Reactive and Proactive Aggression Questionnaire
Reactive and Proactive Aggression Questionnaire
Reactive and Proactive Aggression Questionnaire
Abstract
criminal behaviours. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the structural
students (Mage = 14 years, SD = 0.81) was recruited randomly and completed the Persian
version of the RPQ, Child Behavior Checklist and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.
Although the two-factor model obtained the best fit across genders, the results did not support
the equivalence of factor loadings across groups. These findings replicated the distinction of
reactive and proactive aggression while implied different models of the RPQ based on
gender. Suggestions for future research and a more accurate assessment of these two kinds of
Invariance
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 2
Introduction
contributing to most crimes (Bobadilla et al., 2012; Cima & Raine, 2009; Justice & Justice,
1999; Vitaro et al., 2006). It is defined as behaviours aimed to harm people and/or objects
(Dodge, 1991). Besides the different forms of aggression, it is believed that aggression has
different functions (Hubbard et al., 2010; Kempes et al., 2005). Therefore, different
Coie (1987), resulting in two subtypes of aggression, proactive and reactive (Cima & Raine,
2009; Conaty, 2006). Reactive aggression is defined by impulsive behaviours that occurred in
motivated by anger (Hubbard et al., 2010; Polman et al., 2007). In contrast, proactive
aggression is the cold-blooded and goal-oriented form of aggression (Dodge, 1991; Hubbard
et al., 2010; Raine et al., 2006). This instrumental kind of aggression is best conceptualized
by the social learning theory that emphasizes the role of operant conditioning and vicarious
(Bushman & Anderson, 2001; Euler et al., 2017), several studies demonstrated differential
correlates of these two subtypes of aggression (Dodge, 1991; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Hubbard
et al., 2010; Kempes et al., 2005; Vitaro et al., 2006), suggesting the different causal pathway
of these subtypes. A literature review shows different behavioural and neurocognitive profiles
among these groups (Smeets et al., 2017). Proactive aggression is found associated with
narcissism (Seah & Ang, 2008), poor peer relationship in early childhood, blunted affect
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 3
(Raine et al., 2006), higher levels of working memory (Hecht & Latzman, 2018), and an
expectation of positive outcomes (Poulin & Boivin, 2000). Past researches indicated that
correlates of reactive aggression include information processing deficits (Dodge & Coie,
problem-solving deficiency (Atkins et al., 1993), social anxiety, lack of close friend (Raine et
al., 2006), narcissistic traits (Bobadilla et al., 2012), increased emotion dysregulation (Hecht
& Latzman, 2018), schizotypal traits, poor interpersonal relations (Seah & Ang, 2008), social
maladjustment, internalizing behavior, and peer rejection (Card & Little, 2006).
However, there are some inconsistent findings of correlated variables with these two
subtypes of aggression. For instance, while in some studies, proactive aggression was
associated with delinquency (Atkins et al., 1993; Brendgen et al., 2001; Raine et al., 2006;
Vitaro et al., 1998), in some others, it was more related to reactive aggression (Card & Little,
2006).
The conflicting outcomes might be due to participants' different sex or ages (Bobadilla
et al., 2012; Connor et al., 2003; Kempes et al., 2005). Although there are many studies about
the development of aggression across the lifespan and comparison of genders, very few
accounts for age and gender differences specifically for these subtypes of aggression. About
gender comparison, one study showed boys were higher in reactive aggression but no
difference in proactive aggression (Kempes et al., 2006; Lickley & Sebastian, 2018).
reactive and proactive aggression can still enhance our understanding of their separate
precursors and outcomes (Dodge, 1991). This knowledge would contribute to the
development of preventive and therapeutic interventions (Hubbard et al., 2010; Merk et al.,
2005; Pechorro et al., 2015). As such, reliable and valid measures which could distinguish
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 4
between these two subtypes of aggression are vital. Dodge and Coie (1987) tried to
age. Their six-item rating scale can be completed by teachers, parents, themselves(Kaat et al.,
2015), and correctional facility staff (Hubbard et al., 2010). However, self-report measures
are privileged over others by accurately recognizing the inherent motivation of aggression,
the central feature of this distinction (Raine et al., 2006). The reactive-Proactive Aggression
Questionnaire (RPQ) is a brief self-report measure developed by Raine et al. (2006). The
psychometric properties of this measure have been investigated by several studies across
different cultures, supporting its reliability and validity. (Cenkseven-Önder et al., 2016;
Fossati et al., 2009; Fung et al., 2009; Pechorro et al., 2015; Seah & Ang, 2008). The
Cronbach's alpha values in the initial validation of the RPQ among schoolboys aged 7 and 16
years old were 0.84 for the reactive scale, 0.86 for the proactive scale, and 0.90 for the total
score (Raine et al., 2006). The Portuguese version of the RPQ showed good psychometric
(Pechorro et al., 2018). Cima et al. (2013) have identified Cronbach's alpha of the Dutch
version as 0.83 for the reactive scale and 0.87 for the proactive scale, supporting the RPQ as a
reliable and valid instrument to identify these subtypes of aggression. Its criterion validity
was also established due to the significantly lower scores of non-offenders compared to
offenders samples (Cima et al., 2013). The study conducted on Turkish children and
adolescents indicated the item-total correlation ranging from 0.40 to 0.74 and an Alpha
coefficient above 0.81 for both subscales and the total score, which supports the high internal
There are studies in favour of the original two-factor model across various cultures.
For instance, Fung et al. (2009) have examined the generalizability of the RPQ in an East
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 5
Asian population. Despite the significant correlation between reactive and proactive
aggression (r> 0.54), the original two-factor model was superior to the one-factor model, and
the item- loadings were over 0.45. A better fit of the two-factor structure was replicated in a
study among Portuguese youth (Pechorro et al., 2018). However, the measurement invariance
across genders was confirmed after excluding item 21. Although a two-factor model of the
Spanish version of the RPQ best fit the data, it was variant across genders and ages, with the
best fit in males under 25 years of age (Toro et al., 2020). The original two-factor model of
the RPQ also obtained the best fit among Dutch (Cima et al., 2013) and Turkish samples
Despite these findings supporting the two-factor structure of the RPQ, some studies
have failed to confirm the original model. Using a person-based approach, Brugman et al.
(2017) revealed a three-factor structure based on the severity of aggression among forensic
and non-forensic samples. Moreover, the four-factor structure has been explored, combining
two subtypes of aggression and various contexts for reactive aggression (playing games and a
defensive form) based on a variable-based approach. However, the four-factor solution was
considered less solid since items were more uniquely related to each subscale of aggression in
the two-factor model of the RPQ. In the same way, Smeets et al. (2017) have extracted three
factors in the clinical sample, including reactive aggression due to external provocation,
reactive aggression due to internal frustration, and proactive aggression. Pang et al. (2013)
also identified three distinct clusters (including high reactive and low proactive aggression,
low on both subscales, and high on both subscales) among adolescents in Singapore,
Therefore, the contradictory results of the prior researches, together with the lack of
study for convergent and criterion validity of the RPQ among the Iranian population, suggest
the need for further research on the reliability of the RPQ to distinguish reactive from
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 6
proactive aggression. Hence, this research aims to investigate the replication of the original
model of the RPQ (Raine et al., 2006) and its psychometric properties among Iranian
adolescents across genders. Thus, we want to examine to what extent these two forms of
these two kinds of aggression. Therefore, the first hypothesis was that the original two-factor
model of the RPQ would also achieve adequate fit while the loadings of items would have a
similar pattern across genders. Secondly, the RPQ is predicted to show good internal
consistency as measured by the Omega and Alpha coefficient, mean inter-item correlation
(MIIC), and corrected item-total correlation in both groups. Thirdly, we hypothesized that
both RPQ subscales would relate positively to criterion-related variables, including rule-
breaking behaviour, aggressive behaviour, and conduct problems, in a similar way in both
(Bobadilla et al., 2012), we examine whether detained individuals can be distinguished based
on their scores in the proactive aggression subscale. It was predicted incarcerated individuals
Method
Participants
A total of 450 students participated in this study (Mage =14 years; S.D. =.81) consist of
217 boys (Mage= 14.29 years; SD= .80) and 233 girls (Mage= 13.89 years; SD= .78). There
were 133 students from grade 7, 146 students from grade 8, and 171 students from grade 9.
The participants were recruited via a stratified sampling method from 6 secondary schools in
A subsample of 145 students including 69 girls (Mage= 13.6 years; SD= .93) and 76
boys (Mage = 13.8 years; SD= 1.05) were used to measure convergent validity. This sample
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 7
comprised of the minimum participants needed (N = 67) to achieve the desired statistical
power (i.e., 0.80) recommended by Cohen (1988) with the medium effect size (i.e., r = 0.30).
The RPQ was administered in a forensic context to establish criterion validity. Since
there was no girl in imprisonment, all the incarcerated boys (N=31) aged 15 to 19 (Mage =
17.4 years; SD= 1.01) in the juvenile detention centre of Yazd were recruited. This sample
size was enough to achieve an acceptable level of statistical power (Cohen, 1988) with a
Measure
questionnaire that is suitable for a wide age range and evaluates two functions of physically
and verbally aggressive behaviours (Raine et al., 2006). There are 11 items in the proactive
and 12 items in the reactive subscale. Each item is rated on a three-point scale scored with 0
The emotional and behavioural problems were assessed by the Youth Self-Report
(YSR) version of the child behaviour checklist for children aged 6-18 (Achenbach &
Rescorla, 2014). This report measure contains 113 items rated on a three-point scale (0 =
Absent, 1 = Occurs sometimes, 2 = Occurs often). It includes subscales that assess anxiety,
depression, somatic complaints, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-
Aggressive Behaviour make externalizing problems score. There is excellent support for the
psychometric properties of CBCL among the Iranian population and different countries
(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2014). In this study, two subscales of aggressive and rule-breaking
A self-report version of the SDQ (Goodman et al., 1998) measured emotional and
children and adolescents across five subscales: Conduct Problems, Inattention- Hyperactivity,
Emotional Symptoms, Peer Problems, and Prosocial Behaviour. Items are rated on a three-
point Likert-type scale with 'not true' (scored 0), 'somewhat true' (achieved 1), and 'certainly
true (scored 2). The Persian version of SDQ demonstrated high validity and reliability in
several studies (Tehranidoust et al., 2007). In this study, only the conduct problem subscale
Procedure
Permission is asked from the developer of the original RPQ before the initial
questionnaire was translated and back-translated into Persian. Face and content validity are
assessed by two experts, causing a minor change in the phraseology. After the approval of the
study in the ethical board of the department of education, three schools were chosen by
chance from each educational region. Randomly selected participants completed the
questionnaire after their consent got obtained. The questionnaire was accomplished in a
group following that the written instruction was read to assure understanding. All subjects
were informed that they could withdraw at any time without negative consequences for them.
They were encouraged to ask any questions about understanding the content.
Permission was obtained from the state prisons organization to attend the juvenile
detention centre of Yazd. The same procedures were done for incarcerated boys, except that
questionnaire was read for five illiterate participants. Data were analysed using IBM SPSS
Statistical analyses
examine the factor structure of the RPQ. We used Bollen- Stine bootstrapping due to the non-
samples in bootstrapping analysis (Hair Jr et al., 2014), the bias-corrected confidence interval
at the 95% confidence level was computed based on 5000 bootstrap samples. Model fit of
different factor structures suggested for this scale were assessed. Therefore, the original two-
factor model has been compared with the one-factor, second-order, and three-factor models
(Smeets et al., 2017). All items are loaded on one latent factor named general aggression in
the one-factor model, while the factor variance was constrained at 1.00. Different Goodness
of fit indices were used for the investigation of model fit, including Comparative Fit Index
(CFI), Tucker–Lewis fit index (TLI), the Goodness of its index (GFI), and the Root Mean
freedom (χ2/df) is also considered in a way that values less than 2 and 5 are held to be good
and acceptable, respectively (Schumacker & Lomax, 2004). The acceptable cutoff value for
CFI is 0.90, indicating adequate fit (Hoyle, 1995). Hu and Bentler (1999) recommended that
CFI >.95, TLI ≥.95 and RMSEA <.06 indicate a good-fitting model. Akaike information
criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criterion (BIC) and expected cross-validation index
(ECVI) were used for non-nested models' comparison in the way that the lowest value
indicates better fit (Brown, 2015; Kline, 2015). Since factor loadings are considered
meaningful when they are above .30 (Pechorro et al., 2018), items with standardized loading
lower than that value were removed. No modification indexes (i.e., estimates resulting from
validity assessment of constrained parameters in the model) were used to improve the model
fit. Then measurement invariance of the primary model was assessed, using multigroup CFA
(Milfont & Fischer, 2010). This approach addresses whether there are differences in the
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 10
measurement of latent constructs across various groups. At first, the configural model is
defined in which all parameters were allowed to be freely estimated across groups. This
model is the baseline for comparison and estimates the equivalence of the overall factor
structure in groups. At the next step, metric invariance would be assessed to investigate
whether the same observations indicate the same latent constructs across different groups.
Therefore, while item intercepts are still allowed to be freely estimated, the factor loadings
are constrained to be equivalent across groups. At the final step, scalar invariance examines
the similarity of item intercepts across groups. Support for scalar invariance allows us for
comparison of factor means between groups(Lee, 2018). In each step, the baseline model is
compared with the constrained model. The difference between these nested models is
assessed based on the significance of the chi-square difference test as well as ΔCFI and the
ΔRMSEA (Tracey & Xu, 2017). Therefore, a significant deterioration in the fitness of the
more constrained model alongside ΔCFI> 0.01 and ΔRMSEA> 0.015 are considered
In the last step, the psychometric properties of the two-factor model were investigated
across genders. Internal consistency was assessed using Omega and Alpha coefficients, mean
inter-item correlation (MIIC) and corrected item-total correlations (CITC). Omega and Alpha
values ≥ .70, the minimum mean inter-item correlations within the range of .15 to .20 and
corrected item-total correlations above .20 were considered adequate internal consistency
(Clark & Watson, 2016; Hair, 2009; Pechorro et al., 2018). Convergent validity was
examined using correlation analysis between the residual scores of reactive and proactive
aggression and conduct problem, aggressive and rule-breaking behaviours. Criterion validity
was examined by comparing the reactive and proactive scores between individuals with and
without delinquency.
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 11
Results
As can be seen in Table 1, all models showed better fit to data compared to the null
model. However, the two-factor model (χ2 /df = 1.92; TLI = .86; CFI = .88; RMSEA = .04)
fit the data better than the one-factor model (χ2 /df = 2.08; TLI = .82; CFI = .84; RMSEA =
.05), second-factor model (χ2 /df = 2.86; TLI = .70; CFI = .72; RMSEA = .06), and three-
factor model (χ2 /df = 1.87; TLI = .86; CFI = .87; RMSEA = .05), considering the lowest
amount of AIC, BIC and ECVI. All item loadings exceed the recommended value of 0.30
In the next stage, two groups were defined based on gender to evaluate the
measurement invariance of the RPQ and the impact of gender on the model fit. The result of
multiple group CFA presented in Table 2 indicated that loadings of items have a different
pattern across genders (Tracey & Xu, 2017). The comparison of the baseline model with the
metric model revealed non-equality of factor loading across genders. As shown in Table 2,
although ΔRMSEA was not more than 0.015, ΔCFI exceeded 0.01 showing weak invariance
was not established (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002). Moreover, the chi-square difference test
(Δχ2=161.59, df= 23, P< .05) showed a significantly poorer fit for the model with constrained
factor loadings across groups. Further assessment was not justified because the least strict
model was not confirmed to be equal. (Lee, 2018; Putnick & Bornstein, 2016).
Factor loadings of items were examined across genders to explore the exact
dissimilarity of model form between groups. The result indicated different items with factor
loading less than .30 across genders (items 4, 15, 18, 21 among girls and 13 and 15 among
boys). The result of confirmatory factor analysis revealed a better fit of the two-factor
structure of the RPQ after removing items with factor loadings less than the threshold
compared to the various models proposed for the RPQ in both groups (Table 3). Therefore,
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 12
the two-factor intercorrelated structures were considered the best representation of the RPQ,
considering reasonable fit and numerous researches supporting evidence for this model. The
standardized item loadings for the two-factor model for each group and the total sample are
presented in Table 4.
Internal consistency
The omega coefficient for RPQ total score, reactive and proactive subscales among
girls was almost .84, .78 and .68, respectively. In the other group, omega coefficient was as
follow: total score = .85, reactive subscale= .74 and proactive subscale= .77. The result of
internal consistency for the RPQ subscales, estimated by Cronbach's alpha, can be considered
acceptable except for the proactive subscale among girls (Table 5).
CITC for reactive aggression ranged .33 to .45 for boys (M= 7.86, SD= 3.50) and .33
to .55 for girls (M= 8.76, SD= 3.94). For proactive aggression, CITC ranged .30 to .44 for
girls (M= 2.03, SD= 2.07) and .32 to .56 for boys (M= 3.47, SD= 3.24). Reactive and
proactive aggression scores were significantly correlated among girls (r= .61, p <001) and
Convergent validity
In a subsample of 145 students (76 males and 69 females), convergent validity was
assessed separately for each gender, evaluating the relationship between one subscale of SDQ
(conduct problem) and two subscales of CBCL (aggressive and rule-breaking behaviours)
and the two RPQ subscales. Once more, we rerun the correlation analysis with residuals of
reactive and proactive aggression scores, considering the substantial correlation between the
with rule-breaking (r= .26, p< .05) and aggressive behaviours subscales of CBCL (r= .36, p<
.01). Aggressive behaviours subscales of CBCL also significantly correlated with the residual
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 13
of reactive aggression scores among girls (r= .30, p< .05). While purely proactive aggression
did not show any significant correlation among boys, it was significantly associated with
rule-breaking (r= 28, p< .05) and aggressive behaviours scores (r= 24, p< .05) in the other
group. Besides, the result revealed that the raw score of both factors and the RPQ total score
were significantly correlated with other variables in a positive direction (Table 6). A
marginally significant correlation between residual reactive aggression and conduct problem
scores (r= 22, p= .06) was seen among girls. In both groups, the strongest correlations were
between the raw score of reactive aggression and aggressive behaviours (Male: r= .64,
criterion validity
students aged 15 with a sample of incarcerated boys (N=31). As shown in table 7, there was
(M=8.93, SD=3.66) and non-incarcerated sample (M=7.86, SD=3.50); t (79) = 1.32, p= .19).
But result demonstrated a significant effect for grouping in proactive aggression scores, t (79)
= 2.32, p= 02., showing incarcerated boys obtained higher scores (M=4.49, SD=.80) than the
other group (M=3.12, SD=.44). Hedges' g was calculated (g = 0.94) to assess the effect size
due to the different sample sizes of the compared groups (Lakens, 2013).
Discussion
This study investigated the factor structure and psychometric properties of reactive
and proactive aggression questionnaires across genders among Iranian adolescents. Overall,
the result confirmed the cross-cultural stability of two distinct types of aggression,
representing a better fit of the two-factor intercorrelated model than the other proposed
structures.
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 14
This result is consistent with research confirming this model's generalizability among
various cultures (Cima et al., 2013; Fung et al., 2009; Goodman et al., 1998; Raine et al.,
2006). However, there are studies indicating different factor structures (Brugman et al., 2017;
Colins, 2016; Pang et al., 2013; Smeets et al., 2017). This discrepancy might be justified by
Besides, findings could be influenced by the different samples among these studies.
Although the CFA revealed that the two-factor model had a better fit than different
suggested models, the fit indices did not reach the acceptable cut off value. However, similar
results in a study by Toro et al. (2020) showed better model fit indicators after executing
residual covariances. In this study, similarly, the modification indexes suggested covariances
of the measurement error among the items of the same factor; however, no post-hoc alteration
The model's poor fit can also be rooted in the wording and ambiguity of phrases so
that some items can be interpreted as both reactive and proactive. This idea is also confirmed
by various studies reporting the cross-loading of several items of RPQ (Brugman et al., 2017;
Fossati et al., 2009). The result of an explorative factor analysis conducted among adults,
which revealed that only a subset of items (1, 5, 7, 11, and 14 for reactive and items 2, 6, 9,
10, 12, and 20 for proactive aggression) could differentiate adequately between these two
subscales (Lobbestael et al., 2013). Moreover, this scale examines aggressive behaviours
coupled with aggressive feelings. For example, in the reactive subscale, items 5,11,13,14 and
somehow 22 tended to examine the feeling of aggression more. It is also suggested that these
two forms of aggression would be better distinguished with behavioural observations and the
questionnaires disentangling form and function (Polman et al., 2007) or based on the target of
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 15
aggression (whether person or object). Future studies should consider the association between
such measures and the RPQ factor structure. Furthermore, this result can lend support for the
dimensional compared to the categorical approach due to the fuzzy distinction between these
two types of aggression (Dodge, 1991) and their considerable corelation. Besides, proactive
aggression could be displayed with a delay from prior provocation (Merk et al., 2005),
In this paper, the measurement invariance of the RPQ was examined across genders.
In line with studies indicating variants of the RPQ across genders among different
populations (Rodríguez et al., 2009; Toro et al., 2020), the result of CFA revealed two
distinctive patterns of item loading across genders. Hence, the comparison of aggression
between Iranian boys and girls based on this measure is not justified. Moreover, the result did
not support replicating the factor structure presented by Raine et al. (2006) across genders.
There was no making obscene phone calls, carrying weapons, and taking things from other
students as proactive aggression in girls. But, boys did not show madness and anger after
losing in games as reactive aggression. In addition, it seems the trait of proactive aggression
among Iranian adolescents did not include using force to obtain money, considering poor
factor loading of item 15 in both groups.These findings are comparable with studies
indicating measurement invariance of the RPQ after modification of the model. For example,
a study by Baker et al. (2008) reported the invariance of the RPQ across genders after
implementing certain constraints (e.g., letting for correlation of error variance). Structural
invariance of the RPQ was also confirmed among Portuguese youth after excluding item 21
The impact of gender on the expression of aggression alongside cultural issues should
be considered to interpret these findings. Additionally, because few studies investigated the
measurement invariance of the RPQ across genders, it is suggested to ascertain the variant of
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 16
this scale by further researches. Moreover, keeping in mind that there were different ages in
each group in this research, further study on the invariance assessment of this scale would
Another purpose of this study was to examine the reliability and validity of the RPQ
across genders. Omega and Alpha coefficients indicated sufficient internal consistency of the
RPQ (Deng & Chan, 2017; Nunnally, 1994), except for the proactive subscale in females.
Because the inter-relatedness of the items in each subscale was not poor, heterogeneity in this
subscale is not the case (Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). However, further investigation is needed
to examine whether a low omega coefficient might result from the reduction in the items of
the proactive subscale (Tavakol & Dennick, 2011) or the inability of this questionnaire to
The total score of the RPQ, like previous studies, was correlated to subscales of
CBCL and SDQ in both groups, supporting convergent validity of the RPQ (Bartels et al.,
2018). In line with previous studies (Fossati et al., 2009; Raine et al., 2006), residual scores
were used to take into account the variance shared with the other aggressive dimension due to
a moderate correlation between both subscales. When the residual scores were used, rule-
breaking behaviours were significantly associated with purely proactive aggression among
girls. This result is consistent with the notion that this kind of aggression is more instrumental
and planned (Fossati et al., 2009). In contrast, the rule-breaking score was correlated with a
purely reactive score among boys, suggesting that boys who demonstrate aggression followed
correlation with purely reactive aggression in both groups, suggesting reactive aggression is a
predominant form of aggression (Thomson & Centifanti, 2018). However, the association
between aggressive behaviours and purely proactive aggression was significant only among
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 17
girls, confirming distinctive patterns of association across genders (Connor et al., 2003). This
result implied that girls with high aggressive behaviours display both profiles of aggression
which is consistent with researches indicating the mixture of both kinds of aggression among
the more aggressive individuals (Colins, 2016; Euler et al., 2017; Fossati et al., 2009; Merk et
al., 2005).
Since residual proactive aggression did not show any significant correlation among
boys, further investigation is needed to figure out the reason behind these results. There might
be some issues with the male's proactive subscale. On the other hand, using the residual score
to control for the effect of the other dimension may contribute to these results due to
increasing error variance (Raine et al., 2006). Besides, keeping in mind that these correlates
are in common between reactive and proactive aggression, subsequent studies should notice
more specific variables to support more evidence that each type of aggression comes in
validity of the RPQ scale was supported while the correlates of the RPQ subscales differed
partly across genders. Therefore, this paper provides more evidence for reactive and
Data on this study support the studies suggesting that proactive aggression is related
to delinquency and antisocial behaviours (Brendgen et al., 2001; Deng & Chan, 2017), as
incarcerated boys showed a significantly higher level of proactive aggression but not reactive
form. This result contrasts with the study showing the association between delinquency and
the severity of aggression but not the types (Stickle et al., 2012). However, this result lends
support for the association between severe aggression, psychopathic and callous-unemotional
traits and proactive but not reactive aggression (Brugman et al., 2017; Cima & Raine, 2009;
Cima et al., 2013). This result signifies differential trajectories for these two kinds of
aggression (Poulin & Boivin, 2000; Raine et al., 2006). However, the non-significance result
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 18
consistent with the fact that a moderate level of reactive aggression is normative (Brugman et
al., 2017; Fossati et al., 2009; Raine et al., 2006), suggesting reactive aggression as a
Furthermore, this study can provide evidence for the ability of this scale to screen
proactive aggressive population (Merk et al., 2005; Smeets et al., 2017) or the ability of the
RPQ in recognizing such group (Brugman et al., 2017). However, this result contrasts with
research reporting significantly higher reactive and proactive aggression scores in violent
offenders than non-offender (Cima et al., 2013). This discrepancy could be explained through
disparate types of crimes in our study, including violent and non-violent together.
The current study confronts some limitations. This study is the first to examine the
psychometric properties of the RPQ while comparing the RPQ factor structure across two
genders in Iranian populations. Since there is strong evidence for validation of the theoretical
factor structure of the RPQ across different cultures, the result of the current study should be
considered with care. More study is needed to examine whether this result is affected by the
data's artefact or actual cultural context differences. Therefore, the results in this regard
should be interpreted cautiously. This cross-sectional study could not present the differential
pathways of these two types of aggression as well as the temporal stability of the RPQ. A
future longitudinal study is encouraged to measure the invariance of factor structure across
time. Since this study used one source of information for all assessments, future examination
for the evaluation of criterion validity of the RPQ should consider observational assessment
and multiple informants like friends, parents and teachers, as well as computational
instruments. In this study, the psychometric properties of the RPQ were confirmed among
non-clinical adolescents. We suggest replicating this study with a higher risk sample to
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 19
evaluate the usefulness of this brief and easy answer questionnaire for the clinical population,
considering the co-occurrence of these two types of aggression among high aggressive
individuals (Merk et al., 2005). Therefore, regarding the moderate to a strong correlation
between these subtypes of aggression (Fossati et al., 2009; Hubbard et al., 2010; Poulin &
Boivin, 2000; Raine et al., 2006) and concerns about the usefulness of this distinction,
repetition of this study among cases with the extreme score is suggested.
Since there was not any female incarcerated, the generalizability of the result in this
regard is limited. Regarding a severity model of reactive and proactive aggression against a
typology model (Stickle et al., 2012), future researches should take into account the severity
of aggression and psychiatric diagnosis in the relationship between types of aggression and
delinquency in a larger sample including both genders (Cima et al., 2013). Such studies
would provide the possibility to compare the exclusive role of reactive and proactive
Altogether, this study adds to the knowledge of two distinct forms of aggression
among Iranian adolescents. Data in this study did not support the invariance of the original
factor structure of the RPQ across genders, implying boys and girls express reactive and
proactive aggression differently. However, the two-factor structure represented the data as a
reasonable well-fitting model with adequate reliability and validity across genders. This study
signifies future investigation of the distinctive correlation between these two forms of
aggression and emotional and cognitive deficits across both genders and ages. In this regard,
configure better the distinctive correlates of reactive and proactive aggression. Such studies
would confirm unique etiological and consequential pathways and shed light on producing
more efficient and specific intervention and prevention strategies through attention to specific
underlying mechanisms.
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 20
References
Atkins, M. S., Stoff, D. M., Osborne, M. L., & Brown, K. (1993). Distinguishing
instrumental and hostile aggression: does it make a difference? Journal of abnormal
child psychology, 21(4), 355-365.
Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Liu, J., & Jacobson, K. C. (2008). Differential genetic and
environmental influences on reactive and proactive aggression in children. Journal of
abnormal child psychology, 36(8), 1265-1278.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. prentice-hall.
Bartels, M., Hendriks, A., Mauri, M., Krapohl, E., Whipp, A., Bolhuis, K., Conde, L. C.,
Luningham, J., Ip, H. F., & Hagenbeek, F. (2018). Childhood aggression and the co-
occurrence of behavioural and emotional problems: results across ages 3–16 years
from multiple raters in six cohorts in the EU-ACTION project. European child &
adolescent psychiatry, 27(9), 1105-1121.
Bobadilla, L., Wampler, M., & Taylor, J. (2012). Proactive and reactive aggression are
associated with different physiological and personality profiles. Journal of Social and
clinical psychology, 31(5), 458-487.
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Lavoie, F. (2001). Reactive and proactive
aggression: Predictions to physical violence in different contexts and moderating
effects of parental monitoring and caregiving behavior. Journal of abnormal child
psychology, 29(4), 293-304.
Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. Guilford
publications.
Brugman, S., Cornet, L. J., Smeijers, D., Smeets, K., Oostermeijer, S., Buitelaar, J. K.,
Verkes, R. J., Lobbestael, J., de Kogel, C. H., & Jansen, L. M. (2017). Examining the
reactive proactive questionnaire in adults in forensic and non‐forensic settings: A
variable‐and person‐based approach. Aggressive Behavior, 43(2), 155-162.
Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on hostile versus
instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychological review, 108(1), 273.
Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and
adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment.
International journal of behavioral development, 30(5), 466-480.
Cenkseven-Önder, F., Avci, R., & Çolakkadioglu, O. (2016). Validity and Reliability of the
Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire in Turkish Adolescents. Educational
Research and Reviews, 11(20), 1931-1943.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing
measurement invariance. Structural equation modeling, 9(2), 233-255.
Cima, M., & Raine, A. (2009). Distinct characteristics of psychopathy relate to different
subtypes of aggression. Personality and individual differences, 47(8), 835-840.
Cima, M., Raine, A., Meesters, C., & Popma, A. (2013). Validation of the Dutch Reactive
Proactive Questionnaire (RPQ): Differential correlates of reactive and proactive
aggression from childhood to adulthood. Aggressive Behavior, 39(2), 99-113.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (2016). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale
development.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences [Internet]. Statistical
Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 567.
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 21
Kempes, M., Matthys, W., De Vries, H., & Van Engeland, H. (2005). Reactive and proactive
aggression in children A review of theory, findings and the relevance for child and
adolescent psychiatry. European child & adolescent psychiatry, 14(1), 11-19.
Kempes, M., Matthys, W., Maassen, G., van Goozen, S., & van Engeland, H. (2006). A
parent questionnaire for distinguishing between reactive and proactive aggression in
children. European child & adolescent psychiatry, 15(1), 38-45.
Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford
publications.
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a
practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 863.
Lee, S. T. (2018). Testing for Measurement Invariance: Does your measure mean the same
thing for different participants? APS Observer, 31(8).
Lickley, R. A., & Sebastian, C. L. (2018). The neural basis of reactive aggression and its
development in adolescence. Psychology, Crime & Law, 24(3), 313-333.
Lobbestael, J., Cima, M., & Arntz, A. (2013). The relationship between adult reactive and
proactive aggression, hostile interpretation bias, and antisocial personality disorder.
Journal of personality disorders, 27(1), 53-66.
Merk, W., Orobio de Castro, B., Koops, W., & Matthys, W. (2005). The distinction between
reactive and proactive aggression: Utility for theory, diagnosis and treatment?
European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2(2), 197-220.
Milfont, T. L., & Fischer, R. (2010). Testing measurement invariance across groups:
Applications in cross-cultural research. International Journal of psychological
research, 3(1), 111-130.
Nunnally, J. C. (1994). Psychometric theory 3E. Tata McGraw-hill education.
Pang, J. S., Ang, R. P., Kom, D. M., Tan, S. H., & Chiang, A. Q. (2013). Patterns of Reactive
and Proactive Aggression in Young Adolescents in S ingapore. Social Development,
22(4), 794-812.
Pechorro, P., Ayala-Nunes, L., Kahn, R., & Nunes, C. (2018). The Reactive–Proactive
Aggression Questionnaire: Measurement invariance and reliability among a school
sample of Portuguese youths. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 49(4), 523-
533.
Pechorro, P., Ray, J. V., Salas-Wright, C. P., Maroco, J., & Goncalves, R. A. (2015).
Adaptation of the Basic Empathy Scale among a Portuguese sample of incarcerated
juvenile offenders. Psychology, Crime & Law, 21(7), 699-714.
Polman, H., de Castro, B. O., Koops, W., van Boxtel, H. W., & Merk, W. W. (2007). A meta-
analysis of the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children and
adolescents. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 35(4), 522-535.
Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: evidence of a two-factor
model. Psychological assessment, 12(2), 115.
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and
reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research.
Developmental review, 41, 71-90.
Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke‐Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., Stouthamer‐
Loeber, M., & Liu, J. (2006). The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire:
Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys.
Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on
Aggression, 32(2), 159-171.
Rodríguez, J. M. A., Fernández, M. E. P., & Ramírez, J. M. (2009). Cuestionario de agresión
reactiva y proactiva: un instrumento de medida de la agresión en adolescentes. Revista
de psicopatología y psicología clínica, 14(1), 37-49.
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 23
Acknowledgements
Ethics approval for this project was granted by the Psychology department of Shahid
Beheshti University Ethics Committee. All participants provided informed consent for their
participation.
Datasets that support the findings of this study can be freely and openly available in
Table 1
One- Factor Model 480.41 230 1.27 761.43 572.41 .84 .91 .82 .05
Second- Order Model 663.84 232 1.67 932.64 751.84 .72 .89 .70 .06
Three- Factor Model 425.88 227 1.16 725.23 523.88 .87 .92 .86 .05
Two- Factor Model 399.72 208 1.09 674.63 489.72 .88 .93 .86 .04
Table 2
The Goodness of Fit Indices for the Assessment of Cross-Gender Invariance of the RPQ
Metric 915.19 481 2.56 1149.19 .75 .73 .045 .08 161.59**
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 26
Table 3
One- Factor Model 381.58 230 2.19 473.58 629.06 .82 .86 .81 .05
Male
Second- Order Model 492.61 232 2.68 580.61 729.34 .70 .85 .67 .07
Three- Factor Model 367.27 227 2.15 465.27 630.88 .84 .87 .82 .05
Two- Factor Model 283.09 188 1.70 369.09 514.42 .88 .90 .86 .04
One- Factor Model 398.44 230 2.11 490.44 649.18 .79 .87 .77 .05
Female
Second- Order Model 496.71 232 2.52 584.71 736.56 .68 .84 .65 .07
Three- Factor Model 378.15 227 2.05 476.25 645.25 .81 .87 .79 .05
Two- Factor Model 300.40 151 1.79 416.40 512.99 .82 .88 .80 .06
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 27
Table 1
Table 5
Female Male
Total aggression 0.83 .83 0.21 0.30- 0.54 0.85 .85 0.21 0.30- 0.53
Psychometric Properties of the RPQ 30
Table 6
Variable Purely reactive Purely proactive Raw Reactive Raw Proactive Total Score
Aggressive
Female .30* .24* .50** .47** .55***
behavior
Aggressive
Male .36** .12 .64** .54** .64**
behavior
Table 7
Independent Samples T-Test for Comparison of the Incarcerated Boys and Non-
t df p Lower Upper