Cambridge International As & A Level Thinking Skills Sample
Cambridge International As & A Level Thinking Skills Sample
Cambridge International As & A Level Thinking Skills Sample
Thinking Skills
Coursebook
Third edition
Andrew Roberts, Mark Dawes,
Ruth Matthews, Geoff Thwaites
and John Butterworth
Thinking Skills
Coursebook
Third edition
University Printing House, Cambridge CB2 8BS, United Kingdom
One Liberty Plaza, 20th Floor, New York, NY 10006, USA
477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia
314–321, 3rd Floor, Plot 3, Splendor Forum, Jasola District Centre, New Delhi – 110025, India
79 Anson Road, #06–04/06, Singapore 079906
Cambridge University Press is part of the University of Cambridge.
It furthers the University’s mission by disseminating knowledge in the pursuit of
education, learning and research at the highest international levels of excellence.
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781108441049 (Paperback)
20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Part 1
Problem solving
6
Chapter 1
What do we mean by a ‘problem’?
Learning objectives
This chapter introduces the idea of a problem and considers some simple examples
to help you identify the key information that is required to solve problems and apply
some simple problem-solving strategies.
Chapter 1: What do we mean by a ‘problem’?
ACTIVITY 1.1
Imagine you are going to book tickets for a concert. List the pieces of information you
need and the processes that you need to go through in order to book the tickets and get
to the concert. In what order should you do them? First list the main things, then try to
break each down into smaller parts.
Imagine that you have to work out the best way to fit a number of rectangular packages
into a box. Which of the four categories of skill identified above would be relevant to
solving the problem?
Commentary
This problem involves mainly categories 1 and 2, but could involve some of category 4
as well:
1 Understand the problem that needs to be solved:
The dimensions of the box and the packages need to be known. Once these have
been worked out you also need to think about the different orientations in which the
packages could be placed into the box. You might make an initial assumption about
the best orientation, which may turn out later to be wrong.
2 Process the information that is available:
Once you know the relevant dimensions, some appropriate calculations should
enable you to work out how many boxes can be fitted in for each orientation.
Chapter 1: What do we mean by a ‘problem’?
Luke has a meeting, in a town that is 50 miles away, at 3 p.m. tomorrow. He is planning
to travel by train from the town where he lives to the town where the meeting is, walking
to and from the station at both ends. 9
List the pieces of information Luke needs in order to decide what time he must leave
home.
Commentary
Let us start by thinking of everything that Luke does from leaving his house to arriving
at the meeting:
1 He leaves his house.
2 He walks to the station.
3 He buys a train ticket.
4 He goes to the platform.
5 He boards the train when it arrives.
6 He sits on the train until it reaches its destination.
7 He leaves the train.
8 He walks to where his meeting is to be held.
We can construct the pieces of information that he needs from this list. They are:
1 The time taken to walk from his house to the station.
2 The time needed to buy a ticket (including the time waiting in a queue).
3 The time to walk to the platform.
4 The train timetable.
5 The time taken to walk from the station to where the meeting is being held.
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
Commentary
We could choose a start time based on when we think Luke should leave home and
then work out what time Luke would arrive, but a better strategy would be to work out
when Luke should leave by working backwards from the meeting start time.
The sections of the journey can be considered one at a time. The start time of one
activity is always the time by which the previous activity must have been completed by.
Of course, you could do the whole thing by guesswork, but you might get it all wrong
and, more to the point, you cannot be confident that you will have got it right.
Julia has been staying in a hotel on a business trip. When she checks out, the hotel’s
computer isn’t working, so the receptionist makes a bill by hand from the receipts, totalling 11
$471. Julia thinks she has been overcharged, so she checks the itemised bill carefully.
Room: 4 nights at $76.00 per night
Breakfast: 4 at $10.00 each
Dinner: 3 at $18.00 each
Telephone: 10 units at $1.70 per unit
Bar: Juices and soft drinks totalling $23.00
Laundry: 3 blouses at $5.00 each
It appears that the receptionist miscounted one of the items when adding up the total.
Which item has Julia been charged too much for?
List the pieces of information Luke needs in order to decide what time he must leave home.
Commentary
Although this example is simple, it illustrates many of the methods used in solving
problems:
• Look at the data provided. Identify which pieces are relevant and which are
irrelevant. In this case all of the information is relevant to the solution as we will
need to know how much Julia should have been charged.
• Make an intermediate calculation before you can reach the answer. In this case
it was necessary to calculate the value that the bill should have been in order to
identify where Julia had been overcharged. The sum of the charges on the itemised
bill is $453, so Julia has been overcharged by $18.
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
• Search the data for a piece of information that helps to solve the problem. In this
case, we need to find an item on the bill that could cost $18 (either singly or for
some number of them). The only item on the itemised bill for which this is true is
dinner, so Julia must have been charged for one extra dinner.
This is an example of using a systematic procedure to solve the problem.
The example below, whilst still being relatively simple, involves a slightly different type
of problem where the method of solution is less obvious.
The SuperSave supermarket sells Sudsy washing up liquid for $1.20 a bottle. At this
price they are charging 50% more than the price at which they buy the item from the
manufacturers. Next week SuperSave is having a ‘Buy two, get a third free’ offer on
this item. The supermarket does not want to lose money on this offer, so it expects the
manufacturers to reduce their prices so SuperSave will make the same actual profit on
every three bottles sold.
By how much will the manufacturers have to reduce their prices?
Commentary
12
There are several pieces of information that we will need in order to solve this problem.
1 How much do SuperSave normally buy a bottle of Sudsy washing up liquid for?
To make a 50% profit on the sale of each bottle, SuperSave must pay 80¢ for each
bottle (two-thirds of the price for which they sell it).
2 How much profit do SuperSave normally make on the sale of three bottles of Sudsy
washing up liquid?
The profit is 40¢ for each bottle, so the profit is normally $1.20 on three bottles.
3 How much would SuperSave need to pay for three bottles in order to make this
profit under their new offer?
Under the offer, SuperSave would only be paid $2.40 for three bottles, so need to pay
just $1.20 for three bottles.
4 By how much do the manufacturers need to reduce the price?
The price needs to be reduced to 40¢ per bottle, which is a reduction of 40¢ per bottle
(or half of the normal price).
In fact, it is possible to deduce that the manufacturers would have to reduce the price
to half the normal price without knowing that SuperSave’s normal selling price is $1.20:
1 Since the amount that SuperSave must pay for the bottles is two-thirds of the price
for which they sell it, the profit that SuperSave make on the sale of three bottles
must be the price for which they sell one bottle. Additionally, SuperSave currently
buy three bottles for the same price at which they sell two.
Chapter 1: What do we mean by a ‘problem’?
2 Once the offer is in place, customers will only be paying for two bottles when they
buy three. To make the same profit on the sale of three bottles, SuperSave will
therefore need to have bought the three bottles for the price at which they sell one.
3 The price that SuperSave pay for bottles must therefore have reduced to half of its
previous value.
There are other ways in which the solution could be found. For example, it would be
possible to choose a value for the reduction, see whether it would give the required
result and then adjust it accordingly. The systematic approach is more efficient
however (unless you are lucky enough to choose the correct value on your first guess).
ACTIVITY 1.2
Match the key terms (1–7) with the correct definition (A–G).
Note: you will not be asked to give definitions in the problem-solving exam, but it will
help you develop your problem-solving skills if you understand these key terms.
Key terms Definitions
1 Data A Using logic, mathematics or a systematic process in order
2 Problem-solving to work out a way to do a task that you have not done
before.
3 Information
B Plan for how to achieve something.
4 Redundant 13
C Involving a method, plan or step-by-step process to do
5 Systematic
something.
6 Strategy
D Way of solving a problem, or finding the best way to
7 Trial and error achieve a desired result, by trying one thing or another
until something succeeds.
E Knowledge, facts or news about a person, situation or
event.
F Facts, numbers and statistics used to calculate, plan or
analyse something.
G Information that is not useful or necessary.
Summary
Having read this chapter you should be able to:
■ identify the information that is important in solving a ■ identify an appropriate method to solve the problem.
problem
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
a What other pieces of information should you consider before you decide which phone
to buy?
b Explain how you would use the information to make a decision.
Exam-style questions
1 Nazim is planning to cook curries and sell them. He will take orders for parties. He will
cook the curry and rice beforehand, then put the cooked food in serving dishes that will
keep it hot. Nazim will pay his friend Raj, who has a van, to make deliveries and to fetch
the serving dishes back after the party.
Nazim’s first order is for a party of 27 people. He has a recipe using vegetables, chick
14
peas, onions and tomatoes. He is now trying to work out the right quantities of spices.
For 15 people he would normally use 100 grams of spices, made up of: ginger (20%),
turmeric , coriander (35%), and cumin seeds (20%).
a What weight of turmeric must Nazim put in a curry for 27 people?
b The food has been ordered to arrive at the party at 7.30 in the evening. Nazim knows
he needs 30 minutes to prepare the vegetables. The curry takes 2 hours to cook and
the rice takes 20 minutes. Putting the food in the dishes takes 10 minutes and putting
the dishes in the van is another 10 minutes. Raj’s journey to the party will take 25
minutes.
What is the latest time Nazim can start preparing the food?
c Nazim is buying rice. The supermarket sells 10 kilograms of basmati rice for $15.45.
Today they have a ‘buy 3, get the 4th free’ offer. The wholesaler also offers 50 kilogram
sacks of rice for $59.00.
Based on price, which rice should Nazim buy?
d Nazim wants to calculate the prices he should charge to customers to make sure he
can make a profit. State three pieces of information that he needs.
168
Chapter 18
Statistical evidence
Learning objectives
The focus of this chapter turns from information in general to statistical information in
particular. Statistics are an important source of evidence in many fields of inquiry, and
it is for this reason that they need to be viewed and assessed critically.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
18.1.3 Sampling
When census data is unavailable, the standard solution is to take a sample of the
population, and to use data drawn from it as an indication of some facts about the whole
population. If successful this solves the first problem listed above, though not the second or
third. But it is not a solution even to the first problem unless the sample is representative
of the population concerned. If it is biased or selective the sample cannot be relied upon
to reflect population-wide trends or patterns. For instance, if a sample of voters contains
a greater proportion of over-35s than is found in the general population, then the sample
will not be representative with respect to age. This would have serious implications for
the reliability of the forecast if younger voters tended to favour Party X, and over-35s Party
Y. A similar risk applies if the sample contains a disproportionate number of people from
one part of the country, or from a different class background. Any of these factors could
undermine the prediction.
The standard way to avoid biased or unrepresentative data is to take a random sample.
This is sometimes referred to as a probability sample, because it supposedly allows
every member of the population a fair chance of being selected. For example, if university
graduates make up 20% of all voters, then – according to probability theory – random
selection should result in 20% of the sample having university degrees; and likewise for the
whole range of variables in the population.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
Comment critically on the following statistical claims. How reliable do you consider the
evidence to be in each case?
1 A survey was conducted to assess public opinion on the recent government
proposal to improve air quality by making a charge on diesel vehicles entering major
cities. Around half of all vehicles on the roads currently run on diesel, but out of two
thousand car drivers randomly questioned in car parks in cities across the country,
61% said they were opposed to the charge.
2 72% of retail spending is by card payment, according to a survey at the checkouts
of a massive hypermarket in central France. 2 900 payments were recorded during a
single day, 2 088 of which were made by card.
3 According to a recent study of the world’s plants by Botanical Gardens Conservation
International (BGCI) there are 60 065 species of trees in the world. A comprehensive
tree list was compiled using data gathered from the BGCI network of 500 member
organisations (http://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/b08kscc6).
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
Commentary
The first statistic is based on the responses of a sample of car drivers to a question
about charges on diesel vehicles. (Governments of several countries are planning to
charge drivers of diesel vehicles in a bid to improve air quality in inner cities.) We are
told that the sample was 2 000, which is a respectable size, and that it was randomly
selected. Therefore, given that around half of all drivers have diesel cars, there is no
reason to suppose that the sample had a bias towards those who would be financially
penalised by the charge – 61% is a substantial majority, above the generally accepted
margin of error for a sample of its size. But the sample was drawn exclusively from
drivers questioned in city car-parks, and that is not the whole population – i.e. the
public at large – that the statistic is intended to represent. The outcome might be
very different if pedestrians, users of public transport, parents out with their children,
asthma sufferers, health workers, etc., are all proportionally represented in the sample.
There are also unanswered questions about the source of the evidence and the
motives for collecting it. Likely sources are motoring organisations, who have drivers’
interests at heart; or vehicle manufacturers or consortiums who would have an obvious
vested interest. Such factors could have an influence on the selection and analysis of
the data and/or the way the question was framed. (We should not forget the recent
scandal over falsified emissions tests on some makes of diesel cars.) Reputable sources
(like the BGCI in the third example) publish their identity along with their findings. If the
source is not given, the reliability of the evidence is further open to question.
The second statistic is drawn from a sample of payments at retail checkouts to
172
ascertain the proportion that are made by card (as opposed to cash or cheque). The
information here is objective, unlike the previous example which concerned public
opinion. It is a plain fact that 72% of the customers in the survey paid by card. The
sample size was large and selection was random insofar as every payment on the day,
in that store, was counted.
These are positive points. But is it safe to assume that data collected from one store,
on one particular day, is representative of the retail-spending population as a whole.
You will recall that a criterion of reliability for experimental or observational data is its
repeatability. To test the findings from the hypermarket survey, similar samples should
be taken from high-street (town-centre) shops, restaurants, bars, bookshops, music
stores, street-markets, and so on, to discover whether payment habits are the same
across all retail outlets. Also the growing volume of E-commerce needs to be taken into
account, especially as online purchases are predominantly paid for by credit or debit
card. It might be argued that a hypermarket, with its enormous range of goods under
one roof, is a good indicator of retail trade in general. But against this there is the fact
that payments at hypermarket checkouts tend be larger than those in smaller shops,
because people are typically buying in quantity. All in all, it would be unsafe to assume
that the survey is either sufficiently random, or properly representative. Without the
corroboration of findings from other samples, the evidence on its own must be treated
with caution.
The third example is census data. It is not a sample, but a comprehensive survey of the
world’s plants to ascertain all and only the number of species of trees. It alleged that
there were over 60 000. How do we judge such a claim? Since we as readers cannot
repeat the experiment for ourselves, our main concern must be with the reliability
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
we place on the source – or sources. Several relevant criteria were discussed in the
previous chapter, in particular reputation, expertise, and corroboration. The reliability
of evidence of this kind obviously depends on extensive botanical knowledge, and the
skill to identify and classify specimens. On the strength of its reputation it is reasonable
to assume that the BGCI researchers possess the requisite expertise and that, with 500
member organisations contributing data, the findings are well corroborated.
These observations on the three surveys do not mean that the findings discussed
above are necessarily right or wrong. What they are intended to demonstrate is the
need for critical questioning of statistical data before rushing to general conclusions.
18.2 Trends
A trend is a pattern of more or less gradual change in a set of data. A single statistic or value
cannot represent a trend, nor can a sudden leap from one value to another, though either of
these can be part of a trend. When a single statistic is part of a trend we can refer to it usefully
as a data point, and plot it on a graph. If a line drawn between a sequence of points can be
seen to travel in a particular direction, then it represents a trend. Compare the two graphs
below, giving sales figures for two business over the course of one year:
Sales 173
30,000
25,000
20,000
Sales (US$)
Sales (US$)
15,000
10,000
5,000
0
Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec
Fe 2
M 12
Ap 2
M 12
Ju 2
Ju 2
Au 2
Se 12
Oc 2
No 2
De 2
12
1
-1
-1
1
l-1
1
t-1
1
n-
b-
r-
n-
g-
p-
v-
c-
ar
ay
Ja
The lines connecting the data points in both graphs change direction frequently, but whilst
there is no general direction of travel recognisable in Fig. 18.1, there is in Fig. 18.2, as shown
by the red line. If a similar straight line were drawn on Fig. 18.1 it would give a very false
impression of how sales had performed over the year.
respect, functions in the same way as a sample – a sample of data points – and is open to
the same critical questions about size, representativeness, and repeatability.
Like a very small sample in a survey, a very short sequence of data points is weak evidence
of a lasting trend. If the company featured in Fig. 18.1 had based its forecasts on the upward
trend from July to October, and planned for a bumper Christmas, it would have been in
for a shock, because the data for those months were not representative of the whole year.
This also emphasises the need for repeatability: a trend can only be good grounds for an
important prediction if it is consistent with other relevant trends – for example, the trends in
other similar companies, and/or trends over longer periods of time. Suppose Fig. 18.1 was
matched with data from previous years, or repeated over several more years, and that the
graph looked very similar each time. Then the company’s analysts could conclude with some
confidence that their sales were likely to peak in the spring and autumn, and dip in summer
and winter.
In the chart below, the top of each column marks a data point, corresponding to the
number of new houses sold in the United States in that year. Joining the points does
not result in a single continuous trend, but it does produce a pattern of sorts that can be
interpreted and described.
1500
174
1200
900
600
300
0
‘90 ‘91 ‘92 ‘93 ‘94 ‘95 ‘96 ‘97 ‘98 ‘99 ‘00 ‘01 ‘02 ‘03 ‘04 ‘05 ‘06 ‘07 ‘08 ‘09 ‘10 ‘11 ‘12 ‘13 ‘14 ‘15 ‘16
Source: U.S. Census Bureau
Figure 18.3
Write a short paragraph describing the fluctuation in sales of new houses in the United
States in the 1990s and first 16 years of the twenty-first century. What does the data
provide by way of evidence for predicting house sales in the next years of the decade?
Commentary
The key word here is ‘fluctuation’. The sales of house prices year on year were not
stable nor was there a single direction of change. The broken line indicates that sales
were practically the same in 2016 as they had been in 1990, but they rose and fell above
and below that level by many hundreds of thousands. For the first fifteen years of the
period there were small ups and downs and short periods of flat-lining. There was also
a marked acceleration in the rate of increase – commonly referred to as a spike – in the
five years leading up to the peak in 2005. All the same, the 15 year trend from 1990 to
2005 was relentlessly upward.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
Following the spike there was an even more dramatic plunge in sales. Anyone familiar
with recent economic history will understand the significance of this. In the years
leading up to the 2007–2008 financial crisis, property prices in the US, the UK, and
many other countries rose to unsustainable levels, fuelling (amongst other things) the
demand for new homes. In retrospect the bubble was bound to burst, but even the
experts at the time could not (or chose not) to see it coming. Whatever the reason, the
market collapsed in value, with consequences clearly reflected in Fig. 18.3.
Around 2011 the downward trend bottomed out and by 2016 had returned to roughly
the same rate of increase as was seen in the 1990s. Is it safe to predict that house sales
in the US will continue to rise again post 2016, or that the cycle of boom and bust
will be repeated? By the time you are reading this page you might be able to give a
particle answer. But that is not really the point of the question because by then you
will have the benefit of hindsight. The question is about the value of past trends as
evidence for predictions. The lesson to be learnt from Fig. 18.3 should be clear enough:
such evidence on its own is inherently unreliable. It has a role to play, but only if
corroborated by evidence of other kinds.
It should be added that some trends are naturally more predictable that others. The
rise and fall of tides can be predicted with near certainty, not just because this has
always happened in the past but because we know why it happens as well as that it
happens. Trends that are governed by human behaviour – like economic fluctuations –
are not so fully understood. If they were, prediction could be improved.
175
18.2.2 Correlation
One of the reasons for studying trends and patterns in data is to identify correlations. A
correlation is a measure of the extent to which two or more variables change in relation
to each other. A correlation is called positive if the variables increase or decrease in line
with each other. In a negative (or inverse) correlation one variable increases as the other
decreases. Correlation can also be described as strong or weak, which is best understood
by examples. There is a strong, positive correlation between age and height during the early
years of a person’s life. The trend, in other words, is to grow taller with age, not necessarily at
the same rate, but more or less continually. Fig. 18.4 depicts one child’s development over
time, correlating their age (on the horizontal axis) with their height (on the vertical axis).
130
120
110
Height (cm)
100
90
80
70
60
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Age (years)
Figure 18.4
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
There is also a strong correlation between height and weight, not just over time, but between
people of different sizes. The data points in Fig. 18.5 – known as a scatter graph – represent
20 young people ranging between 1.34 m and 1.6 m in height and weighing between 28 kg
and 70 kg. If there was a perfect correlation the data points would form a single straight line.
But humans do not conform in that way: there are variations in the ratio of height to weight
from one person to the next. We can say there is a positive correlation because the points
cluster around a straight line (known as a line of best fit) which angles upwards from the
bottom left-hand corner towards the top right. Because the sample is small this cannot be
taken as the average weight/height ratio for the whole population. However, the cluster is
close enough to a straight line to confirm that there is a strong correlation between height
and weight generally – something we knew, or could have guessed, anyway.
66
64
62
60
58
56
54
52
Weight (kg)
50
48
46
44
42
40
176
38
36
34
32
30
0 134 136 138 140 142 144 146 148 150 152 154 156 158 160
Height (cm)
Figure 18.5
The closer the points are to a single line the stronger the correlation, whether positive or
negative. If there is no recognisable line of fit, we must assume there is no correlation.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
ACTIVITY 18.3
Suggest a pair of variables that would illustrate each of the examples of different
patterns of data shown in Fig. 18.6. For example, which pattern might reflect the
sales of umbrellas in relation to hours of rainfall on a given day?
This is even true when one thing is deliberately done with the intention of bringing about the
other: it may have the desired effect, but on occasions the desired effect may just be a happy
coincidence.
Commentary
There are a number of statistical facts stated in the report. The question we are
concerned with is how they are inter-related. Take the first two trends: the five-year
increase in visible police patrols and a concurrent reduction in crime. (‘Concurrent’
178 means happening at the same time, or alongside, as opposed to ‘consecutive’
which means one after the other.) Numerically speaking there is an inverse correlation
between the two trends. However, in qualitative terms (as opposed to purely
numerical) a fall in crime would be seen as a positive outcome; a success (though
evidently not enough of a success to prevent a growth in fear of crime). A rise in fear of
crime, qualitatively speaking, is a negative outcome. But as already stated we
have to be careful how we use these terms. As soon as we start talking about ‘success’
or positive (or negative) outcomes we are already running the risk of jumping to
unsupported conclusions. There is no question about the existence of a correlation,
but looking beyond that can we attribute the fall in crime to the police initiative? Is
the report evidence that higher numbers of police on the streets has the effect of
reducing crime?
The short answer is no. All we can conclude is that on this occasion there was a fall
in crime that coincided with increased policing. The danger here is that because the
increased police presence is so plausible as an explanation for the correlated fall in
crime, we assume that it is the explanation. It is perfectly possible, however, that crime
rates might have been dropping everywhere in the country during the same period and
for altogether different reasons, even in cities where police numbers were being cut; so
that although the two trends are matters of fact one may have had no impact at all on
the other.
What about the third trend, the increase in fear of crime? Initially this might seem a
negative outcome in terms of what was hoped for from the initiative. Moreover, it is a
surprising fact. The natural expectation would be that better policing and a fall in crime
would reduce anxiety, not raise it. Because the correlation is a surprising one, we find
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
ourselves looking for an explanation for it, and assuming that if it fits the facts it must
be the case. This too is a temptation, and one to be wary of. It is not too far-fetched
to imagine that the sight of more police might have the effect of making people more
aware of the threat posed by criminals, and this has increased their fears. But just
because this would explain the correlation, if true, it does not mean that it is true.
So what can be concluded from a single initiative like the imaginary example above?
The answer is: very little. Without more research and corroborating evidence the report
provides little more than anecdotal evidence: a single experiment, conducted in one
city and over a relatively short period of time.
The issues introduced here are discussed in more detail in the coming chapters on
inference, argument, and explanation. In this section they relate to statistical evidence
in particular, but they have much wider application to critical thinking in general.
18.3 Presentation
We end this chapter by looking at modes of presentation: ways in which statistical
information is commonly put before the audience. One might think that data is data, and
so long as it is accurate, representative, etc. it is merely a matter of preference as to how it is
displayed. But how a statistic is displayed can make a big difference to the effect it has on its
audience. On the positive side a well-chosen mode of presentation can make a statistic more
comprehensible or digestible; but on the negative side there are ways in which essentially
179
accurate data can create a false or misleading impression – intentionally or otherwise.
The plainest and generally the most neutral way to present a body of numerical information
is in a table. Here, for example, is a table showing the weekly fluctuation in the exchange rate
of the euro against the US dollar over a five-week period in 2017.
Table 18.1
The data from the table can alternatively be presented in visual form.
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
Compare the next two figures. Do they represent the same, or different, information?
2.500
2.000
1 EUR = (US$)
1.500
1.000
0.500
0.000
17
17
7
.1
.1
.1
.1
4.
4.
.3
.3
.3
.4
1.
8.
11
18
25
15
Figure 18.7 Euro–dollar exchange rates
1.080
1.075
1.070
1 EUR = (US$)
180
1.065
1.060
1.055
1.050
7
17
17
7
.1
.1
.1
.1
4.
4.
.3
.3
.3
.4
1.
8.
11
18
25
15
Commentary
Both bar-charts convey the same raw data as the table in Fig. 18.7, and as each other.
The difference between the two figures is purely presentational. However, that is not
to say that there is no difference in the impression given by each one. Fig. 18.8 suggests
that the exchange rate practically flat-lined for the five weeks in question, whereas
Fig. 18.9 depicts sharp fluctuations over the same period. It is easy to see why the
difference occurs: it is due entirely to the range of values on the vertical axis – in
other words, scale. In Fig. 18.8 the scale begins at zero, and ranges up to 2.5 dollars,
rendering the 1 or 2 cent variation barely visible. The range on the axis in Fig. 18.9 is
itself only 3 cents ($1.05–$1.08), creating a ‘zoom-in’ effect on the narrow range within
which the rate varies.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
Is one of the graphs the ‘right’ visualisation, and the other ‘wrong’? Not exactly: both
give a somewhat distorted picture. One exaggerates the fluctuations in the rate, the
other registers no significant movement at all. Unless you are exchanging very large
amounts of currency, a cent either way is not going to have much impact on your life,
but still it is useful to know the direction in which the rate is changing, and Fig. 18.8 fails
to do that. Fig. 18.9 is certainly more sensitive to variation, but perhaps unnecessarily
alarmist, if used to prompt headlines such as:
A 357
B 488 181
C 361
D 580
E 16,676
Examine the three graphs below (Figs. 18.9–11), each of which is meant to depict the
sales figures in Table 18.3 visually. Explain the problem with each of the visualisations.
18000
16000
14000
12000
10000
8000
6000
4000
2000
0
A B C D E
Figure 18.9
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
1800
16676
1600
1400
1200
1000
800
600
400
200
0
A B C D E
Figure 18.10
20000
10000
1000
800
600
182 400
200
0
A B C D E
Figure 18.11
Commentary
In Fig. 18.9 an ordinary scale is used on the vertical axis, with a range from 0 to 20 000.
This range is needed because the volume of sales of one of the books is more than
27 times greater than next-best seller in the list. The effect is to squash the columns
representing the other titles, so that the differences between them are almost
indistinguishable.
The solution adopted by the next two figures is to truncate some of the data in one
of two ways. This just means shortening, or condensing, one or more of the columns.
For example in Fig 18.10 the column representing Book E is broken, to show that there
is a missing section. The actual number of sales is displayed above the column to
compensate. This permits visual discrimination between the sales of the other titles,
but at the cost of losing any sense of the gulf between Book E and the rest. Fig. 18.11
has a similar purpose, but achieves it by compressing the vertical axis. Above 1 000 the
intervals change from 200 to 5 000. But unless this is noticed and adjustment made in
how the graph is interpreted, it could be misleading.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
The message to take from this is the need for vigilance: to be aware of the purpose and
usefulness of such techniques but also of their potential for abuse. Imagine a politically
sensitive report on, say, funding for schools by different educational authorities.
Depending on who is publishing the report, and why, there may be a motive for
presenting the data in a manner that either maximises or minimises the impression
created by a particular mode of presentation without actually falsifying the data.
As with the previous example, there is a large discrepancy between the numbers, making 183
a meaningful graph difficult to construct. But in this instance it is not only the size of
numbers that are different, it is what kind of set each one represents: one a number of
people, the other a number of acts or events. It is, as the saying goes, like ‘comparing apples
and oranges’.
A common solution to such discrepancies is to use a different scale for each variable, as
below. The scale corresponding to police numbers (blue) is on the left; crime numbers (red)
on the right. Time, as usual, is on the horizontal axis.
Police Crimes
numbers
20 000
400 19 000
18 000
200 17 000
16 000
0 1 2 3 4 5
Years
Figure 18.12
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
Comment critically on the presentation of the data in Fig. 18.12. Does it accurately
represent the statistical information in Table 18.4? Are there any respects in which you
think the graph could give a misleading impression? If so, does it matter?
Commentary
The graph employs two different scales in order to depict the rising trend in police
numbers and the simultaneous fall in crime. The former increases from 420 to 483, a
rise of 15% as recorded in the table. The latter decreases from 19 002 to 17 672, a fall of
7%. Individually both trends are correctly plotted, but due to the choice of scales, the
steepness of the decline in crime is exaggerated. In percentage terms the increase in
policing is over twice that of the fall in crime, yet in visual terms it appears that the fall
in crime is relatively greater. Does it matter? Yes, if the impression given by the graph
has the effect of persuading the reader to draw an incorrect or overstated conclusion;
and if the purpose of the report were to support the view that the initiative had been
highly effective, when in fact it had a minimal effect.
Graphs and diagrams have a useful role to play in depicting trends and patterns that
are hard to recognise in large and complex tables. But they can also distort data,
sometimes harmlessly, sometimes to the point of falsifying evidence.
Be on guard.
184
Summary
Following on from the discussion of evidence in ■ sources of data
general, this chapter examined some important aspects ■ collecting and sampling
of statistical evidence. In particular it considered the ■ trends, and their relation to prediction
importance of: ■ correlation
■ modes of presentation.
Chapter 18: Statistical evidence
Our recent drive to cut prices is paying off. We compared last month’s prices
on all food items at four supermarkets. Guess who was the cheapest?
Cotes 662
Dasamart 1983
Shrewsbury 553
185
Hendriks 1035
Figure 18.13
Make five criticisms of the statistics in the advertisement and/or the support they give to
the claim,
“To save money on your weekly shopping, come to Dasamart!”
CI A Level Thinking Skills 9694/42 Paper 4 Applied Reasoning May/June 2016
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
2 Figure 18.14 shows reported reactions to common allergens. A newspaper article has
published this chart under the headline, ‘Food additives make children more hyperactive.’
Identify and explain two weaknesses in the way the newspaper has used the data.
Penicillin
allergy 150
Hayfever 100
Asthma 88
Dermatitis 80
General food
sensitivity 20
Sensitivity to 2
food additives
0 50 100 150
Figure 18.14
Adapted from CIE AS Level Thinking Skills 9694/02 Critical Thinking Specimen Paper 2 (published 2010)
186
203
Chapter 20
Identifying argument
Learning objectives
This chapter introduces the major topic of argument. It examines standard and
non-standard forms of argument, and argument in the sense of dispute. Activities in
the chapter include identifying arguments and their components, and first steps in
analysis and evaluation.
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
Reading through this chapter and attempting the Activities will help you identify an
argument, in the critical thinking sense, and the key components of an argument: one or
more reasons and a conclusion. In this textbook a more specialist word is generally used
instead of ‘reason’. ‘Premise’ has a very similar meaning to ‘reason, as this chapter explains.
If you are preparing for the Cambridge International A Level Thinking Skills, you will find that
the questions in those examinations use the term ‘reason’ rather than ‘premise’.
This chapter also introduces the concept of the ‘soundness’ of an argument. Soundness is
important if you are interested in the wider application of critical thinking, but you will not
be tested on it in the Cambridge International A Level Thinking Skills examinations.
In the CI Critical Thinking exams you will need to be able to:
We can imagine the circumstances in which someone might put forward this argument. The
speaker wants to persuade one or more companions that it is possible and/or safe to cross to
an island that is usually cut off by the sea. The reason the speaker gives is that the tide is out.
The argument consists of two claims – a premise and a conclusion – joined by the word
‘so’. ‘So’ is one of several words and phrases which are used to mark the conclusion of an
argument. Others include: ‘therefore’, ‘thus’, ‘consequently’ and ‘for this reason’. Because
drawing a conclusion is very similar to inferring (see previous chapter), these words are often
called inference indicators (or argument indicators). What the author (or proponent) of
this argument is doing is inferring from the observed state of the tide that the island can be
reached. That is what makes (1) an argument.
Note: a proponent is someone who propounds, i.e. puts forward, an argument.
Figure 20.1
Chapter 20: Identifying argument
To judge whether or not (1) is a good argument two criteria must be met. First, it must be true
that if the tide is out the island truly can be reached. This, as stated above, is what it means
for the conclusion to follow from the reasons. If it does, the argument can be approved as
valid. If this condition is not met, then it could be the case that the tide is out, but the island
is still beyond reach, which would make the inference unsafe and the argument invalid.
Second, the tide must actually be out. Only then can the argument be judged sound.
These are quite technical points, which may be unfamiliar to some readers. Here they are
introduced briefly, but they will be revisited at various points in the coming chapters 22–24.
(2) If the Earth were flat, ships sailing away from land would appear to get smaller and
smaller until they disappear. Instead they appear to drop out of sight after a few
miles. Therefore the Earth cannot be flat.
205
This argument consists of two premises, corresponding to the first two sentences, and a
conclusion at the end indicated by ‘therefore’. However, when we refer to something as an
argument, that does not necessarily mean the text. The text expresses the argument, but the
argument itself – like any other claim or assertion – could be expressed in other ways without
being a different argument. For example:
(2a) The Earth cannot be flat because, if it were flat, ships sailing away from land would
appear to get smaller and smaller until they disappeared. Instead they appear to drop
out of sight after a few miles.
In this version of the argument the sentences are in a different order, with the conclusion at
the front and the reasons (premises) following. But they have the same logical function as
they do in (2). Notice, too, that as the sentences are reversed in (2a), a different connective
is needed, i.e. one that identifies the reasons. In this case the proponent has used ‘because’,
but could just have well used ‘since’. These words can also function as argument indicators,
but in a different way.
There is a third option which is to use no connective or indicator at all.
(2b) The Earth cannot be flat. If it were flat, ships sailing away from land would appear
to get smaller and smaller until they disappeared. Instead they appear to drop out of
sight after a few miles.
You will often find natural-language arguments without argument indicators, simply because
the context and sentence-meaning make it clear which sentence is the conclusion and which
are the premises.
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
in which ‘C’ stands for the conclusion and ‘R’ reason. (Use ‘P’ for ‘premise’ if preferred.) As
you can see (2) is already expressed in a very standard form, with its sentences in the same
order as the schema. (2a) and (2b) are variant forms, but still very recognisable as argument
texts. Some arguments, however, are less obvious, and need to be extracted from the text
and reconstructed to show their argument form.
The customary way do this, both in logic and critical thinking, is to place the reasons
(premises) in a list, and to separate them from the conclusion by a horizontal line. So, with
some abbreviation, you could set out argument (2) as follows:
(2.1) R1 If the Earth were flat, ships sailing away would appear to get smaller . . .
R2 But they appear to drop out of sight . . . .
Reconstructing an argument to give it this standard form helps to make the reasoning clear,
206
and assist with its subsequent evaluation. It also gives the critic a convenient way of referring
to the components of the argument. Instead of having to write out a premise or conclusion
each time it is mentioned, the labels can be used instead. For example:
R1 and R2 are given as reasons for C.
It may seem a lot of trouble to go to when an argument is as brief, and as recognisable, as
(2). But with more complex reasoning, which you will encounter as you progress through the
chapters, this kind of formal reconstruction is a valuable tool.
(3) Kris: Did you know that Earth is a huge ball hanging in space?
Bart: Don’t be ridiculous. The Earth is flat. Can’t you see that?
Kris: It can’t be flat. If you just let me explain—
Bart: There’s nothing to explain. Just use your eyes.
Kris: I am using my eyes, and they tell me the Earth is round. Look at that ship.
Soon you will only see the top of the mast. That wouldn’t happen if the
Earth was flat.
Bart: (in a lowered voice): Well let me tell you something. If you go around talking
this kind of nonsense, someone is going to lock you up and throw away the
key. Or worse.
Chapter 20: Identifying argument
Texts (2) and (3) are both examples of argument, though in different senses of the word. This
is due to the fact that in English the word ‘argument’ has two distinct meanings, as seen in
the following dictionary definition:
argument (noun)
1 a reason or reasons supporting a conclusion; a case made for or against a point of view.
2 a debate or dispute, especially a heated one; disagreement, quarrel, row.
As you can see, example (2) is an argument of the first sort. It can be called a reasoned
argument to distinguish it from mere dispute. Example (3) is quite obviously an argument in
the second sense – a dispute, bordering on a quarrel. There is very little reasoned argument
going on in (3). Kris tries to explain his position, but his opponent shouts him down. The two
speakers are mostly just exchanging opinions, without giving any serious reasons to back
them up. There is no single conclusion to which the text as a whole is leading.
Interestingly, the English language is unusual in giving these two senses to the same word.
In Spanish, for example, argumento has the reason-giving sense; the disputational sense is
given by discusión, controversia or disputa. 207
However, it would be wrong to think that the two meanings of ‘argument’ are completely
unrelated. As stated at the beginning of the chapter, arguments typically exist to persuade,
and it is clear that in a dispute like (3) each of the participants is trying to change the mind
of the other. For (2) there is no such context available, but the most likely context would be
some real or imagined debate. Why else would its proponent feel any need to give reasons
or evidence to support the claim? Arguments in the reason-giving sense occur typically in the
face of opposition, i.e. in the context of dispute or disagreement.
Moreover, most arguments of the disputational kind have some elements of reason-giving in
them. Even in (3), which is predominantly a quarrel, both participants draw upon the evidence
of their senses (i.e. sight) to support their differing conclusions. Bart says that anyone can
see the Earth is flat; that all you need to do is use your eyes. Kris responds that his eyes tell a
different story, and tries to explain by bringing in the evidence of disappearing ships.
If you wanted to represent Bart’s contribution to the dispute as a standard argument, you
could summarise it as follows:
No one today would rate this a good argument. Though the premise is true – the Earth does
look flat – the conclusion is false. However, in the fictional dispute between Bart and Kris
it was not known that the Earth was spherical. Nobody had sailed round it or seen it from
space. But even without that knowledge it can still be seen that Bart’s argument is invalid
because the evidence – though true – does not justify the inference from R to C. In general,
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
We have established that Bart’s argument (B) is a bad one. Once the solitary premise
and conclusion are identified it is very easy to see that the grounds are inadequate. But
not all arguments that you hear will be as simple or as obviously defective as (B).
208
Compare (B) with the argument Kris attempts in the course of the dispute:
(K) I am using my eyes, and they tell me the Earth is round. Look at that ship.
Soon you will only see the top of the mast. That wouldn’t happen if the Earth
was flat.
Is (K) a good argument, or not? Is it better than (B)? Would it persuade you that the
Earth’s surface was curved if you had previously believed it was flat?
Commentary
(K) might seem like a reasonable argument, because we now accept that the Earth is
roughly spherical. But, as we also know from history, arguments like (K) – or (2), which
makes the same point – were not enough to convince the general public straight away.
People needed more reasons if they were going to give up a belief that had persisted
for centuries. Judged critically it becomes clear that (K) is has much the same weakness
as (B) because, like (B), it also argues from appearances. If the flat appearance of the
Earth does not mean that it is flat, then can we be sure that the appearance of ships
dropping out of sight does not prove, on its own, the surface of the Earth is curved. It
could be an optical illusion; some kind of mirage perhaps. It isn’t a mirage: it is perfectly
true both that ships appear to sink and that the Earth’s curvature is the reason. But
today we know that fact independently of the argument. The single reason given in (K)
does not, on its own, establish its conclusion.
Chapter 20: Identifying argument
ACTIVITY 20.2
(4) Ships appear to sink lower and lower in the water the further they are from land.
But they cannot actually be sinking, or they would not come back. Also, sailors have
proved that if you set off in one general direction, for example east or west, and keep
going, you eventually arrive back where you started from. These facts show that the
Earth cannot be flat. Besides, photographs have been taken from space that show the
Earth’s curvature.
Here four reasons are given in support of the conclusion. The conclusion is introduced by
the phrase: ‘These facts show that’ – another way of saying, ‘so’ or ‘therefore’. Three of the 209
reasons are given first; then the conclusion; then a further, seemingly indisputable premise.
So the structure of the argument could be depicted as follows:
Obviously (4) is a much stronger argument than (2). Whether it actually convinces its
audience will still depend on their willingness to accept the evidence. But if they understand
and believe the claims you are making, then it would be irrational of them not to accept the
conclusion also.
Of course, the ‘if’ is a big one. In all probability the audience from that time would not accept
your claims because they would not understand them. What could ‘pictures from space’
mean to a fourteenth-century fisherman? They would lock you up or declare you mad (or
both), and carry on believing what they had always believed, and could see with their own
eyes: a flat Earth surrounded by flat sea.
This is why ‘claim’ is the right word for the statements that appear in arguments. Some of the
claims made in an argument may be known facts, but others may be forecasts, suggestions,
beliefs or opinions. Claims may also be false. It is perfectly possible to construct an argument
from false claims, either out of ignorance, or to misinform the audience. We hear all too
much of this practice these days under the heading of ‘fake news’. (That is probably what
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
people hundreds of years ago would have suspected you of doing, as they slammed the
dungeon door.)
ACTIVITY 20.3
1 Think of a suitable conclusion that you could add to the following to make it into an
argument:
Police forces the world over face a dilemma. On top of dealing with murders and
other major incidents, they have to divide their limited time and finite resources
between tackling minor crimes such as shoplifting and street robbery, and traffic
offences such as speeding or careless driving. Of course, the consequences of
speeding can be as bad or worse than the theft of a wallet or a mobile phone. They
can be fatal. But there is a big difference of another sort. The thief intends to do harm
and to deprive people of their rightful property, whereas any harm that is done by a
car-driver, however serious, is usually accidental.
There is no argument here. ‘Since’ in the phrase means ‘ever since’, which is different from
the meaning it has in front of a premise. For another example, compare,
With
There is no argument in (6). The word ‘so’ in that context means ‘also’, not ‘therefore’. But
with very little change (7) is a simple argument, an inference from Julia’s alleged ambitious
nature to prediction about her daughter. The point to take from this is that although the
argument indicators can help to identify possible arguments in a text, it cannot be assumed
that wherever the words occur they have that function.
Besides, as observed earlier in the chapter, there are plenty of examples of natural-
language arguments which contain no connectives. An argument may be expressed by a
plain sequence of statements (or claims), yet obviously not every sequence of claims is an
argument. There it is no argument here, for example:
(8) Photographs from space show the Earth’s surface as curved. The curvature does not
show when a photograph is taken from ground level.
Being able to identify arguments means being able to distinguish confidently between
211
argument and non-argument. It is not always as straightforward as it might seem.
(9) Completed tax forms and payments must be received by 31 July. Late payment may
result in a fine not exceeding $100. Your payment did not reach the tax office until
12 August.
Cambridge International AS & A Level Thinking Skills
There are three possible configurations of the sentences, using ‘so’ or ‘therefore’:
• Completed tax forms and payments must be received by 31 July. Late payment may
result in a fine not exceeding $100. Therefore your payment did not reach the tax office
until 12 August.
• Late payment may result in a fine not exceeding $100. Your payment did not reach the tax
office until 12 August. So completed tax forms and payments must be received by 31 July.
• Completed tax forms and payments must be received by 31 July. Your payment did
not reach the tax office until 12 August. Therefore late payment may result in a fine not
exceeding $100.
In each rearrangement the attempt to use an argument indicator sounds unnatural, which
indicates that none of the sentences is the kind of claim that could follow from the others in
the way that a conclusion follows from reasons.
Using the ‘therefore/so’ test, and the definition of an argument as reasons given for a
conclusion, decide which of the following could be interpreted as arguments.
For those that are arguments, identify the conclusion and note what kind of claim it is.
Lastly, discuss how well supported you think the conclusion is, given the reasons.
1 The Tokyo train leaves at 4:24. It can take up to 40 minutes to get to the station if the
traffic is bad. We should leave for the station by 3:40.
212
2 Raisa is the only person with a key to the safe. The police are bound to treat her as a
suspect. The money went missing when she was in the building on her own.
3 You are likely to get a fine. Completed tax forms and payments must be received by
31 July and people who miss the deadline are usually fined $100. Your payment did
not reach the tax office until 12 August.
4 From the fifteenth century European sailors reached the lands of the east by sailing
west. Those who sailed on and survived eventually arrived back in Europe. When
they claimed they had sailed around the world, few people believed them.
5 There are only three possible causes of the leak in your system: the pump could be
worn, a hose could be split or one of the connections could be loose. I’ve checked
the hoses and tightened all the connections, but the machine still leaks.
Commentary
(1) can be understood as an argument. The conclusion, which is at the end, is a
recommendation. This also is a useful clue: recommendations are often accompanied
by reasons. Here there are two: the time of the train’s departure and the possibility of
a 40-minute journey to the station. If they are both true, then clearly they justify the
conclusion.
Chapter 20: Identifying argument
(2) The same applies: the conclusion is a prediction that the police will (definitely)
suspect Raisa, firstly because she is the only key-holder, and secondly because she
was alone in the building. The argument is perhaps not quite as solid as (a). Do police
always treat people as suspects in these circumstances? The words ‘bound to . . .’ make
the conclusion a very strong claim. Even if both premises are true, there may be other
factors – CCTV footage for instance – that show Raisa was nowhere near the safe, and
therefore make it less than definite that she will be treated as a suspect. Nonetheless, it
is a safe interpretation of the text to read it as an argument – good or bad.
(3) is also an argument. The conclusion is another prediction (of sorts). You could
also have described it as a statement of probability: ‘You are likely to get a fine.’ The
reasoning for the conclusion is that payment did not reach the tax office until 12
August, together with the second sentence which establishes that the payment was
late and that late payment usually results in a fine. The argument is quite sound,
mainly because the conclusion is a fairly weak claim. If fines are usual for lateness, then
a fine is likely.
(4) is not an argument. None of the three sentences makes sense with ‘therefore’ in
front of it, e.g. ‘From the fifteenth century European sailors reached the lands of the
east by sailing west. Those who sailed on and survived eventually arrived back in
Europe. Therefore when they claimed they had sailed around the world, few people
believed them.’ The connective that makes most sense is ‘but’, not ‘therefore’. None of
the claims is a conclusion drawn from either or both of the other two; and it is the same
whichever order the claims are placed in.
(5) is not an argument either – at least not an explicit one – because, as in (4) – none 213
of its actual sentences is a natural conclusion. However, (5) does point towards a
conclusion, even though it is not stated. In fact there is really only one conclusion
that you could draw from (5) – that the pump must be worn – because both the
other possibilities are ruled out. What we can say about (5) is that it is not a complete
argument. It is left to you (the reader or listener) to draw a conclusion – though in this
case it leaves you in little doubt as to what the conclusion would be if the author had
meant (5) to be an argument. We could say therefore that (5) is an implicit argument, or
that it has an implicit conclusion.
ACTIVITY 20.5
Explain why the ‘so/therefore test’ is useful but not the last word when identifying
arguments.
Is the conversation above just a quarrel, or is there reasoned argument going on here
as well? If there is, identify some examples of arguments propounded by one or other of
the participants.
Commentary
Overall, this conversation is a quarrel, and parts of it are no more than exchanges of
opinion, laced with mild insults. But in the course of the exchange there are examples
of developed, reasoned arguments as well, coming from both sides.
The clearest example is Anita’s first long paragraph. This is practically a standard
argument, with three numbered reasons and a conclusion signalled by ‘so’. Bara
responds with a counter-argument. This gives three reasons which challenge or
contradict Anita’s claims, then two further reasons (the value of keeping people in
touch, and of saving lives in emergencies) to support a position which is the complete
opposite of Anita’s. Bara’s conclusion is expressed by the first sentence of the
paragraph: ‘You just can’t say that.’ In other words: ‘It is not true that mobile phones
do more harm than good’ (as Anita has just asserted). In natural-language arguments,
conclusions may not always be spelled out in full, as they are in a standard argument.
Expressions such as ‘Yes’, ‘No’ and ‘You’re wrong!’ can be understood as conclusions, if
it is clear what they refer to, and they are supported by reasons. 215
In the three paragraphs that follow we see Anita and Bara each trying to reinforce their
arguments with further reasons and objections. Then, as their tempers begin to fray,
they go back to mere quarrelling and personal remarks.
In some textbooks the impression is given that critical thinking is concerned only with
arguments in the standalone, reason–conclusion form, and not with argument in the
sense of dispute. But as suggested earlier, we miss something important about the
practical meaning and purpose of argument if we ignore the most obvious context in
which it occurs. Much of our reasoning – perhaps all of it – arises in or from differences
of belief or opinion. An argument that the Earth is not flat makes practical sense only if
someone – past or present – thinks that it is flat, or needs proof that it is. A defence of
the use of mobiles phones is unlikely to be called for unless there is understood to be
some opposition or objection to it.
critical thinking skills and techniques whenever you are attempting to present a point of
view, either verbally or in writing.
ACTIVITY 20.7
Think back over what you have learnt up till now about good and poor reasoning. As a
group discuss what you think makes reasoning persuasive. Can you identify any writers
or speakers who you think are persuasive? Is it their use of reasoning that is persuasive
or is it something else?
ACTIVITY 20.8
Think of one or two reasons that could be used to support each of the following claims,
making them into arguments.
a It is wrong to charge foreign students higher fees than other students.
b Private cars with fewer than four occupants should be banned from city centres.
c The stars of football, baseball and other popular sports deserve every cent of the
millions that they are paid.
ACTIVITY 20.9
Use newspapers or the internet to find a current issue that interests you. Write your own
216
short argument for or against a conclusion in the form, ‘The Government should ...’
Your argument should contain at least three reasons as well as the conclusion.
Follow the steps below to help you develop your argument.
1 Clearly state your conclusion. (You can use argument indicators and you could put
your conclusion either at the beginning or at the end of the argument.)
2 Make sure your reasons link to your conclusions.
3 Decide whether you should make your conclusion stronger or less strong. (If it is less
strong, it may link more closely to your reasons.)
Summary
The topic of the chapter was argument. ■ Argument indicators were discussed as means of
identifying and testing texts for the presence of
■ An argument was defined as a complex construction
argument.
in which a conclusion is claimed to follow from stated
■ The distinction between reasoned argument and a mere
reasons (alternatively referred to as premises).
dispute was discussed.
■ A good argument was defined as one in which the
■ The critical activities of simple analysis and evaluation
premises are true and follow from the reasons, meaning
that if the reasons are true then the conclusion cannot were introduced.
be false. ■ The activity of writing reasoned argument was
front of the Sun. Even when this does happen, every eighteen months or so, it is only
visible in certain parts of the world, different every time.
2 Which one of the following is an argument? Why are the others not?
a Although some people think that microwave radiation from mobile phones may
damage the user’s brain, no hard evidence has been found of harmful effects on
health. ‘Hands-free’ kits are supposed to make the phones safer because the user
does not need to hold the phone as close to their head.
b Land animals such as tortoises often spread to small islands aboard ships. However,
the tortoises on the Galapagos Islands must have arrived by some other means. The
first people to visit the Galapagos found tortoises already there.
c In most countries cyclists are not legally required to wear a helmet, in the way car-
drivers are required to wear a seat belt. This inconsistency in the law persists despite
the growing number of road accidents in which a cyclist is injured or even killed.
Exam-style questions
People are always fascinated by the question of whether there are extra-terrestrial life forms.
By this they usually mean ‘aliens’ as portrayed in popular science fiction series like Star Trek.
However, if there were other life forms of this type in outer space they would have visited us or
they would have managed to send some sort of radio signal which we would have intercepted.
As this has not happened, it looks as if we are the only life form in the universe. So the dream of
meeting people from other galaxies needs to be dismissed as a childish fantasy.
Adapted from CI Thinking Skills 8435/1 Paper 1 October/November 2002
State, using the author’s words, the conclusion of the above argument.