International
International
Rosa M. Rodríguez-Izquierdo
Universidad Pablo de Olavide, Sevilla
DOI: 10.5944/educXX1.30143
ABSTRACT
KEY WORDS
RESUMEN
PALABRAS CLAVE
INTRODUCTION
The results of this process are also visible in Spain. The Spanish
government has made great efforts to promote the internationalization of
HE, partly by supplying grants for study abroad programs and partly by
offering opportunities for foreign students in its institutions. As a result of its
adherence to the Bologna Process, Spanish universities also adopted several
instruments to offer students various intercultural experiences such as
internships and international dimension courses, in the belief that students
must be educated in a global context to develop intercultural skills (Rodríguez-
Izquierdo, 2015). Since then, it is essential to position cultural differences
centrally within university training since Spain is one of the European
countries with the highest mobility among university students. However,
Vande Berg, Paige and Lou (2012) refer to the “immersion assumption”,
which presumes that students automatically mix on university campuses
and, thus, develop intercultural competence. Nonetheless, a raft of scientific
production questions whether mere interaction works. In this vein, Brewer
(2003) posits that often when culturally diverse individuals or groups meet,
interactions are difficult as they encounter different behaviors, norms,
values and beliefs, leading to feelings of anxiety or avoidance behavior.
As an outcome, feelings of suspicion, distrust and threat can result in
THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
study, we drew on the complex and polysemous notion of IS, which has
given rise to different approaches (Arasaratam-Smith, 2017).
Table 1
Bennett’s model (Bennett and Bennett 2004)
LITERATURE REVIEW
METHOD
This study used a two-phase method design. The first gathered data
was based on a short-term longitudinal method approach to investigate
the levels of IS overtime, and to gather data which would allow for the
exploration of the relationship between students’ IS development and
international experiences during university. The second was a correlational-
predictive design with the intention to identify the variables that might
predict the development of IS.
Participants
Measures
The dependent variable was the IDI score. The IS predictor variables
were: sex, year of study, mobility experiences, program duration,
international travel, knowledge of other languages, friends from other
cultures and field of study.
Instruments
Procedure
Data collection was carried out online. The students received an email
informing them of the objectives of the study, assuring confidentiality, asking
for informed consent and providing the link to access the instruments. The
estimated average response time was 30 minutes.
The data collection was carried out in two phases. The first gathered
quantitative data using the IDI which was administered to first-year and
final-year students in two waves of data collection: in october 2019 and
in May 2020 using the IDI and IEI to learn about students’ development
alongside their contact experiences. Data collection followed the same
model as outlined above. The design thus not only considered the changes
that occur between these two temporal periods but also traced the possible
changes experienced by students who carry out an international mobility
experience with those who do not.
Data analysis
RESULTS
Table 2
Students’ intercultural contact experience
Category Percentage of students (%)
Mobility experiences
Yes 21%
No 79%
Duration of the program
one semester 49%
Less than one semester 15%
More than one semester 36%
International travel
Never traveled abroad 21.4%
Traveled only once 43.2%
Have traveled more than once 35.4%
Friends from other cultures
Agree 67.8%
Disagree 22.1%
Neither agree nor disagree 12.1%
Number of intercultural interactions per
week 11.8%
1 to 3 14.9%
4 to 6 19.8%
7 to 10 25.7%
11 to 14 16.0%
15 or more
The first part of the study was to examine whether first-year and fourth-
year students developed IS. For this question, their mean IDI scores from
waves I and II were compared. The mean decreased slightly from 93.41 in
wave I to 89.98 in wave II. The mean change in score was -1.43 (SD = 11.29).
Although these means were somewhat different, both were in the same
developmental stage, minimization, characterized by being able to recognize
cultural differences in a superficial way, indicating that overall, there was
little change between the first-year and final-year students. This was verified
by means of a t-test comparing the mean scores t (112) = 1.41, p =.19.
Figure 1
IDI developmental stages of irst-year and inal-year students at wave I and
wave II
Table 3
Pearson’s correlations between intercultural development, language knowledge,
mobility experiences, friends from other cultures, international travel and program
duration; and partial correlations controlling for year and sex
Total sample
Total sample controlling for Females Males
n = 1645 year and sex n = 864 n = 781
SI
Language knowledge .07* .07* 11* .05
Mobility experiences .43*** .42*** .40*** .46***
Friends from other .36*** .36*** .31*** .40***
cultures
Number of intercul- .29*** .27*** .27*** .29***
tural interactions
per week
Program duration -.25*** -.23*** -.33*** -.26***
International travel -.29*** -.27*** -.39*** -.29***
Note. * p <.05 ** p <.01 *** p <.001
Table 4
Multiple regression results: predicting IDI change
Standard β
R R² ΔR² B Error Constant t
SI
Total sample .
Mobility experiences .488 238 .237 .339 .058 3.366 .286 .583***
Sex .563 .317 .315 -6.704 .697 14.095 -.280 -9.61***
Friends from other .580 .336 .334 .282 .059 14.092 .235 4.78***
cultures
Females
Mobility experiences .549 .301 .299 .409 .086 4.678 .341 4.730***
Friends from other .574 .330 .326 .314 .085 4.916 .266 3.960***
cultural
Knowledge language .583 .339 .333 .050 .022 -2.845 .096 2.230*
Males
Mobility experiences .449 .202 .200 .310 .078 2.943 .292 3.962***
Friends from other .464 .215 .211 .215 .081 2.897 .185 2.649**
cultures
Note. * p<.05. ** p<.01. *** p<.001
DISCUSSION
Regarding the first objective, the results suggested that the development
of IS was very limited, with few differences in IDI scores between first and
fourth-year students. The fact that most students were in the minimization
stage, characterized by the widespread belief that everyone is quite similar,
is not surprising since recognizing the value of cultural differences can be
very difficult (Bennett, 1993; Deardorff, 2006; Hammer & Bennett, 2002).
Minimization is within the ethnocentric phase and still presupposes that
values are universal. This stage is indicated by statements such as: “In other
cultures you just have to be yourself”, “what you have to apply is common
sense” or “there are things that work everywhere”. The lack of significant
changes in the development of IS within the ethnocentric stages is very
similar to the results found by Paige et al. (2004).
and that more work is needed to provide a context in which the number
of intercultural encounters is positive and meaningful. Thus, the number
of interactions might not be enough to promote the development of IS for
most students who remain in the ethnocentric phases. The results aligned
with the fact that the presence of many international students on campus
does not mean that contact will occur (He, Lundgren & Pynes, 2017). These
findings support the concern found in the literature that universities need
to be more pro-active in introducing intercultural training in their programs
(Cost et al., 2016).
Finally, this study identified that only three of the predictive variables
studied present statistically significant differences in relation to IS: having
mobility experiences, being a woman and having friends from other cultures.
With respect to sex, women showed a higher level of IS towards differences.
These results concur with the findings of Anderson et al. (2006) and Vande
Berg (2007). one possible explanation offered to account for the higher IS
of women could be related to the different models of socialization, which
could partially explain the difference in developmental levels between men
and women. However, this is an issue that should be examined in future
research. Furthermore, the data for variables that were found to be poor IS
predictors require further consideration, one of which is having knowledge
of other languages. one of the possible reasons to explain this might be: on
the one hand, in the sample, the number of students who were able to speak
a second language was very small (28.4%); and on the other hand, knowledge
of languages might not necessarily imply maintaining contact with people
from different cultures. Whilst these findings are exploratory, they are
consistent with those found by Liu (2019). It is likely that with other kinds
of samples (with more knowledge of second languages), these correlations
might have been different. Concerning the number of intercultural
interactions, although participants reported having frequent interactions,
it is hard to examine how in-depth students’ intercultural interactions were
and if they were the kind of exchanges that lead to intercultural learning.
Educational implications
The present study has limitations and elicits suggestions for future
studies. Firstly, it should be noted that, since the data are correlational in
nature, they provide little information on the causal relationship that might
exist between the variables studied. lopment (Deardorff & Arasaratam-
Smith, 2017). The next limitation is a clear lack of prior research studies
on this topic in Spain, so it is tough to compare our findings with other
scholars to offer a more complete view on the issue.
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
Allport, g. W. (1954). The nature of Bunch, J. C., Rampold, S. D., Cater, M., &
prejudice. Addison-Wesley. Blackburn, J. J. (2018). The impact of
a short-term international experience
Anderson, P. H., Lawton, L., Rexeisen,
on undergraduate students’ cultural
R. J., & Hubbard, A. C. (2006).
competency. Journal of Agricultural
Short-term study abroad and
Education, 59(4), 120-136. https://doi.
intercultural sensitivity: a pilot study.
org/10.5032/jae.2018.04120
International Journal of Intercultural
Relations, 30(4), 457-469. https://doi. Chen, g. M., & Starosta, W. (2000). The
org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.10.004 development and validation of the
intercultural sensitivity scale. Human
Arasaratam-Smith, L. A. (2017).
Communication, 3(1), 2-14. https://doi.
Intercultural competence: an overview.
org/10.1037/t61546-000
In D. Deardorff, & L. A. Arasaratam-
Smith (Eds), Intercultural competence Cots, J. M., Aguilar, M., Mas-Alcolea,
in Higher Education: International S., & Llanes, À. (2016). Studying
approaches, assessment and application the impact of academic mobility on
(pp. 7-18). Routledge. intercultural competence: a mixed-
methods perspective. The Language
Bennett, M. (1986). A developmental
Learning Journal, 44(3), 304-322.
approach to training for intercultural
https://doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2016
sensitivity. International Journal of
.1198097
Intercultural Relations, 10(2), 179-
195. https://doi.org/10.1016/0147- Deardorff, D. (2006). Identification
1767(86)90005-2 and assessment of intercultural
competence as a student outcome
Bennett, M. (1993). Towards
of internationalization. Journal
ethnorelativism: a developmental
of Studies in International Educa-
model of intercultural sensitivity. tion, 10(3), 241-266. https://doi.
In R. Paige (Ed.), Education for the org/10.1177/1028315306287002
intercultural experience (pp. 21-71).
Intercultural Press. Deardorff, D. (2009). The SAGE
Handbook of Intercultural Competence.
Bennett, M. (2004). Becoming SAgE International Inc.
interculturally competent. In J. Wurzel
(Ed.), Toward multiculturalism: a Deardorff, D., & Arasaratam-Smith, L.
reader in multicultural education (pp. A. (2017). Intercultural competence
62-78). Intercultural Resource. in Higher Education: International
approaches, assessment and
Bennett, J., & Bennett, M. (2004). application. Routledge.
Developing intercultural sensitivity:
an integrative approach to global Deardorff, D., Wit, H., & Heyl, J. (2012).
and domestic diversity. In D. Landis, The SAGE Handbook of international
J. Bennett, & M. Bennett (Eds.), Higher Education. Sage Publications.
Handbook of intercultural training (pp.
147-165). Sage. Engle, L., & Engle, J. (2004). Assessing
language acquisition and intercultural
Brewer, M. B. (2003). Intergroup sensitivity development in relation
Relations. open University Press. to study abroad program design.