Kim Dindia 2011
Kim Dindia 2011
Kim Dindia 2011
net/publication/306285101
CITATIONS READS
45 24,775
2 authors, including:
Jinsuk Kim
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Jinsuk Kim on 28 October 2016.
CHAPTER NINE
Online Self-Disclosure:
A Review of Research
Jinsuk Kim
Kathryn Dindia
Computer-mediated environments, such as social networking sites and online
dating sites, provide us with a variety of opportunities to initiate, develop, and
maintain interpersonal relationships. Self-disclosure is a key factor in developing
relationships in online environments as it is in face-to-face (FtF) contexts (Dindia,
2000). Researchers have been examining how individuals present themselves in
online environments and the factors that influence the way individuals disclose
personal information in online settings. There has been an accumulation of re-
search on self-disclosure with respect to FtF contexts (e.g., Collins & Miller, 1994;
Derlega, Metts, Petronio, & Margulis, 1993; Dindia, 2002; Dindia & Allen, 1992;
Jourard, 1971); however, there has been no comprehensive review of research on
how people disclose personal information in various online settings and how this
disclosure affects interpersonal relationships.
The traditional definition of self-disclosure refers to only intentional, “verbal”
expressions of the self, and does not include nonverbal cues, such as how people
dress, as disclosure. However, this definition of self-disclosure may not be adequate
for online communication. Online self-disclosure may include nonverbal communi-
cation, including pictures posted of self, which may be a conscious mechanism used
to disclose self. This is not the case in FtF settings where there is less opportunity to
change appearances. How we present ourselves physically, both online and offline,
can be manipulated (through dress, make up, etc.); however, in online settings, de-
pending on an individual’s intention, a person’s physical appearance may or may
not be shared. Also, when people include their favorite links on their websites, it
reveals information about themselves. This is another device that can be used, with
or without intention, to reveal information about self that is not available offline.
We feel that it is worthwhile to explore these possible aspects of self-disclosure on
the web in addition to verbal expressions of self. Our definition of online self-
disclosure extends the traditional definition of self-disclosure (verbally revealing self)
to include pictures of self and favorite links posted on the web.
Considering the growth of online social interactions and their impact on
people’s personal and social lives, it is worthwhile to examine self-disclosure in
computer-mediated communication (CMC). How do various aspects of CMC af-
fect self-disclosure? What do we know about online self-disclosure in comparison
to FtF self-disclosure? In this review, we assess the state of the literature on online
Online Self-Disclosure 157
self-disclosure and suggest avenues for future research, including theoretical and
methodological considerations that are important to advancing our understanding
of online self-disclosure. We begin with an examination of the qualities of online
self-disclosure in various modes of CMC. Next, we review empirical findings on
self-disclosure in CMC versus FtF settings and some of the influences on online
self-disclosure such as gender, culture, age, and motivations for using CMC media.
Online Self-Disclosure
Self-Disclosure versus Self-Presentation
CMC researchers have used the term self-presentation and self-disclosure interchangea-
bly to refer to expressions of self in online settings. While there are distinctions,
conceptually and operationally, between self-disclosure and self-presentation
(Schlenker, 1986), those distinctions may not be as fundamental as one supposes.
Self-presentation refers to selectively presenting aspects of oneself to control how
one is perceived by others and is concerned with impression management (Goff-
man, 1959). Self-disclosure refers to revealing personal or private information about
self that is generally unknown and not available from other sources (Derlega et al.,
1993; Jourard & Jaffee, 1970; Pearce & Sharp, 1973; Worthy, Gary, & Kahn,
1969). Self-disclosure always involves an element of self-presentation.
Some researchers have elaborated dimensions of self-disclosure that border on
self-presentation. Cozby (1973) considered depth or intimacy as a dimension of self-
disclosure. Jourard (1971) discussed honesty of self-disclosure. Pearce and Sharp
(1973) implied that conscious, deliberate intent to disclose, or willingness to dis-
close, as well as honesty or authenticity are basic dimensions of self-disclosure.
Valence is another dimension of self-disclosure that directly bears relevance
on the issue of self-disclosure versus self-presentation. Gilbert and Horenstein
(1975) distinguished between intimacy (high or low) and valence of self-disclosure
(positive or negative) based on the writing of Blau (1964). According to Blau,
when engaging in self-disclosure, a person attempts to present qualities that make
himself/herself an attractive person, especially in early stages of relationship de-
velopment. Blau further explains that in initial stages of relationship development,
disclosing aspects of oneself that are negative does not lead to attraction.
Gilbert and Horenstein (1975) studied the relationship of intimacy and valence
of self-disclosure to interpersonal attraction. In an earlier study, Gilbert (1972)
found a negative relationship between intimacy of self-disclosure and attraction,
such that subjects were more attracted to a confederate in a low versus a high inti-
macy condition. Gilbert reasoned that it might be because of the negative informa-
tion in the high-disclosure condition (a girl’s negative feelings about her mother and
herself). Gilbert and Horenstein (1975) hypothesized that valence of self-disclosure,
not intimacy, would predict interpersonal attraction. To test this, they manipulated
both intimacy and valence of self-disclosure. Results of their study indicated a main
158 Computer-Mediated Communication in Personal Relationships
effect of valence of self-disclosure. Participants were more attracted to a confederate
when they disclosed positive rather than negative self-disclosure. There was no effect
of intimacy or the interaction of intimacy and valence on attraction. The authors
concluded that disclosure of negative information to a stranger violates social norms
of appropriateness and does not lead to attraction.
Wheeless (1976) also elaborated multiple dimensions of self-disclosure. A 32-
item measure of various dimensions of self-disclosure was factor-analyzed and Whee-
less concluded that there are at least five dimensions of self-disclosure: intention of
self-disclosure, amount of self-disclosure (including frequency and duration), valence
of self-disclosure (positive or negative), honesty-accuracy of self-disclosure (e.g., “I am
not always honest in my self-disclosure”), and control of general depth or intimacy
of self-disclosure. All of these dimensions of self-disclosure—intention, amount, va-
lence, honesty-accuracy, intimacy of self-disclosure—may be consciously and inten-
tionally altered in order to selectively present self.
The issue of selective self-disclosure is an issue for both FtF and online con-
texts. Self-disclosure is a vehicle of self-presentation, and self-disclosure is not al-
ways open and honest but may involve a conscious and intentional decision to
reveal positive rather than negative aspects of self in order to be perceived as at-
tractive and rewarding. This is no different from FtF self-disclosure. For instance,
people can lie about their age, marital status, and even about their weight and
height to a certain degree; however, lying online is easier because of the lack of or
limited warranting information (Walther & Parks, 2002).
The focus of this chapter is on self-disclosure rather than self-presentation al-
though we will talk about how self-disclosure can be modified to positive presenta-
tion of self. In the following section, we discuss how online self-disclosure differs
from FtF self-disclosure and examine the variables that influence online self-
disclosure.
Self-Disclosure on the Web
Certain attributes of CMC such as reduced nonverbal cues, asynchronity, and
anonymity may influence self-disclosure (McKenna & Bargh, 2000; Walther,
1996) in addition to all the factors found to affect self-disclosure (motivation, set-
ting, target, etc.). Indeed, the web offers unprecedented opportunities for people
to disclose themselves that are unavailable offline. Countless people have taken
advantage of this new setting to disclose information about self in the form of
personal web pages, blogs, social networking sites, dating sites, etc.
Self-disclosure online differs in fundamental ways from FtF self-disclosure:
Online self-disclosure typically is to multiple people whereas FtF self-disclosure
typically occurs in dyads and small groups. However, FtF self-disclosure can occur
in public settings to multiple people, such as an individual disclosing at a meeting,
or in a classroom. But in these cases, the audience is physically present and the
discloser can see the audience. Also, in FtF self-disclosure, whether to an individ-
Online Self-Disclosure 159
ual or multiple people, we try to present our ideal selves in general. Thus, in this
sense, self-disclosure in web pages does not differ from self-disclosure in public FtF
settings.
On the web, communicators do not know who is on the other side of the
screen. The authors can only imagine who their audience could be based on
their assumptions about the audience when producing their messages. In some
instances they imagine their audience as strangers, in other instances they imag-
ine their audience predominantly as friends and relatives. As authors create a
web page, self-presentation motivations may affect their self-disclosure. Con-
structing an ideal self reflects the authors’ desires of how they want to be per-
ceived by the invisible audience. Control of their self-disclosure, what they
disclose and do not disclose about themselves, is one way authors construct
their ideal self.
Most online self-disclosure research has been conducted in largely text-based
and asynchronous online channels. With the emergence of social networking sites
such as MySpace and Facebook, online self-presentations are no longer limited to
text-based descriptions. The photograph is now a central component of online
self-disclosure. On web pages and social networking sites, and even dating sites,
pictures are prime means of conveying information about self. By posting pictures
of themselves and sharing pictures of their family and friends, authors choose to
reveal their ideal self. The viewers make assumptions about how the authors look
and who they are based on these pictures.
In this chapter, we review studies that looked at CMC channels that poten-
tially facilitate self-disclosure, ranging from asynchronous text-based channels to
synchronous media-rich channels. We examine how self-disclosure may occur in
such CMC channels including personal web pages, blogs, social networking sites,
instant messaging, online dating sites (e.g., Match.com and eHarmony.com), and
interactive online games (e.g., MOOs and MUDs). We ultimately explore what
and how people reveal about themselves and factors that prompt people to reveal
themselves in these online settings.
Self-report:
589 (349 Social net-
Choi et al. Depth and Breadth Measures by Parks &
Americans; working site: −.883
(2008) Floyd (1996). 5 depth items were used to
240 Koreans) Not specified
compute the effect size
Social net-
Kim & 176 (92
working sites: Content analysis:
Dindia Americans; .984
MySpace and Self-disclosure units in About me
(2008) 84 Koreans)
Cyworld
172 Computer-Mediated Communication in Personal Relationships
# of Effect
Authors Type of CMC Measure of Self-Disclosure
Participants Size (d)*
Content analysis:
Kim & 98 (49 10 categorical items: Name, gender, role-
Personal
Papacharissi Americans; status, occupation, family information, self- .217
homepages
(2003) 49 Koreans) ascribed identity, origin, religion, resident
information, and physical description
361(112 Self-report: Koreans
Online rela-
Yum & Americans; Depth and Breadth Measures by Parks & d = 1.392
tionship: Not
Hara (2005) 123 Koreans; Floyd (1996). Only depth measures were Japanese
specified
126 Japanese) included to compute the effect size d = .881
Note. +d = American self-disclosure higher than non-American self-disclosure
Cooper and Sportolari (1997) highlight media accounts of people who are certain
they have found their “soul mate” and leave an established relationship, travelling
across the country, to meet people who don’t turn out to be who they seemed.
Others have noted that Internet romantic relationships progress through an
inverted developmental sequence (Merkle & Richardson, 2000). In real life we
meet people, then get to know them; in online we get to know someone and then
choose to meet them (Rheingold, 1993). Some say this makes for an unstable rela-
tionship (Levine, 2000). However, the opposite is plausible. CMC may be charac-
terized by a higher degree of personal investment of time and self-disclosure than
is typical in FtF relationships. This greater investment may result in a stronger
relationship (Merkle & Richardson, 2000).
Again, there is no empirical evidence to support the boom and bust phe-
nomenon. Nonetheless, some have cautioned that when developing relationships
online, you should move from virtual to FtF in a short period of time before un-
realistic expectations have time to build up (Levine, 2000).
Conclusion
In this chapter, we provided a review of online self-disclosure research examining
the effects of key variables in online self-disclosure across various CMC channels.
Some CMC environments that are gaining more popularity, such as audio and
video-conferencing, are not asynchronous. Similarly, audio environments include
vocal cues and video-environments (video-conferencing such as Skype) include a
number of nonverbal cues such as visual appearance, clothing and artifacts, etc.
The only difference between video-conferencing and FtF communication would
be the lack of touch and smell. These CMC environments are becoming more and
more popular. Technology and the uses of technology (for instance, the relatively
recent use of web cameras) change so quickly that any generalizations regarding
self-disclosure on the Internet are problematic. Thus, this review of research on
self-disclosure in CMC will likely be out-of-date before it is published.
References
AP-AOL Instant-Messaging Trends Survey (2007). AP-AOL instant messaging trends survey reveals
popularity of mobile instant messaging. Retrieved July 20, 2009, from AOL: corporate web site
http://corp.aol.com/press-releases/2007/11/ap-aol-instant-messaging-trends-survey-reveals-
popularity-mobile-instant-mess
Banczyk, B., Krämer, N. C., & Senokozlieva, M. N. (2008, May). “The wurst” meets “fatless” in
MySpace. The relationship between self-esteem, personality and self-presentation in an online-
community. Paper presented at the annual conference of the International Communication
Association, Montreal, Canada.
Online Self-Disclosure 177
Barak, A., & Gluck-Ofri, O. (2007). Degree and reciprocity of self-disclosure in online forums.
CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10, 407–417.
Barnlund, D. C. (1975). Public and private self in Japan and the United States. Tokyo: Simul.
Blau, P.M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: John Wiley.
Chen, G. M. (1995). Differences in self-disclosure patterns among Americans versus Chinese.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 84–91.
Cho, S. H. (2007). Effects of motivations and gender on adolescents’ self-disclosure in online
chatting. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10, 339–345.
Choi, W. M., Kim, Y., Sung, Y., & Sohn, D. (2008, May). Motivations and social relationships: A
comparative study of social network sites in the U.S. and Korea. Paper presented at the annual
conference of the International Communication Association, Montreal, Canada.
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. San Diego, CA: Academic.
Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 116, 457–475.
Cooper, A., & Sportolari, L. (1997). Romance in cyberspace: Understanding online attraction.
Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 22, 7–14.
Cozby, P. C. (1973). Self-disclosure: A literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 73–91.
Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. L. (1987). Information technologies. In F. Jablin, L. L. Putnam,
K. Roberts, & L. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizational communication (pp. 420–443).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Margulis, S. T. (1993). Self-disclosure. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Dindia, K. (2000). Self-disclosure, identity, and relationship development: A dialectical perspec-
tive. In K. Dindia & S. W. Duck (Eds.), Communication and personal relationships (pp. 147–
162). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Dindia, K. (2002). Self-disclosure research: Knowledge through meta-analysis. In M, Allen, R.
W., Preiss, B. M., Gayle, & N. Burrell (Eds.), Interpersonal communication: Advances through
meta-analysis (pp. 169–186). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dindia, K., & Allen, M. (1992). Sex differences in self-disclosure: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 112, 106–124.
Dominick, J. R. (1999). Who do you think you are? Personal home pages and self-representation
on the World Wide Web. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 76, 646–658.
Ellison, N., Heino, R., & Gibbs, J. (2006). Managing impressions online: Self-presentation
processes in the online dating environment. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,
11(2), article 2. Retrieved July 20, 2009, from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol11/issue2/
ellison.html
Gibbs, J. L., Ellison, N. B., & Heino, R. D. (2006). Self-presentation in online personals: The
role of anticipated future interaction, self-disclosure, and perceived success in Internet dat-
ing. Communication Research, 33, 1–26.
Gilbert, S. J. (1972).A study of the effects of self-disclosure on interpersonal attraction and trust as a
function of situational appropriateness and the self-esteem of the recipient. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS.
Gilbert, S. J., & Horenstein, D., (1975). The communication of self-disclosure: Level versus
valence. Human Communication Research, 1, 316–322.
Goffman, E. (1959). Presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.
Green, S. C. (2006). Computer-mediated communication in society: A study on self-disclosure. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Indiana State University, Terre Haute, IN.
178 Computer-Mediated Communication in Personal Relationships
Gudykunst, W. B., & Nishida, T. (1986). Attributional confidence in low- and high- context
cultures. Human Communication Research, 12, 525–549.
Hofstede, G. (1998). I, we, and they. In J. N. Martin, T. K. Nakayama, & L. A. Flores (Eds.),
Readings in cultural contexts (pp. 345–356). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Huffaker, D. A., & Calvert, S. L. (2005). Gender, identity, and language use in teenage blogs.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 10(2), article 1. Retrieved July 20, 2009, from
http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol10/issue2/huffaker.html
Joinson, A. N. (2001). Self-disclosure in computer-mediated communication: The role of self-
awareness and visual anonymity, European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 177–192.
Jourard, S. M. (1971). Self-disclosure: An experimental analysis of the transparent self. New York:
Wiley Interscience.
Jourard, S. M., & Jaffe, P .E. (1970). Influence of an interviewer’s disclosure on the self-
disclosing behavior of interviewees. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 17, 252–257.
Kane, C. M. (2008). I’ll see you on MySpace: Self-presentation in a social network website. Unpub-
lished master’s thesis. Cleveland State University, Cleveland, OH.
Kim, J. (2007, November). Gender, motivation and self-disclosure online: A study of Korean social
networking websites. Paper presented at the annual conference of the National Communica-
tion Association, Chicago, IL.
Kim, J., & Dindia, K. (2008, May). Gender, culture, and self-disclosure in cyberspace: A study of Ko-
rean and American social network websites. Paper presented at the annual conference of the
International Communication Association, Montreal, Canada.
Kim, J., Klautke, H., & Serota, K. (2009, May). Effects of relational motivation and age on online self-
disclosure: A content analysis of MySpace profile pages. Paper presented at the annual confer-
ence of the International Communication Association, Chicago, IL.
Kim, H., & Papacharissi, Z. (2003). Cross-cultural differences in online self-presentation: A
content analysis of personal Korean and US homepages. Asian Journal of Communication,
13, 100–119.
Levine, D. (2000). Virtual attraction: What rocks your boat? CyberPsychology and Behavior, 3 (4),
565–574.
Lin, C. A. (1999). Uses and gratifications. In S. G. Singletary & V. P. Richmond (Eds.), Clarifying
communication theories: A hands-on approach (pp. 199–208). Ames: Iowa State University Press.
Ma, M. L., & Leung, L. (2006). Unwillingness-to-communicate, perceptions of the Internet and
self-disclosure in ICQ. Telematics and Informatics, 23, 22–37.
Mallen, M. J., Day, S. X., & Green, M. A. (2003). Online versus face-to-face conversation: An
examination of relational and discourse variables. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice,
Training, 40 (1–2), 155–163.
McKenna, K. Y. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2000). Plan 9 from cyberspace: The implications of the Inter-
net for personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 57–75.
Merkle, E. R., & Richardson, R. A. (2000). Digital dating and virtual relating: Conceptualizing
computer-mediated romantic relationships. Family Relations, 49, 187–192.
Papacharissi, Z. (2002). The self online: The utility of personal home pages, Journal of Broadcast-
ing & Electronic Media, 46, 346–368.
Papacharissi, Z., & Rubin, A. M. (2000). Predictors of Internet use. Journal of Broadcasting and
Electronic Media, 44, 175–196.
Park, N., Jin, B., & Jin, S. A. (2009, May). Motivations, impression management, and self-disclosure in
social network sites. Paper presented at the annual conference of the International Commu-
nication Association, Chicago, IL.
Online Self-Disclosure 179
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communication, 46,
80–97.
Parks, M. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). Making MOOsic: The development of personal relation-
ships online and a comparison to their offline counterparts. Journal of Social and Personal Re-
lationships, 15, 517–537.
Pearce, W. B., & Sharp, S. M. (1973). Self-disclosing communication. Journal of Communication,
23, 409–425.
Pew Internet & American Life Project. (2008). Latest trends. Retrieved July 20, 2009, from
http://www.pewinternet.org/trends.asp
Rheingold, H. (1993). The virtual community: Homesteading on the electronic frontier. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Schlenker, B. R. (1986). Self-identification: Toward an integration of the private and public self.
In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 21–62). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Schouten, A. P., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007a, May). An experimental test of processes
underlying self-disclosure in computer-mediated communication. Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the International Communication Association, San Francisco, CA.
Schouten, A. P., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007b). Precursors and underlying processes of
adolescents’ online self-disclosure: Developing and testing an “Internet-attribute-
perception” model. Media Psychology, 10, 292–315.
Shaw, L. H. (2004). Liking and self-disclosure in computer-mediated and face-to-face interactions. Un-
published master’s thesis, University of California, Berkeley, CA.
Stern, S. R. (2004). Expressions of identity online: Prominent features and gender differences in
adolescents’ world wide web home page. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48, 218–243.
Stritzke, W., G. K., Nguyen, A., & Durkin, K. (2004). Shyness and computer-mediated com-
munication: A self-presentational theory perspective. Media Psychology, 6, 1–22.
Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). Computer-mediated communication effects on disclo-
sure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one another a bit at a
time. Human Communication Research, 28, 317–348.
Toma, C., Hancock, J., & Ellison, N. (2008). Separating fact from fiction: An examination of
deceptive self-presentation in online dating profiles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
34, 1023–1036.
Trammell, K. D., Tarkowski, A., Hofmokl, J., & Sapp, A. M. (2006). Rzeczpospolita blogów
[Republic of Blog]: Examining Polish bloggers through content analysis. Journal of Com-
puter-Mediated Communication, 11(3), article 2. Retrieved July 20, 2009, from
http://jcmc.indiana. edu/vol11/issue3/trammell.html
Tufekci, Z. (2008). Can you see me now? Audience and disclosure regulation in online social
network sites. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 28, 20–36.
Wallace, P. (1999). The Psychology of the Internet. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational per-
spective. Communication Research, 19, 52–90.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal and hy-
perpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3–43.
Walther, J. B. (2007). Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication: Hyper-
personal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition. Computers in Human Behavior,
23, 2538–2557.
Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues filtered out, cues filtered in: Computer-mediated
communication and relationships. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of Inter-
personal Communication (3rd ed., pp. 529–563). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
180 Computer-Mediated Communication in Personal Relationships
Walther, J. B., Van Der Heide, B., Hamel, L., & Shulman, H. (2009). Self-generated versus
other-generated statements and impressions in computer-mediated communication: A test
of warranting theory using Facebook. Communication Research, 36, 229–253.
Walther, J. B., Van Der Heide, B., Kim, S. Y., Westerman, D., & Tong, S. T. (2008). The role
of friends’ behavior on evaluations of individuals’ Facebook profiles: Are we known by the
company we keep? Human Communication Research, 34, 28–49.
Wang, H., & Andersen, P. A. (2007, May). Computer-mediated communication in relationship main-
tenance: An examination of self-disclosure in long-distance friendships. Paper presented at the an-
nual meeting of the International Communication Association, San Francisco, CA.
Wheeless, L. R. (1976). Self-disclosure and interpersonal solidarity: Measurement, validation,
and relationships. Human Communication Research, 3, 47–61.
Wheeless, L. R., Erickson, K. V., & Behrens, J. S. (1986). Cultural difference in disclosiveness
as a function of locus of control. Communication Monographs, 23, 36–46.
Williams, A. L., & Merten, M. J. (2008). A review of online social networking profiles by ado-
lescents: Implications for future research and intervention. Adolescence, 43(170), 253–274.
Worthy, M., Gary, A..L., & Kahn, G. M. (1969). Self-disclosure and exchange process. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 13, 59–63.
Yum, Y.-O., & Hara, K. (2005). Computer-mediated relationship development: A cross-cultural
comparison. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11(1), article 7. Retrieved July 20,
2009, from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol11/issue1/yum.html