color Survey) into groups based on color terms have revealed the gradual sharpening of color
category boundaries (Bonnardel & Pitchford 2006, Raskin et al. 1983, Witzel et al. 2021). Re-
cent cross-cultural studies of children’s color naming using a set of 93 colors find that while input
frequency and category size determine the rate of learning category centroids (as in Yurovsky
et al. 2015), it is the inconsistency with which a color is named by adults that determines whether
Japanese and German 3- and 5-year-olds name boundary colors accurately (Imai et al. 2020, Saji
et al. 2020). These studies highlight the importance of considering color term acquisition as a pro-
cess of category learning rather than a process of simply learning the names for the best examples
of a term.
Another line of research has considered the effect that color term acquisition has on color
perception. Research testing the Whorfian hypothesis that language affects perception has found
that color category effects on perceptual tasks are lateralized to the language-dominant left hemi-
sphere (e.g., Drivonikou et al. 2007, Gilbert et al. 2006). Developmental research has also found
a switch in the hemispheric lateralization of the effect of color categories from the right hemi-
sphere to the left hemisphere as color terms are learnt (Franklin et al. 2008a,b). However, these
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
laterality studies suffer from the constraints of the split-field measure of hemispheric lateraliza-
tion, and lateralization of color category effects in adults has been found to be unreliable across
studies (Witzel & Gegenfurtner 2011), potentially due to the fact that factors such as spatial at-
tention and expertise determine the pattern of lateralization. Nevertheless, there are other hints
that learning color terms corresponds with changes in color perception. For example, as outlined
in Section 3.1, Rogers et al.’s (2020) investigation of color constancy in preschoolers found a cor-
relation between the number of color terms a child knows and the degree of their color constancy,
although the direction of this relationship is unknown.
4.2.1. Children’s color preferences and the development of sex differences. Like those
of adults, children’s color preferences are also reliable and systematic and vary with hue: Ling
& Hurlbert (2011) identified that the hue-preference curves of 8- to 9- and 11- to 12-year-olds
were highly similar to those of adults, with a few notable differences. One major focus of devel-
opmental color preference studies has been the question of whether sex differences are culturally
constructed or biologically driven. Potentially in support of this, Ling & Hurlbert (2011) found
that sex differences in color preference were amplified at the start of adolescence. However, other
developmental work has favored cultural accounts of sex differences in color preference. For ex-
ample, girls’ preference for and boys’ aversion to pink start to arise around 2.5 years of age, when
sex-stereotyped behaviors also appear (LoBue & deLoache 2011). Sex differences in color prefer-
ence are so strong around this age that they affect toy preference (Wong & Hines 2015), and some
have argued that coding the gender of toys using color leads to gender differences in social and
developmental outcomes (e.g., Orenstein 2011). In support of a cultural account of sex differences
in color preference, sex differences are not found in 4- to 11-year-olds from three small-scale soci-
eties that are not strongly influenced by global industrialization (Davis et al. 2021; see also Taylor
et al. 2013a for adult data).
4.2.2. Infant color preferences. Research on infants’ visual preferences for color can also con-
tribute to understanding color preferences in their mature form. Studies that have recorded how
long infants look at patches of color when shown individually or in pairs have found that around
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
3–4 months of age, infants look longest at blues, a long time at reds and purples, and the least
time at yellows and greens (e.g., Bornstein 1975, Skelton & Franklin 2020, Taylor et al. 2013b,
Zemach et al. 2007); as for adults, their response follows a systematic hue-preference curve (e.g.,
Brown & Lindsey 2013). The nature of these early visual preferences has been examined, and psy-
chophysical experiments have established that they are not determined by adult-like brightness or
saturation differences, colorimetric purity, or infants’ chromatic detection thresholds; rather, they
are best accounted for by “spontaneous hue preference” (Zemach et al. 2007). Other work has
shown that infants’ color preferences, like adults’, can be summarized in terms of weights on the
cone-opponent axes (e.g., Taylor et al. 2013b), and infant color preferences from three studies have
been effectively modeled with a cone-opponent model of color vision (Brown & Lindsey 2013).
This modeling also found that infants’ preferences were best explained when the luminance of the
stimuli was not taken into account, providing compelling evidence that infants of this age are able
to respond to colors on the basis of their hue, independent of their other perceptual dimensions
(see also Rogers et al. 2018).
One question is the extent to which these early visual preferences for color relate to adults’
aesthetic color preferences in their mature form. Infant looking preferences, of course, do not
necessarily indicate that an infant likes something, and a number of processes are known to be
related to how long an infant looks at a stimulus, such as novelty and complexity (Houston-Price &
Nakai 2004). However, infants’ visual preferences for color do bear a striking similarity to adults’
aesthetic color responses. For example, how long a 4- to 6-month-old infant looks at different
colors significantly correlates with adults’ ratings of how much they like those same colors, with
almost half of the variance shared between the two measures (Skelton & Franklin 2020). This
striking similarity between adults’ aesthetic color preferences and infants’ visual preferences for
colors raises the question of whether or not these two types of preference are driven by similar
mechanisms. Both infant and adult preferences can be summarized by how colors activate cone-
opponent mechanisms (e.g., Skelton & Franklin 2020), and one possibility is that the preferences
of both groups have a similar sensory component. Color preferences may start in infancy as an
early sensory bias for looking longer at some hues over others and then develop into aesthetic
and hedonic preferences during development, when meaningful interactions with colored objects,
concepts, and environments increase. As these aesthetic preferences develop, they are likely to be
shaped by an individual’s identity, experiences, and culture (e.g., Schloss et al. 2011).
Beyond the developmental work on color preferences for individual colors, we know little
about whether color contributes to children’s aesthetic appreciation of art as it does in adults (e.g.,
Nascimento et al. 2017) or even to the development of aesthetics generally (e.g., Krentz & Earl
2013). Further developmental work has the potential to shed light on the nature of aesthetics. Sim-
ilarly, other than one study that identifies color–emotion associations at 3 years of age (Zentner
2001), there is little understanding of how these associations develop, and further developmental
work could clarify how color–emotion associations are shaped by experience.
5.1.1. Diagnosis of color vision deficiency in children. Screening for CVD in schools or
by opticians is not routine in many countries, and in the United Kingdom around 80% of af-
fected pupils starting secondary school (age 11–12) are unaware that they have CVD (Albany-
Ward 2005). Tests used to diagnose CVD in adults (e.g., the gold-standard anomaloscope) are
difficult for young children to complete, and nonvisual factors often cloud the interpretation of
the results when testing children (Tang et al. 2022). A number of pediatric tests for CVD do ex-
ist (e.g., the Ishihara test for the unlettered; Ishihara & Ishihara 2016), yet many of these can
also be difficult for young children, do not control for nonvisual factors, or lack sufficient data
on sensitivity and specificity at various ages (see Tang et al. 2022, table 1). These pediatric tests
for CVD are also often not kept at hand by opticians or are costly for teachers or parents to
obtain. A new test, ColorSpot, aims to make pediatric CVD testing more readily available, child-
friendly, and accurate (Tang et al. 2022). The test combines gamification and psychophysics and
is designed as an iPad app using color calibrations for numerous iPad models. ColorSpot can be
self-administered, is completed in around 5 minutes by children as young as 4 years old, and diag-
noses the same children as the Ishihara for the unlettered while being more decisive about children
who are unclassified by that test (Tang et al. 2022). The test is open access for research purposes
(https://osf.io/v5p2y/) and may serve as a useful screening tool for research with children that
involves color or as a measure of children’s color discrimination.
5.1.2. Impact of color vision deficiency on children’s daily life, education, and well-being.
Although red-green color blindness is commonly interpreted as an inability to see red and green, it
actually affects any color discriminations to which differences in redness or greenness contribute.
For example, purple and blue are commonly confused by those with CVD, as these differ in red-
ness. In adults, everyday tasks such as seeing the blush or pallor of skin, identifying whether meat is
cooked, or pairing colored socks are affected, and adults with CVD commonly report problems in
daily life related to their CVD (e.g., Tagarelli et al. 2004). In the case of children, education and the
classroom are highly color coded: For example, color is used in teaching materials, to give feed-
back, in topics such as art, math, geography, and science (e.g., litmus paper) as well as in the color
coding of sports teams (e.g., Mashige 2019). Suero et al. (2005) showed that CVD in preschoolers
has a negative impact on their performance on standard classroom tasks (e.g., counting beads on
a colored abacus or reproducing colored geometrical figures), and that teachers unaware of their
diagnoses rate CVD preschoolers as poorer achievers. Another study found that 10% of exercises
in math textbooks for 5- to 7-year-old Catalan children are inaccessible for observers with CVD
(Torrents et al. 2011). Grassivaro Gallo et al. (2002) also found lower school achievement for CVD
high school students compared to students without CVD.
A quality-of-life questionnaire developed for adults with CVD [the Colorblind Quality of Life
(CBQoL); Barry et al. 2017] indicates that adults frequently experience emotional difficulties in
relation to their difficulties with color, such as feeling anxious, depressed, or embarrassed. A pe-
diatric version of the CBQoL would be useful to determine the extent of such emotional diffi-
culties in children and adolescents with CVD. One large study of 8- to 11-year-olds found that
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock 1983) identified greater behavioral and
emotional difficulties in children with CVD compared to children without CVD (Thomas et al.
2018), although differences on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Muris et al. 2003) at
8–11 years of age have not been found (Nithiyaananthan et al. 2020).
Another question for further research is whether early diagnosis and support help alleviate any
of the impacts of CVD on quality of life. Although there is currently no available treatment for
CVD, early diagnosis and support from teachers and parents could be beneficial. For children who
have received a diagnosis, teaching materials can be made CVD-friendly with simple modifications
(e.g., by making them gray scale or adding texture in place of color; e.g., Rubin et al. 2009). If
children know why they are struggling with some tasks at school, this could also prevent the
buildup of behavioral and emotional difficulties. A large-scale intervention study is needed that
systematically evaluates the benefits of CVD diagnosis and subsequent support on the quality of
life of children with CVD at different stages of development.
5.1.3. Color vision deficiency and perceptual development. Adults with CVD are surpris-
ingly good at naming colors, particularly for highly saturated focal colors. One theory is that those
with CVD learn strategies to compensate for their CVD, such as attending to lightness differences
between colors (e.g., Lillo et al. 2014), although it is unclear when such strategies are learnt. An
fMRI study also suggests that adults with anomalous trichromacy neurally compensate for the
deficiency in their color signals, finding that the color signals in V2 and V3 of anomalous trichro-
mats are boosted (Tregillus et al. 2021). It is unclear whether this neural compensation for CVD
is learnt during development, when neural plasticity is heightened. Heightened visual plasticity in
early development may also affect the efficacy of glasses that have been designed to boost color
vision in anomalous trichromats (the lenses use notch filters that amplify differences in color; e.g.,
Werner et al. 2020). For example, if the perceptual effects of such lenses harness the plasticity of
cortical color signals, then their effects could be potentially stronger if used early in development,
when the visual system is more open to calibration. These proposals have not yet been tested.
that lower levels of retinal dopamine in ADHD reduce the efficiency of short wavelength cones
(e.g., Kim et al. 2014). The case of autism is outlined next.
Anecdotal reports of atypical color perception and cognition in autism are common, with re-
ports of hypersensitivity to certain colors or obsessions and aversions for particular colors that
govern daily life (e.g., Ludlow et al. 2014). For example, one case study identifies an autistic child
whose color obsessions were so strong that he painted the family dog blue and only wore dark blue
clothes, his bedroom was painted purple and black to try to alleviate stress, and he would only eat
foods with little color (Ludlow et al. 2014). Despite the anecdotal evidence, there has not yet been
a systematic investigation of the prevalence of color obsessions or atypical sensitivity to color, an
explanation for why this affects some autistic individuals but not others, or an investigation of what
underpins the phenomenon. It does appear, however, that various aspects of color perception and
cognition are atypical in autism. Early studies that assessed children’s performance when searching
for, memorizing, sorting, making similarity judgments, or attending to large suprathreshold color
differences pointed to atypical color perception in autistic children (e.g., Franklin et al. 2008c,
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Heaton et al. 2008). For example, children with autism performed more poorly at memorizing
a colored target and searching for it compared to a control group of children without autism
matched on nonverbal cognitive ability (Franklin et al. 2008c).
Chromatic discrimination in autistic children and adolescents also appears to be poorer relative
to that in non-autistic individuals, both on the Farnsworth-Munsell 100 Hue Test, controlling
for nonverbal ability (Franklin et al. 2010), and on tasks that measure chromatic thresholds for
both red-green and blue-yellow discriminations (Cranwell et al. 2015, Franklin et al. 2010, Zachi
et al. 2017) where performance is not predicted by IQ (Cranwell et al. 2015). Further evidence
for atypical color discrimination comes from a VEP study that finds prolonged N1 latencies (a
negative component of around 100 ms) to chromatic gratings in adolescents and adults with autism
(Fujita et al. 2011). It is worth pointing out here that the difference in group chromatic thresholds,
although significant, is small and may have limited importance for daily functioning (Franklin et al.
2010), and only a subset of autistic individuals (e.g., 30% in Zachi et al. 2017) perform outside of
the “normal” range. In addition, discrimination of moving chromatic contrast is typical: Koh et al.
(2010) find typical sensitivity to moving chromatic gratings in autistic adolescents, and siblings of
autistic individuals even have heightened chromatic sensitivity under these conditions.
The findings on atypical color discrimination in autism have been used to inform models of
perceptual and neural functioning in autism (e.g., Pellicano & Burr 2012). A series of studies that
identified atypical perception of colored ensembles (Maule et al. 2017) but typical color adaptation
(Maule et al. 2018) in adults with autism has also informed the theory that perceptual priors are
attenuated in autism (Pellicano & Burr 2012); however, these color phenomena have not yet been
investigated in children. Further work that more fully characterizes color perception and cognition
in autistic children, identifying the source of individual variability and the neural basis of atypical
performance, may be informative for understanding autism and perceptual development more
generally.
6. CONCLUSION
As seen throughout this review, there has been a concerted effort to understand how infants and
children perceive and think about color and to understand how such perception and cognition
develop. We have seen how many aspects of color perception are present early in infancy in a rudi-
mentary form. Trichromatic color vision is in place just a few months after birth (e.g., Teller 1998),
and by 6 months of age infants have developed perceptual mechanisms, such as color constancy
(e.g., Yang et al. 2013), that enable them to start to use color cues to perceive objects and scenes
(e.g., Káldy & Blaser 2009). However, we have also seen that, despite these early competencies,
color perception and cognition are slow to mature, and many aspects such as chromatic discrim-
ination and color constancy do not mature until late childhood or adolescence (e.g., Knoblauch
et al. 2001). The rudimentary processes in infancy and protracted maturation throughout child-
hood have the potential to provide insight into the neural basis of visual development as well as
the relative contributions of biology, experience, culture, and environment to color perception
and cognition. For example, the similarity of infants’ early visual color preferences and color cate-
gories to common patterns of color preference and categorization in adults potentially suggests at
least a partial sensory and biological basis for these phenomena in their mature form (e.g., Skelton
& Franklin 2020, Skelton et al. 2017).
We have also seen how comparing the developmental trajectory of color perception and cog-
nition to other perceptual traits can help identify the domain-general aspects of perceptual and
cognitive development. For example, shape and color constancy have similar developmental trajec-
tories, potentially pointing to common perceptual mechanisms. As for other perceptual domains,
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
at least some aspects of color perception appear to be affected by and potentially tuned to early
experience (e.g., Laeng et al. 2007). In addition, while color term acquisition was once thought of
as a special case, it now appears that the factors that influence color term acquisition are similar
to those that influence other types of word learning (e.g., Saji et al. 2020). However, other com-
parisons suggest that color cues may be more difficult for infants and children to draw on than
perceptual cues such as shape—for instance, in the case of object perception and cognition (e.g.,
Wilcox 1999).
While the relative importance of color compared to other perceptual features can be debated, it
is clear that color has an impact on how children perceive, think about, and interact with the world
around them. We have seen that even in children as young as 2 years old, color preferences affect
toy choice (e.g., Wong & Hines 2015), and autistic children can have strong color obsessions that
govern what they eat and other aspects of daily life (e.g., Ludlow et al. 2014). Children with CVD
are potentially disadvantaged in an education system in which color-coded teaching materials
are inaccessible to them (e.g., Torrents et al. 2011). Finally, there is even some suggestion that
a reduced ability to see color can affect children’s well-being and emotional functioning (e.g.,
Thomas et al. 2018).
There have been notable advances in our understanding of the development of color perception
and cognition, although there remain a number of key issues for future research (see the Future
Issues section below). These future issues illustrate the potential for developmental color science
to have implications beyond the topics of color and perceptual development. Further research
on the development of color perception and cognition can contribute to a broad range of funda-
mental topics in psychology, such as the role of experience and environment in human develop-
ment, the statistical basis of perception, the nature of cortical representation, and the factors that
influence children’s educational outcomes and well-being.
FUTURE ISSUES
1. What aspects of color perception tune to early experience, and what is the time course of
any tuning? For example, does having congenital cataracts in infancy affect the long-term
development of aspects of color perception, such as chromatic sensitivity at high spatial
frequencies or color constancy? Do infants raised in different chromatic environments
develop different patterns of hue sensitivity that can be predicted by the environmental
differences in chromatic scene statistics?
2. When and how do infants and children draw on real-world color cues? For example,
what statistical regularities in the colors of objects and natural scenes are infants and
children sensitive to, and how are these used in object perception and cognition?
3. What changes in the cortical representation of color underpin the development of color
perception and cognition? For example, are regions of the ventral visual cortex special-
ized for color in early infancy as in adulthood, or does this specialization develop with
visual experience?
4. What is the impact of color on children’s daily functioning, education, and well-being?
For example, does CVD impact children’s quality of life as it does in adults, and does
early diagnosis of CVD make a difference to developmental outcomes?
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors are not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that
might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
J.M. and A.E.S. made an equal contribution to this article and are joint first authors. We thank
Jenny Bosten and Christoph Witzel for constructive feedback on an earlier draft of this review.
The article is part of project COLOURMIND, which has received funding from the European
Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program
(grant agreement 772193, awarded to A.F.).
LITERATURE CITED
Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. 1983. Manual for the Child: Behavior Checklist and Child Behavior Profile. Burling-
ton: Univ. Vt.
Albany-Ward K. 2005. What do you really know about colour blindness? Br. J. Sch. Nurs. 10(4):123–24
Barbur J, Rodriguez-Carmona M. 2015. Color vision changes in normal aging. In Handbook of Color Psychology,
ed. A Elliot, M Fairchild, A Franklin, pp. 180–96. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
Barry J, Mollan S, Burdon M, Jenkins M, Denniston A. 2017. Development and validation of a questionnaire
assessing the quality of life impact of Colour Blindness (CBQoL). BMC Ophthalmol. 17(1):179
Birch J. 2012. Worldwide prevalence of red-green color deficiency. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 29(3):313–20
Bonnardel V, Pitchford NJ. 2006. Colour categorization in preschoolers. In Progress in Colour Studies, Vol. 2,
ed. N Pitchford, CP Biggam, pp. 121–38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Bornstein MH. 1975. Qualities of color vision in infancy. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 19(3):401–19
Bornstein MH, Kessen W, Weiskopf S. 1976. The categories of hue in infancy. Science 191(4223):201–2
Bosten JM, Beer RD, MacLeod DIA. 2015. What is white? J. Vis. 15(16):5
Bosworth RG, Dobkins KR. 2009. Chromatic and luminance contrast sensitivity in fullterm and preterm
infants. J. Vis. 9(13):15.1–16
Bosworth RG, Dobkins KR. 2013. Effects of prematurity on the development of contrast sensitivity: testing
the visual experience hypothesis. Vis. Res. 82:31–41
Brown AM, Lindsey DT. 2013. Infant color vision and color preferences: a tribute to Davida Teller. Vis. Neu-
rosci. 30(5–6):243–50
Changizi MA, Zhang Q, Shimojo S. 2006. Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision. Biol.
Lett. 2(2):217–21
Chien SHL, Bronson-Castain K, Palmer J, Teller DY. 2006. Lightness constancy in 4-month-old infants. Vis.
Res. 46(13):2139–48
Clifford A, Franklin A, Davies IR, Holmes A. 2009. Electrophysiological markers of categorical perception of
color in 7-month old infants. Brain Cogn. 71(2):165–72
Clifford A, Witzel C, Chapman A, French G, Hodson R, et al. 2014. Memory colour in infancy? Perception
43:151 (Abstr.)
Conway BR, Chatterjee S, Field GD, Horwitz GD, Johnson EN, et al. 2010. Advances in color science: from
retina to behavior. J. Neurosci. 30(45):14955–63
Conway BR, Ratnasingam S, Jara-Ettinger J, Futrell R, Gibson E. 2020. Communication efficiency of
color naming across languages provides a new framework for the evolution of color terms. Cognition
195:104086
Cranwell MB, Pearce B, Loveridge C, Hurlbert AC. 2015. Performance on the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue
test is significantly related to nonverbal IQ. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 56(5):3171–78
Crognale MA. 2002. Development, maturation, and aging of chromatic visual pathways: VEP results. J. Vis.
2(6):438–50
Dannemiller JL. 1989. A test of color constancy in 9-and 20-week-old human infants following simulated
illuminant changes. Dev. Psychol. 25(2):171–84
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Dannemiller JL, Hanko SA. 1987. A test of color constancy in 4-month-old human infants. J. Exp. Child.
Psychol. 44(2):255–67
Davies I, Franklin A. 2002. Categorical similarity may affect colour pop-out in infants after all. Br. J. Dev.
Psychol. 20(2):185–203
Davis JT, Robertson E, Lew-Levy S, Neldner K, Kapitany R, et al. 2021. Cultural components of sex differ-
ences in color preference. Child Dev. 92(4):1574–89
Dobkins KR, Bosworth RG, McCleery JP. 2009. Effects of gestational length, gender, postnatal age, and birth
order on visual contrast sensitivity in infants. J. Vis. 9(10):19
Drivonikou GV, Kay P, Regier T, Ivry RB, Gilbert AL, et al. 2007. Further evidence that Whorfian effects
are stronger in the right visual field than the left. PNAS 104(3):1097–1102
Forbes SH, Plunkett K. 2019. Infants show early comprehension of basic color words. Dev. Psychol. 55(2):240–
49
Forbes SH, Plunkett K. 2020. Linguistic and cultural variation in early color word learning. Child Dev.
91(1):28–42
Franklin A, Drivonikou GV, Bevis L, Davies IR, Kay P, Regier T. 2008a. Categorical perception of color is
lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults. PNAS 105(9):3221–25
Franklin A, Drivonikou GV, Clifford A, Kay P, Regier T, Davies IR. 2008b. Lateralization of categorical
perception of color changes with color term acquisition. PNAS 105(47):18221–25
Franklin A, Pilling M, Davies I. 2005. The nature of infant color categorization: evidence from eye movements
on a target detection task. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 91(3):227–48
Franklin A, Sowden P, Burley R, Notman L, Alder E. 2008c. Color perception in children with autism.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38(10):1837–47
Franklin A, Sowden P, Notman L, Gonzalez-Dixon M, West D, et al. 2010. Reduced chromatic discrimination
in children with autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Sci. 13(1):188–200
Fujita T, Yamasaki T, Kamio Y, Hirose S, Tobimatsu S. 2011. Parvocellular pathway impairment in autism
spectrum disorder: evidence from visual evoked potentials. Res. Autism Spectrum Disord. 5(1):277–85
Gegenfurtner KR, Rieger J. 2000. Sensory and cognitive contributions of color to the recognition of natural
scenes. Curr. Biol. 10(13):805–8
Gegenfurtner KR, Wichmann FA, Sharpe LT. 1998. The contribution of color to visual memory in
X-chromosome-linked dichromats. Vis. Res. 38(7):1041–45
Gibson E, Futrell R, Jara-Ettinger J, Mahowald K, Bergen L, et al. 2017. Color naming across languages
reflects color use. PNAS 114(40):10785–90
Gilbert AL, Regier T, Kay P, Ivry RB. 2006. Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not
the left. PNAS 103(2):489–94
Granrud CE. 2006. Size constancy in infants: 4-month-olds’ responses to physical versus retinal image size.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 32(6):1398–404
Granrud CE. 2009. Development of size constancy in children: a test of the metacognitive theory. Atten.
Percept. Psychophys. 71(3):644–54
Granrud CE, Schmechel TT. 2006. Development of size constancy in children: a test of the proximal mode
sensitivity hypothesis. Percept. Psychophys. 68(8):1372–81
Grassivaro Gallo P, Oliva S, Lantieri P, Viviani F. 2002. Colour blindness in Italian art high school students.
Percept. Motor Skills 95(3):830–34
Heaton P, Ludlow A, Roberson D. 2008. When less is more: poor discrimination but good colour memory in
autism. Res. Autism Spectrum Disord. 2(1):147–56
Hensch TK. 2005. Critical period mechanisms in developing visual cortex. Curr. Top. Dev. Biol. 69:215–37
Houston-Price C, Nakai S. 2004. Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference proce-
dures. Infant Child Dev. Int. J. Res. Pract. 13(4):341–48
Huebner GM, Shipworth DT, Gauthier S, Witzel C, Raynham P, Chan W. 2016. Saving energy with light?
Experimental studies assessing the impact of colour temperature on thermal comfort. Energy Res. Soc. Sci.
15:45–57
Hurlbert AC, Ling Y. 2007. Biological components of sex differences in color preference. Curr. Biol.
17(16):R623–25
Imai M, Saji N, Große G, Schulze C, Asano M, Saalbach H. 2020. General mechanisms of color lexicon
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
acquisition: insights from comparison of German and Japanese speaking children. In Proceedings of the
42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. S Denison, M Mack, Y Xu, BC Armstrong,
pp. 3315–21. Austin, TX: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
Ishihara S, Ishihara M. 2016. Ishihara’s Design Charts for Colour Deficiency of Unlettered Persons. Tokyo: Kanehara
Trading
Jandó G, Mikó-Baráth E, Markó K, Hollódy K, Török B, Kovacs I. 2012. Early-onset binocularity in preterm
infants reveals experience-dependent visual development in humans. PNAS 109(27):11049–52
Johnson EK, McQueen JM, Huettig F. 2011. Toddlers’ language-mediated visual search: They need not have
the words for it. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 64(9):1672–82
Jonauskaite D, Abdel-Khalek AM, Abu-Akel A, Al-Rasheed AS, Antonietti JP, et al. 2019. The sun is no fun
without rain: Physical environments affect how we feel about yellow across 55 countries. J. Environ.
Psychol. 66:101350
Káldy Z, Blaser E. 2009. How to compare apples and oranges: infants’ object identification tested with equally
salient shape, luminance, and color changes. Infancy 14(2):222–43
Káldy Z, Leslie AM. 2003. Identification of objects in 9-month-old infants: integrating “what” and “where”
information. Dev. Sci. 6(3):360–73
Kay P, Berlin B, Maffi L, Merrifield WR, Cook R. 2009. The World Color Survey. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publ.
Kim S, Al-Haj M, Chen S, Fuller S, Jain U, et al. 2014. Colour vision in ADHD: part 1—testing the retinal
dopaminergic hypothesis. Behav. Brain Funct. 10:38
Kimura A, Wada Y, Yang J, Otsuka Y, Dan I, et al. 2010. Infants’ recognition of objects using canonical color.
J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 105(3):256–63
Knoblauch K, Vital-Durand F, Barbur JL. 2001. Variation of chromatic sensitivity across the life span. Vis. Res.
41(1):23–36
Knudsen EI. 2004. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
16(8):1412–25
Koh HC, Milne E, Dobkins K. 2010. Contrast sensitivity for motion detection and direction discrimination
in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders and their siblings. Neuropsychologia 48(14):4046–56
Kowalski K, Zimiles H. 2006. The relation between children’s conceptual functioning with color and color
term acquisition. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 94(4):301–21
Krentz UC, Earl RK. 2013. The baby as beholder: Adults and infants have common preferences for original
art. Psychol. Aesthet. Creativity Arts 7(2):181–90
Laeng B, Brennen T, Elden Å, Paulsen HG, Banerjee A, Lipton R. 2007. Latitude-of-birth and season-of-birth
effects on human color vision in the Arctic. Vis. Res. 47(12):1595–607
Lillo J, Moreira H, Alvaro L, Davies I. 2014. Use of basic color terms by red-green dichromats: 1. General
description. Color Res. Appl. 39(4):360–71
Lindsey DT, Brown AM. 2021. Lexical color categories. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 7:605–31
Ling BY, Dain SJ. 2018. Development of color vision discrimination during childhood: differences between
Blue-Yellow, Red-Green, and achromatic thresholds. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 35(4):B35–42
Ling Y, Hurlbert A. 2011. Age-dependence of colour preference in the U.K. population. In New Directions
in Colour Studies, ed. CP Biggam, CA Hough, CJ Kay, DR Simmons, pp. 347–60. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins
Linhares JM, Nascimento SMC, Foster DH, Amano K. 2004. Chromatic diversity of natural scenes. Perception
33(1):65
LoBue V, DeLoache JS. 2011. Pretty in pink: the early development of gender-stereotyped colour preferences.
Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29(3):656–67
Ludlow AK, Heaton P, Hill E, Franklin A. 2014. Color obsessions and phobias in autism spectrum disorders:
the case of JG. Neurocase 20(3):296–306
Luo MR. 2006. Applying colour science in colour design. Opt. Laser Technol. 38(4–6):392–98
Mani N, Johnson E, McQueen JM, Huettig F. 2013. How yellow is your banana? Toddlers’ language-mediated
visual search in referent-present tasks. Dev. Psychol. 49(6):1036–44
Mashige KP. 2019. Impact of congenital color vision defect on color-related tasks among schoolchildren in
Durban, South Africa. Clin. Optom. 4130(11):97–102
Maule J, Franklin A. 2019. Color categorization in infants. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 30:163–68
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Maule J, Stanworth K, Pellicano E, Franklin A. 2017. Ensemble perception of color in autistic adults. Autism
Res. 10(5):839–51
Maule J, Stanworth K, Pellicano E, Franklin A. 2018. Color afterimages in autistic adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
48(4):1409–21
Maurer D, Werker JF. 2014. Perceptual narrowing during infancy: a comparison of language and faces. Dev.
Psychobiol. 56(2):154–78
McKyton A, Ben-Zion I, Doron R, Zohary E. 2015. The limits of shape recognition following late emergence
from blindness. Curr. Biol. 25(18):2373–78
Muris P, Meesters C, van den Berg F. 2003. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)–further
evidence for its reliability and validity in a community sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Eur.
Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 12:1–8
Nascimento SM, Albers AM, Gegenfurtner KR. 2021. Naturalness and aesthetics of colors—preference for
color compositions perceived as natural. Vis. Res. 185:98–110
Nascimento SM, Linhares JM, Montagner C, João CA, Amano K, et al. 2017. The colors of paintings and
viewers’ preferences. Vis. Res. 130:76–84
Nithiyaananthan PJ, Kaur S, Anne BT, Eliana AN, Mahadir A. 2020. Behavioural issues among primary
schoolchildren with colour vision deficiency. IIUM Med. J. Malays. 19(3):37–45
Oliva A, Schyns PG. 2000. Diagnostic colors mediate scene recognition. Cogn. Psychol. 41(2):176–210
Orenstein P. 2011. Cinderella Ate My Daughter. New York: HarperCollins
Osorio D, Vorobyev M. 1996. Colour vision as an adaptation to frugivory in primates. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol.
Sci. 263(1370):593–99
Ossenblok P, Reits D, Spekreijse H. 1992. Analysis of striate activity underlying the pattern onset EP of
children. Vis. Res. 32(10):1829–35
Palmer SE, Schloss KB. 2010. An ecological valence theory of human color preference. PNAS 107(19):8877–82
Paramei GV, Oakley B. 2014. Variation of color discrimination across the life span. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A
31(4):A375–84
Pellicano E, Burr D. 2012. When the world becomes “too real”: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 16(10):504–10
Peña M, Arias D, Dehaene-Lambertz G. 2014. Gaze following is accelerated in healthy preterm infants. Psychol.
Sci. 25(10):1884–92
Pereverzeva M, Chien SHL, Palmer J, Teller DY. 2002. Infant photometry: Are mean adult isoluminance
values a sufficient approximation to individual infant values? Vis. Res. 42(13):1639–49
Peterzell DH, Werner JS, Kaplan PS. 1995. Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: longitudinal
study of 4-, 6- and 8-month-old human infants. Vis. Res. 35(7):961–79
Pitchaimuthu K, Sourav S, Bottari D, Banerjee S, Shareef I, Kekunnaya R, Röder B. 2019. Color vision in sight
recovery individuals. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 37(6):583–90
Pitchford NJ, Mullen KT. 2002. Is the acquisition of basic-colour terms in young children constrained?
Perception 31(11):1349–70
Raskin LA, Maital S, Bornstein MH. 1983. Perceptual categorization of color: a life-span study. Psychol. Res.
45(2):135–45
Regier T, Kay P, Cook RS. 2005. Focal colors are universal after all. PNAS 102(23):8386–91
Rice M. 1980. Cognition to Language: Categories, Word Meanings, and Training. University Park, PA: Univ. Park
Press
Rogers M, Franklin A, Knoblauch K. 2018. A novel method to investigate how dimensions interact to inform
perceptual salience in infancy. Infancy 23(6):833–56
Rogers M, Witzel C, Rhodes P, Franklin A. 2020. Color constancy and color term knowledge are positively
related during early childhood. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 196:104825
Rubin LR, Lackey WL, Kennedy FA, Stephenson RB. 2009. Using color and grayscale images to teach
histology to color-deficient medical students. Anat. Sci. Educ. 2(2):84–88
Saji N, Imai M, Asano M. 2020. Acquisition of the meaning of the word orange requires understanding of the
meanings of red, pink, and purple: constructing a lexicon as a connected system. Cogn. Sci. 44(1):e12813
Samuelson LK, Horst JS. 2008. Confronting complexity: insights from the details of behavior over multiple
timescales. Dev. Sci. 11(2):209–15
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Sandhofer CM, Smith LB. 1999. Learning color words involves learning a system of mappings. Dev. Psychol.
35(3):668–79
Schloss KB, Lessard L, Racey C, Hurlbert AC. 2018a. Modeling color preference using color space metrics.
Vis. Res. 151:99–116
Schloss KB, Lessard L, Walmsley CS, Foley K. 2018b. Color inference in visual communication: the meaning
of colors in recycling. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 3:5
Schloss KB, Poggesi RM, Palmer SE. 2011. Effects of university affiliation and “school spirit” on color
preferences: Berkeley versus Stanford. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18(3):498–504
Simmons DR, Robertson AE, McKay LS, Toal E, McAleer P, Pollick FE. 2009. Vision in autism spectrum
disorders. Vis. Res. 49(22):2705–39
Simoncelli EP, Olshausen BA. 2001. Natural image statistics and neural representation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
24:1193–216
Siuda-Krzywicka K, Boros M, Bartolomeo P, Witzel C. 2019. The biological bases of colour categorisation:
from goldfish to the human brain. Cortex 118:82–106
Skelton A, Catchpole G, Abbott J, Bosten J, Franklin A. 2017. Biological origins of color categorization. PNAS
114(21):5545–50
Skelton AE, Franklin A. 2020. Infants look longer at colours that adults like when colours are highly saturated.
Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27(1):78–85
Skelton AE, Franklin A, Bosten J. 2022a. Colour vision is aligned with natural scene statistics at 4-months of
age. bioRxiv 494927. https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.06.06.494927
Skelton AE, MacInnes A, Panda K, Maule J, Bosten J, et al. 2021. Infants look longer at urban scenes than scenes of
nature, and chromatic and spatial scene statistics can account for their looking. Paper presented at the Lancaster
Conference on Infant and Early Child Development, Lancaster, UK, Aug. 25–27
Skelton AE, Maule J, Franklin A. 2022b. Infant color perception: insight into perceptual development. Child
Dev. Perspect. 16:90–95
Smithson HE. 2005. Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human colour constancy. Philos.
Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360(1458):1329–46
Sorokowski P, Sorokowska A, Witzel C. 2014. Sex differences in color preferences transcend extreme differ-
ences in culture and ecology. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21(5):1195–201
Spence C. 2015. On the psychological impact of food colour. Flavour 4:21
Stephen ID, Coetzee V, Law Smith M, Perrett DI. 2009. Skin blood perfusion and oxygenation colour affect
perceived human health. PLOS ONE 4(4):e5083
Suero MI, Pérez ÁL, Díaz F, Montanero M, Pardo PJ, et al. 2005. Does Daltonism influence young children’s
learning? Learn. Individ. Diff. 15(2):89–98
Sugita Y. 2004. Experience in early infancy is indispensable for color perception. Curr. Biol. 14(14):1267–71
Tagarelli A, Piro A, Tagarelli G, Lantieri PB, Risso D, Olivieri RL. 2004. Colour blindness in everyday life
and car driving. Acta Ophthalmol. Scand. 82:436–42
Tang T, Álvaro L, Alvarez J, Maule J, Skelton A, et al. 2022. ColourSpot, a novel gamified tablet-based test
for accurate diagnosis of color vision deficiency in young children. Behav. Res. Methods 54:1148–60
Taylor C, Clifford A, Franklin A. 2013a. Color preferences are not universal. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142(4):1015–
27
Taylor C, Franklin A. 2012. The relationship between color-object associations and color preference: further
investigation of ecological valence theory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19(2):190–97
Taylor C, Schloss K, Palmer SE, Franklin A. 2013b. Color preferences in infants and adults are different.
Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20(5):916–22
Teller DY. 1998. Spatial and temporal aspects of infant color vision. Vis. Res. 38(21):3275–82
Teller DY, Peeples DR, Sekel M. 1978. Discrimination of chromatic from white light by two-month-old hu-
man infants. Vis. Res. 18(1):41–48
Tham DSY, Sowden PT, Grandison A, Franklin A, Lee AKW, et al. 2020. A systematic investigation of con-
ceptual color associations. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149(7):1311–32
Thomas BAWM, Kaur S, Hairol MI, Ahmad M, Wee LH. 2018. Behavioural and emotional issues among
primary school pupils with congenital colour vision deficiency in the Federal Territory of Kuala Lumpur,
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Yurovsky D, Wagner K, Barner D, Frank MC. 2015. Signatures of domain-general categorization mechanisms
in color word learning. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. DC
Noelle, R Dale, AS Warlaumont, J Yoshimi, T Matlock, et al., pp. 2775–80. Austin, TX: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
Zachi EC, Costa TL, Barboni MT, Costa MF, Bonci DM, Ventura DF. 2017. Color vision losses in autism
spectrum disorders. Front. Psychol. 8:1127
Zemach I, Chang S, Teller DY. 2007. Infant color vision: prediction of infants’ spontaneous color preferences.
Vis. Res. 47(10):1368–81
Zentner MR. 2001. Preferences for colours and colour–emotion combinations in early childhood. Dev. Sci.
4(4):389–98
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
Annual Review of
Psychology
Contents
Surviving While Black: Systemic Racism and Psychological Resilience
James M. Jones p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 1
Understanding the Need for Sleep to Improve Cognition
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.
vi
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
Indexes
Errata
Contents vii
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
Related Articles
Jerome Kagan
What Do We Know About the Genetic Architecture of Psychopathology?
Evan J. Giangrande, Ramona S. Weber, and Eric Turkheimer
Training the Next Generation of Clinical Psychological Scientists: A Data-Driven
Call to Action
Dylan G. Gee, Kathryn A. DeYoung, Katie A. McLaughlin, Rachael M. Tillman,
Deanna M. Barch, Erika E. Forbes, Robert F. Krueger, Timothy J. Strauman,
Mariann R. Weierich, and Alexander J. Shackman
Measurement-Based and Data-Informed Psychological Therapy
Wolfgang Lutz, Brian Schwartz, and Jaime Delgadillo
Behavioral Interventions to Reduce Cardiovascular Risk Among People with
Severe Mental Disorder
Amanda L. Baker, Erin Forbes, Sonja Pohlman, and Kristen McCarter
Real-Time Functional MRI in the Treatment of Mental Health Disorders
Vincent Taschereau-Dumouchel, Cody A. Cushing, and Hakwan Lau
The Genetic, Environmental, and Cultural Forces Influencing Youth Antisocial
Behavior Are Tightly Intertwined
S. Alexandra Burt
The Invisibility of Power: A Cultural Ecology of Development in the
Contemporary United States
Tasneem M. Mandviwala, Jennifer Hall, and Margaret Beale Spencer
Differences/Disorders of Sex Development: Medical Conditions at the
Intersection of Sex and Gender
David E. Sandberg and Melissa Gardner
A Current Learning Theory Approach to the Etiology and Course of Anxiety
and Related Disorders
Richard E. Zinbarg, Alexander L. Williams, and Susan Mineka
viii
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
Related Articles ix
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
x Related Articles
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
The Critical Roles of Early Development, Stress, and Environment in the Course
of Psychosis
T.G. Vargas and V.A. Mittal
Use of Population-Level Administrative Data in Developmental Science
Barry J. Milne, Stephanie D’Souza, Signe Hald Andersen,
and Leah S. Richmond-Rakerd
Related Articles xi
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
Related Articles xv
PS74_FrontMatter ARjats.cls November 25, 2022 13:54
The Essential Role of the Choriocapillaris in Vision: Novel Insights from Imaging
and Molecular Biology
Kelly Mulfaul, Jonathan F. Russell, Andrew P. Voigt, Edwin M. Stone, Budd A. Tucker,
and Robert F. Mullins
Calcium Channels in Retinal Function and Disease
Brittany Williams, J. Wesley Maddox, and Amy Lee
Cellular and Molecular Determinants of Retinal Cell Fate
Eleni Petridou and Leanne Godinho
Do You See What I See? Diversity in Human Color Perception
Jenny M. Bosten
Feature Detection by Retinal Ganglion Cells
Daniel Kerschensteiner
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2023.74:87-111. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Access provided by 85.103.55.164 on 03/24/23. See copyright for approved use.