2010 AlibaliNathan JCD Tools
2010 AlibaliNathan JCD Tools
2010 AlibaliNathan JCD Tools
rl
N
m
"".. Cognitive development unfolds in many contexts, and one of the most
r--
rl
397
398 ALIBALI AND NATHAN
'"
(])
The demographics of samples obtained in schools often differ substantially
ry,
(])
rl
from those obtained via other methods of recruitment. It is often possible to
rl
U
o obtain more diverse samples, or samples more representative of a region,
c:
o when testing participants in schools rather than in on-campus laboratories.
+J
[JJ
o
co
Of course, the diversity and representativeness of any sample depends on
the particular schools chosen as research sites. Further, the sample of students
who participate often does not accurately represent the schools' populations,
because students from different subgroups are not equally likely to provide
informed consent.
In this era of increasing accountability in education, it can be challenging
to obtain permission to conduct research that involves pulling children out of
class. Under current federal laws, there are greater pressures regarding stan-
dardized test performance and consequences for schools that do not show
adequate yearly progress. As a result, many schools do not allow students
to be taken out of class for research purposes during instructional time.
Researchers who desire to work in schools solely to obtain a large or diverse
sample may wish to consider other ways to obtain their samples. We return to
this issue below.
m
memory tasks at the end of first grade and made greater improvements
""..
r--
during the first-grade year.
~
..
+J
'"
(])
Schools as a Setting for Experiments
ry,
(])
c-j
c-j
o Schools also provide an appropriate setting for certain types of experiments.
u
c:
o
Indeed, for some research questions (e.g., questions about how curricula affect
+J
[JJ
o
learning), experimentation in school settings is the optimal methodological
co
approach. Experiments conducted in schools include not only traditional, con-
trolled experiments but also "design experiments" (Brown, 1992), which involve
theoretically based interventions implemented in natural settings (such as class-
rooms), using an engineering-style approach of design-evaluate-redesign, typi-
cally without control or comparison groups. For both traditional and design
experiments, teachers are essential collaborators in the research process.
As one example of a design experiment, Lehrer, Strom, and Confrey (2002)
examined the feasibility of teaching mathematical similarity to third-grade
students. Two polygons are similar if their corresponding angles are equal
and their corresponding sides are proportional. In instructing students about
similarity, Lehrer et al. used a carefully designed set of tasks and mathemat-
ical notations, in a classroom that used frequent whole-group discussion, and
a mode of discourse that focused on conjecture and evidence. In this environ-
ment, third-grade students came to understand multiple senses of mathemat-
ical similarity. This design experiment showed that it is possible to successfully
introduce the idea of mathematical similarity at the elementary level.
As an example of a classroom experiment with a control group, Connor,
Morrison, Fishman, Schatschneider, and Underwood (2007) tested whether
400 ALIBALI AND NATHAN
m
questions about instruction and learning but seek to contribute to other litera-
""..
r--
tures as well. For example, Church, Ayman-Nolley, and Mahootian (2004)
rl
'" children, using video lessons presented in classroom settings. They found that
(])
ry,
(])
gesture contributed significantly to learning for both groups of children.
rl
rl
o
U
c:
o
+J
The Research Cycle
[JJ
o
co
Classroom research is sometimes part of a cycle that proceeds from labora-
tory to classroom and back again. As one example, in a laboratory-based
training study, Chen and Klahr (1999) developed a procedure for instructing
elementary-school children about the control-of-variables strategy, a funda-
mental aspect of scientific reasoning. Based on this laboratory research, Toth,
Klahr, and Chen (2000) designed a lesson plan and tested it in classrooms.
The results included some surprises that suggested new directions for further
lab-based and classroom research. Thus, basic research on cognition and
development can inform classroom practices, and research on classroom
practices and their effectiveness can guide basic research and theory.
You may wish to spend some time reading recent press about current issues
in local districts or volunteering in local schools.
How does one get started actually conducting research in schools? The
first step is to ascertain whether a review process is required by the school
district you wish to work in. Some districts have their own institutional
review board (IRB) or external research oversight committee. In the school
district where we conduct much of our research, this committee meets on a
monthly basis to review proposals for research. The committee requires
proof of IRB approval from our institution before they will consider our
proposals, and we provide our university IRB with information about the
research sites after we have obtained approval from the district. However,
it is worth noting that some college or university IRBs require approval
from schools before they will consider a research proposal.
The school district external research committee often suggests changes or
revisions to our research plan or consent documents, and these then require
re-review by the university IRB. In some cases, obtaining final approval
rl
N
m
takes several rounds of revisions that need to be cleared with both the uni-
""..
r--
versity and the school district, and the process can take 2 months or more.
rl
.. Once approval has been granted, the district assigns the project a "liaison,"
+J
'" who facilitates contacts with principals and teachers who must also agree to
(])
ry,
(])
be involved in the research.
rl
rl
o Other districts have more streamlined processes for approving research
U
c:
o
to be conducted in schools. At one district where we worked in the past,
+J
[JJ
o
a single individual reviewed and approved all research requests, and this
co
approval was typically granted based on a brief telephone conversation
about the proposed research. In other districts, there is no formal review
process at all. In such cases, principals make decisions about whether to
allow the research to be conducted in their schools.
Note that many districts require that researchers complete a criminal
background check before they commence work in schools. This should be
completed before contacting schools.
m
that certain students do not miss certain lessons?). Meeting with teachers
""..
r--
also provides an opportunity for them to offer input or ideas regarding your
rl
lOne exception is when a district liaison facilitates direct contact between researchers and
teachers.
CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SCHOOLS 403
research team. In such cases, teachers are engaged in the research and must
also complete appropriate IRB training. Because such studies take place in
school, one must first obtain approval from the district and from school
principals as necessary, and may then request the opportunity to solicit
teachers' involvement. If the district assigns projects "liaisons," these indivi-
duals may facilitate contact with teachers as well.
In some cases, teachers and/or teacher practices are the focus of the
research. Being the subject of study can be enriching for teachers, but it
can also be uncomfortable and challenging (French & Nathan, 2004). If
your research addresses teacher practices, it is important to be mindful of
teachers' experiences as objects of scrutiny and to be caring and respectful
of teachers' feelings and approaches.
m
step is to request consent from students. In many cases, recruitment letters
""..
r--
and parent consent forms are sent home from school with students, and
rl
.. signed consent forms are collected by teachers when students return them .
+J
'" In other cases, it is desirable to send letters home in the mail. For privacy
(])
ry,
(])
reasons, researchers are typically not allowed to see students' names and
rl
rl
o addresses, so school staff must be involved in this process. If this is the case,
U
c:
o
it is important to provide all the needed materials (e.g., envelopes, labels,
+J
[JJ
o
postage) so that the research does not incur expenses for the school. It
co
may be appropriate to offer something in return for the staff time needed
to label envelopes. Researchers can offer volunteer time to offset the time
they require from school staff.
If schools serve a population that includes many families where English is
not spoken in the home, researchers should provide informational letters and
consent forms in the other languages commonly used in the area. Such mate-
rials insure that the opportunity to participate in research is available to all
students. If you do provide materials in other languages, you should also
provide the phone number of a contact person who can speak with parents
in their preferred language about questions or concerns about the research.
Sometimes the research plan calls for activities that involve all students in
a given classroom. For example, a study in which teachers alter their instruc-
tion necessarily involves all students who are present during that instruction.
If some students do not consent (or assent) to participate in the research, an
alternative activity must be provided for them in another location, and this
alternative activity may need to be supervised by a licensed teacher. In some
cases, if a teacher adopts a new form of instruction or curriculum, then that
lesson becomes the "regular instruction" provided by the teacher, and
404 ALIBALI AND NATHAN
student consent (or assent) is for the use of data collected during the lesson,
rather than for participation in the lesson itself. If the research involves
videotaping naturally occurring instruction, but some students do not
provide consent to be videotaped, it may be possible to arrange seating
and to position the cameras so that those students are not recorded.
rl
N
Be Patient
m
""..
r--
We are often very eager to begin collecting data. We have worked for weeks
rl
'" However, schools and teachers operate with many constraints on their time.
(])
ry,
(])
It is important to allow ample lead time for any project and not to be pushy
rl
rl
o about your desired timetable. There are often special events and activities
U
c:
o
(standardized testing, spirit week, etc.) that make it impossible to collect data
+J
[JJ
o
for prolonged periods of time. School administrators may allow data to be
co
collected only during noninstructional time (e.g., during recess or study hall
or before or after school) or during certain class periods (e.g., for a math-
ematical reasoning study, only during math classes). Collecting data in
schools is often much more protracted than researchers expect. Allow for this
in your planning, and be patient.
Be Flexible
Many challenges arise in conducting research in schools. Space challenges are
common. For example, the only space available may be a corridor or cafe-
teria, or the room next to the band room, where it is difficult to get a good
audio recording. Or, the room you used to collect data for the last 2 weeks
may suddenly become unavailable. You may be assigned to a room without
a desk or a blackboard. In designing your protocol, you should anticipate
limited space and not presuppose the availability of standard furniture.
Availability of participants during the school day can also be a challenge.
You may find out when you arrive for data collection one day that the
children are all on a field trip. A teacher may decide that no child can miss
CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SCHOOLS 405
a special activity. Due to spelling tests, visits from Officer Friendly, practice
fire alarms, and assemblies, the time available for research in any given day is
limited. Ratchet down your expectations for how many participants you can
work with in a given day.
It may be helpful to survey teachers about their schedules, so they can
indicate times that are generally acceptable for data collection. If you are
working with multiple teachers or grade levels, you may be able to stagger
data collection so that you can work with students from one classroom or
grade level when other students are unavailable.
In general, the more flexible you can be, the more likely you will have a
positive experience working in schools. If you are flexible about the space
you use to conduct your study, you will be more likely to complete your
data collection in a timely way. If you allot extra days for data collection,
you will be more likely to meet your goals.
rl
N
Follow Up With Results
m
""..
r--
Principals, teachers, and parents often complain that they never learn the
rl
.. results of the research that they are involved in. Of course, research is usually
+J
'" a protracted process, and it sometimes takes a very long time for the results of
(])
ry,
(])
research to be available. However, a letter to principals and teachers that
rl
rl
o provides preliminary results is always appreciated. In some cases, it may also
U
c:
o
be appropriate to send a brief letter with findings home to parents as well,
+J
[JJ
o
thanking them for their participation. Later, when the results of the research
co
are published, be certain to thank school administrators, teachers, parents,
and students (although you may not wish to reveal specific sites). Send a copy
of the published paper to the school with a thank-you letter.
the end of the study. When your study is completed, you might also send a
thank-you note, a gift, or a donation to the school. We often give small gifts
to schools (e.g., books for the school library) or directly to teachers (e.g.,
items they desire for their classrooms).
Beyond these small tokens of appreciation, researchers can also give some-
thing back by sharing their knowledge and perspectives. Some of the things
that the two of us have done over the years include leading a semester-long
professional development course for teachers, leading a professional devel-
opment workshop on an in-service day, presenting findings at a staff meet-
ing, serving on a district-wide task force about curricula, and serving on
the district external research committee. These are ways to give something
in return for the time, energy, and accommodations that administrators
and teachers give us.
CONCLUSION
rl
N
m
""..
r--
Conducting research in schools presents both challenges and opportunities.
rl
.. Some research questions can only be addressed in school settings (e.g., ques-
+J
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Preparation of this article was funded in part by grants from the Institute of
Education Sciences (R305H060097) and the National Science Foundation
(DRL 0816406 and DRL 0909699).
CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SCHOOLS 407
We thank Susan Wagner Cook and Elizabeth Albro for helpful comments
on a previous version of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2007). Teachers' gestures as a means of scaffolding students'
understanding: Evidence from an early algebra lesson. In R. Goldman, R. Pea, B. Barron, &
S. J. Derry (Eds.), Video research in the learning sciences (pp. 349-365). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Fujimori, Y. (in press). Gestures in the mathematics class-
room: What's the point? In N. Stein & S. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental cognitive science
goes to school. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American
Psychologist, 32, 513-531.
Brown, A L. (1992). Design experiments: Theoretical and methodological challenges in
creating complex interventions in classroom settings. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2,
141-178.
rl
N
Chen, Z., & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Children's acquisition of the control
m of variables strategy. Child Development, 70, 1098-1120.
""..
r-- Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S., & Mahootian, S. (2004). The role of gesture in bilingual
rl
.. education: Does gesture enhance learning? International Journal of Bilingual Education and
+J
Bilingualism, 7, 303-319.
'"
(]) Coffman, J. L., Ornstein, P. A, McCall, L. E., & Curran, P. J. (2008). Linking teachers'
ry,
(])
rl
memory-relevant language and the development of children's memory skills. Developmental
rl
U
o Psychology, 44, 1640-1654.
c:
o
Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B. J., Schatschneider, c., & Underwood, P. (2007).
+J
[JJ Algorithm-guided reading instruction. Science, 315, 464-465.
o
co French, A, & Nathan, M. J. (2004). Under the microscope of research and into the classroom:
Reflections on early algebra learning and instruction. In J. O. Masingila (Ed.), Teachers
engaged in research (pp. 49-68). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Hygge, S. (2003). Classroom experiments on the effects of different noise sources and sound
levels on long-term recall and recognition in children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17,
895-914.
Lehrer, R., Strom, D., & Confrey, J. (2002). Grounding metaphors and inscriptional resonance:
Children's emerging understanding of mathematical similarity. Cognition & Instruction, 20,
359-398.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Stipek, D., Feiler, R., Daniels, D., & Milburn, S. (1995). Effects of different instructional
approaches on young children's achievement and motivation. Child Development, 66,
209-223.
Toth, E. E., Klahr, D., & Chen, Z. (2000). Bridging research and practice: A cognitively based
classroom intervention for teaching experimentation skills to elementary school children.
Cognition & Instruction, 18, 423-459.