Davison and McCarthy - 1988 - Matching Law - A Research Review
Davison and McCarthy - 1988 - Matching Law - A Research Review
Davison and McCarthy - 1988 - Matching Law - A Research Review
COGNITIVE SCIENCE
Volume 8
Publisher’s Note
The publisher has gone to great lengths to ensure the quality of this reprint but
points out that some imperfections in the original copies may be apparent.
Disclaimer
The publisher has made every effort to trace copyright holders and would welcome
correspondence from those they have been unable to trace.
THE
MATCHING
LAW
A RESEARCH REVIEW
MICHAEL DAVISON
DIANNE MCCARTHY
University of Auckland, New Zealand
Bibliography: p.
Includes index.
I. Psychometrics. 2. Matching theory I. McCarthy,
Dianne. II. Title
BF39.D 324 1987 150.72 87-15544
ISBN 0-90959-923-7
iii
iv CONTENTS
Index 273
PREFACE
Michael Davidson
Dianne McCarthy
vii
Acknowledgments
Numerous people have contributed to this book in many different ways. John A.
Nevin has been both a supportive and a critical colleague to us for several years,
and many thanks are due to him for maintaining our motivation and for critically
reading a version of this book. Particular thanks are due to Douglas Elliffe and
Brent Alsop, who critically read and edited this work throughout its preparation.
We also thank, as a group, the students and staff who have worked in our
laboratory over the last 15 years. We would particularly like to acknowledge the
stimulation of Peter E. Jenkins, who spent over a year with us as a Postdoctoral
Fellow, and who contributed greatly to our research with his quick and sure grasp
of what we were trying to achieve.
Finally, we thank John Milkins and John Tull, who expertly and lovingly
cared for our experimental subjects since 1969, and who have recently retired.
W ithout these people, and others too numerous to mention, this book could
not have been written.
ix
HISTORICAL ANTECEDENTS
1
1.1. INTRODUCTION
Beyond the collection o f uniform relationships lies the need for a formal
representation o f the data reduced to a minimal number o f terms. A theoretical
construction may yield greater generality than any assemblage o f facts. But such a
construction will not refer to another dimensional system and will not, therefore,
fall within our present definition [of theory]. It will not stand in the way o f our
search for functional relations because it will arise only after relevant variables
have been found and studied. Though it may be difficult to understand, it will not
be easily m isunderstood....
We do not seem to be ready for a theory in this sense. At the moment we make little
effective use o f empirical, let alone rational, equations. A few o f the present curves
could have been fairly closely fitted. But the most elementary preliminary research
shows that there are many relevant variables, and until their importance has been
experimentally determined, an equation which allow s for them will have so many
arbitrary constants that a good fit will be a matter o f course and cause for very little
satisfaction. (Skinner, 1950, pp. 215-216)
Skinner (1956) related how, on a Saturday, he ran short of food pellets for the
rats that he was training. To maintain the experiment, he decided to reward the
rats once every minute, rather than for each response. Over the weekend, the
behavior changed and then stabilized, showing a different but very regular
pattern. For any scientist, an orderly pattern of results immediately suggests a
fruitful vein of research, and Skinner naturally followed up his finding—
discovering the schedules of reinforcement, which were fully described and
empirically investigated by Ferster and Skinner (1957). Reinforcers for behavior
1
2 1. HISTORICAL ANTECEDENTS
LLi
KEYS:
or LEFT RIGHT
w (w)
0 8 VI X s VI Y s
^cr
h -C L
KEYS:
MAIN SWITCHING
W J
>-
LLI VI X s 1 RESP
f UJ
W
1°
8 0 VI Y s 1 RESP
— o
So:
l/l Q_
R W
VI X s
FIG. 1.1. Diagrams of the two-key concurrent-schedule procedure (upper) and the Findley, or
changeover-key, concurrent-schedule procedure (lower). The letters within the circles refer to the
colors presented on the keys. See text for further explanation.
diagrammed in the upper part of Fig. 1.1. The subject (we will assume a pigeon)
is faced with two manipulanda (response keys), which may be illuminated any
color but are shown as white in Fig. 1.1. Responding on the left key provides
reinforcers on a VI x-s schedule, and responding on the right key provides
reinforcers on a VI y-s schedule. During reinforcer delivery, both keys are
usually blacked out, and responses on them are ineffective. If the schedules are
interval schedules, they continue timing even when the subject is not responding
on that manipulandum. If they are ratio schedules, each response on one
manipulandum contributes only to the count on the associated schedule. The
1.1b. THE CHANGEOVER-KEY 5
subject is free to change over between the schedules at any time, but changeovers
may be penalized by reinforcers being unavailable immediately after the
changeover (the changeover delay, Hermstein, 1961; see Section 1.2; or the
changeover ratio, see Section 1.1b). In the two-key procedure, the changeover
delay normally starts from the first response on a manipulandum after responses
have been emitted on the other manipulandum. All responses are counted as
being part of the performance on the schedules (that is, responses that are
changeovers are not deducted from the schedules’ totals). When time allocation
is measured, the allocation to one schedule’s total normally starts with the first
response on a manipulandum and ends with the first response on the other
manipulandum.
response does not affect the availability of a reinforcer for another response.
W hen the schedule on one key sets up a reinforcer, the schedule on the other key
or keys continues timing, and may set up a reinforcer for that key or those keys
also. It is therefore possible that both keys (or all keys, if there are more than two
responses available) may have a reinforcer available at any one time. When one
of these reinforcers is collected, only the schedule associated with that reinforcer
is restarted, and the other reinforcer(s) remain available. Contrasted with this
procedure is the dependent (also termed nonindependent, interdependent, or
forced-choice) procedure. In this, when one schedule arranges a reinforcer, the
other schedule(s) stop timing until that reinforcer is collected. At this point, the
schedule from which the reinforcer was obtained is restarted, and the timing of
the intervals on the other schedule(s) continues. The dependent-scheduling
procedure ensures that the subject obtains the same distribution of reinforcers
between manipulanda as that arranged by the experimenter. Clearly, if one
response is never emitted because choice is extreme, the subject will obtain no
more reinforcers once a reinforcer is arranged for that response. The indepen-
dent-scheduling procedure does not ensure any particular distribution of rein
forcers. If choice is extreme, the subject may not take any of the nonpreferred
reinforcers. Dependent- versus independent-scheduling procedures normally
apply only to concurrent interval schedules, since a dependency on concurrent
ratio schedules will change the number of responses required for a reinforcer to
be delivered.
It is not particularly easy to capture the essence of concurrent schedules
because of the many different nuances of procedure that have been used. For
example, our description has largely focused on two-schedule concurrents, but
any number of schedules may be concurrently arranged (see Section 7.10). Most
procedural differences have not been shown to cause differential effects.
However, some, like changeover delays and changeover ratios, clearly cause
performance differences. We deal with these differences in Section 8 . 6 .
Procedures even different from those described above exist which have also been
termed “ concurrent schedules.” We stress that the name of a procedure
generally provides insufficient information for an experimental analysis. Further,
a name is certainly a very poor independent variable to manipulate in an
experiment or to use to explain an experimental result.
concurrent VI FI. In the latter, typical scalloping failed to develop on the key
associated with the FI schedule (but see Catania, 1962). Ferster and Skinner also
introduced the notion of the “ one-key concurrent schedule,” a procedure in
which two or more schedules are simultaneously arranged on a single key. They
used concurrent FI avoidance schedules. Research on one-key concurrent
schedules has not subsequently been common, but they are logically of interest.
For instance, a one-key concurrent VI 60-s VI 60-s schedule is nominally a VI
30-s schedule. But, if different nominal schedules are arranged on a single key,
the results may not be verbally classifiable. However, the feedback function (see
Section 7.1) may be mathematically specifiable.
Findley (1958) provided more parametric data on concurrent VI VI schedule
performance by keeping one schedule constant and varying the rate of reinforcers
provided by the other. He noted that the response rate on each key was both
increased by higher reinforcer rates on the correlated schedule and decreased by
higher reinforcer rates on the alternate schedule. He also reported that the time
spent responding on a schedule was similarly affected by the two reinforcer rates.
Findley’s major contribution was the specification of, at that time, a novel
method of arranging concurrent schedules— the changeover-key, or switching-
key, procedure, in which a response on a switching key changed the schedules
and associated stimuli on the main key (see Section 1.1b).
(i d
B x + B2 R x + R2
where B refers to the behavior frequency and R refers to the obtained reinforcer
frequency. The subscripts refer to the two choices (e.g., responses on the two
keys). Equation 1.1 was thus termed the matching equation, but in order to
8 1. HISTORICAL ANTECEDENTS
1
HEiRRNSTEIN ( 1 9 6 1 )
CO
LU .75 ■
CO
21
o
Cl_
CO
LU
cl:
.5 -
LU
> ♦BIRD 55
I— oBIRO 231
a=
_i
LU
25 - °BIRD 641
cl :
__ 1____ ____1_____
.25 .5 . 75 1
RELATIVE REINFORCERS
FIG. 1.2. Relative response rates emitted on two keys as a function of the relative rate of
reinforcers obtained from the two keys. The data are from Hermstein (1961).
differentiate it here from other similar equations we shall call it the strict
matching equation.
H erm stein’s (1961) results are shown in Fig. 1.2. The data from all three birds
fell close to the major diagonal which describes the equality in Equation 1.1.
Hermstein discussed the possibility that the matching relation was a result of the
rate of behavior being a linear function of the rate of reinforcers, i.e .,
B; = kRt ,
which, when applied to the behavior on each key, produces Equation 1.1.
However, data from performance on single schedules, and from Findley’s (1958)
experiment, did not support that notion. Thus, Hermstein concluded that the
relation between a rate of behavior and its rate of reinforcement was more likely
to be concave downward (or negatively accelerated). We return to this issue in
Chapter 2.
Hermstein (1961) also compared performance on concurrent VI VI schedules
using a changeover delay with that produced when no changeover delay was in
effect. He reported that the changeover delay substantially decreased the
frequency of switching between the two keys. He also noted that it might play a
role in the production of strict matching. A comparison of the matching
performance with and without a changeover delay for two birds on a concurrent
VI 135-s VI 270-s schedule (i.e., a relative reinforcer frequency of about 0.66)
showed that relative response measures were closer to 0.5 when no changeover
delay was arranged. These were somewhat limited data, but a later and more
1. 2 . INITIAL QUANTIFICATIONS 9
extensive set from Catania and Cutts (1963), using humans, validated the use of
the changeover delay in concurrent-schedule research. This procedure subse
quently became standard.
Research reported by Catania (1963a) supported Hermstein’s (1961) finding
of strict matching on concurrent VI VI schedules. Catania used a switching-key,
rather than a two-key, concurrent-schedule arrangement. He also compared
performance with, and without, a 2-s changeover delay. Like Hermstein, he
reported some extreme deviations from strict matching if no changeover delay
was in effect. Catania’s data also showed that the response rate on one key was
not a linear function of the reinforcer rate on that key. He offered a hyperbolic
equation to describe absolute response rates. In this equation, the response
rate on one key is increased by higher reinforcer rates on that key but is decreased
by higher reinforcer rates on the alternate key:
B, = ^ -0 8 3 • (1.2)
(B, + B 2)083
The fact that response rates on one key are a joint function of the reinforcer rates
obtained for those responses and for alternate responses has not subsequently
been doubted, but the form of the equation has been varied. Note particularly that
Equation 1.2 has no upper asymptote— as reinforcer rates increase, the response
rate continues to increase without bound. Both the constant k and the power
modifying the denominator are fitted constants. Neither seem to have any
particular psychological significance. Equation 1.2 describes strict matching
because when relative response rates are predicted, both the constant k and the
denominator cancel out:
Catania (1963a) was the first to propose a total output equation, that is, an
equation that describes the total rate of all measured behavior. This was:
B, + B 2 = SB = k ( R i + B2)° 17 . (1.3)
Equation 1.3 can easily be fitted to data by plotting the logarithm of the total
response rate against the logarithm of the total reinforcer rate and fitting a
straight line. The slope of the resulting line is the value of the power in Equation
1.3 (and is also the complement of the power on the denominator in Equation
1.2). It is evident from Catania’s plot of this relation that, for all subjects except
Findley’s (1958) Bird 5, the slopes of the lines were close to 0.17, and all the
data were well fitted by straight lines.
Another important aspect of Catania’s (1963a) paper was his interpretation of
10 1. HISTORICAL ANTECEDENTS
the effects of a changeover delay. He stated that this “ prevented one schedule of
reinforcement exerting accidental control over the changeover-key pecking and
the pecks maintained by the other schedule” (p. 255). This interpretation was
supported by Catania and Cutts (1963), and has remained one of the standard
interpretations of the procedural effects of changeover delays.
It has become evident from the experiments discussed so far that varying
reinforcer rates on single schedules have rather small effects on response rates
(Catania, 1963a, Equation 1.2), but the effects are much greater on concurrently
arranged schedules (Equation 1.1). In other words, concurrent VI VI schedule
performance is more sensitive to the variation of one of the reinforcer rates than
is single Vl-schedule performance. This increased sensitivity led to the
extensive use of concurrent VI VI schedules, simply because of the better
properties of the measurement. For instance, Catania (1963b) showed that
variations in reinforcer duration had little effect on the response rate on a single
VI 120-s schedule, but had a substantial effect on the response rate on one key
of a concurrent VI 120-s VI 120-s schedule. Because the total duration of food
reinforcers was constant, Catania’s (1963b) result can be taken as an example of
the strict matching of relative response rates to relative reinforcer durations:
B, Mx
(1.4)
B j + B2 Mx + M2
where M is the duration of the food presentation. Catania’s results are shown in
Fig. 1.3. The generality of the strict matching law thus, at this time, extended
across both reinforcer-rate and reinforcer-duration variations.
1
CRTRNIfl ( 1963B)
LU
I— ■75
az
C£
LU
CO
Z
o 5
d_
CO
LU
QZ
_l
LU 25
QZ •
25 .5 .75
REL. REINFORCER DURRTION
FIG. 1.3. Relative response rate as a function o f relative reinforcer duration. The data, from
Catania (1963b), have been averaged across subjects.
1.3 SUMMARY 11
1.3. SUMMARY
By 1963, the empirical fact that relative response rates closely matched relative
reinforcer rates was well substantiated and replicated. No systematic deviations
from strict matching had been reported, and the empirical matching law gained
more support from a related area— concurrent-chain schedules (see also Section
6.5 and Chapter 10). These schedules arrange concurrent VI VI schedules, but
the reinforcers are not the immediate delivery of food. Rather, completion of
each concurrent schedule produces a stimulus change that is itself associated with
a schedule of food reinforcement. These latter schedules, known as the terminal
links, or outcome phase, occur as single schedules— that is, only one terminal
link is in effect at a time, and the concurrent VI VI initial links are unavailable
during the terminal links. Autor (1960) and Hermstein (1964a) reported strict
matching between relative initial-link response rates and relative terminal-link
reinforcer rates. Reynolds (1963a) also reported strict matching on a concurrent-
chain schedule. These data sets initially seemed to support the notion of strict
matching in quite different procedures, and hence to provide increased general
ity. In the light of subsequent research (Chapter 10), however, they should not
be taken as supporting strict matching. Indeed, Hermstein (1964b) failed to find
strict matching when the terminal links were FI versus VI schedules.
extreme on the (unequal) concurrent VI 60-s VI 180-s schedules. For the (equal)
concurrent VI 90-s VI 90-s performance, these measures showed no consistent
trend. Given that strict matching is an equality between relative response and
relative reinforcer rates, it is evident from their data that strict matching was more
likely to result with longer changeover delays. When the changeover delays were
short, relative response rates were less extreme than relative obtained reinforcer
rates. H erm stein’s (1961) and Catania’s (1963a) findings were thus systematically
replicated and supported.
A caveat is in order here. As we see in Section 9.3, the design of this
experiment cannot discriminate between two different ways in which an equality
between relative response rates and relative reinforcer rates can occur. Shull and
Pliskoff’s (1967) results were taken as indicating that a changeover delay of
sufficient length must be provided if strict matching is to be obtained. As a
result, most subsequent studies rather unquestioningly used changeover-delay
values of, usually, between 2 and 5 s, despite the fact that even 5 s was not long
enough to produce an equality between relative response rates and relative
reinforcer rates in Shull and Pliskoff’s study. On the other hand, changeover
delays of 1.5 s and 2 s, respectively, were sufficient to obtain the equality in
H erm stein’s (1961) and Catania’s (1963a) studies.
Shull and Pliskoff’s (1967) study is notable for being the first to measure time
allocation on a concurrent schedule. They noted that relative response and
relative time measures were isomorphic and suggested that this showed that the
local response rates (i.e., the number of responses divided by the time allocated
to that schedule) were equal between the two keys. This further suggests that
subjects respond at a particular constant overall rate but distribute these
responses between the two keys via time allocation.
BROWNSTEIN &
PLISKOFF (1968)
o
75
I—
a=
CJ
o
_i
_i
a= .5
LU
2:
1—
♦BIRD 33
_i
LU
25
□ BIRD 787
a BIRD 6494
.25 .5 .75
REL. REINFORCER RATE
FIG. 1.4. Relative time spent responding as a function of relative reinforcer rate reported by
Brownstein and Pliskoff (1968). A different changeover-delay duration was arranged for each bird:
2 s for Bird 93; 7.5 s for Bird 787; and 5 s for Bird 6494.
1.6. SUMMARY
By 1969, two measures (relative response rate and relative time allocated) had
been shown to equal or match relative obtained reinforcer rates. These two
measures had usually been obtained from different experiments, but Catania
(1966) showed that both isomorphic measures could be obtained from standard
14 1. HISTORICAL ANTECE DENTS
Mx
B\ "1" B 2 A/] -f M 2
In the same year, Chung and Hermstein (1967) studied the effects of delay of
reinforcers on concurrent VI VI schedule performance. Reinforcers for each of
the concurrent-schedule responses were delayed by fixed times in blackout, with
the concurrent schedules unavailable during the delay. They reported that
relative response rates matched relative delays (D) to reinforcers according to the
following matching relation:
_ &2
B\ + B2 + D2
Bx _ /,
B\ A B 2 I\ + I 2
1.7. SOME OTHER INDEPENDENT VARIABLES 15
1
CHUNG &
HERRNSTEIN (1967)
c
LU
I— .75
ai
(H
LU
CO
z
o .5
Q_
CO
LU
(H
_l
LU 25
.25 .5 .75
REL. REINFORCER IMMEDIRCY
FIG. 1.5 Relative response rates as a function of relative imm ediacies of reinforcers.
These data w ere obtained using concurrent VI 60-s VI 60-s schedules. The delays
w ere varied between 1 and 30 s, but the 1-s delay was taken as effectively a 1.6-s
delay because the subjects took about this tim e to start eating from the food hopper
(see Chung & Herm stein, 1967).
Chung and Hermstein’s data are shown in Fig. 1.5 as a function of the relative
immediacy of reinforcers. The immediacy form of the equation asserts the
matching of relative response rates to the relative reinforcer rates in the delay
periods. It is thus the same result as Hermstein (1964a) obtained with
concurrent-chain schedules. Indeed, Chung and Hermstein’s procedure is a
concurrent-chain schedule.
Thus, the generality of strict matching had been asserted across reinforcer
delay and magnitude variation and even into the concurrent-chain procedure with
either fixed-delay or variable-interval terminal links. However, we must again
issue a caveat: Chung and Herm stein’s (1967) experiment has been replicated
and reanalyzed (Williams & Fantino, 1978). This further work shows that Chung
and H erm stein’s data were not consistent with strict matching (see Section 6.5).
Also, subsequent concurrent-chain results have consistently failed to find strict
matching to terminal-link relative reinforcer rates (Chapter 10).
The results summarized above, all of which supported strict matching (when
the changeover delay was of sufficient length), culminated in a paper by
Hermstein (1970). H erm stein’s approach to matching and behavior allocation is
the subject of Chapter 2.
REFERENCES
A inslie, G .W . (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis
o f B eh avior, 27, 485-489.
A inslie, G .W . (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory o f im pulsiveness and
impulse control. P sych ological B ulletin, 82, 463-496.
Akaike, H. (1969). Fitting autoregressive models for prediction. Annals o f the Institute o f
Statistical M athem atics, 21, 243-247.
Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on
Autom atic Control, 19, 716-722.
Allen, C .M . (1981). On the exponent in the “ generalized” matching equation. Journal o f
the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 3 5 , 125-127.
A lsop, B ., & Davison, M. (1986). Preference for multiple versus mixed schedules
o f reinforcement. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 45,
33-45.
Anderson, N .H . (1978). Measurement o f motivation and incentive. Behavior Research
M ethods & Instrumentation, 10, 360-375.
Autor, S.M . (1960). The strength o f conditioned reinforcers as a function o f frequency
and probability o f reinforcement. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Harvard
University, 1960. Reprinted in Hendry, D .P. (1969). Conditioned reinforcement
(pp. 127-162). H om ew ood, IL: The Dorsey Press.
Bacotti, A .V . (1977). Matching under concurrent fixed-ratio variable-interval schedules
o f food presentation. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 2 5 , 171-182.
Barron, B ., & Davison, M .C. (1972). Performance on multiple fixed-interval schedules.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 17, 375-379.
Baum, W .M . (1972). Choice in a continuous procedure. Psychonom ic Science, 28,
263-265.
Baum, W .M . (1973). Time allocation and negative reinforcement. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 20, 313-322.
255
256 REFERENCES
Baum, W .M . (1974a). On two types o f deviation from the matching law: Bias and
undermatching. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 2 , 231-242.
Baum, W .M . (1974b). Choice in free-ranging wild pigeons. Scien ce, 1 8 5 , 78-79.
Baum , W .M . (1974c). Chained concurrent schedules: Reinforcement as situation transi
tion. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 2 , 91-101.
Baum , W .M . (1976). Tim e-based and count-based measurement o f preference. Journal
o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 6 , 27-35.
Baum, W .M . (1979). M atching, undermatching and overmatching in studies o f choice.
Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 32, 269-281.
Baum, W .M . (1982). Choice, changeover, and travel. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 8 , 35-49.
Baum, W .M . (1983). M atching, statistics, and com m on sense. Journal o f the E xperi
m ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 9 , 499-501.
Baum , W .M ., & Rachlin, H .C . (1969). Choice as time allocation. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 12, 861-874.
Bauman, R .A ., Shull, R .L ., & Brownstein, A .J. (1975). Time allocation on concurrent
schedules with asymmetrical response requirements. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 4 , 53-57.
Beautrais, P .G ., & D avison, M .C . (1977). Response and time allocation in concurrent
second-order schedules. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 5 ,
61-69.
Blough, D ., & Blough, P. (1977). Animal psychophysics. In W .K . H onig & J.E .R .
Staddon (E ds.), H andbook o f O peran t B ehavior (pp. 514-539). Englew ood C liffs,
NJ: Prentice Hall.
B oelens, H ., & Kop, P .F .M . (1983). Concurrent schedules: Spatial separation o f
response alternatives. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 4 0 , 35-45.
Boldero, J., D avison, M ., & McCarthy, D. (1985). Reinforcer frequency and reinforcer
magnitude as biasers o f signal detection. Behavioural P rocesses, 10, 131-143.
Bouzas, A ., & Baum , W .M . (1976). Behavioral contrast o f time allocation. Journal o f
the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 5 , 179-184.
Bradley, J.V . (1968). D istribution-free statistical tests. Englewood C liffs, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Bradshaw, C .M ., Szabadi, E ., & Bevan, P. (1976). Behavior o f humans in variable-
interval schedules o f reinforcement. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f
B eh avior, 2 6 , 135-141.
Bradshaw, C .M ., Szabadi, E ., & Bevan, P. (1977). Effect o f punishment on human
variable-interval performance. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 27,
275-279.
Bradshaw, C .M ., Szabadi, E ., & Bevan, P. (1978). Effects o f variable-interval
punishment on the behavior o f humans in variable-interval schedules o f monetary
reinforcement. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 2 9 , 161-166.
Bradshaw, C .M ., Szabadi, E ., & Bevan, P. (1979). The effect o f punishment on
free-operant choice behavior in humans. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f
B ehavior, 31, 71-81.
Brethower, D .M ., & Reynolds, G .S . (1962). A facilitative effect o f punishment on
unpunished behavior. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 5 , 191-199.
257
C ohen, I.L. (1973). A note on H erm stein’s equation. Journal o f the E xperim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 19, 527-528.
Cohen, S .L . (1975). Concurrent second-order schedules o f reinforcement. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 4 , 333-341.
C om m ons, M .L ., Herrnstein, R .J., & Rachlin, H. (1982) (E ds.). Q uantitative A nalyses
o f B ehavior, Vol. 11: M atching and M axim izing Accounts. Cambridge, MA:
Ballinger.
C oom bs, C .H . (1964). A theory o f data. NY: W iley.
D avison, M .C . (1968). Reinforcement rate and im m ediacy o f reinforcement as factors in
choice. P sychonom ic Science, 10, 181-182.
D avison, M .C . (1969). Preference for mixed-interval versus fixed-interval schedules.
Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 12, 247-252.
D avison, M . (1972). Preference for mixed-interval versus fixed-interval schedules:
Number o f com ponent intervals. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior,
17, 169-176.
D avison, M. (1974). A functional analysis o f chained fixed-interval schedule perfor
mance. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 1 , 323-330.
D avison, M .C . (1976). Preference for fixed-interval schedules: Effects o f unequal
initial-link length. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 5 , 371-376.
D avison, M , (1982). Performance in concurrent variable-interval fixed-ratio schedules.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 7 , 81-96.
D avison, M. (1983). Bias and sensitivity to reinforcement in a concurrent-chain schedule.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 4 0 , 15-34.
D avison, M . (1987). The analysis o f concurrent-chain performance. In M .L. Com m ons,
J.E. Mazur, J.A . N evin, & H. Rachlin (E ds.), Q uantitative analyses o f behavior,
Vol. V: Effects o f delay an d o f intervening events on reinforcem ent value (pp.
225-244). H illsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
D avison, M ., & Ferguson, A . (1978). The effects o f different com ponent response
requirements in multiple and concurrent schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 9 , 283-295.
D avison, M ., & H ogsden, I. (1984). Concurrent variable-interval schedule performance:
Fixed versus m ixed reinforcer durations. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
B eh avior, 4 1 , 169-182.
D avison, M .C ., & Hunter, I.W . (1976). Performance on variable-interval schedules
arranged singly and concurrently. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior,
2 5 , 335-345.
D avison, M .C ., & Hunter, I.W . (1979). Concurrent schedules: Undermatching and
control by previous experimental conditions. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
B ehavior, 3 2 , 233-244.
D avison, M ., & Jenkins, P.E . (1985). Stimulus discriminability, contingency discrim in
ability, and schedule performance. Anim al Learning and B ehavior, 13, 77-84.
D avison, M ., & McCarthy, D . (1980). Reinforcement for errors in a signal-detection
procedure. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 4 , 35-47.
D avison, M ., & McCarthy, D . (1981). Undermatching and structural relations.
B ehaviou r A nalysis L etters, 1, 67-72.
D avison, M ., McCarthy, D ., & Jensen, C. (1985). Component probability and com po
259
nent reinforcer rate as biasers o f free-operant detection. Journal o f the Experim ental
Analysis o f B ehavior, 4 4 , 103-120.
D avison, M ., & Smith, C. (1986). Som e aspects o f preference between immediate and
delayed periods o f reinforcement. Journal o f Experim ental P sychology: Anim al
B ehavior P rocesses, 12, 291-300.
Davison, M .C ., & Tem ple, W. (1973). Preference for fixed-interval schedules: An
alternative model. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 20, 393-403.
Davison, M .C ., & Tem ple, W. (1974). Preference for fixed-interval terminal links in a
three-key concurrent-chain schedule. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
B ehavior, 22, 11-19.
D avison, M .C ., & Tustin, R .D . (1978). The relation between the generalized matching
law and signal-detection theory. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior,
2 9, 331-336.
Deluty, M .Z. (1976). Choice and the rate o f punishment in concurrent schedules. Journal
o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 25, 75-80.
D eluty, M .Z ., & Church, R .M . (1978). Time-allocation matching between punishing
situations. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 29, 191-198.
de Villiers, P .A . (1972). Reinforcement and response-rate interaction in multiple
random-interval avoidance schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
Behavior, 18, 499-507.
de Villiers, P .A . (1974). The law o f effect and avoidance: A quantitative relationship
between response rate and shock-frequency reduction. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f Behavior, 21, 223-235.
de Villiers, P. A. (1977). Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation o f
the law o f effect. In W .K . Honig & J.E.R . Staddon (E ds.), H andbook o f operant
behavior (pp. 233-287). Englewood C liffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
de Villiers, P .A . (1980). Toward a quantitative theory o f punishment. Journal o f the
Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 33, 15-25.
de Villiers, P. A. (1982). Toward a quantitative theory o f punishment. In M .L. Com mons,
R.J. Hermstein, & H. Rachlin (E ds.), Q uantitative analyses o f behavior. Vol. II:
M atching and m aximizing accou nts^pp. 327-344). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger,
de Villiers, P .A ., & Hermstein, R.J. (1976). Toward a law o f response strength.
P sych ological Bulletin, 83, 1131-1153.
D ow d, J.E ., & R iggs, D .S . (1965). A comparison o f estimates o f M ichaelis-M enten
kinetic constants from various linear transformations. The Journal o f B iological
C hem istry, 2 4 0 , 863-869.
Dreyfus, L .R ., Dorman, L .G ., Fetterman, J.G ., & Stubbs, D .A . (1982). An invariant
relation between changing over and reinforcement. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 8 , 327-338.
Duncan, B ., & Fantino, E. (1970). Choice for periodic schedules o f reinforcement.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 14, 73-86.
Duncan, B ., & Fantino, E. (1972). The Psychological distance to reward. Journal o f the
E xperim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 18, 23-34.
Dusoir, T. (1983). Isobias curves in som e detection tasks. Perception and Psychophysics,
3 3, 403-412.
Edmon, E.L. (1978). M ultiple-schedule component duration: A reanalysis o f Shimp and
260 REFERENCES
Wheatley (1971) and Todorov (1972). Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f
B eh avior, 3 0 , 239-241.
Elliffe, D ., & D avison, M. (1985). Performance in continuously available multiple
schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior 4 4 , 343-353.
Elsm ore, T .F. (1972). Duration discrimination: Effects o f probability o f stimulus
presentation. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 18, 465-469.
Epstein, R. (1981). Amount consum ed as a function o f m agazine-cycle duration.
B ehaviour A nalysis L etters, 1, 63-66.
Estes, W .K . (1969). Outline o f a theory o f punishment. In B. Campbell & R .M . Church
(E ds.), Punishm ent and aversive behavior, (pp. 57-82). Englewood C liffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Fantino, E. (1967). Preference for m ixed- versus fixed-ratio schedules. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 10, 35-43.
Fantino, E. (1968). Effects o f required rates o f responding upon choice. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 11, 15-22.
Fantino, E. (1969). Choice and rate o f reinforcement. Journal o f the E xperim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 12, 723-730.
Fantino, E ., & D avison, M. (1983). Choice: Som e quantitative relations. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 4 0 , 1-13.
Fantino, E ., & Dunn, R. (1983). The delay-reduction hypothesis: Extension to three-
alternative choice. Journal o f E xperim ental P sych ology: Anim al B ehavior P ro
cesses, 9, 132-146.
Fantino, E ., & M oore, J. (1980). Uncertainty reduction, conditioned reinforcement, and
observing. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 3 , 3-13.
Fantino, E ., & Navarick, D . (1974). Recent developm ents in choice. In G .H . Bower
(E d.), The psych ology o f learning and m otivation (V ol. 8) (pp. 147-185). NY:
Academ ic Press.
Fantino, E ., Squires, N ., Delbriick, N ., & Peterson, C. (1972). Choice behavior and the
accessibility o f the reinforcer. Journal o f the E xperim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 18,
35-43.
Farley, J. (1980). Reinforcement and punishment effects in concurrent schedules: A test
o f two m odels. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 3 , 311-326.
Farley, J., & Fantino, E. (1978). The symmetrical law o f effect and the matching relation
in choice behavior. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 9 , 37-60.
Felton, M ., & Lyon, D .O . (1966). The post-reinforcement pause. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 9, 131 -134.
Ferguson, G .A . (1965). N onparam etric trend analysis. Montreal: M cG ill University
Press.
Ferster, C .B ., & Skinner, B .F . (1957). Schedules o f reinforcem ent. NY: Appleton-
Century-Crofts.
Findley, J.D . (1958). Preference and sw itching under concurrent scheduling. Journal o f
the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 1, 123-144.
Galanter, E ., & H olm an, G .L . (1967). Som e invariances o f the isosensitivity function and
their im plications for the utility function o f money. Journal o f Experim ental
P sych ology, 73, 333-339.
261
Gentry, G .D ., & Marr, M.J. (1980). Choice and reinforcement delay. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 33, 27-37.
Green, D .M . & M cG ill, W.J. (1970). On the equivalence o f detection probabilities and
well-known statistical quantities. P sych ological R eview , 77, 294-301.
Green, D .M . & Sw ets, J.A . (1974). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. NY:
W iley.
Green, L ., & Snyderman, M. (1980). Choice between rewards differing in amount and
delay: Toward a choice model o f self control. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis
o f B ehavior, 34, 135-147.
Grier, J.B. (1971). Nonparametric indices for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas.
P sych ological Bulletin, 75, 424-429.
Hack, M .H . (1963). Signal detection in the rat. Science, 139, 758-759.
Hardy, G .A ., Littlewood, J.E ., & Polya, G. (1959). Inequalities. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Harnett, P ., McCarthy, D ., & Davison, M. (1984). Delayed signal detection, differential
reinforcement, and short-term memory in the pigeon. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 42, 87-111.
Hayes, S .C ., Kapust, J., Leonard, S .R ., & Rosenfarb, I. (1981). Escape from freedom:
Choosing not to choose in pigeons. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f
Behavior, 36, 1-7.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1958). Som e factors influencing behavior in a two-response situation.
T ransactions o f the N ew York Academ y o f Sciences, 2 1 , 35-45.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength o f response as a function o f
frequency o f reinforcement. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 4,
267-272.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1964a). Secondary reinforcement and rate o f primary reinforcement.
Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 7, 27-36.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1964b). Aperiodicity as a factor in choice. Journal o f the Experim ental
Analysis o f B ehavior, 7, 179-182.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1966). Superstition: A corollary o f the principles o f operant condition
ing. In W .K . Honig (E d.), O perant behavior: A reas o f research and application (pp.
33-51). NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1970). On the law o f effect. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f
B ehavior, 13, 243-266.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1974). Formal properties o f the matching law. Journal o f the
Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 21, 159-164.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1979). Derivatives o f matching. P sych ological R eview , 86, 486-495.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1982). Melioration as behavioral dynamism. In M .L. Com mons, R.J.
Herrnstein, & H. Rachlin (E ds.), Quantitative analyses o f beh avior, Vol. II:
M atching and maximizing accounts (pp. 433-458). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Herrnstein, R .J., & Heyman, G .M . (1979). Is matching compatible with reinforcement
maximization on concurrent variable interval, variable ratio? Journal o f the E xper
im ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 3 1 , 209-223.
Herrnstein, R .J., & Loveland, D .H . (1974). Hunger and contrast in multiple schedules.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 21, 511-517.
262 REFERENCES
Herm stein, R .J., & Loveland, D .H . (1975). M axim izing and matching on concurrent
ratio schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B eh avior, 2 4 , 107-116.
H erm stein, R .J., & Vaughan, W ., Jr. (1980). M elioration and behavioral allocation. In
J.E .R . Staddon (E d.), L im its to action. NY: Academ ic Press.
H ey man, G .M . (1979). A Markov model description o f changeover probabilities on
concurrent variable-interval schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
B ehavior, 3 1 , 41-51.
H ineline, P .N . (1970). N egative reinforcement without shock reduction. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 14, 259-268.
H inson, J.M ., & Staddon, J.E .R . (1978). Behavioral competition: A m echanism for
schedule interactions. Science, 2 0 2 , 432-434.
H obson, S.L . (1975). Discriminability o f fixed-ratio schedules for pigeons: Effects
o f absolute ratio size. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 3 ,
25-35.
H obson, S.L . (1978). Discrim inability o f fixed-ratio schedules for pigeons: Effects o f
payoff value. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 0 , 69-81.
H odos, W ., & Bonbright, J.C ., Jr. (1972). The detection o f visual intensity differences
by pigeons. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 18, 471-479.
H ollander, M ., & W olfe, D .A . (1973). N onparam etric statistical m ethods. NY: W iley.
Hollard, V ., & D avison, M .C . (1971). Preference for qualitatively different reinforcers.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 16, 375-380.
Hollard, V ., & D avison, M .C . (1978). H istological data: Hollard and Davison (1971).
Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 9 , 149.
H olz, W .C . (1968). Punishment and rate o f positive reinforcement. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 11, 285-292.
Hum e, A .L . (1974a). Auditory detection and optimal response biases. P erception and
P sych ophysics, 15, 425-433.
Hum e, A .L . (1974b). Optimal response biases and the slope o f ROC curves as a function
o f signal intensity, signal probability, and relative payoff. P erception and
P sych ophysics, 16, 377-384.
Hum e, A .L ., & Irwin, R.J. (1974). Bias functions and operating characteristics o f rats
discriminating auditory stimuli. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior,
2 1 , 285-295.
Hunter, I.W ., & D avison, M .C . (1978). R esponse rate and changeover performance on
concurrent variable-interval schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
B eh avior, 2 9 , 535-556.
Hunter, I ., & D avison, M. (1982). Independence o f response force and reinforcement rate
on concurrent variable-interval schedule performance. Journal o f the E xperim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 7 , 183-197.
Hunter, 1.1., & D avison, M. (1985). Determination o f a behavioral transfer function:
W hite noise analysis o f session-to-session response-ratio dynamics on concurrent VI
VI schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior 4 3 , 43-59.
Hursh, S.R . (1980). Econom ic concepts for the analysis o f behavior. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 4 , 219-238.
Hursh, S .R ., & Fantino, E. (1973). Relative delay o f reinforcement and choice. Journal
o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 19, 437-450.
263
M cS w eeney, F.K . (1980). D ifferences between rates o f responding emitted during sim ple
and multiple schedules. A nim al Learning and B eh avior, 8 , 392-400.
M cS w eeney, F.K . (1982). Positive and negative contrast as a function o f com ponent
duration for key-pecking and treadle pressing. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis
o f B ehavior, 3 7 , 281-293.
M cS w eeney, F.K . (1983). Positive behavioral contrast when pigeons press treadles
during multiple schedules. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 39,
149-156.
M cS w eeney, F .K ., & D eR icco, D .A . (1976). Rates o f responding in the pigeon
generated by simple and com plex schedules which provide the same rate o f
reinforcement. A nim al Learning and B eh avior, 4 , 379-385.
M cS w eeney, F .K ., Farmer, V .A ., D ougan, J .D ., & W hipple, J.E. (1986). The
generalized matching law as a description o f m ultiple-schedule responding. Journal
o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 4 5 , 83-101.
M cS w eeney, F .K ., M elville, C .L ., Buck, M .A . & W hipple, J.E. (1983). Local rates o f
responding and reinforcement during concurrent schedules. Journal o f the E xperi
m ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 4 0 , 79-98.
M enlove, R.L. (1975). Local patterns o f responding maintained by concurrent and
multiple schedules. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 3 , 309-337.
M erigan, W .H ., M iller, J .S ., & G ollub, L.R . (1975). Short-component multiple
schedules: Effects o f relative reinforcement duration. Journal o f the E xperim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 4 , 183-189.
M iller, H .L ., Jr. (1976). M atching-based hedonic scaling in the pigeon. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 6 , 335-347.
M iller, H .L ., Jr. (1984). On the further maturing o f behavior analysis: A review o f
C om m ons, Hermstein and Rachlin’s “ Matching and M axim izing A ccou n ts.”
Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 4 2 , 159-164.
M iller, H .L ., & Loveland, D .H . (1974). Matching when the number o f response
alternatives is large. A nim al Learning and B ehavior, 2 , 106-110.
M iller, J.T ., Saunders, S .S ., & Bourland, G. (1980). The role o f stimulus disparity in
concurrently available reinforcement schedules. Anim al Learning and B ehavior, 8 ,
635-641.
M itchell, P ., & W hite, K .G . (1977). Responding in the presence o f free food: Differential
exposure to the reinforcement source. Bulletin o f the P sychonom ic S ociety, 10,
121-124.
M oore, J. (1979). Choice and number o f reinforcers. Journal o f the E xperim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 2 , 51-63.
M oore, J., & Fantino, E. (1975). Choice and response contingencies. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 3 , 339-347.
M ullins, E ., Agunwamba, C .C ., & Donahoe, A.J. (1982). On the analysis o f studies o f
choice. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 7 , 323-327.
M yers, D .L ., & M yers, L.E. (1977). Undermatching: A reappraisal o f performance on
concurrent variable-interval schedules o f reinforcement. Journal o f the E xperim ental
A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 7 , 203-214.
Navarick, D .J ., & Fantino, E. (1972). Transitivity as a property o f choice. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 18, 389-401.
267
Navarick, D .J., & Fantino, E. (1974). Stochastic transitivity and unidimensional behavior
theories. P sych ological R e view , 81, 426-441.
Navarick, D .J ., & Fantino, E. (1975). Stochastic transitivity and the unidimensional
control o f choice. Learning and M otivation , 6 , 179-201.
Navarick, D .J ., & Fantino, E. (1976). S elf control and general m odels o f choice. Journal
o f Experim ental P sychology: Anim al B ehavior P ro c esses, 2 , 75-87.
Neuringer, A.J. (1967). Effects o f reinforcement magnitude on choice and rate o f
responding. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 10, 417-424.
Neuringer, A.J. (1969). D elayed reinforcement versus reinforcement after a fixed
interval. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 12, 375-383.
N evin, J.A . (1969). Signal-detection theory and operant behavior: A review o f David M.
Green and John A. S w ets’ “ Signal-detection theory and psychophysics.’’ Journal o f
the Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 12, 475-480.
N evin, J.A . (1971). Rates and patterns o f responding with concurrent fixed-interval and
variable-interval reinforcement. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior,
16, 241-247.
N evin, J.A . (1979). Reinforcement schedules and response strength. In M .D . Zeiler & P.
Harzem (E ds.), A dvances in analysis o f behavior: Vol. 1, Reinforcem ent and the
organisation o f behavior (pp. 117-158). NY: W iley.
Nevin, J.A . (1981). Psychophysics and reinforcement schedules. In M .L. Commons &
J.A. Nevin (E ds.), Q uantitative analyses o f behavior, Vol. I: D iscrim inative
pro p erties o f reinforcem ent schedules (pp. 3-27). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
N evin, J .A ., & Baum, W .M . (1980). Feedback functions for variable-interval reinforce
ment. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 3 4 , 207-217.
N evin, J .A ., Jenkins, P ., Whittaker, S ., & Yarensky, P. (1977). Signal detection and
matching. Paper presented at the November meeting o f the Psychonom ic Society,
W ashington, D .C .
N evin, J .A ., Jenkins, P ., Whittaker, S ., & Yarensky, P. (1982). Reinforcement
contingencies and signal detection. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehav
ior, 37, 65-79.
N evin, J .A ., & M acW illiam s, S. (1983). Ratio reinforcement o f signal detection.
Behaviour A nalysis L etters, 3 , 317-324.
N evin, J .A ., O lson, K ., M andell, C ., & Yarensky, P. (1975). Differential reinforcement
and signal detection. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 2 4 , 355-367.
Pliskoff, S .S . (1971). Effects o f symmetrical and asymmetrical changeover delays on
concurrent performances. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 16,
249-256.
Pliskoff, S .S ., & Brown, T.G . (1976). Matching with a trio o f concurrent variable-
interval schedules o f reinforcement. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f
Behavior, 25, 69-73.
Pliskoff, S .S ., Cicerone, R ., & N elson, T .D . (1978). Local response-rate constancy on
concurrent variable-interval schedules o f reinforcement. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f Behavior, 29, 431-446.
Pliskoff, S .S ., Shull, R .L ., & G ollub, L.R. (1968). The relation between response rates
and reinforcer rates in multiple schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
Behavior, 11, 271-284.
268 REFERENCES
Schulman, A .I., & Greenberg, G .Z. (1970). Operating characteristics and a priori
probability o f the signal. P erception and P sych ophysics, 8, 317-320.
Scow n, J.M . (1983). Changeover delay and concurrent schedules. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. Waikato University, New Zealand.
Shettleworth, S ., & N evin, J.A. (1965). Relative rate o f response and relative magnitude
o f reinforcement in multiple schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f
B eh avior, 8, 199-202.
Shimp, C .P. (1968). Magnitude and frequency o f reinforcement and frequencies of
interresponse times. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 1 1 , 525-535.
Shimp, C.P. (1969a). Optimum behavior in free-operant experiments. P sychological
R eview , 76, 97-112.
Shimp, C.P. (1969b). The concurrent reinforcement o f two interresponse times: The
relative frequency o f an interresponse time equals its relative harmonic length.
Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 12, 403-411.
Shimp, C.P. (1971). Matching in a concurrent FI FI schedule. Psychonom ic Science, 22,
27-28.
Shimp, C .P ., & W heatley, K .L. (1971). Matching to relative reinforcement frequency in
multiple schedules with a short component duration. Journal o f the Experim ental
A nalysis o f Behavior, 15, 205-210.
Shull, R .L ., & Pliskoff, S .S . (1967). Changeover delay and concurrent schedules: Some
effects on relative performance measures. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f
B ehavior, 10, 517-527.
Siegel, S. (1956). N onparam etric statistics fo r the behavioral sciences. NY: McGraw-
Hill.
Silberberg, A ., & Fantino, E. (1970). C hoice, rate o f reinforcement, and the changeover
delay. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 13, 187-197.
Silberberg, A ., & Schrot, J. (1974). A yoked-chamber comparison o f concurrent and
multiple schedules: The relation between component duration and responding.
Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 22, 21-30.
Skinner, B.F. (1950). Are theories o f learning necessary? P sych ological R eview , 57,
193-216.
Skinner, B .F. (1956). A case history in scientific method. Am erican P sych ologist, 11,
221-233.
Snyderman, M. (1983). Delay and amount o f reward in a concurrent chain. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 39, 437-447.
Snyderman, M. (1984). Body weight and response strength. Behaviour A nalysis L etters,
3, 255-265.
Spealman, R .D ., & G ollub, L.R. (1974). Behavioral interactions in multiple variable-
interval schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 22, 471-481.
Squires, N ., & Fantino, E. (1971). A model for choice in simple concurrent and
concurrent-chains schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 15,
27-38.
Staddon, J.E.R . (1968). Spaced responding and choice: A preliminary analysis. Journal
o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 11, 669-682.
Staddon, J.E.R . (1977). On H erm stein’s equation and related forms. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 28, 163-170.
270 REFERENCES
Staddon, J.E .R ., & Motheral, S. (1978). On matching and m axim izing in operant choice.
P sych ological R e view , 85, 436-444.
Stubbs, D .A . (1968). The discrimination o f stimulus duration by pigeons. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 11, 223-238.
Stubbs, D .A . (1976). Response bias and the discrimination o f stimulus duration. Journal
o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 25, 243-250.
Stubbs, D .A . (1980). Temporal discrimination and a free-operant psychophysical
procedure. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 3 3 , 167-185.
Stubbs, D .A ., & Pliskoff, S .S . (1969). Concurrent responding with fixed relative rate o f
reinforcement. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 12, 887-895.
Stubbs, D .A ., P liskoff, S .S ., & Reid, H .M . (1977). Concurrent schedules: A quantitative
relation between changeover behavior and its consequences. Journal o f the E xper
im ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 2 7 , 85-96.
Sw ets, J .A ., Tanner, W .P ., Jr., & Birdsall, T .G . (1961). D ecision processes in
perception. P sych ological R eview , 68, 301-340.
Tanner, W .P ., Jr., & Birdsall, T .G . (1958). Definitions o f d ’ and n as psychophysical
measures. Journal o f the A coustical Society o f A m erica, 3 0 , 922-928.
Tanner, W .P ., Jr., & Sw ets, J.A . (1954). A decision-m aking theory o f visual detection.
P sych ological R eview , 61, 401-409.
Taylor, R ., & D avison, M. (1983). Sensitivity to reinforcement in concurrent arithmetic
and exponential schedules. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 3 9 ,
191-198.
Terman, M. (1970). Discrimination o f auditory intensities by rats. Journal o f the
E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 13, 145-160.
Terman, M. (1981). Behavioral dynam ics o f the psychometric function. In M .L .
Comm ons & J.A . N evin (E ds.), Q uantitative analyses o f b eh avior, Vol. I:
D iscrim in ative p ro p erties o f reinforcem ent schedules (pp. 321-344). Cambridge,
MA: Ballinger.
Terman, M ., & Terman, J.S. (1972). Concurrent variation o f response bias and
sensitivity in an operant-psychophysical test. P erception and P sych oph ysics, 11,
428-432.
Terman, M ., & Terman, J.S. (1973). Latency differentiation o f hits and false alarms in
an operant-psychophysical test. Journal o f the E xperim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior,
2 0 , 439-445.
Thijssen, J.M ., & Vendrick, A .J.H . (1968). Internal noise and transducer function in
sensory detection experiments: Evaluation o f psychometric curves and o f ROC
curves. P erception and P sych ophysics, 3 , 387-400.
Thorndike, E.L. (1913). E ducational psych ology, Vol. 11: The p sych ology o f learning.
NY: Teachers C ollege, Columbia University.
Timberlake, W . (1982). The emperor’s clothes: Assumptions o f the matching theory. In
M .L . C om m ons, R.J. Herrnstein & H. Rachlin (E ds.), Q uantitative analyses o f
behavior, Vol. II: M atching and m axim izing accounts (pp. 549-569). Cambridge,
MA: Ballinger.
Todorov, J.C. (1971). Concurrent performances: Effect o f punishment contingent on the
switching response. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 16, 51-62.
271
Todorov, J.C. (1972). Component duration and relative response rates in multiple
schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 17, 45-49.
Todorov, J.C. (1973). Interaction o f frequency and magnitude o f reinforcement on
concurrent performances. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B eh avior, 19,
451-458.
Todorov, J.C ., Castro, J.M . de O ., Hanna, E .S ., Bittencourt de Sa, M .C .N ., & Barreto,
M. de Q. (1983). C hoice, experience, and the generalized matching law. Journal o f
the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 4 0 , 99-111.
Treisman, M. (1977). On the stability o f ds. P sych ological Bulletin, 84, 235-243.
Trevett, A .J., Davison, M .C ., & W illiam s, R.J. (1972). Performance in concurrent
interval schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 17, 369-374.
Tustin, R .D ., & Davison, M .C. (1978). Distribution o f response ratios in concurrent
variable-interval performance. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 29,
561-564.
Tustin, R .D ., & Davison, M. (1979). Choice: Effects o f changeover schedules on
concurrent performance. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 32,
75-91.
Vaughan, W ., Jr. (1981). M elioration, matching, and maximization. Journal o f the
Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 3 6 , 141-149.
Vaughan, W ., Jr., & M iller, H .L ., Jr. (1984). Optimization versus response-strength
accounts o f behavior. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f B ehavior, 42,
337-348.
Wardlaw, G .R ., & D avison, M .C . (1974). Preference for fixed-interval schedules:
Effects o f initial-link length. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 2 1 ,
331-340.
Wearden, J.H. (1980). Undermatching on concurrent variable-interval schedules and the
power law. Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 3 3 , 149-152.
Wearden, J.H. (1981). Bias and undermatching: Implications for Herm stein’s equation.
Behaviour A nalysis L etters, 1, 177-185.
Wearden, J.H ., & Burgess, I.S. (1982). Matching since Baum (1979). Journal o f the
Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 38, 339-348.
W etherington, C .L ., & Lucas, T.R . (1980). A note on fitting H erm stein’s equation.
Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f Behavior, 34, 199-206.
W hite, A .J., & Davison, M .C. (1973). Performance in concurrent fixed-interval
schedules. Journal o f the Experim ental Analysis o f B ehavior, 19, 147-153.
W hite, K .G ., & M itchell, P. (1977). Preference for response-contingent versus free
reinforcement. Bulletin o f the Psychonom ic Society, 10, 125-127.
W hite, K .G ., Pipe, M .-E ., & M cLean, A. (1984). Stimulus and reinforcer relativity in
multiple schedules: Local and dimensional effects on sensitivity to reinforcement.
Journal o f the Experim ental A nalysis o f Behavior, 4 1 , 69-81.
W ickelgren, W .A . (1968). Unidimensional strength theory and component analysis o f
noise in absolute and comparative judgements. Journal o f M athem atical P sychology,
5 , 102 - 122 .
W ilkinson, G .N . (1961). Statistical estimation in enzym e kinetics. Biochem ical Journal,
80, 324-332.
272 REFERENCES